Abstract
Rates of ADHD diagnosis vary across regions in many countries. However, no prior study has investigated how much within-country geographic variation in ADHD diagnoses is explained by variation in ADHD symptom levels. We examine whether ADHD symptom levels explain variation in ADHD diagnoses among children and adolescents using nationwide survey and register data in Norway. Geographical variation in incidence of ADHD diagnosis was measured using Norwegian registry data from the child and adolescent mental health services for 2011–2016. Geographical variation in ADHD symptom levels in clinics’ catchment areas was measured using data from the Norwegian mother, father and child cohort study for 2011–2016 (n = 39,850). Cross-sectional associations between ADHD symptom levels and the incidence of ADHD diagnoses were assessed with fractional response models. Geographical variation in ADHD diagnosis rates is much larger than what can be explained by geographical variation in ADHD symptoms levels. Treatment in the Norwegian child and adolescent mental health services is free, universally available upon referral, and practically without competition from the private sector. Factors beyond health care access and unequal symptom levels seem responsible for the geographical variation in ADHD diagnosis.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00787-022-01996-7.
Keywords: Health services; Psychiatry; Child health; Adolescent; Norwegian mother, father and child cohort study; MoBa; Norwegian patient registry; ADHD; Symptoms
Introduction
Diagnosis rates of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) vary across many countries [1]. International comparisons suffer validity problems due to differing diagnostic standards and methodology (Fig. 1A) [1]. Similar geographic variation in diagnostic prevalence, however, exists within countries with a uniform diagnostic standard, for example, Norway (Fig. 1B) [2–6].
Although ADHD symptoms are fundamental in diagnosing ADHD, no prior research investigates the extent to which geographical variation in ADHD symptoms explain geographical variation in ADHD diagnoses. ADHD diagnosis is a precondition for ADHD treatment, especially pharmacological treatment. Geographical variation in ADHD diagnoses that does not correlate with variation in symptoms would thus raise concerns about over- and undertreatment of ADHD [7–10].
We study the extent to which geographic variation in ADHD symptoms explains geographic variation in ADHD diagnosis in Norway, where 5% of children and adolescents are diagnosed with ADHD [11]. Studying Norway has three distinct advantages. First, the availability of comprehensive, nationwide, and geo-coded data on ADHD symptoms and diagnosis. Specifically, we combine nationwide survey data on ADHD symptoms from the Norwegian mother, father and child cohort study (MoBa) with nationwide register data on the incidence of ADHD diagnosis from the Norwegian Patient Registry (NPR). Second, Norway’s universal and free health care system (with a marginal private sector) largely rules out variation in healthcare access as an explanation for variation in ADHD diagnosis. Third, nationwide diagnostic standards largely rule out another explanation for geographical variation in ADHD diagnosis. Norwegian child and adolescent mental health services (CAMHS) are organized by clinics serving catchment areas comprised of one or more municipalities/city districts, where only specialists diagnose patients with ADHD and initiate treatment using national treatment guidelines [12].
The aim of this study is to examine whether ADHD symptom levels explain variation in ADHD diagnoses among children and adolescents. We explore three research questions: (1) Does between-clinics variation in the incidence rate of ADHD diagnosis exceed chance variation? (2) Does between-clinics variation in symptom levels of ADHD exceed chance variation? (3) Does between-clinics variation in the incidence rate of ADHD diagnosis, conditional on symptoms levels of ADHD, exceed chance variation?
Methods
ADHD symptoms
We measured ADHD symptoms levels for the general population using mother-reported data from MoBa for 2011–2016. We used two measures of ADHD symptoms: (1) The proportion of the population in a clinics’ catchment area with ADHD symptoms equals to or above the 95th percentile, in line with a country prevalence of 5% among children and adolescents [11]. (2) As a sensitivity measure, we used a 90th percentile cut-off as some clinics may be more prone to diagnose ADHD than others. Proportions of children with high ADHD symptom levels were calculated with individuals who scored above the thresholds as the numerator and the total participants as the denominator.
Data were reported when the child was 8 years old, corresponding to the average age of diagnosis in Norway [13]. The Parent/Teacher Rating Scale for Disruptive Behavior Disorders (RS-DBD) was used, which has good instrument validity and reliability [14, 15], and corresponds with rating scales used in the Norwegian CAMHS. RS-DBD measures inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity on 18 items with the same response options: (1) Never/Rarely, (2) Sometimes, (3) Often, (4) Very often (Table S1) [12]. We pooled data on ADHD symptoms for 2011–2016; birth years 2003–2008 (n = 39,850). 323 individuals in MoBa were dropped as they did not have data on municipality.
