Abstract
The most appropriate strategies for managing secondary genomic findings (SF) in clinical research are being developed and evaluated. We surveyed patients at the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases (NIAID) to evaluate decisional conflict regarding enrolling in a study that returns SF. Responses were collected using a cross-sectional survey after informed consent but before return of SF. Sixty-six adults of 116 eligible participants responded. No participant explicitly declined because they did not want to possibly receive a SF. Sixty-five of 66 (98%) participants thought it was appropriate to return SFs in research; one participant was unsure. Decisional conflict regarding enrolling in a study returning SF was low overall with 68% of participants reporting a score of less than 10 on a 100-point decisional conflict scale, implying that they felt informed, clear on what they wanted, and supported. Lower genetic literacy was weakly associated with higher decisional conflict (Spearman’s rho = −0.297, p = 0.015). Six participants reported confusion related to the choices about SFs. Our data suggest that participants in our study feel it is appropriate to receive SF and have little decisional conflict about potentially receiving such information; however, some participants may need further education and counseling.
Keywords: Exome sequencing, secondary findings, clinical research, decision making, literacy, genetic counseling
Introduction
The return of individual genetic results from clinical research varies considerably by study (Wynn et al., 2015). This variability is partly dependent on study context, such as the relationship between investigator and participant, available resources, institutional norms, and investigator preference (Jarvik et al., 2014). For example, a genomic research study where physicians provide individual medical recommendations or administer treatment to participants has different ethical obligations than a research study doing genomic analysis on samples from de-identified biobank participants. One area without consensus is the return of secondary findings (SF) in a research setting (Wolf, Burke, & Koenig, 2015). Return of a selected consensus set of SF is recommended in the clinical setting by the American College of Medical Genetics and Genomics (ACMG) (Green et al., 2013; Kalia et al., 2017).
One way to inform policy on the return of SF in the research setting is to conduct research on patient knowledge and preferences. In one study conducted at the National Institute of Health (NIH) Clinical Center (CC), where the current study was also conducted, Sapp and colleagues interviewed 13 individuals who had recently received a positive SF report. They found that these participants reported minimal psychological distress although four had not yet accessed the recommended follow-up care. This study also surveyed 107 individuals who received negative SF reports and found that most expressed a good understanding of what that result meant for them, although a minority expressed confusion about the difference between primary findings (PF) and SF (Sapp et al., 2018). Other studies show that most (> 95%) research participants opt to receive SF when given the choice (Facio et al., 2013; Loud et al., 2016). While these studies provide some insight that SF may be acceptable and useful to research participants, gaps in our understanding of this issue still exist. These gaps specifically include patient experiences in undergoing consent for the return of SF.
In this study, we aimed to quantify the extent and type of decisional conflict that clinical research participants had about enrolling in a study that returns SF, among other relevant variables. We specifically chose to focus on decisional conflict due the uncertain outcomes, risks, and benefits that are inherent when discussing and making decisions regarding SF. We use the term “decisional conflict” to refer to participants feeling: (1) uncertain about which choice is best for them; (2) uninformed, unclear about personal values, or unsupported; or (3) ineffective in their decision making, as conceptualized by the widely-used decisional conflict scale (O’Connor, 1995). We also sought to identify participant attributes associated with such conflict, most notably risk perception and genetic literacy level. Participants’ assessments of how likely they are to receive personally useful genetic information might affect motivation for study participation and subsequent behavior (Biesecker, Schwartz, & Marteau, 2013; Heshka, Palleschi, Howley, Wilson, & Wells, 2008; Tong et al., 2015), consistent with the broadly-referenced theory of planned behavior (Godin & Kok, 1996). These perceptions can also help us identify potential misunderstandings about study procedures and results.
In a similar manner, patients’ literacy levels have been associated with multiple patient-centered outcomes; those with lower literacy often have a greater degree of misunderstanding and confusion about genetic testing (Roter, Ellington, Erby, Larson, & Dudley, 2006; Roter, Erby, Larson, & Ellington, 2007), are less likely to participate in health care decisions (Cooper, Beach, & Clever, 2004) and are less likely to make decisions about genetics that are consistent with their stated values (Dormandy, Michie, Hooper, & Marteau, 2005; van den Berg, Timmermans, Ten Kate, van Vugt, & van der Wal, 2005). Literacy not only reflects one’s ability to read and write, but is also associated with one’s ability to understand and remember complex information communicated verbally (Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Health Literacy, 2004; Williams, Baker, Parker, & Nurss, 1998). Given that literacy level is typically a fixed attribute unless one undergoes a targeted intervention, it is an appropriate and relevant variable to consider when assessing a population’s response to new genetic information as well as patient education and counseling on genetics. Measures of general literacy tend to be highly correlated with context-specific literacy measures, such as the genetic literacy measure used in this study.
