Skip to main content
Molecular Autism logoLink to Molecular Autism
. 2023 Aug 31;14:32. doi: 10.1186/s13229-023-00564-3

Linking functional and structural brain organisation with behaviour in autism: a multimodal EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP) study

Lennart M Oblong 1,, Alberto Llera 1,2, Ting Mei 1, Koen Haak 1, Christina Isakoglou 1, Dorothea L Floris 1,10, Sarah Durston 4, Carolin Moessnang 5, Tobias Banaschewski 6, Simon Baron-Cohen 7, Eva Loth 8, Flavio Dell’Acqua 8, Tony Charman 3, Declan G M Murphy 8, Christine Ecker 8,9, Jan K Buitelaar 1,2, Christian F Beckmann 1; The EU-AIMS LEAP Group, Natalie J Forde 1
PMCID: PMC10472578  PMID: 37653516

Abstract

Neuroimaging analyses of brain structure and function in autism have typically been conducted in isolation, missing the sensitivity gains of linking data across modalities. Here we focus on the integration of structural and functional organisational properties of brain regions. We aim to identify novel brain-organisation phenotypes of autism. We utilised multimodal MRI (T1-, diffusion-weighted and resting state functional), behavioural and clinical data from the EU AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP) from autistic (n = 206) and non-autistic (n = 196) participants. Of these, 97 had data from 2 timepoints resulting in a total scan number of 466. Grey matter density maps, probabilistic tractography connectivity matrices and connectopic maps were extracted from respective MRI modalities and were then integrated with Linked Independent Component Analysis. Linear mixed-effects models were used to evaluate the relationship between components and group while accounting for covariates and non-independence of participants with longitudinal data. Additional models were run to investigate associations with dimensional measures of behaviour. We identified one component that differed significantly between groups (coefficient = 0.33, padj = 0.02). This was driven (99%) by variance of the right fusiform gyrus connectopic map 2. While there were multiple nominal (uncorrected p < 0.05) associations with behavioural measures, none were significant following multiple comparison correction. Our analysis considered the relative contributions of both structural and functional brain phenotypes simultaneously, finding that functional phenotypes drive associations with autism. These findings expanded on previous unimodal studies by revealing the topographic organisation of functional connectivity patterns specific to autism and warrant further investigation.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s13229-023-00564-3.

Keywords: Linked ICA, Autism spectrum disorder, Brain-behaviour associations

Background

Autism spectrum disorders (autism) are characterised by difficulty with communication and social interaction and repeated stereotyped behaviours and/or altered sensory processing [1]. Neuroimaging studies of autism have furthered our understanding of its neurobiology; however, the majority of studies thus far have been limited in their ability to identify valid and reliable biomarkers. Classical neuroimaging studies have used case–control designs, together with a typical focus on small samples and single data modalities. This approach potentially obfuscates biomarker identification since individual variability is masked by focusing only on between group effects, thereby ignoring group heterogeneity. Furthermore, sensitivity to associations is lacking due to the fragmented analytical approach of single modality analysis.

Recent significant progress in neuroimaging studies has been made by addressing and utilising the heterogeneity of autism—focusing on dimensional measures of behaviour that cut across diagnostic boundaries [27]—and by using advanced methodologies that account for diverse patterns of variance across the brain rather than focal deviations [3, 813]. Three of these latter studies [3, 8, 9] revealed covariation patterns of grey matter density and deviations in cortical thickness, associated with autism in the same sample considered in the present paper. As well as primary univariate findings of association with autism, these papers also revealed complex associations between all isolated imaging phenotypes and multiple continuous clinical measures as determined in a multivariate analysis conducted with canonical correlation analysis (CCA). These papers show the potential of data-driven approaches to leverage the heterogeneity of autism in pursuit of biomarkers.

Moreover, it has been demonstrated that moving from separate unimodal MRI analysis to integrated multimodal analysis with linked independent component analysis (LICA) generates brain phenotypes that strongly relate to demographic and behavioural data, far exceeding the potential of any individual unimodal approach seen to date [14, 15]. The assumption with multimodal approaches is that underlying pathophysiological processes are reflected in multiple aspects of neurobiology, such that different indices of these biological measures can be used to achieve a joint integrated picture of these processes. Thus, by integrating information from multiple sources we gain increased sensitivity to detect associations with behaviour. Specifically, LICA, an extension of traditional ICA, decomposes such multimodal data to generate a set of spatial maps, subject-specific and modality-specific contributions for each independent component (IC). Recently, this method has been successfully implemented in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) research to identify novel brain phenotypes associated with ADHD severity [16] and in autism research to integrate clinical and event related potential (ERP) data, which identified early neuronal processes that predicted clinical outcome [17]. There are also a small number of autism studies that integrate different MRI modalities using LICA. However, one of these utilised LICA for visualisation purposes only [18] while the other 2 investigated the LICA subject courses in analysis. Both these studies integrated voxel based morphology (VBM) grey matter density maps and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) metrics. Itahashi and colleagues identified one component that showed covariations in both grey matter and white matter morphology associated with autism diagnosis [19] in a relatively small sample (n = 92 total) of adult males, all with an IQ of > 80. More recently a second study from Mei and colleagues [20] analysed a larger group with broader spread across IQ, age and sex, and identified one multimodal pattern associated with autism diagnosis [20]. The grey and white matter covariation patterns differed between these two papers, likely due to the differences in the sample demographics.

Previous implementations of MRI-based LICA, mentioned above, used traditional unimodal data extraction techniques. Here we attempted to improve upon previous approaches by incorporating more analytically advanced initial processing steps developed in recent years, leading to more biologically plausible unimodal representations of the brain prior to integration. Using these advanced unimodal feature extraction methods may provide more accurate characterisation of the fine-grained features of the brain. For instance, we know that there is behaviourally relevant functional organisation within brain regions and potential multiplicity of these regions [2125]. Connectopic mapping is able to characterise within region of interest (ROI) variations in functional connectivity while simultaneously dealing with potential functional multiplicity [2125], substantially improving upon traditionally used network-based functional connectivity metrics. We therefore use connectopic mapping to identify functional connectopies (gradients) within our resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) data. Additionally, structural organisation in the brain can be modelled from diffusion MRI (dMRI) data with non-tensor based tractography, providing a more biologically plausible representation of the white matter than traditional tensor-based methods which fail to model complex fibre architectures [26]). We therefore implement a non-tensor based method to better address this complexity. Further, implementing probabilistic tractography from each voxel within an ROI allows us to probe the potential spatial organisations of structural connectivity. We hypothesised that these advanced approaches to unimodal feature extraction coupled with multimodal integration via LICA and implemented on a large sample would provide us with additional sensitivity to detect brain-behaviour relationships relevant to autism.