MoBa is a population-based pregnancy cohort study conducted by the Norwegian Institute of Public Health. Participants were recruited from all over Norway from 1999 to 2008. Women consented to participation in 41% of the pregnancies. The cohort includes 114,500 children, 95,200 mothers and 75,200 fathers [16]. MoBa was established with a license from the Norwegian Data Protection Agency and approval from The Regional Committees for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REK), and is now regulated by the Norwegian Health Registry Act. We use version 12 of the quality-assured data files released for research in January 2019, where geo-linkage was available for cohorts from 2002. This study was approved by REK (2017/2205).
ADHD diagnosis
We used municipality-level data on all new patients registered with ADHD diagnosis in NPR (ICD-10, F90.0) between 2011 and 2016. We calculated the cumulative incidence proportion of ADHD among individuals aged 0–18 years, defined as the number of new ADHD diagnoses (n = 19,342) divided by the number of all individuals in that population using population data from Statistics Norway [17, 18]. For that purpose, we used the population mid-value for 2011–2016, conventionally defined for even numbers as the mean of the two mid-values (n = 1,189,496).
Clinics’ catchment areas
Clinics are our unit of analysis because decision-making on diagnosis and potential treatment cultures manifest at clinic level. Clinics’ catchment area was inferred in collaboration with NPR using data on patient contacts at clinics by patients’ residence municipality in 2009. CAMHS are organized with clinics serving one or more municipalities (and/or city districts in Norway’s four largest cities) which comprise the clinics’ catchment area. The clinic a municipality is served by was defined as the clinic with the highest number of patient contacts from that municipality. For example, if a clinic in northern Norway is registered with 25 contacts from patients residing in a municipality in western Norway, and 800 contacts from patients residing in a municipality in northern Norway, the latter was defined as the main municipality the clinic serves. There were no major changes in municipality codes during the period of this study. Cities are represented by one clinic as we only have municipality-level data, reducing number of clinics from 73 to 63. The clinics catchment area list was quality-assessed by examining clinics’ own descriptions of catchment areas. When combining data from MoBa and NPR, six municipalities were not merged as these were not represented in MoBa in 2011–2016, giving a total of 416 municipalities. We use geographical data on latitude and longitude collected by Fiva et al. [19] to map and examine clusters of ADHD diagnosis. NPR data follow municipality classification per 2018 (n = 422) while the map data follow the municipality classification prior to 2018 (n = 428). For the map data, we adjusted for five municipality mergers providing six additional municipalities (n = 428) given same value as the municipality they were merged to. NPR is a health registry with information on all individuals who have received or are awaiting treatment in specialist healthcare services since 2008 [20]. (Figure 1A) is based on prevalence data on studies from the UK [21], Sweden [22], Finland [23], Greece [24], Ireland [25], and Norway [11], with the remaining countries covered in a comparative study [26].
Statistical analyses
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was applied to measure the latent ADHD symptoms construct from the symptoms score items in RS-DBD [27]. Goodness-of-fit statistics for the CFA used to measure ADHD symptoms aligns with commonly accepted values (CFI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.07, SRMR = 0.05, p > χ2 = < 0.0001; full model in Supplement). Data on symptoms and diagnosis of ADHD on the individual- and municipality-levels were aggregated to the clinic-level to examine between-clinics variation and associations between ADHD symptoms levels and ADHD diagnosis.
We examined the extent to which ADHD diagnosis and ADHD symptoms varied at clinic level by comparing observed proportions to expected values under of equal probability of diagnosis/symptoms across clinics. Confidence intervals under were bootstrapped using 10,000 draws from the binomial distribution with probabilities equal the grand mean. Observed proportions outside of the bootstrapped 95% CI were considered larger than chance variation.
Variance-components models were used to partition the variance in ADHD symptoms and ADHD diagnosis with municipality-level data nested within clinics. We examined variation in ADHD diagnosis and symptoms levels using the coefficient of variation (CV), a variability measure for the extent of variation relative to the mean calculated as the variable’s standard deviation (SD) divided by its mean value. Bootstrapping was used to derive the expected distribution of CV under of equal probability of symptoms/diagnosis across clinics. The observed CV was compared to the null distribution to examine the probability of observing the CV by chance under .