To address our research aim to characterize decisional conflict in regard to participating in a study that returns SF and to explore potential correlates of this decisional conflict, including risk perception and genetic literacy, we administered a cross-sectional survey to patients with immunological disorders prior to the return of results of SF analysis. Participants were recruited from studies within the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases (NIAID) at the NIH CC in order to evaluate genetic causes, i.e., PF, of their immune-related diseases. This specialty population is infrequently the focus of social and behavioral research on genetic testing and represents a novel and interesting group of patients who in many cases have symptoms that arise from the interaction of genetic susceptibility and key pathogen exposures. This study was part of a larger project at the NIH CC focusing on outcomes in genomics.
Methods
Participants
One-hundred-thirty-six participants were enrolled in this study, all of whom were referred for whole exome sequencing (WES) by other studies at NIAID and related collaborations. One individual died and was subsequently enrolled on this study for further genetic analysis following a conversation with that individual’s next-of-kin; this participant did not receive SF analysis. The clinical indication for referral included a mixed group of patients with primary immune deficiency (PID) and their relatives (n = 65), children diagnosed with pediatric autoimmune neuropsychiatric disorders associated with Streptococcal infections (PANDAS) and their relatives (n = 32), and patients with familial mastocytosis and their relatives (n = 39). All living participants or their guardians provided written informed consent for participating in NIH IRB-approved study protocol 15-I-0113.
Instrumentation
Surveys included four scales with 6–17 items each plus novel items chosen to address additional questions in this population. Genetic literacy was assessed using a previously published modification of the Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Genetics (REAL-G) (Erby, Roter, Larson, & Cho, 2008; Hooker et al., 2014) in order to provide a proxy for overall literacy level as well as anchor to the genetic content of the research questions. Decisional conflict was assessed with a widely-used 17-item scale, which includes sub-scales assessing decision quality related to feeling informed, feeling one’s decision is consistent with one’s values, experiencing an acceptable degree of uncertainty around the decision, feeling supported, and feeling effective in one’s decision making (test-retest reliability coefficient = 0.81; α = 0.78–0.92) (O’Connor, 1995). General self-efficacy was assessed using a short-form, 6-item scale (α = 0.79–0.88) (Brignardello-Petersen, 2017; David et al., 2018; Raghuram Pillai et al., 2020). Lastly, the survey included previously-used questions regarding risk perception (Tong et al., 2015), and seven novel items regarding previously hearing of WES, ability to cope with receiving a PF or SF, and acceptability of returning SF in a research setting.
Procedures
Participants or their guardians gave consent to WES with SF disclosure. Consent discussions were conducted by a genetic counselor (MNS) and generally included discussion of the indication for genetic testing, collection of a family history, basic education about genetics and genetic testing, discussion of PF and SF as possible results from genetic testing coupled with numerical risk estimates, and elicitation of participant questions or concerns. Participants were made explicitly aware of other sequencing studies where SF were not returned as part of the exploration of their opinions on that element of the study. Risk-benefit assessment and alternatives to participation were also covered, in addition to an explanation of the survey research. The majority of participants being evaluated for PID had prior negative PID gene panel DNA testing. The validity of novel PFs were established by the peer review process. SFs were analyzed by an experienced, board-certified molecular geneticist using the ACMG variant interpretation and SF guidelines (Kalia et al., 2017; Richards et al., 2015).
Participants 12 years of age and older were invited to participate in the survey after consent was completed and remained eligible for survey participation until they received their results at about 6 months after consent. Electronic and pen-and-paper versions of the survey were offered. The electronic administration of surveys was performed using a secure extension of our institute’s electronic medical record. Participants who did not initially respond to the survey invitation were sent two reminders.
Survey data was analyzed using SPSS V26 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). The primary analyses regarding decisional conflict were conducted using bivariate models and intended to be primarily descriptive. Only data from adult participants were used in analyses, as the survey instruments were not well validated in adolescents. Descriptive statistics were presented as percentages or means. Bootstrapping was used to derive robust estimates of confidence intervals using resampling with replacement from the original dataset. We derived 95% confidence intervals (CI) reflecting the 2.5th and 97.5th percentiles of the bootstrap values as the lower and upper bounds of the interval derived based on 1000 bootstrap samples. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient, rho, with the associated p-value was used for bivariate correlations between continuous variables. Student’s t-tests – when the assumption of equal variances between two groups could be met – and Welch’s test for equality of means – when two or more samples had unequal variances – with their associated p-values were used for comparisons of means. The limited sample size precluded more complex modeling and sub-group analyses.