Here we test if the implementation of improved modelling of functional and structural connectivity may yield a more fine-grained characterisation of neurobiological variation in autism, and explore if an integrated multimodal approach can identify new, MRI-based autism-related brain phenotypes. Ultimately, the identification of more fine-grained, multimodal autism related phenotypes could enhance our understanding of how autism is represented in the brain across modality boundaries, and additionally increase sensitivity to detect associations with behavioural measures of autism. This could improve the early detection of autism and might lead to novel avenues of intervention down the line.

Methods

Participants

Data from the European Autism Interventions—A Multicentre Study for Developing New Medications (EU-AIMS) Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP) [27] was used. This is a large European multicentre study focusing on identifying and validating biomarkers for autism. In total, six centres are involved; however, due to limited availability of high quality MRI data in all modalities, one site was excluded. The 5 remaining sites were: Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, United Kingdom; Radboud University Medical Centre, Nijmegen, the Netherlands; Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany; Cambridge University, Cambridge, UK and University Medical Centre Utrecht, the Netherlands. Local ethics committees in each participating centre approved the study and written informed consent was provided by all participants and/or their legal guardians (for those < 16/18 years old [country dependent] or legally incapacitated). Details of the project have been outlined elsewhere [27], but briefly; participants with and without autism underwent clinical, cognitive and MRI assessment at multiple timepoints.

Only participants with all of the required modalities (T1-weighted, resting state-fMRI and diffusion weighted imaging [DWI] at any one time point), of sufficient quality (see Additional file 1 for quality assessment details), were included in this study. This resulted in 206 autism and 196 neurotypical (NT) participants. 97 of these had good quality data of all modalities available at 2 timepoints. See Table 1 and Additional file 1: Table S2 for demographic and clinical information. This sample was not sufficient for longitudinal analysis. Data from both timepoints is used regardless, as it increases our sample size by an additional 97 scans, giving us more statistical power.

Table 1.

Demographics of individuals at first or only time point

Autism Control Test statistic p value
N 206 196
Sex m:f 147:59 124:72 χ2 = 2.6 0.1
Age mean (SD), years 17.8 (5.2) 17.3 (5.2) χ2 = 393 0.5
IQ mean (SD) 101 (20) 105 (18) χ2 = 188 0.05
Timepoint t1:t2 166:40 149:47 χ2 = 1 0.3
Site
Cambridge 14 14 χ2 = 9 0.06
KCL 74 67 χ2 = 9 0.06
Mannheim 24 36 χ2 = 9 0.06
Nijmegen 81 57 χ2 = 9 0.06
Utrecht 13 22 χ2 = 9 0.06

Clinical assessment

All clinical assessments that were utilised were collected at both wave 1 and wave 2. Participants were included in the autism group if they had a clinical diagnosis according to the DSM-IV or DSM-5 criteria. Within the autism group, symptoms in the domains of social affect and restricted repetitive behaviours (RRB) were assessed with the Autism Diagnostic Observational Schedule 2 (ADOS-2) [28]. To assess symptoms in adaptive behaviour impairment the Vineland adaptive behaviour scale (VABS) was used [29]. Specifically, impairments in socialisation, communication, daily living skills and motor skills were determined with the VABS scale. In both autistic and non-autistic participants we further assessed autistic symptoms and repetitive and rigid behaviours with the Social Responsiveness Scale 2nd Edition (SRS) [30] and the Repetitive Behaviour Scale-Revised (RBS) [31], respectively. Finally, sensory processing was assessed with the Short Sensory Profile (SSP) [32]. Given the high rate of comorbidity of ADHD in autism we assessed ADHD symptoms with the DSM-5 rating scale (parent report or self-report when parent report was unavailable). Full scale IQ (fsIQ) was estimated from 4 subtests of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (WASI) or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)/Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) as appropriate and available in local languages during participants' first visit (LEAP wave 1).

MRI data acquisition

All participants were scanned on 3T MRI scanners at both time points. T1-, functional and diffusion- weighted MRI data were acquired across five sites using largely the same scanner parameters. The details on the individual scanning parameters and quality control and pre-processing procedures are described in the Additional file 1 (Sects. 2–5). See Additional file 1: Table S3 for scan parameters.

Unimodal feature extraction

The unimodal features we use for multimodal integration with LICA are derived from advanced MRI data processing pipelines, further described in the sections below. Briefly, we derive whole-brain grey matter density maps from T1-weighted images, connectopic maps from rs-fMRI scans and white matter connectivity measures from probabilistic tractography. See Fig. 1 for an overview of the unimodal feature extraction and multimodal integration.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Overview of methods. This figure shows a conceptual overview of the analysis pipeline. The upper portion shows the unimodal feature extraction prior to integration. The bottom part shows the outputs of multimodal integration using LICA. DWI—diffusion weighted imaging, T1—T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging, fMRI—functional magnetic resonance imaging, PCA—principal component analysis, IC—independent component, ICA—independent component analysis, VBM—voxel based morphometry

T1-weighted data

T1-weighted MRI data were processed with the Voxel Based Morphometry (VBM) pipeline in the computational anatomy toolbox (CAT12: http://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/) in statistical parametric mapping software (SPM12: Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). T1-weighted images were automatically segmented into grey matter, white matter and cerebrospinal fluid and affine registered to the MNI template. Resulting segmented grey matter maps were then used to generate a study-specific template (excluding longitudinal data) and registered to MNI space via a high-dimensional, nonlinear diffeomorphic registration algorithm (DARTEL) [33]. All data were then processed using this template. A Jacobian modulation step was included using the flow fields to preserve voxel-wise information on local tissue volume. Images were smoothed with a 4 mm full-width half-max (FWHM) isotropic Gaussian kernel and downsampled (for computational reasons) to 2 mm isotropic voxel dimension.

ROI selection for DWI and RS analysis

Due to computational restrictions, analysis had to be limited to select ROIs. We chose to focus on regions that have previously been implicated in autism such as the amygdala, striatum, post central gyrus, fusiform gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex [34, 35]. Subcortical ROIs were isolated from the Harvard–Oxford atlas while cortical ROIs were isolated from the Desikan-Kiliany atlas [36]. The striatum ROI was formed by combining the Nucleus Accumbens, Putamen and Caudate structures per hemisphere as per previous connectopic mapping studies [22].

