Abstract
Emerging evidence indicates that the predictability of signals early in life may influence the developing brain. This study examines links between a novel indicator of maternal mood dysregulation, mood entropy, and child neurodevelopmental outcomes. Associations between prenatal maternal mood entropy and child neurodevelopment were assessed in two longitudinal cohorts. Maternal mood was measured several times over pregnancy beginning as early as 15 weeks’ gestation. Shannon’s entropy was applied distributions of mothers’ responses on mood questionnaires. Child cognitive and language development were evaluated at 2 and 6–9 years of age. Higher prenatal maternal mood entropy was associated with lower cognitive development scores at 2 years of age and lower expressive language scores at 6-9 years of age. These associations persisted after adjusting for maternal pre and postnatal mood levels and for other relevant sociodemographic factors. Our findings identify maternal mood entropy as a novel predictor of child neurodevelopment. Characterizing components of maternal mood in addition to level of severity or valence may further our understanding of specific processes by which maternal mood shapes child development.
Keywords: prenatal, unpredictability, maternal, mood, child development
The effects of maternal mood and psychopathology on the developing child are well-documented (Goodman et al., 2011; Kingston, Tough, & Whitfield, 2012; Martins & Gaffan, 2000). Accumulating evidence indicates that the prenatal period in particular is a sensitive window during which maternal mood states may shape the developing fetal brain and exert persisting influences on child emotional and cognitive function (Dunkel Schetter & Tanner, 2012; O’Connor, Monk, & Fitelson, 2014; Sandman, Glynn, & Davis, 2016; Talge, Neal, & Glover, 2007). Prenatal maternal negative mood (depression, anxiety, pregnancy-specific anxiety) predicts child cognitive and language outcomes from infancy (Davis & Sandman, 2010; Huizink, Robles De Medina, Mulder, Visser, & Buitelaar, 2003; Tran et al., 2013) and toddlerhood (Brouwers, Van Baar, & Pop, 2001; Ibanez et al., 2015; Koutra et al., 2013; Lin et al., 2017; Tse, Rich-Edwards, Rifas-Shiman, Gillman, & Oken, 2010) into childhood (Buss, Davis, Hobel, & Sandman, 2011; Evans et al., 2012; Loomans et al., 2012) and adolescence (Mennes, Stiers, Lagae, & Van den Bergh, 2006; Van Den Bergh et al., 2005). Complementary to cognitive and language outcome findings are studies linking prenatal maternal negative mood states with structural alterations in brain regions governing these functions (Adamson, Letourneau, & Lebel, 2018; Buss, Davis, Muftuler, Head, & Sandman, 2010; Lebel et al., 2016; Qiu et al., 2013; Sandman, Buss, Head, & Davis, 2015). Cumulatively, these results suggest that maternal negative mood states during pregnancy have lasting implications for child neurodevelopment.
In addition to valence (e.g., positive or negative) or levels (e.g., severity) of maternal mood states, other indicators of maternal mood dysregulation may shape fetal and child neurodevelopment and predict cognitive and language outcomes. A broader literature indicates that intraindividual patterns in mood (e.g., its instability, variability) are quantifiable and meaningful components of mood (Ebner-Priemer, Eid, Kleindienst, Stabenow, & Trull, 2009; Eid & Diener, 1999; Jahng, Wood, & Trull, 2008; Thompson, Dizén, & Berenbaum, 2009), emotion regulation (Cole, Michel, & Teti, 1994; Thompson, 1994), and mental health (Broome, Saunders, Harrison, & Marwaha, 2015; Fernandez, Jazaieri, & Gross, 2016; Houben, Van Den Noortgate, & Kuppens, 2015; Koenigsberg, 2010). These aspects of mood dysregulation have been largely ignored when examining associations between maternal mood and fetal and child development.
Mood dynamics have been characterized through examination of patterning across time (e.g., across days or weeks); across situations; and at a single point in time, capturing a snapshot of emotional experience (Ed & Diener, 1999; Kuppens & Verduyn, 2015; Penner, Shiffman, Paty, & Fritzsche, 1994). Recently, we identified a novel indicator of mood dysregulation using a measure of normalized entropy (Glynn, Howland, Sandman, et al., 2018). Shannon’s entropy is used to measure the randomness or unpredictability of the probability distribution of a random variable (Cover & Thomas, 2006); we applied it to the distribution of item responses on maternal mood questionnaires. In two independent, prospective, longitudinal cohorts, we found that elevated prenatal maternal mood entropy was associated with higher levels of child negative affectivity across childhood and higher levels of depressive and anxiety symptoms in early adolescence. Importantly, the associations were independent of maternal mood level.
The current investigation expanded upon Glynn et al. (2018) to examine associations between prenatal maternal mood entropy and child neurodevelopmental outcomes at 2 and 6-9 years of age in the same independent, prospective, longitudinal cohorts of women and their children. We hypothesized that elevated prenatal maternal mood entropy would be associated with impaired cognitive and language development and that these associations would not be explained by the well-established effects of maternal mood level. We expected to replicate established effects of maternal negative mood level, hypothesizing that maternal negative mood levels would be associated with lower cognitive and language development scores.
Methods
Study Overview
Women recruited during the first trimester of pregnancy completed three to five prenatal laboratory visits and participated with their children in postnatal visits at 2 years and/or 6-9 years of age (see Figure 1 for data collection protocol). Maternal psychological distress was evaluated at each pre and postnatal assessment with reliable and valid self-report indices, from which entropy measures were computed. Child neurodevelopment was evaluated at each postnatal visit using standardized, developmentally appropriate assessments of verbal and non-verbal cognition. The study protocol was approved by the institutional IRB, and written informed consent was obtained from the mothers and assent from the children beginning at age 7.
Figure 1.
Description of study protocol for the two cohorts (total N = 299). CESD = Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale– Short Form. STAI = State Trait Anxiety Inventory. PSS = Perceived Stress Scale. PANX = Pregnancy Specific Anxiety Scale. BDI = Beck Depression Inventory. BSID-II = Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition. WISC-IV = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition. EVT-2 = Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition. The PSS was not administered at 25 weeks’ gestation in Cohort 2.
Participants
Pregnant women were initially recruited from affiliated clinics at two large university medical centers in Southern California through separate National Institutes of Health (NIH)-funded projects. Mother-child pairs from both cohorts were then recruited together when the children were 6-9 years of age into a third NIH-funded study (see Supplemental Material for additional details). Inclusion criteria for women at recruitment were: English-speaking, adult (≥ 18 years old) women with intrauterine, singleton pregnancies. Exclusion criteria were: the presence of uterine or cervical abnormalities; conditions such as endocrine, hepatic or renal disorders or use of corticosteroid medication that could dysregulate neuroendocrine function; and abuse of tobacco, alcohol, or recreational drugs in the pregnancy. Mother-child pairs were included in the current study if child neurodevelopmental outcome data were available at 2 years and/or 6-9-years of age (total N = 299). Different numbers of children were assessed at each age. This was based upon the number of children in these longitudinal cohorts who had aged into the assessment window during each of the grant cycles. Demographic characteristics of the mother-child pairs are presented in Table 1. Sample sizes for the three cohorts were based on power analyses conducted for initial NIH grant applications, which were designed to address prenatal influences (including maternal mood) on child development.
Table 1.
Participant characteristics for two cohorts (overall N = 299).
Cohort 1 n = 178 |
Cohort 2 n = 121 |
|
---|---|---|
Maternal age at delivery (mean years) | 30.8 | 29.7 |
Maternal race (%) | ||
Caucasian, non-Hispanic | 49.4 | 47.9 |
Latina | 21.3 | 33.9 |
Asian | 11.8 | 6.6 |
Black | 10.7 | 3.3 |
Multi-ethnic | 6.7 | 8.3 |
Maternal education (%) | ||
High school or less | 16.9 | 13.3 |
Associates or vocational degree | 30.9 | 38.8 |
4-year college degree | 30.3 | 29.8 |
Graduate degree | 19.7 | 18.2 |
Annual household income (mean USD) | 77,669 | 70,103 |
Cohabitation with child’s father (% yes) | 87.1 | 88.4 |
Child sex (% female) | 53.9 | 47.1 |
Child birth order (% first born) | 57.9 | 53.7 |
Length of gestation (mean weeks) | 39.0 | 39.3 |
Child BSID-II MDI scores | - | 97.4 |
Child WISC-IV PRI scores | 108.3 | 108.1 |
Child EVT-2 scores | 106.8 | 107.3 |
Note. Sociodemographic information was collected during the prenatal period. BSID-II = Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition. WISC-IV = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition. EVT-2 = Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition.
Measures
Maternal mood.
Assessments.
Reliable and valid measures of maternal depressive symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory: Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961; Beck, Steer, & Carbin, 1988; Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale-Short Form: Santor & Coyne, 1997), anxiety symptoms (State-Trait Personality Inventory: Spielberger, Jacobs, Crane, & Russell, 1979; Spielberger & Reheiser, 2009), pregnancy-specific anxiety (Dunkel-Schetter & Glynn, 2010; Rini, Dunkel-Schetter, Wadhwa, & Sandman, 1999), and perceived stress (Perceived Stress Scale: Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983; Cohen & Williamson, 1988) were administered.
Mood entropy.
Shannon’s entropy was computed for each mother’s responses on each of the four questionnaires at each assessment. For a single questionnaire at a single point in time the entropy score quantifies the unpredictability or inconsistency of responses across the items on that questionnaire. The responses are tabulated over items into a probability distribution that records the relative frequency of each response choice. The formula for Shannon’s entropy is: Σi Pi log2 Pi, where Pi is the proportion of items for which the i-th response choice was made, log2 indicates the logarithm to the base two, and Σ indicates that the sum is to be taken over all possible response choices. For example, a mother who reports never worried and always relaxed on the state anxiety scale would have a low entropy score, whereas a mother with entirely random responses on the anxiety scale would have a very high entropy score. To apply this approach across the different mood questionnaires, entropy scores were normalized by expressing each as a percentage of its maximum value, with entropy scores ranging from 0 (perfectly consistent or predictable) to 100 (random and unpredictable). Mood entropy scores were calculated using an R (R Core Team, 2019) function which is available at: https://contecenter.uci.edu/measuring-maternal-mood/. Each questionnaire also was scored conventionally as an index of mood level.
Test of discriminant validity.
To address the alternate explanation that our entropy measure reflects a general tendency to report or respond to questionnaire items in an unpredictable or inconsistent manner, rather than a profile that is specific to mood or affect, we calculated entropy in responses on a questionnaire unrelated to mood (parallel to our test of discriminant validity in Glynn et al., 2018). Entropy was computed from a Likert-type scale measuring physical activity during the prenatal period, and its associations with mood entropy and with child neurodevelopmental outcomes were examined.
Characterization of mood entropy: Links to indicators of emotion dysregulation.
We have previously reported that mood entropy is positively associated with an established, momentary measure of mood variability across hours and days assessed with ecological momentary assessment (affective instability using the standard root mean square successive difference; see Glynn, Howland, Sandman, et al., 2018). In the current study, we further examined correlates of our mood entropy measure by assessing its associations with widely-used, validated self-report measures of affective instability, emotional awareness, and emotional clarity. In a third prospective longitudinal study of pregnant women, mood entropy scores were derived exactly as described above from the same four mood questionnaires collected at a laboratory visit at approximately 35 weeks’ gestation. At this same visit, women completed three self-report measures of emotional experience. The 18-item version of the Affective Lability Scale (Look, Flory, Harvey, & Siever, 2010) assesses the degree to which individuals report that they tend to shift from normal mood states to states of anger, depression, elation, and anxiety, as well as their tendency to oscillate between different mood states. To assess emotional awareness and emotional clarity, we administered the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994) as well as and the 36-item Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (Gratz & Roemer, 2004), which includes lack of emotional awareness (6 items) and lack emotional clarity (5 items) subscales.
Child neurodevelopmental outcomes.
Assessment at 2 years of age.
At 2 years of age, the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition (BSID-II; Bayley, 1993) was administered. The composite Mental Development Index (MDI) assesses a combination of early cognitive (sensory perception, knowledge, memory, problem solving) and receptive and expressive language abilities. The BSID is one of the most widely used measures of infant development, both for research and clinical applications (Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). Examiners in the current study were trained by a clinician with more than 15 years of experience with BSID administration and were directly supervised by a clinical psychologist. An independent observer reviewed 20% of videotaped assessments, with an interrater reliability of 93%.
We acknowledge that the BSID has demonstrated limited predictive validity for later child cognitive outcomes, specifically among high-risk infants (Hack et al., 2005). However, in our sample, BSID-II MDI scores were associated with 6-9-year non-verbal cognitive ability (Perceptual Reasoning Index of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition; r (70) = .24, p = .04) and expressive language (Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition; r (70) = .50, p < .001) scores.
Assessment at 6-9 years of age.
At the 6-9-year assessment, children were administered the Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI) of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition (WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003). The PRI measures perceptual and fluid reasoning, visual-motor and visual-spatial skills and comprises three subtests (Block Design, Picture Concepts, and Matrix Reasoning). The PRI is relatively language free, and two of the subscales (Block Design and Matrix Reasoning) have been designated as good indicators of general intelligence (Groth-Marnat, 2009).
Children also completed the Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition (EVT-2; Williams, 2007), which assesses expressive vocabulary and word retrieval of the spoken word in standard American English. Parts of speech assessed include nouns, verbs, and attributes. The measure has demonstrated good validity, internal consistency, and stability in children ages 6 – 10 years of age (Williams, 2007).
Maternal cognitive ability.
At the child’s 6-9-year assessment, mothers were administered the Perceptual Organization Index (POI) of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1997). The three subtests comprising the POI are parallel to the WISC-IV PRI subscales administered to children. This index is similarly considered to be language and culture free and a valid indicator of general intelligence (Groth-Marnat, 2009).
Statistical Analyses
At 2 years, 112 children provided BSID-II MDI scores. At 6-9 years, 259 children had neurodevelopmental outcome data; WISC-IV PRI scores were missing for 2 children, and 1 child was missing an EVT-2 score. All mothers had mood entropy scores, and no data were missing for covariates considered in final analyses.
Mood entropy scores were relatively stable across gestation (rs = .32 - .61; see Supplemental Table 1 and Supplemental Figure 1), so entropy scores were averaged within each assessment across gestation (e.g., CESD entropy scores from each of the 5 timepoints were averaged to create a prenatal CESD entropy score). The four prenatal mood entropy measures were moderately associated (see Supplemental Table 2), and there were no reliable differences in their patterns of associations with child neurodevelopmental outcomes (see Supplemental Table 3). Also, gestational timing did not moderate associations between maternal mood entropy and child outcomes (see Supplemental Table 4). Therefore, the four scores were averaged to create a single prenatal mood entropy index. Similar composite variables also were created for prenatal maternal mood level and for maternal mood entropy and mood level at the time of the child assessments.
Bivariate correlations were performed to assess relations between prenatal mood entropy and child neurodevelopment. Child neurodevelopmental outcomes that were associated with prenatal mood entropy were then treated as dependent variables in multiple regression models to assess whether associations held when adjusting for other potential confounds. Covariates were selected based on the literature and included: gestational age at birth, child sex, birth order, maternal age, socioeconomic status (composite of standardized prenatal annual household income and maternal years of education), maternal self-identified race/ethnicity, cohabitation with the child's father during pregnancy, maternal cognitive ability and study cohort. Only variables associated with both the predictor and the outcome at p < .10 were included as covariates in the multiple regression models because these are the variables most likely to impact associations between mood entropy and the outcomes (see Supplemental Table 5 for zero-order correlations between these study variables). Those that met criteria for inclusion were: maternal age, socioeconomic status, cohabitation with the child’s father, and maternal cognitive ability. Because prenatal maternal mood entropy scores were correlated with prenatal maternal mood levels (see Supplemental Table 2), and because pre and postnatal maternal mood levels are established predictors of child neurodevelopment, both prenatal and concurrent postnatal maternal mood levels were included in the models. Additionally, to test if maternal mood entropy effects are specific to the prenatal period, we included mood entropy assessed concurrently at the time of child assessment in each model. Finally, because sex differences have been reported for fetal exposures to stress and adversity (Sandman, Glynn, & Davis, 2013; Sutherland & Brunwasser, 2018), exploratory analyses tested the addition of child sex and its interaction with prenatal maternal mood entropy in the models. All analyses were conducted using SPSS (v. 24.0).
Results
Maternal Mood Entropy
Paired samples t-tests indicated that maternal mood entropy scores were higher during the prenatal period (M = 55.97, SD = 12.32) compared with values at 2 years (M = 48.65, SD = 18.29) and 6-9 years (M = 47.88, SD = 16.09) postnatally, t (112) = 2.83, p = .01 and t (260) = 8.92, p < .001, respectively. Prenatal maternal mood entropy was associated with mood entropy at both postnatal time points, r (110) = .54 and r (259) = .42, ps < .001.
Prenatal Maternal Mood Level and Child Neurodevelopment
Consistent with previous findings in the literature, higher levels of prenatal maternal negative mood were associated with lower cognitive development (BSID-II MDI) scores at 2 years of age, r (110) = −.31, p = .001, and lower expressive language (EVT- 2) scores at 6-9 years of age, r (256) = −.15, p = .02. Prenatal maternal mood level was not associated with children’s non-verbal cognitive development (WISC-IV PRI) scores at 6-9 years of age, r (255) = −.04, p = .52.
Prenatal Maternal Mood Entropy and Child Neurodevelopment
Higher prenatal maternal mood entropy was associated with lower cognitive development (BSID-II MDI) scores at 2 years of age, r (110) = −.37, p < .001, and lower expressive language (EVT-2) scores at 6-9 years of age, r (256) = −.21, p = .001 (Figure 2). Prenatal maternal mood entropy was not associated with non-verbal cognitive (WISC-IV PRI) scores at 6-9 years of age, r (255) = −.07, p = .26.
Figure 2.
Associations between prenatal maternal mood entropy and child neurodevelopmental outcomes at 2 and 6-9 years of age.
In multiple regression models, prenatal maternal mood entropy was associated with children’s cognitive development scores at 2 years of age, B = −0.34, β = −.27, p = .03, and children’s expressive language scores at 6-9 years of age, B = −0.18, β = −.16, p = .04, after adjusting for relevant sociodemographic variables (maternal age, socioeconomic status, cohabitation with child’s father during pregnancy), maternal cognitive ability, pre and postnatal maternal mood levels, and concurrent postnatal maternal mood entropy (see Table 2).
Table 2.
Multiple regression models predicting child neurodevelopmental outcomes.
Predictors | DV: Child cognitive development at 2 years of age (N = 112) |
DV: Child expressive language at 6-9 years of age (N = 258) |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B (SE) | 95% CI | β | B (SE) | 95% CI | β | |
Maternal age | −0.42 (0.34) | [−1.10, 0.25] | −.13 | 0.05 (0.14) | [−0.23, 0.33] | .02 |
SES | 5.78* (2.54) | [0.75, 10.81] | .29 | 5.50*** (1.09) | [3.35, 7.65] | .36 |
Cohabitation with child’s father | 2.01 (4.90) | [−7.71, 11.73] | .04 | 0.15 (2.23) | [−4.25, 4.54] | .00 |
Maternal cognitive ability | 0.30** (0.10) | [0.10, 0.49] | .31 | 0.19*** (0.05) | [0.10, 0.28] | .24 |
Prenatal maternal mood level | 2.12 (3.46) | [−4.75, 8.98] | .09 | 2.16 (1.39) | [−0.57, 4.90] | .13 |
Concurrent maternal mood level | 0.40 (3.17) | [−5.88, 6.68] | .02 | −1.06 (1.13) | [−3.29, 1.18] | −.07 |
Concurrent maternal mood entropy | 0.02 (0.13) | [−0.24, 0.28] | .02 | 0.02 (0.06) | [−0.10, 0.15] | .03 |
Prenatal maternal mood entropy | −0.34* (0.15) | [−0.64, −0.03] | −.27 | −0.18* (0.08) | [−0.34, −0.01] | −.16 |
Total R2 | .33*** | .31*** |
p < .05
p < .01
p < .001
When child sex (1 = female) and its interaction with prenatal maternal mood entropy were included in the models, the results provided some evidence that the slope relating maternal mood entropy to children’s cognitive development scores at 2 years of age differed by child sex, B = −0.37, β = .24, p = .08. Simple slope analyses indicated that higher prenatal maternal mood entropy was associated with lower child cognitive development scores at 2 years in girls, slope = −0.47, t = −2.80, p = .01, but not in boys, slope = −0.10, t = −0.54, p = .59. There was no evidence for a difference in slope for children’s expressive language scores at 6-9 years of age, B = −0.03, β = −.02, p = .83 for the interaction term.
Test of Discriminant Validity
Prenatal maternal mood entropy was not associated with entropy in maternal responses to the prenatal physical activity questionnaire, r = .05, p = .38. Further, when entropy in responses on the physical activity questionnaire replaced prenatal maternal mood entropy in the same regression models described above, it was not predictive of child cognitive development at 2 years of age or expressive language development at 6-9 years of age, ps > .30.
Characterization of Mood Entropy: Links to Indicators of Mood Dysregulation
Mood entropy scores were positively associated with self-reported levels of affective lability (r = .49, p < .001), alexithymia (r = .44, p = .001), lack of emotional awareness (r = .38, p = .004), and lack of emotional clarity (r = .33, p = .01). See Supplemental Table 6 and Figure 2 for further details.
Discussion
Maternal mood entropy was identified as a novel predictor of child neurodevelopmental outcomes at 2 and 6-9 years of age. Importantly, prenatal maternal mood entropy was negatively associated with child neurodevelopmental outcomes after accounting for the well-established effects of pre and postnatal mood levels. In concert with our previous work (Glynn, Howland, Sandman, et al., 2018; Sandman, Davis, & Glynn, 2012), these results support the premise that entropy in maternal mood may have persisting effects on offspring development that are independent of other established influences. Furthermore, results indicate that considering components of maternal mood dysregulation such as mood entropy can enhance our understanding of the specific processes by which maternal signals influence child development.
Fetal exposure to maternal mood entropy was associated with lower scores on assessments of cognitive development at 2 years of age and of expressive language at 6-9 years of age. The EVT-2 assesses of expressive language, and many of the BSID-II MDI items rely heavily upon verbal ability (Bayley, 1993). In contrast, subtests of the WISC-IV PRI assess primarily nonverbal abilities. In our sample, 2-year BSID-II MDI scores were more strongly associated with 6-9-year EVT-2 scores than WISC-IV PRI scores. Maternal mood entropy may have greater effects on verbal cognitive ability compared to non-verbal cognitive ability. However, this possibility requires replication and further exploration in the future.
The potential for child sex to determine these associations also requires additional investigation. That our exploratory analyses provide some evidence that the association between prenatal mood entropy and 2-year child cognitive development scores appears to be present among girls, but not boys is consistent with findings from previous studies documenting sex differences in fetal programming (Sandman et al., 2013; Sutherland & Brunwasser, 2018). However, we did not find a sex difference in the association between maternal mood entropy and 6-9-year child expressive language scores.
Mood entropy was relatively stable over gestation and across measures, consistent with findings demonstrating that related metrics (mood variability and affective instability) may be stable traits (Eid & Diener, 1999; Penner et al., 1994). The current investigation supports our earlier finding of discriminant validity of the mood entropy measure as a predictor of child outcomes (Glynn, Howland, Sandman, et al., 2018) by demonstrating that entropy scores from a scale unrelated to mood did not predict child neurodevelopmental outcomes. This indicates that the associations between maternal mood entropy and child outcomes are not simply reflective of maternal reporting or response style, but instead are specific to mood. We documented previously that mood entropy is positively associated with variability in mood across hours and days collected via ecological momentary assessment (Glynn et al., 2018). In the current study, we also demonstrate that mood entropy scores are associated with higher levels of affective instability and lower levels of emotional awareness and emotional clarity as measured by validated self-report questionnaires. These findings provide further support for mood entropy as an indicator of multiple aspects of mood dysregulation, specifically that women who exhibit higher mood entropy may experience more variation in their emotional experiences (e.g., affective instability) or difficulties identifying or differentiating between emotions (e.g., alexithymia, emotional awareness, emotional clarity). In addition to the possibility that mood entropy indexes several components of mood dysregulation, another strength is that it relies on item response patterns rather than direct self-report of mood dysregulation.
Our findings complement evidence from cross-species research indicating that exposure to unpredictable maternal signals has consequences for neurodevelopmental outcomes, particularly hippocampal-dependent memory tasks and their underlying neural circuitry (Baram et al., 2012; Davis et al., 2017, 2019; Ivy et al., 2010; Molet et al., 2016). Throughout gestation, the fetal brain is rapidly developing, and its primary structures and neural pathways are formed and refined (Fox, Levitt, & Nelson, 2010; Lebel et al., 2016; Stiles, Brown, Haist, & Jernigan, 2015; Tau & Peterson, 2010). It is plausible that predictable patterns of maternal signals have organizing effects, whereas unpredictable patterns have disorganizing effects, on the developing fetal brain. Rodent models demonstrate that patterns of stimulation during early life influence synaptic development and function (Baram et al., 2012; Gunn et al., 2013; Singh-Taylor et al., 2018). For example, exposure to predictable and recurrent patterns of maternal signals results in a reduction of excitatory synapses onto stress-sensitive hypothalamic neurons (Singh-Taylor et al., 2018), but exposure to unpredictable signals results in enhanced excitatory synaptic transmission in these same neurons (Gunn et al., 2013). In humans, exposure to greater entropy of maternal sensory signals during the first year of life is associated with lower child cognitive development at age 2 and with poorer performance on a delayed-recall memory task at age 6 (Davis et al., 2017). Notably, the quantity or quality of the maternal signals do not account for these associations, underscoring the importance of predictability of inputs to the developing brain (Baram et al., 2012; Davis et al., 2017; Molet et al., 2016).
The mechanisms underlying associations between unpredictability of maternal signals and child development in humans are currently unknown. Studies examining possible mechanisms linking prenatal maternal mood levels with fetal and child outcomes have considered neural, endocrine, immune and cardiovascular pathways (Christian, 2012; Coussons-Read, Okun, & Nettles, 2007; Dipietro, 2010; Giesbrecht, Campbell, Letourneau, Kooistra, & Kaplan, 2012; Kane, Dunkel Schetter, Glynn, Hobel, & Sandman, 2014; Teixeira, Fisk, & Glover, 1999), which also may be relevant for mood entropy. One possibility is that maternal mood entropy is associated with vagally mediated heart rate variability (HRV), a physiological marker of emotion regulation (Appelhans & Luecken 2006). Lower HRV is associated with indicators of emotional dysregulation such as higher affectivity instability (Koval et al., 2013) and alexithymia (Lischke et al., 2018; Panayiotou & Constantinou, 2017) in non-pregnant individuals. While these associations have not been assessed in the context of pregnancy, one study has demonstrated that experimentally-induced reductions in HRV modulate maternal-fetal heart rate synchrony (Van Leeuwen et al., 2009). Fetal heart rate is an established indicator of fetal neurodevelopment and central nervous system integrity, and fetal heart rate variability predicts cognitive functioning into postnatal life (DiPietro, Bornstein, Hahn, Costigan, & Achy-Brou, 2007). These studies highlight the potential for prenatal maternal mood entropy-induced changes in HRV to alter fetal neurodevelopment.
Pregnancy is a period of heightened vulnerability for some mood disturbances in the female lifespan, with documented consequences for both mother and developing child (Glynn, Howland, & Fox, 2018; Sandman, Davis, Buss, & Glynn, 2012). To date, interventions focused on preventing or reducing prenatal maternal psychological distress have met with limited success (Fontein-Kuipers, Nieuwenhuijze, Ausems, Budé, & De Vries, 2014). In the current study, we observed that fetal exposure to maternal mood entropy was associated with less optimal child neurodevelopmental outcomes, even after adjusting for maternal mood level. These findings, along with evidence indicating the maladaptive effects of mood instability on mental health outside of the prenatal period (Houben et al., 2015), suggest that maternal mood patterns may be a new target for prenatal intervention. In non-pregnant samples, increased self-reported mindfulness is associated with lower levels of mood variability and instability (Hill & Updegraff, 2012; Keng & Tong, 2016), and meditation has been shown to increase HF-HRV, a potential physiological signature of mood variability or instability (Libby, Worhunsky, Pilver, & Brewer, 2012; Tang et al., 2009). Other interventions may include those aimed at reducing affective instability and lability in mood, such as dialectal behavioral therapy (Stepp, Epler, Jahng, & Trull, 2008). If mood entropy is demonstrated as a modifiable target, it may inform new avenues of prenatal intervention to support mothers and their developing children.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements:
The authors thank the families who participated in these projects. We also thank the dedicated staff at the Early Human and Lifespan Development Program and the Women and Children’s Health and Well-Being project. This research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (HD-28413, HD-40967, HD-51852, NS-41298 and MH-96889).
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors have no conflicts of interest to disclose.
References
- Adamson B, Letourneau N, & Lebel C (2018). Prenatal maternal anxiety and children’s brain structure and function: A systematic review of neuroimaging studies. Journal of Affective Disorders. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Appelhans BM, & Luecken LJ (2006). Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology, 10, 229–240. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.3.229 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bagby RM, Parker JD, & Taylor GJ (1994). The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1), 23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baram TZ, Davis EP, Obenaus A, Sandman CA, Small SL, Solodkin A, & Stern H (2012). Fragmentation and unpredictability of early-life experience in mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayley N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition: Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, & Carbin MG (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, & Erbaugh J (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broome MR, Saunders KEA, Harrison PJ, & Marwaha S (2015). Mood instability: Significance, definition and measurement. British Journal of Psychiatry. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.114.158543 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brouwers EPM, Van Baar AL, & Pop VJM (2001). Maternal anxiety during pregnancy and subsequent infant development. Infant Behavior and Development. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00062-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Buss C, Davis EP, Hobel CJ, & Sandman CA (2011). Maternal pregnancy-specific anxiety is associated with child executive function at 6-9 years age. Stress. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.623250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss C, Davis EP, Muftuler LT, Head K, & Sandman CA (2010). High pregnancy anxiety during mid-gestation is associated with decreased gray matter density in 6-9-year-old children. Psychoneuroendocrinology. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.07.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christian LM (2012). Psychoneuroimmunology in pregnancy: Immune pathways linking stress with maternal health, adverse birth outcomes, and fetal development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.07.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, Kamarck T, & Mermelstein R (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. doi: 10.2307/2136404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, & Williamson GM (1988). Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. The social psychology of health. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1983.tb02325.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cole PM, Michel MK, & Teti LO (1994). The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01278.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coussons-Read ME, Okun ML, & Nettles CD (2007). Psychosocial stress increases inflammatory markers and alters cytokine production across pregnancy. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2006.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cover TM & Thomas JA (2006). Elements of information theory (2nd ed). Hoboken NJ: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Davis EP, Korja R, Karlsson L, Glynn LM, Sandman CA, Vegetabile B, … & Karlsson H. (2019). Across continents and demographics, unpredictable maternal signals are associated with children's cognitive function. EBioMedicine, 46, 256–263. doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2019.07.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis EP, & Sandman CA (2010). The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Development. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01385.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis EP, Stout SA, Molet J, Vegetabile B, Glynn LM, Sandman CA, … Baram TZ (2017). Exposure to unpredictable maternal sensory signals influences cognitive development across species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1703444114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiPietro JA (2010). Maternal influences on the developing fetus. In Maternal Influences on Fetal Neurodevelopment: Clinical and Research Aspects. doi: 10.1007/978-1-60327-921-5_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- DiPietro JA, Bornstein MH, Hahn CS, Costigan K, & Achy-Brou A (2007). Fetal heart rate and variability: Stability and prediction to developmental outcomes in early childhood. Child Development, 78, 1788–1798. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01099.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunkel Schetter CD, & Glynn LM (2011). Stress in pregnancy: Empirical evidence and theoretical issues to guide interdisciplinary research. In The handbook of stress science biology, psychology and health. New York: Springer Publishing Company. [Google Scholar]
- Dunkel Schetter C, & Tanner L (2012). Anxiety, depression and stress in pregnancy: Implications for mothers, children, research, and practice. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503680 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebner-Priemer UW, Eid M, Kleindienst N, Stabenow S, & Trull TJ (2009). Analytic strategies for understanding affective (in)stability and other dynamic processes in psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0014868 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eid M, & Diener E (1999). Intraindividual variability in affect: Reliability, validity, and personality correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.4.662 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Evans J, Melotti R, Heron J, Ramchandani P, Wiles N, Murray L, & Stein A (2012). The timing of maternal depressive symptoms and child cognitive development: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02513.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez KC, Jazaieri H, & Gross JJ (2016). Emotion regulation: A transdiagnostic perspective on a new RDoC domain. Cognitive Therapy and Research. doi: 10.1007/s10608-016-9772-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fontein-Kuipers YJ, Nieuwenhuijze MJ, Ausems M, Budé L, & De Vries R (2014). Antenatal interventions to reduce maternal distress: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised trials. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.12500 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox SE, Levitt P, & Nelson CA (2010). How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Development. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01380.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giesbrecht GF, Campbell T, Letourneau N, Kooistra L, & Kaplan B (2012). Psychological distress and salivary cortisol covary within persons during pregnancy. Psychoneuroendocrinology. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glynn LM, Howland MA, & Fox M (2018). Maternal programming: Application of a developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418000524 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glynn LM, Howland MA, Sandman CA, Davis EP, Phelan M, Baram TZ, & Stern HS (2018). Prenatal maternal mood patterns predict child temperament and adolescent mental health. Journal of Affective Disorders. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glynn LM, Stern HS, Howland MA, Risbrough VB, Baker DG, Nievergelt CM, … Davis EP (2018). Measuring novel antecedents of mental illness: the Questionnaire of Unpredictability in Childhood. Neuropsychopharmacology. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0280-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, & Heyward D (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gratz KL, & Roemer L (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1), 41–54. [Google Scholar]
- Groth-Marnat G. (2009). Handbook of psychological assessment (5th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Gunn BG, Cunningham L, Cooper MA, Corteen NL, Seifi M, Swinny JD, … Belelli D (2013). Dysfunctional astrocytic and synaptic regulation of hypothalamic glutamatergic transmission in a mouse model of early-life adversity: Relevance to neurosteroids and programming of the stress response. Journal of Neuroscience. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1337-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hack M, Taylor HG, Drotar D, Schluchter M, Cartar L, Wilson-Costello D, … & Morrow M (2005). Poor predictive validity of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development for cognitive function of extremely low birth weight children at school age. Pediatrics, 116, 333–341. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-0173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill CLM, & Updegraff JA (2012). Mindfulness and its relationship to emotional regulation. Emotion. doi: 10.1037/a0026355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houben M, Van Den Noortgate W, & Kuppens P (2015). The relation between short-term emotion dynamics and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. doi: 10.1037/a0038822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huizink AC, Robles De Medina PG, Mulder EJH, Visser GHA, & Buitelaar JK (2003). Stress during pregnancy is associated with developmental outcome in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ibanez G, Bernard JY, Rondet C, Peyre H, Forhan A, Kaminski M, & Saurel-Cubizolles MJ (2015). Effects of antenatal maternal depression and anxiety on children’s early cognitive development: A prospective cohort study. PLoS ONE. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0135849 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivy AS, Rex CS, Chen Y, Dube C, Maras PM, Grigoriadis DE, … Baram TZ (2010). Hippocampal dysfunction and cognitive impairments provoked by chronic early-life stress involve excessive activation of CRH receptors. Journal of Neuroscience. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1784-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jahng S, Wood PK, & Trull TJ (2008). Analysis of affective instability in ecological momentary assessment: Indices using successive difference and group comparison via multilevel modeling. Psychological Methods. doi: 10.1037/a0014173 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins BN, Hunter JF, Cross MP, Acevedo AM, & Pressman SD (2018). When is affect variability bad for health? The association between affect variability and immune response to the influenza vaccination. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.11.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane HS, Dunkel Schetter C, Glynn LM, Hobel CJ, & Sandman CA (2014). Pregnancy anxiety and prenatal cortisol trajectories. Biological Psychology. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keng SL, & Tong EMW (2016). Riding the tide of emotions with mindfulness: Mindfulness, affect dynamics, and the mediating role of coping. Emotion. doi: 10.1037/emo0000165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kingston D, Tough S, & Whitfield H (2012). Prenatal and postpartum maternal psychological distress and infant development: A systematic review. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. doi: 10.1007/s10578-012-0291-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koenigsberg HW (2010). Affective instability: Toward an integration of neuroscience and psychological perspectives. Journal of Personality Disorders. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2010.24.1.60 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koutra K, Chatzi L, Bagkeris M, Vassilaki M, Bitsios P, & Kogevinas M (2013). Antenatal and postnatal maternal mental health as determinants of infant neurodevelopment at 18 months of age in a mother-child cohort (Rhea Study) in Crete, Greece. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. doi: 10.1007/s00127-012-0636-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koval P, Ogrinz B, Kuppens P, Van den Bergh O, Tuerlinckx F, & Sütterlin S (2013). Affective instability in daily life is predicted by resting heart rate variability. PloS one, 8, e81536. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuppens P, & Verduyn P (2015). Looking at emotion regulation through the window of emotion dynamics. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 72–79. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2015.960505 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lebel C, Walton M, Letourneau N, Giesbrecht GF, Kaplan BJ, & Dewey D (2016). Prepartum and postpartum maternal depressive symptoms are related to children’s brain structure in preschool. Biological Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Libby DJ, Worhunsky PD, Pilver CE, & Brewer JA (2012). Meditation-induced changes in high-frequency heart rate variability predict smoking outcomes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin Y, Xu J, Huang J, Jia Y, Zhang J, Yan C, & Zhang J (2017). Effects of prenatal and postnatal maternal emotional stress on toddlers’ cognitive and temperamental development. Journal of Affective Disorders. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.09.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lischke A, Pahnke R, Mau-Moeller A, Behrens M, Grabe HJ, Freyberger HJ, … & Weippert M (2018). Inter-individual differences in heart rate variability are associated with inter-individual differences in empathy and alexithymia. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 229. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Look AE, Flory JD, Harvey PD, & Siever LJ (2010). Psychometric properties of a short form of the Affective Lability Scale (ALS-18). Personality and Individual Differences, 49(3), 187–191. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loomans EM, van der Stelt O, van Eijsden M, Gemke RJBJ, Vrijkotte TGM, & Van den Bergh BRH (2012). High levels of antenatal maternal anxiety are associated with altered cognitive control in five-year-old children. Developmental Psychobiology. doi: 10.1002/dev.20606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lotzin A, Schiborr J, Barkmann C, Romer G, & Ramsauer B (2016). Maternal emotion dysregulation is related to heightened mother-infant synchrony of facial affect. Development and Psychopathology. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000516 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins C, & Gaffan EA (2000). Effects of early maternal depression on patterns of infant-mother attachment: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. doi: 10.1017/S0021963099005958 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazursky-Horowitz H, Felton JW, MacPherson L, Ehrlich KB, Cassidy J, Lejuez CW, & Chronis-Tuscano A (2015). Maternal emotion regulation mediates the association between adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and parenting. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9894-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mennes M, Stiers P, Lagae L, & Van den Bergh B (2006). Long-term cognitive sequelae of antenatal maternal anxiety: Involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molet J, Heins K, Zhuo X, Mei YT, Regev L, Baram TZ, & Stern H (2016). Fragmentation and high entropy of neonatal experience predict adolescent emotional outcome. Translational Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molet J, Maras PM, Kinney-Lang E, Harris NG, Rashid F, Ivy AS, … Baram TZ (2016). MRI uncovers disrupted hippocampal microstructure that underlies memory impairments after early-life adversity. Hippocampus. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor TG, Monk C, & Fitelson EM (2014). Practitioner Review: Maternal mood in pregnancy and child development - Implications for child psychology and psychiatry. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panayiotou G, & Constantinou E (2017). Emotion dysregulation in alexithymia: Startle reactivity to fearful affective imagery and its relation to heart rate variability. Psychophysiology, 54, 1323–1334. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12887 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Penner LA, Shiffman S, Paty JA, & Fritzsche BA (1994). Individual differences in intraperson variability in mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.712 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qiu A, Rifkin-Graboi A, Chen H, Chong YS, Kwek K, Gluckman PD, … Meaney MJ (2013). Maternal anxiety and infants’ hippocampal development: Timing matters. Translational Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.79 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL: https://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
- Rini CK, Dunkel-Schetter C, Wadhwa PD, & Sandman CA (1999). Psychological adaptation and birth outcomes: The role of personal resources, stress, and sociocultural context in pregnancy. Health Psychology. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.18.4.333 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandman CA, Buss C, Head K, & Davis EP (2015). Fetal exposure to maternal depressive symptoms is associated with cortical thickness in late childhood. Biological Psychiatry. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandman CA, Davis EP, Buss C, & Glynn LM (2012). Exposure to prenatal psychobiological stress exerts programming influences on the mother and her fetus. Neuroendocrinology. doi: 10.1159/000327017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandman CA, Davis EP, & Glynn LM (2012). Prescient human fetuses thrive. Psychological Science. doi: 10.1177/0956797611422073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandman CA, Glynn LM, & Davis EP (2013). Is there a viability-vulnerability tradeoff? Sex differences in fetal programming. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.07.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sandman CA, Glynn LM, & Davis EP (2016). Neurobehavioral consequences of fetal exposure to gestational stress. In Fetal development: Research on brain and behavior, environmental influences, and emerging technologies. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-22023-9_13 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Santor DA, & Coyne JC (1997). Shortening the CES-D to improve its ability to detect cases of depression. Psychological Assessment. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.9.3.233 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Singh-Taylor A, Molet J, Jiang S, Korosi A, Bolton JL, Noam Y, … Baram TZ (2018). NRSF-dependent epigenetic mechanisms contribute to programming of stress-sensitive neurons by neonatal experience, promoting resilience. Molecular Psychiatry. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Jacobs G, Crane R, & Russell S (1979). Preliminary manual for the state-trait personality inventory (STPI). Unpublished Manuscript. Tampa, FL: University of South Florida. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7402.2009.02409.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, & Reheiser EC (2009). Assessment of emotions: Anxiety, anger, depression, and curiosity. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01017.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stepp SD, Epler AJ, Jahng S, & Trull TJ (2008). The effect of Dialectical Behavior Therapy skills use on borderline personality disorder features. Journal of Personality Disorders. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2008.22.6.549 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stiles J, Brown TT, Haist F, & Jernigan TL (2015). Brain and cognitive development. In Lerner RM (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Strauss E, Sherman EM, & Spreen O (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sutherland S, & Brunwasser SM (2018). Sex differences in vulnerability to prenatal stress: A review of the recent literature. Current Psychiatry Reports, 20, 102. doi: 10.1007/s11920-018-0961-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talge NM, Neal C, & Glover V (2007). Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: How and why? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01714.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang Y-Y, Ma Y, Fan Y, Feng H, Wang J, Feng S, … Fan M (2009). Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is altered by short-term meditation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0904031106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tau GZ, & Peterson BS (2010). Normal development of brain circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology. doi: 10.1016/j.jsv.2011.11.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teixeira JMA, Fisk NM, & Glover V (1999). Association between maternal anxiety in pregnancy and increased uterine artery resistance index: cohort based study. BMJ. doi: 10.1136/bmj.318.7177.153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson RA (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson RJ, Dizén M, & Berenbaum H (2009). The unique relations between emotional awareness and facets of affective instability. Journal of Research in Personality. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tran TD, Biggs BA, Tran T, Simpson JA, Hanieh S, Dwyer T, & Fisher J (2013). Impact on infants’ cognitive development of antenatal exposure to iron deficiency disorder and common mental disorders. PLoS ONE. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074876 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tse AC, Rich-Edwards JW, Rifas-Shiman SL, Gillman MW, & Oken E (2010). Association of maternal prenatal depressive symptoms with child cognition at age 3 years. Paediatric and Perinatal Epidemiology. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.2010.01113.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Den Bergh BRH, Mennes M, Oosterlaan J, Stevens V, Stiers P, Marcoen A, & Lagae L (2005). High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to impulsivity during performance on cognitive tasks in 14- and 15-year-olds. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Leeuwen P, Geue D, Thiel M, Cysarz D, Lange S, Romano MC, … & Grönemeyer DH (2009). Influence of paced maternal breathing on fetal–maternal heart rate coordination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 13661–13666. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901049106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. (1997). WAIS-III administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV) administration and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Williams KT (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.