Results
WES Results
PFs for participants (n = 4 families) have been reported separately including both known and novel contributions to disease related to pathogenic variants in ADA2 (Arts et al., 2018), CARD8 (Mao et al., 2018), STAT3 (Natarajan et al., 2018), and NCKAP1L (Cook et al., 2020.). None of the participants reported here was found to carry reportable SF as defined by the ACMG. All results returned to participants were confirmed in a CLIA laboratory.
Survey
One-hundred thirty-six participants were enrolled on this protocol, of which 116 were eligible for the survey. Seventy-six participants returned the survey between 1 day and 6 months after consent for a response rate of 66%. Sixty-six of these were adults. All surveys were completed prior to return of results. Survey participants were primarily white (82%), not Hispanic or Latino (79%), female (54%), and affected (73%, in contrast to unaffected relatives). See Table 1. Adult respondents were not significantly different from adult non-respondents in sex (X2(1, n = 102) = 0.25, p = 0.617); disease affection status (X2(1, n = 102) = 0.756, p = 0.385; or age (t(66.05) = −0.969, p = 0.336). Due to the limited size of our sample, we did not have enough power to assess differences in race and ethnicity between respondents and non-respondents.
Table 1.
Demographics (n = 66) | n,% |
---|---|
White | 54, 81.8% |
Not Hispanic or Latino | 52, 78.8% |
Female | 36, 54.5% |
Affected | 48, 72.7% |
Descriptive results (n = 66) | n,% |
Previously heard of exome sequencing | 23, 34.8% |
Reported returning SF is appropriate | 65, 98.5% |
Zero or low decisional conflict about SF | 45, 68.2% |
Unsure if they are in a study disclosing SF | 6, 9.1% |
Correlations with decision conflict (n = 66) | Test statistic, p-value |
Unsure if they are in a study disclosing SF | t = −2.226, p = 0.075 |
Perceived appropriateness of returning SF | Spearman’s rho = −0.296, p = 0.016 |
Perceived likelihood of receiving a PF | Spearman’s rho = −0.231, p = 0.063 |
Perceived likelihood of receiving a SF | Spearman’s rho = −0.298, p = 0.015 |
Genetic literacy level | Spearman’s rho = −0.297, p = 0.015 |
Overall, most participants reported not previously hearing or being unsure if they had previously heard about WES (n = 38, 58% and n = 5, 7.6%, respectively). None of the participants approached for this study explicitly declined due to this study returning SF or asked to opt-out of SF receipt. When asked generally about genomic research projects, 98% of survey participants reported thinking it was appropriate to return SF (n = 65/66; 10-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree, mean = 9.35 with bootstrapped 95% CI = 9.08 – 9.59); one individual was unsure. When asked about the sources for this generally positive appraisal of SF disclosure for research studies, participants reported their opinions were based on their perceptions of what is in their best interest, what is best for others, and what is the best use of research resources (mean = 8.15; 6.39; 7.71 with 95% CI = 7.62 – 8.61; 5.74– 6.97; 7.12– 8.24, respectively).
Decisional conflict regarding enrolling in a study returning SF was low overall with 68% of participants reporting zero or low decisional conflict (n = 22 and n = 23, respectively; “low” being a score of less than 10 on a 100-point scale). Most participants reported agreeing or strongly agreeing (score ≤ 25 on a 100-point scale) with statements related to decision quality such as feeling informed (n = 53, 80%), feeling that their decision was consistent with their values (n = 53, 80%), experiencing an acceptable degree of uncertainty around the decision (n = 60, 91%), feeling supported (n = 64, 97%), and feeling effective in their decision making (n = 64, 97%). Self-efficacy was not associated with decisional conflict (Spearman’s rho = −0.096, p = 0.443).
Higher decisional conflict was weakly associated with lower perceived appropriateness of receiving SF (Spearman’s rho = −0.296, p = 0.016). Higher decisional conflict was also weakly associated with lower perceived likelihood of receiving SF (Spearman’s rho = −0.298, p = 0.015) and lower genetic literacy (Spearman’s rho = −0.297, p = 0.015). Decisional conflict was not associated with perceived likelihood of receiving PF (Spearman’s rho = −0.231, p = 0.063). Lower genetic literacy was associated with multiple sub-variables of decisional conflict, corresponding to higher decisional conflict regarding effectiveness, i.e., feeling satisfaction and confidence in making an informed choice (Spearman’s rho = −0.293, p = 0.017), support, i.e., feeling of having enough advice, support, and independence to make a decision (Spearman’s rho = −0.285, p = 0.020); informed, i.e., knowing the risks, benefits, and options available (Spearman’s rho = −0.280, p = 0.023); and values, i.e., being clear about what is most personally important in regard to the decision (Spearman’s rho = −0.400, p = 8.8×10−4). Genetic literacy was not correlated with the decisional conflict uncertainty subscale, i.e., feeling sure about the decision (Spearman’s rho = −0.119, p = 0.343). Lower genetic literacy was also associated with lower reported capacity to “deal with” receiving a PF and SF (Spearman’s rho = 0.505, p = 1.5×10−5; Spearman’s rho = 0.350, p = 0.004, respectively).