Diffusion weighted imaging (DWI)

DWI data were extensively preprocessed to correct for imaging and movement artefacts, see Additional file 1 for details. Data were then processed with Bayesian Estimation of Diffusion Parameters Obtained using Sampling Techniques (BEDPOSTX) and probabilistic tractography (ProbtrackX) [37]. ProbtrackX was performed with seeding from select ROIs and a list of target atlas ROIs to generate a voxel by target ROI connectivity matrix for each seed ROI. The target list was composed of cortical regions from the multi-modal parcellation developed on the Human Connectome Project (HCP) data [38] and subcortical ROIs from the Harvard–Oxford atlas. A MNI 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 template was used for atlas regions to ensure spatial correspondence across subjects. Principal component analysis (PCA) was used to reduce the dimensionality of these data. Matrix rank-1 PCs were kept for analysis in LICA. Participant-by-PC matrices for the different cortical or subcortical seed ROIs were stacked to produce one cortical and one subcortical input for LICA.

Resting state functional MRI (rs-fMRI)

Rs-fMRI data were preprocessed to account for movement and scanning artefacts as previously described [39] and outlined in the Additional file 1. On the preprocessed data we performed connectopic mapping to generate 3 connectopic maps of each ROI determined by their functional connectivity to the rest of the cortex [21]. Reference gradients were produced by averaging gradients from 20 subjects from the Human Connectome Project [40]. All subject gradients were then checked against these to ensure consistent ordering and direction (i.e. not flipped). Gradients 1 and 2 were selected for further analysis. Gradient 1 for each cortical or subcortical ROI was combined into single spatial maps, while gradient 2 was similarly combined into others, thereby producing 4 inputs for LICA; cortical G1, cortical G2, subcortical G1 and subcortical G2.

Multimodal integration

LICA is an extension to ICA that allows for the integration of multi-modal data linked through a shared mixing matrix [41]. For each independent component (IC) isolated the algorithm provides a set of spatial maps (one per original modality), a vector describing the contribution of each subject and finally a vector of the loading weights showing the contribution of each modality to that component. We use the vector containing the subject loadings per (multimodal) IC to investigate the relationship between the (multimodal) brain phenotypes and demographic/behavioural measures. Given our sample size, we generated 80 components for this analysis. We additionally calculated a multimodal index (MMI) per IC as previously described [16], indicating if a component was driven exclusively by one or multiple of the original data modalities.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses and graph generation were all performed in R (V3.5.1, [42]). Building on previous work that found group differences in structural brain phenotypes between autistic and non-autistic participants in the same cohort [3, 20] we first used linear mixed effects models to test the association between our imaging derived brain phenotypes and autism diagnosis while accounting for age, sex, scan site and time point of collection. Non-independence of some participants (longitudinal data) were accounted for with subject ID as a random factor. Analyses of diagnosis were False-Discovery Rate (FDR) corrected for the number of IC’s tested [43].

Similar models were then utilised to investigate continuous associations with ADOS total and its subscales and VABS scales within the autism group, and the SSP, RRB, SRS questionnaire data across the full sample. FDR [43] multiple comparison correction was used in these analyses.

Significant associations were further probed to see if they were robust to the inclusion of fsIQ in the model. Additionally, we tested the associations for interaction effects with site, sex, age and time point.

Additional analysis

Data integration and statistical analyses were repeated in structural or functional only subsets of features to determine if combined or separate analysis yielded more promise.

As a post-hoc analysis we applied trend surface modelling (TSM) to the connectopic gradient of our main finding. Essentially, TSM breaks down a surface into fewer trend coefficients that reflect the general trends of the surface, here, of the connectopic gradient per subject [22, 44]. We applied model order (MO) of 3 to avoid overfitting. This yielded 9 coefficients which we tested for association with autism diagnosis using linear mixed effects models identical to those described above. Then, we applied Bonferroni correction as multiple comparison correction (MCC), adjusting for the number of coefficients analysed.

Results

Participants

The final cohort of participants consisted of N = 206 autistic and N = 196 non-autistic individuals. The groups did not differ significantly in terms of sex distribution, age or fsIQ across sites or collection wave (all p values > 0.05, Table 1). Longitudinal data was only available for 2 of the 5 sites (Nijmegen and Mannheim; Additional file 1: Table S2). The longitudinal cohort consisted of N = 51 autistic participants and N = 46 non-autistic participants. Similar to the cross-sectional cohort there were no significant differences between the groups in terms of sex, age, fsIQ or diagnostic distribution across sites (all p values > 0.09, Additional file 1: Table S2). Given the small amount of longitudinal data available in comparison with the whole sample and the short duration between timepoints we choose to analyse the whole sample together focussing on main effects of diagnosis and behaviour rather than longitudinal effects.

Group associations

Our analysis yielded one significant association after MCC, between IC62 and autism diagnosis, where the contribution from the autistic group was lower than the non-autistic group (coefficient = 0.33, padj = 0.02; Fig. 2). This effect was robust to the addition of full-scale IQ (fsIQ) and no significant interactions were found between diagnosis and sex, age, site, timepoint or fsIQ (p values > 0.1; data plotted per site is shown in Additional file 1: Figure S3). Furthermore, as this component was almost solely driven by the functional data we tested for confounding effects of in-scanner motion, indexed as average framewise displacement (FD) during the resting state scan. We found our diagnosis effect was robust to its inclusion in the model (coefficient = 0.33, padj = 0.02) and there was no significant effect of FD on the subject course (χ2 = 0.19, p = 0.66).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

IC62 subject course per group. The violin plots show the distribution of the subject courses while the boxplots indicate the first and third quartile with the median denoted with a thick horizontal line. There was a significant main effect of diagnostic group. The FDR-adjusted p value is shown in the top left corner. There was no significant effect of site or diagnosis-by-site interaction

The cortical connectopic gradient 2 feature contributed mostly to this component (99%), localised in the fusiform gyrus of the right hemisphere (Fig. 3). The component additionally had a VBM contribution of 1% (Fig. 3), while the other modalities contributed close to 0%.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Spatial maps of IC62. Modality contributions of IC62 are shown. Modalities contributing < 1% are excluded from visualizstion. The scale represents the Z-score of spatial contribution within each feature. The red lines on the sagittal slice indicate the position of the axial slices displayed. VBM—voxel based morphology, fMRI G2—function MRI gradient 2. |Z|> 2 is shown

