Abstract
Background and Hypothesis
Diminished social motivation is a negative symptom of schizophrenia and leads to severe functional consequences for many patients suffering from the illness. However, there are no effective medications available to treat this symptom. Despite the lack of approved treatments for patients, there is a growing body of literature on the effects of several classes of drugs on social motivation in healthy volunteers that may be relevant to patients. The aim of this review is to synthesize these results in an effort to identify novel directions for the development of medications to treat reduced social motivation in schizophrenia.
Study Design
In this article, we review pharmacologic challenge studies addressing the acute effects of psychoactive drugs on social motivation in healthy volunteers and consider how these findings may be applied to deficits in social motivation in schizophrenia. We include studies testing amphetamines and 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), opioids, cannabis, serotonergic psychedelics, antidepressants, benzodiazepines, and neuropeptides.
Study Results
We report that amphetamines, MDMA, and some opioid medications enhance social motivation in healthy adults and may represent promising avenues of investigation in schizophrenia.
Conclusions
Given the acute effects of these drugs on behavioral and performance-based measures of social motivation in healthy volunteers, they may be particularly beneficial as an adjunct to psychosocial training programs in patient populations. It remains to be determined how these medications affect patients with deficits in social motivation, and in which contexts they may be most effectively administered.
Keywords: Amotivation, approach and avoidance, human behavioral pharmacology
Introduction
Asociality, or impaired social motivation, is a negative symptom of schizophrenia and a major contributor to functional impairment in this population.1,2 Social motivation refers to the drive to spend time with and form lasting relationships with others. Diminishment of this drive has severe functional consequences for those suffering from the illness including deficits in independent living, work, and maintenance of relationships.3,4 Deficits in social motivation in schizophrenia do not respond to antipsychotic medications, and asociality remains a common, debilitating symptom of the disease.5,6 Some patients benefit from social skills training, but the effects are modest, and low social motivation may further limit effectiveness of such interventions.7 Thus, there is currently no effective treatment for impaired social motivation.
Few psychopharmacology studies have targeted impairments in social motivation in patients with schizophrenia. However, there is evidence that certain psychoactive drugs can enhance social motivation in healthy volunteers, and these may be worth investigating in patients. Medications may enhance social motivation in their own right, or they may enable patients to benefit from psychosocial training by ameliorating social motivational deficits. Here we review acute effects of psychoactive drugs on social motivation in healthy volunteers and speculate on how these drugs may also treat impairments in patients. We will not discuss antipsychotic drugs because, despite their value in treatment of schizophrenia, they have little effect on clinician ratings of social motivation, and to the best of our knowledge there are no acute challenge studies.
Fulford et al.1 note that patients with schizophrenia exhibit deficits in both social cognition and social motivation. We focus this discussion on indices of social motivation, distinct from other social deficits. Fulford et al. further distinguishes between deficits that are related to diminished approach to positive social stimuli and deficits that result from heightened avoidance of perceived negative social stimuli. This distinction is important in that these categories of motivation may have distinct underlying neural substrates; reward circuitry is important for approach deficits, whereas fear circuitry may underlie avoidance. As a result, different therapeutic interventions may be required to target each component of motivation. Consistent with this distinction, we review 7 classes of drugs—amphetamines/3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA), opioids, cannabis, serotonergic psychedelics, antidepressants, benzodiazepines, and neuropeptides/neurosteroids—and consider how each might increase the pleasurable aspects of social stimuli and/or decrease the avoidance of perceived aversive aspects of social stimuli (Table 1). These classes of drugs have been selected on the basis of there being at least some available literature investigating their effects on social motivation. While the cholinergic neurotransmitter system is of particular interest in schizophrenia and in cognitive processes, this system has been less studied in the context of social motivation, and to the best of our knowledge, no studies have been conducted directly assessing the effects of cholinergic medications on these measures.
Table 1.
Drug Effects on Measures of Social Motivation in Healthy Volunteers.
Measure of Approach Motivation | Description | Drug Effects | References |
---|---|---|---|
Subjective ratings | Self-report ratings of sociability, friendliness, desire to socialize | MDMA↑ d-amphetamine↑ methamphetamine↑ |
Tancer et al.15 Van Wel et al.16 Kirkpatrick et al.17 |
Memory of positive social cues | Memory for spacial location of emotional face cues | Buprenorphine↑ | Syal et al.59 |
Autobiographical Memory Task | Positivity ratings of recalled social memories | Oxytocin↑ | Cardoso et al.14 |
Affective responses to social stimuli | |||
Emotional images | |||
Ratings | Ratings of positivity | MDMA ↑ (for social content) D-amphetamine ↑ (for all images) Buprenorphine ↑ (for social content) |
Wardle and de Wit20 Wardle et al.21 Bershad et al.14 |
Facial EMG | Physiological emotional reactivity to positive faces | MDMA↑ | Wardle and de Wit20 |
Emotional faces | |||
Facial EMG | Physiological emotional reactivity to positive faces | MDMA↑ | Wardle and de Wit20 |
fMRI | Striatal responses to happy faces | MDMA↑ Oxytocin↑ Mirtazapine↑ |
Bedi et al.41 Norbury et al.115 Lieberz et al.135 Rawlings et al.113 |
Attractive faces | Ratings of and desire to view images of faces | Morphine↑ | Chelnokova et al.55 |
Multifaceted Empathy Test | Ratings of “feeling for” others in emotional images | MDMA↑ d-amphetamine↑ |
Schmid et al.22 Dolder et al.23 Hysek et al.24 Kuypers et al.25 |
Welfare Tradeoff Task | Prosocial resource allocation | MDMA↑ | Kirkpatrick et al.33 |
Prisoner’s Dilemma Task | Cooperative behavior | MDMA↑ | Gabay et al.32 |
Speech | |||
Quantity | Number of words spoken | Methamphetamine ↑ d-amphetamine↑ | Wardle et al.21 Marrone et al.35 Ward et al.36 Griffiths et al.37 |
Fluency | Measured by the number of pauses of disfluencies during speech | Methamphetamine ↑ MDMA↓ | Marrone et al.35 |
Content | Use of specific types of words/concepts (e.g., positive, social) | MDMA↑ | Baggott et al.38 Bedi et al.39 Agurto et al.40 Wardle and de Wit20 |
Attention bias to social stimuli | Initial orienting toward positive faces, visual exploration of eyes | MDMA↑ Morphine↑ Oxytocin↑ |
Bershad et al.64 Chelnokova et al.56 Domes et al.24 Bradley et al.145 |
Affective responses to touch | Ratings of pleasantness | MDMA↑ | Bershad et al.64 |
Measure of avoidance motivation | |||
Subjective ratings | Self-report ratings of social anxiety | MDMA↓ | Baggott et al.19 |
Affective responses to emotional faces (fMRI) | Amygdala responses to threatening faces | MDMA↓ Oxytocin↓ Propranolol↓ SSRIs, Mirtazapine↓ |
Bedi et al.. 2010 Spengler et al.17 Hurlemann et al.120 Murphy et al.122 Rawlings et al.113 |
Cyberball | Responses to simulated social exclusion | Buprenorphine↓ MDMA↓ |
Bershad et al.14, Frye et al.47 |
Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) | Responses to a stressful public speaking task | Buprenorphine↓ Hydromorphone↓ Propranolol↓ |
Bershad et al.63,64 MacCormack et al.119 |
Attention bias to social stimuli | Initial orienting toward negative faces | Buprenorphine↓ | Bershad et al.14 |
Note: ↑, increase; ↓, decrease; EMG, electromyography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging.
Measures of social motivation reviewed here include self-reported and clinician-rated affiliative tendencies as well as performance-based tasks assessing social motivation. Some studies examine psychophysiological responses and neural responses to social stimuli using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG). It is worth mentioning there is a developing literature on drug effects on preclinical models of social motivation in schizophrenia, but we will limit our discussion here to human studies. We review evidence from studies with healthy volunteers to identify processes that may enhance function in patients with schizophrenia.