A notable minority of participants reported confusion about basic aspects of study participation, such as being unsure when asked later if they did in fact opt to receive SFs by participating in this study (n = 6/66, 9.1%). Overall decisional conflict was not significantly different between the group of participants with confusion and the group of participants who did not report confusion about whether the study returned SF (t(5.118) = −2.226, p = 0.075; mean difference = −24.74; 95% CI = −53.12, 3.64). Having previously heard of WES was not associated with participants’ reported decisional conflict (F(2,10.67) = 2.853, p = 0.102).
Participants reported a high perceived likelihood of receiving a positive result, with 46% and 39% of participants reporting it was “likely” or “very likely” that they would receive a PF or SF, respectively. Risk perception for PF and SF were correlated (Spearman’s rho = 0.421, p = 4.3×10−4). The most common likelihood estimate, however, for both PF and SF was “neither likely or unlikely” (46% and 42%, respectively).
Discussion
This study assesses patient acceptance and understanding of receiving SF analysis in a heterogeneous cohort of immune disease patients. We found that the majority of our participants reported that it was appropriate to return SF, with overall low or zero decisional conflict regarding their decision to enroll in a genomic sequencing study that returns SFs. This was reflected in the observation that most participants reported feeling informed and effective in their decision to participate, and that their decision was in line with their values and conferred an acceptable degree of uncertainty. We also found a few important correlates of decisional conflict: lower genetic literacy, lower perceived appropriateness of receiving SF, and lower perceived likelihood of receiving SF were correlated with higher decisional conflict. Furthermore, a proportion of participants was unsure whether they had decided to receive SF. Risk perception of receiving a positive PF and SF was relatively high. Finally, the PFs are consistent with prior reports that WES is useful for discovering genes responsible for immune-mediated phenotypes (Yska et al., 2019). Our participants, especially those with suspected PID, had been undiagnosed by previous genetic testing and the diagnoses by WES in this study were either recently described or novel.
To date, the literature on SF disclosure in research has primarily focused on whether individuals should have this data returned to them (Jarvik et al., 2014; Wolf et al., 2015) and what participants elect (Facio et al., 2013; Loud et al., 2016). In this study, we went further by examining when participants elected to receive SF, how did they feel about that decision, and what the correlates of conflict related to decision making were. Overall, the survey data show that most participants reported thinking it was appropriate to include SF in results returned from research studies, particularly from the perspective of what they perceived to be in their best interest. Our findings present a novel facet of factors in play when individuals make decisions about participating in genome sequencing research that returns SFs, adding to the body of prior research (Facio et al., 2013; Loud et al., 2016). Specifically, most participants in our study reflected positively on the quality of their decision to participate in a study returning SF and reported feeling informed, supported, effective, and unlikely to regret their decision. Consistent with the theory of planned behavior (Godin & Kok, 1996), those who judged the likelihood of receiving clinically relevant genetic information as higher had the least conflict about participation. Additionally, genetic literacy emerged as an important factor in decisional conflict. Participants who had more limited genetic literacy (although still functionally literate), were slightly less confident in their decision-making.]
Accordingly, this study revealed areas needing further attention. A minority of participants expressed confusion about options related to SF in this and other studies. Overall, this is consistent with numerous studies examining misunderstandings of clinically relevant information, especially among those with limited literacy (Flory & Emanuel, 2004; Montalvo & Larson, 2014; Tamariz, Palacio, Robert, & Marcus, 2013). Indeed, genetics is relatively new to clinical care and quickly changing. Genetics discussions can also include terminology and numeracy skills unfamiliar to the average patient (Roter et al., 2007). That lower genetic literacy was correlated with higher decisional conflict in this research study highlights the important need for supporting literacy across clinical and research settings.