To determine if we could detect group specific connectopic gradients we went on to visualise the average gradient for each group. We found that the group average gradient maps display the same general pattern in their connectopic maps. There is a medial–lateral primary axis to the gradient with the medial aspect extending to the posterior and anterior extremities of the structure. Notably, however, these group average gradient maps subtly visually differ (Fig. 4A, B). To highlight differences in the connectivity patterns, we calculated the difference map between the gradients by subtracting the non-autistic group average map from the autistic group average map (Fig. 4C). We see a slight shift, on average, in the spatial organisation of functional processing between groups. We further tested this by extracting TSM-coefficients of the right hemisphere fusiform gyrus for each subject, thereby reducing the dimensionality of the complex connectopic gradient to nine coefficients that reflect the general connectopic trend across the ROI. Using linear mixed effects models. We found three out of nine coefficients were significantly associated with diagnosis after correcting for multiple testing (TSM-coefficient 1: coefficient = − 0.11, padj = 0.008; TSM-coefficient 6: coefficient = 0.07, padj = 0.0004; TSM-coefficient 7: coefficient = 0.03, padj = 0.001; Fig. 4D). The other 6 TSM-coefficients were not significantly associated with diagnosis (padj-values > 0.05). To determine the reproducibility of these associations we conducted a split-half analysis outlined in the Additional file 1. The outcome of this analysis showed that the same coefficients are associated with diagnosis in random halves of the same data.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Group average connectopic maps of the right fusiform gradient 2 and TSM coefficients per group. A Shows the average gradient of the autistic group. B Shows the average gradient of the non-autistic group. The colour scale represents the similarity of functional connectivity between voxels and the rest of the brain, with similar colours representing a similar connectivity pattern. C Is the group difference map. Red indicates where the autistic group had higher connectivity gradient values compared to the non-autistic group. Blue indicates the opposite—where the autistic group had lower connectivity gradient values compared to the non-autistic group. D Shows the values of TSM-Coefficients as boxplots showing the first and third quartile with the median denoted with a thick horizontal line. The plots are split by group. Coefficients that are significantly different between groups post-MCC are denoted with asterisks. **padj ≤ 0.01, ***padj ≤ 0.001

Behavioural associations

The present work found only nominal (uncorrected p < 0.05) associations between imaging derived phenotypes and dimensional clinical measures (ADOS, SRS, SSP, RBS, VABS), meaning that the associations are initially significant but do not survive MCC using FDR. See Additional file 1 for more information on these nominal results.

Additional analyses

Structural only analysis revealed one multimodal component, IC42, that nominally related to group and many nominal behavioural associations. Functional only analysis also revealed multiple nominal associations, IC20 and IC55, with both group and behavioural measures. Details are presented in the Additional file 1. IC55 in particular showed very strong correlations with our main finding of IC62, in both the subject contributions (rho = 0.63, pperm < 0.001) and the spatial maps (cortical gradient 2 r = 0.96).

Discussion

In the present paper we aimed to improve upon previous approaches that investigated the complex neurobiology of autism. We implemented advanced unimodal feature extraction pipelines to focus on brain structural and functional organisation with the hope of producing imaging derived phenotypes that are more sensitive to the microstructural properties of the brain prior to multimodal integration. The increased sensitivity to subtle variations within ROIs allowed us to decompose across more fine-grained representations of unimodal brain features. We identified one component, IC62, as significantly associated with autism diagnosis after accounting for multiple testing. This component was mainly driven by the functional connectopic gradient 2, specifically localised to the right fusiform gyrus. This result was found to be robust to confounding effects of acquisition site and fsIQ. There were also multiple nominal associations of other ICs with diagnosis and/or behavioural measures relevant to autism but these did not survive MCC.

Our primary result identified one component, IC62, which is mainly driven by cortical gradient 2 in the fusiform gyrus and significantly associated with autism diagnosis. The localisation of this finding is in accordance with previous unimodal literature which identified decreased activation of the fusiform gyrus when autistic participants view faces [45, 46]. The fusiform gyrus has been shown to be highly involved in higher-order visual processing, specifically the perception of faces, object recognition and reading comprehension [47]. Face perception has furthermore been shown to be highly lateralized, with the right fusiform gyrus playing a larger role in distinguishing between face versus non-face [48]. Altered responses to emotions in facial expressions are among the key behavioural phenotypes associated with autism [1]). Based on structural connectivity patterns of the fusiform gyrus it was previously found that the fusiform gyrus can be clustered into 3 subregions (medial, lateral and anterior), with each subregion associated with a distinct functional connectivity pattern [49]. Due to the advanced unimodal feature extraction methods applied in this work our results are more sensitive to functional connectivity changes within the fusiform gyrus. Here our first gradient displayed a main axis in the anterior–posterior direction. While our second gradient, the one significantly associated here with autism diagnosis, displayed a medial–lateral organisation with the medial pattern extending anterior and posterior to the peripheral extents of the fusiform. Analysis of extracted TSM-coefficients from the right fusiform second gradient confirmed the association of topographically organised functional connectivity patterns with autism diagnosis. This expands on the results of previous unimodal studies by identifying a topographical organisation of functional connectivity within the fusiform gyrus associated with autism.

Across all associations found (including nominal), we see little shared variance between functional and structural modalities. Moreover, the majority of findings were driven by unimodal functional variation rather than structural. This implies that our functional data processing using connectopic mapping may be fruitful in providing insights into the neurobiology of autism, behaviours relevant to autism, and neurodevelopmental conditions more generally. While the integration of functional and structural data presented here did not provide specific insights into how function and structure together vary with behaviour, it notably allows us to simultaneously characterise phenotypes across structure and function, thereby directly testing their relative contributions. Thus, this approach meaningfully contributes to the ongoing structure–function debate. Llera and colleagues [15] found their structural measures captured the variance associated with multiple behavioural traits and the addition of functional connectivity data to the analysis contributed little in terms of variance explained. This is in contrast to our current findings which show that the functional data dominate components throughout the decomposition and provide the majority of behavioural associations. This contrast likely stems from the different methods of unimodal feature extraction used. Here we used connectopic mapping in contrast to functional network based connectivity measures previously utilised. These findings imply that inter-individual variance of network based connectivity measures can mostly be captured by variance in structure alone while connectopic maps show inter-individual variability independent of structure. While some of the extracted unimodal connectopic map based components may be noise related, the prominence of connectopic mapping in the ICs that are associated with autism and/or behaviours found here indicates there is potentially behavioural relevance captured. However, many of these findings were nominal and therefore require further investigation and validation.