Amphetamines and 3,4-Methylenedioxymethamphetamine
Approach Motivation
As a class, amphetamines represent some of the most promising medications to enhance approach motivation. Before exploring the possible benefits of this class of medications, we address 2 clinical concerns about amphetamines: (1) that they may exacerbate positive symptoms of schizophrenia, and (2) that they have potential for abuse. With regard to the first, several studies have shown that low to moderate doses of amphetamines are safe.8,9 It has been shown that most (if not all) of the potential adverse effects of these agents in patients with schizophrenia can be prevented with concurrent antipsychotic treatment.10,11 With regard to the second, stimulant drugs have been prescribed safely for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder for decades with little evidence for abuse when the drugs are used orally as prescribed.12 MDMA has shown promise as an adjunct to psychotherapy in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD),13 perhaps by facilitating social engagement during the therapeutic session. MDMA has not yet been tested in patients with schizophrenia, and few studies have directly compared social motivation effects of MDMA to those of other stimulants.14
Both amphetamines (d-amphetamine, methamphetamine) and MDMA increase several dimensions of sociability including subjective ratings and affective responses to social stimuli. Both increase subjective ratings of sociable emotions, including how talkative, friendly, and social individuals report feeling.15–19 MDMA enhances ratings of positivity for images with social content,20 while d-amphetamine enhances positive ratings of images, regardless of social content.21 Several studies have investigated the effects of amphetamines and MDMA on empathy. Both MDMA and d-amphetamine increase emotional empathy for positive situations,22,23 and MDMA may increase empathy for negative situations as well.24–26
One laboratory study has used a less traditional measure of social reactivity, that is, the subjective ratings of the pleasantness of social touch.27 Social touch is a fundamental psychosocial process that acts via well-characterized physiological circuits.28–30 Although therapeutic touch is not used in most modern psychotherapy, it has recently been reintroduced as a component of psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy.31 The processes through which touch may facilitate the therapeutic experience (e.g., through grounding, reducing anxiety, and facilitating social connection) are unknown. While the above-referenced study did not specifically investigate processes that could be of value in psychotherapy, it shows that acute doses of MDMA, but not methamphetamine, enhance pleasantness of touch in healthy volunteers. While fully recognizing potential ethical complexities that arise with the use of touch in psychotherapy, the role of social touch in treatment of psychiatric disorders, including schizophrenia, remains an understudied area.
In addition to the above effects, MDMA also enhances prosocial behavior. It increases cooperative behavior during a Prisoner’s Dilemma Task32 and increases generosity toward friends on a Welfare Tradeoff Task,33 suggesting MDMA may motivate prosocial resource allocation. Another important behavioral measure of social motivation is speech. Speech may reflect a continuous effort to connect during a social interaction, and thus is a form of social motivation.34 Both methamphetamine and d-amphetamine increase speech quantity,21,35–37 and methamphetamine increases fluency.35 MDMA specifically increases the use of social words and concepts.38–40 Additionally, MDMA, but not methamphetamine, increases initial orienting to positive facial expressions.27
Other studies have examined the effects of amphetamines and MDMA on physiological and neural measures of affective reactivity to social stimuli. MDMA enhances positive psychophysiological responses to emotional faces,20 which may indicate increased reward during a positive social encounter. MDMA also enhances ventral striatal responses to positive faces.41 Although these studies have been done in healthy controls, the findings suggest that amphetamines in general, and particularly MDMA, may show promise in improving social motivation in schizophrenia.
Avoidance Motivation
While both MDMA and amphetamines appear to enhance approach motivation, only MDMA also dampens responses to negative social interactions. Clinical trials with MDMA suggest that the drug can reduce social anxiety or recall of emotionally intense memories.19,42–44 In line with this idea, MDMA reduces amygdalar responses to threatening faces,41 and increases the threshold of reactivity to negative facial expressions.20,22,24,45,46 One study showed that MDMA reduces affective responses to social rejection in the simulated social rejection paradigm Cyberball.47
Neither amphetamines, nor MDMA, seem to dampen subjective or physiological responses to a laboratory model of acute social stress.48,49 MDMA has paradoxical effects on anxiety: It induces anxiety in some contexts,19 but may also ameliorate social anxiety and fear of negative evaluation in social settings specifically.19,43 The contexts in which MDMA induces vs. ameliorates anxiety is a topic for future investigation.
Opioids
Approach Motivation
The opioid system plays a key role in social behavior of humans and other species. Receptors in both the mu-opioid systems and the kappa-opioid systems are important in social function, although in different ways. Mu-opioid system activation appears to mediate affiliative motivation in preclinical animal models and in humans.50–53 Conversely, for the kappa-opioid system it is antagonism of the receptors that favors prosociality, and kappa-opioid system antagonists decrease anxiety and responses to social stress across species.54 Thus, the systems appear to act in opposite directions.
Studies in healthy volunteers have suggested that mu-opioid activation favors approach motivation. One study investigated the effects of the opioid agonist morphine on “liking” and “wanting” of attractive faces. “Liking” was measured by ratings of attractiveness, and “wanting” was measured by participants changing the viewing time to continue viewing each face or to move to the next one. For the most attractive faces, morphine increases both ratings of attractiveness and time spent viewing the faces, suggesting an increase in both “wanting” and “liking” for a high-value social reward.55 On another behavioral measure of social motivation, visual attention to social stimuli, morphine increases visual exploration of faces and eyes.56 The effects of morphine on pleasantness ratings of social touch have also been investigated, though, unlike MDMA, morphine does not appear to affect this measure.57
While morphine is a pure mu-agonist, buprenorphine is a partial agonist at the mu-opioid system but antagonist at the kappa-opioid system. Buprenorphine has shown promise for patients with social deficits. Low doses of buprenorphine selectively increase ratings of positivity of images with social content, suggesting the drug may enhance the pleasantness of social experiences.58 Furthermore, it boosts memory for positive social cues,59 suggesting it may enhance approach motivation.
Given the effects of a kappa antagonist, it is potentially informative to consider the effects of a kappa agonist. Salvinorin A is an agonist at the kappa-opioid receptor and is considered an “atypical dissociative hallucinogen.” To the best of our knowledge, no studies have directly assessed the effects of the drug on social motivation, but studies suggest salvinorin A alters the experience of one's self, which may indirectly affect social experiences.60,61
Avoidance Motivation
Both mu-agonists and kappa-antagonists also reduce certain dimensions of avoidance motivation in humans and other species. Social avoidance motivation is often assessed using the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST).62 in which participants present a speech in front of a panel of nonreactive strangers. It provides both subjective measures (threat appraisal, anxiety) and physiological measures (heart rate, blood pressure, and cortisol). During this task, buprenorphine reduces how threatened participants feel even before they give the speech, and reduces cortisol responses to the task.63 The mu-agonist hydromorphone also reduces cortisol responses to the task, but does not affect subjective threat appraisals.64 The compound mu- and kappa- actions of buprenorphine may account for the drugs’ differential effects.
Beyond reducing responses to acute social stress, opioids also dampen responses to negative facial expressions and reduce feelings of social rejection. Morphine reduces perceived anger from ambiguous facial expressions.65 Buprenorphine reduces negative affective responses to rejection during Cyberball58 and reduces responses to faces expressing negative emotions.58,66 Buprenorphine has also shown promise in the treatment of suicidality, possibly in part due to its effects on social behavior.67
While there is no selective kappa antagonist available for use in humans, preclinical evidence suggests kappa-antagonists reduce social distress. In rodents, kappa antagonists54,68–70 reduce effects of social defeat stress, and buprenorphine reverses stress-induced social interaction deficits.71 These results are in line with evidence that buprenorphine, acting as both a mu-agonist and kappa antagonist, reduces avoidance motivation in healthy human volunteers, perhaps more than its pure mu-opioid analogs.