Another important finding to consider is that participants’ risk perceptions were largely overestimated, consistent with prior literature (Lupo et al., 2016; Roberts et al., 2018). This is perhaps partially due to participants not having the option of choosing “I don’t know” or “unsure” when asked about risk in this study, and instead choosing “neither likely or unlikely”, potentially creating a systematic overestimation of risk perception in our data (Ellis, Ferrer, & Klein, 2018; Ellis, Ferrer, Taber, & Klein, 2018). Nonetheless, it is necessary to consider that participants may have simply overestimated their chances of receiving a either a PF or SF. A novel finding in our study is that higher perceived likelihood of receiving a result were associated with lower decisional conflict about participation. Notably, a lower perceived likelihood of a SF was correlated with higher decisional conflict regarding participation in a study that returned SF. Particularly in the context of PF, one should not discount the influence of optimism bias, or unrealistic beliefs, which seem to be beneficial for both quality of life and overall mortality, despite being inaccurate (Chida & Steptoe, 2008; Larson, 1998). In this case, patients may be expressing their hope that this test, or most suggested tests for that matter, might end up being helpful. Such optimism may be linked to slightly easier research participation decisions in this study.
Participant likelihood estimates for PF and SF in our study were also moderately correlated. While the PF versus SF distinction is meaningful in the world of clinical genetics, it is possible some participants merge these abstract categories into a single idea of ‘clinical relevance’. Indeed, much prior work has shown the extent to which patients reinterpret communication about genetics in a personally meaningful way, embedding the information in their lived experience. This reinterpretation mediates risk-perception accuracy and subsequent behavior, even when one can correctly recall what was told to them by their provider (J. Vos et al., 2011; Joël Vos, Gómez-García, et al., 2012; Joël Vos, Oosterwijk, et al., 2012; Joël Vos et al., 2008). Further, it has also been well-characterized that individuals very frequently use heuristics or cognitive shortcuts to arrive at a decision, rather than taking a detailed, deliberative approach based on all available information for all decisions (Blumenthal-Barby & Krieger, 2015; Marewski & Gigerenzer, 2012).
Study Limitations
This study has several limitations. First, the small sample size precluded multivariate analyses or clinical sub-group analyses which may have provided more nuance. For example, unaffected relatives and affected study probands were combined in these analyses although the perceptions and priorities of these groups may systematically differ. Second, only participants who choose to take part in the study agreed to the survey, although the sequencing and survey were presented separately. Specifically, individuals who were contacted for this study remotely but who were lost to follow up without enrolling and who may have had concerns about SF were not available for study. None of the individuals approached in-person declined. Third, some participants might not have felt they had a real choice about receiving SF or not, due to the way participants were enrolled. Fourth, the wide variation in time between consent and survey completion may have confounded the data. This information was not systematically recorded, and thus cannot be controlled for in the presented analyses. Fifth, there also may be limits to the generalizability of the NIH population compared to other clinical groups of patients with immune disorders, based on possible differences in their range of experiences. Finally, importantly, pre-test counseling in this study occurred via an in-person visit with a certified genetic counselor. Findings from this study should be interpreted within the context of a face-to-face informed consent process that may be distinct from other approaches with varying levels of individualization. Additional exploration of alternative approaches to informed consent, including chatbots and combinations of chatbot and in-person or tele-consent models – and their effect on decisional conflict regarding enrolling in studies returning SFs – are warranted.
Conclusions
Taken together, despite the study limitations, these data suggest implications for practice and for future research. These data add to the literature on the general acceptability from the patient perspective of SF return in a clinical research setting and demonstrate that the described approach allows most patients to make a decision that they reflect upon positively. Clinically, these findings, along with a broad literature on patient education and counseling, underscore the importance of communications skills which meaningfully engage and clearly impart the most important information to the patient. To do so, these skills should be based upon the rich context of the patient’s personal experiences and insights, identify areas of potential misunderstandings, and proactively prevent poor health outcomes related to misunderstandings (Roter & Hall, 2006). This type of tailored communication is needed for all patients; certain patient factors, including but in no way limited to literacy level, can guide such tailoring. Indeed, future research should investigate work to better understand the origins and consequences of overly-high risk perceptions or confusion about PF versus SF, as well as develop communication or other interventions aimed at addressing such issues.
Human Studies and Informed Consent:
All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study.
