Skip to main content
NPJ Science of Learning logoLink to NPJ Science of Learning
. 2023 Oct 6;8:45. doi: 10.1038/s41539-023-00194-7

Generalization of procedural motor sequence learning after a single practice trial

B P Johnson 1,2, I Iturrate 1,3, R Y Fakhreddine 1,4, M Bönstrup 5, E R Buch 1,, E M Robertson 6, L G Cohen 1,
PMCID: PMC10558563  PMID: 37803003

Abstract

When humans begin learning new motor skills, they typically display early rapid performance improvements. It is not well understood how knowledge acquired during this early skill learning period generalizes to new, related skills. Here, we addressed this question by investigating factors influencing generalization of early learning from a skill A to a different, but related skill B. Early skill generalization was tested over four experiments (N = 2095). Subjects successively learned two related motor sequence skills (skills A and B) over different practice schedules. Skill A and B sequences shared ordinal (i.e., matching keypress locations), transitional (i.e., ordered keypress pairs), parsing rule (i.e., distinct sequence events like repeated keypresses that can be used as a breakpoint for segmenting the sequence into smaller units) structures, or possessed no structure similarities. Results showed generalization for shared parsing rule structure between skills A and B after only a single 10-second practice trial of skill A. Manipulating the initial practice exposure to skill A (1 to 12 trials) and inter-practice rest interval (0–30 s) between skills A and B had no impact on parsing rule structure generalization. Furthermore, this generalization was not explained by stronger sensorimotor mapping between individual keypress actions and their symbolic representations. In contrast, learning from skill A did not generalize to skill B during early learning when the sequences shared only ordinal or transitional structure features. These results document sequence structure that can be very rapidly generalized during initial learning to facilitate generalization of skill.

Subject terms: Human behaviour, Learning and memory, Motor control

Introduction

Early learning of a new sequential motor skill develops rapidly during the initial training session1. Marked stepwise performance improvements are evident over short, seconds-long rest periods interspersed with practice1,2. These accumulating micro-offline gains account for most of total early motor learning, identifying a rapid form of consolidation of skill2. These findings extended the concept of consolidation to faster timescales than the hours or days previously reported3,4.

Previous work over longer time scales (e.g., several minutes or hours) that extend beyond early learning showed that acquiring one skill can facilitate the learning of subsequent new ones5. The property associated with this phenomena—generalization—has been previously identified across different dimensions of skill learning, including inter-manual or oculo-manual transfer (i.e., transfer of skill learning from one hand to the other or between eye and hand effectors)68, or transfer between different memory or skill domains (e.g., - transfer of learning from a word list to a motor skill or vice versa)911. The observed skill learning facilitation is dependent, in part, upon the history of previous experience1215.

Over these longer time scales, both training schedule and similarity of skill sequences may dictate how generalizable prior knowledge is, and how rapidly new skills can be learned1618. For example, the overall amount of training performed on a first skill prior to exposure of a second one may impact how learning generalizes18. Other features influencing generalization of sequential skills include the location of a given action in the sequence (e.g., the third action is the same in both sequences, shared ordinal location structure)19,20, or the repetition of pairs of consecutive actions (shared transition structure)21. In some cases, skills share more abstract features, beyond ordinal or transitional structures, like sequence regularity or a common parsing rule. Extracting such information could be viewed as a form of algebraic pattern learning21, and is potentially useful for identifying break-points where complete action sequences can be segmented into a series of smaller subunits – facilitating a process commonly referred to as “chunking”22 (e.g., when repeatedly typing the keyboard sequence 4-1-3-2-4 without interruption, the sequence can be divided into 4-4 and 1-3-2 based on the rule that the sequence starts and ends with the same keypress). This rule could later facilitate the learning of new sequences with similar abstract composition qualities23. The influence of these different factors on the generalization of early skill learning are not known.

Here, we tasked subjects with first learning a keyboard sequence skill A, followed by a second sequence skill B. We investigated how a newly acquired skill generalizes to subsequent ones during early learning over timescales as short as several seconds1. Specifically, we asked which features of practice schedule (i.e., – varying the amount of practice on skill A before beginning practice on skill B or the duration of the practice break in between) and sequence structure (i.e. – ordinal, transitional, or more abstract parsing rule structure) generalize during this early learning stage.

Results

We studied 2095 human subjects over four experiments using the Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) platform.