Structural only analysis (see Additional file 1) revealed one component, IC42, that was nominally associated with the autism diagnosis. This component showed co-varying white matter organisation and grey matter density patterns (i.e. was multimodal). The component also relates to components reported previously, on partially overlapping samples, that investigated grey matter co-variation patterns in autism [3] and co-varying grey and white matter co-variation patterns [20]. However, our finding was not significant after multiple comparison correction, nor was it stable across sites (see Additional file 1 for details).

Functional only analysis (see Additional file 1) revealed two components, IC20 and IC55, that were significantly associated with autism diagnosis. The high correspondence between the subject course and the spatial map of IC55 to our main finding implies that we capture largely the same variance in the combined LICA and the functional data only LICA.

Limitations

Our proxies of organisation for functional and structural data are based on different methodologies. Connectopic mapping is a nonlinear method which generates multiple functional topographic maps based on the connectivity of a region to the rest of the brain. We use a linear decomposition (PCA) on our probabilistic tractography data to address the sparsity of connectivity data before integration with LICA. Connectopic mapping of the tractography data [50] would also have been possible but is computationally resource heavy and selecting the number of gradients to retain would have been arbitrary given the scarcity of studies utilising this method to date. By using PCA we retained more of the variance of the data while still reducing the dimensionality. However, investigation of both structurally and functionally derived connectopic maps together is warranted. Second, for computational reasons we limited our analysis to select ROIs. These ROIs were selected based on their previous implication in autism. However, a brain-wide approach may have uncovered more associations and are encouraged in the future work. Finally, our need for all 3 imaging modalities of good quality to be available for each individual for inclusion in our sample reduced our sample size compared to the LEAP sample as a whole. The longitudinal aspect of the study suffered most from this constraint with only 97 participants having a full set of data available at each time point. Additionally, the time between wave 1 and 2 was only an average of 1.5 years in our sample. Given the relatively small longitudinal sample and short time frame for changes to develop, we chose to focus on the main effects of diagnosis and behavioural associations in the current study. The 3rd wave of data collection in LEAP is currently ongoing which has a longer interim period of approximately 8 years. This will provide future researchers the opportunity to delve with greater power into longitudinal trajectories in autism. Our sample size was additionally diminished for certain analyses due to missing clinical data. Most notably the VABS was not available from one site.

Previously it was shown that LICA reproducibility of the first ICs is high, with decreasing reproducibility for subsequent ICs (Llera et al. 2019). Furthermore, reproducibility quickly decreases as the number of participants reduces. Given that our main result is IC62, we expect the reproducibility of this pattern to be low. However, increasing sample sizes may improve reproducibility. Importantly this exploratory analysis directed our attention to the fusiform cortical gradient 2 where we have verified our finding with TSM analysis. Moreover this TSM analysis was reproducible in a split-half analysis (see Additional file 1: subsection 6.1.1). Out of 80 components, only one yielded a significant association with diagnosis when integrating across all modalities. This, combined with the moderate effect size, suggests that group differences are small in the joint measure and do not differ in most brain areas. These findings warrant multimodal integration across larger cohorts to detect more fine-grained differences between the autistic and non-autistic groups.

Conclusion

Here, we successfully utilised multimodal data integration methods to derive a novel autism-related brain phenotype that revealed group differences in the functional organisation of the right fusiform gyrus. Advanced techniques in unimodal feature extraction enhanced the sensitivity to detect within-ROI functional and structural connectivity changes. Our analysis considered the relative contributions of both structural and functional brain phenotypes simultaneously, uncovering that functional phenotypes seem to drive associations with autism diagnosis and related behavioural measures. Furthermore, these findings expand on previous unimodal approaches implicating the fusiform gyrus in autism by identifying a functional organisation of the fusiform gyrus which relates to autism diagnosis and warrants further investigation. When investigating structural modalities alone we identify a component nominally related to autism diagnosis in line with previous studies [3, 20]. This secondary finding exemplifies the potential of LICA to decompose many structural data domains into ICs that capture significant cross modality covariation.

Supplementary Information

13229_2023_564_MOESM1_ESM.docx (3MB, docx)

Additional file 1. Supplemental Material.

Acknowledgements

We thank all participants and their families for participating in this study. We gratefully acknowledge the contributions of all members of the EU-AIMS LEAP group: Jumana Ahmad, Sara Ambrosino, Bonnie Auyeung, Tobias Banaschewski, Simon Baron-Cohen, Sarah Baumeister, Christian F Beckmann, Sven Bölte, Thomas Bourgeron, Carsten Bours, Michael Brammer, Daniel Brandeis, Claudia Brogna, Yvette de Bruijn, Jan K Buitelaar, Bhismadev Chakrabarti, Tony Charman, Ineke Cornelissen, Daisy Crawley, Flavio Dell’Acqua, Guillaume Dumas, Sarah Durston, Christine Ecker, Jessica Faulkner, Vincent Frouin, Pilar Garcés, David Goyard, Lindsay Ham, Hannah Hayward, Joerg Hipp, Rosemary J Holt, Mark H Johnson, Emily J H Jones23, Prantik Kundu, Meng-Chuan Lai, Xavier Liogier D’ardhuy, Michael V. Lombardo, Eva Loth, David J Lythgoe, René Mandl, Andre Marquand, Luke Mason, Maarten Mennes, Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg, Carolin Moessnang, Nico Mueller, Declan G M Murphy, Bethany Oakley, Laurence O’Dwyer, Marianne Oldehinkel, Bob Oranje, Gahan Pandina, Antonio M Persico, Annika Rausch, Barbara Ruggeri, Amber Ruigrok, Jessica Sabet, Roberto Sacco, Antonia San José Cáceres, Emily Simonoff, Will Spooren, Julian Tillmann, Roberto Toro, Heike Tost, Jack Waldman, Steve C R Williams, Caroline Wooldridge, Iva Ilioska, Ting Mei and Marcel P Zwiers.