Acknowledgements:
The CCGO sequencing was funded by the Deputy Director’s Challenge Funding Program of the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Health. The implementation of the CCGO sequencing for NIAID participants and this survey were supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, NIAID. We are grateful to Angela Wang who provided study support to this protocol, Paul Juneau for lending his statistical expertise and reviewing the manuscript draft, and Andrew Oler who led the bioinformatic analysis of the WES data, reported in greater detail elsewhere.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest:
Morgan Similuk, Jia Yan, Michael Setzer, Leila Jamal, Patty Little, Michael Lenardo, and Helen Su declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References:
- Arts K, Bergerson JRE, Ombrello AK, Similuk M, Oler AJ, Agharahimi A, Mace EM, Hershfield M, Wouters C, De Somer L, Morren M-A, Diego RP, Moens L, Freeman AF, & Meyts I (2018). Warts and DADA2: A Mere Coincidence? Journal of Clinical Immunology, 38(8), 836–843. 10.1007/s10875-018-0565-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biesecker BB, Schwartz MD, & Marteau TM (2013). Enhancing informed choice to undergo health screening: A systematic review. American Journal of Health Behavior, 37(3), 351–359. 10.5993/AJHB.37.3.8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumenthal-Barby JS, & Krieger H (2015). Cognitive biases and heuristics in medical decision making: A critical review using a systematic search strategy. Medical Decision Making: An International Journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making, 35(4), 539–557. 10.1177/0272989X14547740 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chida Y, & Steptoe A (2008). Positive psychological well-being and mortality: A quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70(7), 741–756. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818105ba [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook S, Comrie W, Poli CM, Similuk M, Oler AJ, Faruqi A, Kuhns D, Yang S, Vargas-Hernandez A, Carisey A, Fournier B, Anderson E, Price S, Smelkinson M, Chahla W, Forbes LR, Mace EM, Cao TN, Coban Akdemir ZH, Jhangiani SN, Muzny DM, … Lenardo MJ (2020). Genetic immunodeficiency and autoimmune disease reveal distinct roles of Hem1 in the WAVE2 and mTORC2 complexes. Science, 369 (6500), 202–207. DOI: 10.1126/science.aay5663 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper L, Beach M, & Clever S (2004). Participatory decision-making in the medical encounter and its relationship to patient literacy. In Schwartzberg J, Van Geest J, Wang C, Gazmararian J, Parker R, Roter D, Rudd R, & Schillinger D (Eds.), Understanding Health Literacy: Implications for Medicine and Public Health (pp. 101–118). [Google Scholar]
- Dormandy E, Michie S, Hooper R, & Marteau TM (2005). Low uptake of prenatal screening for Down syndrome in minority ethnic groups and socially deprived groups: A reflection of women’s attitudes or a failure to facilitate informed choices? International Journal of Epidemiology, 34(2), 346–352. 10.1093/ije/dyi021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis EM, Ferrer RA, & Klein WMP (2018). Factors beyond Lack of Knowledge That Predict “I Don’t Know” Responses to Surveys That Assess HPV Knowledge. Journal of Health Communication, 23(10–11), 967–976. 10.1080/10810730.2018.1554729 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis EM, Ferrer RA, Taber JM, & Klein WMP (2018). Relationship of “don’t know” responses to cancer knowledge and belief questions with colorectal cancer screening behavior. Health Psychology: Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association, 37(4), 394–398. 10.1037/hea0000587 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erby LH, Roter D, Larson S, & Cho J (2008). The rapid estimate of adult literacy in genetics (REAL-G): A means to assess literacy deficits in the context of genetics. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A, 146A(2), 174–181. 10.1002/ajmg.a.32068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Facio FM, Eidem H, Fisher T, Brooks S, Linn A, Kaphingst KA, Biesecker LG, & Biesecker BB (2013). Intentions to receive individual results from whole-genome sequencing among participants in the ClinSeq study. European Journal of Human Genetics: EJHG, 21(3), 261–265. 10.1038/ejhg.2012.179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flory J, & Emanuel E (2004). Interventions to improve research participants’ understanding in informed consent for research: A systematic review. JAMA, 292(13), 1593–1601. 10.1001/jama.292.13.1593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Godin G, & Kok G (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its applications to health-related behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion: AJHP, 11(2), 87–98. 10.4278/0890-1171-11.2.87 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green RC, Berg JS, Grody WW, Kalia SS, Korf BR, Martin CL, McGuire AL, Nussbaum RL, O’Daniel JM, Ormond KE, Rehm HL, Watson MS, Williams MS, Biesecker LG, & American College of Medical Genetics and Genomics. (2013). ACMG recommendations for reporting of incidental findings in clinical exome and genome sequencing. Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics, 15(7), 565–574. 