We used fractional response regression models (FRM) [28] to test whether ADHD diagnosis is associated with ADHD symptoms. The mean incidence proportion of ADHD diagnosis was predicted by ADHD symptom levels in two separate models: one with 95th percentile cut-off and one with 90th percentile cut-off as the predictor. Heteroskedasticity-consistent standard errors were used. The model was weighted by number of MoBa-respondents within catchment areas. Average marginal effects (AME) were reported.
We examined the extent of unexplained variation in incidence of ADHD diagnosis by CV for the residual and for the squared correlation coefficient between the observed and the predicted values. To formally test whether the unexplained variation in ADHD diagnosis conditional on ADHD symptoms was larger than expected by chance, we compared the observed CV to the distribution of expected CVs under , where was given by the predicted values from the FRM model. Since this prediction also contains statistical uncertainty, we conducted this analysis using a bootstrap approach.
Results
The cumulative incidence of ADHD diagnosis was 0.016 (SD: 0.007, min–max: 0.004–0.039, IQR: 0.01–0.02) in 2011–2016. The proportion of children scoring over the 95th percentile on ADHD symptoms was 0.05 (SD: 01, min–max: 0–0.14, IQR: 0.045–0.053). For children scoring over the 90th percentile, the proportion was 0.1 (SD: 0.14, min–max: 0–0.14, IQR: 0.09–0.11). Two clinics had no MoBa-respondents scoring ≥ 95%, while one clinic had no participants scoring over ≥ 90%. There was nearly a tenfold difference in the incidence of ADHD diagnosis proportion from the clinic with the lowest to the highest level. (Figure 1) presents municipality-level geographical variation in the incidence rate of ADHD diagnosis showing clustering of areas with higher and lower levels of incidence of ADHD diagnosis. The intra-class correlation (ICC) from variance-components models for the incidence of ADHD diagnosis was 50.2% [CI 95%: 39 to 61] indicating that half of the total variance was attributed to the clinic level. The ICC for ADHD symptoms is < 0.01% for proportions of children with symptom scores ≥ 95% and 0.15% [CI 95%: 0.03 to 0.9] for proportions of children with symptom scores ≥ 90%.
In (Fig. 2), the upper graph in Panel A presents ADHD diagnosis proportions by clinics. The vertical line is the grand mean of observed population-weighted ADHD diagnosis with 95% CI for chance variation from 10,000 draws, whereas observed proportions (blue circles) outside 95% CI were larger than expected by chance. The observed coefficient of variation (CV) for the incidence of ADHD diagnosis proportions across clinics was 45.6% (p = 0).
The lower graph of Panel A presents the null distribution for CV with the excess variation in ADHD diagnosis compared to the mean of the null distribution measured in percentage difference. Similarly, the upper graph in Panel B presents proportions of children scoring ≥ 95th percentile for ADHD symptoms. Here, few observed proportions were outside 95% CI. The CV was 18% (p = 0.23), thus there was not support for more than chance variation as the observed CV was well within the null distribution (lower graph Panel B). In Panel C, there are more observed proportions of children scoring ≥ 90th percentile for ADHD symptoms compared to Panel B. The CV was 15% (p = 0.025), and there is evidence for more than chance variation.
From fractional response regression models (FRM) at the clinic level with the incidence of ADHD diagnosis as the outcome, the average marginal effect (AME) shows that the proportion of ADHD diagnosis increases 0.26 percentage points (95% CI: [0.09 to 0.42], p = 0.002) when the proportion of children and adolescents with ADHD symptoms ≥ 95% increase with one percentage point. We did not find support for an association between ADHD symptoms ≥ 90% and ADHD diagnosis (AME: 0.09, 95% CI: [− 0.06 to 0.24], p = 0.25) (Supplementary, Table S3).
Predicted values from FRMs were used for analyses of unexplained variation (Fig. 3). The 95% CI were centered at 0 for no differences between observed and predicted values. There was large between-clinics variation in residuals, with few observed residuals in the 95% CI for chance variation for both models with proportions of ADHD symptoms ≥ 95% (Fig. 3A, upper graph) and ≥ 90% (Fig. 3B, upper graph) as predictors. Moreover, the observed CV for the residuals was considerably higher than the distribution of CVs under the null distribution for both models (Fig. 3A, B, lower graphs), which was supported by formal tests (Table 1). Overall, the residuals were still large after adjusting for ADHD symptoms ≥ 95% (or ADHD symptoms ≥ 90%), indicating that other factors are influential in explaining the remaining difference between observed and predicted proportions of ADHD diagnoses.
Table 1.
Model | (1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
ADHD diagnosis, unconditional | ADHD symptoms ≥ 95%, unconditional | ADHD symptoms ≥ 90%, unconditional | Residuals:ADHD diagnosis, conditional on symptoms ≥ 95% | Residuals:ADHD diagnosis, conditional on symptoms ≥ 90% | |
Coefficient of variation (CV) | |||||
Observed CV | .46 | .18 | .14 | .45 | .44 |
Mean CV under | .06 | .17 | .12 | .17 | .09 |
[Min, Max] | [.04–.08] | [.12–.24] | [.08–.16] | [.08–.54] | [.05–.18] |
Test statistic: Percent deviation between observed CV and mean CV under H0 | 713.9 | 7.6 | 19.4 | 192.3 | 414.0 |
p-value | 0 | .23 | .06 | 0 | 0 |
R2 | – | – | – | .13 | .04 |
The coefficient of variation (CV) shows how much variation there is relative to the mean and is calculated as the variable’s standard deviation divided by its mean value. for CV is that variation does not exceed chance variation. p value is proportion of expected values under with values equal to, or above, observed value from 10,000 trials. Models 4 and 5 are weighted by participants in MoBa. R2 from fractional regression models with diagnosis as response and symptom levels as explanatory variable
Discussion
Summary of findings
We found support for large between-clinics variation in the incidence rate of ADHD diagnosis and considerably less variation in high levels of ADHD symptoms at the ≥ 90 percent level. There was no evidence for more than chance variation in symptoms at the ≥ 95 percent level. Municipalities clustered into areas with higher and lower levels of ADHD diagnostic incidence, where half of this variance could be ascribed to the clinic level. While there was evidence for a positive association between the incidence rate of ADHD diagnosis and high levels of ADHD symptoms, the explained variance in the incidence rate of ADHD diagnosis after controlling for ADHD symptoms was low.
Strengths and limitations
There are two considerable strengths to this study. First, the analyses are based on a unique combination of nationwide geo-coded data on both symptoms and diagnosis of ADHD. We are not aware of other similar data sources that can be used to examine the research questions in this study, nor have we discovered any study on within-country variation in ADHD diagnosis that includes data on ADHD symptoms. Second, the Norwegian context is ideal due to the single provider healthcare system with only a small portion of patients using private sector healthcare reducing concerns of selection biases into healthcare.
There are limitations to consider. First, ecological bias may be a concern as both symptoms and diagnosis of ADHD are individual-level data aggregated to clinic-level variables. However, we examined clinic-level variation and associations and did not draw inferences for the individual level [29]. Second, statistical bias could be introduced by the modifiable areal unit problem since several units of observation can be used [29]. Clinics are arguably more relevant compared to other area definitions as patients are diagnosed at clinics and there may be local treatment cultures [30]. Third, the association between symptoms and diagnosis of ADHD may be subject to confounding bias either toward or away from the null. The proportion of individuals with high levels of ADHD symptoms includes treated and untreated ADHD, where the former reduces symptoms and the association between symptoms and diagnosis. As well, population composition and other potential confounders may vary between clinics. Moreover, ADHD is highly heritable (88%) [31]. Siblings live in the same catchment area which may inflate familial risk factors for ADHD. However, the focus of this study is the unconditional, and conditional on ADHD symptoms, between-clinics variation in ADHD diagnosis. Fourth, areas with high levels of ADHD diagnosis may raise awareness and increase parent- and teacher reporting of ADHD symptoms and referral rates to specialist health services, causing a reverse causal path between rates of diagnosis and ADHD symptoms. There is currently no strong empirical evidence supporting this concern. Fifth, there are at least two potential sources of selection bias. MoBa may be affected by sampling bias with overrepresentation of individuals with high SES [32], and underrepresentation of non-Norwegians, young females, single households, mothers with > 2 births or previous stillbirths, and smokers [33]. NPR only includes patients in the specialist health services and lower SES predicts more health services use [34]. Both selection mechanisms can affect observed variations and associations between symptoms and diagnosis of ADHD. Sixth, a concern may be chance findings, e.g., due to sample size, statistical power, or researcher degrees of freedom. While the Type 1 error rate is constant in increasing sample size, the Type II error rate decreases. Thus, if this study is underpowered, there is no way of knowing whether failing to reject the null hypothesis is due to insufficient sample size or a real lack of effect. As the sample consists of clinics in Norway, we could only increase the sample size using city–district codes for the four largest cities, which we did not have access to. Sixth, our measure of ADHD symptoms is only restricted to children when they are 8 years old. While this corresponds with the mean age at diagnosis, it may not perfectly reflect symptom levels for the children and adolescents from 0 to 18 years whom we have diagnosis data on.
Contribution and interpretation
This is the first study to combine nationwide data on both symptoms and diagnosis of ADHD to examine the extent to which within-country variation in ADHD diagnosis is explained by ADHD symptoms. We find considerable between-clinics variation in ADHD diagnosis despite free access to healthcare, a comprehensive welfare state, and comparatively low social inequality, which reduces the potential impact of socioeconomic conditions. This finding is in line with existing research on within-country variation in ADHD diagnosis, where clusters of municipalities with high and low incidence of ADHD diagnosis have been identified [4–6]. Regional differences in diagnostic practice have been presented as the most plausible explanation in another Norwegian study on geographic variation in ADHD diagnosis [6]. A survey supports that clinician’s policy toward ADHD treatment varies [35]. The main question from a health policy perspective is whether the observed variation is unwarranted or fully explained by patient and provider characteristics [30]. The high remaining residual variation in ADHD diagnosis after controlling for ADHD symptoms suggests that other factors are important drivers of between-clinics variation in ADHD diagnosis.
Implications
ADHD symptoms should arguably explain a considerable part of between-clinics variation in ADHD diagnosis since the diagnosis is based on the assessment of symptoms, functional impairment, and differential diagnosis. The inherent puzzle clinicians are faced with in diagnosing patients with symptoms around the threshold for diagnosis may introduce a random component in being diagnosed with ADHD based on the patient’s geographical residence. Accordingly, for some patients, being diagnosed with ADHD and receiving ADHD medication may ultimately come down to residing in one catchment area rather than another. From a health policy perspective, this is worrisome as it challenges the principle of equal healthcare regardless of geography. From a research perspective, the between-clinics variation in ADHD diagnosis presents a potential quasi-experiment that can inform clinical practice on effects of ADHD diagnosis and treatment [5, 36]. Future research may consider a quasi-experimental approach that exploits geographical variation in diagnosis or medication rates to fill knowledge gaps that are challenging to address with randomized experiments.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
The Norwegian mother, father and child cohort study is supported by the Norwegian Ministry of Health and Care Services and the Ministry of Education and Research. We are grateful to all the participating families in Norway who take part in this on-going cohort study. The study was supported by funding from Research Council of Norway (288585/IAR) and Western Norway Regional Health Authority (912197). HDZ and EY was supported by funding from the European Research Council Consolidator Grant ERC-CoG-2018 EQOP [grant number 818425].
Author contribution
TW-H: conceptualization, data curation, methodology, formal analysis, visualization, writing–original draft, project administration. SM: conceptualization, data curation, methodology, formal analysis, visualization, writing–original draft. FE: conceptualization, writing–original draft. IL: conceptualization, project administration. AC: conceptualization. IB: writing–original draft. AH: writing–original draft. EY: writing–original draft, resources. AM: conceptualization, supervision, methodology, writing–original draft, resources, project administration. HDZ: conceptualization, supervision, data curation, methodology, formal analysis, visualization, writing–original draft, resources, project administration.
Funding
Open access funding provided by University of Bergen (incl Haukeland University Hospital).
Data availability
Data cannot be publicly shared due to strict data privacy laws. Data from the Norwegian mother, father and child cohort study (MoBa) and the Norwegian Patient Registry can be made available by application to the data owners.
Declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Polanczyk G, et al. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(6):942–948. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Fulton BD, et al. National variation of ADHD diagnostic prevalence and medication use: health care providers and education policies. Psychiatr Serv. 2009;60(8):1075–1083. doi: 10.1176/ps.2009.60.8.1075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Price A, et al. Regional analysis of UK primary care prescribing and adult service referrals for young people with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. BJPsych Open. 2020 doi: 10.1192/bjo.2019.94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Madsen KB, et al. Geographic analysis of the variation in the incidence of ADHD in a country with free access to healthcare: a Danish cohort study. Int J Health Geogr. 2015;14:24–24. doi: 10.1186/s12942-015-0018-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mykletun A, et al. Causal modelling of variation in clinical practice and long-term outcomes of ADHD using Norwegian registry data: the ADHD controversy project. BMJ Open. 2021;11(1):e041698. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-041698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Surén, P et al (2018) Diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder among children in Norway. Tidsskr Nor Legeforen. 10.4045/tidsskr.18.0418 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 7.Biederman J. Is ADHD overdiagnosed in Scandinavia? Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012;126(2):85–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01878.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Parens E, Johnston J. Facts, values, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): an update on the controversies. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2009;3(1):1–17. doi: 10.1186/1753-2000-3-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kazda L, et al. Overdiagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic scoping review. JAMA Netw Open. 2021;4(4):e215335–e215335. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.5335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kjeldsen BV, Jensen SOW, Munk-Jørgensen P. Increasing number of incident ADHD cases in psychiatric treatment. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012;126(2):151–152. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01875.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ullebø AK, et al. Prevalence of the ADHD phenotype in 7-to 9-year-old children: effects of informant, gender and non-participation. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2012;47(5):763–769. doi: 10.1007/s00127-011-0379-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.The Norwegian Directorate of Health . ADHD/hyperkinetic disorder - national guideline for assessment, treatment and follow-up. Oslo, Norway: The Norwegian Directorate of Health; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Oerbeck B, et al. Child and parental characteristics of medication use for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2020;30(7):456–464. doi: 10.1089/cap.2019.0019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Silva RR, et al. A rating scale for disruptive behavior disorders, based on the DSM-IV item pool. Psychiatr Q. 2005;76(4):327–339. doi: 10.1007/s11126-005-4966-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Friedman-Weieneth JL, et al. The disruptive behavior rating scale—parent version (DBRS-PV): factor analytic structure and validity among young preschool children. J Atten Disord. 2009;13(1):42–55. doi: 10.1177/1087054708322991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Magnus P, et al. Cohort profile update: the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa) Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45(2):382–388. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lash TL, Rothman KJ. Measures of Occurrence, in Modern Epidemiology. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer; 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Statistics Norway (2021) Table 7459: Age and gender distribution by municipality, county, and country population (K) 1986–2021
- 19.Fiva JH, Halse AH, Natvik GJ (2017) Local Government Dataset. Available at https://www.jon.fiva.no/data.htm
- 20.The Norwegian Health Directorate. (2019) Data set [web document]. https://www.helsedirektoratet.no/tema/statistikk-registre-og-rapporter/helsedata-og-helseregistre/norsk-pasientregister-npr/datasett. 14 Apr 2021
- 21.Russell G, et al. Prevalence of parent-reported ASD and ADHD in the UK: findings from the millennium cohort study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(1):31–40. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1849-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kadesjö B, Gillberg C. The comorbidity of ADHD in the general population of Swedish school-age children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(4):487–492. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Smalley SL, et al. Prevalence and psychiatric comorbidity of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in an adolescent finnish population. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(12):1575–1583. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3181573137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Skounti M, et al. Prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in schoolchildren in Athens, Greece association of ADHD subtypes with social and academic impairment. ADHD Atten Deficit Hyperact Disord. 2010;2(3):127–132. doi: 10.1007/s12402-010-0029-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Lynch F, et al. Challenging times: prevalence of psychiatric disorders and suicidal behaviours in Irish adolescents. J Adolesc. 2006;29(4):555–573. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Fayyad J, et al. Cross-national prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190:402–409. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.034389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kline RB. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York: The Guilford Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wooldridge JM. Econometric analysis of cross section and panel data. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Morgenstern H, Wakefield J, et al. Ecologic Studies and Analysis. In: Lash TL, et al., editors. Modern epidemiology. Philadelphia: Wolters Kluwer; 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wennberg J, McPherson K, Goodman D. Small area analysis and the challenge of practice variation, in medical practice variations. In: Johnson A, Stukel T, editors. Springer. Boston MA: US; 2016. pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Larsson H, et al. The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psychol Med. 2014;44(10):2223–2229. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Biele G, et al. Bias from self selection and loss to follow-up in prospective cohort studies. Eur J Epidemiol. 2019;34(10):927–938. doi: 10.1007/s10654-019-00550-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Nilsen RM, et al. Self-selection and bias in a large prospective pregnancy cohort in Norway. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2009;23(6):597–608. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2009.01062.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Wichstrøm L, et al. Predicting service use for mental health problems among young children. Pediatrics. 2014;133(6):1054–1060. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dekkers TJ, et al. Which factors determine clinicians’ policy and attitudes towards medication and parent training for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2022;31(3):483–493. doi: 10.1007/s00787-021-01735-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Widding-Havneraas T, et al. Preference-based instrumental variables in health research rely on important and underreported assumptions: a systematic review. J Clin Epidemiol. 2021;139:269–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data cannot be publicly shared due to strict data privacy laws. Data from the Norwegian mother, father and child cohort study (MoBa) and the Norwegian Patient Registry can be made available by application to the data owners.