Abstract
Adolescence and emerging adulthood is likely a sensitive period for the neural effects of stress due to increasing life stress, onset of stress-related disorders, and continued gray matter (GM) development. In adults, stress is associated with GM differences in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), hippocampus, and amygdala, but little is known about these relations, and whether they differ by gender, during adolescence and emerging adulthood. Further, it is unknown whether dependent (self-generated) and independent (fateful) stressors have distinct associations with GM, as each have distinct relations with internalizing psychopathology. We tested relations between recent dependent and independent stressor frequency (ALEQ-R) and GM structure using MRI in a priori regions of interest (mPFC, amygdala, and hippocampus) and across the cortex in youth from the Denver/Boulder metro area ages 14–22 (N=144). Across both genders, no effects passed multiple comparison correction (FDR q>.05). However, there were significant differences between male and female youth (FDR q<.05), with opposite relations between dependent stressor frequency and cortical GM thickness in the salience network and emotion regulation regions and with surface area in default mode network regions. These results motivate future investigations of gender differences in neural mechanisms of stress generation and reactivity.
Adolescents and emerging adults experience many life stressors, which increase risk for psychopathology (Kendler & Gardner, 2016). Stressor exposure is additionally associated with differences in gray matter (GM) structure in adult humans, most notably in regions associated with psychopathology (e.g., Radley et al., 2015), and can alter GM in rodents. As GM develops during adolescence and emerging adulthood, individuals may be especially vulnerable to these neural impacts (Andersen & Teicher, 2008; Romeo, 2017), but it is unknown how stress experienced during this developmental period relates to GM structure. Additionally, previous research does not distinguish between dependent stressors (i.e., self-generated, e.g., getting a bad grade) and independent stressors (i.e., fateful, e.g., illness in the family), despite evidence for a stronger link between dependent (vs. independent) stressors and psychopathology (Technow et al., 2015). The current study addresses these gaps by testing how the frequency of recent dependent and independent stressors are associated with GM morphometry in a sample of adolescents and emerging adults.
In rodents, stress alters GM in areas high in glucocorticoid receptors, increasing vulnerability to depression- and anxiety-like behavior (Sousa, 2016). Stress causes dendritic branching in the amygdala, a key region for fear learning, salience encoding and activating the stress response, and dendritic atrophy in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and hippocampus, regions crucial for regulating the stress response. Similarly, in human adults, life stress is associated with lower GM volume (GMV) in the hippocampus and mPFC (Ansell et al., 2012; Gianaros et al., 2007; Papagni et al., 2010), with mixed findings of increased or decreased GMV in the amygdala (Gerritsen et al., 2014; Ganzel et al., 2008; Lotze et al., 2020; Sublette et al., 2016). Importantly, in humans, life stress is additionally associated with GMV differences in regions associated with psychopathology and involved in detecting and reacting to stressful stimuli. These regions include the insula (Ansell et al., 2012; H. Li et al., 2014) and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC; Kuhn et al., 2015; Y. Li et al., 2017; Papagni et al., 2010), key nodes of the salience network crucial for integrating internal and external signals and involved in cognitive emotion regulation (Ochsner et al., 2008; Stevens et al., 2011), and the parahippocampal gyrus (H. Li et al., 2014; Papagni et al., 2010), a region involved in emotion regulation (Phillips et al., 2008).
However, these regions are inconsistently identified across studies, potentially due to methodological issues. Many studies use a stress measure that combines stressor frequency, appraisals, perceived coping ability, and distress, which is problematic because these dimensions have distinct relations with psychopathology (Fassett-Carman et al., 2020; Hewitt et al., 1992; Krueger & Markon, 2006) and therefore likely have distinct neural correlates.
Further, research has not distinguished between dependent and independent stressors. Dependent stressors, or those that individuals likely contribute to causing (e.g., getting bad grades, fights with friends), are more strongly associated with internalizing psychopathology than independent, or fateful stressors (e.g., illness in the family, disruptive neighbors; Connolly et al., 2010, Kendler et al., 1999). This difference in association with internalizing symptoms is possibly due to bidirectional effects. Consistent with the stress generation hypothesis, internalizing symptoms affect behavior in ways that generate more dependent stressors (for review, see Hammen, 2018). In turn, consistent with attributional style theory, attributing negative events to the self, which is more likely for dependent stressors, is depressogenic (for meta-analysis, see Huang, 2015). Despite differences in links between dependent and independent stressors and mental health, it is unknown if dependent and independent stressors are distinctly and/or similarly related to GM structure, which could help elucidate pathways between these stressor types and internalizing symptoms.
Adolescence and emerging adulthood may be a sensitive period for stress effects, marked by increased life stressors, the onset of stress-related internalizing psychopathology, heightened HPA axis reactivity to stressors, and ongoing development of brain regions involved in facilitating and regulating stress responses (e.g., mPFC, hippocampus, and amygdala; Romeo, 2017; Hankin et al., 2016). The major developmental trend during adolescence is a reduction in GMV across the brain, due to neurodevelopmental mechanisms such as synaptic pruning, that can affect cortical thickness, and can be influenced by environmental factors (Tau & Peterson, 2010). The timing of pruning varies across regions, including those involved in stress reactivity: amygdala and hippocampus GMV increases until late adolescence (~17 and 19 years respectively), then declines (Wierenga et al., 2014), whereas insula, mPFC, and ACC cortical thickness decreases across this period (Tamnes et al., 2017; Weirenga et al., 2014; Ducharme et al., 2016). In contrast, cortical surface area (SA)—largely shaped by the number of cortical columns (Rakic, 1988; 2009; Tau & Peterson, 2010)—is less affected by environmental input (Aleman-Gomez et al., 2013, Seldon, 2005). It shows inconsistent developmental trajectories during this period across studies, with some evidence for gradual decreases (Tamnes et al., 2017) and other evidence for increases (Vijayakumar et al, 2016), including in areas associated with stress reactivity such as the PFC (Wierenga et al., 2014, Vijayakumar, et al., 2016).
Surprisingly, little research exists on the association of brain morphology and stressful life events that occur during this sensitive adolescent and emerging adult period, compared to effects of early life adversity (for review, see Teicher & Samson, 2016) or childhood and early adolescent trauma, which captures more extreme stressors such as abuse and neglect (for meta-analysis, see Paquola et al., 2016). Only one study tested whether common adolescent stressors are associated with GM structure and found that being disliked by peers was associated with reduced normative decline of PFC GMV and increased typical hippocampal GMV growth between ages 14 and 17, demonstrating that this stressor can affect neurodevelopment across adolescence (Tyborowska et al., 2018). However, these results could be caused by other factors (e.g., behavioral factors that affect interactions with peers, psychopathology) and may not generalize to other stressors.
The current study addresses these gaps by investigating GM correlates of stressful life events in a community sample of adolescents and emerging adults using a robust measure of dependent and independent stressor frequency. Because there is limited previous research during this developmental period, we based preregistered exploratory hypotheses1 on overlap of regions identified across rodent and adult human research: (1) greater stress frequency will be associated with GMV differences in the amygdala (no directionality hypothesis due to mixed evidence), lower GMV in the hippocampus, and lower cortical thickness and SA in the mPFC, and (2) these effects will be stronger for dependent than independent stressors, as dependent stressors are more predictive of psychopathology than independent stressors, and are likely particularly salient to adolescents and emerging adults, as they include social and academic stressors. We also performed exploratory whole-brain analyses. We tested for gender differences given gender differences in GM developmental trajectories (Gennatas et al., 2017) and due to the greater strength of life stress-psychopathology associations in females than males (Hankin et al., 2007; Fassett-Carman et al., 2019), potentially suggesting differences in stress-GM relations. We additionally tested age moderation due to the somewhat wide age range of our sample across adolescence. Finally, we conducted follow-up analyses testing moderation of the stress-GM association in ROIs by pubertal development, given links between pubertal and GM development (Herting & Sowell, 2017; Juraska & Willing, 2017).
Method
Participants
Participants (N=144, 50.1% Female) were unselected community adolescents and emerging adults ages 14–22 drawn from the greater Denver/Boulder metro area as part of the Colorado Cognitive Neuroimaging Family Emotion Research (CoNiFER) study. Participants were originally recruited for the GEM (Genes, Environment and Mood) study (NIH Grant R01 MH077195) and an associated follow-up study (R21MH102210) via public schools and direct mail to target zip codes to maximize demographic and socioeconomic diversity. Inclusion criteria were no neurological insult, English as a first language, and no dyslexia or difficulty reading. Participants gave informed consent (ages 18–22) or assent with parental consent (ages 14–17), and received cash compensation for their participation. All procedures were approved by the University of Colorado Institutional Review Board.
The recruited sample size was determined by the needs of other analyses that were the focus of the Colorado CoNiFER study. Power analysis demonstrated the sample size provided >80% power to detect medium effect sizes in regression analyses. All participants that completed the neuroimaging portion of the study (n=126) as well as the ALEQ stress measure (n=129) were included in the current analyses (final n=114).
