Skip to main content
Journal of Vision logoLink to Journal of Vision
. 2023 Sep 7;23(10):4. doi: 10.1167/jov.23.10.4

The double-drift illusion biases the marmoset oculomotor system

Nicholas M Dotson 1,1, Zachary W Davis 2,2, Jared M Salisbury 3,3, Stephanie E Palmer 4,4, Patrick Cavanagh 5,5, John H Reynolds 6,6
PMCID: PMC10494983  PMID: 37676672

Abstract

The double-drift illusion has two unique characteristics: The error between the perceived and physical position of the stimulus grows over time, and saccades to the moving target land much closer to the physical than the perceived location. These results suggest that the perceptual and saccade targeting systems integrate visual information over different time scales. Functional imaging studies in humans have revealed several potential cortical areas of interest, including the prefrontal cortex. However, we currently lack an animal model to study the neural mechanisms of location perception that underlie the double-drift illusion. To fill this gap, we trained two marmoset monkeys to fixate and then saccade to the double-drift stimulus. In line with human observers for radial double-drift trajectories with fast internal motion, we find that saccade endpoints show a significant bias that is, nevertheless, smaller than the bias seen in human perceptual reports. This bias is modulated by changes in the external and internal speeds of the stimulus. These results demonstrate that the saccade targeting system of the marmoset monkey is influenced by the double-drift illusion.

Keywords: double-drift illusion, marmoset monkey, saccadic eye movements

Introduction

Perceptual illusions provide an opportunity to uncover fundamental mechanisms of sensory processing (Eagleman, 2001; Gregory, 1968). One example is the double-drift illusion (also called the curveball or infinite regress illusion), which creates a dramatic dissociation between the physical and perceived position of a moving stimulus (Gurnsey & Biard, 2012; Lisi & Cavanagh, 2015; Shapiro et al., 2010; Tse & Hiegh, 2006). The double-drift illusion can be created using a Gabor patch with internal motion that is orthogonal to the aperture motion. When viewed in the periphery, the patch appears to move in a direction ∼45° from the actual direction (Figure 1). This position offset between the physical and perceived location accumulates over time (Lisi & Cavanagh, 2015; Liu et al., 2019; Marius’t, Henriques, & Cavanagh, 2022). The magnitude of the illusion is dependent on the relative speeds of the internal and external motion (Heller et al., 2021), with the strongest effect occurring with fast internal motion combined with slow external motion. The illusion is present only when the stimulus is viewed in the periphery, but it persists even when the eyes move, including when the stimulus is kept at a fixed location on the retina (Cavanagh & Tse, 2019).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Illustration of the double-drift illusion. Fast internal drift combined with slower external drift in an orthogonal direction produces a profound positional offset when viewed in the periphery. In the case where the aperture is drifting to the lower right with an internal motion that is up and right, the stimulus will appear to be moving nearly horizontally.

The double-drift illusion also demonstrates an intriguing dissociation between perceptual and saccadic reports. In human subjects asked to saccade to a double-drift stimulus, there is little to no bias in saccadic reports for stimuli moving vertically to the right or left of fixation with slow internal motion (Lisi & Cavanagh, 2015). Nevertheless, saccades do show a reliable bias in the direction of the illusion when the double-drift stimulus moves toward or away from the fovea with fast internal motion (Lisi & Cavanagh, 2022). These findings indicate that the illusion influences the saccade targeting system under certain conditions but to a lesser degree than the perceptual system. This link between saccade bias and perception for saccades to radial target paths with fast internal motion offers an opportunity to utilize saccadic reports in nonhuman primates as a proxy for a perceptual report.

Functional imaging studies in humans have revealed several potential cortical areas of interest, including the prefrontal cortex and visual motion processing areas of the visual system (Liu et al., 2019; Steinberg et al., 2022). However, we currently lack an animal model to study the neural mechanisms underlying the double-drift illusion. Marmosets are an emerging model organism for visual neuroscience (Davis et al., 2020; D'Souza et al., 2021; Mitchell et al., 2014; Solomon & Rosa, 2014), with several advantages over the commonly utilized macaque monkey, including a lissencephalic (smooth) cortex that facilitates array and laminar recordings in cortical areas that are buried within sulci in macaques (Davis et al., 2020; Davis et al., 2023; Johnston et al., 2019; Selvanayagam et al., 2019). This is particularly useful for studying cortical areas such as the frontal and visual cortex which have been shown in human subjects to be relevant for the double-drift illusion.

Here we test whether or not the marmoset oculomotor system is biased by the double-drift illusion. Marmosets were trained to perform the Double-Drift Saccade Task (DDST), which requires them to fixate and then wait a brief period of time before saccading to the double-drift stimulus. Similar to human observers, we find that their saccade endpoints show a bias that is modulated by changes in the external and internal velocity of the stimulus. These results demonstrate that the saccade targeting system of the marmoset monkey is biased by the illusion, indicating that they are a suitable model for studying the neural mechanisms of location perception that underlie the double-drift illusion.

Methods

Two adult male marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) were used in this study. To stabilize the head and track the eye position, a headpost was surgically implanted on each monkey. All surgical procedures were performed with the animal under general anesthesia in an aseptic environment in accordance with the recommendations in the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals of the National Institutes of Health (NIH). All experimental methods were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Salk Institute for Biological Studies and conformed with NIH guidelines.

Marmosets were trained to freely enter a custom-made marmoset chair, which was then positioned 41 cm from a calibrated and gamma-corrected LCD monitor (ASUS VG248QE; 100 Hz refresh rate; 75 cd/m2 background luminance). Eye position was measured with an IScan CCD infrared camera (500 Hz sampling rate). MonkeyLogic was used to calibrate and record eye position (1 kHz sampling rate), along with stimulus presentation and behavioral control (Asaad & Eskandar, 2008; Hwang et al., 2019).

Marmosets performed the DDST. After fixation was acquired and held for 250 ms within a fixation window (2.25 degrees of visual angle [dva] radius), a drifting Gabor stimulus appeared at a random location 12 dva from the fixation point (Figure 2). The stimulus traveled up to 6 dva over 1,000 ms at a slight angle (12 degrees) to the fixation point. The direction of internal motion was always orthogonal and inward with respect to the fixation point. On trials with no external motion, the stimulus remained in a fixed location (70% of the path length). After 500 ms, the fixation point disappeared and the marmoset was allowed 500 ms to saccade to the stimulus. During the saccade period, the stimulus continued to travel along its path but was extinguished as soon as fixation was broken. To receive a reward, the eye position had to land in the saccade window (3.5 dva radius centered at the end of the stimulus path) within 100 ms after fixation was broken and remain within the window for 100 ms. These parameters ensured that (a) saccades were directed toward the stimulus without constraining the endpoints, (b) only one saccade was made to the stimulus, and (c) the saccade landed within the saccade window rather than passing through it. If these criteria were met, then a small reward (marshmallow fluff and water mixture [3 g fluff/1 ml water]) was given using a syringe pump.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Illustration of the DDST. (A) Marmosets were required to maintain fixation in the presence of the double-drift stimulus and then saccade to the stimulus. The stimulus was extinguished at the onset of the saccade. The top and bottom panels in the Wait period indicate the conditions with and without external motion. In the Fixation, Wait, and Saccade panels, the red circle indicates eye position. The Saccade panels illustrate that the stimulus remains on (left) up to 500 ms until a saccade is made (right). In the rightmost Saccade panel, the red line indicates eye trajectory and the red circle indicates postsaccadic eye position. (B) Diagram of the relative positions of the fixation point, stimulus path, and the fixation (2.25 dva radius) and saccade (3.5 dva radius) windows. The gray arrow indicates the stimulus path. The blue dot indicates the start position of the stimulus (nonzero external speed). The green dot indicates the position of the stimulus when the external speed is zero. (C) Illustration of saccade angle calculation. The saccade bias angle is the angle between the physical path and the saccade endpoint. Positive angles are in the direction of internal motion. (D, E) Example saccades from a marmoset to double-drift stimuli with very slow internal motion (D) and fast internal motion (E). Blue dots and red dots indicate the start and end of the saccades. The solid black and open black circles indicate the start position of the stimulus and the position of the stimulus at the time the saccade starts. The insets show a schematic of the physical path (black line), the direction of internal motion (orange arrow), and the angle of the saccade bias. (D) In this example (control condition), the internal motion is 0.1 dva/s, and there is almost no saccade error. (E) A large saccade bias angle is observed in this example with internal motion at 8 dva/s.

The Gabor patch (80% Michelson contrast) consisted of a sinusoid carrier with a spatial frequency of 0.4 cycles/dva and a Gaussian envelope with a standard deviation of 0.5 dva. The mean luminance matched the background luminance. In two of the conditions, the external speed was fixed at 6 dva/s, while the internal motion speed was either 4 dva/s or 8 dva/s. In the third condition, the external speed was 0 dva/s and the internal speed was 8 dva/s. Since the patch was not moving across the screen, the location was fixed at a point where the marmosets would typically saccade to the moving patch (70% of the full path length). Only one condition was used during a recording session. For all conditions, on 50% of the trials, the internal speed was 0.1 dva/s (nonillusory). These trials, which do not result in an illusion in human observers, were used to control for any bias in each animal's saccade endpoints, independent of the illusion.

Eye data noise was reduced offline by using a moving average (20-ms window). To determine the eye position during the Wait period, we combined eye position data across trials (500 sample points for each trial). Instantaneous eye velocity was calculated using the difference in eye position between samples separated by 10 ms. The MATLAB functions risetime.m and falltime.m were used to identify the start and stop times of the saccades based on the eye velocity. We calculated the angle bias as the angle between the saccade endpoint and the stimulus path (Figures 2C–E). In the static condition where the external speed was 0 dva/s, we used the path the stimulus was placed on, even though it did not travel along it. We assigned the sign (positive or negative) based on whether the saccade endpoint was on the side the internal motion was oriented toward (positive) or away from (negative). A positive angle thus indicates that the saccade endpoint is biased in the direction of the internal motion. To account for any bias due to the configuration of the stimuli or eye calibration offsets, we subtracted the median angle of the nonillusory control trials from the illusory trials for each condition. Saccade amplitudes were calculated as the distance between the center of fixation and the endpoint of the saccade.

Results

We collected behavioral data from two marmoset monkeys performing the DDST under three experimental conditions. For each condition, data are from four sessions, two from each monkey. The DDST requires the monkey to briefly hold fixation and then saccade to the stimulus (Figure 2). During the Wait period, the monkey's gaze was typically less than 1 dva from the center of the fixation window (median = 0.6 dva and 90% ≤ 1.25 dva). Based on results from human observers, we predicted that the saccades would be biased by the internal motion when parameters were in a range that caused strong perceptual effects (Lisi & Cavanagh, 2022; Massendari et al., 2018). In general, slow external motion combined with fast internal motion should lead to larger saccade biases than slow external motion combined with slow internal motion or just internal motion alone (Heller et al., 2021). First, to determine if saccade endpoints are biased by the internal speed, we compared sessions with slow (4 dva/s) and fast (8 dva/s) internal speeds and a fixed external speed of 6 dva/s. Next, we kept the internal speed fixed at 8 dva/s and compared external speeds of 0 dva/s and 6 dva/s.

To determine if saccade endpoints are biased by the internal speed, we compared sessions with slow (4 dva/s) and fast (8 dva/s) internal speeds and a fixed external speed (6 dva/s) (Figure 3A). In the slow (4 dva/s) condition, saccades were very slightly biased toward the direction of internal motion (median angle bias = 1.35 degrees; p = 0.03, Wilcoxon signed rank test). However, in the fast (8 dva/s) condition, saccade endpoints were strongly biased (median angle bias = 4.93 degrees; p = 1.1E-7, Wilcoxon signed rank test), and we found a large difference in the angle bias between the two conditions (p = 4.9E-4, Wilcoxon rank sum test).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Saccade endpoints are parametrically biased by internal and external speed. (A) Comparison of conditions with a fixed external speed of 6 dva/s and either 4 dva/s (labeled: 4/6) or 8 dva/s (labeled: 8/6) internal speed. (B) Comparison of conditions with a fixed internal speed of 8 dva/s and either 0 dva/s (green; labeled: 8/0) or 6 dva/s (blue; labeled: 8/6) external speed. (C) Summary of median angles for all three conditions. The blue dashed line shows the change observed by increasing only the internal speed. In panels B and C, blue and green indicate external speeds of 6 and 0 dva/s, respectively.

To determine if the observed saccade bias is simply due to the internal motion as seen by Schafer and Moore (2007) in macaques and not the combined effect of the internal and external drift, we used a Gabor stimulus with 8 dva/s internal speed and 0 dva/s external speed (Figure 3B). In this condition, the saccade endpoints were slightly but significantly biased in the direction of internal motion (median angle bias = 1.99 degrees; p = 0.01, Wilcoxon signed rank test). However, the angle bias was substantially less than the matched condition with 8 dva/s internal motion and 6 dva/s external motion (1.99 vs. 4.93 degrees; p = 0.01, Wilcoxon rank sum test).

Finally, we sought to determine if the amplitude or timing of saccades differed between conditions, which could potentially influence the observed saccade angles. Across all conditions and trial types, the mean saccade amplitude was 5.4 (SD = 1.0) dva, and the mean saccade onset time was 239 (SD = 115) ms after the beginning of the saccade period. Between the conditions with a fixed external speed of 6 dva/s and a variable internal speed of 4 dva/s or 8 dva/s, we did not find any significant differences in the saccade amplitudes (p = 0.23, Wilcoxon rank sum test) or the saccade onset times (p = 0.97, Wilcoxon rank sum test). We also did not find any significant differences in the saccade amplitudes (p = 0.09, Wilcoxon rank sum test) or saccade onset times (p = 0.40, Wilcoxon rank sum test) between the conditions with a fixed internal speed of 8 dva/s and a variable external speed of 0 dva/s or 6 dva/s.

Discussion

We trained two marmoset monkeys to report the location of a double-drift stimulus with a saccade. Their saccade endpoints show a bias similar to the bias of human observers for saccades to targets on a radial path with fast internal motion (Lisi & Cavanagh, 2022) and of human observers performing delayed saccades (Massendari et al., 2018), and the bias is modulated by changes in the external and internal velocity of the stimulus (Figure 3C). Based on previous reports from humans (Kosovicheva et al., 2014) and monkeys (Schafer & Moore, 2007), we expect that a static (no external drift) Gabor patch with fast internal motion will, on its own, bias saccade endpoints in the direction of internal motion. Indeed, we find a slight bias in the direction of internal motion for the static Gabor; however, when external motion is added, the bias is more than doubled (Figure 3C at 8 dva/s internal speed). These results demonstrate that the saccade targeting system of the marmoset monkey is biased by the double-drift illusion beyond the effect of internal motion alone. Remarkably, this occurs in the absence of any discernible differences in the amplitude or timing of saccades.

In the case of the double-drift illusion, the saccade targeting system demonstrates a bias that is less than that seen for perceptual judgments in humans (Lisi & Cavanagh, 2015, 2022). These findings generally support the presence of multiple visual representations for different purposes (Goodale & Milner, 1992), although they do not necessarily require completely distinct neural substrates. A simple explanation supported by our experiments is that the oculomotor system does not integrate visual information over long temporal windows like the perceptual system; rather, it uses only recent information to estimate the position of the stimulus (Lisi & Cavanagh, 2022; Liu et al., 2019). Temporal integration windows may also help to explain other dissociations between perceptual and saccadic reports, as well as instances where no differences are observed. For example, in the Müller–Lyer illusion, the perceptual and saccadic reports do not differ when appropriate controls are applied to the data (Bruno et al., 2010). The absence of a difference may be because the perceptual effect does not grow over time as it does in the double-drift illusion, resulting in a similar error for both perception and saccadic reports.

The internal simulation that is constructed by our visual system is not a simple point-to-point mapping of the world. This is evidenced by visual illusions, which are capable of generating profound differences between the physical and the perceived, thus providing a unique tool for dissecting this process (Eagleman, 2001; Gregory, 1968). Nonhuman primates provide a crucial link for using visual illusions to study visual processing and perception at the level of single neurons (Sundberg et al., 2006). Our results establish that the marmoset monkey oculomotor system is parametrically biased by the double-drift illusion and suggest that marmosets may experience effects similar to those seen by humans for the double-drift illusion. Many open questions remain about the sources of error and the role of prediction (Palmer et al., 2015). The sources of visual information utilized by the perceptual and oculomotor systems, as well as the degree to which they share visual information and neural substrates, are also open matters of debate.

Acknowledgments

Supported by Fiona and Sanjay Jha Chair in Neuroscience (JHR).

Commercial relationships: none.

Corresponding author: Nicholas M. Dotson.

Email: ndotson@salk.edu.

Address: The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA, USA.

References

  1. Asaad, W. F., & Eskandar, E. N. (2008). A flexible software tool for temporally-precise behavioral control in Matlab. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 174(2), 245–258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bruno, N., Knox, P. C., & de Grave, D. D. (2010). A metanalysis of the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on saccadic eye movements: No general support for a dissociation of perception and oculomotor action. Vision Research, 50(24), 2671–2682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cavanagh, P., & Tse, P. U. (2019). The vector combination underlying the double-drift illusion is based on motion in world coordinates: Evidence from smooth pursuit. Journal of Vision, 19(14), 2, 10.1167/19.14.2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Davis, Z. W., Dotson, N. M., Franken, T. P., Muller, L., & Reynolds, J. H. (2023). Spike-phase coupling patterns reveal laminar identity in primate cortex. Elife, 12, e84512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Davis, Z. W., Muller, L., Martinez-Trujillo, J., Sejnowski, T., & Reynolds, J. H. (2020). Spontaneous travelling cortical waves gate perception in behaving primates. Nature, 587(7834), 432–436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. D'Souza, J. F., Price, N. S., & Hagan, M. A. (2021). Marmosets: A promising model for probing the neural mechanisms underlying complex visual networks such as the frontal–parietal network. Brain Structure and Function, 226(9), 3007–3022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Eagleman, D. M. (2001). Visual illusions and neurobiology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(12), 920–926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Gregory, R. L. (1968). Visual illusions. Scientific American, 219(5), 66–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gurnsey, R., & Biard, M. (2012). Eccentricity dependence of the curveball illusion. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 66(2), 144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Heller, N. H., Patel, N., Faustin, V. M., Cavanagh, P., & Tse, P. U. (2021). Effects of internal and external velocity on the perceived direction of the double-drift illusion. Journal of Vision, 21(8), 2, 10.1167/jov.21.8.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hwang, J., Mitz, A. R., & Murray, E. A. (2019). NIMH MonkeyLogic: Behavioral control and data acquisition in MATLAB. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 323, 13–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Johnston, K., Ma, L., Schaeffer, L., & Everling, S. (2019). Alpha oscillations modulate preparatory activity in marmoset area 8Ad. Journal of Neuroscience, 39(10), 1855–1866. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Kosovicheva, A. A., Wolfe, B. A., & Whitney, D. (2014). Visual motion shifts saccade targets. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(6), 1778–1788. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Lisi, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2015). Dissociation between the perceptual and saccadic localization of moving objects. Current Biology, 25(19), 2535–2540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Lisi, M., & Cavanagh, P. (2022). Different extrapolation of moving object locations in perception, smooth pursuit and saccades. bioRxiv, 10.1101/2022.10.26.513821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  17. Liu, S., Yu, Q., Tse, P. U., & Cavanagh, P. (2019). Neural correlates of the conscious perception of visual location lie outside visual cortex. Current Biology, 29(23), 4036–4044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Marius't Hart, B., Henriques, D. Y., & Cavanagh, P. (2022). Measuring the double-drift illusion and its resets with hand trajectories. Journal of Vision, 22(2), 16–16, 10.1167/jov.22.2.16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Massendari, D., Lisi, M., Collins, T., & Cavanagh, P. (2018). Memory-guided saccades show effect of a perceptual illusion whereas visually guided saccades do not. Journal of Neurophysiology, 119(1), 62–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Mitchell, J. F., Reynolds, J. H., & Miller, C. T. (2014). Active vision in marmosets: A model system for visual neuroscience. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(4), 1183–1194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Palmer, S. E., Marre, O., Berry, M. J., & Bialek, W. (2015). Predictive information in a sensory population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(22), 6908–6913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Schafer, R. J., & Moore, T. (2007). Attention governs action in the primate frontal eye field. Neuron, 56(3), 541–551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Selvanayagam, J., Johnston, K. D., Schaeffer, D. J., Hayrynen, L. K., & Everling, S. (2019). Functional localization of the frontal eye fields in the common marmoset using microstimulation. Journal of Neuroscience, 39(46), 9197–9206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Shapiro, A., Lu, Z. L., Huang, C. B., Knight, E., & Ennis, R. (2010). Transitions between central and peripheral vision create spatial/temporal distortions: A hypothesis concerning the perceived break of the curveball. PLoS ONE, 5(10), e13296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Solomon, S. G., & Rosa, M. G. (2014). A simpler primate brain: The visual system of the marmoset monkey. Frontiers in Neural Circuits, 8, 96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Steinberg, N. J., Roth, Z. N., Movshon, J. A., & Merriam, E. P. (2022). Neural basis of the double drift illusion. bioRxiv, 10.1101/2022.01.25.477714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  27. Sundberg, K. A., Fallah, M., & Reynolds, J. H. (2006). A motion-dependent distortion of retinotopy in area V4. Neuron, 49(3), 447–457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Tse, P. U., & Hsieh, P. J. (2006). The infinite regress illusion reveals faulty integration of local and global motion signals. Vision Research, 46(22), 3881–3885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Vision are provided here courtesy of Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology

RESOURCES