One final medication that should be mentioned in a discussion of opioids and social motivation is the opioid antagonist, naltrexone, which appears to inhibit social motivation. Naltrexone acts as an antagonist of both the kappa receptor (which may favor increased social motivation), and the mu receptor (which may counteract beneficial effects). In healthy volunteers, naltrexone inhibits social bonding,72 self-disclosure,73 feelings of warmth during social interactions,74 and reduces ventral striatal responses to images of close social contacts.75
Overall, the findings described above indicate that opioid agonists at the mu receptor and antagonists at the kappa receptor may have promise in enhancing approach motivation and reducing avoidance motivation in clinical populations.
Cannabis
Approach Motivation
The effects of cannabis or its primary active constituent delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) on the perceived value of positive social interactions are mixed. One popular idea is that cannabis produces “amotivational” states that decrease motivation in general, including social motivation.76 However, the evidence that cannabinoids either increase or decrease the value of positive social interactions is mixed.77 Early studies conducted with cannabis users in a residential laboratory found that cannabis does not increase either time spent in a social area or time spent talking.78 There is some evidence that THC reduces the willingness to exert effort to earn points redeemable for money in healthy volunteers,79 but willingness to work for a social reward has not been studied. Cannabis and THC have little effect on other indices of social motivation, such as self-reported sociability, reactivity to positive facial emotions or amount of time spent talking.80–82 Only one study found that cannabis increases social and verbal interaction, but this occurred only in cannabis users who had high baseline levels of interaction.83,84 The evidence thus far does not strongly support the idea that cannabis might increase the motivation to engage in positive social interaction.
Many patients with schizophrenia use cannabis, for reasons that vary widely.85 These include reasons related to affect (depression, happiness), relaxation, socialization, physical effects (euphoria, energy, tiredness, etc.), treating symptoms (e.g. paranoia, suspiciousness, confusion)and treating side effects of antipsychotic medication (e.g. restlessness). Although the category of socialization suggests an effect on social motivation, it is also possible that cannabis use simply provides an opportunity to engage with others. Overall, there is little evidence that cannabis or related drugs increase the motivation to engage in positive social interactions.
Avoidance Motivation
The effects of cannabis on perceived negative effects of social interactions have received little attention, and the findings are mixed.86 A low dose of THC dampens anticipatory anxiety on the trier social stress test (TSST), but a higher dose increases response to stress.87 Imaging studies suggest that low doses of THC (7.5 mg) reduce amygdala reactivity to angry and fearful faces.88–90 THC has mixed effects on measures of attentional bias to negative emotional faces: One study reports that THC impairs recognition of fear and anger emotions in faces,91 but another shows that THC increases attention to negatively valenced stimuli,82 suggesting that it could heighten responses to negative social situations in patients. There has been concern that THC administration can induce psychotic, negative, and other psychiatric symptoms with large effect sizes, and the small, mixed effects of the drug on social motivation may not be enough to warrant the risk.92 Although one of the constituents of the cannabis plant, cannabidiol, has received much attention as a possible medication to decrease social anxiety, or to dampen anxiogenic effects of THC,93,94 this idea is not supported by a comprehensive recent review.95 It is possible that some of the positive reports about cannabidiol are related to positive expectancies.96
Serotonergic Psychedelics
Approach Motivation
Serotonergic psychedelics, or “classic psychedelics,” have garnered interest in clinical psychiatry in the treatment of depression and other conditions. These substances act as 5HT2A agonists, and include psilocybin, dimethyltryptamine (the active ingredient in ayahuasca), and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD). Psychedelic drugs present an unusual category of potential medications for deficits in social motivation. Unlike most psychiatric medications, these drugs are typically administered in a single, controlled session, at relatively high doses, and their therapeutic effects are usually assessed well after the drug experience. Beyond this dosing strategy, there has been an interest in “microdosing” regimens, in which individuals take very low doses of the substance at 2–3 day intervals. Despite promising anecdotal reports of the clinical effectiveness of microdosing to improve mood and cognition, controlled studies have shown very small effects.97–99 Few studies have examined the effects of psychedelic drugs specifically on social motivation. Psychedelics reportedly affect the sense of self, including a loosening of self-boundaries, oneness, unity, or ego dissolution.100 Such effects might influence future social interactions, including social motivation by helping patients to regain pleasure from social interaction. As with stimulant drugs, a common concern is that these drugs might worsen symptoms, especially because they can induce experiences that resemble symptoms of schizophrenia.101 Nevertheless, it has been proposed that psychedelic drugs may have a role in schizophrenia treatment, and the potential benefits remain to be determined.102 It is not known whether psychedelic drugs enhance social motivation in healthy adults, using tasks specifically designed to assess this construct (eg, social incentive delay task or willingness to exert effort to obtain a social reward).
Avoidance Motivation
Few studies have examined the effects of psychedelic drugs on avoidance of social interactions. While several studies have explored the effects of this class of drugs on anxiety in general, few have specifically investigated social anxiety. One recent study administered ayahuasca to 17 volunteers with social anxiety disorder and assessed the effects of the drug on performance on the TSST. The authors report the drug increases self-assessment of performance, but does not dampen social anxiety related to the task.103 As noted above, the potential therapeutic effects of these drugs are typically assessed well after the acute effects have dissipated, distinguishing them from other typical psychiatric medications. It is possible that psychedelic drugs have lasting psychological effects that enhance social motivation, for example by decreasing sensitivity to negative emotional stimuli. This is an important future topic for research. It is notable that psychedelics such as ayahuasca and psilocybin have shown promise in the treatment of psychiatric disorders such as PTSD and depression,104–107 which are also characterized by social withdrawal, raising the possibility that these drugs have some place in treatment of social avoidance in patients with schizophrenia.
Antidepressants
Approach Motivation
Antidepressants are a heterogenous category of medications that include drugs with different mechanisms of action which, in addition to their efficacy in treating symptoms of depression, also affect other behaviors. Serotonergic antidepressant drugs may increase motivation for positive social interactions by modifying processing of emotional stimuli.108–111 Harmer et al. have argued that antidepressant drugs acutely (within hours) enhance the processing of positive emotional information, relieving the negative processing bias associated with depression. This acute effect ultimately leads to a change in social behavior that is thought to contribute to the longer-term clinical effectiveness of the medications through cognitive restructuring and learning.109 Harmer and her colleagues have demonstrated, using functional neuroimaging, that a range of antidepressant drugs (from selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors [SSRIs], to serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors [SNRIs], and to mirtazapine) both acutely (hours after administration)112,113 and subacutely (after 7 days of treatment)114 alter fronto-limbic and extra-striatal circuitry in ways that affect processing of affective stimuli. In this way, these classes of medications may modify evaluation of self, the world, and the future, in patients with depression, and perhaps also in schizophrenia. Antidepressant drugs such as citalopram, fluoxetine, duloxetine, and reboxetine, even after acute administration, positively bias responses to social cues,111 and they modulate amygdalar responses to images of emotional faces.115 Serotonergic antidepressants have shown some benefit in treating the negative symptoms of schizophrenia in clinical trials, suggesting they may act to enhance social motivation.116,117
Avoidance Motivation
Antidepressant drugs may affect not only the perceived value of positive social encounters (above), but may also affect the avoidance of perceived negative social interactions. That is, antidepressants may counter deficits in social motivation by lessening the perceived negativity of social stimuli. In a review of studies of acute administration of antidepressants, Harmer et al. describe this negative processing bias: “Depression is associated with the tendency to perceive social cues as more negative, to preferentially attend to aversive information, and to recall negative more than positive information concerning oneself.”109,111 Thus, the authors conclude that antidepressant drugs like citalopram, fluoxetine, duloxetine, and reboxetinetypically reduce neural and behavioral responses to negative facial expressions, consistent with a decreased avoidance of negative social encounters. This suggests that they may have the potential to relieve the avoidance of negative social encounters experienced by some patients with schizophrenia.