Procedure
Analyses presented here use Time Point 1 data from the Colorado CoNiFER longitudinal study. Time Point 1 consisted of two 1-hour structural and functional neuroimaging sessions, and one behavioral tasks and psychopathology assessment session.
Measures
Demographics.
Participants selected their gender from the options “Male” or “Female” and report their current age.
Stressful life events.
The Adolescent Life Event Questionnaire – Revised (ALEQ-R; Fassett-Carman et al., 2019) assesses occurrence in the past 6 months of 24 dependent and 25 independent negative life events commonly experienced by youth (See Fassett-Carman et al., 2019 for details regarding classification of items as dependent or independent). For nine infrequent events (eight independent and one dependent stressor; e.g., “Parents getting divorced”), participants indicated “This event did not happen” [0] or “This event happened” [4]. For all other items, participants rated how often each event occurred from 0 (“Never”) to 4 (“Always”). Dependent and independent stressor frequency scores were calculated by summing frequency ratings of items within the stressor category for each participant. Severity and controllability ratings were collected but are the focus of a separate manuscript (Fassett-Carman et al., 2022).
Pubertal development.
The Pubertal Development Scale (PDS; Carskadon & Acebo, 1993) measures changes that typically occur throughout puberty. Boys and girls are both asked about growth in height and body hair, and skin changes. Boys are additionally asked about deepening of the voice and facial hair growth. Girls are additionally asked about breast growth and menstruation. Participants indicate (1) “Not yet started”, (2) “Barely started”, (3) “Definitely started”, and (4) “Seems complete” for each item except the menstruation item, which is rated dichotomously (0=menstruation has not begun, 4=menstruation has begun). Scores are calculated by averaging the ratings of the five items applicable to each participant. The PDS shows good reliability and validity (Carskadon & Acebo, 1993).
Structural MRI acquisition and surface-based morphometry.
Whole brain high-resolution structural MRI data were acquired at the Intermountain Neuroimaging Consortium located at the University of Colorado Boulder using a Siemens 3-Tesla PRISMA MRI scanner for all but 18 participants, who were scanned on the pre-upgrade version of the same magnet (TIM TRIO). A 32-channel headcoil was used for radiofrequency transmission and reception. GM structure data were acquired via a T1-weighted Magnetization Prepared Gradient Echo sequence in 224 sagittal slices, with a repetition time=2400 ms, echo time=2.07 ms, flip angle=8 degrees, field of view=256 mm, and voxel size=.8 mm3.
T1-weighted structural images were brain extracted using a hybrid watershed/surface deformation procedure (Segonne et al., 2004), followed by a transformation into Talaiarch space, intensity normalization (Sled et al., 1998), tessellation of the gray/white matter boundary (Fischl et al., 2001), and surface deformation along intensity gradients to optimally differentiate gray matter, white matter, and cerebral spinal fluid (CSF) boundaries (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl and Dale, 2000). The resulting segmented surfaces were registered to a standard spherical inflated brain template (Fischl et al., 1999a, b), parcellated according to gyral and sulcal structure (Desikan et al., 2006; Fischl et al., 2004), and then used to compute a range of surface-based measurements, including cortical SA and thickness. Prior to running surface-based analyses, data quality assurance was checked using FreeSurfer’s standard quality assurance tools (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/fswiki/QATools), including visual inspection of the relevant parcellations and checking for outliers in morphometry measures, signal-to-noise ratio, and white matter intensity.
Analysis
To test whether frequency of dependent and independent life stressors is associated with ROI GM structure, we conducted separate linear regressions for each of these stressor types with GMV as the outcome measure for subcortical ROIs (left and right amygdala and hippocampus), and SA and thickness as outcome measures for cortical ROIs (left and right mPFC). Additional linear regressions tested for gender and age moderation of these relations. Whole-brain exploratory analyses were conducted with the Permutation Analysis of Linear Models (PALM) tool (Winkler et al., 2014). Data at each vertex were shuffled 10,000 times, using sign-flipping for main effect and age-moderation models, and regrouping for tests of gender moderation. Exchangeability blocks accounted for dependence between data points created by 14 sibling-pairs and one sibling-trio in the dataset.
All analyses controlled for age and gender. Cortical SA and subcortical GMV analyses were conducted with and without controlling for average SA and total brain volume excluding ventricles, respectively, because these GM measures are associated with total head size, whereas cortical thickness is not (Barnes et al., 2010).
For clusters in which gender moderation passed FDR correction, we conducted follow-up analyses to determine what drove the moderation. We used the FreeSurfer function “mri_label2label” to map clusters from fsaverage space to subject space and subsequently extracted SA and thickness measurements for each participant. These values were used as outcome variables in gender-specific linear regressions, with stressor frequency as the predictor and age as a covariate.
Given the wide age-range of the sample and links between pubertal and neural development (Herting & Sowell, 2017; Juraska & Willing, 2017), we conducted follow-up analyses investigating whether pubertal development moderated the associations between dependent and independent stressor frequency and ROI gray matter morphometry.
To account for the large number of analyses conducted, but balance this fact with the exploratory nature of the study (given limited prior research as described in the Introduction), we analyzed and present the data at multiple levels of correction. For ROIs analyses, we present all uncorrected analyses in the tables, but focus the results and discussion on analyses that pass FDR correction (q<.05). For whole-brain analyses, we present uncorrected clusters with a vertex-wise threshold of p<.01 and a maximum vertex of p<.001 in tables and figures, but focus the results and discussion on clusters that pass FDR or FWE correction (q<.05). We present both FDR and FWE corrections for whole-brain analyses to illustrate which vertices passed the most stringent correction, and which passed a more lenient yet still stringent threshold.
Transparency and Openness
We report how we determined our sample size, data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures. Data and analysis code for ROI analyses performed using SPSS, version 27, are available2. Whole brain data are not available on OSF. Analyses were preregistered (footnote 1).
Results
Outlier datapoints (+/− 3 SD from the mean) in ALEQ or ROI measures were excluded from analysis (n=2 for dependent stress frequency and n=1 each for left and right hippocampus and amygdala GMV). On average, participants endorsed (i.e., rated frequency above “Never” [0]) 5.98 dependent stressors (SD=3.86, range: 0–17) and 2.74 independent stressors (SD=2.30, range: 0–11; Table S1). Dependent and independent stressor frequency correlated with each other and with internalizing symptoms, consistent with past work (Fassett-Carman et al., 2019; 2020; Supplemental Method and Table S2). Table S3 gives descriptive information regarding pubertal development of the sample.
ROI Results
No relations between dependent or independent stressor frequency and ROI GM passed FDR correction (Table 1). No gender (Table S4 & S5) or age (Table S6) differences in the relations between stressor frequency with ROI GM passed FDR correction. Pubertal development moderated the association between independent stressor frequency and left and right mPFC SA in regressions without controlling for whole-brain SA, and right mPFC SA when including whole-brain SA as a covariate: greater independent stressor frequency was associated with less SA earlier in puberty and greater SA later in puberty (Table S7, Figure S1).
Table 1.
Associations between Dependent and Independent Stress Frequency and ROIs Controlling for Age and Gender, with and without Whole Brain Control
With whole brain control | Without whole-brain control | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ROI | b | β | SE | t | p | b | β | SE | t | p | |
| |||||||||||
Dependent Stressors | L. Amygdala | −1.498 | −0.048 | 1.989 | −0.753 | .453 | −0.650 | −0.021 | 2.526 | −0.257 | .797 |
R. Amygdala | −4.682 | −0.146 | 1.983 | −2.361 | .020 | −4.331 | −0.135 | 2.500 | −1.732 | .086 | |
L. Hippocampus | 5.769 | 0.091 | 4.684 | 1.232 | .221 | 7.720 | 0.121 | 5.751 | 1.342 | .182 | |
R. Hippocampus | 2.412 | 0.040 | 3.876 | 0.622 | .535 | 4.313 | 0.072 | 5.067 | 0.851 | .396 | |
L. mPFC | |||||||||||
Area | 1.006 | 0.029 | 2.781 | 0.362 | .718 | 1.795 | 0.052 | 3.122 | 0.575 | .566 | |
Thickness | - | - | - | - | - | 0.000 | −0.010 | 0.003 | −0.102 | .919 | |
R. mPFC | |||||||||||
Area | 1.991 | 0.065 | 2.230 | 0.893 | .374 | 2.882 | 0.094 | 2.756 | 1.046 | .298 | |
Thickness | - | - | - | - | - | −0.001 | −0.043 | 0.003 | −0.454 | .651 | |
Independent Stressors | L. Amygdala | .239 | 0.006 | 2.421 | 0.099 | .921 | −1.763 | −0.046 | 3.052 | −0.577 | .565 |
R. Amygdala | −2.139 | −0.055 | 2.438 | −0.877 | .382 | −4.268 | −0.110 | 3.000 | −1.422 | .158 | |
L. Hippocampus | .672 | 0.009 | 5.763 | 0.117 | .907 | −3.494 | −0.044 | 7.055 | −0.495 | .621 | |
R. Hippocampus | −.312 | −0.004 | 4.761 | −0.066 | .948 | −4.353 | −0.059 | 6.196 | −0.702 | .484 | |
L. mPFC | |||||||||||
Area | −2.115 | −0.050 | 3.349 | −0.631 | .529 | −2.748 | −0.064 | 3.759 | −0.731 | .466 | |
Thickness | - | - | - | - | - | <0.001 | 0.011 | 0.003 | 0.120 | .905 | |
R. mPFC | |||||||||||
Area | .224 | 0.006 | 2.686 | 0.084 | .934 | −0.496 | −0.013 | 3.325 | −0.149 | .882 | |
Thickness | - | - | - | - | - | −0.004 | −0.124 | 0.003 | −1.330 | .186 |
Note. Bold=p<.05. Outcome measure for subcortical ROIs=GMV; Whole brain control for subcortical ROIs=total brain volume excluding ventricles; Whole brain control for cortical surface area=whole brain average area. No whole-brain control for thickness analyses as GM thickness is not associated with head size (Barnes et al., 2010). No results passed FDR correction for multiple comparisons.