Generalization of early learning after different practice durations

The first experiment (n = 551) evaluated the effects of training of a skill A on generalization to a skill B as a function of skill A practice duration. Participants (See Supplementary Table 1) trained on a 5-item explicit motor sequence task performing keypresses with their non-dominant left hand. They were randomly assigned to one of four skill A (sequence 4-1-3-2-4) practice length groups (1, 2, 5, or 12 trials lasting 10 seconds each; 1 T, 2 T, 5 T or 12 T). After practice, subjects were tested on their performance of skill B (sequence 2-3-1-4-2; five 10-second trials). Practice trials were separated by 10-second rest intervals. Participants were instructed to repetitively tap the 5-item sequence indicated on the screen as quickly and accurately as possible on a computer keyboard of their choosing2. Written instructions (including illustrations) were visually displayed on the screen at the beginning of the task. Instantaneous performance was calculated as correct sequence tapping speed (keypresses/sec)1,24 to quantify absolute performance levels and microscale changes during early learning and as the number of correct sequences per trial, a trial-to-trial skill measure traditionally used in this task2527 (Fig. 1a, b; Supplementary Figs. 14).

Fig. 1. Influence of length of training of skill A on generalization to skill B during early learning.

Fig. 1

Experiment 1 evaluated the influence of length of training of a skill A on generalization to a skill B (n = 551; see Supplementary Table 1 for demographics). a Task: Participants were randomized to practice a skill A for 1, 2, 5 or 12 trials. Rest intervals between trials were 10 s duration. Practice of skill A (4-1-3-2-4) was followed in all groups by five testing trials of skill B (2-3-1-4-2). Skill was measured as the average inter-tap interval within correct sequences (tapping speed measured in keypresses/s). b Performance of the training groups (grey: skill A; cyan: skill B; mean ± s.e.m.). Training in skill A resulted in rapid performance improvements, consistent with previous work. Specifically, participants demonstrated rapid motor skill learning primarily during micro-offline periods which reached plateau by trial 11 in the 12 trials group9 (Supplementary Fig. 1). c Skill at the onset of skill B (red: 1 trial; blue: 2 trials; green: 5 trials; yellow: 12 trials). Following the end of practice on skill A, performance at the onset of skill B was comparable between experimental groups. d Change in skill from the end of skill A to the onset of skill B (i.e., GenB0Af). All groups had significant changes in performance between the end of skill A to the onset of skill B. The group that practiced one trial of skill A was the only group that showed GenB0Af, suggestive of a micro-offline contribution to generalization (shaded bar). *p < 0.05, where * over individual group plots indicate significant one-sample t-test within-group differences between B0 and Af and * between group plots indicate significant Kruskal-Wallis (pairwise comparison) between-group differences.

Performance at the onset of skill B was comparable across groups (Fig. 1c). Relating performance at the end of skill A and the onset of skill B (GenB0Af), we found that the one trial practice group was the only group that started skill B at a higher performance level than they ended skill A (p = 0.001, Fig. 1d). This finding, reminiscent of micro-offline gains reported during early learning2830, is suggestive of a form of rapid generalization of skill during wakeful rest1 (one-trial generalization, see Supplementary Discussion for Experiment 1 and Supplementary Fig. 1a)31.

One-trial generalization as a function of inter-skill rest interval

Next, we investigated the effect of varying rest interval durations between a single practice trial of skill A and skill B (Experiment 2; n = 795). Participants (See Supplementary Table 2) trained on a 5-item explicit motor sequence task with their non-dominant left hand for one trial of skill A (sequence 4-1-3-2-4) followed by 5 trials of skill B (sequence 2-3-1-4-2) interleaved with 10 s of rest. Participants were randomly assigned to one of five different inter-skill rest intervals groups ranging from 0 to 30 s (0 s, 2 s, 5 s, 10 s or 30 s). All task instructions and outcome measures were identical to those described for Experiment 1.

We found that varying inter-skill rest intervals did not modify performance at the onset of skill B nor micro-offline gains from the end of skill A to the onset of skill B (GenB0Af) (see Fig. 2 and Supplementary Discussion for Experiment 2 for details).

Fig. 2. Varying rest interval durations does not impact rapid generalization during early learning.

