Abstract
Background:
Childhood adversity is a potent determinant of health across development. Altered DNA methylation (DNAm) signatures have been identified in children exposed to adversity and may be more common among children exposed during sensitive periods in development. However, it remains unclear if adversity has persistent epigenetic associations across childhood and adolescence. We examined the relationship between time-varying adversity and genome-wide DNAm, measured three times from birth to adolescence using prospective data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.
Methods:
We investigated the relationship between the timing of exposure to seven adversity types (measured 5–8 times between ages 0–11) and blood DNAm at age 15 using a structured life course modeling approach. We also assessed the persistence of adversity-DNAm associations we previously identified from age 7 blood DNAm into adolescence and the influence of adversity on DNAm trajectories from ages 0–15. We attempted to replicate our age 15 associations using data from the Raine Study and Future of Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS).
Findings:
Adversity associated with differences in age 15 DNAm at 41 loci (R2≥0.035). Most loci (20/41; 49%) were associated with adversities occurring between ages 3–5. Most associations were identified for exposures to one-adult households (20/41; 49%), financial hardship (9/41; 22%), or physical/sexual abuse (4/41; 10%). Differences in age 15 DNAm were not present in age 7 DNAm; DNAm differences previously identified at age 7 resolved by age 15. We identified six distinct DNAm trajectories from these patterns of stability and persistence. We replicated the direction of associations for 90% (18/20 loci) of one-adult household loci using adolescent blood DNAm from the Raine Study and 64% of loci (18/28 loci) using saliva DNAm from the FFCWS. The direction of effects for 11 one-adult household loci were replicated in both cohorts.
Interpretation:
These findings highlight the time-varying impact of childhood adversity on DNAm profiles across development, providing a potential biological mechanism linking adversity to adverse health outcomes in children and adolescents.
Funding:
CIHR, CLOSER, European Union’s Horizon 2020, NICHD, NIMH, NIMHD, NHMRC.
Keywords: ALSPAC, longitudinal, epigenetic, DNA methylation, childhood adversity, adolescence, trajectories, The Raine Study, FFCWS
INTRODUCTION
Children exposed to adversity, such as abuse or maltreatment, family disruption or dysfunction, or poverty, frequently have poorer physical and mental health outcomes later in development and across the life course(1). Epigenetic processes, including DNA methylation (DNAm), are increasingly recognized as potential underlying mechanisms for these associations, as DNAm is responsive to experiences(2) and may mediate the link between environmental exposures and health outcomes(3). Indeed, hundreds of studies in humans, including population-based studies, systematic reviews, and meta-analyses have shown links between childhood adversity, DNAm, and adverse health outcomes across the life course (reviewed in (4)). However, prior studies investigating the epigenome of children exposed to adversity have not yet explored two key dimensions of the adversity-DNAm relationship: 1) the timing of adversity, and 2) the timing of DNAm measurement and its stability over time. These dimensions are critical to understand the biological risk posed by childhood adversity, identify children at risk for poor health, and improve intervention targets for health promotion and disease prevention in children and adolescents.
First, it remains unclear how the timing of childhood adversity might shape DNAm. Both human and animal studies suggest there may be sensitive periods for epigenetic programming when physiological and neurobiological systems are primed for external influences, allowing experiences to impart more enduring effects(5, 6). Notably, we have previously identified a potential sensitive period for the effects of adversity on childhood DNAm between the ages of 3–5 (7, 8). However, no prior studies have investigated sensitive periods for epigenetic patterns in adolescence.
Second, little is known about how DNAm profiles of children exposed to adversity vary across development and how DNAm variation across time may shape health. In a recent article, Oh and Petronis(9) argued that the dynamic nature of epigenetic mechanisms is best examined through longitudinal studies that model chrono-epigenetic patterns, meaning the dynamics of epigenetic processes across time, rather than at single timepoints. Although previous studies have shown the epigenome is dynamic across development(10–17), no study has determined how childhood adversity might influence DNAm trajectories.
To address these gaps, we examined the longitudinal relationship between early-life adversity and genome-wide DNAm across childhood and adolescence, using data collected over two decades from a subsample of youth in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) cohort. We examined the associations between exposure to seven types of childhood adversity, assessed repeatedly between birth and age 11, and DNAm at age 15. Given the unique availability of three waves of DNAm in ALSPAC (measured from cord blood, and blood at ages 7 and 15), we also examined DNAm trajectories from birth to adolescence.
Our aims were to: 1) determine whether childhood adversity has time-dependent associations with adolescent DNAm; 2) characterize the developmental trajectories of DNAm linked to adversity; and 3) evaluate the persistence of associations between childhood adversity and DNAm at age 7 that we previously identified in ALSPAC(8) (see Figure S1 for analytic flow-chart). This study is the first to investigate the time-varying influences of childhood adversity on adolescent DNAm and DNAm trajectories from childhood to adolescence.
METHODS
Study design and participants
ALSPAC is a large population-based birth cohort from Avon, UK of 14,451 children followed from before birth through early adulthood(18, 19). Blood-based DNAm profiles were generated for a subsample of ALSPAC mother-child pairs as part of the Accessible Resource for Integrated Epigenomic Studies (ARIES), which includes cord blood at birth (n=905), whole blood at age 7 (n=970), and peripheral blood leukocytes at age 15 (n=966)(20) (Appendix p.3).
We examined seven types of childhood adversity previously associated with DNAm: 1) caregiver physical or emotional abuse; 2) sexual or physical abuse (by anyone); 3) maternal psychopathology; 4) one-adult households; 5) family instability; 6) financial hardship; and 7) neighborhood disadvantage. These adversities were reported by mothers via mailed questionnaires, collected 5–8 times between birth and age 11 (Figure 1; Table S1).
DNAm was measured from blood at 485,577 CpG sites using the Infinium HumanMethylation450 BeadChip microarray (Illumina, San Diego, CA). Laboratory procedures, preprocessing, and quality control steps were described previously(20–21). We removed non-variable CpGs (<5% DNAm difference between children in the 10th and 90th percentile), resulting in 302,581 CpGs for analyses (Appendix p.3). DNAm was analyzed as beta values, which represent the percent of methylation at each site.
Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Consent for biological samples has been collected in accordance with the Human Tissue Act (2004). Informed consent was obtained from participants following the recommendations of the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee. Secondary analyses of these data were approved with oversight by the Mass General Brigham Institutional Review Boards (Protocol 2017P001110).
Statistical analysis
We examined time-dependent associations for each adversity among children with DNAm data and no missing data among covariates or the adversity timepoints shown in Figure 1 (N=609–665). To adjust for known potential confounders(7), we controlled for age of blood collection, sex, race/ethnicity, maternal age at birth, maternal education at birth, birthweight, number of previous pregnancies, maternal smoking during pregnancy, and cell type proportions (Appendix p.3 and Figure S2).
Our primary analyses focused on identifying time-dependent associations between each type of childhood adversity and DNAm measured in adolescence (age 15). We used the structured life course modeling approach (SLCMA), a two-stage method that simultaneously compares a priori life course hypotheses explaining exposure-outcome relationships(22–24). SLCMA first uses variable selection to identify the life course hypothesis explaining the greatest proportion of outcome variation. Effect estimates, confidence intervals, and p-values are then calculated for the selected life course hypothesis using post-selective inference. SLCMA detects time-varying associations with more statistical power and less bias than traditional epigenome-wide association studies of ever/never-exposed or cross-sectional paradigms (7, 8, 25).
We generated variables corresponding to six separate life course hypotheses, including four sensitive periods hypotheses encoding exposure to each childhood adversity during: 1) very early childhood (birth to before age 3), 2) early childhood (ages 3–5), 3) middle childhood (ages 6–7), 4) late childhood (ages 8–11); and two additive hypotheses: 5) accumulation of exposures (total exposures of the specific adversity across childhood; Table S2), and 6) recency of exposures (total exposures of the specific adversity weighted by age) to determine whether more recent exposures had a stronger impact than distal exposures. We tested associations using selective inference and accounted for multiple-testing using the false-discovery rate (FDR). SLCMA, Quantile-quantile plots (Figure S3), genomic inflation estimates, and functional analyses of top loci are in Appendix p.4.
As sensitivity analyses. we completed internal validation analyses of the SLCMA results using ordinary nonparametric bootstrapping, and investigated the impact of potential confounders or alternate mediators of the association between childhood adversity and DNAm at age 15, including exposures to other types of childhood adversity in the same or different sensitive periods (Appendix p.5–7, 10–12).
We sought to replicate primary associations between childhood adversity and DNAm levels in adolescence using data from The Raine Study(26, 27) and the Future of Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS)(28). In the Raine Study, we analyzed the loci linked to one-adult households using blood DNAm measured at age 17 (N=382–529). In the FFCWS, we analyzed the loci linked to caregiver abuse, financial hardship, maternal psychopathology, and one-adult households using saliva DNAm measured at age 15 (N=662–1,859). The timing of adversity exposures was matched with the one identified in ALSPAC (see Appendix p.7–10).
Finally, the three waves of longitudinal DNAm data available in ALSPAC also allowed us to investigate three subsequent analyses of DNAm trajectories across development (Appendix p.12–13). First, we assessed whether DNAm differences identified at age 15 emerged earlier in development, using linear regression to test whether exposure to the same type and timing of childhood adversity was associated with DNAm at the same top loci at birth or age 7. Second, we investigated DNAm patterns in our top loci beyond the age 15 time point, studying longitudinal change and stability of DNAm across age 0, 7, and 15 among children from three distinct exposure groups: 1) children who had adversity exposure during the sensitive period identified from the SLCMA (labeled as exposed-SP); 2) children who had adversity exposure outside the sensitive period identified from the SLCMA (exposed-other); and 3) children who were never exposed to adversity.
Third, we previously identified associations between time-varying exposures to childhood adversity and DNAm levels at age 7 for 46 loci across the epigenome(8). To determine whether these DNAm alterations persisted to adolescence, we performed linear regressions between the same type and timing of childhood adversity and DNAm levels measured at age 15 for these 46 loci.
Role of the funding sources
The funding sources played no role in the writing of the manuscript or decision to submit for publication. The authors were not paid to write this article by a pharmaceutical company or other agency.
RESULTS
Demographic characteristics did not differ between the ARIES sample and children exposed to any adversity between ages 0–11 (Table S3). The prevalence of exposure to a given adversity between ages 0–11 ranged from 15.1% (sexual/physical abuse, 100 of 663 children) to 34.8% (maternal psychopathology, 222 of 639 children) (Figure S4; Table S4). The tetrachoric correlation of exposure within adversity across development ranged from 0.36 (family instability) to 0.786 (one-adult households). Different types of adversity were weakly correlated (ravg=−0.04–0.16).
Across all types of adversity, 41 loci showed significant associations between exposure to adversity and DNAm levels at age 15 (≥3.5% of DNAm variance explained by adversity; largest p-value=5.94×10−6; Table 1; Table S5). Of these, 22 loci were significant after multiple-test correction (FDR<0.05). As prior studies show that p-values are poor metrics of statistical inference on their own(29, 30), particularly in the context of time-varying associations(8), we focused downstream analyses on CpGs meeting the R2 threshold.
Table 1.
Adversity | Timing | Age (years) | CpG | DNAm unexp1 | DNAm SP2 | Δ DNAm3 | Effect estimate4 | SE* | 95% CI* | R25 | P-value | FDR-adjusted p-value |
Nearest gene | Trajectory class |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||||||||
Caregiver physical or emotional abuse | Early childhood | 5 | cg14855874 | 0.091 | 0.121 | 0.030 | 0.030 | 0.005 | 0.019; 0.041 | 0.041 | 3.32E-07 | 1.01E-01 | BANK1 | Emergent |
cg15454534 | 0.885 | 0.868 | −0.017 | −0.017 | 0.003 | −0.023; −0.01 | 0.039 | 6.76E-07 | 1.02E-01 | OR2T1 | Latent | |||
cg06215562 | 0.847 | 0.826 | −0.021 | −0.021 | 0.004 | −0.029; −0.013 | 0.035 | 2.37E-06 | 1.81E-01 | Latent | ||||
| ||||||||||||||
Sexual or physical abuse by anyone) | Early childhood | 3.5 | cg26970800 | 0.902 | 0.847 | −0.055 | −0.055 | 0.010 | −0.074; −0.036 | 0.044 | 8.51E-08 | 2.08E-02 | CBLIF | Emergent |
cg15723468 | 0.822 | 0.779 | −0.043 | −0.045 | 0.009 | −0.062; −0.028 | 0.041 | 1.89E-07 | 2.08E-02 | GALNT2 | Latent | |||
cg17928317 | 0.681 | 0.785 | 0.104 | 0.076 | 0.015 | 0.045; 0.106 | 0.041 | 2.06E-07 | 2.08E-02 | MAGEC3 | Primed | |||
| ||||||||||||||
Late childhood | 8 | cg27558057 | 0.257 | 0.289 | 0.032 | 0.107 | 0.024 | 0.059; 0.155 | 0.036 | 1.53E-06 | 1.16E-01 | TAF1 | Stable | |
| ||||||||||||||
Family instability | Very early childhood | 2.5 | cg02735620 | 0.877 | 0.857 | −0.021 | −0.019 | 0.004 | −0.027; −0.012 | 0.036 | 2.07E-06 | 4.63E-01 | PKD2 | Emergent |
| ||||||||||||||
Financial hardship | Very early childhood | 0.66 | cg14455319 | 0.289 | 0.339 | 0.050 | 0.052 | 0.011 | 0.032; 0.074 | 0.036 | 3.87E-06 | 2.00E-01 | ANKK1 | Stable |
cg13204236 | 0.861 | 0.824 | −0.037 | −0.037 | 0.007 | −0.051; −0.023 | 0.036 | 5.94E-06 | 2.00E-01 | STPG4 | Latent | |||
| ||||||||||||||
Early childhood | 5 | cg15037420 | 0.780 | 0.746 | −0.035 | −0.034 | 0.007 | −0.049; −0.021 | 0.036 | 3.04E-06 | 2.00E-01 | BSPH1 | Latent | |
cg06410970 | 0.860 | 0.825 | −0.035 | −0.033 | 0.006 | −0.046; −0.022 | 0.036 | 5.56E-06 | 2.00E-01 | ANXA11 | Overcompensation | |||
| ||||||||||||||
Late childhood | 11 | cg02011706 | 0.861 | 0.799 | −0.062 | −0.064 | 0.013 | −0.089; −0.039 | 0.036 | 5.35E-06 | 2.00E-01 | LMF1 | Emergent | |
cg04659536 | 0.901 | 0.873 | −0.029 | −0.028 | 0.006 | −0.039; −0.017 | 0.035 | 5.52E-06 | 2.00E-01 | SDK1 | Latent | |||
| ||||||||||||||
Recency | cg17670999 | 0.817 | 0.807 | −0.010 | −0.002 | 0.000 | −0.003; −0.001 | 0.041 | 8.76E-07 | 2.00E-01 | ARHGAP39 | Stable | ||
cg25459301 | 0.769 | 0.756 | −0.013 | −0.003 | 0.001 | −0.004; −0.002 | 0.036 | 4.24E-06 | 2.00E-01 | XKR6 | Overcompensation | |||
cg06812747 | 0.837 | 0.825 | −0.012 | −0.003 | 0.001 | −0.004; −0.002 | 0.035 | 4.98E-06 | 2.00E-01 | FBXL16 | Stable | |||
| ||||||||||||||
Maternal psychopathology | Very early childhood | 2.75 | cg16813552 | 0.898 | 0.883 | −0.015 | −0.015 | 0.003 | −0.021; −0.01 | 0.045 | 7.11E-08 | 2.15E-02 | OGA | Stable |
| ||||||||||||||
Neighborhood disadvantage | Very early childhood | 2.75 | cg04288299 | 0.914 | 0.905 | −0.009 | −0.021 | 0.004 | −0.029; −0.013 | 0.039 | 4.52E-07 | 7.00E-02 | NELFA | Overcompensation |
cg25019631 | 0.201 | 0.223 | 0.023 | 0.044 | 0.009 | 0.028; 0.061 | 0.038 | 6.16E-07 | 7.00E-02 | CASP9 | Overcompensation | |||
cg04224851 | 0.907 | 0.894 | −0.013 | −0.014 | 0.003 | −0.02; −0.009 | 0.038 | 6.94E-07 | 7.00E-02 | ZFP36L2 | Overcompensation | |||
| ||||||||||||||
One adult in the household | Very early childhood | 1.75 | cg05491478 | 0.908 | 0.880 | −0.028 | −0.027 | 0.006 | −0.039; −0.016 | 0.038 | 7.33E-07 | 2.81E-02 | LRRFIP1 | Overcompensation |
| ||||||||||||||
Early childhood | 3.9 | cg16907527 | 0.853 | 0.824 | −0.030 | −0.032 | 0.005 | −0.041; −0.022 | 0.060 | 4.17E-10 | 1.26E-04 | VEGFA | Flat emergent | |
cg08818094 | 0.847 | 0.798 | −0.048 | −0.050 | 0.008 | −0.067; −0.034 | 0.051 | 8.79E-09 | 1.33E-03 | TBC1D19 | Latent | |||
cg01060989 | 0.824 | 0.794 | −0.031 | −0.031 | 0.005 | −0.042; −0.021 | 0.047 | 6.73E-08 | 6.78E-03 | DUSP10 | Latent | |||
cg15814750 | 0.723 | 0.684 | −0.039 | −0.040 | 0.009 | −0.058; −0.025 | 0.039 | 6.57E-07 | 2.81E-02 | WDR72 | Latent | |||
cg15783822 | 0.868 | 0.848 | −0.021 | −0.021 | 0.004 | −0.031; −0.014 | 0.039 | 8.08E-07 | 2.81E-02 | PRR4 | Latent | |||
cg15864691 | 0.907 | 0.889 | −0.018 | −0.018 | 0.004 | −0.025; −0.011 | 0.038 | 8.36E-07 | 2.81E-02 | HOXA10 | Overcompensation | |||
cg02584161 | 0.661 | 0.603 | −0.057 | −0.058 | 0.011 | −0.081; −0.038 | 0.038 | 1.28E-06 | 3.42E-02 | Latent | ||||
cg02810291 | 0.840 | 0.818 | −0.022 | −0.023 | 0.005 | −0.033; −0.014 | 0.037 | 1.35E-06 | 3.42E-02 | AKAP13 | Overcompensation | |||
cg04036644 | 0.882 | 0.855 | −0.027 | −0.026 | 0.005 | −0.037; −0.016 | 0.037 | 1.36E-06 | 3.42E-02 | LOC286083 | Latent | |||
cg11811897 | 0.758 | 0.711 | −0.047 | −0.047 | 0.010 | −0.067; −0.03 | 0.037 | 1.68E-06 | 3.64E-02 | PKD1L1 | Latent | |||
cg15817130 | 0.794 | 0.759 | −0.036 | −0.038 | 0.007 | −0.051; −0.025 | 0.037 | 1.83E-06 | 3.69E-02 | MYO10 | Latent | |||
cg06711254 | 0.686 | 0.631 | −0.055 | −0.056 | 0.012 | −0.08; −0.036 | 0.036 | 2.15E-06 | 3.98E-02 | FSIP2 | Flat emergent | |||
cg19096460 | 0.845 | 0.821 | −0.024 | −0.024 | 0.005 | −0.035; −0.015 | 0.035 | 2.89E-06 | 4.85E-02 | HERC3 | Latent | |||
cg18980650 | 0.800 | 0.760 | −0.040 | −0.036 | 0.007 | −0.05; −0.024 | 0.035 | 3.31E-06 | 5.08E-02 | NOX1 | Emergent | |||
cg27504269 | 0.771 | 0.733 | −0.038 | −0.040 | 0.008 | −0.056; −0.026 | 0.036 | 3.52E-06 | 5.08E-02 | SLCO1A2 | Latent | |||
| ||||||||||||||
Late childhood | 10 | cg12096528 | 0.890 | 0.874 | −0.016 | −0.016 | 0.003 | −0.023; −0.01 | 0.036 | 2.24E-06 | 3.98E-02 | SLC25A41 | Overcompensation | |
| ||||||||||||||
Accumulation | cg00807464 | 0.052 | 0.057 | 0.006 | 0.003 | 0.001 | 0.002; 0.004 | 0.040 | 7.56E-07 | 2.81E-02 | CUX2 | Stable | ||
cg10420609 | 0.559 | 0.522 | −0.037 | −0.014 | 0.003 | −0.02; −0.009 | 0.039 | 7.71E-07 | 2.81E-02 | DSP | Latent | |||
cg14579651 | 0.634 | 0.605 | −0.028 | −0.012 | 0.002 | −0.018; −0.008 | 0.037 | 1.68E-06 | 3.64E-02 | STK38L | Stable |
DNAm unexp. = mean DNA methylation levels in children with no exposure to adversity from ages 0 to 11.
DNAm exp. SP = mean DNA methylation levels in children with exposure to adversity that occurred during the selected sensitive period (SP). Accumulation hypotheses show the mean DNA methylation levels in children with at least one exposure to adversity.
ΔDNAm= difference in mean DNA methylation levels between children exposed to adversity during the selected sensitive period and individuals unexposed to adversity (i.e., DNAm exp. SP – DNAm unexp.)
Effect estimates were calculated using linear regression of exposure to adversity from the theoretical model and DNA methylation, correcting for the covariates described in the methods. Standard error and confidence intervals are shown for these estimates.
R2 is the proportion of variation in DNAm at this CpG that is explained by differences in this adversity at this timing, after removing the associations with covariates.
CI = Confidence Interval; SE = standard error; Very early childhood = 0–3 years, Early childhood = 3–5 years; Late childhood = 8–11 years.
Sensitive periods were the most often selected life course hypothesis by the SLCMA, with 35 loci showing associations with childhood adversity that occurred during very early childhood (20%; 18/41), early childhood (56%; 23/41), or late childhood (10%; 4/41) (Figure 2). Only 3 loci (7%) showed associations with the accumulation or recency of adversity. Most of these associations were for exposure to one-adult households (20 loci), followed by financial hardship (9 loci), sexual or physical abuse by anyone (4 loci), caregiver physical or emotional abuse (3 loci), neighborhood disadvantage (3 loci), family instability (1 locus), and maternal psychopathology (1 locus).
Childhood adversity was mainly associated with a decrease in DNAm (35/41 loci). On average, childhood adversity exposure was linked to a 3.5% absolute difference in DNAm (range 0.9–10.4%). For loci associated with accumulated time living in one-adult households, each additional exposure timepoint associated with a 1% difference in DNAm (range 0.3–1.4%). For loci associated with the recency of financial hardship, one additional exposure was linked to a −1.3% to 2.3% change in DNAm per year of age at exposure.
Top loci showed higher representation in low CpG density regions, such as enhancers (p=0.008) and Open Seas (p=0.018) (Figure S5). Most loci (28/41) had weak, positive brain-blood correlations in individuals without exposure to adversity (28/41 positive; ravg=0.10; 10 with p<0.05; Table S6; Figure S6)(31), suggesting adversity-associated differences in blood DNAm could be reflected in the central nervous system. No biological processes were significantly enriched in top loci using the DAVID or missMethyl gene ontology tools(32, 33)(Figures S7–S8). Seven genes linked to sexual/physical abuse (TAF1), family instability (PKD2), financial hardship (FBXL16, XKR6), or one-adult households (DSP, CUX2, STK38L) showed evidence of strong functional constraint through analyses of probability of intolerance to loss-of-function mutations(34)(Table S5; Figure S9). Finally, several loci were previously associated with gestational age (7 loci), sex (6 loci), smoking (1 locus), inflammatory bowel disease (1 locus), and rheumatoid arthritis (4 loci). Together, these findings suggest different types of childhood adversity may act through diverse biological processes (Appendix p.4–5).
Internal validation of top associations yielded nearly identical results to the initial analyses (largest difference in effect estimates=2.03%) (Figure S10; Table S7). Our results remained stable when correcting for exposure to other adversities during the sensitive period or across childhood, suggesting they were not influenced by co-occurring adversity (Appendix p.6–7; Figure S11–13). Together, these results point to the robustness and specificity of associations between time-varying childhood adversity and DNAm at age 15.
We attempted to replicate these associations in two independent datasets, the Raine Study and FFCWS (Figure S14). Using data from the Raine Study (blood DNAm), we tested associations for the 20 CpGs associated with one-adult households (Table S8). Of these, 18 CpGs (90%) showed the same direction of effects in the Raine Study, which was more likely than random chance (p=2×10−4; Figure S15). Three CpGs were nominally significant (p<0.05) in the Raine Study; none of the effect estimate confidence intervals contained zero and all had the same direction as ALSPAC. Effect estimates in the Raine Study were smaller compared to ALSPAC. These differences were mitigated when correcting for winner’s curse effects (Figure S15).
Using data from FFCWS (saliva DNAm), we attempted to replicate associations for 28 loci associated with four childhood adversities. Of these, 64% of CpGs (18/28) showed the same direction of effects in the FFCWS (p=0.092), with 73% of one-adult household loci (11/15) showing concordant directions (p=0.059; Figure S16; Table S9). Importantly, all 11 of these one-adult household loci showed the same direction of effects in the Raine Study. While the magnitudes of effects were smaller in FFCWS, one CpG associated with the accumulation of one-adult household exposures (cg00807464; CUX2) showed nearly identical effect estimates between cohorts. These results point to the partial replication of associations from ALSPAC in independent cohorts, particularly for exposures to one-adult households.
For the 41 loci identified in age 15 DNAm, none showed associations between adversity and DNAm at birth (Table S10) or age 7 (Table S11). Notably, the age 7 estimates were smaller than the age 15 associations, with consistent directions-of-effect in about half of loci (20/41) (Figure 3A). Agnostic of adversity exposure, correlations in DNAm levels across ages were low at the individual-level (ravg=0.11; Figure S17). The emergence of these associations was not explained by early-life confounders (<10% change in effect estimates for parental socio-economic position, maternal BMI, or gestational age) or biological mediators during adolescence (<5% of the association mediated through age at pubertal onset, adolescent BMI, CRP levels, or smoking), suggesting some adolescent differences may emerge later in development and become stronger with time (Appendix p.10–12); Figures S2, S18–24).
Moving beyond adolescent DNAm, 34 of the 41 loci had significant adversity exposure group-by-age interactions (FDR<0.05), suggestive of more complex patterns of change and stability across development. From these loci, we identified five additional types of longitudinal DNAm trajectories (Figure 4), which showed distinct DNAm patterns across ages and adversity exposure groups (Figures S25–28; Table S12), but not between the FDR and R2 subsets of CpGs (Figure S29).
Finally, of the 46 CpG sites previously showing time-varying associations between adversity and DNAm at age 7 (8), only one showed an association at age 15 (p<0.05; Table S13), which did not pass multiple-test correction. Again, approximately half of loci showed consistent direction-of-effect between age 7 and 15 (24/46) (Figure 3B). These findings suggest some childhood epigenetic responses to adversity may not persist into adolescence.
DISCUSSION
This study’s main finding is that associations between childhood adversity and DNAm vary across the life course, manifesting at different developmental stages through distinct patterns of persistence and latency. To our knowledge, this is the first study to incorporate time-dependent measures of childhood adversity when assessing longitudinal epigenetic patterns.
Our findings point to early childhood – the period between ages 3 to 5 – as a possible sensitive period for the biological embedding of childhood adversity that manifests in adolescence. These findings are consistent with prior human and animal studies showing that exposures earlier in life may have greater influence on epigenetic patterns measured in childhood(7, 8) or adolescence(35). As early childhood is a time of rapid cognitive, social, emotional, and regulatory development(36), epigenetic processes may be more malleable(12), resulting in increased sensitivity to life experiences that shape DNAm levels and trajectories across development. These findings suggest early childhood may be a period for focused interventions to limit or prevent the long-term sequelae of childhood adversity.
Of the seven types of adversity examined, exposure to single parent households had the greatest number of associations to DNAm in adolescence. By contrast, previous research on DNAm from the same children at age 7 identified no associations with one-adult households(8), suggesting these associations are adolescent-specific. Prior studies have shown the effects of single parent households begin to emerge around puberty, manifesting through shifts in puberty timing (37), poorer self-esteem(38), and higher depressive symptoms(39) and externalizing behaviors(39). Of note, we did not detect any mediation of the associations of one-adult households and DNAm through pubertal onset age, nor were any loci previously linked to pubertal onset or sex hormone levels, or confounded by socioeconomic factors (Figure S19). We also replicated the direction of associations for 11 loci associated with one-adult households in two independents cohort. These results are particularly salient given the differences in the sociodemographic contexts and in the DNAm tissue assessed between studies. Beyond broad tissue differences, saliva is more heterogenous across individuals than blood (40), which further increased the stringency of the replicated effects and highlights the potential relevance of these top loci. Overall, these findings suggest a latency to the effects of one-adult households on biological processes and health outcomes, which may not become apparent until the rapid developmental changes occurring during puberty.
Curiously, we observed fewer associations for other adversities, such as maternal psychopathology and experiences of sexual, physical, or emotional abuse. These adversities may have subtler influences on the adolescent epigenome, requiring larger sample sizes or meta-analyses to uncover. None of our top loci overlapped between different types of childhood adversity, nor were they present among top loci from a twin study of adolescents exposed to severe victimization (N=118)(11). As discussed in ongoing debates surrounding the “lumping or splitting” of childhood adversities in clinical research(41), different dimensions of adversity could result in distinct epigenetic signatures, a hypothesis supported by the finding that adjusting for other types of adversity only modestly influenced associations.
Arguably the most novel finding from our study concerned the patterns of stability and change in the relationship between adversity and DNAm. Most DNAm trajectories showed primarily latent associations with adversity, meaning they did not emerge until age 15 in youth exposed to adversity. These findings align with previous longitudinal studies of genome-wide DNAm from ALSPAC and Project Viva, which showed that early-life stressors, such as prenatal maternal smoking(13) and socio-economic disadvantage during childhood(10, 14), can have both immediate and latent associations with DNAm during childhood and adolescence. Subtle desynchronization of DNAm levels may appear earlier in development, while evading immediate detection until later in life. These “sleeper” patterns may explain why complex diseases unfold over years of development, rather than immediately after exposures or risk factors(9). We also note that most of our top loci showed little individual-level stability over time, suggesting these latent effects may be located within regions of the epigenome that change across development. Future research is needed to determine whether latent associations between childhood adversity and the epigenome persist into adulthood and whether they are more likely to influence physical and mental health than alterations arising earlier in development.
Similarly, the DNAm differences we previously observed at age 7 did not persist into adolescence(8). Studies on early-life stressors(10, 14), birthweight and gestational age(16), and maternal weight before and during pregnancy(15) parallel these findings, showing that DNAm differences linked to early-life environments rarely persist across time. Whether these patterns resolve naturally or due to active intervention is unknown and should be investigated to determine whether interventions can be beneficial in reversing epigenetic effects of early-life stressors. Nevertheless, even short-term alterations that eventually fade over time could alter the developmental trajectories of downstream cellular pathways to influence future health.
Several differentially methylated genes we identified were implicated in processes that could influence downstream disease. For instance, CUX2 is transcription factor involved in dendrite and synapse formation(43), alterations to which could influence neurodevelopment and vulnerability to mental disorders. Several top genes, including DUSP10, DSP, and VEGFA, are also linked to cardiac function, and may partially reflect mechanisms linking childhood adversity to heart disease(44). We note, however, that findings from epigenome and genome-wide association studies have different interpretations and have not yet converged on common mechanisms underlying human health and disease. As DNAm alterations may not reflect concomitant changes in gene function or expression, experimental studies are needed to identify the true functional and health consequences of these epigenetic differences and determine whether short- and/or long-term DNAm changes could link childhood adversity to adverse health outcomes across the lifespan.
If replicated, our results may reveal how the biological embedding of early-life exposures through DNAm contribute to disease risk across development, which could have important clinical implications for early risk prediction, disease prognosis, and therapeutic guides for individuals and populations exposed to adversity. Several recent studies have shown that DNAm can predict risk and progression of diseases such as cancer(45) and depression(46). It may be that certain adversity-associated DNAm trajectories predict concomitant trajectories of disease risk. If true, repeated measures of DNAm could serve as a biological indicator or early warning-sign of initiated disease processes, helping identify people at greater risk for future disease. Moreover, these adversity-associated DNAm trajectories may also act as biological measures of treatment response, for example to salutary interventions or protective factors designed to buffer against the effects of adversity. Recent research shows that DNAm differences among adults with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (compared to those without PTSD) resolved following psychotherapy treatment; such DNAm changes corresponded to a reduction in PTSD symptom severity(47). Thus, repeated measures of DNAm could be used as a marker of therapeutic efficacy, tracking possible disease progress and/or resolution.
Our study had limitations. First, DNAm data were generated from slightly different tissue types at each wave. Although we corrected for cell type composition using established methods, differences in the stability of DNAm differences between waves may have been partially driven by tissue-based differences and variability. Second, we could not replicate all findings, partially due to the lack of available data from the Raine Study and FFCWS. Further, differences in associations between cohorts could reflect differences in the socio-economic environment or the specific timing and tissue of DNAm measurements, among other factors. Future studies should confirm these longitudinal epigenetic responses to childhood adversity and triangulate the socio-biological factors that modulate adversity-induced epigenetic differences and health outcomes. Third, we cannot rule out the possibility that unmeasured confounding or technical factors influenced our findings. However, our results were robust in internal validation analyses and when controlling for 11 potential confounders and investigating four potential mediators. Similarly, we could not assess the impact of time-varying confounding, which could have influenced our results(48). Fourth, our analytic subsample was mainly composed of children from European descent. This lack of diversity limited the generalizability of our findings, emphasizing the importance of replicating this work in more diverse cohorts. Finally, the differences in DNAm observed in youth exposed to adversity may not reflect concomitant phenotypic alterations, as epigenetic alterations in peripheral tissues may only partially reflect the causal mechanisms that drive health and disease. Thus, studies that combine both model systems and human populations are necessary to fully delineate the relationships among adversity, DNAm, and health.
In sum, this study highlights developmental variability in the relationship between adversity and DNAm trajectories and its potential role in adversity-related health outcomes across childhood and adolescence. Future studies should continue to investigate longitudinal measures of DNAm to identify the potential role of latent and persistent epigenetic alterations in driving the short- and long-term health outcomes that result from childhood adversity. Ultimately, this research will help guide intervention strategies and identify individual at higher risk for physical and mental disorders arising from exposure to childhood adversity.
Supplementary Material
RESEARCH IN CONTEXT.
Evidence before this study
We searched PubMed from inception to July 29, 2022 for articles on childhood adversity and DNA methylation measured during childhood and adolescence in human populations. Search terms included “DNA methylation OR epigenetics”, “trauma OR adversity OR abuse”, “child OR childhood”, “adolescent OR adolescence”. Our search did not identify any previous studies that investigated time-varying associations between childhood adversity on adolescent DNA methylation or trajectories of DNA methylation across development.
Added value of this study
To our knowledge, this is the first human study to incorporate time-dependent measures of childhood adversity in the study of longitudinal epigenetic patterns. Our findings are the first to demonstrate the dynamic developmental associations between adversity on the human epigenome. These analyses extend prior work that revealed sensitive periods for the association of childhood adversity with epigenetic alterations at age 7 in ALSPAC, further highlighting that exposure to adversity between the ages of 3–5 may be more closely linked to biological processes and future health than exposure during other time periods.
Implications of all the available evidence
Our study suggests epigenetic mechanisms may serve as a biological link between childhood adversity and long-term health. If replicated, these findings could explain why there are both immediate and latent manifestations of disease among people with histories of childhood adversity. Our findings also support the need for further studies investigating the role of DNA methylation trajectories in predicting child and adolescent health, including risk for immune dysfunction, metabolic disorder, and mental health problems.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health (grant number R01MH113930 awarded to ECD). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Dr. Dunn and Dr. Lussier were also supported by a grant from One Mind. Dr. Lussier is supported by a postdoctoral fellowship from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR).
Dr. Walton is funded by CLOSER, whose mission is to maximize the use, value, and impact of longitudinal studies (www.closer.ac.uk). CLOSER was funded by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) and the Medical Research Council (MRC) between 2012 and 2017. Its initial five-year grant has since been extended to March 2021 by the ESRC (grant reference: ES/K000357/1). The funders took no role in the design, execution, analysis, or interpretation of the data or in the writing up of the findings. Dr. Walton is also supported by the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant n° 848158).
We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in the ALSPAC study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 217065/Z/19/Z) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf); This research was specifically funded by grants from the BBSRC (BBI025751/1; BB/I025263/1), MRC IEU (MC_UU_00011/5), National Institute of Child and Human Development (R01HD068437), NIH (5RO1AI121226–02), and CONTAMED EU (212502). This publication is the work of the authors, whom will serve as guarantors for the contents of this paper.
We would like to acknowledge the Raine Study participants and their families for their ongoing participation in the study and the Raine Study team for study co-ordination and data collection. We also thank the NHMRC for their long-term contribution to funding the study over the last 30 years. The core management of the Raine Study is funded by The University of Western Australia, Curtin University, Telethon Kids Institute, Women and Infants Research Foundation, Edith Cowan University, Murdoch University, The University of Notre Dame Australia and the Raine Medical Research Foundation. The Pawsey Supercomputing Centre provided computation resources to carry out analyses required with funding from the Australian Government and the Government of Western Australia. The Gen2–1, 2, 3, 5, 8, 10 year follow-ups were supported by grants from the NHMRC and The Raine Medical Research Foundation. DNA or GWAS data from the Gen2–17 year follow-up were support by grants from the NHMRC (Palmer et al, ID 572613; Beilin et al, ID 403981; Huang et al, ID 1059711) and Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR; Lye et al, MOP-82893).
Funding for the Future of Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS, formerly Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study) was provided by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development through grants R01HD036916, R01HD039135, and R01HD040421 and by a consortium of private foundations. Epigenetic data generation and processing were funded through grants by the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities (R01MD011716 to CM), the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R01HD076592 to DN), the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH103761), and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (R01HL149869 to DN).
Finally, we would also like to thank Dr. Garrett Fitzmaurice for his guidance on the characterization of DNAm trajectories across development. We also thank Elizabeth Leblanc for her assistance with the prediction of age at pubertal onset using superimposition by translation and rotation (SITAR).
Footnotes
COMPETING INTEREST
The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
DATA SHARING
ALSPAC data are available by request from the ALSPAC Executive Committee for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data (bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/). Data from the Raine Study are available with the permission of the Raine Study. Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for this study. The FFCWS data analyzed in the current study are available with permission from the Future of Families and Childhood Wellbeing Study repository (fragilefamilies.princeton.edu/documentation)
REFERENCES
- 1.Grummitt LR, Kreski NT, Kim SG, Platt J, Keyes KM, McLaughlin KA. Association of Childhood Adversity With Morbidity and Mortality in US Adults: A Systematic Review. JAMA pediatrics. 2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Aristizabal MJ, Anreiter I, Halldorsdottir T, Odgers CL, McDade TW, Goldenberg A, et al. Biological embedding of experience: A primer on epigenetics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2020;117(38):23261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Fujii R, Sato S, Tsuboi Y, Cardenas A, Suzuki K. DNA methylation as a mediator of associations between the environment and chronic diseases: A scoping review on application of mediation analysis. Epigenetics. 2021;ahead-of-print:1–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Parade SH, Huffhines L, Daniels TE, Stroud LR, Nugent NR, Tyrka AR. A systematic review of childhood maltreatment and DNA methylation: candidate gene and epigenome-wide approaches. Translational Psychiatry. 2021;11(1):134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Knudsen E Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16:1412–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Shonkoff JP, Boyce WT, McEwen BS. Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities. JAMA. 2009;301(21):2252–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Dunn EC, Soare TW, Zhu Y, Simpkin AJ, Suderman MJ, Klengel T, et al. Sensitive periods for the effect of childhood adversity on DNA methylation: results from a prospective, longitudinal study. Biological Psychiatry. 2019;85(10):838–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Lussier AA, Zhu Y, Smith BJ, Simpkin AJ, Smith ADAC, Suderman MJ, et al. Updates to data versions and analytic methods influence the reproducibility of results from epigenome-wide association studies. Epigenetics. 2022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Oh ES, Petronis A. Origins of human disease: the chrono-epigenetic perspective. Nat Rev Genet. 2021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Laubach ZM, Perng W, Cardenas A, Rifas-Shiman SL, Oken E, DeMeo D, et al. Socioeconomic status and DNA methylation from birth through mid-childhood: a prospective study in Project Viva. Epigenomics. 2019;11(12):1413–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kandaswamy R, Hannon E, Arseneault L, Mansell G, Sugden K, Williams B, et al. DNA methylation signatures of adolescent victimization: analysis of a longitudinal monozygotic twin sample. Epigenetics. 2020:1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mulder RH, Neumann A, Cecil CAM, Walton E, Houtepen LC, Simpkin AJ, et al. Epigenome-wide change and variation in DNA methylation in childhood: trajectories from birth to late adolescence. Hum Mol Genet. 2021;30(1):119–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Richmond RC, Simpkin AJ, Woodward G, Gaunt TR, Lyttleton O, McArdle WL, et al. Prenatal exposure to maternal smoking and offspring DNA methylation across the lifecourse: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Hum Mol Genet. 2015;24(8):2201–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Alfano R, Guida F, Galobardes B, Chadeau-Hyam M, Delpierre C, Ghantous A, et al. Socioeconomic position during pregnancy and DNA methylation signatures at three stages across early life: epigenome-wide association studies in the ALSPAC birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2019;48(1):30–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sharp GC, Lawlor DA, Richmond RC, Fraser A, Simpkin A, Suderman M, et al. Maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and gestational weight gain, offspring DNA methylation and later offspring adiposity: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44(4):1288–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Simpkin AJ, Hemani G, Suderman M, Gaunt TR, Lyttleton O, McArdle WL, et al. Prenatal and early life influences on epigenetic age in children: a study of mother–offspring pairs from two cohort studies. Human Molecular Genetics. 2016;25(1):191–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Martins J, Czamara D, Sauer S, Rex-Haffner M, Dittrich K, Dorr P, et al. Childhood adversity correlates with stable changes in DNA methylation trajectories in children and converges with epigenetic signatures of prenatal stress. Neurobiol Stress. 2021;15:100336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A, Golding J, Davey Smith G, et al. Cohort Profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):97–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, et al. Cohort Profile: the ‘children of the 90s’--the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):111–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Relton CL, Gaunt T, McArdle W, Ho K, Duggirala A, Shihab H, et al. Data Resource Profile: Accessible Resource for Integrated Epigenomic Studies (ARIES). Int J Epidemiol. 2015;44(4):1181–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Min JL, Hemani G, Davey Smith G, Relton C, Suderman M. Meffil: efficient normalization and analysis of very large DNA methylation datasets. Bioinformatics (Oxford, England). 2018;34(23):3983–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mishra G, Nitsch D, Black S, De Stavola B, Kuh D, Hardy R. A structured approach to modelling the effects of binary exposure variables over the life course. Int J Epidemiol. 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Smith ADAC, Heron J, Mishra G, Gilthorpe MS, Ben-Shlomo Y, Tilling K Model Selection of the Effect of Binary Exposures over the Life Course. Epidemiology. 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Smith ADAC, Hardy R, Heron J, Joinson CJ, Lawlor DA, Macdonald-Wallis C, et al. A structured approach to hypotheses involving continuous exposures over the life course. Int J Epidemiol. 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Zhu Y, Simpkin AJ, Suderman MJ, Lussier AA, Walton E, Dunn EC, et al. A Structured Approach to Evaluating Life Course Hypotheses: Moving Beyond Analyses of Exposed Versus Unexposed in the Omics Context. Am J Epidemiol. 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Newnham JP, Evans SF, Michael CA, Stanley FJ, Landau LI. Effects of frequent ultrasound during pregnancy: a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 1993;342(8876):887–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.McKnight CM, Newnham JP, Stanley FJ, Mountain JA, Landau LI, Beilin LJ, et al. Birth of a cohort--the first 20 years of the Raine study. Med J Aust. 2012;197(11):608–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Reichman NE, Teitler JO, Garfinkel I, McLanahan SS. Fragile families: sample and design. Children and Youth Services Review. 2001;23(4/5):303–26. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Amrhein V, Greenland S. Remove, rather than redefine, statistical significance. Nature Human Behaviour. 2018;2(1):4-. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.McShane BB, Gal D, Gelman A, Robert C, Tackett JL. Abandon Statistical Significance. The American Statistician. 2019;73(sup1):235–45. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hannon E, Lunnon K, Schalkwyk L, Mill J. Interindividual methylomic variation across blood, cortex, and cerebellum: implications for epigenetic studies of neurological and neuropsychiatric phenotypes. Epigenetics. 2015;10(11):1024–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Huang da W, Sherman BT, Lempicki RA. Systematic and integrative analysis of large gene lists using DAVID bioinformatics resources. Nat Protoc. 2009;4(1):44–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Phipson B, Maksimovic J, Oshlack A. missMethyl: an R package for analyzing data from Illumina’s HumanMethylation450 platform. Bioinformatics. 2016;32(2):286–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lek M, Karczewski KJ, Minikel EV, Samocha KE, Banks E, Fennell T, et al. Analysis of protein-coding genetic variation in 60,706 humans. Nature. 2016;536(7616):285–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Essex MJ, Boyce WT, Hertzman C, Lam LL, Armstrong JM, Neumann SM, et al. Epigenetic vestiges of early developmental adversity: childhood stress exposure and DNA methylation in adolescence. Child Development. 2013;84(1):58–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Shonkoff JP, Phillips DA. From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 2000. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Aghaee S, Deardorff J, Greenspan LC, Quesenberry CP, Kushi LH, Kubo A. Early life household intactness and timing of pubertal onset in girls: a prospective cohort study. BMC Pediatrics. 2020;20(1):464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Alami A, Khosravan S, Sadegh Moghadam L, Pakravan F, Hosseni F. Adolescents’ self-esteem in single and two-parent families. Int J Community Based Nurs Midwifery. 2014;2(2):69–76. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Daryanani I, Hamilton JL, Abramson LY, Alloy LB. Single Mother Parenting and Adolescent Psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2016;44(7):1411–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Middleton LYM, Dou J, Fisher J, Heiss JA, Nguyen VK, Just AC, et al. Saliva cell type DNA methylation reference panel for epidemiological studies in children. Epigenetics. 2022;17(2):161–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Smith KE, Pollak SD. Rethinking Concepts and Categories for Understanding the Neurodevelopmental Effects of Childhood Adversity. Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science. 2021;16(1):67–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Lambert HK. Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;47:578–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Cubelos B, Sebastián-Serrano A, Beccari L, Calcagnotto ME, Cisneros E, Kim S, et al. Cux1 and Cux2 regulate dendritic branching, spine morphology, and synapses of the upper layer neurons of the cortex. Neuron. 2010;66(4):523–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Jakubowski KP, Cundiff JM, Matthews KA. Cumulative childhood adversity and adult cardiometabolic disease: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2018;37(8):701–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Deng Y, Wan H, Tian J, Cheng X, Rao M, Li J, et al. CpG-methylation-based risk score predicts progression in colorectal cancer. Epigenomics. 2020;12(7):605–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Barbu MC, Shen X, Walker RM, Howard DM, Evans KL, Whalley HC, et al. Epigenetic prediction of major depressive disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26(9):5112–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Vinkers CH, Geuze E, van Rooij SJH, Kennis M, Schür RR, Nispeling DM, et al. Successful treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder reverses DNA methylation marks. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26(4):1264–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Mansournia MA, Etminan M, Danaei G, Kaufman JS, Collins G. Handling time varying confounding in observational research. BMJ. 2017;359:j4587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
ALSPAC data are available by request from the ALSPAC Executive Committee for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data (bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/). Data from the Raine Study are available with the permission of the Raine Study. Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for this study. The FFCWS data analyzed in the current study are available with permission from the Future of Families and Childhood Wellbeing Study repository (fragilefamilies.princeton.edu/documentation)