Beyond SSRI and SNRIs, the beta-adrenergic antagonist propranolol is another drug that has been studied in the treatment of anxiety and PTSD.118 Several studies have shown propranolol decreases physiological and subjective responses to the TSST,119 suggesting the drug may reduce avoidance motivation. The effects of propranolol may not be specific to social avoidance, or even social processes more generally, however. One study showed that propranolol reduces amygdalar responses to faces expressing all types of emotion.120 In fact, propranolol may reduce the salience of social cues. Another study used a learning paradigm that involves both social trials (happy vs. angry faces) and nonsocial trials (red vs. green colors). Under placebo, participants acquire associations with social cues more quickly than nonsocial cues, but, propranolol abolishes this distinction.121 Therefore, while the drug may reduce avoidance motivation, it may reduce the salience of social signals overall, which may be a detrimental effect in clinical contexts.
Benzodiazepines
Approach Motivation
To the best of our knowledge, the effects of benzodiazepines on the motivation to engage in positive social interactions have not been tested. Typical benzodiazepines such as diazepam and lorazepam have little effect on measures of facial emotion recognition effect,122,123 especially on recognition of happy expressions that might predict increased social motivation.
Avoidance Motivation
Benzodiazepines may facilitate engagement in social interaction by decreasing social anxiety. However, they have mixed effects on the Trier Social Stress Test. Both diazepam and alprazolam reduce physiological indicators of stress, but fail to decrease self-report ratings of anxiety during this paradigm.124–126 On emotion recognition tasks,127 diazepam reduces attention toward emotional faces and decreases overall startle reactivity, but does not significantly affect facial expression recognition.122 Lorazepam dampens amygdalar reactivity to faces expressing both positive and negative emotions.128 While benzodiazepines may ameliorate autonomic hyperreactivity associated with increased avoidance motivation, these effects are inconsistent. Therefore this class of medications shows limited promise as treatment of social anxiety that might inhibit engagement in social interactions.
Neuropeptides and Neurosteroids
Approach Motivation
The neuropeptide oxytocin is known to mediate social motivation in humans and other animals (reviewed in129). In healthy volunteers, intranasal oxytocin may either enhance approach in positive social contexts, or enhance avoidance in potentially threatening social contexts.130–135 This paradoxical effect has led to the “social salience” hypothesis, according to which oxytocin increases the salience of social stimuli, regardless of valence.136 In the realm of approach, intranasal oxytocin increases the positivity of memories of social interactions in healthy volunteers.137 It also enhances gazes toward positive, but not negative, social stimuli,138 and toward socially salient eyes and faces in social images.139,140 These effects on eye gaze have led to the study of intranasal oxytocin in the treatment of autism.141–143
Oxytocin is one of the few drugs that have been tested to improve social motivation in schizophrenia. Intranasal oxytocin has been assessed in relation to several symptoms of schizophrenia, including deficits in social cognition and social motivation. However, the results are mixed.144 Paralleling the autism literature, intranasal oxytocin increases time spent looking at the eye region of faces in patients with schizophrenia.145 Oxytocin also increases motivation for the social reward of winning on an auction task in patients with schizophrenia,146 though it does not enhance the motivating effects of social encouragement.147 To identify neural changes underlying possible effects of oxytocin on social motivation, one study used EEG to measure mu-suppression. Mu-suppression refers to suppression of oscillations in the 8–12 Hz range over central electrodes in response to social stimuli, and is thought to be an index of mirror neuron activity.148 This study investigated a range of 8 doses of oxytocin, reporting that two mid-range doses (36 and 48 International Units) of oxytocin enhance mu-suppression in patients.149 This study was unique in that it included a full range of doses, while other studies may have failed to show effects because they tested only 1 or 2 doses. Thus the findings with oxytocin are difficult to interpret, perhaps because of the heterogeneity of measures used or the limited doses tested.
Despite promising findings after acute doses of oxytocin, longer-term clinical trials of oxytocin for social deficits in schizophrenia have been disappointing. Although few studies have examined social motivation in particular, several clinical trials have examined effects of oxytocin alone and in combination with social cognitive training on social cognition and negative symptoms over 6–12 weeks. Daily intranasal oxytocin improves social cognition150 and decreases negative symptoms in some studies.150,151 But another trial reports only minor improvements in social symptoms on one subscale of the interpersonal reactivity index.152 Oxytocin has also been tested as an adjunct to social cognitive skills training, but not as a treatment for social motivation. One study suggests oxytocin combined with social cognitive training enhances empathic accuracy,153 but another study reports no effect on measures of social cognition or symptom ratings.154–156 There are numerous technical challenges with intranasal oxytocin administration, including large variability in effects of oxytocin at different doses,157 questions about incomplete absorption,158 issues related to assay measurement,159 and different effects for acute vs. chronic administration.160 A primary issue is that absorption of oxytocin by the intranasal route is controversial. It may be absorbed directly into the brain, peripherally by crossing the blood-brain barrier, or by influencing vagal projections. One study attempted to dissect these considerations in relation to observed effects on social motivation by comparing lingual oxytocin spray to intranasal oxytocin. They show that the two routes of administration lead to very different, sometimes opposing effects; oral oxytocin, for instance, increases amygdalar responses to faces, while intranasal oxytocin decreases such responses.161 Further work is needed to determine the exact contexts in which oxytocin may act on social motivation.
Beyond oxytocin, a handful of other hormonal treatments have been considered in the treatment of social motivation deficits. The neuropeptide arginine vasopressin promotes both aggressive and affiliative behaviors in different social contexts, and has been tested as a treatment of social deficits in autism.162 However, the one trial assessing the effects of intranasal arginine vasopressin on cooperative behavior in schizophrenia reported no effect.163 Although there has been interest in the use of pregnenolone and estrogen to treat negative symptoms of schizophrenia,164,165 the evidence for acute effects on social motivation is limited. Overall, there is limited evidence that neuropeptides and neurosteroids modulate social motivation in schizophrenia, but factors such as dosing, sex, and social context remain to be explored.
Avoidance Motivation
Beyond its well-studied role in social approach motivation, oxytocin has also been implicated in avoidance motivation, but the findings are mixed.166 Some studies report that it reduces anxiety and avoidance in healthy volunteers,167,168 while others indicate that it increases avoidance, facilitating withdrawal from potentially dangerous situations.133,134 The effects of oxytocin on avoidance motivation may be different in healthy adults vs. in patients with schizophrenia. One fMRI study showed that intranasal oxytocin administration decreases amygdalar reactivity to emotional faces in patients with schizophrenia, but increases it in healthy controls.169
Arginine vasopressin appears to enhance avoidance motivation in some contexts, although to the best of our knowledge, no studies have examined patients with schizophrenia.170 Other hormones such as pregnenolone and estrogen have not been investigated.
Conclusions and Future Directions
In conclusion, several drugs show promise in both enhancing approach and reducing avoidance motivation. MDMA reliably enhances approach motivation and reduces avoidance motivation in healthy volunteers, but has not been tested in schizophrenia. Certain opioid drugs that activate the mu-opioid system and deactivate the kappa-opioid system, such as buprenorphine, may also dampen emotional responses to negative social stimuli and boost reward responses to positive social stimuli. SSRIs, SNRIs, and mirtazapine may facilitate a favorable social motivation profile by positively biasing social cognition. Amphetamines may increase approach motivation, but do not reduce avoidance motivation. The effects of other categories of drugs on social motivation are mixed.