Fig. 2

Experiment 2 evaluated the influence of varying rest duration between skill A and skill B on generalization (n = 795; see Supplementary Table 2 for demographics). a Task: Participants were randomized to practice 1 trial of skill A followed by five testing trials of skill B (2-3-1-4-2) with either 0, 2, 5, 10 or 30 s following the end of practice of the skill A trial. Subjects in all groups performed equal number of total trials (six). Performance is shown for the single trial of skill A and the first trial of skill B for each of the five groups (grey: 0 s; orange: 2 s; green: 5 s; yellow: 10 s; purple: 30 s; mean ± s.e.m.). Performance in all 5 trials of skill B are shown in Supplementary Fig. 5. b Skill at the onset of skill B. Note the similar performance at the onset of skill B regardless of interval durations. c Change in skill from the end of skill A to the onset of skill B (i.e., GenB0Af). Note that all groups but the 0 s group experienced significant micro-offline (i.e., GenB0Af) gains. *p < 0.05, where * over individual group plots indicate significant one-sample t-test within-group differences between B0 and Af.

One-trial generalization of content

In a third experiment, we asked what specific sequence structure content is encoded in one-trial generalization (n = 537). All participants practiced the same skill A (sequence 4-1-3-2-4) as in Experiments 1 and 2 (see Supplementary Table 3). Subsequently, they were randomly assigned to 1 of 4 groups where skill B shared one of the following: (a) sequence parsing rule structure (i.e., the sequence started and ended with the same keypress; sequence 2-3-1-4-2; PARSING RULE), (b) transition structure (sequence 2-4-1-4-3 shares transitions 2-4 and 4-1 with skill A; TRANSITION), (c) both ordinal and transition structure (sequence 2-4-3-2-4 shares the triplet 3-2-4 in the third, fourth, and fifth positions, respectively; O + T), or (d) was identical to skill A (i.e., – extended exposure to A; SAMEA). Practice trials and rest intervals were 10 s long (Fig. 3a, b) and participants’ instructions and performance measurements were identical to Experiments 1 and 2.

Fig. 3. Content of generalization during early skill learning.

Fig. 3

Experiment 3 evaluated the content of generalization of skill A to four different skill Bs (n = 537; see Supplementary Table 3 for demographics). a Task: Each skill B shared with skill A different features: a similar parsing cue structure (sequence started and ended with the same number, i.e., 2-3-1-4-2; PARSING), similar transitions (i.e., 2-4-1-4-3, transitions 2-4 and 4-1; TRANSITION), a combination of similar transitional/ordinal features (i.e., 2-4-3-2-4; O + T) or a repetition of skill A (i.e., 4-1-3-2-4; SAMEA). b Performance in the first trial of skill A and testing of the four groups performing different types of skill B (grey: PARSING; green: TRANSITION; yellow: O + T; purple: SAMEA; mean ± s.e.m.). Note the similar performance in skill A across groups. Performance in all 5 trials of skill B are shown in Supplementary Fig. 7. c Skill at the onset of skill B. Onset of B was highest in the group that continued performance of skill A (SAMEA group). Note that skill at the onset of skill B in the SAMEA and PARSING groups are significantly larger than the TRANSITION group. d Change in skill from the end of skill A to the onset of skill B (i.e., GenB0Af). GenB0Af was highest in the SAMEA group, being significantly greater than the TRANSITION and O + T groups. Note that the SAMEA and PARSING groups showed significant micro-offline gains (i.e., GenB0Af), while the TRANSITION and O + T groups did not. *p < 0.05, where * over individual group plots indicate significant one-sample t-test within-group differences between B0 and Af and * between group plots indicate significant Kruskal-Wallis test (pairwise comparison) between-groups differences (Fig. 3c) or one-way ANOVA (pairwise comparison) between-group differences (Fig. 3d).

Performance at the onset of skill B was significantly different between groups (Kruskal-Wallis test: X2(3, 533) = 25.305, p < 0.001; Fig. 3c), higher in the SAMEA and PARSING RULE than in the TRANSITION group (both p < 0.001; FWE-corrected). Performance at the onset of skill B was higher than at the onset of skill A in all groups (GenB0A0; all p ≤ 0.015, Supplementary Fig. 6a). Micro-offline gains from the end of the first skill to the onset of the second (GenB0Af), were largest with continued practice of skill A (Fig. 3d). Further, micro-offline gains with continued practice of skill A were larger than those identified when skill B shared only transition or both ordinal and transition information with skill A (SAMEA vs TRANSITION, p < 0.001; SAMEA vs O + T, p = 0.050; both FWE-corrected) (See Supplementary Discussion for Experiment 3 and Supplementary Fig. 7).

