Version Changes
Revised. Amendments from Version 1
We have made changes in response to all the reviewer’s comments. We have updated the abstract to make it clearer we are presenting cross-sectional data (collected as part of a wider longitudinal cohort study), rather than longitudinal data. We have also clarified a number of methodological details, corrected one incorrect reference and updated Figure 14 with the correct figure image.
Abstract
Background: Cognitive ability and sensorimotor function are crucial aspects of children’s development, and are associated with physical and mental health outcomes and educational attainment. This paper describes cross-sectional sensorimotor and cognitive function data collected on over 15,000 children aged 7-10 years, collected as part of the Born in Bradford (BiB) longitudinal birth-cohort study. Methodological details of the large-scale data collection process are described, along with initial analyses of the data involving the relationship between cognition/sensorimotor ability and age and task difficulty, and associations between tasks.
Method: Data collection was completed in 86 schools between May 2016 and July 2019. Children were tested at school, individually, using a tablet computer with a digital stylus or finger touch for input. Assessments comprised a battery of three sensorimotor tasks (Tracking, Aiming, & Steering) and five cognitive tasks (three Working Memory tasks, Inhibition, and Processing Speed), which took approximately 40 minutes.
Results: Performance improved with increasing age and decreasing task difficulty, for each task. Performance on all three sensorimotor tasks was correlated, as was performance on the three working memory tasks. In addition, performance on a composite working memory score correlated with performance on both inhibition and processing speed. Interestingly, within age-group variation was much larger than between age-group variation.
Conclusions: The current project collected computerised measures of a range of cognitive and sensorimotor functions at 7-10 years of age in over 15,000 children. Performance varied as expected by age and task difficulty, and showed the predicted correlations between related tasks. Large within-age group variation highlights the need to consider the profile of individual children in studying cognitive and sensorimotor development. These data can be linked to the wider BiB dataset including measures of physical and mental health, biomarkers and genome-wide data, socio-demographic information, and routine data from local health and education services.
Keywords: sensorimotor, cognition, cohort, longitudinal
Introduction
The Born in Bradford (BiB) longitudinal birth cohort study was established in 2007 to explore how behavioural, environmental, social, and genetic factors impact on developmental outcomes, including health and education ( Wright et al., 2013). The study has recruited 13,776 children and their families, who reflect the city’s multi-ethnic population. BiB has produced an extensive, connected dataset including measures of physical and mental health, biomarkers and genome-wide data, socio-demographic information, as well as data linkage to routine data from local health and education services.
In 2011, as the cohort began to approach school age, measurement of children’s cognitive and sensorimotor abilities was of primary importance because these abilities are vital to educational success and to broader developmental outcomes ( Cameron et al., 2012; Giles et al., 2018; Lingam et al., 2012; Marmot & Bell, 2012; Moffit et al., 2011). Poor sensorimotor function and impairments in cognitive ability in childhood are associated with a range of adverse outcomes, such as increased mental health problems, poorer physical health (including increased risk of obesity), lower academic attainment, and poorer quality of life ( Hill et al., 2016; Marteau & Hall, 2013; Stautz et al., 2016; Zwicker et al., 2013). Therefore, having large-scale, objective assessments of these essential skills in primary school children is invaluable to researchers, particularly given the ability to link these with the wide range of connected data available via BiB.
Consequently, between 2012 and 2014 the “Starting School” study recruited over 3,400 BiB children aged 4–5 years who were in their first year of schooling, collecting measures of school readiness including cognition and fine motor skills ( Shire et al., 2020). This was followed in 2017 by the “Growing Up in Bradford” project, when the children were aged 7 to 11 years old. This large-scale multi-method programme of data collection involved children and their families in both the community and schools ( Bird et al., 2019). Within this study, measuring children’s cognitive and sensorimotor function continued to be a core priority, alongside assessing social and emotional wellbeing, growth, adiposity, and cardiometabolic health.
The current paper outlines in detail the recruitment, data collection, and initial results from the school-based measures of cognition and sensorimotor skills, which were undertaken during this project. Further, because this testing was done at a whole-class level it included responses from BiB children and their classmates. This paper focuses on providing the detailed description of the methodology, and some initial, high-level analyses by key variables of this cross-sectional dataset, which will ultimately be linked to the wider BiB cohort data.
Sensorimotor function
Sensorimotor abilities underpin our capacity to execute sensory-guided movements that achieve goal-directed actions ( Tresilian, 2012). As such, they are integral to manual manipulation, tactile and kinaesthetic processing, visual-motor integration and hand-eye coordination ( Goyen et al., 2011; Krakauer & Mazzoni, 2011; Snapp-Childs et al., 2013). Proficient sensorimotor control is essential for a plethora of everyday tasks such as getting dressed, using cutlery, and manipulating a pen or pencil in the classroom ( Miller et al., 2001; Prunty et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2009). These abilities develop throughout childhood ( Flatters et al., 2014; Sugden et al., 2013), with theory suggesting that competent sensorimotor control is vital for one’s ability to interact with, and understand the environment through “purposeful, coordinated movements” ( Latash, 2012, p. 1; Piaget, 1952). For example, basic sensorimotor abilities in infancy and early childhood are associated with emerging cognitive abilities, such as sustained and joint visual attention and inhibitory control ( D’Souza et al., 2017; Gottwald et al., 2016; Yu & Smith, 2017; Yuan et al., 2019). There is also evidence for the importance of sensorimotor function later in childhood with studies showing the importance of the role of action in learning processes and the retention of information ( Gathercole et al., 2008; James, 2017; LeBarton et al., 2015; Waterman et al., 2017; Yang et al., 2017).
Meanwhile, difficulties with sensorimotor control are documented as limiting the development of perception, cognition and motivation ( Leonard, 2016; von Hofsten, 2004). Coordination difficulties are also associated with an abundance of adverse developmental outcomes, including lower levels of physical activity ( Kwan et al., 2016), obesity ( Augustijn et al., 2018), social and emotional wellbeing ( Hill et al., 2016), quality of life ( Zwicker et al., 2013), and academic attainment ( Harrowell et al., 2017).
Moreover, whilst clinically significant problems with sensorimotor control are associated with a concurrent increased risks of mental health problems ( Lingam et al., 2012), difficulties with sensorimotor control are also frequently found to co-occur in children already diagnosed with a range of other specific genetic ( Cunningham et al., 2019; Cunningham et al., 2020) and developmental disorders ( Pieters et al., 2012).
The current project assessed sensorimotor function by recording end-point kinematic data on participants’ movements as they performed the Clinical Kinematic Assessment Tool (CKAT) ( Culmer et al., 2009). This standardised computerised battery has participants performing a series of visuo-manual tasks that require them to use a handheld stylus to interact with 2D visual stimuli presented on a tablet computer. CKAT has previously been used to study sensorimotor function in both typically ( Flatters et al., 2014) and atypically ( Cunningham et al., 2019) developing populations and was previously used to measure sensorimotor function at 5 years old in a subset of the BiB cohort ( Shire et al., 2020). Performance on tasks within the CKAT battery have also been shown to be correlate with performance on other sensorimotor tasks, such as: steering in a virtual reality driving simulator ( Raw et al., 2012) and maintaining postural stability ( Flatters et al., 2014).
Cognitive function
Cognitive skills are equally essential for academic success ( Ahmed et al., 2019; Alloway et al., 2009; Blakey et al., 2020; Blair & Razza, 2007; Lee et al., 2011) and are linked to outcomes such as social functioning ( McQuade et al., 2013) and long term health ( Marteau & Hall, 2013; Stautz et al., 2016). In the current study we assessed three core components of cognition: working memory, inhibitory control, and processing speed. The rationale for focussing on each of these areas is discussed in the following sections.
Working memory. Working memory is a limited capacity system that is used to store and process information for immediate use in ongoing cognitive activity ( Baddeley et al., 2021; Cowan, 1999). Working memory ability increases throughout childhood and adolescence, reaching adult-like levels at approximately 15 years of age ( Gathercole et al., 2004). Working memory is essential for learning and predicts educational achievement including attainment in reading, mathematics and science ( Alloway et al., 2014; Cragg et al., 2017; Gathercole et al., 2004; Holmes & Adams, 2006; Monette et al., 2011; Swanson et al., 2006). Impairments to working memory also co-occur with several developmental disorders, for example, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), developmental coordination disorder (DCD), and dyslexia ( Alloway & Archibald, 2008; Beneventi et al., 2010; Martinussen et al., 2005; Smith-Spark & Fisk, 2007). We measured working memory using three separate tasks: forward digit recall (FDR), the Corsi task, and backward digit recall (BDR). FDR measures the ability to retain verbal information, Corsi measures the ability to retain visuospatial information, and BDR taps into the ability to engage in executive control of information. Each of these tasks is used extensively in the working memory literature ( Alloway et al., 2006; Berry et al., 2018; Berry et al., 2019; Gathercole et al., 2004; Waterman et al., 2017).