The EU AIMS LEAP group affiliations: Jumana Ahmad11, Sara Ambrosino4, Bonnie Auyeung7,14, Tobias Banaschewski6, Simon Baron-Cohen7, Sarah Baumeister6, Christian F Beckmann1, Sven Bölte15,16, Thomas Bourgeron17, Carsten Bours1, Michael Brammer12, Daniel Brandeis6, Claudia Brogna18, Yvette de Bruijn1, Jan K Buitelaar1, Bhismadev Chakrabarti7,19, Tony Charman3, Ineke Cornelissen1, Daisy Crawley8, Flavio Dell’Acqua8, Guillaume Dumas17, Sarah Durston4, Christine Ecker3,8,9, Jessica Faulkner8, Vincent Frouin13, Pilar Garcés20, David Goyard13, Lindsay Ham21, Hannah Hayward8, Joerg Hipp20, Rosemary J Holt7, Mark H Johnson23, Emily J H Jones23, Prantik Kundu24, Meng-Chuan Lai7,25, Xavier Liogier D’ardhuy20, Michael V. Lombardo7,26, Eva Loth8,11, David J Lythgoe12, René Mandl4, Andre Marquand1, Luke Mason23, Maarten Mennes1, Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg5, Carolin Moessnang5, Nico Mueller6, Declan G.M. Murphy8,11, Bethany Oakley8, Laurence O’Dwyer1, Marianne Oldehinkel1, Bob Oranje4, Gahan Pandina27, Antonio M. Persico18,28, Jack Price3, Annika Rausch1, Barbara Ruggeri29, Amber N V Ruigrok7, Jessica Sabet8, Roberto Sacco18, Antonia San Jóse Cáceres8, Emily Simonoff31, Will Spooren30, Julian Tillmann3, Roberto Toro17, Heike Tost5, Jack Waldman7, Steve C R Williams12, Caroline Wooldridge12, Iva Ilioska1, Ting Mei1 and Marcel P Zwiers1

1Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Kapittelweg 29, 6525 EN, Nijmegen, The Netherlands; 3Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London, SE5 8AF, UK; 4Department of Psychiatry, Brain Center Rudolf Magnus, University Medical Center Utrecht, Universiteitsweg 100, 3584 CG, Utrecht, The Netherlands; 5Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, 68159, Mannheim, Germany; 6Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Central Institute of Mental Health, University of Heidelberg, Medical Faculty Mannheim, J5, 68159, Mannheim, Germany; 7Autism Research Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Douglas House, 18b Trumpington Road, Cambridge, CB2 8AH, UK; 8Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London, SE5 8AF, UK; 9Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Frankfurt am Main, Goethe University, Deutschordenstrasse 50, 60528, Frankfurt, Germany; 11Sackler Institute for Translational Neurodevelopment, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London, SE5 8AF, UK; 12Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London, SE5 8AF, UK; 13Neurospin Centre CEA, Saclay, 91191, Gif sur Yvette, France; 14The School of Philosophy, Psychology, and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, Dugald Stewart Building, 3 Charles Street, Edinburgh, EH8 9AD, UK; 15Center for Neurodevelopmental Disorders at Karolinska Institutet (KIND), Stockholm, Sweden; 16Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Center of Psychiatry Research, Stockholm County Council, Stockholm, Sweden; 17Institut Pasteur, Human Genetics and Cognitive Functions Unit, 25 Rue du Docteur Roux, Paris, Cedex 15, France; 18University Campus Bio-Medico, Via Álvaro del Portillo, 21, Rome, Italy; 19School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Whiteknights, Reading, RG6 6AL, UK; 20Roche Pharma Research and Early Development, Neuroscience, Ophthalmology and Rare Diseases, Roche Innovation Center Basel, Grenzacherstrasse 124, B.001 N.667, CH-4070, Basel, Switzerland; 21Regulatory Affairs, Pharmaceutical Development, F. Hoffmann-La Roche Pharmaceuticals, Grenzacherstrasse 124, CH-4070, Basel, Switzerland; 22Department of Neuroscience, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden; 23Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, University of London, 32 Torrington Square, London, WC1E 7JL, UK; 24Department of Radiology, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, NY, USA; 25Child and Youth Mental Health Collaborative, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health and The Hospital for Sick Children, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, 80 Workman Way, Toronto, ON, M6J 1H4, Canada; 26Center for Applied Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Cyprus, PO Box 20537, 1678, Nicosia, Cyprus; 27Janssen Research & Development, 1125 Trenton Harbourton Road, Titusville, NJ, 08560, USA; 28Child and Adolescent Neuropsychiatry Unit, “Gaetano Martino” University Hospital, University of Messina, via Consolare Valeria 1, I-98125, Messina, Italy; 29Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, Denmark Hill, London, UK; 30Roche Pharmaceutical Research and Early Development, NORD Discovery and Translational Area, Roche Innovation Center Basel, Grenzacherstrasse 124, CH-4070, Basel, Switzerland; 31Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, London, SE5 8LF, UK

Abbreviations

ADHD

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

ADOS

Autism Diagnostic Observational Schedule

BEDPOSTX

Bayesian Estimation of Diffusion Parameters Obtained using Sampling Techniques

CAT12

Computational anatomy toolbox

CCA

Canonical correlation analysis

DARTEL

Diffeomorphic registration algorithm

dMRI

Diffusion magnetic resonance imaging

DSM-5

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental, 5th edition

DTI

Diffusion tensor imaging

DWI

Diffusion weighted imaging

ERP

Event related potential

EU-AIMS

European Autism Interventions-A Multicentre Study for Developing New Medications

FD

Framewise displacement

FDR

False-discovery rate

fMRI

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

fsIQ

Full-scale intelligence quotient

FWHM

Full-width half-max

HCP

Human connectome project

IC

Independent component

ICA

Independent component analysis

IQ

Intelligence quotient

LEAP

Longitudinal European Autism Project

LICA

Linked independent component analysis

MCC

Multiple comparison correction

MMI

Multimodality index

MO

Model order

MRI

Magnetic resonance imaging

NT

Neurotypical

PC

Principal component

PCA

Principal component analysis

ProbtrackX

Probabilistic tractography

RBS

Repetitive Behaviour Scale

ROI

Region of interest

RRB

Restricted repetitive behaviour

SD

Standard deviation

SPM

Statistical parametric mapping

SSP

Short sensory profile

SRS

Social Responsiveness Scale

TSM

Trend surface modelling

VABS

Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scale

VBM

Voxel based morphology

WAIS

Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence

WASI

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

WISC

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children

Author contributions

LMO and NJF substantially contributed to this manuscript by performing the statistical analyses, interpreting the data and designing and writing the article. AL, TM, KH, JKB and CFB provided invaluable input on the statistical analyses used in the manuscript, greatly aided in the interpretation of the data and revised it critically for important intellectual insight. CI, DLF, SD, CM, TB, SB-C, EL, FD, TC, DGMM and CE all aided this article by collecting the data used, quality controlling and analysing the data going into the present analysis, and critically reviewing the written proposal and then approving it for publication. The EU-AIMS LEAP group substantially contributed to this article by collecting and quality controlling the data that was used in the present analysis.