Contribution of parsing rule structure and sensorimotor mapping to one-trial generalization

In a fourth experiment, we intended to separate the relative contributions of shared parsing rule structure and sensorimotor mapping (i.e., familiarizing the use of the non-dominant left hand to keyboard use) between skills to one-trial generalization. We tested two additional groups (n = 212) (see Supplementary Table 4and Supplementary Fig. 8). One group performed one trial of random order keypresses in response to single numbers (1–4) displayed on the screen, followed by a 5 trials test of skill B (2-1-3-4-2; RANDOM). We reasoned that generalization documented in this group would solely reflect improved sensorimotor mapping of the symbolic numeric sequence to keypress actions since no sequential skill A was learned. A second group performed one trial of skill B (2-1-3-4-2) followed by 5 trials of extended practice of the same skill B (SAMEB).

Analysis of these two groups with those acquired in Experiment 3 showed a between-group difference in performance at the onset of skill B (Kruskal-Wallis test: X2(5, 759) = 36.813, p < 0.001; Fig. 4b). Performance at the onset of skill B was significantly higher in the PARSING, SAMEA and SAMEB groups than in the RANDOM group (p = 0.046, p = 0.016, and p = 0.008 respectively). The contribution of ordinal and transitional information to performance at the onset of skill B was comparable to that of typing random keypresses (Fig. 4b).

Fig. 4. Contribution of similar parsing cue structure to generalization during very early learning.

Fig. 4

Experiment 4 attempted to dissociate the relative contributions of parsing cue and sensorimotor mapping structure to generalization during very early learning (additional sample added onto that of Experiment 3: n = 212; see Supplementary Table 4 for demographics). a Task: Experiment 4 added two groups: in one, subjects performed a single trial of random order keypresses (replacing skill A) followed by 5 test trials of skill B (2-1-3-4-2; RANDOM) and the second group trained over one trial of skill B (2-1-3-4-2) followed by 5 test trials of the identical skill B (SAMEB). Performance in all 5 trials of skill B for the SAMEB and RANDOM groups are shown in Supplementary Fig. 8. b Skill at the onset of skill B (mean ± s.e.m). Onset of B in the PARSING, SAMEA, and SAMEB groups were all greater than in the RANDOM group, demonstrating generalization of content beyond simple sensorimotor transformations observed in the RANDOM group. Conversely, onset of B was comparable in the RANDOM, TRANSITION, and O + T groups. Thus, ordinal and transitional information contribution to generalization was negligible, largely comparable to that of typing random keypresses. c Onset of B was greater in the PARSING group than in the RANDOM group, providing an estimate for the relative contribution of knowledge of the parsing cue to generalization during very early skill learning. *p < 0.05, where * between group plots indicate significant Kruskal-Wallis (pairwise comparison) between-group differences.

Performance at the onset of skill B was approximately 19% larger in the PARSING RULE than in the RANDOM group (Independent-samples t-test: t (250) = 3.577, p < 0.001; Fig. 4c) indicative of the presence of content and not only sensorimotor transformation in one-trial generalization. Micro-offline gains from the end of the first skill to the onset of the second (GenB0Af) in the PARSING RULE group represented 46% and 81% of those observed in the SAMEA and SAMEB groups (See Supplementary Fig. 9a, b)1, highlighting the substantial variation in generalization associated with skill structure during early learning.

To further disambiguate the contribution of shared parsing rule structure from nonspecific finger-to-keyboard sensorimotor mapping, we related performance at the onset of skill B in the RANDOM group from those in the PARSING, SAMEA AND SAMEB groups (See Supplementary Fig. 9d). Performance at the onset of skill B in the PARSING group was 76% and 66% of those in the SAMEA and SAMEB groups respectively. Thus, generalization of shared parsing rule structure was evident after one trial of practice, was substantial in comparison to micro-offline gains observed for continued practice of the skill1, and was not explained by non-specific improvements in sensorimotor mapping.

Discussion

We observed generalization to a novel skill B after a single practice trial of a different previously learned skill A. This one-trial generalization was explained by the combination of shared parsing cue structures between the two skills (i.e., two sequences composed of completely different keypresses start and end with the same keypress) and sensorimotor mapping. Lengthening practice in skill A or modifying the time interval between a single practice trial of skill A and skill B did not influence generalization. Overall, these results provide evidence for within-seconds, one-trial generalization of parsing cue structure during early learning of a new skill.

We identified micro-offline generalization consisting of better performance levels at the onset of skill B compared to the end of a single practice trial of skill A. The finding of rapid generalization during early learning is consistent with previous work showing that generalization can occur before memories consolidate32, when relationships between units within sequences form33, and memories are still malleable9. Even exposure to only one trial of practice in other memory domains is linked to significant performance improvements34.