Whole-Brain Results
Main Effects of Stressor frequency (Table S8, Figure S2)
No clusters passed multiple comparison correction.
Gender Moderation (Tables 2 & 3, Figures 1–4)
Table 2.
Gender moderation of Vertex-Wise Cortical Thickness Correlates of Dependent and Independent Stressor Frequency.
Stressor Type | Hemi | +/− | Region | Size (mm2) | MaxP (log(10)p) | X | Y | Z | Passes FDR | Passes FWE | Yeo Network |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||||
l | - | insula | 780.25 | 4.000 | −37.2 | 3.4 | 12.6 | y | y | VA | |
l | - | SFG | 366.33 | 4.000 | −8.8 | 49.1 | 21.4 | y | y | DMN | |
l | - | mOFC | 299.68 | 4.000 | −6.6 | 24.4 | −9.9 | y | n | Limbic | |
l | - | preC | 239.73 | 4.000 | −55.3 | 6.3 | 27.1 | y | n | DA/SM | |
l | - | rMFG | 211.18 | 4.000 | −22.9 | 38.4 | 22.9 | y | n | DA/DMN | |
l | - | SFG/MFG | 190.20 | 4.000 | −21.4 | 23.6 | 32.5 | y | n | DMN | |
l | - | MTG/temp pole | 616.07 | 3.699 | −48.1 | 1.5 | −28.3 | y | n | Limbic/DMN | |
l | - | rMFG | 399.48 | 3.699 | −37.0 | 48.0 | 7.1 | y | n | FPCN | |
l | - | SMG | 276.41 | 3.699 | −51.2 | −43.4 | 43.6 | y | n | FPCN | |
l | - | SPL | 124.68 | 3.699 | −24.3 | −50.0 | 60.5 | y | n | DA | |
l | - | rACC | 473.53 | 3.523 | −5.8 | 38.0 | 5.7 | y | n | DMN/FPCN | |
l | - | SPL | 126.14 | 3.523 | −31.4 | −38.0 | 41.1 | y | n | DA | |
l | - | EC | 273.75 | 3.398 | −22.4 | −9.4 | −27.2 | y | n | Limbic | |
l | - | IPL | 127.20 | 3.398 | −30.0 | −60.2 | 40.0 | y | n | DMN | |
l | - | STG | 118.25 | 3.398 | −59.6 | −45.1 | 19.3 | y | n | DMN | |
Dependent | l | - | IPL | 97.01 | 3.398 | −42.1 | −52.2 | 24.0 | y | n | FPCN |
l | - | lOFC | 392.40 | 3.301 | −33.6 | 29.1 | −12.3 | y | n | DMN/limbic | |
l | - | SMG | 85.31 | 3.046 | −43.9 | −51.5 | 38.3 | n | n | FPCN | |
r | - | MFG/FP/lOFC | 1316.52 | 4.000 | 22.5 | 58.7 | −2.1 | y | n | Limbic/FPCN/DMN | |
r | - | EC/fusiform | 650.18 | 4.000 | 35.9 | −16.0 | −26.2 | y | n | Limbic | |
r | - | pars operc/preC/postC | 519.26 | 4.000 | 43.2 | 9.3 | 17.4 | y | y | VA/DA/SM | |
r | - | insula | 442.16 | 4.000 | 32.3 | 8.1 | 9.7 | y | y | VA | |
r | - | IPL/SPL | 289.10 | 4.000 | 32.8 | −62.5 | 37.7 | y | n | DA/FPCN | |
r | - | cACC/PCC/SFG | 182.51 | 4.000 | 10.5 | 10.5 | 33.2 | y | n | VA | |
r | - | postC | 169.82 | 3.523 | 52.0 | −16.9 | 47.3 | y | n | SM | |
r | - | STG | 733.25 | 3.398 | 63.6 | −13.5 | 2.5 | y | n | SM | |
r | - | lOFC | 233.81 | 3.398 | 26.1 | 18.5 | −18.7 | y | n | Limbic | |
r | - | MTG | 168.38 | 3.398 | 59.8 | −52.4 | 5.5 | y | n | VA | |
r | - | mOFC | 134.43 | 3.398 | 9.6 | 14.2 | −15.1 | y | n | Limbic | |
r | - | ICC | 121.54 | 3.398 | 18.1 | 46.7 | 3.8 | y | n | visual | |
r | - | ITG | 248.15 | 3.301 | 51.1 | −43.9 | −14.2 | y | n | FPCN/DA | |
r | - | lingual | 114.85 | 3.046 | 6.7 | −85.8 | −4.6 | y | n | Visual | |
r | - | precun | 128.15 | 3.000 | 5.1 | −56.6 | 27.7 | y | n | DMN | |
Independent | l | - | SPL | 144.50 | 3.301 | −17.9 | −64.8 | 36.9 | n | n | DA |
l | + | insula | 207.73 | 3.097 | −36.1 | 2.2 | −9.7 | n | n | VA | |
r | - | SPL | 122.08 | 3.222 | 22.3 | −52.7 | 54.2 | n | n | DA |
Note. All results passed a vertex-wise threshold of .01 with a max p less than .001. Clusters with sub-clusters passing FDR and FWE corrections are indicated. y=yes; n=no; hemi=hemisphere; r=right; l=left; SFG=superior frontal gyrus; mOFC=medial orbitofrontal gyrus; preC=precentral gyrus; rMFG=rostral middle frontal gyrus; MFG=middle frontal gyrus; MTG=middle temporal gyrus; temp pole=temporal pole; SMG=supramarginal gyrus; SPPL=superior parietal lobule; rACC=rostral anterior cingulate cortex; EC=entorhinal cortex; IPL=inferior parietal lobule; lOFC=lateral orbitofrontal cortex; FP=frontal pole; fusiform=fusiform gyrus; pars operc=pars opercularis; postC=postcentral gyrus; cACC=caudal anterior cingulate cortex; PCC=posterior cingulate cortex; STG=superior temporal gyrus; ICC=isthmus cingulate cortex; ITG=inferior temporal gyrus; lingual=lingual gyrus; precun=precuneus.
Table 3.
Gender moderation of Vertex-Wise Cortical Surface Area Correlates of Dependent and Independent Stressor Frequency.
Stressor Type | Cortical Measure | Hemi | +/− | Region | Size (mm2) | MaxP (log(10)p) | X | Y | Z | Passes FDR | Passes FWE | Yeo Network |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||||||
Area | l | + | precun/ICC/SPL | 1879.11 | 4.000 | −6.9 | −48.2 | 46.0 | n | y | DMN/FPCN/DA | |
Area | l | + | STG | 314.45 | 4.000 | −58.3 | −12.8 | −3.0 | n | n | DMN/SM | |
Area | l | + | IPL | 297.33 | 3.699 | −36.2 | −80.7 | 24.1 | n | n | visual/DA | |
Area | l | + | postC/preC | 107.90 | 3.301 | −60.6 | −7.3 | 10.9 | n | n | SM | |
Area | r | + | precun/ICC/SPL | 2300.75 | 4.000 | 14.2 | −48.6 | 34.0 | y | y | DMN/FPCN/DA | |
Dependent | Area | r | + | SFG | 1083.61 | 4.000 | 21.0 | 20.0 | 50.7 | y | n | DMN/FPCN |
Area | r | + | IPL | 548.67 | 3.301 | 42.2 | −46.4 | 38.5 | y | n | FPCN | |
Area | r | + | IPL | 382.14 | 3.301 | 42.7 | −70.2 | 19.9 | y | n | DA | |
Area | r | + | SPL | 347.33 | 3.046 | 34.1 | −45.5 | 58.2 | y | n | DA | |
Area w/ WBC | l | - | cuneus/precun/periC | 1315.15 | 3.699 | −12.4 | −74.8 | 24.1 | n | n | Visual | |
Area w/ WBC | l | + | precun | 804.60 | 4.000 | −8.4 | −46.6 | 45.6 | n | n | DMN | |
Area w/ WBC | l | + | STG | 150.04 | 3.301 | −60.4 | −11.9 | 0.1 | n | n | SM | |
Area w/ WBC | r | - | pars operc | 536.72 | 3.097 | 49.1 | 6.8 | 8.5 | n | n | FPCN/DA/VA | |
Area w/ WBC | r | + | precun/ICC | 775.64 | 4.000 | 14.9 | −44.5 | 36.8 | n | n | DMN | |
Area w/ WBC | r | + | SFG | 548.74 | 3.301 | 21.2 | 23.9 | 49.5 | n | n | DMN/FPCN | |
Area | l | + | SPL | 288.91 | 3.097 | −14.6 | −62.1 | 57.8 | n | n | DA | |
Independent | Area | r | + | SPL | 906.59 | 4.000 | 34.0 | −42.0 | 47.2 | n | n | DA |
Area | r | + | precun | 224.31 | 3.301 | 7.2 | −64.4 | 34.6 | n | n | DMN | |
Area w/ WBC | l | - | fusiform | 608.17 | 3.046 | −31.1 | −73.6 | −7.0 | n | n | Visual | |
Area w/ WBC | r | - | banksSTS | 300.47 | 3.699 | 51.3 | −33.1 | 6.0 | n | n | SM | |
Area w/ WBC | r | + | SPL | 256.85 | 3.699 | 33.5 | −41.7 | 45.8 | n | n | DA | |
Area w/ WBC | r | + | precun | 217.26 | 3.699 | 8.9 | −63.7 | 35.0 | n | n | DMN |
Note. All results passed a vertex-wise threshold of .01 with a max p less than .001. Clusters with sub-clusters passing FDR and FWE corrections are indicated. WBC=whole brain control (average area); y=yes; n=no; hemi=hemisphere; r=right; l=left; precun=precuneus; ICC=isthmus cingulate cortex; SPL=superior parietal lobule; STG=superior temporal gyrus; IPL=inferior parietal lobule; preC=precentral gyrus; postC=postcentral gyrus; SFG=superior frontal gyrus; periC=pericalcarine; pars operc=pars opercularis; fusiform=fusiform gyrus; banksSTS= Banks superior temporal sulcus.
Figure 1.
Note. All results passed a vertex-wise threshold of .01 with a max p less than .001. Sub-clusters passing FDR and FWE corrections are indicated. RH=right hemisphere; LH=left hemisphere; SFG=superior frontal gyrus; mOFC=medial orbitofrontal gyrus; preC=precentral gyrus; rMFG=rostral middle frontal gyrus; MFG=middle frontal gyrus; MTG=middle temporal gyrus; temp pole=temporal pole; SMG=supramarginal gyrus; SPPL=superior parietal lobule; rACC=rostral anterior cingulate cortex; EC=entorhinal cortex; IPL=inferior parietal lobule; lOFC=lateral orbitofrontal cortex; FP=frontal pole; fusiform=fusiform gyrus; pars operc=pars opercularis; postC=postcentral gyrus; cACC=caudal anterior cingulate cortex; PCC=posterior cingulate cortex; STG=superior temporal gyrus; ICC=isthmus cingulate cortex; ITG=inferior temporal gyrus; lingual=lingual gyrus; precun=precuneus.
Figure 4.
Relation between Dependent Stressor Frequency and Cortical Surface Area for Male and Female Youth in Clusters demonstrating Significant Gender Moderation in Whole-Brain Analyses
Note. Clusters passed vertex-wise threshold of p<.01, with a max p<.001, and contained a sub-cluster that passed FWE and/or FDR correction; *p<.05; rh=left hemisphere; rh=right hemisphere; precun=precuneus; ICC=isthmus cingulate cortex; SPL=superior parietal lobule; SFG=superior frontal gyrus; IPL=inferior parietal lobule.
Only dependent, and not independent, stressors had gender moderation effects that passed multiple comparison correction. Gender moderated the relations between dependent stress frequency and cortical thickness in 32 clusters that passed FDR correction, four of which passed FWE correction (Figure 1, Table 2). These clusters were widely distributed, located bilaterally in the insula, superior frontal gyrus (SFG), medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC), precentral gyrus, middle frontal gyrus (MFG), middle temporal gyrus (MTG), superior parietal lobule (SPL), inferior parietal lobule (IPL), entorhinal cortex, superior temporal gyrus (STG), and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC). Further, gender moderation clusters were found in the left temporal pole, rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), and supramarginal gyrus (SMG), and the right frontal pole, fusiform cortex, pars opercularis, postcentral gyrus, caudal anterior cingulate cortex (cACC), posterior cingulate cortex, postcentral gyrus, isthmus cingulate cortex (ICC), inferior temporal gyrus, lingual gyrus, and precuneus. Gender moderation was driven by greater dependent stress frequency being associated with lower GM thickness in female youth and greater GM thickness in male youth (Figure 3, Table S9). In most clusters these relations were significant for both genders.
Figure 3.
Relation between Dependent Stressor Frequency and Cortical Thickness for Male and Female Youth in Clusters demonstrating Significant Gender Moderation in Whole-Brain Analyses
Note. Clusters passed vertex-wise threshold of p<.01, with a max p<.001, and contained a sub-cluster that passed FWE and/or FDR correction. *p<.05; SFG=superior frontal gyrus; mOFC=medial orbitofrontal gyrus; preC=precentral gyrus; rMFG=rostral middle frontal gyrus; MFG=middle frontal gyrus; MTG=middle temporal gyrus; temp pole=temporal pole; SMG=supramarginal gyrus; SPPL=superior parietal lobule; rACC=rostral anterior cingulate cortex; EC=entorhinal cortex; IPL=inferior parietal lobule; lOFC=lateral orbitofrontal cortex; FP=frontal pole; fusiform=fusiform gyrus; pars operc=pars opercularis; postC=postcentral gyrus; cACC=caudal anterior cingulate cortex; PCC=posterior cingulate cortex; STG=superior temporal gyrus; ICC=isthmus cingulate cortex; ITG=inferior temporal gyrus; lingual=lingual gyrus; precun=precuneus.
Gender additionally moderated the relations between dependent stressor frequency and cortical SA in clusters largely non-overlapping with the thickness clusters (Table 3, Figure 2). Clusters passing correction (five FDR, two FWE) were located bilaterally in precuneus, ICC, and SPL, and in the right SFG, IPL, and SPL. Gender moderation was driven by greater dependent stressor frequency being associated with lower SA in male youth and greater SA in female youth (Figure 4, Table S10). These relations were significant for both genders in most clusters. Controlling for whole-brain average area, no SA gender moderation clusters passed correction for multiple comparisons.
Figure 2.
Note. All results passed a vertex-wise threshold of .01 with a max p less than .001. Sub-clusters passing FDR and FWE corrections are indicated. RH=right hemisphere; LH=left hemisphere; control=whole brain average area; precun=precuneus; ICC=isthmus cingulate cortex; SPL=superior parietal lobule; STG=superior temporal gyrus; IPL=inferior parietal lobule; preC=precentral gyrus; postC=postcentral gyrus; SFG=superior frontal gyrus; periC=pericalcarine; pars operc=pars opercularis; fusiform=fusiform gyrus; banksSTS=Banks superior temporal sulcus.
Gender Moderation Follow-Up Analyses
Given the widespread gender moderation of relations between dependent stressor frequency and cortical thickness and SA, we tested whether this moderation extended to whole-brain mean thickness and SA. Gender moderated the relation between dependent stressor frequency and mean thickness (β=−.261, p=.005), driven by a significant negative relation for girls (β=−.349, p=.007) but not boys (β=.198, p=.143). There was a non-significant trend of gender moderation in the relation between dependent stress frequency and whole-brain average SA (β=.140, p=.057): split by gender, there was a significant positive relation for girls (β=.265, p=.042) but not boys (β=−.100, p=.464).
Age Moderation
Age moderated the relation between independent stressor frequency and GM thickness in one cluster in the IPL which passed FWE correction (Table S11, Figure S3). No other age moderation effects passed correction (Table S11, Figures S3 & S4).
Discussion
In the full sample of adolescents and emerging adults, there were no associations that passed multiple comparisons corrections between dependent or independent stressors and GM morphometry in the mPFC, hippocampus, and amygdala, or across the cortex. However, there were significant gender differences in relations between dependent stressor frequency and cortical thickness and SA, as well as pubertal developmental differences in relations between independent stressor frequency and mPFC SA.
Effects of Dependent vs. Independent Stressors
Our results supported our hypothesis that dependent stressor frequency would be more closely related to GM morphometry than independent stressor frequency: most relations that passed multiple comparison correction were for dependent rather than independent stressors. There are multiple mechanisms that may account for this. First, dependent stressors may cause greater stress reactivity than independent stressors, due to the knowledge that one contributed to causing these events and subsequent negative cognitions such as self-blame and rumination that can increase and prolong biological stress reactivity (LeMoult, 2020). These cognitive reactions or the greater biological reactivity could lead to structural GM changes seen for dependent but not independent stressors. Second, dependent stressors may be particularly common and salient during adolescence and emerging adulthood, given the heightened complexity and salience of peer relationships and greater academic pressure, which can lead to dependent stressors. This heightened exposure and salience of dependent stressors could increase their impact on adolescent brain development (e.g., Fuhrman et al., 2015), consistent with their heightened relation to psychopathology (Kendler et al., 1999; Technow et al., 2014). On the other hand, pre-existing structural differences may affect neural function and behavior in ways that increase the generation of dependent stressors, but not independent stressors. This possibility is consistent with the stress generation theory and evidence that traits or experiences (e.g., internalizing symptoms, poor EF, rumination) can influence behavior in ways that increase stressful events (Hammen, 2018; Snyder et al., 2016; 2019; Stroud, 2018). This can create a cycle whereby life stress increases symptoms and increased symptoms generate more stress.
Observed Gender Differences in GM Correlates of Stress
Greater dependent stressor frequency was associated with thinner GM for female youth and thicker GM for male youth in portions of the salience network (insula, ACC) and regions involved in cognitive aspects of emotion regulation in the frontoparietal control network (FPCN; MFG, lOFC, IPL, rACC) and limbic network (SMG, MTG and STG). Further, greater dependent stress frequency was associated with a thinner mean thickness across the entire cortex in girls but not boys. Thus, the gender differences in links between dependent stressor exposure and cortical thickness are widespread and may not be specific to the regions that passed correction for multiple comparisons. This possibility is consistent with recent evidence that chronic stress in rodents results in whole-brain volumetric changes compared to unstressed controls (Magalhães et al., 2018). Given that all neurons contain glucocorticoid receptors, the morphometric effects of high levels of glucocorticoids resulting from chronic stress may occur throughout the brain, rather than being limited to regions with the highest glucocorticoid receptor density (amygdala, hippocampus, mPFC). This possible generalized effect of stress may characterize adolescence, a critical developmental period, whereas more specific targets may be associated with life stress in adulthood. Further, these glucocorticoid effects may differ for girls and boys (Shanksy & Murphy, 2021), potentially leading to the widespread gender differences identified here.
Despite the possibility that the clusters with significant gender differences are part of more widespread effects, many of the regions identified in the current study are implicated in stress reactivity and regulation. The salience network is important for responding to stressful stimuli and coordinating neural and autonomic responses (Menon & Uddin, 2010; Gogolla, 2017). The insula and ACC, salience network hubs, are thought to integrate internal and external stimuli and use this information to regulate the engagement of the FPCN and default mode network (DMN; Menon, 2015; Gogolla, 2017), which showed gender differences in relations between dependent stressor frequency and cortical thickness and SA respectively in the current study.
Critically, the FPCN and DMN are both involved in stress processing. The FPCN is involved in cognitive regulation of stress and emotion, while DMN activity underlies inwardly-directed thought processes triggered by stressors, such as rumination. For example, FPCN-sites of gender-specific relations between GM thickness and dependent stress frequency in the current study, such as the dlPFC, vlPFC, and IPL, are crucial for the executive function (EF) processes that underlie cognitive reappraisal, a cognitively demanding strategy for emotion regulation. These regions support holding of reappraisal goals in working memory, selectively attending to information helpful to the goal of regulating emotion, and inhibiting interpretations of information that may interfere with appraisal goals (Ochsner et al., 2012; Banich et al., 2009). Further, the SMG and temporal regions (e.g., MTG, STG) identified in the current analyses outside of the FPCN are involved in representing perceptual and semantic knowledge and are activated during reappraisal, potentially as reappraisal works to change perceptions of stressful stimuli (Ochsner et al., 2012). Thus, the regions showing gender-specific relations between dependent life stress and GM thickness are implicated in the stress-emotion experience.
In addition to gender differences in the relations between stress and thickness, we observed opposite relations between SA and dependent stressors for male and female youth but in largely non-overlapping clusters (precuneus, isthmus cingulate, SFG, SPL and IPL) and in the opposite direction as for thickness: greater dependent stressor frequency is associated with less SA in male youth and greater SA in female youth. Different effects for SA and thickness are understandable given that these appear to be independent properties of brain morphology (Winkler et al., 2010). Most of the SA clusters identified are in the dorsal attention network or DMN, which subserve externally directed attention and internally directed thought, respectively (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002). The largest clusters identified (bilateral precuneus, right SFG) are part of the DMN which is associated with rumination (Zhou et al., 2020), a maladaptive response to life stress characterized by internally focused, repetitive negative thinking (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Higher exposure to stressors increases rumination (Snyder et al., 2016), which is a strong, transdiagnostic risk factor for internalizing psychopathology (Aldao et al., 2016). Importantly, female youth are more likely to ruminate than male youth (Rood et al., 2009), potentially putting them at heightened risk for internalizing psychopathology. However, it is not clear whether this aspect of morphology might compromise the ability to deal with stressors and/or lead to behaviors that would increase the effects of stressors (e.g., rumination) in female youth.
Potential Mechanisms of Gender Differences in Stress-Brain Relations
Here we consider why male and female youth show opposite relations between dependent stressor frequency and regional GM thickness, in particular. First, there may be gender differences in the neural impacts of stressors. Although the current study asked participants to indicate their gender and did not collect information regarding biological sex, we can draw from rodent model research investigating sex differences in the neural effects of stressor exposure for possible underlying mechanisms of the gender differences found. Rodent models reveal well-documented effects of chronic stress exposure on dendritic structure in male rats (atrophy in mPFC and hippocampus, hypertrophy in amygdala; Radley et al., 2016). In contrast, female rats exhibit largely opposite effects: chronic stress elicits dendritic shrinkage in the amygdala (Blume et al., 2019), no atrophy (or hypertrophy) in the hippocampus (Galea et al., 1997), and dendritic branching in the mPFC (Garrett & Wellman, 2009; Shansky et al., 2010; Farrell et al., 2016) that is dependent on the presence of estrogen (Garrett & Wellman, 2009). These results highlight that sex hormones are involved in shaping neural stress outcomes.
Studies in humans also provide evidence for sex differences in neural reactivity to acute stress, which could contribute to different relations with GM structure during development. Although different from the stressful events assessed in the current study, a recent study found that men and women showed opposite patterns of neural reactivity to aversive images in prefrontal regions (sgACC, OFC, dmPFC; increased BOLD signal for men, blunted signal for women) and limbic regions (hippocampus, insula/putamen, pallidum; increased BOLD signal for women, blunted signal for men; Goldfarb et al., 2019). Importantly, sex differences in neural reactivity to stressors could impact developmental GM thickness trajectories by driving synaptic pruning and branching, potentially leading to different associations between life stressor exposure and GM thickness in male and female youth.
Alternatively, gender differences could be due to sex differences in GM developmental trajectories. Male youth appear to lag 1–2 years behind female youth in their cortical GMV and SA trajectories and puberty onset (Lenroot et al., 2007). There is evidence for sex differences in the rate of GM thickness change across childhood and adolescence (Zhou et al., 2015) and region-specific sex differences in cortical thickness development (Gennatas et al., 2017; Ducharme et al., 2016) suggesting potential sex-specific trajectories depending on region in humans (see Kaczkurkin et al., 2019 for review).
It is thus possible that recently experienced stressors affect male and female youth differently due to neural sex differences in ongoing developmental processes. Perhaps female youth, having undergone more substantial pruning compared to the male youth (e.g., Zhou et al., 2015; Drzewiecki et al., 2016), demonstrate a stress-GM profile more similar to adults: stress, either having a neurotoxic effect or causing excessive pruning in these regions due to repeated activation during development, leads to excess thinning. Indeed, reduced GMV in prefrontal and salience networks are associated with internalizing psychopathology in adulthood, which is more prevalent in women than men (Lai, 2013; Stratmann et al., 2014; Herring et al., 2012). Male youth, being at an earlier stage in GM developmental or undergoing different region-specific developmental timing, may exhibit opposite effects of stress. This possibility lends further support to calls for investigations of sex-specific effects of developmental timing on the neural impacts of life stressors (Helpman et al., 2017).
A third possibility is that pre-existing differences in GM thickness could influence the frequency of dependent stressors via stress-generation. Individual differences in the morphology of brain regions involved in cognitive regulation might affect behavior in ways that bring about stressors. FPCN regions, and their regulation by the salience network, are crucial not only for emotional regulation but also for non-emotional aspects of cognitive control. Atypical GM thickness in these networks might impair cognitive control, both directly via GM structural abnormalities in the FPCN, or due to problems engaging the FPCN resulting from a dysfunctional salience network. This possibility is consistent with evidence that lower EF longitudinally predicts greater dependent stressor frequency, leading to psychopathology (Snyder et al., 2016). Further, EF improves across adolescence as FPCN areas undergo cortical thinning (Kharitonova et al., 2013), highlighting the possibility that individual differences in GM developmental trajectories could affect stress generation. It is unclear why the structural effects promoting stress generation would be opposite in male and female youth, but could be an instance of equifinality in which different neural mechanisms bring about a similar behavioral phenotype (Shansky & Murphy, 2021). Investigating how the GM clusters identified in the current study relate to behavior will be important for understanding their potential effects on exacerbating or protecting against dependent stress generation.
Pubertal Development Moderation of Associations between Independent Stressor Frequency and mPFC Surface Area
Pubertal development moderated links between independent stressor frequency and right and left mPFC SA such that greater independent stressor frequency is associated less mPFC SA earlier in puberty and greater mPFC SA later in puberty (Figures S1). This result for independent stressor frequency contrasts with the widespread gender differences seen in the neural correlates of dependent stressor frequency in the current study, and suggests that fateful life stressors are differently associated with SA at different points of pubertal development. This result builds on previous work suggesting that developmental timing of stressful experiences may affect their neural impacts (Teicher et al., 2016; Helpman et al., 2017), and highlights that pubertal events may be important mechanisms in these relations. One possibility is that changes in sex hormone levels occurring throughout puberty lead to altered neural structural impacts of stress. Sex hormones have been shown to interact with stress hormones to shape gray matter alterations resulting from stressful experiences in adult rodents (Garrett & Wellman, 2009), yet it is unknown how this occurs during puberty. Understanding the specific biological mechanisms driving the change in the neural correlates of stressors across puberty will be an important direction for future work.
Limitations and Future Directions
This study has limitations that are important to investigate in future work. These analyses are cross-sectional, preventing conclusions regarding directionality of effects. It will be important to try to differentiate stress generation versus neural sequelae of stressor mechanisms through longitudinal analyses. Additionally, despite the relatively large sample size, a recent study found that much larger samples may be necessary to detect robust brain-wide associations (Marek et al., 2022). However, in addition to widespread gender difference clusters in our cortex-wide exploratory analyses, we additionally found significant gender moderation of the link between dependent stressor frequency and whole-brain thickness. As this result does not rely on cortex-wide tests, it greatly strengthens the evidence for true gender differences in links between life stressor frequency and GM thickness. Future work should replicate these analyses in a larger, independent, sample to evaluate the replicability of clusters identified in the current study, including the precision of their location. As discussed earlier, it is possible that our observed effects are not location-specific but rather represent gender differences in links between dependent stress frequency and GM thickness across the cortex. The current study is also not highly-powered enough to investigate three-way interactions between age or pubertal development, gender, and stress. Our results highlighted the importance of looking at gender differences in relations between stress and GM–understanding how these interact with GM development will provide a better understanding of the mechanisms behind these effects.
Further, given neural developmental trajectories associated with increasing age (e.g., Giedd & Rapaport, 2010), the current study largely focused on age as an index of adolescent development. However, pubertal development is also associated with neural developmental changes (Herting & Sowell, 2017; Juraska & Willing, 2017), and may dissociate from age given individual differences in onset and trajectory of puberty. Indeed, pubertal development moderated the association between independent stressor frequency and mPFC SA in the current sample, highlighting pubertal changes as potential mechanisms shaping the impacts of stressor exposure during adolescence. The current study did not collect other indices of pubertal development such as sex hormone levels, which will be important for future investigations given evidence that stress hormones interact with sex hormones to impact neural structure (Garrett & Wellman, 2009). Further, a sample including younger participants than the current study would be better suited for a deeper investigation into how pubertal development may shape the effects of recent stressor exposure, as the current sample had already begun puberty at the time of the study (Table S3).
Despite these limitations, a strength of this study is that it investigated the relations between stressor frequency during adolescence and emerging adulthood and GM structure, which is an important step for understanding whether and how this developmental period acts as a sensitive period for the effects of stress, as many have proposed, but few have investigated (Romeo, 2017). Nonetheless, we did not investigate a comparable adult sample within these analyses, which will be important to fully understand how the correlates of life stress differ during adolescent and emerging adult development compared to adulthood, as others in the field have called for (Romeo, 2017; Fuhrmann et al., 2015).
Conclusion
Dependent stressor frequency was associated with GM thickness in salience and FPCN regions, and SA in DA and DMN regions, but in opposite directions for male and female youth. These results corroborate calls for greater focus on sex differences broadly in neuroscience research (Shanksy & Murphy, 2021) and specifically in work on neural effects of stressful experiences (Helpman et al., 2017). Further, pubertal development moderated the association between independent stressor frequency and mPFC SA. It will be crucial to better understand the importance of developmental changes on relations between life stress and brain structure, in particular during adolescence and emerging adulthood, a time posited to be a sensitive period for the effects of stress (Romeo, 2017; Fuhrmann et al., 2015). Future work should investigate these relations longitudinally and directly compare results in youth and adults to understand effects of life stress across development (Romeo, 2017; Fuhrmann et al., 2015). Last, these results highlight the importance of investigating dependent and independent life stressors separately to fully understand relations between life stress and the brain.
Data and Analysis: https://osf.io/p9xts/?view_only=0671799dd8c347cab1d7d3af2379d6d6
Supplementary Material
Public Significance Statement.
This study demonstrates that recent life stressor exposure is associated with opposite patterns of brain structure differences in female and male youth. This highlights the need for better understanding gender differences in how stress affects, and/or is affected by, brain structure during this developmental period.
Funding
NIMH grant R01 MH105501 to M. Banich and B. Hankin NIMH and NIGMS training grants T32 MH019929 and T32 GM084907 that supported A. Fassett-Carman Preregistration: https://osf.io/vnhbd/?view_only=b613871f41f44782b015f9867969e146
Footnotes
https://osf.io/vnhbd/?view_only=b613871f41f44782b015f9867969e146 The full set of preregistered analyses is beyond the scope of this manuscript. Additional analyses included in the preregistration are published elsewhere. See Supplemental Materials for explanation of analyses chosen for the current manuscript.
References
- Aldao A, Gee DG, Reyes ADL, & Seager I. (2016). Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the development of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology: Current and future directions. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt1), 927–946. 10.1017/s0954579416000638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alemán-Gómez Y, Janssen J, Schnack H, Balaban E, Pina-Camacho L, Alfaro-Almagro F, Castro-Fornieles J, Otero S, Baeza I, Moreno D, Bargalló N, Parellada M, Arango C, & Desco M. (2013). The Human Cerebral Cortex Flattens during Adolescence. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(38), 15004–15010. 10.1523/jneurosci.1459-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL, & Teicher MH (2008). Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(4), 183–191. 10.1016/j.tins.2008.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ansell EB, Rando K, Tuit K, Guarnaccia J, & Sinha R. (2012). Cumulative Adversity and Smaller Gray Matter Volume in Medial Prefrontal, Anterior Cingulate, and Insula Regions. Biological Psychiatry, 72(1), 57–64. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Banich MT, Mackiewicz KL, Depue BE, Whitmer AJ, Miller GA, & Heller W. (2009). Cognitive control mechanisms, emotion and memory: A neural perspective with implications for psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(5), 613–630. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes J, Ridgway GR, Bartlett J, Henley SMD, Lehmann M, Hobbs N, Clarkson MJ, MacManus DG, Ourselin S, & Fox NC (2010). Head size, age and gender adjustment in MRI studies: a necessary nuisance? NeuroImage, 53(4), 1244–1255. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fassett-Carman AN, Smolker H, Hankin BL, Snyder HR, & Banich MT (2022). Data and Analysis. Retrieved from https://osf.io/vnhbd/?view_only=b613871f41f44782b015f9867969e146 [Google Scholar]
- Fassett-Carman AN, Smolker H, Hankin BL, Snyder HR, & Banich MT (2020). Preregistration. Retrieved from https://osf.io/p9xts/?view_only=0671799dd8c347cab1d7d3af2379d6d6 [Google Scholar]
- Blume SR, Padival M, Urban JH, & Rosenkranz JA (2019). Disruptive effects of repeated stress on basolateral amygdala neurons and fear behavior across the estrous cycle in rats. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 12292. 10.1038/s41598-019-48683-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carskadon MA, & Acebo C. (1993). A Self-Administered Rating Scale for Pubertal Development. Journal of Adolescent Health, 14, 190–195. 10.1016/1054-139x(93)90004-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Connolly CG, Ho TC, Blom EH, LeWinn KZ, Sacchet MD, Tymofiyeva O, Simmons AN, & Yang TT (2017). Resting-state functional connectivity of the amygdala and longitudinal changes in depression severity in adolescent depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 86–94. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.09.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, & Shulman GL (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215. 10.1038/nrn755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale AM, Fischl B, & Sereno MI (1999). Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction. NeuroImage, 9(2), 179–194. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, & Killiany RJ (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968–980. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drzewiecki CM, Willing J, & Juraska JM (2016). Synaptic number changes in the medial prefrontal cortex across adolescence in male and female rats: A role for pubertal onset. Synapse, 70(9), 361–368. 10.1002/syn.21909 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ducharme S, Albaugh MD, Nguyen T-V, Hudziak JJ, Mateos-Pérez JM, Labbe A, Evans AC, Karama S, & Group F. the B. D. C. (2016). Trajectories of cortical thickness maturation in normal brain development — The importance of quality control procedures. NeuroImage, 125, 267–279. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrell MR, Holland FH, Shansky RM, & Brenhouse HC (2016). Sex-specific effects of early life stress on social interaction and prefrontal cortex dendritic morphology in young rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 310, 119–125. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fassett-Carman A, Hankin BL, & Snyder HR (2019). Appraisals of dependent stressor controllability and severity are associated with depression and anxiety symptoms in youth. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 32(1), 32–49. 10.1080/10615806.2018.1532504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fassett-Carman AN, DiDomenico GE, Steiger J. von& Snyder HR(2020). Clarifying stress-internalizing associations: Stress frequency and appraisals of severity and controllability are differentially related to depression-specific, anxiety-specific, and transdiagnostic internalizing factors. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 638–645. 10.1016/j.jad.2019.09.053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fassett-Carman AN, Smolker H, Hankin BL, Snyder HR, & Banich MT (2022). Neuroanatomical Correlates of Perceived Stress Controllability in Adolescents and Emerging Adults. Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience, 22(4), 655–671. 10.3758/s13415-022-00985-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, & Dale AM (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Procedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97(20), 11050–11055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Kouwe A. van der, Destrieux C, Halgren E. Ségonne F, Salat DH, Busa E, Seidman LJ, Goldstein J, Kennedy D, Caviness V, Makris N, Rosen B, & Dale AM (2004). Automatically Parcellating the Human Cerebral Cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 14(1), 11–22. 10.1093/cercor/bhg087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Liu A, & Dale AM (2001). Automated Manifold Surgery: Constructing Geometrically Accurate and Topologically Correct Models of the Human Cerebral Cortex. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 20(1), 70. 10.1109/42.906426 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Sereno MI, & Dale AM (1999). Cortical Surface-Based Analysis II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System. NeuroImage, 9(2), 195–207. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RBH, & Dale AM (1999). High‐resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Human Brain Mapping, 8(4), 272–284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuhrmann D, Knoll LJ, & Blakemore S-J (2015). Adolescence as a Sensitive Period of Brain Development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(10), 558–566. 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galea LAM, McEwen BS, Tanapat P, Deak T, Spencer RL, & Dhabhar FS (1997). Sex differences in dendritic atrophy of CA3 pyramidal neurons in response to chronic restraint stress. Neuroscience, 81(3), 689–697. 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00233-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ganzel BL, Kim P, Glover GH, & Temple E. (2008). Resilience after 9/11: Multimodal neuroimaging evidence for stress-related change in the healthy adult brain. NeuroImage, 40(2), 788–795. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garrett JE, & Wellman CL (2009). Chronic stress effects on dendritic morphology in medial prefrontal cortex: sex differences and estrogen dependence. Neuroscience, 162(1), 195–207. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gennatas ED, Avants BB, Wolf DH, Satterthwaite TD, Ruparel K, Ciric R, Hakonarson H, Gur RE, & Gur RC (2017). Age-Related Effects and Sex Differences in Gray Matter Density, Volume, Mass, and Cortical Thickness from Childhood to Young Adulthood. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(20), 5065–5073. 10.1523/jneurosci.3550-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerritsen L, Kalpouzos G, Westman E, Simmons A, Wahlund LO, Bäckman L, Fratiglioni L, & Wang HX (2014). The influence of negative life events on hippocampal and amygdala volumes in old age: a life-course perspective. Psychological Medicine, 45(06), 1219–1228. 10.1017/s0033291714002293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gianaros PJ, Jennings JR, Sheu LK, Greer PJ, Kuller LH, & Matthews KA (2007). Prospective reports of chronic life stress predict decreased grey matter volume in the hippocampus. NeuroImage, 35(2), 795–803. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd JN, & Rapoport JL (2010). Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going?. Neuron, 67(5), 728–734. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogolla N. (2017). The Insular Cortex. Current Biology, 27(12), R580–R586. 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogtay N, & Thompson PM (2010). Mapping gray matter development: Implications for typical development and vulnerability to psychopathology. Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 6–15. 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldfarb EV, Seo D, & Sinha R. (2019). Sex differences in neural stress responses and correlation with subjective stress and stress regulation. Neurobiology of Stress, 11, 100177. 10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammen C. (2018). Risk Factors for Depression: An Autobiographical Review. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 14(1). 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050817-084811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Mermelstein R, & Roesch L. (2007). Sex Differences in Adolescent Depression: Stress Exposure and Reactivity Models. Child Development, 78(1), 279–295. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00997.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Snyder HR, Gulley LD, Schweizer T, Bijttebier P, Nelis S, Toh G, & Vasey MW (2016). Understanding comorbidity among internalizing problems: Integrating latent structural models of psychopathology and risk mechanisms. Developmental Psychopathology, 28(4pt1), 987–1012. 10.1017/s0954579416000663 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Young JF, Abela JRZ, Smolen A, Jenness JL, Gulley LD, Technow JR, Gottlieb AB, Cohen JR, & Oppenheimer CW (2015). Depression from childhood into late adolescence: Influence of gender, development, genetic susceptibility, and peer stress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(4), 803–816. 10.1037/abn0000089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Helpman L, Zhu X, Suarez-Jimenez B, Lazarov A, Monk C, & Neria Y. (2017). Sex Differences in Trauma-Related Psychopathology: a Critical Review of Neuroimaging Literature (2014–2017). Current Psychiatry Reports, 19(12), 104. 10.1007/s11920-017-0854-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herringa R, Phillips M, Almeida J, Insana S, & Germain A. (2012). Post-traumatic stress symptoms correlate with smaller subgenual cingulate, caudate, and insula volumes in unmedicated combat veterans. Psychiatry Research, 203(2–3), 139–145. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herting MM, & Sowell ER (2017). Puberty and Structural Brain Development in Humans. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 44, 122–137. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2016.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hewitt PL, Flett GL, & Mosher SW (1992). The Perceived Stress Scale: Factor structure and relation to depression symptoms in a psychiatric sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 14(3), 1–11. [Google Scholar]
- Hollis F, Isgor C, & Kabbaj M. (2013). The consequences of adolescent chronic unpredictable stress exposure on brain and behavior. Neuroscience, 249, 232–241. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang C. (2015). Relation Between Attributional Style and Subsequent Depressive Symptoms: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 39(6), 721–735. 10.1007/s10608-015-9700-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Isgor C, Kabbaj M, Akil H, & Watson SJ (2004). Delayed effects of chronic variable stress during peripubertal‐juvenile period on hippocampal morphology and on cognitive and stress axis functions in rats. Hippocampus, 14(5), 636–648. 10.1002/hipo.10207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SB, Blum RW, & Giedd JN (2009). Adolescent Maturity and the Brain: The Promise and Pitfalls of Neuroscience Research in Adolescent Health Policy. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45(3), 216–221. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.05.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juraska JM, & Drzewiecki CM (2020). Cortical reorganization during adolescence: What the rat can tell us about the cellular basis. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 45, 100857. 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juraska JM, & Willing J. (2017). Pubertal onset as a critical transition for neural development and cognition. Brain Research, 1654(Part B), 87–94. 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaczkurkin AN, Raznahan A, & Satterthwaite TD (2019). Sex differences in the developing brain: insights from multimodal neuroimaging. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(1), 71–85. 10.1038/s41386-018-0111-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, & Gardner CO (2016). Depressive vulnerability, stressful life events and episode onset of major depression: a longitudinal model. Psychological Medicine, 46(9), 1865–1874. 10.1017/s0033291716000349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, & Prescott CA (1999). Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(6), 837–841. 10.1176/ajp.156.6.837 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kharitonova M, Martin RE, Gabrieli JDE, & Sheridan MA (2013). Cortical gray-matter thinning is associated with age-related improvements on executive function tasks. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 61–71. 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.07.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koolschijn PCMP, & Crone EA (2013). Sex differences and structural brain maturation from childhood to early adulthood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 106–118. 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, & Markon KE (2006). Reinterpreting Comorbidity: A Model-Based Approach to Understanding and Classifying Psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 2(1), 111–133. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095213 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn M, Scharfenort R, Schümann D, Schiele MA, Münsterkötter AL, Deckert J, Domschke K, Haaker J, Kalisch R, Pauli P, Reif A, Romanos M, Zwanzger P, & Lonsdorf TB (2015). Mismatch or allostatic load? Timing of life adversity differentially shapes gray matter volume and anxious temperament. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(4), 537–547. 10.1093/scan/nsv137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lai C-H (2013). Gray matter volume in major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 211(1), 37–46. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LeMoult J. (2020). From Stress to Depression: Bringing Together Cognitive and Biological Science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(6), 592–598. 10.1177/0963721420964039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenroot RK, Gogtay N, Greenstein DK, Wells EM, Wallace GL, Clasen LS, Blumenthal JD, Lerch J, Zijdenbos AP, Evans AC, Thompson PM, & Giedd JN (2007). Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. NeuroImage, 36(4), 1065–1073. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li H, Li W, Wei D, Chen Q, Jackson T, Zhang Q, & Qiu J. (2014). Examining brain structures associated with perceived stress in a large sample of young adults via voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage, 92(C), 1–7. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li Y, Hou X, Wei D, Du X, Zhang Q, Liu G, & Qiu J. (2017). Long-Term Effects of Acute Stress on the Prefrontal-Limbic System in the Healthy Adult. PLoS ONE, 12(1), e0168315-16. 10.1371/journal.pone.0168315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lotze M, Domin M, Schmidt CO, Hosten N, Grabe HJ, & Neumann N. (2020). Income is associated with hippocampal/amygdala and education with cingulate cortex grey matter volume. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 18786. 10.1038/s41598-020-75809-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magalhães R, Barrière DA, Novais A, Marques F, Marques P, Cerqueira J, Sousa JC, Cachia A, Boumezbeur F, Bottlaender M, Jay TM, Mériaux S, & Sousa N. (2018). The dynamics of stress: a longitudinal MRI study of rat brain structure and connectome. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(10), 1998–2006. 10.1038/mp.2017.244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, Montez DF, Kay BP, Hatoum AS, Donohue MR, Foran W, Miller RL, Hendrickson TJ, Malone SM, Kandala S, Feczko E, Miranda-Dominguez O, Graham AM, Earl EA, Perrone AJ, Cordova M, Doyle O, Moore LA, … Dosenbach N. (2022). Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature, 603(7902), 654–660. 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V. (2015). Large-Scale Functional Brain Organization. In Toga AW (Ed.), Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference (Vol. 2, pp. 597–611). Elsevier. 10.1016/b978-0-12-397025-1.00052-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V, & Uddin LQ (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 655–667. 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, & Lyubomirsky S. (2008). Rethinking Rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ochsner KN, & Gross JJ (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242–249. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ochsner KN, & Gross JJ (2008). Cognitive Emotion Regulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), 153–158. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00566.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, & Buhle JT (2012). Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251(1), E1–E24. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papagni SA, Benetti S, Arulanantham S, McCrory E, McGuire P, & Mechelli A. (2010). Effects of stressful life events on human brain structure: A longitudinal voxel-based morphometry study. Stress, 14(2), 227–232. 10.3109/10253890.2010.522279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paquola C, Bennett MR, & Lagopoulos J. (2016). Understanding heterogeneity in grey matter research of adults with childhood maltreatment—A meta-analysis and review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 299–312. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, & Drevets WC (2008). A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(9), 833–857. 10.1038/mp.2008.65 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radley J, Morilak D, Viau V, & Campeau S. (2015). Chronic stress and brain plasticity: Mechanisms underlying adaptive and maladaptive changes and implications for stress-related CNS disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 58(C), 79–91. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rakic P. (1988). Specification of Cerebral Cortical Areas. Science, 241, 170–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rakic P. (2009). Evolution of the neocortex: a perspective from developmental biology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(10), 724–735. 10.1038/nrn2719 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raznahan A, Shaw P, Lalonde F, Stockman M, Wallace GL, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Gogtay N, & Giedd JN (2011). How Does Your Cortex Grow? The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(19), 7174–7177. 10.1523/jneurosci.0054-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romeo RD (2017). The impact of stress on the structure of the adolescent brain: Implications for adolescent mental health. Brain Research, 1654(Pt B), 185–191. 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.03.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rood L, Roelofs J, Bögels SM, Nolen-Hoeksema S, & Schouten E. (2009). The influence of emotion-focused rumination and distraction on depressive symptoms in non-clinical youth: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(7), 607–616. 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ségonne F, Dale AM, Busa E, Glessner M, Salat D, Hahn HK, & Fischl B. (2004). A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. NeuroImage, 22(3), 1060–1075. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seldon HL (2005). Does brain white matter growth expand the cortex like a balloon? Hypothesis and consequences. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 10(1), 81–95. 10.1080/13576500342000310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shansky RM, Hamo C, Hof PR, Lou W, McEwen BS, & Morrison JH (2010). Estrogen Promotes Stress Sensitivity in a Prefrontal Cortex–Amygdala Pathway. Cerebral Cortex, 20(11), 2560–2567. 10.1093/cercor/bhq003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shansky RM, & Murphy AZ (2021). Considering sex as a biological variable will require a global shift in science culture. Nature Neuroscience, 24(4), 457–464. 10.1038/s41593-021-00806-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sled JG, Zijdenbos AP, & Evans AC (1998). A nonparametric method for automatic correction of intensity nonuniformity in MRI data. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 17(1), 87–97. 10.1109/42.668698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder HR, & Hankin BL(2016). Spiraling Out of Control: Stress Generation and Subsequent Rumination Mediate the Link Between Poorer Cognitive Control and Internalizing Psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(6), 1047–1064. 10.1177/2167702616633157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder Hannah R., Friedman NP, & Hankin BL (2019). Transdiagnostic Mechanisms of Psychopathology in Youth: Executive Functions, Dependent Stress, and Rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43(5), 834–851. 10.1007/s10608-019-10016-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sousa N. (2016). The dynamics of the stress neuromatrix. Molecular Psychiatry, 21(3), 302–312. 10.1038/mp.2015.196 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens FL, Hurley RA, & Taber KH (2011). Anterior cingulate cortex: unique role in cognition and emotion. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 23(2), 121–125. 10.1176/jnp.23.2.jnp121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stratmann M, Konrad C, Kugel H, Krug A, Schöning S, Ohrmann P, Uhlmann C, Postert C, Suslow T, Heindel W, Arolt V, Kircher T, & Dannlowski U. (2014). Insular and hippocampal gray matter volume reductions in patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS ONE, 9(7), e102692. 10.1371/journal.pone.0102692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stroud CB, Sosoo EE, & Wilson S. (2016). Rumination, Excessive Reassurance Seeking, and Stress Generation Among Early Adolescent Girls. Journal of Early Adolescence, 38(2), 139–163. 10.1177/0272431616659559 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sublette ME, Galfalvy HC, Oquendo MA, Bart CP, Schneck N, Arango V, & Mann JJ (2016). Relationship of recent stress to amygdala volume in depressed and healthy adults. Journal of Affective Disorders, 203, 136–142. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.05.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamnes CK, Herting MM, Goddings A-L, Meuwese R, Blakemore S-J, Dahl RE, Güroğlu B, Raznahan A, Sowell ER, Crone EA, & Mills KL (2017). Development of the Cerebral Cortex across Adolescence: A Multisample Study of Inter-Related Longitudinal Changes in Cortical Volume, Surface Area, and Thickness. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(12), 3402–3412. 10.1523/jneurosci.3302-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tau GZ, & Peterson BS (2010). Normal Development of Brain Circuits. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 147–168. 10.1038/npp.2009.115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Technow JR, Hazel NA, Abela JRZ, & Hankin BL (2015). Stress sensitivity interacts with depression history to predict depressive symptoms among youth: prospective changes following first depression onset. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(3), 489–501. 10.1007/s10802-014-9922-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher MH, & Samson JA (2016). Annual Research Review: Enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(3), 241–266. 10.1111/jcpp.12507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher MH, Samson JA, Anderson CM, & Ohashi K. (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17, 652–666. 10.1038/nrn.2016.111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tyborowska A, Volman I, Niermann HCM, Pouwels JL, Smeekens S, Cillessen AHN, Toni I, & Roelofs K. (2018). Early-life and pubertal stress differentially modulate grey matter development in human adolescents. Scientific Reports, 8(1). 10.1038/s41598-018-27439-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vijayakumar N, Allen NB, Youssef G, Dennison M, Yücel M, Simmons JG, & Whittle S. (2016). Brain development during adolescence: A mixed‐longitudinal investigation of cortical thickness, surface area, and volume. Human Brain Mapping, 37(6), 2027–2038. 10.1002/hbm.23154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Giedd J, Dale AM, & Brown TT (2017). Through Thick and Thin: a Need to Reconcile Contradictory Results on Trajectories in Human Cortical Development. Cerebral Cortex, 27(2), 1472–1481. 10.1093/cercor/bhv301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wierenga L, Langen M, Ambrosino S, Dijk S. van, Oranje B& Durston S (2014). Typical development of basal ganglia, hippocampus, amygdala and cerebellum from age 7 to 24. NeuroImage, 96, 67–72. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wierenga LM, Langen M, Oranje B, & Durston S. (2014). Unique developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and surface area. NeuroImage, 87, 120–126. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, & Nichols TE (2014). Permutation inference for the general linear model. NeuroImage, 92, 381–397. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou D, Lebel C, Treit S, Evans A, & Beaulieu C. (2015). Accelerated longitudinal cortical thinning in adolescence. NeuroImage, 104, 138–145. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou H-X, Chen X, Shen Y-Q, Li L, Chen N-X, Zhu Z-C, Castellanos FX, & Yan C-G (2020). Rumination and the default mode network: Meta-analysis of brain imaging studies and implications for depression. NeuroImage, 206, 116287. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.