Drugs may improve social motivation either through their direct effects or as an adjunct to a psychosocial treatment. In the case of the former, drugs would be taken daily to improve social interactions. In the case of the latter, the drugs would be taken a limited number of times during social training or psychotherapy. Amphetamines, MDMA, and opioid medications may be best administered in a controlled setting to enhance psychosocial treatments. More work should be done investigating these medications as adjuncts to such interventions in patient populations.
This review illustrates some of the challenges in defining and measuring the construct of social motivation. Although diminished social motivation is recognized as an important feature in schizophrenia, it is typically assessed clinically as part of a broader cluster of negative symptoms using clinician-rated interviews171 in which patients report on their social activities during their normal daily lives. In contrast, controlled studies typically assess effects of drugs on social motivation using tasks that target core deficits of social motivation. Even farther removed from clinical trials are studies using preclinical models of social motivation, which have the advantage of tightly controlled experimental design, but are limited by their relevance to human experience. It is difficult to apply the knowledge gained from the preclinical and human laboratory setting to the clinical setting, or vice versa. Moreover, it is not known whether effects of acute drug administration on tasks would provide a good index of patients’ willingness to form and maintain social relationships in their normal daily environments.1 Therefore, we recommend that future clinical trials investigating pharmacologic treatments for deficits in social motivation in schizophrenia include behavioral and performance-based measures of social motivation that are comparable to those used with healthy volunteers, such as visual attention to social cues, social decision-making paradigms, and effort expenditure tasks.
Contributor Information
Anya K Bershad, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior, Los Angeles, CA, USA.
Harriet de Wit, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA.
Conflicts of Interest
HdW is on the Board of Directors of PharmAla Biotech and consultant to Awakn Life Sciences and Gilgamesh Pharmaceuticals. These are unrelated to this manuscript.
Funding
HdW is supported by a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (DA02812).
References
- 1. Fulford D, Campellone T, Gard DE.. Social motivation in schizophrenia: How research on basic reward processes informs and limits our understanding. Clin Psychol Rev. 2018;63:12–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Ventura J, Wood RC, Jimenez AM, Hellemann GS.. Neurocognition and symptoms identify links between facial recognition and emotion processing in schizophrenia: Meta-analytic findings. Schizophr Res. 2013;151(1–3):78–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Juckel G, Morosini PL.. The new approach: Psychosocial functioning as a necessary outcome criterion for therapeutic success in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2008;21(6):630–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Kalin M, Kaplan S, Gould F, Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Harvey PD.. Social cognition, social competence, negative symptoms and social outcomes: Inter-relationships in people with schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. 2015;68:254–260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Kirkpatrick B, Fenton WS, Carpenter WT, Marder SR.. The NIMH-MATRICS consensus statement on negative symptoms. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(2):214–219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Javed A, Charles A.. The importance of social cognition in improving functional outcomes in schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9:157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Kurtz MM, Mueser KT.. A meta-analysis of controlled research on social skills training for schizophrenia. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76(3):491–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Swerdlow NR, Bhakta SG, Talledo JA, et al. Effects of amphetamine on sensorimotor gating and neurocognition in antipsychotic-medicated schizophrenia patients. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43(4):708–717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Swerdlow NR, Bhakta SG, Talledo J, et al. Lessons learned by giving amphetamine to antipsychotic-medicated schizophrenia patients. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44(13):2277–2284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Lasser RA, Dirks B, Nasrallah H, et al. Adjunctive lisdexamfetamine dimesylate therapy in adult outpatients with predominant negative symptoms of schizophrenia: Open-label and randomized-withdrawal phases. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(11):2140–2149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Modell W, Hussar AE.. Failure of dextroamphetamine sulfate to influence eating and sleeping patterns in obese schizophrenic patients: Clinical and pharmacological significance. JAMA. 1965;193(4):275–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Lee SS, Humphreys KL, Flory K, Liu R, Glass K.. Prospective association of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and substance use and abuse/dependence: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2011;31(3):328–341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Mitchell JM, Bogenschutz M, Lilienstein A, et al. MDMA-assisted therapy for severe PTSD: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled phase 3 study. Nat Med. 2021;27(6):1025–1033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Bershad AK, Miller MA, Baggott MJ, de Wit H.. The effects of MDMA on socio-emotional processing: Does MDMA differ from other stimulants? J Psychopharmacol. 2016;30(12):1248–1258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Tancer M, Johanson C-E, Johanson C-E.. Reinforcing, subjective, and physiological effects of MDMA in humans: A comparison with d-amphetamine and mCPP. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;72(1):33–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. van Wel JH, Kuypers KP, Theunissen EL, Bosker WM, Bakker K, Ramaekers JG.. Effects of acute MDMA intoxication on mood and impulsivity: Role of the 5-HT2 and 5-HT1 receptors. PLoS One. 2012;7(7):e40187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Kirkpatrick MG, Gunderson EW, Perez AY, Haney M, Foltin RW, Hart CL.. A direct comparison of the behavioral and physiological effects of methamphetamine and 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012;219(1):109–122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Regan A, Margolis S, de Wit H, Lyubomirsky S.. Does±3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (ecstasy) induce subjective feelings of social connection in humans? A multilevel meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2021;16(10):e0258849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Baggott MJ, Coyle JR, Siegrist JD, Garrison KJ, Galloway GP, Mendelson JE.. Effects of 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine on socioemotional feelings, authenticity, and autobiographical disclosure in healthy volunteers in a controlled setting. J Psychopharmacol. 2016;30(4):378–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Wardle MC, de Wit H.. MDMA alters emotional processing and facilitates positive social interaction. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2014;231(21):4219–4229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Wardle MC, Garner MJ, Munafò MR, de Wit H.. Amphetamine as a social drug: Effects of d-amphetamine on social processing and behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012;223(2):199–210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Schmid Y, Hysek CM, Simmler LD, Crockett MJ, Quednow BB, Liechti ME.. Differential effects of MDMA and methylphenidate on social cognition. J Psychopharmacol. 2014;28(9):847–856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Dolder PC, Müller F, Schmid Y, Borgwardt SJ, Liechti ME.. Direct comparison of the acute subjective, emotional, autonomic, and endocrine effects of MDMA, methylphenidate, and modafinil in healthy subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2018;235(2):467–479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Hysek CM, Domes G, Liechti ME.. MDMA enhances “mind reading” of positive emotions and impairs “mind reading” of negative emotions. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012;222(2):293–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Kuypers KP, Dolder PC, Ramaekers JG, Liechti ME.. Multifaceted empathy of healthy volunteers after single doses of MDMA: A pooled sample of placebo-controlled studies. J Psychopharmacol. 2017;31(5):589–598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Kuypers KP, de la Torre R, Farre M, et al. No evidence that MDMA-induced enhancement of emotional empathy is related to peripheral oxytocin levels or 5-HT1a receptor activation. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e100719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Bershad AK, Mayo LM, Van Hedger K, McGlone F, Walker SC, de Wit H.. Effects of MDMA on attention to positive social cues and pleasantness of affective touch. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44(10):1698–1705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Morrison I. Touching to connect, explore, and explain: How the human brain makes social touch meaningful. Senses Soc. 2023;18:1–18. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Field T. Touch for socioemotional and physical well-being: A review. Dev Rev. 2010;30(4):367–383. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Jablonski NG. Social and affective touch in primates and its role in the evolution of social cohesion. Neuroscience. 2021;464:117–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Annie Mithoefer B, Jerome L, Ruse J, Doblin R, Gibson E, Marcela Ot’alora G.. A manual for MDMA-assisted psychotherapy in the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder: Citeseer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 32. Gabay AS, Kempton MJ, Gilleen J, Mehta MA.. MDMA increases cooperation and recruitment of social brain areas when playing trustworthy players in an iterated Prisoner’s Dilemma. J Neurosci. 2019;39(2):307–320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Kirkpatrick M, Delton AW, Robertson TE, de Wit H.. Prosocial effects of MDMA: A measure of generosity. J Psychopharmacol. 2015;29(6):661–668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT.. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(4):231–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Marrone GF, Pardo JS, Krauss RM, Hart CL.. Amphetamine analogs methamphetamine and 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) differentially affect speech. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2010;208(2):169–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Ward AS, Kelly TH, Foltin RW, Fischman MW.. Effects of d-amphetamine on task performance and social behavior of humans in a residential laboratory. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1997;5(2):130–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Griffiths RR, Stitzer M, Corker K, Bigelow G, Liebson I.. Drug-produced changes in human social behavior: Facilitation by d-amphetamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1977;7(4):365–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Baggott MJ, Kirkpatrick MG, Bedi G, de Wit H.. Intimate insight: MDMA changes how people talk about significant others. J Psychopharmacol. 2015;29(6):669–677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Bedi G, Cecchi GA, Slezak DF, Carrillo F, Sigman M, De Wit H.. A window into the intoxicated mind? Speech as an index of psychoactive drug effects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(10):2340–2348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Agurto C, Cecchi GA, Norel R, et al. Detection of acute 3, 4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) effects across protocols using automated natural language processing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(5):823–832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Bedi G, Phan KL, Angstadt M, De Wit H.. Effects of MDMA on sociability and neural response to social threat and social reward. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009;207(1):73–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Doss MK, Weafer J, Gallo DA, de Wit H.. MDMA impairs both the encoding and retrieval of emotional recollections. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018;43(4):791–800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Danforth AL, Grob CS, Struble C, et al. Reduction in social anxiety after MDMA-assisted psychotherapy with autistic adults: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled pilot study. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2018;235(11):3137–3148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Luoma JB, Shahar B, Kati Lear M, Pilecki B, Wagner A.. Potential processes of change in MDMA‐Assisted therapy for social anxiety disorder: Enhanced memory reconsolidation, self‐transcendence, and therapeutic relationships. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 2022;37(3):e2824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Kirkpatrick MG, Lee R, Wardle MC, Jacob S, De Wit H.. Effects of MDMA and intranasal oxytocin on social and emotional processing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(7):1654–1663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Hysek CM, Schmid Y, Simmler LD, et al. MDMA enhances emotional empathy and prosocial behavior. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(11):1645–1652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Frye CG, Wardle MC, Norman GJ, de Wit H.. MDMA decreases the effects of simulated social rejection. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2014;117:1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Bershad AK, Miller MA, de Wit H.. MDMA does not alter responses to the Trier Social Stress Test in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2017;234(14):2159–2166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Childs E, Bershad AK, de Wit H.. Effects of d-amphetamine upon psychosocial stress responses. J Psychopharmacol. 2016;30(7):608–615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Trezza V, Baarendse PJ, Vanderschuren LJ.. The pleasures of play: Pharmacological insights into social reward mechanisms. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2010;31(10):463–469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Achterberg E, van Swieten M, Houwing D, Trezza V, Vanderschuren L.. Opioid modulation of social play reward in juvenile rats. Neuropharmacology. 2019;159:107332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Panksepp J, Herman B, Vilberg T, Bishop P, DeEskinazi F.. Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1980;4(4):473–487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Herman BH, Panksepp J.. Effects of morphine and naloxone on separation distress and approach attachment: Evidence for opiate mediation of social affect. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1978;9(2):213–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Wells AM, Ridener E, Bourbonais CA, et al. Effects of chronic social defeat stress on sleep and circadian rhythms are mitigated by kappa-opioid receptor antagonism. J Neurosci. 2017;37(32):7656–7668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Chelnokova O, Laeng B, Eikemo M, et al. Rewards of beauty: The opioid system mediates social motivation in humans. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(7):746–747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Chelnokova O, Laeng B, Løseth G, Eikemo M, Willoch F, Leknes S.. The µ-opioid system promotes visual attention to faces and eyes. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(12):1902–1909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Løseth GE, Eikemo M, Leknes S.. Effects of opioid receptor stimulation and blockade on touch pleasantness: A double-blind randomised trial. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2019;14(4):411–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Bershad AK, Seiden JA, de Wit H.. Effects of buprenorphine on responses to social stimuli in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;63:43–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Syal S, Ipser J, Terburg D, et al. Improved memory for reward cues following acute buprenorphine administration in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;53:10–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Maqueda AE, Valle M, Addy PH, et al. Salvinorin-A induces intense dissociative effects, blocking external sensory perception and modulating interoception and sense of body ownership in humans. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2015;18(12):ppyv065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Pepperberg IM, Shive HR.. Simultaneous development of vocal and physical object combinations by a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus): Bottle caps, lids, and labels. J Comp Psychol. 2001;115(4):376–384. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Kirschbaum C, Pirke K-M, Hellhammer DH.. The “Trier Social Stress Test”–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(1-2):76–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Bershad AK, Jaffe JH, Childs E, de Wit H.. Opioid partial agonist buprenorphine dampens responses to psychosocial stress in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;52:281–288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Bershad AK, Miller MA, Norman GJ, de Wit H.. Effects of opioid-and non-opioid analgesics on responses to psychosocial stress in humans. Horm Behav. 2018;102:41–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Loseth GE, Ellingsen D-M, Leknes S.. State-dependent μ-opioid modulation of social motivation. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Ipser JC, Terburg D, Syal S, et al. Reduced fear-recognition sensitivity following acute buprenorphine administration in healthy volunteers. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(1):166–170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Cameron CM, Nieto S, Bosler L, et al. Mechanisms underlying the anti-suicidal treatment potential of buprenorphine. Adv Drug Alcohol Res. 2021;1:10009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. McLaughlin JP, Li S, Valdez J, Chavkin TA, Chavkin C.. Social defeat stress-induced behavioral responses are mediated by the endogenous kappa opioid system. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(6):1241–1248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Zan G-y, Sun X, Wang Y-j, et al. Amygdala dynorphin/κ opioid receptor system modulates depressive-like behavior in mice following chronic social defeat stress. Acta Pharmacol Sin. 2022;43(3):577–587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Donahue RJ, Landino SM, Golden SA, Carroll FI, Russo SJ, Carlezon WA Jr. Effects of acute and chronic social defeat stress are differentially mediated by the dynorphin/kappa-opioid receptor system. Behav Pharmacol. 2015;26(7 0 0):654–663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Browne CA, Falcon E, Robinson SA, Berton O, Lucki I.. Reversal of stress-induced social interaction deficits by buprenorphine. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;21(2):164–174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Charles SJ, Farias M, van Mulukom V, et al. Blocking mu-opioid receptors inhibits social bonding in rituals. Biol Lett. 2020;16(10):20200485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Tchalova K, MacDonald G.. Opioid receptor blockade inhibits self-disclosure during a closeness-building social interaction. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2020;113:104559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Inagaki TK, Irwin MR, Eisenberger NI.. Blocking opioids attenuates physical warmth-induced feelings of social connection. Emotion. 2015;15(4):494–500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Inagaki TK, Hazlett LI, Andreescu C.. Opioids and social bonding: Effect of naltrexone on feelings of social connection and ventral striatum activity to close others. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2020;149(4):732–745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Volkow ND, Swanson JM, Evins AE, et al. Effects of cannabis use on human behavior, including cognition, motivation, and psychosis: A review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73(3):292–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Kelly TH, Foltin RW, Mayr MT, Fischman MW.. Effects of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol and social context on marijuana self-administration by humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1994;49(3):763–768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Foltin RW, Fischman MW.. Effects of smoked marijuana on human social behavior in small groups. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1988;30(2):539–541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Wardle MC, Pabon E, Webber HE, de Wit H.. Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol reduces willingness to exert effort in women. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2022;239(5):1487–1497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Higgins ST, Stitzer ML.. Acute marijuana effects on social conversation. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1986;89:234–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Skumlien M, Mokrysz C, Freeman TP, et al. Anhedonia, apathy, pleasure, and effort-based decision-making in adult and adolescent cannabis users and controls. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2023;26(1):9–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Metrik J, Aston ER, Kahler CW, Rohsenow DJ, McGeary JE, Knopik VS.. Marijuana’s acute effects on cognitive bias for affective and marijuana cues. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2015;23(5):339–350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Foltin R, Brady J, Fischman M, Emurian C, Dominitz J.. Effects of smoked marijuana on social interaction in small groups. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1987;20(1):87–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Rachlinski JJ, Foltin RW, Fischman MW.. The effects of smoked marijuana on interpersonal distances in small groups. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1989;24(3):183–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Santacana A, Pérez-Solá V.. Reasons and subjective effects of cannabis use among people with psychotic disorders: A systematic review. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2014;42(2):83–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Sharpe L, Sinclair J, Kramer A, de Manincor M, Sarris J.. Cannabis, a cause for anxiety? A critical appraisal of the anxiogenic and anxiolytic properties. J Transl Med. 2020;18:1–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Childs E, Lutz JA, de Wit H.. Dose-related effects of delta-9-THC on emotional responses to acute psychosocial stress. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2017;177:136–144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Phan KL, Angstadt M, Golden J, Onyewuenyi I, Popovska A, de Wit H.. Cannabinoid modulation of amygdala reactivity to social signals of threat in humans. J Neurosci. 2008;28(10):2313–2319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Bhattacharyya S, Morrison PD, Fusar-Poli P, et al. Opposite effects of Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on human brain function and psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35(3):764–774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Fusar-Poli P, Crippa JA, Bhattacharyya S, et al. Distinct effects of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on neural activation during emotional processing. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(1):95–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Ballard ME, Bedi G, de Wit H.. Effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol on evaluation of emotional images. J Psychopharmacol. 2012;26(10):1289–1298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Hindley G, Beck K, Borgan F, et al. Psychiatric symptoms caused by cannabis constituents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7(4):344–353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Blessing EM, Steenkamp MM, Manzanares J, Marmar CR.. Cannabidiol as a potential treatment for anxiety disorders. Neurotherapeutics. 2015;12(4):825–836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Bergamaschi MM, Queiroz RHC, Chagas MHN, et al. Cannabidiol reduces the anxiety induced by simulated public speaking in treatment-naive social phobia patients. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(6):1219–1226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Britch SC, Babalonis S, Walsh SL.. Cannabidiol: Pharmacology and therapeutic targets. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2021;238:9–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Spinella TC, Stewart SH, Naugler J, Yakovenko I, Barrett SP.. Evaluating cannabidiol (CBD) expectancy effects on acute stress and anxiety in healthy adults: A randomized crossover study. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2021;238:1965–1977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Kuypers KP, Ng L, Erritzoe D, et al. Microdosing psychedelics: More questions than answers? An overview and suggestions for future research. J Psychopharmacol. 2019;33(9):1039–1057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Bershad AK, Schepers ST, Bremmer MP, Lee R, de Wit H.. Acute subjective and behavioral effects of microdoses of lysergic acid diethylamide in healthy human volunteers. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86(10):792–800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. de Wit H, Molla HM, Bershad A, Bremmer M, Lee R.. Repeated low doses of LSD in healthy adults: A placebo‐controlled, dose–response study. Addict Biol. 2022;27(2):e13143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Vollenweider FX, Preller KH.. Psychedelic drugs: Neurobiology and potential for treatment of psychiatric disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2020;21(11):611–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Halberstadt AL, Geyer MA.. Serotonergic hallucinogens as translational models relevant to schizophrenia. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013;16(10):2165–2180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Wolf G, Singh S, Blakolmer K, et al. Could psychedelic drugs have a role in the treatment of schizophrenia? Rationale and strategy for safe implementation. Mol Psychiatry. 2023;28(1):44–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Dos Santos RG, de Lima Osório F, Rocha JM, et al. Ayahuasca improves self-perception of speech performance in subjects with social anxiety disorder: A pilot, proof-of-concept, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2021;41(5):540–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Palhano-Fontes F, Barreto D, Onias H, et al. Rapid antidepressant effects of the psychedelic ayahuasca in treatment-resistant depression: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychol Med. 2019;49(4):655–663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Carhart-Harris R, Giribaldi B, Watts R, et al. Trial of psilocybin versus escitalopram for depression. N Engl J Med. 2021;384(15):1402–1411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Goodwin GM, Aaronson ST, Alvarez O, et al. Single-dose psilocybin for a treatment-resistant episode of major depression. N Engl J Med. 2022;387(18):1637–1648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107. Krediet E, Bostoen T, Breeksema J, van Schagen A, Passie T, Vermetten E.. Reviewing the potential of psychedelics for the treatment of PTSD. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;23(6):385–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Harmer CJ. Serotonin and emotional processing: Does it help explain antidepressant drug action? Neuropharmacology. 2008;55(6):1023–1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Harmer CJ, Duman RS, Cowen PJ.. How do antidepressants work? New perspectives for refining future treatment approaches. Lancet Psychiatry. 2017;4(5):409–418. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Harmer C, Bhagwagar Z, Perrett D, Völlm B, Cowen P, Goodwin G.. Acute SSRI administration affects the processing of social cues in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(1):148–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Sabino ADV, Chagas MHN, Osório FL.. Effects of psychotropic drugs used in the treatment of anxiety disorders on the recognition of facial expressions of emotion: Critical analysis of literature. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:802–809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112. Capitão L, Murphy S, Browning M, Cowen P, Harmer C.. Acute fluoxetine modulates emotional processing in young adult volunteers. Psychol Med. 2015;45(11):2295–2308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113. Rawlings NB, Norbury R, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ.. A single dose of mirtazapine modulates neural responses to emotional faces in healthy people. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2010;212:625–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. Godlewska B, Norbury R, Selvaraj S, Cowen P, Harmer C.. Short-term SSRI treatment normalises amygdala hyperactivity in depressed patients. Psychol Med. 2012;42(12):2609–2617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Norbury R, Taylor MJ, Selvaraj S, Murphy SE, Harmer CJ, Cowen PJ.. Short-term antidepressant treatment modulates amygdala response to happy faces. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009;206:197–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Singh SP, Singh V, Kar N, Chan K.. Efficacy of antidepressants in treating the negative symptoms of chronic schizophrenia: Meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2010;197(3):174–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117. Rummel C, Kissling W, Leucht S.. Antidepressants as add-on treatment to antipsychotics for people with schizophrenia and pronounced negative symptoms: A systematic review of randomized trials. Schizophr Res. 2005;80(1):85–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118. Young C, Butcher R.. Propranolol for post-traumatic stress disorder: A review of clinical effectiveness. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Agency for Drugs and Technologies in Health; 2020. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. MacCormack JK, Armstrong-Carter E, Gaudier-Diaz MM, et al. Beta-adrenergic contributions to emotion and physiology during an acute psychosocial stressor. Psychosom Med. 2021;83(9):959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Hurlemann R, Walter H, Rehme A, et al. Human amygdala reactivity is diminished by the β-noradrenergic antagonist propranolol. Psychol Med. 2010;40(11):1839–1848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Mihov Y, Mayer S, Musshoff F, Maier W, Kendrick KM, Hurlemann R.. Facilitation of learning by social-emotional feedback in humans is beta-noradrenergic-dependent. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(10):3168–3172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Murphy S, Downham C, Cowen P, Harmer C.. Direct effects of diazepam on emotional processing in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2008;199:503–513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Kamboj S, Curran H.. Scopolamine induces impairments in the recognition of human facial expressions of anger and disgust. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2006;185:529–535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Bahr L-M, Maurer F, Weigl J, et al. Dissociation of endocrine responses to the Trier Social Stress Test in Virtual Reality (VR-TSST) by the benzodiazepine alprazolam and the translocator protein 18 kDa (TSPO) ligand etifoxine. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2021;124:105100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Fries E, Hellhammer DH, Hellhammer J.. Attenuation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis responsivity to the Trier Social Stress Test by the benzodiazepine alprazolam. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31(10):1278–1288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. Zuardi AW, Cosme R, Graeff FG, Guimarães F.. Effects of ipsapirone and cannabidiol on human experimental anxiety. J Psychopharmacol. 1993;7(1_suppl):82–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127. Haime Z, Watson AJ, Crellin N, Marston L, Joyce E, Moncrieff J.. A systematic review of the effects of psychiatric medications on social cognition. BMC Psychiatry. 2021;21(1):1–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128. Paulus MP, Feinstein JS, Castillo G, Simmons AN, Stein MB.. Dose-dependent decrease of activation in bilateral amygdala and insula by lorazepam during emotion processing. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(3):282–288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129. Gordon I, Martin C, Feldman R, Leckman JF.. Oxytocin and social motivation. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2011;1(4):471–493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130. Cohen D, Shamay-Tsoory SG.. Oxytocin regulates social approach. Social Neurosci. 2018;13(6):680–687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131. Bartz JA, Zaki J, Bolger N, Ochsner KN.. Social effects of oxytocin in humans: Context and person matter. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(7):301–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132. Fang A, Treadway MT, Hofmann SG.. Working hard for oneself or others: Effects of oxytocin on reward motivation in social anxiety disorder. Biol Psychol. 2017;127:157–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133. Eckstein M, Scheele D, Weber K, Stoffel‐Wagner B, Maier W, Hurlemann R.. Oxytocin facilitates the sensation of social stress. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(9):4741–4750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134. Beery AK. Antisocial oxytocin: Complex effects on social behavior. Curr Opinion Behav Sci. 2015;6:174–182. [Google Scholar]
- 135. Lieberz J, Scheele D, Spengler FB, et al. Kinetics of oxytocin effects on amygdala and striatal reactivity vary between women and men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45(7):1134–1140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Abu-Akel A.. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79(3):194–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137. Cardoso C, Orlando MA, Brown CA, Ellenbogen MA.. Oxytocin and enhancement of the positive valence of social affiliation memories: an autobiographical memory study. Social Neurosci. 2014;9(2):186–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138. Domes G, Steiner A, Porges SW, Heinrichs M.. Oxytocin differentially modulates eye gaze to naturalistic social signals of happiness and anger. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2013;38(7):1198–1202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139. Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Dadds MR.. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(1):3–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140. Le J, Kou J, Zhao W, et al. Oxytocin facilitation of emotional empathy is associated with increased eye gaze toward the faces of individuals in emotional contexts. Front Neurosci. 2020;14:803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141. Stavropoulos KK, Carver LJ.. Research review: Social motivation and oxytocin in autism–implications for joint attention development and intervention. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(6):603–618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142. Gordon I, Jack A, Pretzsch CM, et al. Intranasal oxytocin enhances connectivity in the neural circuitry supporting social motivation and social perception in children with autism. Sci Rep. 2016;6(1):1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143. Spengler FB, Schultz J, Scheele D, et al. Kinetics and dose dependency of intranasal oxytocin effects on amygdala reactivity. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82(12):885–894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144. Zheng W, Zhu X-M, Zhang Q-E, et al. Adjunctive intranasal oxytocin for schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials. Schizophr Res. 2019;206:13–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145. Bradley ER, Seitz A, Niles AN, et al. Oxytocin increases eye gaze in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2019;212:177–185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146. Bradley ER, Brustkern J, De Coster L, et al. Victory is its own reward: Oxytocin increases costly competitive behavior in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2020;50(4):674–682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147. Fulford D, Treadway M, Woolley J.. Social motivation in schizophrenia: The impact of oxytocin on vigor in the context of social and nonsocial reinforcement. J Abnorm Psychol. 2018;127(1):116–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148. Muthukumaraswamy SD, Johnson BW, McNair NA.. Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. Cogn Brain Res. 2004;19(2):195–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149. Wynn JK, Green MF, Hellemann G, Reavis EA, Marder SR.. A dose-finding study of oxytocin using neurophysiological measures of social processing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44(2):289–294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150. Gibson CM, Penn DL, Smedley KL, Leserman J, Elliott T, Pedersen CA.. A pilot six-week randomized controlled trial of oxytocin on social cognition and social skills in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2014;156(2-3):261–265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151. Jarskog LF, Pedersen CA, Johnson JL, et al. A 12-week randomized controlled trial of twice-daily intranasal oxytocin for social cognitive deficits in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2017;185:88–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152. Halverson T, Jarskog LF, Pedersen C, Penn D.. Effects of oxytocin on empathy, introspective accuracy, and social symptoms in schizophrenia: A 12-week twice-daily randomized controlled trial. Schizophr Res. 2019;204:178–182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153. Davis MC, Green MF, Lee J, et al. Oxytocin-augmented social cognitive skills training in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(9):2070–2077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154. Strauss GP, Granholm E, Holden JL, et al. The effects of combined oxytocin and cognitive behavioral social skills training on social cognition in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2019;49(10):1731–1739. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155. Cacciotti-Saija C, Langdon R, Ward PB, et al. A double-blind randomized controlled trial of oxytocin nasal spray and social cognition training for young people with early psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(2):483–493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156. Buchanan RW, Kelly DL, Strauss GP, et al. Combined oxytocin and CBSST for social function in people with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2021;41(3):236–243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157. Insel TR. Translating oxytocin neuroscience to the clinic: A national institute of mental health perspective. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79(3):153–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158. Leng G, Ludwig M.. Intranasal oxytocin: Myths and delusions. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;79(3):243–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159. Tabak BA, Leng G, Szeto A, et al. Advances in human oxytocin measurement: Challenges and proposed solutions. Mol Psychiatry. 2023;28(1):127–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160. Williams DR, Buerkner P-C.. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on symptoms of schizophrenia: A multivariate Bayesian meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;75:141–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161. Kou J, Lan C, Zhang Y, et al. In the nose or on the tongue? Contrasting motivational effects of oral and intranasal oxytocin on arousal and reward during social processing. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11(1):94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162. Parker KJ, Oztan O, Libove RA, et al. A randomized placebo-controlled pilot trial shows that intranasal vasopressin improves social deficits in children with autism. Sci Transl Med. 2019;11(491):eaau7356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163. Purushothaman D, Jacob A, Kumar V, Varambally S, Venkatasubramanian G, Rao NP.. “To co-operate or not?” Risky co-operative behavior in Schizophrenia and the effect of vasopressin. Schizophr Res. 2020;222:153–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164. Marx CE, Bradford DW, Hamer RM, et al. Pregnenolone as a novel therapeutic candidate in schizophrenia: Emerging preclinical and clinical evidence. Neuroscience. 2011;191:78–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165. Kulkarni J, Gavrilidis E, Wang W, et al. Estradiol for treatment-resistant schizophrenia: A large-scale randomized-controlled trial in women of child-bearing age. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20(6):695–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166. Steinman MQ, Duque-Wilckens N, Trainor BC.. Complementary neural circuits for divergent effects of oxytocin: Social approach versus social anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;85(10):792–801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167. Heinrichs M, Baumgartner T, Kirschbaum C, Ehlert U.. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54(12):1389–1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E.. Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature. 2005;435(7042):673–676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169. Shin NY, Park HY, Jung WH, et al. Effects of oxytocin on neural response to facial expressions in patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(8):1919–1927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M.. Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: Social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(9):524–538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171. Barch DM. The CAINS: Theoretical and practical advances in the assessment of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(2):133–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]