10.1038/gim.2013.73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heshka JT, Palleschi C, Howley H, Wilson B, & Wells PS (2008). A systematic review of perceived risks, psychological and behavioral impacts of genetic testing. Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics, 10(1), 19–32. 10.1097/GIM.0b013e31815f524f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hooker GW, Peay H, Erby L, Bayless T, Biesecker BB, & Roter DL (2014). Genetic literacy and patient perceptions of IBD testing utility and disease control: A randomized vignette study of genetic testing. Inflammatory Bowel Diseases, 20(5), 901–908. 10.1097/MIB.0000000000000021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Health Literacy. (2004). Health Literacy: A Prescription to End Confusion (Nielsen-Bohlman L, Panzer AM, & Kindig DA, Eds.). National Academies Press (US). http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK216032/ [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvik GP, Amendola LM, Berg JS, Brothers K, Clayton EW, Chung W, Evans BJ, Evans JP, Fullerton SM, Gallego CJ, Garrison NA, Gray SW, Holm IA, Kullo IJ, Lehmann LS, McCarty C, Prows CA, Rehm HL, Sharp RR, … Burke W (2014). Return of genomic results to research participants: The floor, the ceiling, and the choices in between. American Journal of Human Genetics, 94(6), 818–826. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalia SS, Adelman K, Bale SJ, Chung WK, Eng C, Evans JP, Herman GE, Hufnagel SB, Klein TE, Korf BR, McKelvey KD, Ormond KE, Richards CS, Vlangos CN, Watson M, Martin CL, & Miller DT (2017). Recommendations for reporting of secondary findings in clinical exome and genome sequencing, 2016 update (ACMG SF v2.0): A policy statement of the American College of Medical Genetics and Genomics. Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics, 19(2), 249–255. 10.1038/gim.2016.190 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larson E (1998). Reframing the meaning of disability to families: The embrace of paradox. Social Science & Medicine (1982), 47(7), 865–875. 10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00113-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loud JT, Bremer RC, Mai PL, Peters JA, Giri N, Stewart DR, Greene MH, Alter BP, & Savage SA (2016). Research participant interest in primary, secondary, and incidental genomic findings. Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics, 18(12), 1218–1225. 10.1038/gim.2016.36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lupo PJ, Robinson JO, Diamond PM, Jamal L, Danysh HE, Blumenthal-Barby J, Lehmann LS, Vassy JL, Christensen KD, Green RC, McGuire AL, & MedSeq Project team. (2016). Patients’ perceived utility of whole-genome sequencing for their healthcare: Findings from the MedSeq project. Personalized Medicine, 13(1), 13–20. 10.2217/pme.15.45 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mao L, Kitani A, Similuk M, Oler AJ, Albenberg L, Kelsen J, Aktay A, Quezado M, Yao M, Montgomery-Recht K, Fuss IJ, & Strober W (2018). Loss-of-function CARD8 mutation causes NLRP3 inflammasome activation and Crohn’s disease. The Journal of Clinical Investigation, 128(5), 1793–1806. 10.1172/JCI98642 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marewski JN, & Gigerenzer G (2012). Heuristic decision making in medicine. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14(1), 77–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montalvo W, & Larson E (2014). Participant comprehension of research for which they volunteer: A systematic review. Journal of Nursing Scholarship: An Official Publication of Sigma Theta Tau International Honor Society of Nursing, 46(6), 423–431. 10.1111/jnu.12097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Natarajan M, Hsu AP, Weinreich MA, Zhang Y, Niemela JE, Butman JA, Pittaluga S, Sugui J, Collar AL, Lim JK, Zangeneh T, Carr T, Oler AJ, Similuk M, Rosen LB, Desai JV, Freeman AF, Holland SM, Kwon-Chung KJ, … Lionakis MS (2018). Aspergillosis, eosinophilic esophagitis, and allergic rhinitis in signal transducer and activator of transcription 3 haploinsufficiency. The Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 142(3), 993–997.e3. 10.1016/j.jaci.2018.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor AM (1995). Validation of a decisional conflict scale. Medical Decision Making: An International Journal of the Society for Medical Decision Making, 15(1), 25–30. 10.1177/0272989X9501500105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sapp JC, Johnston JJ, Driscoll K, Heidlebaugh AR, Miren Sagardia A, Dogbe DN, . . . Biesecker LG (2018). Evaluation of Recipients of Positive and Negative Secondary Findings Evaluations in a Hybrid CLIA-Research Sequencing Pilot. Am J Hum Genet, 103(3), 358–366. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30122538. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2018.07.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards S, Aziz N, Bale S, Bick D, Das S, Gastier-Foster J, Grody WW, Hegde M, Lyon E, Spector E, Voelkerding K, Rehm HL, & ACMG Laboratory Quality Assurance Committee. (2015). Standards and guidelines for the interpretation of sequence variants: A joint consensus recommendation of the American College of Medical Genetics and Genomics and the Association for Molecular Pathology. Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics, 17(5), 405–424. 10.1038/gim.2015.30 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts JS, Robinson JO, Diamond PM, Bharadwaj A, Christensen KD, Lee KB, Green RC, McGuire AL, & MedSeq Project team. (2018). Patient understanding of, satisfaction with, and perceived utility of whole-genome sequencing: Findings from the MedSeq Project. Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics, 20(9), 1069–1076. 10.1038/gim.2017.223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romppel M, Herrmann-Lingen C, Wachter R, Edelmann F, Düngen H-D, Pieske B, & Grande G (2013). A short form of the General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSE-6): Development, psychometric properties and validity in an intercultural non-clinical sample and a sample of patients at risk for heart failure. Psycho-Social Medicine, 10, Doc01. 10.3205/psm000091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roter D, Ellington L, Erby LH, Larson S, & Dudley W (2006). The Genetic Counseling Video Project (GCVP): Models of practice. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part C, Seminars in Medical Genetics, 142C(4), 209–220. 10.1002/ajmg.c.30094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roter DL, Erby LH, Larson S, & Ellington L (2007). Assessing oral literacy demand in genetic counseling dialogue: Preliminary test of a conceptual framework. Social Science & Medicine (1982), 65(7), 1442–1457. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.05.033 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roter Debra, & Hall J (2006). Doctors Talking with Patients/Patients Talking with Doctors: Improving Communication in Medical Visits (2nd Edition). Greenwood Publishing Group. [Google Scholar]
- Tamariz L, Palacio A, Robert M, & Marcus EN (2013). Improving the informed consent process for research subjects with low literacy: A systematic review. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 28(1), 121–126. 10.1007/s11606-012-2133-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tong A, Kelly S, Nusbaum R, Graves K, Peshkin BN, Valdimarsdottir HB, Wood M, McKinnon W, Garber J, McCormick SR, Jandorf L, & Schwartz MD (2015). Intentions for risk-reducing surgery among high-risk women referred for BRCA1/BRCA2 genetic counseling. Psycho-Oncology, 24(1), 33–39. 10.1002/pon.3560 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Berg M, Timmermans DRM, Ten Kate LP, van Vugt JMG, & van der Wal G (2005). Are pregnant women making informed choices about prenatal screening? Genetics in Medicine: Official Journal of the American College of Medical Genetics, 7(5), 332–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vos J, Oosterwijk JC, Gómez-García E, Menko FH, Jansen AM, Stoel RD, van Asperen CJ, Tibben A, & Stiggelbout AM (2011). Perceiving cancer-risks and heredity-likelihood in genetic-counseling: How counselees recall and interpret BRCA 1/2-test results. Clinical Genetics, 79(3), 207–218. 10.1111/j.1399-0004.2010.01581.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vos Joël, Gómez-García E, Oosterwijk JC, Menko FH, Stoel RD, van Asperen CJ, Jansen AM, Stiggelbout AM, & Tibben A (2012). Opening the psychological black box in genetic counseling. The psychological impact of DNA testing is predicted by the counselees’ perception, the medical impact by the pathogenic or uninformative BRCA1/2-result. Psycho-Oncology, 21(1), 29–42. 10.1002/pon.1864 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vos Joël, Oosterwijk JC, Gomez-Garcia E, Menko FH, Collee MJ, van Asperen CJ, Jansen AM, Stiggelbout AM, & Tibben A (2012). Exploring the short-term impact of DNA-testing in breast cancer patients: The counselees’ perception matters, but the actual BRCA1/2 result does not. Patient Education and Counseling, 86(2), 239–251. 10.1016/j.pec.2011.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vos Joël, Otten W, van Asperen C, Jansen A, Menko F, & Tibben A (2008). The counsellees’ view of an unclassified variant in BRCA1/2: Recall, interpretation, and impact on life. Psycho-Oncology, 17(8), 822–830. 10.1002/pon.1311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams MV, Baker DW, Parker RM, & Nurss JR (1998). Relationship of functional health literacy to patients’ knowledge of their chronic disease. A study of patients with hypertension and diabetes. Archives of Internal Medicine, 158(2), 166–172. 10.1001/archinte.158.2.166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf SM, Burke W, & Koenig BA (2015). Mapping the Ethics of Translational Genomics: Situating Return of Results and Navigating the Research-Clinical Divide. The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics: A Journal of the American Society of Law, Medicine & Ethics, 43(3), 486–501. 10.1111/jlme.12291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wynn J, Martinez J, Duong J, Zhang Y, Phelan J, Fyer A, Klitzman R, Appelbaum PS, & Chung WK (2015). Association of Researcher Characteristics with Views on Return of Incidental Findings from Genomic Research. Journal of Genetic Counseling, 24(5), 833–841. 10.1007/s10897-014-9817-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yska HAF, Elsink K, Kuijpers TW, Frederix GWJ, van Gijn ME, & van Montfrans JM (2019). Diagnostic Yield of Next Generation Sequencing in Genetically Undiagnosed Patients with Primary Immunodeficiencies: A Systematic Review. Journal of Clinical Immunology 10.1007/s10875-019-00656-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]