Inhibitory control. Inhibitory control refers to the ability to suppress prepotent responses and ignore irrelevant or distracting information. It is one of the central constructs of executive function ( Diamond, 2013). Indeed, some researchers have argued that working memory and inhibition are the two key factors underlying executive function ( Roberts & Pennington, 1996; Senn et al., 2004). The ability to inhibit irrelevant information and actions increases over childhood ( Diamond & Taylor, 1996; Durston et al., 2002; Williams et al., 1999). Improved inhibitory control is linked to academic attainment ( Blair & Razza, 2007; Kieffer et al., 2013; Szucs et al., 2013) and to broader long-term health outcomes such as body mass index (BMI) and alcohol use ( Marteau & Hall, 2013; Moffit et al., 2011; Stautz et al., 2016). The inhibition task we used was based on a classic Flanker design ( Blakey & Carroll, 2015; Eriksen & Schulz, 1979; Rueda et al., 2005). This involves inhibition of irrelevant stimuli whilst attending to the relevant stimulus (attentional inhibition) and is sometimes referred to as a selective attention task ( Diamond, 2013).
Processing speed. Processing speed is a fundamental part of cognition ( Kail & Salthouse, 1994), enabling increased efficiency and improved performance on other cognitive tasks. Speed of processing increases considerably during early and middle childhood, with rate of increase slowing during late childhood, and then stabilising by late adolescence ( Kail, 1991; Kail & Ferrer, 2007). Improved processing speed is linked to broader academic attainment ( Gordon et al., 2018; Mulder et al., 2010; Rohde & Thompson, 2007), as well as to reading ( Kail & Hall, 1994; Lobier et al., 2013) and mathematics ( Geary, 2011; Geary & Brown, 1991). Poor processing speed also co-occurs with many learning difficulties such as ADHD and Autism ( Dickerson Mayes & Calhoun, 2007). The measure used in the current study was based on a speeded counting task methodology that has been used previously ( Fry & Hale, 1996; Weiler et al., 2003).
General aims and hypotheses
The current project aimed to collect data on sensorimotor and cognitive function, using objective computerised measures, on approximately 6,000 BiB children attending Bradford Primary Schools (covering ages 7–11 years) as well as their classmates (N ~ 9,000). Cross-sectional data on this scale on children’s sensorimotor and cognitive functions is, in and of itself, valuable to researchers interested in exploring the relationships between these domains. Moreover, for BiB children involved in this study their data can also be linked to the further genetic, environmental, demographic, socioemotional, health, and educational data collected as part of the broader cohort study. Thus, providing opportunities to improve our understanding of the complex interplay of factors affecting child development.
The current paper describes the data collection process for the school-based assessment of sensorimotor and cognitive function in detail and presents an initial analysis of the data. The key research questions studied in relation to each task are:
-
1.
How does performance change with age?
-
2.
How does performance change with increasing difficulty within each task?
-
3.
What are the associations between performance across the different tasks, within each of the two key domains (I.e. cognition and sensorimotor ability)?
Methods
Setting
Dates. Data collection for the study took place across four academic years (May – July 2016 and September – July 2016/17, 2017/18, and 2018/2019).
Location. Over the period of data collection, the Bradford District (West Yorkshire) was the fifth-largest metropolitan district in the UK (over half a million people), with nearly a quarter (23.7%) of residents aged under 16 years old ( Office for National Statistics, 2019). Over the years, between 22 and 29% of the district's children were classified as living in Poverty ( Office for National Statistics, 2019; The English Indices of Deprivation, 2019). During this time, around 64% of the Bradford District’s population self-identified as being of White British ethnic origin, with a further 20% identifying instead as Pakistani.
Participants
Schools. Eligibility: Schools in the Bradford district were initially invited to participate based on whether they had participated in a previous study called ‘Starting School’ that was carried out when the BiB children were in Reception year of school (ages 4–5) ( Shire et al., 2020). Additional schools were invited based on the team’s capacity, starting with those schools that had the highest numbers of children attending who were already part of the BiB study.
Method of recruitment: BiB researchers sent out individual emails addressed to Head teachers (or a key member of staff identified during previous years’ recruitment), with an information sheet attached. If no response was received, telephone calls were made and, if desired and feasible, a member of the research team would organise to meet a member of staff to explain the project. Recruitment of schools recurred annually over the 4 years of the study and if schools did not respond one academic year they were still contacted again in subsequent years, unless they requested otherwise. Head teachers had to provide written, opt-in consent on behalf of their school in order to participate.
Children. Eligibility: the intention was to assess children as close as possible to when they were 8 years old. There were three stages to recruitment. During a pilot phase between May 2016 and July 2016, only children in Year 4 (ages 8–9 years old) were invited to take part. In the main phase of recruitment, between September 2016 and July 2018, children in Years 3 (ages 7–8 years old) and 4 were eligible. If we had visited a school in the previous year, we would only capture data from the new Year 3 children the next academic year. In the final phase of recruitment (September 2018 – July 2019) children in Years 3, 4 and 5 were eligible in newly recruited schools for that year.
Method of recruitment: Recruited schools were given information sheets and opt-out consent forms to distribute to parents of children in the eligible year groups, which went out one to two weeks ahead of the school visit. The opt-out consent approach had been successfully used in the previous BiB Starting School study ( Shire et al., 2020), where it was chosen due to the low-risk nature of participation, the high numbers of children targeted, and the risks of excluding groups of children from homes where school forms are frequently not returned. One school asked for opt-in consent forms to be used and these were provided.
Parents were asked to return the opt-out consent forms to the school, and these were collected by researchers at the start of a visit. Only a small minority of parents withdrew their child from the study. For example, in the final phase of recruitment (Sept 18 – July 19), out of 5570 parents of children approached, 252 opted-out of the study (4%). Two further pathways by which a child could be withdrawn from the study also existed. Firstly, on a few occasions, parents would verbally inform their child’s teacher that they did not wish their child to take part. In these circumstances the teacher’s verbal account of opt-out for that child was recorded, and the child was not assessed. Secondly, Teachers would also occasionally inform the researchers that they felt that a child’s special education needs (SEN) would prevent them from participating; we took the teachers decision on whether the child should participate or not. Assent from the children was also obtained before every assessment to ensure the child was happy to take part. Again, refusal at this stage was also rare. In the final phase of recruitment mentioned previously, only six children refused to take part in the assessments.
Design and general procedure
Ethical approval. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the NHS Health Research Authority’s Yorkshire and the Humber - Bradford Leeds Research Ethics Committee (reference: 16/YH/0062) on the 24 th March 2016.
Design and measurements. For both the sensorimotor and cognitive measures, data collection for this cross-sectional study took place during school time. All the assessments were presented on a tablet computer (Lenovo ThinkPad Helix type 20CG or 20CH) that used a digital stylus and finger touch for input. The children were presented with the sensorimotor tasks first (Tracking, Aiming and Steering) followed by the cognitive tasks (Forwards Digit Recall, Backwards Digit Recall, Corsi Block test, Flanker Task, and Processing Speed). The procedure was identical to the one used for the same tablet-based sensorimotor assessments during the Starting Schools study (methods described in Shire et al., 2020). The only difference being an additional 20–25 minutes of cognitive tasks on the same tablet computers. The children interacted with the visual stimuli on the touch screen tablet using a digital stylus for the sensorimotor tasks and finger touch for the cognitive tasks. Tasks were presented in the same order to all children, following standardised instructions and a practice trial.
We offered schools the opportunity for both BiB children and their classmates (non-BiB children) to take part in the study. It was up to the school's discretion whether they preferred just BiB children to take part, to minimise the impact on the school day. In the majority of cases, schools opted for both BiB children and non-BiB children to be tested.
Procedure. Once the school consented to participate, they were asked to provide class lists ahead of the school visit that included: children’s name; date of birth; Unique Pupil Number (UPN) 1 ; home post code; gender and ethnicity. This information was used to allow us to identify children eligible for assessment within their classes and to link the data collected back to the wider BiB cohort database, for those children who were part of the cohort. It also provided a means to resolving data entry errors (e.g. if the UPN was missing then linkage could be made using other identifiable information, reducing loss of data). Data was collected and stored securely following the Bradford Teaching Hospitals Foundation Trust procedures, which helps to protect all the BiB studies and is in accordance with the Data Protection Act 1998. Transfer of data onto the secure central archive at the BiB research office was via encrypted devices (e.g. tablets, memory sticks) or encrypted emails.
Schools were asked to allocate a quiet room or area within their premises for the research team to use to conduct their assessments (e.g. an empty classroom). Typically, a team of five researchers took 10 children out of class at a time, with a ratio of no more than two children to one assessor. The assessor would administer the sensorimotor and cognitive assessments to the children, explaining what was required of each task at the beginning of each assessment and ensuring the child understood. Assessments took approximately 40–45 minutes per child.
Senior staff within the BiB cohort study trained the assessment team. Refresher training was provided at the start of each academic year and when required throughout the year to ensure high compliance with procedural protocols.
Feedback to schools. The results of the sensorimotor and cognitive assessments were compiled into individual reports for each child, and delivered back to class teachers, along with a document explaining why these skills were developmentally important, how the children were assessed, what was measured, what the scores meant and how they should be interpreted. These feedback documents were used at the discretion of the school, typically in conjunction with their own data on each child, to consider possible further assessment and evaluation for some children.
Sensorimotor methods
Materials and procedure. The Clinical-Kinematic Assessment Tool (CKAT), an objective computerised assessment implemented using the software development environment LabVIEW (Version 8.2.1, NationalInstruments TM), was used to record sensorimotor function (see Culmer et al., 2009 for full technical details). This battery contains three tasks (tracking, aiming and steering), and takes approximately 12–15 minutes in total to administer. In all tasks, the children used a pen-shaped stylus to interact with the touchscreen tablet and movement of the stylus is recorded at 120 Hz, allowing task-relevant kinematic features of these movements to then be calculated. For example, temporal indices can measure the elapsed time between presentation of on-screen stimuli and consequent actions being initiated (Reaction Time; RT); and then the time between initiation and the end of the subsequent action (movement time; MT). Combining RT and MT then gives a measure of Total Response Time (TRT). Efficiency and accuracy of movements are described using spatial indices. These includes Path Accuracy, which compares the recorded trajectory produced by participant to a model trajectory. Finally, dynamic indices summarise speed/accuracy trade-offs in sensorimotor control. For example, root-mean-square error (RMSE) measures average spatial error whilst tracking a target over time, and penalised Path Accuracy (pPA) records path accuracy after adjusting for length of movement time. Consistent with procedures in Flatters et al. (2014), one kinematic metric per CKAT task was selected, to be used as an index of task performance (RMSE for Tracking, Total Response Time for Aiming and Penalised Path Accuracy for Steering). A brief description of each task follows here, along with illustration of these tasks in Figure 1– Figure 3. For a fuller description see Flatters et al., 2014:
Tracking
A moving green circle is presented on the screen and participants are required to track the circle around the screen, keeping the tip of the stylus within the circle (which moved in a ‘figure-of-eight’ pattern for nine revolutions). The speed of the moving circle increases every three revolutions, producing Slow, Medium, and Fast conditions (42, 84, and 168 mm/s, respectively). This task comprises of two consecutive conditions; an unguided condition (Without Guide) where the path could not be seen and a guided trial (With Guide; where the ‘figure-of-eight’ is displayed on the screen). Each trial lasts approximately 84 s. See Figure 1 for illustration.
Aiming
A series of circles are presented on the screen and the child is required to move the stylus as quickly as possible (without disconnecting the stylus from the screen) from one green circle to the next, as they appear. Each time the tip of the stylus reaches a circle, that circle disappears, and a new target circle appears in a new location. A total of 75 discrete aiming movements are made taking approximately 2–4 minutes. In six of the final 25 movements the target shifted position (referred to Jump trials) after the participant initiated movement and got within 4 cm of the initially aimed at target, forcing the participant to make an online correction to ‘jump’ to the new target location. All targets appeared at a fixed distance from each other. See Figure 2 for illustration.
Steering
The steering task comprised of two trials, Path A and Path B (Path B was the reverse order of path A) and each took approximately 40 s to complete. This task was shortened to two trials instead of six, compared to the version used in Flatters et al., 2014, due to time constraints. In this earlier study this task is also referred to as Tracing rather than Steering. A 5-mm-wide pathway consisting of two parallel lines was presented on the screen, running from a ‘start’ to a ‘finish’ position on the other side of the screen. Children are instructed to keep the tip of the stylus inside the pathway whilst also keeping their stylus within a ‘pacing box’ (sequentially moved along the path in 5-s intervals). This pacing box attempts to standardise the pace of movement and encourage a greater focus on accuracy during this set time period. See Figure 3 for illustration.
Domain-specific hypotheses. Consistent with response patterns reported previously in a much smaller sample of 7–11-year-olds (see Flatters et al., 2014), it was predicted that with increasing age, performance across all tasks would significantly improve. For Tracking specifically, improved RMSE was also predicted within the guided condition and at slower target speeds. These effects were also expected to interact with each other, whereby the positive effect of including the visual guide would be larger at slower speeds and this advantage would be more evident in older age groups. Within the Aiming task specifically, slower total response time was expected within Jump trials compared to ‘baseline’ trials, where no online correction was required. However, no interaction of this effect with age was predicted. Lastly, in the Steering task only an effect of age was predicted, with no difference in Penalised Path Accuracy as a function of condition expected (Path A versus Path B). Lastly, it was expected that there would be a significant correlation between performance on each of the three sensorimotor tasks.
Cognitive methods
Materials and procedure. Working memory, inhibition and processing speed were assessed through five cognitive tasks, which took approximately 20–25 minutes to complete. Working memory was assessed using three different tasks: Forward Digit Recall (FDR), Backwards Digit Recall (BDR) and the Corsi task (Corsi). Inhibition was assessed using a Flanker task. The order of presentation of these tasks were as follows: FDR, BDR , Corsi, Flanker task, and Processing Speed (PS). All cognitive tasks were programmed using PsychoPy ( Peirce, 2007).
FDR
Children were presented with a sequence of numbers through headphones and subsequently asked to recall these numbers in the order they were audibly presented, by touching the appropriate boxes on the screen in order (see Figure 4). Nine boxes were ordered sequentially from 1 to 9 on the screen. The tasks progressed from sequence length three to six, with four trials for each sequence length, with a total of 16 trials. Response accuracy (correct or incorrect) and reaction time (s) was recorded for each trial. For each sequence length, the primary outcome variable was mean proportion correct. Reaction time per response was also recorded but is not reported here.
BDR
This task was similar to FDR but this time children were asked to recall the numbers in reverse order. As this task is more difficult than FDR, sequence length started at two digits and increased to sequence length five, with four trials at each length. The same outcome variables were recorded for this task as for FDR.
Corsi block tapping
Children were presented with nine randomly arranged blue squares, in which a random and unique sequence of boxes flashed yellow. The task was for the child to remember the order and once the sequence was finished, to tap the blue boxes in the order in which the yellow boxes flashed (see Figure 5). Sequence length increased as the task progressed, from three squares to six squares, with four standardised sequences presented for each sequence length, equalling a total of 16 trials. Both response accuracy (correct and incorrect) and reaction time (s) were recorded for each item. Mean proportion correct was the primary outcome. As with the digit recall tasks, reaction time per response was also recorded but is not reported here.
Inhibition (Flanker task)
A line of five arrows was presented to the children in the centre of the screen. Children were required to identify the direction of the middle arrow, whilst the surrounding four arrows were either pointing in the same (congruous) or an opposite (incongruous) direction to the middle arrow (see Figure 6). There was a total of four practice trials and 40 test trials. In each case, these were equally balanced between congruous and incongruous trials, with an equal number to the left and right. The children were asked to answer as quickly and accurately as possible. Accuracy and RT were captured, with the primary outcome variable being: Mean (RT to congruous trials – RT to incongruous trials).
Processing speed
Children were asked to identify how many red circles were present on the screen, amongst a random number of red triangles and blue circles, and to respond by tapping the box located at the bottom of the screen containing the correct number (see Figure 7). The boxes at the bottom of the screen included number options 1–9. There was a total of 18 trials and the children were asked to carry out each trial at quickly and as accurately as possible. Response accuracy (correct and incorrect) was recorded for each trial as well as RT (s). The primary outcome variable was mean RT for correct trials.
Domain-specific hypotheses. For the three working memory tasks, accuracy was expected to improve with age (e.g. Gathercole, 1999; Gathercole et al., 2004). Due to the increased processing demands involved in reversing the digit sequence for recall (e.g. Alloway et al., 2006), BDR was expected to be more difficult than FDR or Corsi and result in lower accuracy overall ( Anders & Lillyquist, 1971). For each of these three tasks, performance was predicted to decline as sequence length increased, in line with the view of working memory as a limited capacity system (e.g. Cowan, 2001).
For the inhibition and processing speed tasks, children were expected to show faster reaction times with increasing age. Accuracy was a secondary dependent variable in these tasks and was expected to be high, but with similar age-related improvements apparent. For the inhibition task, responses were predicted to be faster (and more accurate) for congruent trials, relative to incongruent trials ( Blakey & Carroll, 2015; Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974; Mullane et al., 2009). This was expected to be more apparent for the younger age groups, indicating developmental changes in executive control and inhibition with age ( Diamond, 2013).
Finally, the interrelationships between the different tasks at an individual differences level was explored. We expected positive correlations between each of the working memory measures, particularly for FDR and BDR, in line with the concept of shared processing components alongside domain-specific storage ( Alloway et al., 2006). We also predicted positive correlations between a working memory composite score, reaction time on the inhibition and processing speed tasks, and the magnitude of the congruency advantage within the inhibition task.
Data analysis
Data cleaning and preparation. Prior to analyses, the sensorimotor and cognitive data collected was reviewed for quality control purposes. Sessions of recorded data were omitted for one of three primary reasons: duplication; incompleteness or issues raised in an accompanying field-note 2 .
Duplicated sessions were cases in which the identifying information for the participant was the same across multiple sessions, this sometimes also included the recorded cognitive and/or sensorimotor data also being identical. In cases of complete duplications (i.e. identifying information and recorded data were identical), one session was retained, and the other(s) omitted. In cases where only the identifying information but not the data were identical this likely occurred as a result of human error in data entry and so both/all occurrences were omitted. An exception to this was where a child began but did not complete data collection within one session but continued it in a second session. In these cases, if there was also a field note confirming this was the case, then these two sessions were combined.
Incomplete cases arose where data was missing for some tasks or some trials within tasks. Participants had to have complete data for a task for it to be included in analysis. For example, if trials were incomplete or missing entirely for a participant’s Tracking task but were complete for their Aiming, Steering, and Cognitive Tasks then only their Tracking data would be omitted. For this reason, the sample sizes for each task varies somewhat, presented in Table 1.
Table 1. Number of participants with data for each of the Sensorimotor and Cognitive tasks, respectively stratified by age group.
Total
sample |
7
Years |
8
Years |
9
Years |
10
Years |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sensorimotor | |||||
Tracking | 15701 | 5217 | 6985 | 3203 | 296 |
Aiming | 15727 | 5230 | 6995 | 3206 | 296 |
Steering | 15720 | 5217 | 6998 | 3208 | 297 |
Cognitive | |||||
FDR | 15473 | 5154 | 6870 | 3152 | 297 |
BDR | 15146 | 5000 | 6725 | 3126 | 295 |
Corsi | 15366 | 5095 | 6826 | 3146 | 299 |
Inhibition | 14051 | 4634 | 6199 | 2927 | 291 |
Processing
Speed |
14933 | 4915 | 6633 | 3086 | 299 |
FDR=forward digit recall; BDR=backwards digit recall
Lastly, field notes were inspected and judged on a case by case basis by two researchers, with data from these sessions excluded if either researcher they felt the nature of the note warranted exclusion. Examples of notes which led to exclusion include technical difficulties (i.e. tablet crashes) or indications that participants were non-compliant with instructions.
Each dataset was analysed using mixed methods ANOVAs with Bonferroni-corrected planned comparisons examining differences between age groups. This was then supplemented by correlational analyses between separate sub-tests within both of the two key domains (sensorimotor and cognitive).
Results
School recruitment
Over the four testing periods, 86 schools in Bradford participated (see Figure 8 for breakdown of number of schools per year). Where schools were approached, but did not actively respond, we were unable to capture reasons for non-recruitment and were not counted as ‘declining’. In the school year 2016/2017, two schools consented and visits arranged but these were subsequently cancelled by the schools (due to other demands). These were unable to be re-booked that year due to capacity of the research team, but all three schools were all visited the following year. A total of 12 schools took part in all four testing periods, 35 in three of the periods, 23 in two, and 16 in only one testing period.
Child recruitment
Over all three phases of testing, 17,774 children were recruited to take part (i.e. were in an eligible year group in a consenting school, and whose parents had not opted them out of the study), see Table 2 for a breakdown of children consented each year.
Table 2. Numbers of children recruited and subsequently assessed, by year.
School year | Sample | Children
recruited |
Children
assessed |
% of total
assessed |
---|---|---|---|---|
2015–2016 | BiB | 832 | 717 | 86.18 |
Non-BiB | 110 | 102 | 92.73 | |
Total | 942 | 819 | 86.94 | |
2016–2017 | BiB | 3189 | 2821 | 88.46 |
Non-BiB | 2773 | 2490 | 89.79 | |
Total | 5962 | 5311 | 89.08 | |
2017–2018 | BiB | 3015 | 2671 | 88.59 |
Non-BiB | 2537 | 2285 | 90.07 | |
Total | 5552 | 4956 | 89.27 | |
2018–2019 | BiB | 3856 | 3395 | 88.04 |
Non-BiB | 1462 | 1339 | 91.59 | |
Total | 5318 | 4734 | 89.02 | |
Total | BiB | 10892 | 9604 | 88.17 |
Non-BiB | 6882 | 6216 | 90.32 | |
Total | 17774 | 15820 | 89.01 |
BiB=Born in Bradford cohort
Not all of these children completed the testing, however, due to either being absent on the day, refusing to take part, or through there being not enough time during the visit to complete testing with all the children. A breakdown of number of children falling into each of these groups was only captured during the final phase of testing (school year 2018–2019). In this year, 242 children were absent (62 BiB), 6 children refused to take part (3 BiB), and 69 (12 BiB) were not tested due to running out of time during the school visit.
Sensorimotor measures
See the data availability statement for the link to the script used to derive the three variables used within this paper (RMSE, TRT and pPA) from the raw output produced by CKAT. Additional processing of these variables was then conducted (negative reciprocal transformation) to normalise the distribution.
Tracking. A 2x3x4 mixed ANOVA was conducted with condition (With Guide versus Without Guide) and speed (Slow, Medium, and Fast) as within-groups factors and age-group (7-, 8-, 9- and 10-year olds) as the between-subjects factor. Significant main effects (all p<.001) of age-group ( = .063), condition ( = .004), and speed ( = .642) on negative reciprocal RMSE were noted, as was a significant three-way interaction ( = .001), illustrated in Figure 9.
Age group comparisons (Bonferroni-corrected) indicated significant differences between all age groups, with greater performance for 10-year-olds (M = .112, SE = .001), compared to 9-year-olds (M = .101, SE < .001), 8-year-olds (M = .091, SE < .001), and 7-year-olds (M = -.084, SE < .001). Speed related effects were attenuated in younger age groups and for 7-year-olds and in younger age groups there was little effect of condition in any of the three speeds. However, with increasing age, there were larger effects of condition, supporting the significant 3-way interaction that was found.
Aiming. A 2x4 mixed ANOVA was carried out with condition as the within-groups factor (Baseline versus Jump) and age group as the between-subjects factor. This revealed significant main effects (at p < .001) of age group ( =.078) and condition ( =.910) and the age group x condition interaction ( =.048).
Further comparisons (Bonferroni-corrected) revealed that performance between all age groups significantly differed from each other with 10-year-old children performing the best. Age group effects observed were larger within the Baseline condition compared to the Jump condition, explaining the significant age-group x condition interaction found, illustrated in Figure 10. In addition, greater differences were found between the Baseline and Jump conditions with increasing age.
Steering. A similar 2x4 mixed ANOVA was conducted for the Steering task with condition as the within-groups factor (Path A versus Path B) and age group as the between-subjects factor. Significant main effects (at p <.001) were found for age group ( =.032) and condition ( =.003) However, a significant interaction was not found ( p = .269).
Further comparisons (Bonferroni-corrected) showed performance across groups significantly differed from each other (all p <.001 except between 9-year-olds and 10-year-olds, p <.01). Whilst children were found to perform significantly better on Path A compared to Path B ( p <.001) ( Figure 11), the effect size was comparatively small, indicating minimal impact in comparison to the effect of age on performance.
Relationships across sensorimotor tasks. Correlational analyses were conducted to observe how performance is related across the three CKAT tasks. Figure 12 show the interrelationships between Tracking, Aiming, and Steering. Positive correlations (significant at p <.001) are apparent between all three sensorimotor tasks.
Cognitive measures
Working memory. Performance on each of the working memory tasks was scored as the mean proportion of correct responses across all trials in the task. This is presented in Figure 13 for each task and age group. Age differences are formally analysed as part of the subsequent section examining sequence length, but it is clear that performance improves with age in each task. There is also substantial variation in performance within each age group, as indicated by the large SDs and spread of individual data points. Finally, the tasks themselves differ in performance levels, with FDR generally superior to Corsi. As befitting its status as a complex WM task, recall accuracy was lowest in BDR, even though this measure used a shorter range of sequence lengths.
Sequence length: Performance was then examined as a function of sequence length, with mean performance for each task and age group illustrated in Figure 14. A set of 4x4 mixed ANOVA were carried out on each cognitive task, with sequence length as the within-subjects factor and age group as the between-subjects factor. For all three tasks, this revealed significant effects (at p < .001) of age group ( = FDR .03, BDR .05, Corsi .05), sequence length ( = FDR .44, BDR .28, Corsi .27) and the age group x sequence length interaction ( = .01 for all tasks). Thus, recall performance improved with age, and declined with sequence length, for all measures. Comparisons (Bonferroni-corrected) revealed that all age groups differed from each other at all lengths, with the exception of the FDR task, where there were no significant age differences at length 3 after correction, or between age 9 and 10 at certain sequence lengths in each task ( p >.05). In general, age group effects were somewhat attenuated at the shortest sequence length for each task, indicating the higher performance levels at the easiest level of each task, and manifesting in the age group x sequence length interactions that were observed.
Inhibition (Flanker). The inhibition task data were first trimmed by removing any reaction time that fell >3 SD above the mean across all participants and conditions. We also removed any child who achieved less than .25 correct across all trials. This resulted in the exclusion of 110 children from the sample, leaving N = 14,425.
Mean proportion correct and reaction time for correct responses are illustrated in Figure 15a and 15b, respectively. As expected, accuracy was typically very high on this task. One-way ANOVAs on each of these outcomes indicated that accuracy and response speed improved with age (p < .001) , with comparisons showing significant differences ( p < .05) between all age groups (apart from age 9 and 10 in RT).
The primary outcome of interest on this task is the difference between RTs on congruent and incongruent trials (RT congruency effect), although we also investigated differences in accuracy between congruent and incongruent trials (accuracy congruency effect). Overall, responses were more accurate (proportion correct difference = .05, SE = .001) and faster (mean RT difference = 199ms, SE = 2.81) for congruent trials. This is plotted, by age group, in Figure 15, for accuracy (c) and reaction time (d). One-way ANOVAs showed that the congruency effect on each of these outcome measures decreased with age ( p < .001); further comparisons showed significant differences ( p < .05) for both measures between all age groups apart from age 9 and 10.
Processing speed. The processing speed task data was trimmed by removing any reaction time that fell >3 SD above the mean across all participants. Mean proportion correct, and reaction time for correct responses are illustrated in Figure 16. The primary outcome variable of interest on this measure is reaction time to correct trials, although we also measured accuracy which was, as expected, typically very high. A one-way ANOVA on each outcome variable indicated significant age group effects in each case (p < .001) ; comparisons indicated all age groups significantly differed from each other (p < .05) , with only two exceptions (on proportion correct, 7–8 years, and 9–10 years).
Relationships across cognitive tasks. Correlational analysis was carried out to examine the relationship between performance on different tasks. These were not corrected for age, though partial correlations controlling for age showed the same pattern of outcomes. Figure 17 shows the interrelationships between FDR, BDR, and Corsi. Positive correlations (significant at p < .001) are apparent between all measures.
A composite working memory score was then developed (using the multicon package in R) to examine the relationship with the reaction time outcomes from the inhibition and processing speed tasks. Figure 18 indicates a significant negative relationship (p < .001 for all outcomes) between working memory and inhibition RT, the inhibition congruent-incongruent RT difference, and processing speed RT.
Discussion
In collecting data on the sensorimotor and cognitive performance from over 6,200 children participating in the Born in Bradford cohort, and a further 9,500 of their classmates, the Growing Up in Bradford project has succeeded in obtaining objective measures of these important aspects of cognitive and motoric functioning at a critical stage in children’s development. This initial paper established the methodological details of this data collection and illustrated that performance on all the assessments showed the changes one would expect in response to age and task difficulty. The predicted associations between tests of related sensorimotor functions, or cognitive abilities, were also observed. Furthermore, while these cross-sectional comparisons fit with the established literature on age-relevant cognitive and sensorimotor development, they also demonstrated heterogeneity within age groups. Indeed, it was apparent that within age-group variation was much larger than between age-group variation, particularly for certain tasks (e.g., working memory). This highlights the need to consider the profile of individual children in studying cognitive and sensorimotor development. It also demonstrates the challenges faced by teachers who may have children within a class that have, for example, a WM ability equivalent to a child several years younger through to several years older than the average age of the class.
Sensorimotor tasks
The present analyses report findings that are consistent with our initial hypotheses and previous work conducted by Flatters et al. (2014), who also found that with increasing age performance across all three tasks increased, whilst increasing task difficulty decreased performance across all three tasks.
Specifically, previous work has demonstrated a significant three-way interaction between age, speed and condition for tracking ( Flatters et al., 2014). Similar to our own findings, they also found that the benefit of slower target speed was greater with increasing age. When the target was moving at the fastest speed, age-related differences were still found but to a lesser extent than at the slower target speeds. This concurs with the view that increasing target speed is believed to influence shift away from feed back to feed forward control mechanisms for guiding action, which places increased reliance on predicting future target trajectory rather than using available online visual feedback; a more complex skill ( Ao et al., 2015; van Roon et al., 2008; Wolpert & Kawato, 1998). In this context, the benefits to be gained from providing additional visual feedback in the guide condition might be somewhat undermined by the need to process this additional information in a timely manner, leading to more reliance on feedforward responses.
Findings from the Aiming task were generally consistent with Flatters et al. (2014), who also found reduced age-related effects within the Jump trials. However, they did not find a significant interaction. Speculating on why such an interaction may have arisen in this dataset, it is instructive to note that whilst children possess the ability to produce on-line corrective movements from around eight years this is not believed to be fully automated until late childhood ( Mackrous & Proteau, 2016; Wilson & Hyde, 2013). Therefore, our data is likely reflective of both typical corrective aiming movement showing improvement across this specific age range, but at a slower rate for, more challenging, corrective movements. It would certainly be of interest in future research to see if these rates of improvement reversed, with further increases in typical aiming movement plateauing, whilst the capacity to make corrective movement continued to mature later into adolescence.
The Steering task (previously referred to as “Tracing” in Flatters et al., 2014), showed a significant main effect of condition which had not been found in previous study. However, a potential explanation for this difference is the slight adaptation of the task for use in the Primary School Years data collection. In previous use, six trials were included within this task, alternating between Path A and Path B. However, the current version of the task was truncated to include only one trial for each task to reduce administration time, which could explain this discrepancy. Path A performance was significantly greater than Path B however the effect size was minimal which should also be considered. Additionally, Path B is identical to Path A in its proportions, with the only difference being that the path is flipped along its horizontal plane. Therefore, in Path B but not A it is partially occluded from view by the participants’ own hand for right-handed participants. The lack of this additional visual information could explain significantly poorer performance in this condition, albeit a difference of negligible magnitude. Further support of this hypothesis comes from findings from the Tracking task, where task performance is compromised when the visual guide is not provided.
Lastly, it was found that performance across all three CKAT tasks were significantly related. These relationships were found to be largest between the Tracking and Steering tasks. Whilst each CKAT task measures a distinct sensorimotor skill, this corroborates previous research that has found a reasonable degree of correlation between sub-tests on other standardised assessments of sensorimotor skill, such as the MABC-2 and the DCD-Q'07 ( Ellinoudis et al., 2011; Parmar et al., 2014).
Cognitive tasks
Across all measures, the cognitive battery produced outcomes that were in line with the starting hypotheses. Firstly, age differences were apparent throughout, with children improving with age on accuracy and (where relevant) response speed on all tasks. These results were in line with established developmental changes in working memory and executive function ( Diamond, 2013; Gathercole, 1999). The data also revealed that within-group variation was much larger than between-group variation, particularly for the working memory tasks. Subsequent papers will analyse these patterns in more detail. Indeed, this large within-group variation is still seen when the data are grouped by age in months. This suggests that individual differences need to be considered within the context of age-related improvements.
Sequence length effects were observed within each working memory task, and these were consistent across the different age groups, indicating that these measures were effective in capturing the limited capacity of working memory. Recall accuracy was also somewhat lower in BDR, relative to FDR or Corsi (even though BDR used a shorter set of lengths), reflecting the more difficult and processing-intensive nature of this task. Finally, for the inhibition (Flanker) task, significant congruency effects were observed in both reaction time and accuracy, indicating that the task was effective in indexing the greater executive cost of responding to a visual stimulus in the presence of incongruent flanker items. Furthermore, this cost decreased from the younger to older children, in line with age-related development in executive function.
The predicted positive interrelationships were observed between the three working memory measures, which remained after controlling for age, with some indication of a closer positive relationship between FDR and BDR. These patterns are likely to reflect a combination of shared and separable components operating within and across domains in working memory. Working memory performance was also related to inhibition and processing speed, including a moderate but still highly significant correlation between the working memory composite score and the magnitude of the inhibition congruency effect.
Applications, strengths and limitations
In the context of the Growing up in Bradford study, these measures are particularly valuable because, for BiB participants, they can be linked with contemporaneous data that has also been collected regarding their social and emotional wellbeing, growth, adiposity, and cardiometabolic health ( Bird et al., 2019). Longitudinally, there is also the capacity to link these cross-sectional data with data at earlier time points from BiB participants. This includes biological samples and maternal and paternal questionnaire responses collected at baseline ( Wright et al., 2013), routine health ( Bishop et al., 2018) and educational data ( Pettinger et al., 2020) that the cohort has permission to access, and further data collected from sub-samples of the cohort as part of nested research projects (e.g. the BiB-1000 sub-cohort, see Bryant et al., 2013). Repeated measurement of cognition and sensorimotor ability in adolescence is also being planned as part of the next phase of data collection within the cohort. Further, the sensorimotor data reported here can be linked with the sensorimotor data collected when the children were 4–5 years old, within the Starting Schools project (see Shire et al., 2020). Altogether this represents a rich set of data on children’s health, wellbeing and development that is an invaluable resource for future research. It will allow closer consideration of how participating children’s sensorimotor control and cognitive abilities integrate, influence, and are influenced by other factors in the course of their development.
The scale and objectivity of these assessments represent are a strength of this work. In particular, the use of precise, computerised assessments methods to measure children’s sensorimotor control at different time points are unique to the BiB cohort. Whilst other birth cohorts have attempted to assess their participant’s motor function, this is typically only captured via more subjective parental reports, collected at a much earlier age, reporting on when children accomplish specific gross motor milestones, such as in the Millenium Cohort Study (see Kelly et al., 2006). The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is the only other birth cohort study we are aware of to attempt a brief standardised assessment of motor function, even then only doing so at single time point in development, 7–8 years of age ( Lingam et al., 2010).
In demonstrating the capacity to undertake relatively brief, laboratory quality, objective measures of cognition and sensorimotor control within a community setting (i.e. over eighty schools across four years), and feedback the results of these assessments to teachers in a timely and informative fashion, this project also demonstrates the potential applied value of using such assessments at scale within health and education services. For example, the data collected here on cognitive and sensorimotor functioning will be important for identifying children who show deficits in these core developmental constructs, and who will therefore be at risk for educational under-achievement and poorer longer-term outcomes. Fine motor skills can be improved by training ( Sugden et al., 2013), and poor cognitive ability can be mitigated by appropriate classroom support ( Gathercole & Alloway, 2008). Thus, these data, and the assessment tools developed for use within this cohort to collect them, can help to inform subsequent interventions that aim to improve the long-term physical and mental health outcomes for children.
However, it should also be acknowledged that neither the specific cognitive nor sensorimotor assessments used here constitute a comprehensive of either of these broad constructs. Collecting large-scale data via school-based assessment meant we were operating under tight time constraints. As such, we selected tasks that represent fundamental sensorimotor and cognitive constructs with a strong body of evidence linking them to key outcomes. Additional nested projects are planned within the BiB cohort to supplement the data presented here. Further, via Connected Bradford, we can link data to school records, including measures of language, literacy and physical literacy (e.g., phonics, EAL status, reading attainment, gross motor skills).
In summary, the collection of objective computerised measures of a range of cognitive and sensorimotor functions at 7–10 years of age in over 15,500 children has created a comprehensive dataset that can be used to answer more specific questions about the development of these constructs, and can facilitate future studies of the relationship between cognition and motor function. These data also have real world applications, such as identifying children within the education system in need of additional support, and providing teachers with feedback on individual children to enable schools to make informed decisions about how to best tailor their approach to supporting individual learners. In particular, in collecting these measures on over 6,200 participants in the Born in Bradford study, the project also greatly enriches the longitudinal dataset available on the participants involved in this birth cohort study. This will help build a deeper understanding of the complex relationships between cognition, sensorimotor ability and other key aspects of childhood development.
Data availability
Underlying data
Scientists are encouraged to make use of the BiB data, which are available through a system of managed open access.
Before you contact BiB, please make sure you have read our Guidance for Collaborators. Our BiB executive review proposals on a monthly basis and we will endeavour to respond to your request as soon as possible. You can find out about all of the different datasets which are available here. If you are unsure if we have the data that you need please contact a member of the BiB team ( borninbradford@bthft.nhs.uk).
Once you have formulated your request please complete the ‘Expression of Interest’ form available here and email the BiB research team ( borninbradford@bthft.nhs.uk).
If your request is approved, we will ask you to sign a collaboration agreement; if your request involves biological samples, we will ask you to complete a material transfer agreement.
Extended data
Open Science Framework: Markdown report containing explanation of how to derive the sensorimotor variables from the raw CKAT output. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/TSVX6 ( Shire, 2020).
Extended data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Zero "No rights reserved" data waiver (CC0 1.0 Public domain dedication).
Notes
1The UPN is a 13-character code allocated on entry into formal education (typically primary school) that identifies a child at a national level within education databases
2At the end of each session conducted in a school the observing researcher had the option to add a free text Field Note to the data collected, to be saved alongside it. Normally this was used to note any unusual circumstances that had arisen during that session.
Acknowledgements
Born in Bradford is only possible because of the enthusiasm and commitment of the Children and Parents in BiB. We are grateful to all the participants, teachers, school staff, health professionals and researchers who have made Born in Bradford happen. Thanks to Liz Andrews for all her help in coordinating the research, and to Sameera Ali and the BiB Research Assistants for their help with data collection. We would also like to acknowledge the work of Amber Seddon in helping to format and prepare the final manuscript for submission.
Funding Statement
Core support for this Born in Bradford research was provided by the Wellcome Trust (WT101597MA). This research was funded by a joint grant from the UK Medical Research Council and UK Economic and Social Science Research Council (MR/N024397/1). L.J.B. Hill, K.A. Shire and A.H. Waterman’s involvement was also supported by the National Institute for Health Research Yorkshire and Humber ARC (reference: NIHR20016). M.L. Wood’s contribution was supported by funding from the UK Economic and Social Science Research Council White Rose Doctoral Training Partnership studentship. The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the National Institute for Health Research or the Departments of Health and Social Care or Education.
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
[version 2; peer review: 3 approved, 1 approved with reservations]
References
- Ahmed SF, Tang S, Waters NE, et al. : Executive function and academic achievement: Longitudinal relations from early childhood to adolescence. J Educ Psychol. 2019;111(3):446–458. 10.1037/edu0000296 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Alloway TP, Alloway RG, Wootan S: Home sweet home: Does where you live matter to working memory and other cognitive skills? J Exp Child Psychol. 2014;124:124–131. 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloway TP, Archibald L: Working Memory and Learning in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder and Specific Language Impairment. J Learn Disabil. 2008;41(3):251–262. 10.1177/0022219408315815 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloway TP, Gathercole SE, Kirkwood H, et al. : The Cognitive and Behavioral Characteristics of Children with Low Working Memory. Child Dev. 2009;80(2):606–621. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01282.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloway TP, Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ: Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Dev. 2006;77(6):1698–716. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00968.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anders TR, Lillyquist TD: Retrieval time in forward and backward recall. Psychon Sci. 1971;22:205–206. 10.3758/BF03332570 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ao D, Song R, Tong KY: Sensorimotor Control of Tracking Movements at Various Speeds for Stroke Patients as Well as Age-Matched and Young Healthy Subjects. PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0128328. 10.1371/journal.pone.0128328 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Augustijn MJCM, Deconinck FJA, D’Hondt E, et al. : Reduced motor competence in children with obesity is associated with structural differences in the cerebellar peduncles. Brain Imaging Behav. 2018;12(4):1000–1010. 10.1007/s11682-017-9760-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ, Allen RJ, et al. : A multicomponent model of working memory. In R.H. Logie, V. Camos, & N. Cowan (Eds). Working memory: State-of -the science. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2021. 10.1093/oso/9780198842286.003.0002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Beneventi H, Tønnessen FE, Ersland L, et al. : Working Memory Deficit in Dyslexia: Behavioral and fMRI Evidence. Int J Neurosci. 2010;120(1):51–59. 10.3109/00207450903275129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berry ED, Allen RJ, Mon-Williams M, et al. : Cognitive offloading: structuring the environment to improve children’s working memory task performance. Cogn sci. 2019;43(8):e12770. 10.1111/cogs.12770 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berry EDJ, Waterman AH, Baddeley AD, et al. : The limits of visual working memory in children: Exploring prioritization and recency effects with sequential presentation. Dev Psychol. 2018;54(2):240–253. 10.1037/dev0000427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bird PK, McEachan RRC, Mon-Williams M, et al. : Growing up in Bradford: protocol for the age 7-11 follow up of the Born in Bradford birth cohort. BMC Public Health. 2019;19(1):939. 10.1186/s12889-019-7222-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bishop CF, Small N, Parslow R, et al. : Healthcare use for children with complex needs: using routine health data linked to a multiethnic, ongoing birth cohort. BMJ Open. 2018;8(3):e018419. 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-018419 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blair C, Razza RP: Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Dev. 2007;78(2):647–663. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakey E, Carroll DJ: A Short Executive Function Training Program Improves Preschoolers’ Working Memory. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1827. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01827 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blakey E, Matthews D, Cragg L, et al. : The Role of Executive Functions in Socioeconomic Attainment Gaps: Results From a Randomized Controlled Trial. Child Dev. 2020;91(5):1594–1614. 10.1111/cdev.13358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryant M, Santorelli G, Fairley L, et al. : Design and characteristics of a new birth cohort, to study the early origins and ethnic variation of childhood obesity: the BiB1000 study. Longit Life Course Stud. 2013;4(2):119–135. 10.14301/llcs.v4i2.221 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron CE, Brock LL, Murrah WM, et al. : Fine Motor Skills and Executive Function Both Contribute to Kindergarten Achievement. Child Dev. 2012;83(4):1229–1244. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01768.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N: An Embedded-Processes Model of working memory.In: A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press.1999;62–101. 10.1017/CBO9781139174909.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cowan N: The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci. 2001;24(1):87–114. 10.1017/s0140525x01003922 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cragg L, Richardson S, Hubber PJ, et al. : When is working memory important for arithmetic? The impact of strategy and age. PLoS One. 2017;12(12):e0188693. 10.1371/journal.pone.0188693 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Culmer PR, Levesley MC, Mon-Williams M, et al. : A new tool for assessing human movement: The Kinematic Assessment Tool. J Neurosci Methods. 2009;184(1):184–192. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.07.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham AC, Fung W, Massey TH, et al. : Movement Disorder Phenotypes in Children With 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome. Mov Disord. 2020;35(7):1272–1274. 10.1002/mds.28078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cunningham AC, Hill L, Mon-Williams M, et al. : Using kinematic analyses to explore sensorimotor control impairments in children with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2019;11(1):8. 10.1186/s11689-019-9271-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond A: Executive Functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–168. 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond A, Taylor C: Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to “Do as I say, not as I do”. Dev Psychobiol. 1996;29(4):315–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson Mayes S, Calhoun SL: Learning, Attention, Writing, and Processing Speed in Typical Children and Children with ADHD, Autism, Anxiety, Depression, and Oppositional-Defiant Disorder. Child Neuropsychol. 2007;13(6):469–493. 10.1080/09297040601112773 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durston S, Thomas KM, Yang Y, et al. : A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control. Developmental Science. 2002;5(4):F9–F16. 10.1111/1467-7687.00235 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- D'Souza H, Cowie D, Karmiloff-Smith A, et al. : Specialization of the motor system in infancy: from broad tuning to selectively specialized purposeful actions. Dev Sci. 2017;20(4):e12409. 10.1111/desc.12409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellinoudis T, Evaggelinou C, Kourtessis T, et al. : Reliability and validity of age band 1 of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children--Second Edition. Res Dev Disabil. 2011;32(3):1046–1051. 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.01.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW: Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics. 1974; (1):143–149. 10.3758/BF03203267 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Eriksen CW, Schultz DW: Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. Percept Psychophys. 1979;25(4):249–263. 10.3758/bf03198804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flatters I, Hill LJ, Williams JH, et al. : Manual control age and sex differences in 4 to 11 year old children. PLoS One. 2014;9(2):e88692. 10.1371/journal.pone.0088692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fry A, Hale S: Processing Speed, Working Memory, and Fluid Intelligence: Evidence for a Developmental Cascade. Psychological Science. 1996;7(4):237–241. 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00051-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole SE: Cognitive approaches to the development of short-term memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3(11):410–419. 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01388-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole S, Alloway TP: Working memory and learning: A practical guide for teachers.Sage. 2008. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole SE, Durling E, Evans M, et al. : Working memory abilities and children's performance in laboratory analogues of classroom activities. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 2008;22(8):1019–1037. 10.1002/acp.1407 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ, Ambridge B, et al. : The Structure of Working Memory From 4 to 15 Years of Age. Dev Psychol. 2004;40(2):177–190. 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC: Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study. Dev Psychol. 2011;47(6):1539–1552. 10.1037/a0025510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geary DC, Brown SC: Cognitive addition: Strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in gifted, normal, and mathematically disabled children. Developmental Psychology. 1991;27(3):787—797. 10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.398 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Giles OT, Shire KA, Hill LJB, et al. : Hitting the Target: Mathematical Attainment in Children Is Related to Interceptive-Timing Ability. Psychol Sci. 2018;29(8):1334–1345. 10.1177/0956797618772502 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon R, Smith-Spark JH, Newton EJ, et al. : Executive Function and Academic Achievement in Primary School Children: The Use of Task-Related Processing Speed. Front Psychol. 2018;9:582. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottwald JM, Achermann S, Marciszko C, et al. : An Embodied Account of Early Executive-Function Development. Psychol Sci. 2016;27(12):1600–1610. 10.1177/0956797616667447 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goyen TA, Lui K, Hummell J: Sensorimotor skills associated with motor dysfunction in children born extremely preterm. Early Hum Dev. 2011;87(7):489–493. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrowell I, Hollén L, Lingam R, et al. : Mental health outcomes of developmental coordination disorder in late adolescence. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2017;59(9):973–979. 10.1111/dmcn.13469 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill LJB, Mushtaq F, O’Neill L, et al. : The relationship between manual coordination and mental health. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;25(3):283–295. 10.1007/s00787-015-0732-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holmes J, Adams JW: Working Memory and Children’s Mathematical Skills: Implications for mathematical development and mathematics curricula. Educational Psychology. 2006;26(3):339–366. 10.1080/01443410500341056 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- James KH: The Importance of Handwriting Experience on the Development of the Literate Brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2017;26(6):502–508. 10.1177/0963721417709821 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kail R: Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychol Bull. 1991;109(3):490–501. 10.1037/0033-2909.109.3.490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kail RV, Ferrer E: Processing Speed in Childhood and Adolescence: Longitudinal Models for Examining Developmental Change. Child Dev. 2007;78(6):1760–1770. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01088.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kail R, Hall LK: Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology. 1994;30(6):949–954. 10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.949 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kail R, Salthouse TA: Processing Speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychol (Amst). 1994;86(2–3):199–225. 10.1016/0001-6918(94)90003-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly Y, Sacker A, Schoon I, et al. : Ethnic differences in achievement of developmental milestones by 9 months of age: The Millennium Cohort Study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48(10):825–30. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2006.tb01230.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kieffer MJ, Vukovic RK, Berry D: Roles of Attention Shifting and Inhibitory Control in Fourth‐Grade Reading Comprehension. Read Res Q. 2013;48(4):333–348. 10.1002/rrq.54 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Krakauer JW, Mazzoni P: Human sensorimotor learning: adaptation, skill, and beyond. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011;21(4):636–644. 10.1016/j.conb.2011.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwan MYW, King-Dowling S, Hay JA, et al. : Longitudinal examination of objectively-measured physical activity and sedentary time among children with and without significant movement impairments. Hum Mov Sci. 2016;47:159–165. 10.1016/j.humov.2016.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Latash M: Fundamentals of Motor Control.Academic Press; Illustrated edition.2012. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- LeBarton ES, Goldin-Meadow S, Raudenbush S: Experimentally-Induced Increases in Early Gesture Lead to Increases in Spoken Vocabulary. J Cogn Dev. 2015;16(2):199–220. 10.1080/15248372.2013.858041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee K, Ng SF, Bull R, et al. : Are patterns important? An investigation of the relationships between proficiencies in patterns, computation, executive functioning, and algebraic word problems. J Educ Psychol. 2011;103(2):269–281. 10.1037/a0023068 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard HC: The Impact of Poor Motor Skills on Perceptual, Social and Cognitive Development: The Case of Developmental Coordination Disorder. Front Psychol. 2016;7:311. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lingam R, Golding J, Jongmans MJ, et al. : The Association Between Developmental Coordination Disorder and Other Developmental Traits. Pediatrics. 2010;126(5):e1109–e1118. 10.1542/peds.2009-2789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lingam R, Jongmans MJ, Ellis M, et al. : Mental Health Difficulties in Children With Developmental Coordination Disorder. Pediatrics. 2012;129(4):e882–e891. 10.1542/peds.2011-1556 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobier M, Dubois M, Valdois S: The Role of Visual Processing Speed in Reading Speed Development. PLoS One. 2013;8(4):e58097. 10.1371/journal.pone.0058097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackrous I, Proteau L: Visual online control of goal-directed aiming movements in children. Front Psychol. 2016;7:989. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00989 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marmot M, Bell R: Fair society, healthy lives. Public Health. 2012;126(Suppl 1):S4–S10. 10.1016/j.puhe.2012.05.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marteau TM, Hall PA: Breadlines, Brains, and Behaviour. BMJ. 2013;347:f6750. 10.1136/bmj.f6750 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, et al. : A Meta-Analysis of Working Memory Impairments in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44(4):377–384. 10.1097/01.chi.0000153228.72591.73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McQuade JD, Murray-Close D, Shoulberg EK, et al. : Working memory and social functioning in children. J Exp Child Psychol. 2013;115(3):422–435. 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller LT, Missiuna CA, MacNab JJ, et al. : Clinical description of children with developmental coordination disorder. Can J Occup Ther. 2001;68(1):5–15. 10.1177/000841740106800101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffit TE, Arsenault L, Belsky D, et al. : A gradient of childhood self‐control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108(7):2693– 2698. 10.1073/pnas.1010076108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monette S, Bigras M, Guay MC: The role of the executive functions in school achievement at the end of Grade 1. J Exp Child Psychol. 2011;109(2):158–173. 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulder H, Pitchford NJ, Marlow N: Processing speed and working memory underlie academic attainment in very preterm children. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2010;95(4):F267–F272. 10.1136/adc.2009.167965 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullane JC, Corkum PV, Klein RM, et al. : Interference Control in Children with and without ADHD: A Systematic Review of Flanker and Simon Task Performance. Child Neuropsychol. 2009;15(4):321–342. 10.1080/09297040802348028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Office of National Statistics: Estimates of the population for the UK England and Wales, Scotland and Northern Ireland [datset].2019. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Parmar A, Kwan M, Rodriguez C, et al. : Psychometric properties of the DCD-Q-07 in children ages to 4–6. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35(2):330–339. 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.10.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peirce JW: PsychoPy--psychophysics software in Python. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;162(1-2):8–13. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettinger KJ, Kelly B, Sheldon TA, et al. : Starting school: educational development as a function of age of entry and prematurity. Arch Dis Child. 2020;105(2):160–165. 10.1136/archdischild-2019-317124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piaget J: The origins of intelligence in children.New York: International Universities Press.1952. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Pieters S, De Block K, Scheiris J, et al. : How common are motor problems in children with a developmental disorder: rule or exception? Child Care Health Dev. 2012;38(1):139–145. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2011.01225.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prunty MM, Barnett AL, Wilmut K, et al. : Handwriting speed in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder: Are they really slower? Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(9):2927–2936. 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raw RK, Kountouriotis GK, Mon-Williams M, et al. : Movement control in older adults: does old age mean middle of the road? J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2012;38(3):735–45. 10.1037/a0026568 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberts RJ, Pennington BF: An interactive framework for examining prefrontal cognitive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1996;12(1):105–126. 10.1080/87565649609540642 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rohde TE, Thompson LA: Predicting academic achievement with cognitive ability. Intelligence. 2007;35(1):83–92. 10.1016/j.intell.2006.05.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rueda MR, Posner MI, Rothbart MK: The development of executive attention: contributions to the emergence of self-regulation. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;28(2):573–594. 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senn TE, Espy KA, Kaufmann PM: Using Path Analysis to Understand Executive Function Organization in Preschool Children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2004;26(1):445–464. 10.1207/s15326942dn2601_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shire K: Markdown report containing explanation of how to derive the sensorimotor variables from the raw CKAT output. 2020. 10.17605/OSF.IO/TSVX6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Shire K, Andrews E, Barber S, et al. : Starting School: a large-scale start of school assessment within the ‘Born in Bradford’ longitudinal cohort [version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. Wellcome Open Res. 2020;5:47. 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15610.1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smith-Spark JH, Fisk E: Working memory functioning in developmental dyslexia. Memory. 2007;15(1):34–56. 10.1080/09658210601043384 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snapp-Childs W, Casserly E, Mon-Williams M, et al. : Active Prospective Control Is Required for Effective Sensorimotor Learning. PLoS One. 2013;8(10):e77609. 10.1371/journal.pone.0077609 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stautz K, Pechey R, Couturier DL, et al. : Do Executive Function and Impulsivity Predict Adolescent Health Behaviour after Accounting for Intelligence? Findings from the ALSPAC Cohort. PLoS One. 2016;11(8):e0160512. 10.1371/journal.pone.0160512 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sugden D, Wade MG, Hart H: Typical and atypical motor development. London: Mac Keith Press. 2013. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Swanson HL, Howard CB, Saez L: Do Different Components of Working Memory Underlie Different Subgroups of Reading Disabilities? J Learn Disabil. 2006;39(3):252–269. 10.1177/00222194060390030501 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szucs D, Devine A, Soltesz F, et al. : Developmental dyscalculia is related to visuo-spatial memory and inhibition impairment. Cortex. 2013;49(10):2674–2688. 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.06.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- The English Indices of Deprivation: Statistical release – main findings. 2019. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Tresilian J: Sensorimotor control and learning: An introduction to the behavioral neuroscience of action.Palgrave Macmillan.2012. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
- Van Roon D, Caeyenberghs K, Swinnen SP, et al. : Development of Feedforward Control in a Dynamic Manual Tracking Task. Child Dev. 2008;79(4):852–865. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01163.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Hofsten C: An action perspective on motor development. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(6):266–272. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang TN, Tseng MH, Wilson BN, et al. : Functional performance of children with developmental coordination disorder at home and at school. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51(10):817–825. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03271.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waterman AH, Atkinson AL, Aslam SS, et al. : Do actions speak louder than words? Examining children’s ability to follow instructions. Mem Cognit. 2017;45(6):877–890. 10.3758/s13421-017-0702-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiler MD, Forbes P, Kirkwood M, et al. : The developmental course of processing speed in children with and without learning disabilities. J Exp Child Psychol. 2003;85(2):178–194. 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00034-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams BR, Ponesse JS, Schachar RJ, et al. : Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Dev Psychol. 1999;35(1):205–213. 10.1037//0012-1649.35.1.205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson PH, Hyde C: The development of rapid online control in children aged 6–12 years: Reaching performance. Hum Mov Sci. 2013;32(5):1138–1150. 10.1016/j.humov.2013.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolpert DM, Kawato M: Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Netw. 1998;11(7–8):1317–1329. 10.1016/s0893-6080(98)00066-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright J, Small N, Raynor P, et al. : Cohort Profile: the Born in Bradford multi-ethnic family cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(4):978–991. 10.1093/ije/dys112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang TX, Allen RJ, Holmes J, et al. : Impaired memory for instructions in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder is improved by action at presentation and recall. Front Psychol. 2017;8:39. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yuan L, Xu TL, Yu C, et al. : Sustained visual attention is more than seeing. J Exp Child Psychol. 2019;179:324–336. 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu C, Smith LB: Hand–Eye Coordination Predicts Joint Attention. Child Dev. 2017;88(6):2060–2078. 10.1111/cdev.12730 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zwicker JG, Harris SR, Klassen AF: Quality of life domains affected in children with developmental coordination disorder: a systematic review. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2013;39(4):562–580. 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2012.01379.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]