We found that generalization following one trial was greater when the two skills shared the same parsing event cue (i.e., same keypresses at start and end of the two sequences) and weaker when they shared only transition structure. In fact, when the skills shared transition information, generalization was only observed for the invariant keypress-to-symbol sensorimotor mapping common to both sequences. On the other hand, generalization was approximately 20% larger when skills shared the same parsing event than when subjects typed random keypresses.

Rapid formation of a generalizable memory for such parsing event affords several behavioral advantages. First, it compresses two keypresses into a single, highly discernible event. Second, it parses the 5-item sequence into two parts: the unique double-tap event, and a smaller sequence chunk consisting of only 3 keypress events. Third, since the sequence is repeatedly typed, the double-tap event also serves as a temporal marker for initiating the execution of the 3-item chunk. Overall, these results suggest that different representations of motor skill may develop at different rates, beginning as early as seconds following the initiation of practice. Additional practice may be required to generalize ordinal and transitional representations of skill21 in the context of this task.

What neural mechanisms could support the formation of abstract memories (i.e., parsing cue structure) that can be generalized to a new skill after only a single practice trial? In the motor domain, early learning has been linked to wakeful hippocampus-neocortical neural replay. Neural replay, the reactivation of neural network activity patterns representing skill sequences, is prominently present during the rest period that follows the very first practice trial of a new skill28,30. Importantly, replay events in the entorhinal cortex and engaged neocortical regions support formation of memories that encode abstract relational structure between motor or cognitive states35, providing a possible substrate for generalization. Future research could evaluate if neural replay supports rapid generalization of abstract structural similarities during procedural learning.

There are several possible limitations that should be noted. First, keypress sequences may be easier or harder to execute depending on biomechanical or neurophysiological factors influencing coupling of finger motions. The impact of sequence difficulty on generalization of early learning remains to be addressed in future experiments. Sequence length is another issue we have not investigated and could potentially influence generalization. For example, the compression effect of a double-tap event may be attenuated as the sequence length increases. It would be interesting to determine whether sequence structure regularities generalized as parsing cues scale in size as sequences get longer (e.g., – a repeated doublet such as 1-2-1-2 would rapidly generalize for a 10-item sequence), or generalize over different time-frames.

In conclusion, our results document parsing structure generalization in the very early stages of skill learning (i.e., within-seconds and after a single trial of practice). This information may be useful for improving the design of practice schedules in the context of sports, music, and physical rehabilitation.

Methods

Participants

This study was approved by the Combined Neuroscience Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the National Institutes of Health. All participants were recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) and agreed to participate via an online acknowledgement of participation, rather than an informed consent form as this study was deemed exempt from the IRB. Inclusion criteria were: > 18 years of age, right-handedness, and living in the United States. The exclusion criterion included participation in previous sequence learning studies from our laboratory. Sample sizes were determined by conducting power analyses on pilot data previously collected on MTurk in our lab2.

After excluding subjects that did not adhere to the task instructions, the total sample size for all experiments in this study was N = 2095. Experiment 1 included 551 participants (216 women, 330 men, 4 other; M ± SD age 36.658 ± 10.748, See Supplementary Table 1), Experiment 2 included 795 participants (350 women, 444 men, 1 other; M ± SD age 36.794 ± 11.031, See Supplementary Table 2), Experiment 3 included 537 participants (268 women, 266 men, 3 other; M ± SD age 37.7 ± 11.800, See Supplementary Table 3) and Experiment 4 included 212 participants (81 women, 131 men, 0 other; M ± SD age 37.08 ± 8.630, See Supplementary Table 4). Participants were randomly assigned to all groups in each experiment.