Funding

DLF is supported by funding from the EuroEuropean Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 101025785. TM is supported by the China Scholarship Council (No. 201806010408). This work has also been supported by the EU-AIMS (European Autism Interventions) and AIMS-2-TRIALS programmes which receive support from Innovative Medicines Initiative Joint Undertaking Grant No. 115300 and 777394, the resources of which are composed of financial contributions from the European Union’s FP7 and Horizon2020 Programmes, and from the European Federation of Pharmaceutical Industries and Associations (EFPIA) companies’ in-kind contributions, and AUTISM SPEAKS, Autistica and SFARI. This work has been further supported by the European Union Seventh Framework Programme Grant Nos. 602805 (AGGRESSOTYPE) (to JKB), 603016 (MATRICS) (to JKB), and 278948 (TACTICS) (to JKB); European Community’s Horizon 2020 Programme (H2020/2014-2020) Grant Nos. 643051 (MiND) (to JKB), 642996 (BRAINVIEW) (to JKB) and 847818 (CANDY) (to JKB and CFB); the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research VICI Grant No. 2020/TTW/00836465 (to CFB); Wellcome Trust Collaborative Award Grant No. 215573/Z/19/Z (to CFB); the Autism Research Trust (to SBC). The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results. Any views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the funders.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from LEAP, the EU-AIMS and AIMS-2-TRIALS programmes, but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under licence for the current study, and so are not publicly available. Data are, however, available from the authors upon reasonable request and with permission of LEAP and EU-AIMS.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The ethical approval to conduct the present research was obtained via the EU AIMS LEAP consortium. Specifically, ethical approval was provided by the external ethics advisory board to AIMS-2-TRIALS.

Consent for publication

The manuscript has been reviewed and approved for publication by the AIMS LEAP project review committee.

Competing interests

The present manuscript was submitted to the journal Molecular Autism for peer review and publication. The authors declare that SBC is editor-in-chief for Molecular Autism. The authors furthermore declare that DLF is associate editor for Molecular Autism.

Footnotes

A list of the authors part of the EU AIMS LEAP group and their affiliations appears at the end of the paper.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Lennart M. Oblong, Email: lennart.oblong@donders.ru.nl

The EU-AIMS LEAP Group:

Jumana Ahmad, Sara Ambrosino, Bonnie Auyeung, Tobias Banaschewski, Simon Baron-Cohen, Sarah Baumeister, Christian F. Beckmann, Sven Bölte, Thomas Bourgeron, Carsten Bours, Michael Brammer, Daniel Brandeis, Claudia Brogna, Yvette de Bruijn, Jan K. Buitelaar, Bhismadev Chakrabarti, Tony Charman, Ineke Cornelissen, Daisy Crawley, Flavio Dell’Acqua, Guillaume Dumas, Sarah Durston, Christine Ecker, Jessica Faulkner, Vincent Frouin, Pilar Garcés, David Goyard, Lindsay Ham, Hannah Hayward, Joerg Hipp, Rosemary J. Holt, Mark H. Johnson, Emily J. H. Jones, Prantik Kundu, Meng-Chuan Lai, Xavier Liogier D’ardhuy, Michael V. Lombardo, Eva Loth, David J. Lythgoe, René Mandl, Andre Marquand, Luke Mason, Maarten Mennes, Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg, Carolin Moessnang, Nico Mueller, Declan G. M. Murphy, Bethany Oakley, Laurence O’Dwyer, Marianne Oldehinkel, Bob Oranje, Gahan Pandina, Antonio M. Persico, Jack Price, Annika Rausch, Barbara Ruggeri, Amber N. V. Ruigrok, Jessica Sabet, Roberto Sacco, Antonia San Jóse Cáceres, Emily Simonoff, Will Spooren, Julian Tillmann, Roberto Toro, Heike Tost, Jack Waldman, Steve C. R. Williams, Caroline Wooldridge, Iva Ilioska, Ting Mei, and Marcel P. Zwiers

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association . Neurodevelopmental. Disorders diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. pp. 81–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Jacobs GR, Voineskos AN, Hawco C, Stefanik L, Forde NJ, Dickie EW, et al. Integration of brain and behavior measures for identification of data-driven groups cutting across children with ASD, ADHD, or OCD. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020 doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00902-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Mei T, Llera A, Floris DL, Forde NJ, Tillmann J, Durston S, et al. Gray matter covariations and core symptoms of autism: the EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project. Mol Autism. 2020;11:86. doi: 10.1186/s13229-020-00389-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Naaijen J, Zwiers MP, Forde NJ, Williams SCR, Durston S, Brandeis D, et al. Striatal structure and its association with N-acetylaspartate and glutamate in autism spectrum disorder and obsessive compulsive disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;28:118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Forde NJ, Naaijen J, Lythgoe DJ, Akkermans SEA, Openneer TJC, Dietrich A, et al. Multi-modal imaging investigation of anterior cingulate cortex cytoarchitecture in neurodevelopment. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;28:13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ameis SH, Lerch JP, Taylor MJ, Lee W, Viviano JD, Pipitone J, et al. A diffusion tensor imaging study in children with ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, OCD, and matched controls: distinct and non-distinct white matter disruption and dimensional brain-behavior relationships. Am J Psychiatry. 2016;17:1213–1222. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15111435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Arunachalam Chandran V, Pliatsikas C, Neufeld J, O’Connell G, Haffey A, DeLuca V, et al. Brain structural correlates of autistic traits across the diagnostic divide: a grey matter and white matter microstructure study. Neuroimage Clin. 2021;32:102897. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Zabihi M, Oldehinkel M, Wolfers T, Frouin V, Goyard D, Loth E, et al. Dissecting the heterogeneous cortical anatomy of autism spectrum disorder using normative models. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2019;4:567–578. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Zabihi M, Floris DL, Kia SM, Wolfers T, Tillmann J, Arenas AL, et al. Fractionating autism based on neuroanatomical normative modeling. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10:384. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-01057-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Grecucci A, Rubicondo D, Siugzdaite R, Surian L, Job R. Uncovering the social deficits in the autistic brain. A source-based morphometric study. Front Neurosci. 2016;10:388. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Di X, Biswal BB. Similarly expanded bilateral temporal lobe volumes in female and male children with autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2016;1:178–185. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2015.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pappaianni E, Siugzdaite R, Vettori S, Venuti P, Job R, Grecucci A. Three shades of grey: detecting brain abnormalities in children with autism using source-, voxel- and surface-based morphometry. Eur J Neurosci. 2018;47:690–700. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Liloia D, Mancuso L, Uddin LQ, Costa T, Nani A, Keller R, et al. Gray matter abnormalities follow non-random patterns of co-alteration in autism: meta-connectomic evidence. Neuroimage Clin. 2021;30:102583. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Groves AR, Smith SM, Fjell AM, Tamnes CK, Walhovd KB, Douaud G, et al. Benefits of multi-modal fusion analysis on a large-scale dataset: life-span patterns of inter-subject variability in cortical morphometry and white matter microstructure. Neuroimage. 2012;63:365–380. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Llera A, Wolfers T, Mulders P, Beckmann CF. Inter-individual differences in human brain structure and morphology link to variation in demographics and behavior. Elife. 2019;8:e44443. doi: 10.7554/eLife.44443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Francx W, Llera A, Mennes M, Zwiers MP, Faraone SV, Oosterlaan J, et al. Integrated analysis of gray and white matter alterations in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage Clin. 2016;11:357–367. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Bussu G, Llera A, Jones EJH, Tye C, Charman T, Johnson MH, et al. Uncovering neurodevelopmental paths to autism spectrum disorder through an integrated analysis of developmental measures and neural sensitivity to faces. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2021;46:E34–E43. doi: 10.1503/jpn.190148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mueller S, Keeser D, Samson AC, Kirsch V, Blautzik J, Grothe M, et al. Convergent findings of altered functional and structural brain connectivity in individuals with high functioning autism: a multimodal MRI study. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e67329. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Itahashi T, Yamada T, Nakamura M, Watanabe H, Yamagata B, Jimbo D, et al. Linked alterations in gray and white matter morphology in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder: a multimodal brain imaging study. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;7:155–169. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mei T, Forde NJ, Floris DL, Dell’Acqua F, Stones R, Ilioska I, et al. Autism is associated with inter-individual variations of gray and white matter morphology. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2022 doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.08.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Haak KV, Marquand AF, Beckmann CF. Connectopic mapping with resting-state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2017;170:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Marquand AF, Haak KV, Beckmann CF. Functional corticostriatal connection topographies predict goal directed behaviour in humans. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1:0146. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Przeździk I, Faber M, Fernández G, Beckmann CF, Haak KV. The functional organisation of the hippocampus along its long axis is gradual and predicts recollection. Cortex. 2019;119:324–335. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kaas JH. Topographic maps are fundamental to sensory processing. Brain Res Bull. 1997;44:107–112. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(97)00094-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Jbabdi S, Sotiropoulos SN, Behrens TE. The topographic connectome. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23:207–215. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Jeurissen B, Leemans A, Tournier J-D, Jones DK, Sijbers J. Investigating the prevalence of complex fiber configurations in white matter tissue with diffusion magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34:2747–2766. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Loth E, Charman T, Mason L, Tillmann J, Jones EJH, Wooldridge C, et al. The EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP): design and methodologies to identify and validate stratification biomarkers for autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism. 2017;8:24. doi: 10.1186/s13229-017-0146-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:205–223. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Sparrow SS, Cicchetti D, Balla DA. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, second edition. https://psycnet.apa.org. 2005. 10.1037/t15164-000.
  • 30.Constantino JN, Gruber CP, Others. Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) Western Psychological Services. Los Angeles, CA. 2005.
  • 31.Lam KSL, Aman MG. The repetitive behavior scale-revised: independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:855–866. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0213-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Tomchek SD, Dunn W. Sensory processing in children with and without autism: a comparative study using the short sensory profile. Am J Occup Ther. 2007;61:190–200. doi: 10.5014/ajot.61.2.190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. 2007;38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wolff JJ, Jacob S, Elison JT. The journey to autism: insights from neuroimaging studies of infants and toddlers. Dev Psychopathol. 2018;30:479–495. doi: 10.1017/S0954579417000980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Anagnostou E, Taylor MJ. Review of neuroimaging in autism spectrum disorders: what have we learned and where we go from here. Mol Autism. 2011;2:4. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-2-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Behrens TE, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MF, Woolrich MW. Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: What can we gain? Neuroimage. 2007;34:144–155. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Glasser MF, Coalson TS, Robinson EC, Hacker CD, Harwell J, Yacoub E, et al. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. 2016;536:171. doi: 10.1038/nature18933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Oldehinkel M, Mennes M, Marquand A, Charman T, Tillmann J, Ecker C, et al. Altered connectivity between cerebellum, visual, and sensory-motor networks in autism spectrum disorder: results from the EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2019;4:260–270. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Groves AR, Beckmann CF, Smith SM, Woolrich MW. Linked independent component analysis for multimodal data fusion. Neuroimage. 2011;54:2198–2217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.R Core Team . R: a language and environment for Statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc. 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gelfand A, Diggle P, Guttorp P, Fuentes M. Handbook of spatial statistics. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Dichter GS. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of autism spectrum disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14:319–351. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.3/gdichter. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, Chabris CF, Clark J, Steele S, et al. Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces. Neuroimage. 2004;22:1141–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Weiner KS, Zilles K. The anatomical and functional specialization of the fusiform gyrus. Neuropsychologia. 2016;83:48–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.06.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Meng M, Cherian T, Singal G, Sinha P. Lateralization of face processing in the human brain. Proc Biol Sci. 2012;279:2052–2061. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Zhang W, Wang J, Fan L, Zhang Y, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB, et al. Functional organization of the fusiform gyrus revealed with connectivity profiles. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37:3003–3016. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Blazquez Freches G, Haak KV, Beckmann CF, Mars RB. Connectivity gradients on tractography data: pipeline and example applications. Hum Brain Mapp. 2021;42:5827–5845. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

13229_2023_564_MOESM1_ESM.docx (3MB, docx)

Additional file 1. Supplemental Material.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from LEAP, the EU-AIMS and AIMS-2-TRIALS programmes, but restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under licence for the current study, and so are not publicly available. Data are, however, available from the authors upon reasonable request and with permission of LEAP and EU-AIMS.


Articles from Molecular Autism are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES