Abstract
The vertebrate stress response (SR) is mediated by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and contributes to generating context appropriate physiological and behavioral changes. Although the HPA axis plays vital roles both in stressful and basal conditions, research has focused on the response under stress. To understand broader roles of the HPA axis in a changing environment, we characterized an adaptive behavior of larval zebrafish during ambient illumination changes. The glucocorticoid receptor (nr3c1) was necessary to maintain basal locomotor activity in light and darkness. The HPA axis was required to adapt to light more efficiently but became dispensable when longer illumination was provided. Light adaptation was more efficient in dimmer light and did not require the mineralocorticoid receptor (nr3c2). Our findings show that the HPA axis contributes to the SR at various stages, facilitating the phasic response and maintaining an adapted basal state, and that certain adaptations occur without HPA axis activity.
Keywords: GR (glucocorticoid receptor), stress response, zebrafish, light assays, light adaptation, GAM (generalized additive models)
Introduction
The stress response (SR) is defined as the body’s response to actual or perceived threats. The SR is mediated by the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and has evolved in vertebrates. When a stimulus reaches the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) of the hypothalamus, the SR is initiated in a hormonal cascade starting with the secretion of corticotropin releasing hormone by the PVN. Subsequently, the anterior pituitary releases adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) that binds its receptor, MC2R (melanocortin receptor type 2) on the adrenal gland. The adrenal gland in turn secretes the effector molecule glucocorticoids (GCs; cortisol for humans and fish; corticosterone for rodents). GCs travel throughout the body and effectuate the SR by binding to their cognate receptors (glucocorticoid receptor [GR; nr3d] and mineralocorticoid receptor [MR; nr3c2]).1,2 Thus, the SR is composed of two parts: the central perception of the stimulus and production of the effector molecule (GCs), and the physiological changes occurring in diverse peripheral tissues by the function of the GCs and their receptor binding.3 Behavioral changes occur together with these physiological changes. The responses from the brain and peripheral tissues are intertwined and modulate each other’s states.4
Despite the connotation of the name, the SR machinery operates in both stressful (phasic) and basal (tonic) states of the body.3,5 During the phasic response after encountering potent stressors, high levels of GCs are secreted and drive a broad range of physiological and behavioral changes. Increased lipolysis and blood glucose levels, increased attention to the stressor, and increased or halted locomotion are some examples.6,7 At the basal level, GCs (cortisol) are secreted in a pulsatile ultradian pattern via the same HPA axis1,8 and (among the many functions) synchronize the circadian cycles between the central clock of the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) and the clocks of the peripheral tissues in the rest of the body.9–12 In this manner, the SR machinery and GCs regulate basal wakefulness and metabolism.13 Based on the time of the day and states of the body, GCs constitutively modulate the expression levels of many genes in diverse tissues.12,14–18
Such broad-scale actions are made possible by the biphasic (slow or rapid) signaling of the GCs and its receptor (GR). While glucocorticoid receptor (GR) is a transcription factor that modulates gene expression levels (slow, genomic response), GR also functions as a signaling molecule enabling a rapid, non-genomic GC response within seconds to minutes of a stimulus exposure.3,19 The long-lasting modulation of gene expression and rapid signaling in both the stressful (phasic) and basal (tonic) states make GCs the key molecule with which our perception in the brain is translated into tangible physiological and behavioral changes. Together with the nervous system, GCs impart our body’s physiological responses and coordinate appropriate adaptations in accordance with the changing environment, both gradually and rapidly.7·20 Thus, it is not a surprise that altered HPA axis activity is one of the most common findings in people with psychiatric disorders. Peripheral physiological symptoms accompany mental burdens in mood disorders and the ways in which psychological and somatic symptoms influence each other continues to be of importance.21,22 Thus, we sought to deepen our understanding of the dynamics and role of the HPA axis during an adaptive process to changing environmental conditions. Such knowledge may bring insight into how the HPA axis operates and how maladaptation of our body to the environmental challenges may arise.
The zebrafish is well-suited for such investigations involving the SR and corresponding changes in physiology and behavior. The hypothalamic-pituitary-interrenal (HPI) axis in zebrafish is functionally homologous to the mammalian HPA axis.23,24 The genes relevant to HPI axis functions are expressed around the time of hatching (2.5 days post-fertilization [dpf]), and the HPI axis responds to exogenous stressors from 4 dpf onward.24–28 Zebrafish are a diurnal species with the same circadian pattern as humans.29 Larval zebrafish develop rapidly, establishing most organ systems by 5 dpf, including the visual system.25,30 Larval zebrafish exhibit a repertoire of phototaxis and prototypical swim behavior.31,32 Larvae (5 dpf) prefer mildly lit environments, swimming toward a lit spot if not overly bright, and searching for light when placed in darkness (positive phototaxis).32–34 Using a larval zebrafish model, we specifically asked what the role of the HPA axis is during the adaptation to changing ambient light conditions. We did not presume that the change itself is a stressful encounter, rather inquired the role of the HPA axis during the period of changing illumination. We leveraged a photo adaptive behavior in larval zebrafish that we previously showed to be HPA axis dependent. Dark-acclimated zebrafish larvae moved significantly more after a 1-min illumination of white light in the post-illumination darkness, which was dependent on the key receptors in the HPA axis.23 With zebrafish that carry a mutation in mc2r (ACTH receptor), nr3c1 (glucocorticoid receptor), or nr3c2 (mineralocorticoid receptor), we had shown that mc2r and nr3c1 homozygous (HM) mutants had decreased locomotion in the post-illumination darkness after 1-min illumination, whereas nr3c2 mutants did not.23 Using the same mutant zebrafish lines, we expanded our investigation into locomotor responses to changing ambient illumination.
We hypothesized that fish with HPA axis perturbation show compromised light adaptation. In the expanded paradigm, we provided alternating illumination and dark conditions. Fish were acclimated in darkness, and dark and light conditions were provided for 7.5 minutes in four cycles finishing the experiment in 25 min of darkness: dark acclimation (30 min) – 4x [dark (7.5 min) – light (7.5 min)] – dark (25 min) (Fig. 1). We observed changes in locomotor responses after changing the duration of illumination to 2, 4, or 6 minutes (i.e., dark acclimation (30 min) – 4x [dark (7.5 min) – light (2 min)] – dark (25 min). When we varied the illumination in duration and intensity, HPA axis receptors were required to mount increased locomotor response only after shorter durations of light (2 and 4 min), but not after longer illumination (7.5 min). At baseline without any changes in illumination, nr3c1 mutant larvae moved less than their WT siblings in constantly lit and dark conditions. Beyond a certain threshold of brightness, dimmer and brighter light elicited the same pattern of response while dimmer light did not require HPA axis signaling to do so even during shorter durations of light. Using the adaptive behavior of zebrafish, we report that a functioning HPA axis is required to facilitate photoadaptation, enabling adaptation even after a short 1 or 2-min illumination, and to sustain basal locomotor activity. Rejecting our initial hypothesis, HPA axis perturbation did not lead to a uniform disruption of light adaptation process. When the stimulus was long enough in duration (≥ 6 min) or was low intensity (300 lux), the adaptive behavior could be achieved without HPA axis activity, indicating multiple pathways that lead to the adaptation. The HPA axis contributes to characteristic locomotor responses during light adaptation at various stages, facilitating initiation and maintaining higher levels of locomotion following basal adaptation.
Figure 1. Schematics of experimental process and behavioral assay paradigms.
A Experimental flow. Embryos obtained on 0 days-post-fertilization (dpf) through natural spawning. Healthy larvae were plated onto a pair of 48-well plates on 3–4 dpf. Assays were performed on 5 dpf unless otherwise stated. B Baseline assays. Larvae were videorecorded without any exogenous stimuli for about 12 hours between 9:30 am and 10:30 pm in light or darkness. C Dark-light repeat assays. Fish were observed in a changing illumination regimen i.e., 30-min acclimation + 4x [7.5-min dark + 7.5-min light] + 25-min dark. Some assays do not include the final [25-min dark] component since they were performed before the experimental protocol was established. D Statistical analysis pipeline post-experiment. Data from the dark-light repeat assays went through the complete statistical analysis to obtain the difference between different assay regimens (i.e., [7.5 + 2-min] assay vs. [7.5 + 7.5-min] assay). Inferences for the proportions were not made for baseline assays because there was only one assay regimen for the baseline assay (continuous recording in a dark or lit condition).
Materials and Methods
Materials and equipment
We listed the materials and equipment used in this study (Supplementary Table S1).
Zebrafish husbandry
Wild-type (WT) zebrafish (Danio rerio) originally purchased from Segrest Farm in Florida were outbred to keep a genetically diverse, healthy stock. Fish were handled and cared for following standard practices.35 The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) in the Mayo Clinic (A345–13-R16, A8815–15) approved the animal husbandry and study protocol. All experiments and methodologies were performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations. The Methods and Results are reported in accordance with ARRIVE guidelines.36 Adult fish were kept in a 9 L (25–30 fish) or 3 L (10–15) housing tank at 28.5°C with a light:dark (14:10) cycle. All experiments in this study were conducted between 4 and 7 dpf before the zebrafish sex is determined at about 15 dpf,30,37–39 and thus sex determination was not made.
Mutant zebrafish lines
The same WT and mutant zebrafish lines that we previously reported were used.23 All fish were maintained through outbreeding. There were three mc2r mutant lines that each carried a frameshift mutation in exon 1 (two 4- and one 5-base pair deletions; mn57, mn58, and mn59, respectively). The annotation on the gene mc2r in the National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI) has changed from having 2 exons to having 1 exon since our previous report. Four nr3c1 frameshift mutants were used, each of which carried a 7- or a 17-bp deletion in exon 2 (mn61, mn62) or a 4- or a 5-bp deletion in exon 5 (mn63, mn65). A nr3c2 frameshift mutant was used that carried a 55-bp deletion in exon 2 (mn67). For detailed information on the mutant lines, refer to our previous paper.23
Behavioral assay preparation
Adult mating pairs were placed in mating tanks separated by dividers (−1 dpf). On the following day, the divider was pulled between 8:30 and 9:00 am and embryos were obtained via natural spawning (0 dpf). Unfertilized and unhealthy embryos were cleaned up on 0, 1, and 3 dpf in the petri dish. Morphological defects (deformity, death) were the only exclusion criteria for an embryo to be excluded from an experiment. Larvae from different parent pairs were mixed in each petri dish to randomize the animals. On 3–4 dpf, a single larva was placed in each well of a 48-well plate. Larvae were caught from varying regions of the dish (i.e., center of the dish, areas close to the wall), from various depths (i.e., surface of the water, close to bottom of the dish), and throughout various swim behavior (i.e., fast swimmer, quiescent sitter) to ensure each 48-well plate contains larvae with diverse behavioral patterns. On 5 dpf, behavioral assays were performed in a custom-built assay chamber. From 0–5 dpf, plates were stored in an incubator with a light:dark cycle (14:10 hrs) at 28.5°C.
Custom light boxes
Light boxes were custom produced by the Mayo Clinic Division of Engineering. The light box has a control panel with two knobs that enable light intensity adjustment. Both infrared (IR) and white light have a low, medium, and high intensity. The illumination was provided from the bottom of the box through an additional translucent white acrylic board to evenly diffuse the light.
The spectral range of white light was approximately between 420 and 780 nm (STS-VIS; Ocean Optics Inc.; Supplementary Figs. S1-S3) and its light power (irradiance; wattage/unit area) was 20.5, 240.0, and 469.4 μW·cm−2 for the low, med, and high, respectively (Benchtop optical power meter; 1936-R; Newport Corp.). When translated into brightness (illuminance; lux) measured by a light meter application on a cell phone, the power measurements were equivalent to ~ 300, 4000, and 8000 lx, respectively.
The spectral range of infrared (IR) light was approximately between 780 and 880 nm (STS-NIR; Ocean Optics Inc.; Supplementary Figs. S4-S6) and its light power was 6.3, 58.1, and 116.0 μW·cm− 2 for the low, med, and high, respectively. The IR light produces 0 lx in brightness measurement. For detailed information on the custom light boxes (e.g., dimensions), refer to our previous paper.23
Basal locomotor activity assays
On the assay day, larvae were acclimated in either dark or light for 30 minutes before videorecording started (HDR-CX560V; Sony Corp.; Supplementary Table S1). Videorecording (30 frames per second) started around 9 am and the recording continued for 13 hours without any changes in the light condition that the fish were acclimated (Fig. 1B). Both white (469.4 μW·cm− 2; 8,000 lx) and IR (116.0 μW·cm− 2; 0 lx) illumination were provided at the high intensity condition. Larvae were tested at 4, 5, 6, and 7 dpf.
Dark-light repeat assays
All assays were performed at the high intensity for both IR (116.0 μW·cm− 2; 0 lx) and white (469.4 μW·cm− 2; 8000 lx) light, except the dim light assays. The dim light assays were done at the high intensity for infrared and at the low (20.5 μW·cm− 2; 300 lx) for white light.
The dark period was recorded in IR. Although zebrafish are thought to be unable to detect infrared and the acclimation to IR led to quiescence in locomotion, negative phototaxis to near-IR was reported.40 The larval zebrafish on 5 dpf were acclimated in IR for 30 minutes and underwent the dark (7.5 min) and light (7.5 min) periods four times. In the assays with shorter durations of illumination, 2, 4, or 6-min illumination was provided while keeping the length of the dark phase constant at 7.5 minutes. Assays ended with a 25-min dark period. The regimens were abbreviated with the repeat element (i.e., [7.5 + 2-min]) in the text. Regimen: 30-min dark acclimation + 4x [7.5-min dark + 7.5-min light] + 25-min dark (Fig. 1C). In some assays that were performed earlier in the project, the final 25-min dark period was omitted since the full experimental protocol was not established.
Dim light assays
In the dim light assays, the same protocol was followed except that the lowest of three intensity settings was used for white light (20.5 μW·cm− 2; 300 lx).23
Statistical analysis
Explanatory (independent) variables.
WT baseline assays: Developmental stage (4, 5, 6, and 7 dpf), illumination (dark and light), and time (videorecorded assay period. 1–43200 seconds [12 hours]; analyzed one data point per thirty seconds). nr3c1 baseline assays: Same as WT baseline assays, except that genotype (WT, heterozygous [HT], and homozygous [HM]) was an explanatory variable instead of the developmental stage. Dark-light repeat assays: genotype (WT, HT, and HM) and time (videorecorded assay period. 1–3600 secs [1 hr]; one data point per second). The experimenters were blind to the genotype of fish since larvae were genotyped post experiments.
Response (dependent) variable.
Locomotor activity was videorecorded (30 fps), and one data point was used for every second. Total distance moved for one minute is summed at every second in a sliding window of a minute (rolling sum; mm/min). The mean of the total distance moved per minute at each second was calculated for each experimental condition of the explanatory variable for each assay (i.e., WT, HT, and HM in assay 1). This rolling sum in each assay was the primary analysis unit. Since the variability in the dark-light assay is not established, we used our previous study to estimate the appropriate number of animals necessary. In our previous paper,23 we used 500–1,000 fish for a set of an experiment (i.e., 1-min light assay with mc2r lineage siblings). We used a pair of 48-well plates and thus 500–1,000 fish translate to approximately 5–10 assays. Our aim was to fit the number of assays between 5 and 10. However, since we intended to perform a set of experiment on multiple days to account for unexpected effects of a particular day and used natural spawning, the final number of assays in each set of experiment varied between 4 and 11, with an exception of dim light assays using WT fish. The dim light assay with WT fish was concluded with 3 assays per experiment.
Statistical analysis workflow.
Using raw movement data obtained in csv files for each assay (Fig. 1D): (1) The total distance moved for a min (rolling sum) at each second was computed for each explanatory variable group, (2) a Generalized Additive Model (GAM)41 was fitted to the data, (3) pairwise comparison on the predicted response variable from the model was conducted at each second, (4) proportion of the significance during a photo period was computed, and (5) comparison between the proportions of the significance was performed among different assay paradigms. All analyses were performed using the R language (4.3.0; Already Tomorrow)42 and all figures except the schematics (Fig. 1) were produced with R. All primary data and supplementary materials were deposited in the open access data repository (figshare.com; 10.6084/m9.figshare.23734398). The core R scripts for GAM modeling were deposited to GitHub.com (https://github.com/moonlarkalto/HPA_gam) and the data sets for test-running the R scripts were deposited to FigShare at the same link).
Model.
A generalized additive model (GAM) was developed to describe the locomotion of larval zebrafish in response to illumination changes. The generic formula for GAMs can be conceptually represented as:
1 |
where g is a link function, E (Y) is mean (expected) values of the response variable, Ai and θ are a design matrix and its parameter vector, respectively, of the linear term of Aiθ, f1 is a group of smooth functions to the term of x1i, f2 is a set of smooth functions to another explanatory variable of x2i, and ϵi is an error term. The response variable E (Y) can come from any exponential family and some non-exponential family distributions.
A GAM with the explanatory variables of genotype and time was fitted to the dark-light repeat assay data in R42 using the mgcv package (v1.8–42):43
2 |
where rsums is the response variable of the rolling sum, ~ denotes the relationship between the response and explanatory variables, geno is a linear explanatory term as a categorical variable (WT, HT, and HM; the main effect of this term is estimated), s constructs a set of smooth functions for the smooth term Time, k (basis dimension) is the number of base functions that constitute the smooth functions for the term (sets the upper limit on the degrees of freedom for the s smoother of the term), and by means a separate set of smooth functions are estimated for the term Time for each condition in geno. The smooth term for anid (representing each assay) is added as a random effect among the different assays where bs is an option to choose the type of spline (the thin plate spline [tp] is used as the default technique to produce smooth curves) and re is a spline option of random effect for the term anid. Refer to the mgcv package manual for information.44
Similarly, a GAM with the explanatory variables of illumination condition and developmental stage was fitted to the baseline assay data. There was no interaction detected among the terms, so the interaction was not modeled.
3 |
When the experimental period is longer (about an hour in the dark-light repeat assay vs. about 12 hours in the baseline assay), the number of base functions necessary to fit the model increased (k = 130 in the equation [2] vs. k = 220 in [3]). In GAMs, increased numbers of base functions produce more wiggly lines, more closely reflecting the changes in the actual data, while a straight line is produced when k = 1. To prevent overfitting, unnecessarily high k values were penalized during the fitting process as well as when competing models were compared for fitness. The experimenter only sets the upper limit (k) and the optimal k value is determined by the underlying optimization algorithm in the mgcv package, which can be further validated for its fitness statistics.
The model was first heuristically evaluated by the proportion of the variation explained, the distribution of the residuals, and the parameters produced by the gam. check function in mgcv. The model was considered inadequate when (1) the predicted values do not reasonably trace the actual data upon visual inspection, (2) the proportion of variation explained is too low, (3) the p-value (different from the p-value for inference on main effects) for each smooth term in the gam. check summary is too small (has to be comfortably not significant). Among the valid models with differing parameters, the fitness of those models was compared using Akaike information criterion (AIC).
Inference for the locomotion.
Once a GAM model is fitted, the overall effects and significance of each linear and smooth term on the response variable were evaluated, visualized, and recorded. Based on the fitted model, the predicted values of the response variables were extracted using the predict function. With the predicted values of the rolling sum, post hoc pairwise comparisons were performed using the emmeans package (v1.8.5).45 The estimated marginal means (EMMs; least-squares means) for each explanatory variable condition was computed (i.e., equation [4]), the difference between a pair of conditions was analyzed, and the significance of the difference was produced at every second (i.e., equation [5]):
(4) |
(5) |
The pairwise comparison (pair) produced the Tukey-adjusted p-values based on the t ratios drawn from the Studentized Range distribution, adjusted for multiple comparisons. The p-values were used to determine whether there is a significant difference in the distance moved between the two conditions (i.e., WT vs. HM) at each second.
Proportion of significance during a photo period.
Based on the inference (significance) drawn at every second, the proportion that a group moved significantly more than the comparison group during a given photo period was computed (i.e., mc2r WT larvae significantly moved more than their HM siblings in 89.3% of the “3rd Dark” photo period in the 7.5 + 2-min illumination assay; Supplementary Fig. S12; Supplementary Table S67; Fig. 4Ac).
Figure 4. mc2r lineage larvae respond differentially in post-illumination darkness based on the durations of illumination and genotype.
Aa, Ba, Ca Locomotor activity (mean predicted value (mm/min) ± 95%CI) for each experimental condition predicted by the GAM for each photo period (gray: dark, white: light period). Ab, Bb, Cb Locomotor response of mc2tex1 larvae during dark-light repeat assays. The scatterplot (points) shows actual mean locomotor activity (mm/min) for each experimental condition of each assay. The line graph shows predicted locomotor activity for each experimental condition by the GAM (predicted value ± 95%CI; gray: dark, white: light period). Ac, Bc, Cc Time points where an experimental condition showed significantly high locomotor activity compared to the other in a pairwise comparison. Ad, Bd, Cd Density distribution of actual mean locomotor activity shows severely right skewed distribution. The integration of the curve equals 100%. (D: dark, L: light, WT: wildtype, HT: heterozygous, HM: homozygous, n.s: not significant)
Inference for the proportion.
The proportions of significance during a photo period were compared using the two-proportions z test with Yates’ continuity correction to see whether the durations of illumination led to difference in the proportions of significant difference in locomotion46–48 (i.e., nr3c1ex5 WT larvae moved significantly more than their HM siblings in 100% of the “5th Dark” photo period in the 7.5 + 2-min illumination assay, whereas the proportion of significant difference in the 7.5 + 6-min illumination assay was 90.1%. It was asked if these two proportions were significantly different; Supplementary Data S32; Supplementary Table S67; Supplementary Figs. S21 and S23).
Results
Generalized additive model describes locomotor response during environmental changes
To better understand the dynamics of locomotor response over time, a generalized additive model (GAM) was developed (Fig. 1D; Model, Statistical Analysis in the Methods). A GAM for each assay consisted of the linear (parametric) and smooth terms that described the response variable, locomotion. The effect of each term on locomotion was evaluated for its main effect. With all the assays, the overall variation in locomotion explained by the model ranged from 68.3–97.5% (mean = 86.6%), demonstrating a satisfactory explanatory power (Supplementary Data S1-S23). The residuals, unexplained variations, and fitted values were visualized to assess their appropriateness (Supplementary Figs. S63-S85). Post hoc pairwise analysis was performed to assess the significance of the difference in locomotion between the groups every second (Supplementary Tables S2-S66). The proportions of the significance between different assay regimens were compared to understand the effect of different durations of illumination (i.e., 4x[7.5 dark + 2-min light] regimen vs. 4x[7.5 + 7.5-min] regimen; Supplementary Figs. S8-S62; Supplementary Data. S24-S59; Supplementary Tables S67-S70).
To make the analysis process more rigorous, we used the mean of each experimental group in each assay, rather than the raw movement measurement of individual fish conventionally utilized in zebrafish behavioral studies (Response variable, Statistical Analysis in the Methods). The aggregation of the locomotor response mitigated the severe skewedness in the data.
WT larvae move more in light than in darkness during the day
To understand the changes in locomotion following illumination changes, we first established the pattern of basal locomotor activity during the day without any stimuli. In addition, we were curious whether zebrafish would move more in a lit or dark condition in the daytime. As a diurnal species, zebrafish have a circadian cycle that has evolved for high activity during the day and rest at night.49 It was not clearly established whether the larvae would move following the circadian rhythm regardless of the presence of illumination or the condition of ambient light (lit or dark) would dictate the levels of locomotion overriding the circadian cycle (called “masking”). We recorded wildtype (WT) larvae (4-, 5-, 6-, and 7-days post-fertilization [dpf]) from 10 am to 10 pm in constant illumination or darkness.
We found that illumination was a critical determinant for the larvae’s locomotion (Fig. 2Ab; Supplementary Data S1). Light had a significant positive effect on locomotion compared to the null hypothesis of no effect (light, main effect, p= 1.31e-12; Supplementary Data S1) and led to a significantly different trend over time (light spline; F= 4.937, edf = 171.881, p < 2e-16). Darkness did not result in a different trend in locomotion (dark spline; F = 0.05, edf = 0.844, p = 0.951). Larvae in light moved significantly more than their age-matched counterpart in darkness across all developmental stages over 12 hours (Fig. 2Ac). That is, 4-, 5-, 6-, and 7-dpf larvae in light moved significantly more than 4-, 5-, 6-, and 7-dpf larvae in dark at all time points (100%), respectively (Supplementary Figs. S8-S9; all p-values provided in Supplementary Tables S3-S4 and all proportions provided in Supplementary Tables S67-S68). A dark environment decreased baseline activity, masking the basal circadian activity levels in light during the day in larvae aged between 4 and 7 days.
Figure 2. Illumination and developmental stages are determinants of basal locomotion in WT larvae.
Aa Locomotor activity (mean predicted value (mm/min) ± 95%CI) for each experimental condition predicted by the generalized additive model (GAM) for each time span. Ab Basal locomotor activity of WTlarvae over 12 hours. The scatterplot (points) shows actual mean locomotor activity (mm/min) for each experimental condition of each assay. The line graph shows predicted locomotor activity for each experimental condition by the GAM (predicted value ± 95%CI). Ac Time points where an experimental condition showed significantly high locomotor activity compared to the other in a pairwise comparison. Ad Density distribution of actual mean locomotor activity shows severely right skewed distribution. The integration of the curve equals 100%. Since individual y-axis bin (distance moved (mm/min)) is smaller than 1 (i.e., 0.2 mm/min), the x-axis values reach over 100% while the area under the curve is still 100%. (D: dark, D4: 4 dpf, L: light, n.s: not significant)
The developmental stage was another important determinant for basal locomotion. Compared to 4-dpf larvae, 5-, 6-, and 7-dpf larvae moved significantly more (5, 6, and 7-dpf, main effect, p = 0.00099, 2.38e-11, and 6.03e-8, respectively). Activity at different ages from highest to lowest was 6-dpf fish followed by 7-, 5-, and 4-dpf fish in either light or darkness. Combined, the levels of locomotion followed the order over 12 hours: (6-dpf larvae in light > ≈ 7 dpf in light) > ≈ 5 dpf in light > (4 dpf in light ≈ 6 dpf in dark) >≈ (7 dpf in dark * 5 dpf in dark) > 4 dpf in dark (Figs. 2Ab and 2Ac). 5-dpf larvae in light moved significantly more than 6-dpf in darkness for a short window of time, and moved significantly more than 7-, 5-, and 4-dpf in darkness for most of the time (Fig. 2Ac; Supplementary Figs. S8-S9; Supplementary Tables S67-S68 and S3-S4).
nr3c1 mutant larvae move less than WT siblings in light and darkness during the day
Following the study of basal locomotion in WT fish, we investigated the basal locomotion of the nr3c1ex5 fish (5 dpf) in which glucocorticoid receptor (nr3c1) is knocked out in homozygous (HM) larvae.23 Similar to the WT stock fish, illumination was a critical determinant of locomotor activity of nr3c1ex5 fish. Compared to darkness, a lit condition increased locomotion (light; main effect, p = 3.27e-6; Supplementary Data S2; Fig. 3Ab). Genotype was another determinant. nr3c1 WT and heterozygous (HT) siblings showed comparable levels of locomotion in darkness or light (HT, main effect, p = 0.683). Homozygosity decreased overall locomotion (HM, main effect, p = 0.014). WT larvae moved significantly more than HM siblings in 34.84% of the time both in darkness and illumination and the significance was concentrated between 10 am and 6 pm (Supplementary Figs. S10-S11; Supplementary Tables S6-S7 and S67-S68). Homozygosity had a sizable impact on locomotion such that HM larvae in light moved more than WT siblings in darkness only in 1.39% for the 12-h period (Fig. 3Ac; Supplementary Fig. S11; Supplementary Table S68).
Figure 3. Illumination and genotype are determinants of basal locomotion in nr3c1ex5 lineage larvae.
Aa Locomotor activity (mean predicted value (mm/min) ± 95%CI) for each experimental condition predicted by the GAM for each time span. Ab Basal locomotor activity of nr3c!ex5 larvae over 12 hours. The scatterplot (points) shows actual mean locomotor activity (mm/min) for each experimental condition of each assay. The line graph shows predicted locomotor activity for each experimental condition by the GAM (predicted value ± 95%CI). Ac Time points where an experimental condition showed significantly high locomotor activity compared to the other in a pairwise comparison. Ad Density distribution of actual mean locomotor activity shows severely right skewed distribution. The integration of the curve equals 100%. (D: dark, L: light, WT: wildtype, HT: heterozygous, HM: homozygous, n.s: not significant)
mc2r mutant larvae show less activity than WT siblings only when illumination duration is shorter
After establishing that both darkness and blocking HPA axis activity decrease basal locomotion in larvae, we investigated the dynamic changes in locomotion in response to changing illumination. We previously showed that, to increase locomotor activity in post-light darkness after a brief 1-min illumination, a functioning HPA axis is essential as loss of mc2r and nr3c1 significantly decreased the locomotor response in these fish.23 Our initial hypothesis on the larval response in the dark-light repeat assay was that fish with a mutation in one of the key HPA axis receptor genes would have a deficiency in mounting an appropriate locomotor response to changing light. However, the duration of illumination is a determinant of the ensuing locomotor activity in darkness, as well as the HPA axis genes.
The ACTH receptor (mc2r) on the adrenal gland (interrenal cells in zebrafish) is a key HPA (HPI) axis receptor that initiates glucocorticoid synthesis. When mc2r was knocked out, the overall locomotion in darkness was significantly less than that of WT siblings when repeated illumination was 2 mins (HM, main effect, p = 8.007e-6) and 4 mins (main effect, p = 0.00015), but not with 7.5-min illumination (main effect, p = 0.758; Fig. 4; Supplementary Data S3-S5). The proportion of locomotion in which WT larvae moved significantly more than their HM siblings in darkness (the 3rd, 4th, and 5th dark periods) was 93.9, 96.34, and 0% in the 2-, 4-, and 7.5-min light repeat assays, respectively (Supplementary Figs. S15-S17; Supplementary Table S69). The difference in proportions was significant between the 2- and 4-min light repeat assays (two-proportions z test, χ2=8.243, p=0.004; Supplementary Data S25), the 2- and 7.5-min light repeat assays (χ2=2429.0, p≈0; Supplementary Data S27), and the 4- and 7.5-min light repeat assays (χ2=2555.1, p≈0; Supplementary Data S29). Therefore, the mc2r HM larvae could mount an equivalent locomotion profile compared to WT siblings as the duration of illumination increased (7.5 min).
nr3c1 mutant larvae move less than WT siblings when illumination duration is shorter
nr3c1 is the canonical glucocorticoid receptor that binds to glucocorticoids (i.e., cortisol, corticosterone) and drives the various stress responses in the central nervous system and peripheral tissues. Homozygous mutant larvae in nr3c1ex5 showed significantly less locomotion in darkness in the 2-, 4-, and 7.5-min repeat assays (HM, main effect, p = 0.00054; p = 0.0029, and p = 0.0465, respectively), but not in 6-min repeat assays (main effect, p = 0.057; Fig. 5; Supplementary Data S6-S9). The proportion of locomotion where the WT fish moved significantly more than their HM siblings in darkness was 100, 87.78, 68.1, and 60.88% when 2-, 4-, 6-, and 7.5-min illumination was repeated (Supplementary Figs. S25–28; Supplementary Table S69). The difference in proportions was significant in all pairwise comparisons between 2- and 4-min (two-proportions z test, χ2=171.48, p = 3.510e-39; Supplementary Data S31), 2- and 6-min (χ2=507.92, p = 1.794e-112; Supplementary Data S33), 2- and 7.5-min (χ2=647.77, p = 6.828e-143; Supplementary Data S35), 4- and 6-min (χ2=153.23, p = 3.42e-35; Supplementary Data S37), 4- and 7.5-min (χ2 =253.14, p = 5.373e-57; Supplementary Data S39), and 6- and 7.5-min repeated illumination (χ2=15.632, p=7.694e-5; Supplementary Data S41), showing that the proportion of the difference in locomotion between the WT and HM siblings changes as the illumination duration changes.
Figure 5. nr3c1ex5 lineage larvae respond differentially in post-illumination darkness based on the durations of illumination and genotype.
Aa, Ba, Ca, Da Locomotor activity (mean predicted value (mm/min) ± 95%CI) for each experimental condition predicted by the GAM for each photo period (gray: dark, white: light period). Ab, Bb, Cb, Db Locomotor response of nr3dex5 larvae during dark-light repeat assays. The scatterplot (points) shows actual mean locomotor activity (mm/min) for each experimental condition of each assay. The line graph shows predicted locomotor activity for each experimental condition by the GAM (predicted value ± 95%CI; gray: dark, white: light period). Ac, Bc, Cc, Dc Time points where an experimental condition showed significantly high locomotor activity compared to the other in a pairwise comparison. Ad, Bd, Cd, Dd Density distribution of actual mean locomotor activity shows severely right skewed distribution. The integration of the curve equals 100%. (D: dark, L: light, WT: wildtype, HT: heterozygous, HM: homozygous, n.s: not significant)
In another lineage of nr3c1 mutant fish where the frameshift mutation was introduced in exon 2, the effect of the duration of illumination was clearer. Homozygous mutant larvae in nr3c1ex2 moved significantly less in darkness when repeated illumination was 4 min (HM, main effect, p = 0.0071), but not when illumination was 6 min (p = 0.197) and 7.5 min (p = 0.615; Supplementary Fig. 7; Supplementary Data S21-S23). The proportion of locomotion where the WT fish moved significantly more than their HM siblings in darkness was 94.65, 71.58, and 0% when the repeated illumination was 4, 6, and 7.5 min (Supplementary Figs. S57-S59; Supplementary Table S69). The difference in overall activity levels in darkness between the WT and HM siblings decreased in both nr3c1ex5 and nr3c1ex2 mutant fish (100, 87.78, 68.1, and 60.88%; ND, 94.65, 71.58, and 0% at 2, 4, 6, and 7.5 min), respectively, as the repeated illumination increased50–52, showing that the nr3c1 HM mutants could mount increasingly similar locomotor responses in darkness as the duration of illumination increased.
Mutations in nr3c2 do not appear to have consistent effects on locomotion
Mineralocorticoid receptor (nr3c2) is another nuclear receptor that binds to glucocorticoids with higher affinity. However, we did not find a clear role in locomotor response to light changes in our previous paper.23 Similarly, again we could not identify any pattern of difference in locomotion between the WT and HM siblings (Fig. 6). The HM mutant larvae in nr3c2ex2 apparently moved less in 2-min repeated illumination at some time points (Fig. 6Ac), but there was no main effect of homozygosity compared to WT siblings (HM, main effect, p = 0.104; Supplementary Data S10). Likewise, in 4- and 7.5-min repeated illumination, the HM larvae apparently moved more at some time points, but there was no main effect of homozygosity compared to WT siblings (p = 0.0728 and p = 0.758), respectively (Supplementary Data S11-S12). Since there was no main effect of homozygosity on locomotion, the meaning of the apparent decrease or increase of locomotion amongst HM siblings at some time points is unlikely to be biologically meaningful.
Figure 6. Mutation in nr3c2ex2 do not have main effect on locomotion.
Aa, Ba, Ca Locomotor activity (mean predicted value (mm/min) ± 95%CI) for each experimental condition predicted by the GAM for each photo period (gray: dark, white: light period). Ab, Bb, Cb Locomotor response of nr3c2ex2 larvae during dark-light repeat assays. The scatterplot (points) shows actual mean locomotor activity (mm/min) for each experimental condition of each assay. The line graph shows predicted locomotor activity for each experimental condition by the GAM (predicted value ± 95%CI; gray: dark, white: light period). Ac, Bc, Cc Time points where an experimental condition showed significantly high locomotor activity compared to the other in a pairwise comparison. Despite such apparent significant difference at some time points, there was no main effect of being HM on locomotor response compared to WT siblings (Refer to the Results). The tile graphs are provided to show the patterns in difference. Ad, Bd, Cd Density distribution of actual mean locomotor activity shows severely right skewed distribution. The integration of the curve equals 100%. (D: dark, L: light, WT: wildtype, HT: heterozygous, HM: homozygous, n.s: not significant)
Locomotor patterns over dark-light repeats are reproduced with much dimmer light
All light experiments were conducted with consistent illumination intensities: IR (116.0 μW·cm−2; 0 lx) and white light (469.4 μW·cm− 2; 8000 lx). Since the duration of illumination (quantity of light by time) was a determinant of locomotor response in darkness, we asked whether lower intensities of illumination (quantity of light by intensity) would elicit the same behavioral adaptation and, if so, whether it would take longer. In the dim light assays, we used the same dark-light repeat regimen but used a much lower intensity of white light (20.5 μW·cm− 2; 300 lx). With dim light repeat assays, WT fish showed the same pattern of locomotor response as the assays with higher intensities of light (Fig. 7), increasing locomotor response in darkness while having lower locomotion in light. However, this increase did not reliably occur until the duration of illumination was 2 min or more (Fig. 8). With the nr3c1ex5 lineage, the HM larvae showed equivalent levels of locomotion to WT fish in all 2-, 4-, and 7.5-min repeated dim illumination and there was no main effect of homozygosity on locomotion compared to WT siblings (HM, main effect, p = 0.224, p = 0.263, and p = 0.485), respectively, (Supplementary Data S18-S20). Since there was no main effect, further analysis was not pursued. The tile graphs (Figs. 8 Bc, Cc, Dc) are provided to show the patterns in difference. Unexpectedly, dimmer illumination effectively facilitates the locomotor response in darkness after repeated illumination without a requirement for HPA axis signaling.
Figure 7. Much dimmer illumination reproduces the same pattern of dark-light responses in WT larvae.
Dimmer illumination (20.5 μW·cm−2; 300 lx) was used compared to that of all other experiments (469.4 μW·cm−2; 8000 lx). IR illumination was the same (116.0 μW·cm−2; 0 lx). Aa, Ba, Ca, Da Locomotor activity (mean predicted value (mm/min) ± 95%CI) predicted by the GAM for each photo period (gray: dark, white: light period). A brief illumination assay (1-min light) without the repeat components was included to understand behavior in dim light (A). Ab, Bb, Cb, Db Locomotor response of WT larvae during dark-light repeat assays. The scatterplot (points) shows actual mean locomotor activity (mm/min) of each assay. The line graph shows predicted locomotor activity by the GAM (predicted value ± 95%CI; gray: dark, white: light period). Ac, Bc, Cc, Dc Time points where an experimental condition showed significantly high locomotor activity compared to the other in a pairwise comparison. Ad, Bd, Cd, Dd Density distribution of actual mean locomotor activity shows severely right skewed distribution. The integration of the curve equals 100%. (D: dark, L: light, n.s: not significant)
Figure 8. Much dimmer illumination reproduces the same pattern of dark-light responses in nr3c1ex5 lineage larvae and may lead to more effect photoadaptation.
Dimmer illumination (20.5 μW·cm−2; 300 lx) was used compared to that of all other experiments (469.4 μW·cm−2; 8000 lx). IR illumination was the same (116.0 μW·cm−2; 0 lx). Aa, Ba, Ca, Da Locomotor activity (mean predicted value (mm/min) ± 95%CI) for each experimental condition predicted by the GAM for each photo period (gray: dark, white: light period). A brief illumination assay (1-min light) without the repeat components was included to understand behavior in dim light (A). Ab, Bb, Cb, Db Locomotor response of nr3c1ex5 larvae during dark-light repeat assays. The scatterplot (points) shows actual mean locomotor activity (mm/min) for each experimental condition of each assay. The line graph shows predicted locomotor activity for each experimental condition by the GAM (predicted value ± 95%CI; gray: dark, white: light period). Ac, Bc, Cc, Dc Time points where an experimental condition showed significantly high locomotor activity compared to the other in a pairwise comparison. Despite such apparent significant difference at some time points, there was no main effect of being HM on locomotor response compared to WT siblings, starting from the 2-min light assays (Refer to the Results). The tile graphs are provided to show the patterns in difference. Ad, Bd, Cd, Dd Density distribution of actual mean locomotor activity shows severely right skewed distribution. The integration of the curve equals 100%. (D: dark, L: light, WT: wildtype, HT: heterozygous, HM: homozygous, n.s: not significant)
Discussion
We found that HPA axis activity is required to facilitate light adaptive behavior and maintain baseline activity. Larvae with mutations in a key HPA axis receptor could not increase their locomotor activity levels in darkness after a brief (1–2 min) illumination. The mutants required longer durations of illumination (≥ 6 min) to increase locomotor response in the ensuing darkness (Figs. 4, 5, and Supplementary Fig. S7). In addition, abrogation of HPA axis function (nr3c1 knockout) led to decreased basal activity under either constant illumination or darkness (Fig. 3). These findings show that the canonical HPA axis contributes to the efficiency of adaptation to changing light conditions, but the photoadaptation can be slowly achieved in HPA axis mutants when light is provided for a long enough duration (≥ 6 min). Thus, the phasic locomotor response can be achieved even without HPA axis activity, while maintaining the tonic (basal) locomotion appropriate for the given illumination required the canonical HPA axis (Fig. 3). Our findings contrast a previous study that found an increased circadian locomotor activity in nr3c1 mutants (grs357) that carried a missense mutation.53 This discrepancy may be due to the difference in mutant types. While our nr3c1 mutants in exon 2 or 5 carried a frameshift mutation that was expected to yield a truncated protein, the DNA-binding function was abrogated in the grs357 variant, possibly leaving the signaling capacity intact.54
The same pattern of phasic locomotor response was reproduced in much dimmer light only with higher efficiency in HPA axis mutant animals (Figs. 7 and 8). Both the dim (20.5 μW·cm− 2; ~300 lx) and high (469.4 μW·cm− 2; ~8,000 lx) intensities of white light increased locomotion in darkness post-illumination. Yet, nr3c1ex5 homozygous (HM) mutant larvae achieved WT-level activity after 2 min in the dim illumination, compared to 6–7.5 minutes they needed in brighter light. Thus, light adaptation was facilitated not only by the number of photons, but also by the appropriate intensity of light. In most animals, the illumination during twilight (dawn, dusk) is a salient zeitgeber (signals that entrain the circadian rhythm).55,56 The electroretinogram (ERG) wave patterns change in birds during this period,57 and there may be distinct photopigments detecting twilight illumination.58 During the twilight period, animals detect changes in the amount and composition (a shift toward more blue spectrum) of illumination and the angle to the sun. Since the amount (intensity) of illumination is the only variable that changed in this experiment, it is unclear whether the increased efficiency in the light adaptive behavior is related to the twilight-time effect. Further investigation is required for the relationship between light adaptation and illumination intensity. nr3c1 WT and HM siblings showed a marginal increase in locomotion (< 4 mm/min) after 1-min illumination in dim light (Fig. 8Ab). That was likely to be noise since those low levels of locomotion (effect size) were within the range of basal locomotion, rather than a response to light changes. The likely reason why general WT stock fish showed a more robust response (Fig. 7A) than the WT siblings of the nr3c1 fish (Fig. 8A) after 1 -min dim illumination may be familial difference in background genetics. Such familial differences are common among different zebrafish lineages in the laboratory and wild strains.59–63
The characterization of the locomotor response in this study reveals complex behavior over time. In the earlier studies using ultrahigh-speed cameras, larvae showed a sharp increase in locomotion immediately after transitioning from dark to light or from light to dark in millisecond scales.64 These reflexive movements were classified into different categories based on the nature of stimuli, the characteristics of the movement repertoire, and the underlying neural circuits responsible for the movements. The auditory startle, visual startle, dark-flash, and light-flash responses, to name a few, could be differentiated and were comprised of distinct basic units of locomotion.31,65–74 Our study characterized the rapid non-genomic locomotor responses in the timescale of hormonal response75–77 that followed the immediate reflexive responses. The adaptive response to changing illumination arose with or without HPA axis activity, demonstrating that vital adaptations essential to the organism would occur by means of multiple redundant pathways. Nevertheless, the canonical HPA axis, as the backbone of the stress response (SR), played a critical facilitative role, making such locomotor adaptation more efficient.
Importantly, a functioning HPA axis was necessary to maintain basal locomotor activity, which implies that a hyper- or hypo-preparedness of the baseline may precede maladaptive phasic responses. Decreased basal locomotion in nr3c1ex5 HM larvae in light and darkness may be related to the role of GCs as the key signaling molecule in synchronizing the circadian cycle in mammals.9–11,18 The circadian regulation is more complex in teleost fish as a range of tissues including the skin, brain, pineal gland, and heart are directly responsive to light and autonomously maintain the circadian clock.78–82 A broad spectrum of light (ultraviolet, visible, and infrared light) could directly induce clock gene expression in zebrafish cell culture while infrared light could not phase-shift the circadian clock.78 Despite such complex interaction between the light and clock systems in zebrafish, the genetic abrogation of DNA-binding in grs357 did not change clock-related gene expression levels while increasing basal locomotor activity.53 Blocking glucocorticoid (GC) signaling did not change the circadian fluctuation of crh (corticotropin releasing hormone) in the neurosecretory preoptic area29 (zebrafish homolog to the mammalian hypothalamus).83 GC signaling was instead necessary to maintain the level of overall basal crh production, contrasting the negative feedback of the phasic GC signaling against crh production.29 Thus, the role of GC/GR signaling and HPA axis dynamics in basal physiology needs further investigation and may add insight into maladaptive HPA axis function during the phasic responses.
Mutations in nr3c2ex2 did not show any consistent direction of influence in behavior. After 2-min illumination, WT fish appeared to move more than their HM siblings whereas, after 4-min illumination, HM siblings moved more (Fig. 6). After 7.5-min illumination, there was no difference in locomotion between the WT and HM siblings. We found no main effect of homozygosity on locomotion in any of these apparent increases and decreases of locomotion among nr3c2fish. Thus, the differential effect of the HM genotype on locomotion is unlikely to have biological significance. It appears that nr3c2 does not play a critical role in the photo-adaptive behavior.
The locomotor response of zebrafish larvae to exogenous stimuli is not normally distributed (non-normal). A large proportion of fish do not move at all or move very little (many zero and negligible values) while a small minority show excessive movement (outliers). The variation in the response is not homogeneous among experimental groups (heteroscedasticity). There is no adequate method to easily transform the response variable to near normal at the individual fish level.84–87 By comparing the single values averaged over a stretch of time (i.e., total movement during the experimental period), we miss the dynamics of the response over time.88,89 Such data structures and over-simplification violate key assumptions on which common parametric tests and linear models depend to make valid hypothesis tests.90–92 Non-normality and violation of homoscedasticity can be addressed, to some degree, by increasing the sample size,87,93 using the sample means (sampling distribution rather than raw data from individual fish),93–95 and including the variation in each experiment as random effects in mixed model approaches.86 The size of the samples that yield reliably accurate hypothesis tests in non-normal distribution is often less than 100. Even with extremely non-normal and heteroscedastic data sets, a less than 500 sample can perform statistical tests based on linear regression models.93 However, it is reported that whereas increasing sample size could address non-normality issues more easily, other types of the assumption violation including homoscedasticity, extreme outliers, and independence of errors may persist despite the increased sample size.87 Moreover, outcome transformations change the result estimates and may bias the result.87
To address these issues, several approaches were proposed in the zebrafish research community.84–86,96 However, with nonparametric tests using rank comparison, we forgo quantitative estimates of the response although the inference may be more justifiable.97 When a generalized linear model analyzes the proportion of the response vs. nonresponse, we do not show the effect size but just summarize the presence or absence of the response. While nonlinear models effectively characterize the properties of the response (i.e., the rate of increase), the rigidity of the underlying equation limits the applicability of a model to a few specific phenomena. In our case, it was difficult to apply the same nonlinear model to the two distinct response profiles during the dark or light photo period. Thus, it is unlikely that a single solution will be able to address all the assumption violations in the zebrafish locomotor data and produce the right inference. Rather, it would be more important to describe the outcome and its effect size in accordance with the scientific question and discuss the biological relevance of the inference.98–102 For example, what to report should be determined by the research interest, when a group of zebrafish consistently moved 10 mm per minute making a 100-mm displacement for 10 min and another group moved 90 mm for a minute and stayed without movement for 9 min. Whether comparing the total distance of 100 mm vs. 90 mm or the pattern of evenness vs. dynamic change is more relevant should be determined by the scientific question in hand. The statistical assumptions violated or satisfied should be chosen by the same standard.
To describe zebrafish locomotor response to changing light, we chose to adopt a statistical model, not commonly used in zebrafish behavioral studies. A generalized additive model (GAM) provided a practical and expandable framework,103,104 successfully describing locomotor response throughout the entire experimental process. A GAM is an extended generalized linear model (GLM) with a linear predictor that is composed of a sum of smooth functions of the explanatory variable (smooth terms) in addition to the parametric explanatory variables.41,43,105 A range of link functions can be used to linearly relate the predictors to the response variable of an exponential or some non-exponential family distribution. However, we focused on estimating the effect size and did not use additional link function (used the identity link function). The variance among the individual assays was modeled as a random effect. The simplicity and flexibility of the GAMs allow describing nonlinear relationships in the linear additive framework. In our GAM analysis pipeline, the main effect of the explanatory variable for the whole experiment was first evaluated, followed by post hoc pairwise analyses at each second that reported the significance of the difference of the means between the compared pair. Then, the proportion of significant difference between each pair was compared among different assay regimens to see if different durations of illumination led to distinct locomotor response compared to another. The analysis pipeline allowed quantitative estimation of the effect of the explanatory variable at each second without losing the dynamics of changes in locomotion and the effect of different assay regimen on locomotion.
The body’s homeostasis represents a point in the continuum of physiological and behavioral states in an organism. Allostasis describes how a homeostatic point is determined in response to the changing environment. The redundancy in the SR ensures an adequate adaptation even if a defective pathway is present. However, the compensatory pathways in achieving allostasis may be biologically expensive over time, eventually leading to maladaptation. In our investigation, the canonical HPA axis (backbone of the stress response) was dispensable to mounting light adaptive phasic responses when the illumination duration was long enough and the intensity was optimal. However, maintaining light adaptive tonic states at the baseline required HPA axis activity. Our findings imply that both phasic and tonic responses of the HPA axis need to be investigated to understand the stress response. Stress-aggravated psychiatric and metabolic disorders may arise not only from the aftereffect of HPA axis activity (hyper- or hypo-cortisolemia) but also from the deficient primary pathways in relevant organ systems where cortisol and its cognate receptors fail to support them in generating optimal adaptive responses.
Acknowledgements
We appreciate the National Institutes of Health for funding this project (R01 GM134732 to KJC), the Division of Engineering at the Mayo Clinic for lending expertise on light power and intensity measurements, and the staff of the Mayo Clinic Library for their literature search service.
Abbreviations
- ACTH
adrenocorticotropic hormone
- CRH
corticotropin releasing hormone
- GAM
generalized additive model
- GC
glucocorticoids; cortisol, corticosterone
- GR
glucocorticoid receptor
- HPA
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal
- HPI
hypothalamic-pituitary-interrenal
- mc2r
melanocortin receptor type 2; ACTH receptor
- MR
mineralocorticoid receptor
- nr3c1
nuclear receptor subfamily 3 group c member 1; glucocorticoid receptor, GR
- nr3c2
nuclear receptor subfamily 3 group c member 2; mineralocorticoid receptor, MR
- SR
stress response
- zeitgeber
environmental signals that entrain the circadian rhythm in an organism
Footnotes
All authors declare no conflict of interest regarding this manuscript.
Supplementary Files
Contributor Information
Han Lee, Mayo Clinic.
Soaleha Shams, Mayo Clinic.
Viet Ha Dang Thi, Mayo Clinic.
Grace Boyum, Mayo Clinic.
Rodsy Modhurima, Mayo Clinic.
Emma Hall, Mayo Clinic.
Izzabella Green, Mayo Clinic.
Elizabeth Cervantes, Mayo Clinic.
Fernando Miguez, Iowa State University.
Karl Clark, Mayo Clinic.
Data availability
All primary data and supplementary materials were deposited in the open access data repository (figshare.com; 10.6084/m9.figshare.23734398). The core R scripts for GAM modeling were deposited to GitHub.com (https://github.com/moonlarkalto/HPA_gam) and the data sets for test-running the R scripts were deposited to FigShare.com in the same link.
References
- 1.Russell G. & Lightman S. The human stress response. Nat Rev Endocrinol 15, 525–534 (2019). 10.1038/s41574-019-0228-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Chrousos G. P. Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nat Rev Endocrinol 5, 374–381 (2009). 10.1038/nrendo.2009.106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Sapolsky R. M., Romero L. M. & Munck A. U. How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocr Rev 21, 55–89 (2000). 10.1210/edrv.21.1.0389 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Nicolaides N. C., Charmandari E., Kino T. & Chrousos G. P Stress-Related and Circadian Secretion and Target Tissue Actions of Glucocorticoids: Impact on Health. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 8, 70 (2017). 10.3389/fendo.2017.00070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Spiga F. & Lightman S. L. Dynamics of adrenal glucocorticoid steroidogenesis in health and disease. Mol Cell Endocrinol 408, 227–234 (2015). 10.1016/j.mce.2015.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Agorastos A. & Chrousos G. P The neuroendocrinology of stress: the stress-related continuum of chronic disease development. Mol Psychiatry 27, 502–513 (2022). 10.1038/s41380-021-01224-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.McEwen B. S. & Lasley E. N. The end of stress as we know it. (Joseph Henry Press, 2002). [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lightman S. L. & Conway-Campbell B. L. The crucial role of pulsatile activity of the HPA axis for continuous dynamic equilibration. Nat Rev Neurosci 11, 710–718 (2010). 10.1038/nrn2914 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Oster H. The interplay between stress, circadian clocks, and energy metabolism. Journal of Endocrinology 247, R13–R25 (2020). 10.1530/joe-20-0124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Spencer R. L., Chun L. E., Hartsock M. J. & Woodruff E. R. Glucocorticoid hormones are both a major circadian signal and major stress signal: How this shared signal contributes to a dynamic relationship between the circadian and stress systems. Front Neuroendocrinol 49, 52–71 (2018). 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.12.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Koch C. E., Leinweber B., Drengberg B. C., Blaum C. & Oster H. Interaction between circadian rhythms and stress. Neurobiol Stress 6, 57–67 (2017). 10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.09.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Leach S. & Suzuki K. Adrenergic Signaling in Circadian Control of Immunity. Front Immunol 11, 1235 (2020). 10.3389/fimmu.2020.01235 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Weger B. D. et al. Extensive Regulation of Diurnal Transcription and Metabolism by Glucocorticoids. PLoS Genet 12, e1006512 (2016). 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006512 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bering T., Hertz H. & Rath M. F. Rhythmic Release of Corticosterone Induces Circadian Clock Gene Expression in the Cerebellum. Neuroendocrinology 110, 604–615 (2020). 10.1159/000503720 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Shimba A. & Ikuta K. Glucocorticoids Regulate Circadian Rhythm of Innate and Adaptive Immunity. Front Immunol 11, 2143 (2020). 10.3389/fimmu.2020.02143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Woodruff E. R., Chun L. E., Hinds L. R. & Spencer R. L. Diurnal Corticosterone Presence and Phase Modulate Clock Gene Expression in the Male Rat Prefrontal Cortex. Endocrinology 157, 1522–1534 (2016). 10.1210/en.2015-1884 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Faught E. & Vijayan M. M. Loss of the glucocorticoid receptor in zebrafish improves muscle glucose availability and increases growth. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 316, E1093–E1104 (2019). 10.1152/ajpendo.00045.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rose A. J. & Herzig S. Metabolic control through glucocorticoid hormones: an update. Mol Cell Endocrinol 380, 65–78 (2013). 10.1016/j.mce.2013.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.McEwen B. S. What Is the Confusion With Cortisol? Chronic Stress (Thousand Oaks) 3 (2019). 10.1177/2470547019833647 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Minnetti M. et al. Fixing the broken clock in adrenal disorders: focus on glucocorticoids and chronotherapy. J Endocrinol 246, R13–R31 (2020). 10.1530/JOE-20-0066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Walker W. H. 2nd, Walton J. C., DeVries A. C. & Nelson R. J. Circadian rhythm disruption and mental health. Transl Psychiatry 10, 28 (2020). 10.1038/s41398-020-0694-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Landgraf D., McCarthy M. J. & Welsh D. K. Circadian clock and stress interactions in the molecular biology of psychiatric disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep 16, 483 (2014). 10.1007/s11920-014-0483-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lee H. B. et al. Novel zebrafish behavioral assay to identify modifiers of the rapid, nongenomic stress response. Genes Brain Behav 18, e12549 (2019). 10.1111/gbb.12549 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schaaf M. J., Chatzopoulou A. & Spaink H. P. The zebrafish as a model system for glucocorticoid receptor research. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol 153, 75–82 (2009). 10.1016/j.cbpa.2008.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kimmel C. B., Ballard W. W., Kimmel S. R., Ullmann B. & Schilling T. F. Stages of embryonic development of the zebrafish. Dev Dyn 203, 253–310 (1995). 10.1002/aja.1002030302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Alsop D. & Vijayan M. The zebrafish stress axis: molecular fallout from the teleost-specific genome duplication event. Gen Comp Endocrinol 161, 62–66 (2009). 10.1016/j.ygcen.2008.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Alsop D. & Vijayan M. M. Development of the corticosteroid stress axis and receptor expression in zebrafish. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 294, R711–719 (2008). 10.1152/ajpregu.00671.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Nesan D. & Vijayan M. M. Role of glucocorticoid in developmental programming: evidence from zebrafish. Gen Comp Endocrinol 181, 35–44 (2013). 10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Yeh C. M. The Basal NPO crh Fluctuation is Sustained Under Compromised Glucocorticoid Signaling in Diurnal Zebrafish. Front Neurosci 9, 436 (2015). 10.3389/fnins.2015.00436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Spence R., Gerlach G., Lawrence C. & Smith C. The behaviour and ecology of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 83, 13–34 (2008). 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00030.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Fernandes A. M. et al. Deep brain photoreceptors control light-seeking behavior in zebrafish larvae. Curr Biol 22, 2042–2047 (2012). 10.1016/j.cub.2012.08.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Burgess H. A., Schoch H. & Granato M. Distinct retinal pathways drive spatial orientation behaviors in zebrafish navigation. Curr Biol 20, 381–386 (2010). 10.1016/j.cub.2010.01.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Horstick E. J., Bayleyen Y., Sinclair J. L. & Burgess H. A. Search strategy is regulated by somatostatin signaling and deep brain photoreceptors in zebrafish. BMC Biol 15, 4 (2017). 10.1186/s12915-016-0346-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Brockerhoff S. E. et al. A behavioral screen for isolating zebrafish mutants with visual system defects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 92, 10545–10549 (1995). 10.1073/pnas.92.23.10545 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Nusslein-Volhard C. & Dahm R. Zebrafish: A practical approach. 1st edn, (Oxford University Press, 2002). [Google Scholar]
- 36.Percie du Sert N. et al. The ARRIVE guidelines 2.0: Updated guidelines for reporting animal research. PLoS Biol 18, e3000410 (2020). 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000410 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Uchida D., Yamashita M., Kitano T. & Iguchi T. Oocyte apoptosis during the transition from ovary-like tissue to testes during sex differentiation of juvenile zebrafish. J Exp Biol 205, 711–718 (2002). 10.1242/jeb.205.6.711 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Takahashi H. Juvenile hermaphroditism in the zebrafish, Brachydanio rerio. Bull Fac Fish Hokkaido Univ 28, 57–65 (1977). [Google Scholar]
- 39.Orban L., Sreenivasan R. & Olsson P E. Long and winding roads: testis differentiation in zebrafish. Mol Cell Endocrinol 312, 35–41 (2009). 10.1016/j.mce.2009.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hartmann S. et al. Zebrafish larvae show negative phototaxis to near-infrared light. PLoS One 13, e0207264 (2018). 10.1371/journal.pone.0207264 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wood N. S. Generalized Additive Models, An Introduction with R. 496 (Taylor & Francis Group, 2017). [Google Scholar]
- 42.R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; (2021). [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wood S. N. Fast Stable Restricted Maximum Likelihood and Marginal Likelihood Estimation of Semiparametric Generalized Linear Models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B: Statistical Methodology 73, 3–36 (2011). 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2010.00749.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mixed GAM computation vehicle with automatic smoothness estimation v. 1.8–42 (2023).
- 45.Lenth R. V. Least-Squares Means: TheRPackagelsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software 69 (2016). 10.18637/jss.v069.i01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wilson E. B. Probable Inference, the Law of Succession, and Statistical Inference. Journal of the American Statistical Association 22 (1927). 10.1080/01621459.1927.10502953 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Newcombe R. G. Two-sided confidence intervals for the single proportion: comparison of seven methods. Statistics in Medicine 17, 857–872 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Newcombe R. G. Interval estimation for the difference between independent proportions: comparison of eleven methods. Statistics in Medicine 17, 873–890 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cahill G. M., Hurd M. W. & Batchelor M. M. Circadian rhythmicity in the locomotor activity of larval zebrafish. Neuroreport 9, 3445–3449 (1998). 10.1097/00001756-199810260-00020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.De Kloet E. R., Vreugdenhil E., Oitzl M. S. & Joels M. Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in health and disease. Endocr Rev 19, 269–301 (1998). 10.1210/edrv.19.3.0331 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.de Kloet E. R., Oitzl M. S. & Joels M. Functional implications of brain corticosteroid receptor diversity. Cell Mol Neurobiol 13, 433–455 (1993). 10.1007/BF00711582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Wingenfeld K. & Otte C. Mineralocorticoid receptor function and cognition in health and disease. Psychoneuroendocrinology 105, 25–35 (2019). 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.09.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jaikumar G., Slabbekoorn H., Sireeni J., Schaaf M. & Tudorache C. The role of the Glucocorticoid Receptor in the Regulation of Diel Rhythmicity. Physiol Behav 223, 112991 (2020). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112991 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Ziv L. et al. An affective disorder in zebrafish with mutation of the glucocorticoid receptor. Mol Psychiatry 18, 681–691 (2013). 10.1038/mp.2012.64 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Roenneberg T. & Foster R. G. Twilight times: light and the circadian system. Photochem Photobiol 66, 549–561 (1997). 10.1111/j.1751-1097.1997.tb03188.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Aschoff J. Exogenous and endogenous components in circadian rhythms. Cold Spring Harb Symp Quant Biol 25, 11–28 (1960). 10.1101/sqb.1960.025.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Manglapus M. K., Iuvone P. M., Underwood H., Pierce M. E. & Barlow R. B. Dopamine mediates circadian rhythms of rod-cone dominance in the Japanese quail retina. J Neurosci 19, 4132–4141 (1999). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Peirson S. N., Halford S. & Foster R. G. The evolution of irradiance detection: melanopsin and the non-visual opsins. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364, 2849–2865 (2009). 10.1098/rstb.2009.0050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.MacPhail R. C. et al. Locomotion in larval zebrafish: Influence of time of day, lighting and ethanol. Neurotoxicology 30, 52–58 (2009). 10.1016/j.neuro.2008.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Padilla S., Hunter D. L., Padnos B., Frady S. & MacPhail R. C. Assessing locomotor activity in larval zebrafish: Influence of extrinsic and intrinsic variables. Neurotoxicol Teratol 33, 624–630 (2011). 10.1016/j.ntt.2011.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.de Esch C. et al. Locomotor activity assay in zebrafish larvae: influence of age, strain and ethanol. Neurotoxicol Teratol 34, 425–433 (2012). 10.1016/j.ntt.2012.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Whiteley A. R. et al. Population genomics of wild and laboratory zebrafish (Danio rerio). Mol Ecol 20, 4259–4276 (2011). 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05272.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Guryev V. et al. Genetic variation in the zebrafish. Genome Res 16, 491–497 (2006). 10.1101/gr.4791006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Burgess H. A. & Granato M. Modulation of locomotor activity in larval zebrafish during light adaptation. J Exp Biol 210, 2526–2539 (2007). 10.1242/jeb.003939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Fernandes A. M., Fero K., Driever W. & Burgess H. A. Enlightening the brain: linking deep brain photoreception with behavior and physiology. Bioessays 35, 775–779 (2013). 10.1002/bies.201300034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Burgess H. A. & Granato M. Sensorimotor gating in larval zebrafish. J Neurosci 27, 4984–4994 (2007). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0615-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Kimmel C. B., Patterson J. & Kimmel R. O. The development and behavioral characteristics of the startle response in the zebra fish. Dev Psychobiol 7, 47–60 (1974). 10.1002/dev.420070109 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Wolman M. & Granato M. Behavioral genetics in larval zebrafish: learning from the young. Dev Neurobiol 72, 366–372 (2012). 10.1002/dneu.20872 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Marques J. C., Lackner S., Felix R. & Orger M. B. Structure of the Zebrafish Locomotor Repertoire Revealed with Unsupervised Behavioral Clustering. Curr Biol 28, 181–195 e185 (2018). 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Heap L. A. L., Vanwalleghem G., Thompson A. W., Favre-Bulle I. A. & Scott E. K. Luminance Changes Drive Directional Startle through a Thalamic Pathway. Neuron 99, 293–301 e294 (2018). 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.06.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Temizer I., Donovan J. C., Baier H. & Semmelhack J. L. A Visual Pathway for Looming-Evoked Escape in Larval Zebrafish. Curr Biol 25, 1823–1834 (2015). 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Lacoste A. M. et al. A convergent and essential interneuron pathway for Mauthner-cell-mediated escapes. Curr Biol 25, 1526–1534 (2015). 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Dunn T. W. et al. Neural Circuits Underlying Visually Evoked Escapes in Larval Zebrafish. Neuron 89, 613–628 (2016). 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Medan V. & Preuss T. The Mauthner-cell circuit of fish as a model system for startle plasticity. J Physiol Paris 108, 129–140 (2014). 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Sarabdjitsingh R. A. & Joels M. Rapid corticosteroid actions on synaptic plasticity in the mouse basolateral amygdala: relevance of recent stress history and beta-adrenergic signaling. Neurobiol Learn Mem 112, 168–175 (2014). 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Joels M., Pasricha N. & Karst H. The interplay between rapid and slow corticosteroid actions in brain. Eur J Pharmacol 719, 44–52 (2013). 10.1016/j.ejphar.2013.07.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Vernocchi S. et al. Membrane glucocorticoid receptor activation induces proteomic changes aligning with classical glucocorticoid effects. Mol Cell Proteomics 12, 1764–1779 (2013). 10.1074/mcp.M112.022947 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Steindal I. A. F. & Whitmore D. Zebrafish Circadian Clock Entrainment and the Importance of Broad Spectral Light Sensitivity. Front Physiol 11, 1002 (2020). 10.3389/fphys.2020.01002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Moore H. A. & Whitmore D. Circadian rhythmicity and light sensitivity of the zebrafish brain. PLoS One 9, e86176 (2014). 10.1371/journal.pone.0086176 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Dekens M. P et al. Light regulates the cell cycle in zebrafish. Curr Biol 13, 2051–2057 (2003). 10.1016/j.cub.2003.10.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Whitmore D., Foulkes N. S. & Sassone-Corsi P. Light acts directly on organs and cells in culture to set the vertebrate circadian clock. Nature 404, 87–91 (2000). 10.1038/35003589 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Whitmore D., Foulkes N. S., Strahle U. & Sassone-Corsi P. Zebrafish Clock rhythmic expression reveals independent peripheral circadian oscillators. Nat Neurosci 1, 701–707 (1998). 10.1038/3703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Herget U. & Ryu S. Coexpression analysis of nine neuropeptides in the neurosecretory preoptic area of larval zebrafish. Front Neuroanat 9, 2 (2015). 10.3389/fnana.2015.00002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Liu Y. et al. Statistical Analysis of Zebrafish Locomotor Behaviour by Generalized Linear Mixed Models. Sci Rep 7, 2937 (2017). 10.1038/s41598-017-02822-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Liu Y. et al. Statistical Analysis of Zebrafish Locomotor Response. PLoS One 10, e0139521 (2015). 10.1371/journal.pone.0139521 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Gauthier P. T. & Vijayan M. M. Nonlinear mixed-modelling discriminates the effect of chemicals and their mixtures on zebrafish behavior. Sci Rep 8, 1999 (2018). 10.1038/s41598-018-20112-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Schmidt A. F. & Finan C. Linear regression and the normality assumption. J Clin Epidemiol 98, 146–151 (2018). 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2017.12.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Ingebretson J. J. & Masino M. A. Quantification of locomotor activity in larval zebrafish: considerations for the design of high-throughput behavioral studies. Front Neural Circuits 7, 109 (2013). 10.3389/fncir.2013.00109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Haigis A. C., Ottermanns R., Schiwy A., Hollert H. & Legradi J. Getting more out of the zebrafish light dark transition test. Chemosphere 295, 133863 (2022). 10.1016/j.chemosphere.2022.133863 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Krzywinski M. & Altman N. Significance, P values and t-tests. Nat Methods 10, 1041–1042 (2013). 10.1038/nmeth.2698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Pek J., Wong O. & Wong A. C. M. How to Address Non-normality: A Taxonomy of Approaches, Reviewed, and Illustrated. Front Psychol 9, 2104 (2018). 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Casson R. J. & Farmer L. D. Understanding and checking the assumptions of linear regression: a primer for medical researchers. Clin Exp Ophthalmol 42, 590–596 (2014). 10.1111/ceo.12358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Lumley T., Diehr P., Emerson S. & Chen L. The importance of the normality assumption in large public health data sets. Annu Rev Public Health 23, 151–169 (2002). 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.100901.140546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Krzywinski M. & Altman N. Points of significance: Importance of being uncertain. Nat Methods 10, 809–810 (2013). 10.1038/nmeth.2613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Kulesa A., Krzywinski M., Blainey P. & Altman N. Sampling distributions and the bootstrap. Nat Methods 12, 477–478 (2015). 10.1038/nmeth.3414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Hsieh J. H. et al. Application of Benchmark Concentration (BMC) Analysis on Zebrafish Data: A New Perspective for Quantifying Toxicity in Alternative Animal Models. Toxicol Sci 167, 92–104 (2019). 10.1093/toxsci/kfy258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Krzywinski M. & Altman N. Points of significance: Nonparametric tests. Nat Methods 11, 467–468 (2014). 10.1038/nmeth.2937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Nuzzo R. Scientific method: statistical errors. Nature 506, 150–152 (2014). 10.1038/506150a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Biau D. J., Jolles B. M. & Porcher R. P value and the theory of hypothesis testing: an explanation for new researchers. Clin Orthop Relat Res 468, 885–892 (2010). 10.1007/s11999-009-1164-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Weissgerber T. L., Milic N. M., Winham S. J. & Garovic V. D. Beyond bar and line graphs: time for a new data presentation paradigm. PLoS Biol 13, e1002128 (2015). 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Sullivan G. M. & Feinn R. Using Effect Size-or Why the P Value Is Not Enough. J Grad Med Educ 4, 279–282 (2012). 10.4300/JGME-D-12-00156.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Altman N. & Krzywinski M. Interpreting P values. Nature Methods 14, 213–214 (2017). 10.1038/nmeth.4210 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Leuthold S. J. et al. Topographic effects on soil microclimate and surface cover crop residue decomposition in rolling cropland. Agriculture, Ecosystems & Environment 320 (2021). 10.1016/j.agee.2021.107609 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Archontoulis S. V. & Miguez F. E. Nonlinear Regression Models and Applications in Agricultural Research. Agronomy Journal 107, 786–798 (2015). 10.2134/agronj2012.0506 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Hastie T. & Tibshirani R. Generalized additive models for medical research. Stat Methods Med Res 4, 187–196 (1995). 10.1177/096228029500400302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All primary data and supplementary materials were deposited in the open access data repository (figshare.com; 10.6084/m9.figshare.23734398). The core R scripts for GAM modeling were deposited to GitHub.com (https://github.com/moonlarkalto/HPA_gam) and the data sets for test-running the R scripts were deposited to FigShare.com in the same link.