Task

Participants completed an explicit sequence learning task via Psytoolkit36,37 using their non-dominant left hand on a computer keyboard device of their choosing in an environment of their choosing. The instructions indicated that participants should type a sequence of five keypresses (e.g., 4-1-3-2-4) as quickly and accurately as possible, as many times as possible throughout the 10 s long practice/test trials, then rest with their fingers on the keyboard and gaze focused on the screen word “rest” during rest periods. The numeric keys of the keyboard were used to perform the task, with the following finger-key setup: pinky finger (anatomical digit 5)-#1, ring finger (anatomical digit 4)-#2, middle finger (anatomic digit 3)-#3, index finger (anatomical digit 2)-#4. Confirmation of each keypress throughout practice trials occurred by displaying a dot on the top of the computer screen each time a key was pressed, regardless of whether the key was correct or not. In Experiment 1, participants were randomized to perform 1, 2, 5, or 12 10 s trials of skill A (4-1-3-2-4) before performing 5 10 s test trials of skill B (2-3-1-4-2), with 10 s of rest between trials and between skills. In Experiment 2, all participants performed 1 10 s trial of skill A (4-1-3-2-4), with 10 s of rest between trials, and were randomized to experience 0 s, 2 s, 5 s, 10 s, or 30 s or rest between skills, before performing 5 10 s trials of skill B (2-3-1-4-2) with 10 s of rest between all trials. In Experiment 3, all participants performed 1 10 s trial of skill A (4-1-3-2-4), with 10 s of rest between trials and between skills, before being randomized to perform 5 10 s trials of one of the following variations of skill B: 4-1-3-2-4; 2-3-1-4-2; 2-4-1-4-3; or 2-4-3-2-4. In Experiment 4, one group performed random keypresses for 1 10 s trial followed by 5 10 s trials of skill B (2-3-1-4-2), with 10 s of rest between trials and between skills; whereas another group performed 1 10 s trial of skill B (2-3-1-4-2) followed by 5 10 s trials of the same skill B (2-3-1-4-2), with 10 s of rest between trials and between skills. Regardless of experiment, all participants completed a questionnaire at the end of the task which included items regarding which hand was used to type the sequence, age and other demographic questions, and history of musical instrument use (see Supplementary Tables 14). We did not ask subjects if they identified this parsing cue consciously and thus could not dissect implicit/explicit contributions38.

Data analysis

Each participant’s performance was checked for adherence to task instructions. Task assignments were deemed to not adhere to task instructions if any of the following occurred: (1) participants answered that they used their right hand to type the sequence when asked at the end which hand they used; (2) completion of only one repetition of the sequence beyond trial 1 of a given sequence; (3) keypresses were consistently different from the instructed sequences; (4) deterioration of tapping speed performance over consecutive trials after an initial increase in tapping speed performance. Micro-online and micro-offline learning was determined as previously reported1,2. Micro-online learning refers to the difference in tapping speed between the first and last correct sequence of a practice trial. Micro-offline learning refers to the difference in tapping speed between the last correct sequence of a practice trial and the first correct sequence of a subsequent practice trial.

Skill performance was analyzed as previously described1,2. The tapping speed was defined as one over the mean temporal interval (in seconds) between consecutive correct keypresses (i.e., keypresses/s). The number of correct sequences per trial included partially correct sequences that were ended by the end of the trial.

Generalization can be measured in several ways (Fig. 5), including initial performance onset of a new skill, immediate or overall changes in performance between two skills, and the speed of acquisition of a new skill sometime referred to as learning to learn31. Here, we were agnostic to the choice of which measure to use, opting to display full results for the reader to evaluate. Generalization of motor skill was thus defined based upon prior literature: (1) change in performance between the end of skill A and the onset of skill B (i.e., GenB0Af)39,40, (2) initial performance at the onset of skill B41,42, (3) change in performance between the onset of skill A to the onset of skill B (i.e., GenB0A0)4346, and (4) the learning rate of skill B (i.e., κB; see Eq. 1)31. Individual subject performance curves were modeled with the following exponential function:

Lt=C1+C21eκt, 1

Fig. 5. Measures of generalization of motor skill.

Fig. 5

Generalization from a skill A to a skill B can be measured as: raw performance at Onset of B or the change in performance from the end of A to onset of B (GenB0Af).

Here, Lt represents an individual subject’s learning state on a given practice trial, t. Parameters C1 and C2 control the pre-training performance and asymptote, respectively. Finally, κ controls the learning rate. For a given sequence, the median correct sequence tapping speed was calculated for each trial. A constrained nonlinear least-squares method (MATLAB’s lsqcurvefit, trust-region-reflective algorithm) was then used to estimate parameters C1 (boundary constraints = [0,5]), C2 ([0,15]) and κ ([0,2]) from these observed tapping speed data.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 27 (IBM Corp). Equality of variance was assessed via Levene’s tests and data normality was assessed through observation of skewness and kurtosis values. Independent t-tests and analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used for normally distributed between-groups data, whereas the non-parametric equivalents of Mann-Whitney U and Kruskal Wallis tests were used for abnormally distributed data, respectively. In the event of statistically significant omnibus tests, post-hoc corrections for multiple comparison-related family-wise error were applied to decrease family-wise error rate. Within-group gains were analyzed with t-tests comparing group values with a reference value of zero. Bayesian ANOVAs or Kruskal Wallis tests, depending on data normality, with reference priors were used in certain cases to confirm that between-group statistics were statistically equivalent. All analyses were performed with an alpha level of 0.05.

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.

Supplementary information

Supplemental Information (902KB, docx)
Reporting summary (1.9MB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH, NINDS. We would like to thank Michele Richman for proofreading this manuscript. We thank Mimi Hayward, Tasneem Malik and Michele Richman for their support.

Author contributions

B.P.J.: Study design, data collection, data analysis, manuscript writing. I.I.: Study design, data collection, data analysis. R.Y.F.: Study design, data collection, data analysis, methods development. M.B.: Study design, data analysis, methods development. E.R.B.: Study design, data analysis, methods development, manuscript writing. E.M.R.: Study design, manuscript writing. L.G.C.: Study design, data analysis, manuscript writing.

Funding

This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the NIH. Open Access funding provided by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Data availability

Behavioral data are available upon request by contacting the corresponding authors (i.e., E.R.B. and L.G.C.).

Code availability

Custom-written code is available upon request by contacting the corresponding authors (i.e., E.R.B. and L.G.C.).

Competing interests

One of the authors (I.I.) works for Amazon, whose Amazon Mechanical Turk platform was used to recruit participants for this study. All other authors declare no competing financial or non-financial interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

E. R. Buch, Email: ethan.buch@nih.gov

L. G. Cohen, Email: cohenl@ninds.nih.gov

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41539-023-00194-7.

References

  • 1.Bönstrup M, et al. A rapid form of offline consolidation in skill learning. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:1346–1351. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bönstrup M, Iturrate I, Hebart MN, Censor N, Cohen LG. Mechanisms of offline motor learning at a microscale of seconds in large-scale crowdsourced data. npj Sci. Learn. 2020;5:1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41539-020-0066-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Robertson EM. Skill memory: mind the ever-decreasing gap for offline processing. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:R287–R289. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Squire LR, Genzel L, Wixted JT, Morris RG. Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 2015;7:a021766. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Krakauer JW, Mazzoni P, Ghazizadeh A, Ravindran R, Shadmehr R. Generalization of motor learning depends on the history of prior action. PLOS Biol. 2006;4:e316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cohen DA, Pascual-Leone A, Press DZ, Robertson EM. Off-line learning of motor skill memory: a double dissociation of goal and movement. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:18237–18241. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506072102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Japikse KC, Negash S, Howard JH, Howard DV. Intermanual transfer of procedural learning after extended practice of probabilistic sequences. Exp. Brain Res. 2003;148:38–49. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1264-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Perez MA, et al. Neural substrates of intermanual transfer of a newly acquired motor skill. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:1896–1902. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.09.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Mosha N, Robertson EM. Unstable memories create a high-level representation that enables learning transfer. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:100–105. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.11.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Robertson EM. Memory leaks: information shared across memory systems. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2022;26:544–554. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mutanen TP, Bracco M, Robertson EM. A common task structure links together the fate of different types of memories. Curr. Biol. 2020;30:2139–2145.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.03.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Korman M, et al. Daytime sleep condenses the time course of motor memory consolidation. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:1206–1213. doi: 10.1038/nn1959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Walker MP, et al. Sleep and the time course of motor skill learning. Learn Mem. 2003;10:275–284. doi: 10.1101/lm.58503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Zeithamova D, Preston AR. Temporal proximity promotes integration of overlapping events. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017;29:1311–1323. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Johnson B. P., Cohen L. G., Westlake K. P. The Intersection of Offline Learning and Rehabilitation. Front. Hum. Neurosci.15, (2021). Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2021.667574/full?&utm_source=Email_to_authors_&utm_medium=Email&utm_content=T1_11.5e1_author&utm_campaign=Email_publication&field=&journalName=Frontiers_in_Human_Neuroscience&id=667574 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 16.Braun DA, Mehring C, Wolpert DM. Structure learning in action. Behav. Brain Res. 2010;206:157–165. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.08.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dhawale AK, Smith MA, Ölveczky BP. The Role of Variability in Motor Learning. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2017;40:479–498. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Ajemian R, D’Ausilio A, Moorman H, Bizzi E. A theory for how sensorimotor skills are learned and retained in noisy and nonstationary neural circuits. PNAS. 2013;110:E5078–E5087. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1320116110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Jensen G, Altschul D, Danly E, Terrace H. Transfer of a Serial Representation between Two Distinct Tasks by Rhesus Macaques. PLOS ONE. 2013;8:e70285. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Terrace HS, Son LK, Brannon EM. Serial expertise of rhesus macaques. Psychol. Sci. 2003;14:66–73. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Dehaene S, Meyniel F, Wacongne C, Wang L, Pallier C. The Neural Representation of Sequences: From Transition Probabilities to Algebraic Patterns and Linguistic Trees. Neuron. 2015;88:2–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ramkumar P, et al. Chunking as the result of an efficiency computation trade-off. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:12176. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hadley RF. The problem of rapid variable creation. Neural Comput. 2009;21:510–532. doi: 10.1162/neco.2008.07-07-572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Vahdat S., Fogel S., Benali H., Doyon J. Network-wide reorganization of procedural memory during NREM sleep revealed by fMRI. Davachi L, editor. eLife. 6, e24987 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 25.Censor, Horovitz SG, Cohen LG. Interference with existing memories alters offline intrinsic functional brain connectivity. Neuron. 2014;81:69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fischer S, Nitschke MF, Melchert UH, Erdmann C, Born J. Motor memory consolidation in sleep shapes more effective neuronal representations. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:11248–11255. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1743-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Karni A, et al. Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature. 1995;377:155–158. doi: 10.1038/377155a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Buch ER, Claudino L, Quentin R, Bönstrup M, Cohen LG. Consolidation of human skill linked to waking hippocampo-neocortical replay. Cell Rep. 2021;35:109193. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Genzel L, Robertson EM. To replay, perchance to consolidate. PLoS Biol. 2015;13:e1002285. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Berners-Lee A, et al. Hippocampal replays appear after a single experience and incorporate greater detail with more experience. Neuron. 2022;110:1829–1842.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Seidler RD. Neural correlates of motor learning, transfer of learning, and learning to learn. Exerc Sport Sci. Rev. 2010;38:3–9. doi: 10.1097/JES.0b013e3181c5cce7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Robertson EM. Memory instability as a gateway to generalization. PLOS Biol. 2018;16:e2004633. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2004633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Henin S, et al. Learning hierarchical sequence representations across human cortex and hippocampus. Sci. Adv. 2021;7:eabc4530. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abc4530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Squire LR, Frascino JC, Rivera CS, Heyworth NC, He BJ. One-trial perceptual learning in the absence of conscious remembering and independent of the medial temporal lobe. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2104072118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2104072118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Behrens TEJ, et al. What is a cognitive map? Organizing knowledge for flexible behavior. Neuron. 2018;100:490–509. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stoet G. PsyToolkit: A software package for programming psychological experiments using Linux. Behav. Res. Methods. 2010;42:1096–1104. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.4.1096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Stoet G. PsyToolkit: A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teach. Psychol. 2017;44:24–31. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Song S, Cohen LG. Practice and sleep form different aspects of skill. Nat. Commun. 2014;5:3407. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Cantarero G, Tang B, O’Malley R, Salas R, Celnik P. Motor learning interference is proportional to occlusion of LTP-like plasticity. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:4634–4641. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4706-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Krakauer JW, Ghez C, Ghilardi MF. Adaptation to visuomotor transformations: consolidation, interference, and forgetting. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:473–478. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4218-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Genzel L, et al. Complex motor sequence skills profit from sleep. Neuropsychobiology. 2012;66:237–243. doi: 10.1159/000341878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.King BR, et al. Schema and motor-memory consolidation. Psychol. Sci. 2019;30:963–978. doi: 10.1177/0956797619847164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ghilardi MF, Moisello C, Silvestri G, Ghez C, Krakauer JW. Learning of a sequential motor skill comprises explicit and implicit components that consolidate differently. J. Neurophysiol. 2009;101:2218–2229. doi: 10.1152/jn.01138.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Krishnan C, Washabaugh EP, Reid CE, Althoen MM, Ranganathan R. Learning new gait patterns: Age-related differences in skill acquisition and interlimb transfer. Exp. Gerontol. 2018;111:45–52. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2018.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lundbye-Jensen J, Petersen TH, Rothwell JC, Nielsen JB. Interference in ballistic motor learning: specificity and role of sensory error signals. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e17451. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sainburg RL, Wang J. Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information. Exp. Brain Res. 2002;145:437–447. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1140-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Information (902KB, docx)
Reporting summary (1.9MB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

Behavioral data are available upon request by contacting the corresponding authors (i.e., E.R.B. and L.G.C.).

Custom-written code is available upon request by contacting the corresponding authors (i.e., E.R.B. and L.G.C.).


Articles from NPJ Science of Learning are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES