Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2023 Oct 13;13:17382. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-44730-2

Emotional labor mediates the associations between self-consciousness and flow in dancers

Xiaohui Liu 1, Yu Liao 2, Jiayi Tan 3, Jifang Duan 3,, Ruixiang Gao 2,4,
PMCID: PMC10576066  PMID: 37833413

Abstract

Emotional labor has been a focal point in occupational well-being literature, but studies have long overlooked an important group of emotional laborers: performers. This research represents a pioneering effort to examine dancers’ adoption of emotional labor strategies, their antecedent of self-consciousness, and the outcome of flow experience. We explored these elements both in the traditional setting of stage dancing and in the novel context of online dance classes without on-site spectators during the COVID-19 pandemic. The results revealed that dancers employed all three common emotional labor strategies: surface acting, deep acting, and expression of naturally felt emotions, with deep acting being the most frequent. In the traditional setting, only the expression of naturally felt emotions mediated the positive effect of private self-consciousness and the negative effect of public self-consciousness on flow experience. In contrast, in the online setting, only private self-consciousness impacted flow through the mediation of deep acting and expression of naturally felt emotions. This exploratory study bridges dramaturgy-originated theories of emotional labor with empirical performing arts research, preliminarily advancing knowledge in the relevant fields of dance education, self-presentation, and flow studies.

Subject terms: Psychology, Human behaviour

Introduction

With an aim to improve sustainability of employee’s occupational development, it is important for organizations to cultivate work environments supportive of employees’ mental health and well-being. Consequently, emotions in work have received a burgeoning rise of research attention in the field of organizational and management psychology. Emotional labor, first proposed by sociologist Arlie Russell Hochschild in 1983, is the idea of managing emotions to create a publicly appropriate display according to the occupational demands1. It is regarded as the third kind of work role, parallel to physical labor and intellectual labor, that is most prototypical in front-line jobs which require “service with a smile”2. As the organizational sciences literature has gradually recognized the value of understanding emotions at work3, the past three decades has seen an unprecedented growth in the focal area of emotional labor research4. While emotional labor examination has been done on a variety of service organizations or workplaces, such as hotels, banks, hospitals, airports, stores, call centers, etc.5, the theory, in fact, originated from a “dramaturgical” perspective comparing service encounters to theatrical performances1. However, despite this dramaturgical origin, there has been a significant gap in empirically examining emotional labor processes among professional performers and actors.

Dancers represent an insightful, albeit complex, performing arts group to investigate emotional labor theories given the emotion-laden and diverse nature of their work. Expressing appropriate emotions through their dancing is a crucial occupational requirement. However, no research has specifically explored dancers use of emotional labor strategies or linked them to indicators of dancers’ performance and well-being. Testing emotional labor theories in dance contexts is particularly insightful, as it not only provides novel evidence to strengthen and expand the framework beyond traditional service industry settings but also brings the investigation back to the setting on which the concepts were originally based.

In the current study, we aimed to address this research gap by investigating the emotional labor strategies utilized by dancers and relating these strategies to dancers’ self-consciousness as the antecedent and their experience of flow as the subsequent well-being outcome. Self-consciousness in this context refers to an acute awareness of oneself, influencing how individuals perceive their role and interactions with the spectators during a performance. Flow, on the other hand, represents a state of optimal experience characterized by focused attention, loss of self-consciousness, and enhanced intrinsic motivation, which is particularly relevant for performance-based occupations like dancing. Exploring these uncharted relationships for dancers bridges dramaturgical theories of emotional labor and empirical performing arts research, advancing conceptual knowledge of emotional labor in an underexamined yet highly relevant occupational context and offering practical implications for facilitating well-being among this understudied group of performers.

Emotional labor strategies of dancers

Interestingly, although emotional labor of actors has rarely been empirically studied, Hochschild’s definition of emotional labor1 actually stemmed from the “dramaturgical” perspective of server-customer interactions, which compared service employees who carried out emotional labor at the workplace for gratification of the customers to actors “employing expressive devices” at the stage to entertain the audience6 (p. 430). There are two major ways for such employment of expressive devices. According to the Stanislavski method of acting7, drama actors are encouraged to truly feel the emotions of the characters that they portrayed at any given moment. In contrast, other viewpoints, such as those of Brecht and Meyerhold, “considered an emotionally detached actor more capable of arousing maximum emotional effects in the spectator”8 (p. 132). The school of Chinese Mei Lanfang also held that actors should play the characters in a stylized and fixed way without the need of inhabiting authentic emotions while on stage9. Similarly, there are two possible strategies for employees to conduct emotional labor. To achieve appropriate outward manifestation of emotions for their occupational goals, employees can either pursue to modify the inner feelings through deep acting (DA), or put efforts into just projecting certain facial expressions or forms of body language through surface acting (SA) without actually experiencing the emotions displayed1. Both of these types of emotion regulation processes to reach congruence between emotional requirements from the jobs and the corresponding emotional performance of individual employees have been widely identified in nearly all trades and professions that involve human interaction10.

Ashforth and Humphrey argued that focusing on DA and SA only might ignore the possibility that individuals at work were sometime able to spontaneously experience and display the appropriate emotions though they might still have to put forth some conscious efforts to ensure that these feelings and displays coincide with the organization’s expectations11. Therefore, Ashforth and Humphrey considered the expression of naturally felt emotions (ENFE) to be a third kind of emotional labor strategies11, and this tripartite dimensionality of emotional labor strategy was later confirmed by Diefendorff et al.’s empirical research12. For acting itself, the genuine way of ENFE also echoes Brook and Grotowski’s approach of self-expression, but its ultimate objective is identical to those of the other two main streams (Stanislavski versus Brecht) mentioned above, which is that the actors should present his most inner self on stage8. Nevertheless, these theatrical theories with respect to actors’ expression of emotions have been developed in the 60s and 70s, yet few empirical evidence has been provided for the debate. Given that drama actors’ use of emotional labor strategies might be a fixed result of their training from a certain acting school, the major purpose of this present paper is to examined emotional labor strategies of a similar group, dance performers, whose area does not have such strong theoretical schools in terms of emotional expression. And the first research question of our study is whether the tripartite dimensionality of emotional labor strategy holds up for dancers, i.e., whether SA, DA, and ENFE are three distinct methods for dancers to display desired emotions.

Self-consciousness as the antecedent of dancers’ emotional labor

In emotional labor research, Grandey’s integrative conceptual framework is a classic model that is often applied and tested in quantitative studies, which posits that individual difference antecedents and individuals’ well-being outcomes are generally mediated by the emotional labor strategies they adopted10. For individual difference antecedents, age, sex, personality traits, work motives, and emotional labor abilities have shown associations with emotional labor strategies13. In the present study, we aimed to focus on an individual characteristic more relevant to the self-presentation of dancers—self-consciousness.

According to the self-presentation theory, when one hopes to leave a good impression on others, they are presenting themselves1416. However, although everyone is constantly presenting themselves, there are significant individual differences in the degree of attention they pay to their public image17. Self-consciousness is the “consistent tendency of persons to direct attention inward or outward” (p. 552) and is therefore conceived as having two major components, private self-consciousness (PrSC) and public self-consciousness (PuSC)18. People with a higher level of PuSC are not only concerned more about their physical appearance and overt behaviors but also more sensitive to others’ gaze, evaluations, and reactions towards their own appearance and behaviors, whereas those with a higher level of PrSC tend to think and reflect more about internal states and are less likely to yield to social pressure and alter their acts according to others’ thoughts and feelings1922. Accordingly, we can develop the following hypotheses linking self-consciousness to the use of emotional labor strategies.

Publicly self-conscious individuals are attuned to role expectations, sensitive to social cues, and accustomed to using these cues to regulate their behaviors2325. This heightened awareness of constant evaluation may prompt them to prioritize the regulation of observable expressions, potentially compromising inner emotional alignment. Consequently, this inclination can foster a reliance on SA, where genuine feelings are suppressed to align with external expectations26,27, leading to a corresponding decrease in the utilization of DA and ENFE. This is because aligning internal emotions with external expectations becomes particularly challenging when the emphasis is on managing observable expressions. These dynamics are likely to be especially pronounced for dancers, who frequently perform under intense public scrutiny and pressure28,29. Based on this, we hypothesize that dancers’ PuSC is positively associated with their use of SA and negatively correlated with both DA and ENFE.

H1:

PuSC is negatively associated with ENFE.

H2:

PuSC is negatively associated with DA.

H3:

PuSC is positively associated with SA.

At the same time, individuals with high Private Self-Consciousness (PrSC) aim to enhance their performance through continuous monitoring and evaluation of their behaviors18. However, in contrast to those with high PuSC, they are more focused on ensuring the congruence between their public presentation and their own beliefs and attitudes, rather than adapting to the expectations of others30,31. This steadfastness in maintaining internal alignment across different situations suggests a potential positive correlation with DA and ENFE, as both involve authenticity and consistency in emotional expression. Correspondingly, individuals with high PrSC are less likely to engage in SA, an insincere and tactical self-presentation strategy32. Thus, we hypothesize that dancers with high PrSC are likely to exhibit a positive association with both DA and ENFE, and a negative association with SA.

H4:

PrSC is positively associated with ENFE.

H5:

PrSC is positively associated with DA.

H6:

PrSC is negatively associated with SA.

Flow experience as the outcome of dancers’ emotional labor

In emotional labor research, employee well-being (e.g., job satisfaction, job burnout) is the most commonly discussed outcome of emotional labor strategy adoption, such that SA is the most problematic, that DA is less but still detrimental, and that ENFE is the most beneficial4,33,34. The dissonance or inauthenticity mechanism and the resource gains/losses mechanism are the two significant theoretical approaches to make sense of these findings (for a review, see13). However, as researchers have pointed out that since employee’s emotional labor is dynamic and varies across time and tasks3537, employee well-being outcomes that could only be manifested in the long run should be better revealed by longitudinal analyses3840. Thus, in this current cross-sectional study, we decided to examine a well-being outcome which is highly momentary and instantaneous and is also of vital relation with dancers’ performance—flow experience, an optimal positive psychological state characterized by deep engagement, absorption, and enjoyment41. This concept, introduced by Csikszentmihalyi in 197542, has been the focus of a large body of empirical work in positive psychology spanning over four decades43 because of its prominent function of promoting people’s well-being44.

The literature claims three essential conditions that lead to the experience of flow: a defined set of goals, a balance between challenges and skills, and the availability of immediate and clear feedback45. Certain activities such as dancing are more conducive to flow as they create a challenging and goal-directed setting for the participants and provide more feasible feedback structures that the participants can utilize to know about their progress in developing new skills46. However, existing quantitative research on flow experience among dancers is very limited47, so our study probing into the relationships between dancers’ flow and use of emotional labor strategies is meaningful.

Both the dissonance or inauthenticity mechanism and the resource gains/losses mechanism can shed light on the possible consequences of emotional labor strategy adoption on flow experience—DA and ENFE might contribute to flow, whereas SA could produce negative or null effects. According to the cognitive dissonance theory48, SA requires one to be incongruent with the self1, and such dissonance between the mind and the body violates the fundamental and inherent nature of flow. However, since DA “involves deceiving oneself as much as deceiving others”1 (p. 33), its consistency at least lays the foundation for the occurrence of flow despite some costs. As long as one is enjoying and devoted to the process of such “deceiving”, they might be able to experience flow therein. And because the authenticity of ENFE enables one to stay true to himself or herself, it is most likely to result in flow experience.

From a different perspective of the resource conservation theory49, emotional labor is the process that “workers invest their personal and social resources to help them cope with the service encounter and in anticipation of being able to recoup their investment as a result of the service encounter”50 (p. 64). However, except for ENFE that scarcely depletes psychological resources, both DA and SA have different degrees of emotional energy consumption, but which one is more of a drain remains disputed at present51,52. In addition, there is also a difference in the social resource gains brought by SA, DA, and ENFE to compensate the energy loss. Deep actors actively reappraise the negative events and try to arouse positive feelings inside, which itself is a process of deriving mental resources53. Moreover, displaying genuine positive emotions such as enthusiasm, care, and sympathy from the bottom of the heart is more likely to elicit positive interpersonal feedback from the targets of emotional labor, which not only functions as an important source of social support but also in turn reinforces the employee’s motives and efficacy to continue DA and ENFE5459. Such a positive loop provides a favorable condition for the rise of flow. By contrast, SA signals a lack of skills to successfully manage the challenging and demanding emotion regulation process at work and often results in negative feedback such as dissatisfaction and anger from the targets of emotional labor54,60, such that there is a net loss in emotional resources for surface actors over time. All in all, the hypothesized relationships between emotional labor strategies and flow are summed as follows.

H7:

ENFE is positively associated with flow experience.

H8:

DA is positively associated with flow experience.

H9:

SA is negatively associated with flow experience.

The relationships between self-consciousness and flow experience

While the above nine hypotheses have already formed a mediating model in line with Grandey’s conceptual framework10, the antecedent (self-consciousness) might have a direct impact on the outcome (flow experience) without the mediation of emotional labor strategy adoption. As research has demonstrated that individuals high on PuSC are more likely to experience negative emotions such as embarrassment, anxiety, and fear in response to the perceptions of others61,62, it can be inferred that PuSC negatively correlates with flow experience. Meanwhile, people with high PrSC tend to have a preoccupation with internal states or an open receptivity to them, and this disposition is believed to overlap with another similar psychological construct of “mindfulness”, a desirable quality of consciousness, awareness, or attention of what is taking place in the present, which is proved facilitate the maintenance and enhancement of well-being63,64. Consequently, PrSC is expected to be positively associated with flow experience.

H10:

PuSC is negatively associated with flow experience.

H11:

PrSC is positively associated with flow experience.

Figure 1 summarizes all 11 assumptions in a mediating model in the light of Grandey’s framework10, and whether this model can be successfully established remains the second important question that our current research intended to resolve.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The hypothesized mediation model.

The present study

To our best knowledge, this is the first empirical study on the emotional labor strategies of dancers as well as its antecedent and outcome. As mentioned above, there are two main research questions in our study. The first one is to verify whether the three-dimensional structure of emotional labor strategies (ENFE, DA, and SA) is tenable for dance performers. As is advised by Nunnally and Bernstein65, this question relates to the construct validity of the emotional labor strategy scale for dancers and can be assessed by exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The second question is to test the eleven-hypothesis mediation model, which is often conducted through structural equation modeling or the improved causal steps approach according to Wen and Liu66. Answering these two questions would add to the literature regarding the application of emotional labor theories (e.g., the Grandey’s classic model, research conservation theory) in the field of dance research and would bring practical implications concerning how to enhance the well-being of dance performers67.

What makes the present study even more interesting is that two samples of dance major college students were recruited during their daily dance training and show rehearsal to complete the same sets of questionnaire that consists of three scales measuring self-consciousness, emotional labor strategies, and flow experience, respectively. While the first sample of students were dancing together at the stage as usual, the second sample were attending the online dance class and dancing alone to the camera as a result of the closure of colleges during the COVID-19 pandemic, which provided us with a perfect opportunity to explore the difference in dancers’ emotional labor strategy employment without spectators on site. Examining dancers’ emotional labor in different situations would better enrich our knowledge about its nature, as Grandey has pointed out that emotional labor is actually an integrated process of emotional requirements (environmental stimulus), emotion regulation (intrapsychic response), and emotion performance (interpersonal behavior)13.

Methods

Participants

Two samples of dance major college students were recruited as the participants to complete the three scales of SC, emotional labor strategies, and flow. These two samples received dance training and prepare for dance shows on a daily basis and can be viewed as identical to each other in terms of the level of dancing skills.

Sample 1 (N = 184, among them 175 females, Mage = 18.636, SDage = 0.913) completed the questionnaire on a break of regular dance training in the physical offline environment. However, Sample 2 (N = 77, among them 74 females, Mage = 19.026, SDage = 1.181) completed the questionnaire during the pandemic of COVID-19, when they were required to continue daily dance training in front of the camera at home and even rehearse remote online performances in such a virtual manner that dancers coordinated with each other through the Zoom meeting. The sizes of the two samples were relatively small but acceptable owning to the rareness of dancer participants.

As these two samples were analyzed separately due to distinct dancing environments and manners.

Measures

Self-consciousness

The Chinese version of Self-Consciousness Scale is a 17-item instrument intended to measure PuSC (7 items, e.g., “I’m always trying to figure myself out”) and PrSC (10 items, e.g., “I’m concerned about the way I present myself”)68. The items were presented on a five-point scale five-point Likert-type scale from 1 (Agree Strongly) to 5 (Disagree Strongly), and each sub-scale was scored by the mean value of corresponding items. The McDonald’s ω values for PuSC and PrSC, respectively, were 0.845 and 0.795 in Sample 1, and were 0.812 and 0.753 in Sample 2, indicating good internal stability of the two sub-scales.

Emotional labor strategies

The scale of emotional labor strategies for dancers was adopted and modified from Yin’s Teacher Emotional Labour Strategy Scale69, which consisting of three dimensions of SA, DA, and ENFE. In the revision, we mainly changed the wording regarding the emotional labor target from students to spectators to make the items suitable for the dance context. For Sample 1, there were five items, four items, and three items for SA, DA, and ENFE, respectively. For Sample 2, not only the questionnaire guideline was set to direct the participants to the online dancing context, but also three items specifically indicating the online dancing context was added to each sub-scale respectively, so there were six items for SA, five for DA, and four for ENFE. The scale is presented in the appendix. All the items were scored on a five-point Likert-style scale from 1 (Agree Strongly) to 5 (Disagree Strongly), and the mean values of corresponding items were calculated for the three sub-scores, respectively. The reliability and the construct validity of the scale are assessed and reported below.

Flow experience

The Chinese version of Flow State Scale consisted of 33 items for nine aspects: balance (four items), merging (three items), goals (three items), feedback (four items), concentration (four items), control (three items), consciousness (four items), time (four items), and autotelic (four items)70. The scale guideline introduced the first sample of dancers to recalling and reporting their feelings engaged in the regular dancing activities, but for the second sample, pointed to the online dancing activities during the pandemic. For each participant, averaging all 33 items, scored on a five-point scale from 1 (Agree Strongly) to 5 (Disagree Strongly), to form a final point indicating their depth of entering the flow state while dancing. The McDonald’s ω was 0.935 in Sample 1 and was 0.909 in Sample 2.

Statistical analyses

The two sample were analyzed separately. For each, factor analyses were conducted first to answer the first research question of the study—whether the three-dimensional structure of emotional labor strategies (ENFE, DA, and SA) was tenable for dancers. Then, as the sample size was relatively small and might be unfitted for structural equation modeling, the improved causal steps approach66 was employed to examine the 11-hypothesis mediation model in Fig. 1 so as to address the second question.

Ethical approval

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee for Non-Clinical Faculties in School of Psychology, South China Normal University (protocol code: SCNU-PSY-2022-180; date of approval: 15 June 2022). Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in the investigation.

Results

Reliability and construct validity assessment for the scale of emotional labor strategies of dancers

In Tables 1 and 2, we present the results of EFA, CFA, and McDonald’s ω calculation for the emotional labor strategy scale in Sample 1 and Sample 2, respectively. It can be seen that for Sample 1, both internal reliability and construct validity reached good conditions. However, for Sample 2, while the results for EFA and McDonald’s ω calculation were satisfactory, the approximate fit indices in CFA were not so desirable perhaps because of the relatively small but passable sample size (KMO = 0.747 < 0.8, df = 105, p < 0.001), but the model could still be considered acceptable as it passed the most direct and important test of model fit (χ2/df < 3, p < 0.001) suggested by Barrett71. These results indicated that the tripartite dimensionality of emotional labor strategies (SA, DA, and ENFE) held up for dancers. Thus, the first research question is addressed.

Table 1.

Reliability and construct validity assessment for the scale of emotional labor strategies of dancers in Sample 1.

Assessment Item Factor loadings Communality
Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3
Exploratory factor analysis SA1 0.785 0.655
SA2 0.735 0.595
SA3 0.739 0.713
SA4 0.787 0.649
SA5 0.724 0.614
DA1 0.846 0.778
DA2 0.858 0.810
DA3 0.753 0.655
DA4 0.834 0.756
ENFE1 0.832 0.711
ENFE2 0.773 0.664
ENFE3 0.723 0.762
Internal consistency McDonald’s ω 0.877 > 0.7 0.809 > 0.7 0.875 > 0.7
Confirmatory factor analysis χ2 76.624
df 51
χ2/df 1.502 < 3
p 0.012 < 0.05
CFI 0.975 > 0.95
TLI 0.967 > 0.95
RMSEA 0.052 < 0.08
SRMR 0.057 < 0.08

Table 2.

Reliability and construct validity assessment for the scale of emotional labor strategies of dancers in Sample 2. *Items were only used in Sample 2.

Assessment Item Factor loadings Communality
Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3
Exploratory factor analysis SA1 0.801 0.662
SA2 0.572 0.420
SA3 0.745 0.585
SA4 0.805 0.681
SA5 0.550 0.497
SA6* 0.806 0.690
DA1 0.816 0.724
DA2 0.877 0.779
DA3 0.849 0.745
DA4 0.819 0.681
DA5* 0.857 0.736
ENFE1 0.827 0.693
ENFE2 0.840 0.723
ENFE3 0.726 0.716
ENFE4* 0.837 0.724
Internal consistency McDonald’s ω 0.872 > 0.7 0.933 > 0.7 0.900 > 0.7
Confirmatory factor analysis χ2 165.942
df 87
χ2/df 1.907 < 3
p 0.000 < 0.001
CFI 0.867 < 0.9
TLI 0.840 < 0.9
RMSEA 0.109 > 0.1
SRMR 0.102 > 0.1

Preliminary analysis

Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlation for the studies variables in Sample 1 and Sample 2 are depicted in Tables 3 and 4, respectively. It showed that dancers in both samples tended to implement DA while dancing.

Table 3.

Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlation for the studied variables in Sample 1.

M SD PuSC PrSC SA DA ENFE Flow
PuSC 3.634 0.538 1
PrSC 3.495 0.424 0.531*** 1
SA 3.364 0.743 0.287*** 0.174** 1
DA 3.878 0.650 0.195** 0.213** 0.359*** 1
ENFE 3.395 0.739 − 0.092 0.113 0.027 0.459*** 1
Flow 3.305 0.442 0.178* 0.439*** 0.187* 0.426*** 0.441*** 1

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Table 4.

Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlation for the studied variables in Sample 2.

M SD PuSC PrSC SA DA ENFE Flow
PuSC 3.758 0.477 1
PrSC 3.635 0.53 0.426*** 1
SA 3.346 0.625 0.055 − 0.096 1
DA 3.932 0.566 0.138 0.448*** 0.045 1
ENFE 3.308 0.691 0.197 0.373*** − 0.262* 0.247* 1
Flow 3.296 0.352 0.084 0.389*** − 0.171 0.480*** 0.56*** 1

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Also, Harman’s one-way test was performed to examine the common method biases in the two samples, respectively. Putting all items into EFA, before rotation, the first factor (the one with the largest eigenvalue) variation was 21.934% for Sample 1 and 22.437% for Sample 2, both less than 40%, indicating the common method bias had no serious effect.

Test of the hypothesized mediation model

The improved causal steps approach developed by Wen and Liu66 was applied to examine the 11-hypothesis mediation model in Fig. 1 so as to answer the second research question of this study. For Sample 1, as is revealed in Table 5 and Fig. 2a, there were one complete mediation path “PuSC → ENFE → Flow” and one partial mediation path “PrSC → ENFE → Flow”, which suggested that in the condition of dancing to spectators, while PuSC of dancers negatively related to their flow experience, PrSC positively related to flow, and that both impacts were mediated by ENFE only. For Sample 2, as is revealed in Table 6 and Fig. 2b, there were two complete mediation paths “PrSC → ENFE → Flow” and “PrSC → DA → Flow”, which indicated that in the condition of dancing without spectators on site, only PrSC related to flow experience in a positive way, and that this impact was mediated by both ENFE and DA. The disfunction of PuSC during online dancing might be accounted for the fact that without facing the spectators, dancers did not need to pay much attention to their outer public images and therefore could focus more on the inner self, which further led to the functional mediating role of DA.

Table 5.

Test of the mediating effects for Sample 1.

Regression 1 Regression 2 Regression 3 Regression 4 Regression 5
Flow ENFE DA SA Flow
Independent variables
 PuSC − 0.064 − 0.291* 0.138 0.375** − 0.043
 PrSC 0.500*** 0.393** 0.233 0.053 0.397***
Mediating variables
 ENFE 0.180***
 DA 0.133**
 SA 0.034
 R2 0.197 0.045 0.055 0.083 0.385
 F 22.206*** 4.253* 5.232** 8.195*** 22.293***

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Verification of the research model in Sample 1 (a) and Sample 2 (b). Non-significant pathways and variables were removed from the model for clarification.

Table 6.

Test of the mediating effects for Sample 2.

Regression 1 Regression 2 Regression 3 Regression 4 Regression 5
Flow ENFE DA SA Flow
Independent variables
 PuSC − 0.074 0.067 − 0.076 0.153 − 0.069
 PrSC 0.287*** 0.460** 0.508*** − 0.172 0.071
Mediating variables
 ENFE 0.227***
 DA 0.209**
 SA − 0.030
 R2 0.160 0.141 0.204 0.020 0.456
 F 7.041** 6.054** 9.488*** 0.773 11.910***

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Discussions

Theoretical contributions

As an exploratory endeavor into the relatively understudied domain of emotional labor among performers, this study provides preliminary insights that could potentially inform and enrich the existing literature. Firstly, it represents one of the initial attempts to empirically examine the emotional labor strategies of dancers, identifying the presence and frequency of SA, DA, and ENFE adoption in the dancing process, with DA being observed as the most prevalent. Since the majority of participants are on the path to becoming professional dancers, this observation indicates the desirability of cultivating genuine inner emotions that align with the expected roles in dance performances, especially from the Stanislavski perspective7. Given these findings are consistent with prior emotional labor literature in other occupations, this study serves as an important, yet previously missing, empirical link bridging the gap between the field of acting studies and vocational research, and underscoring the dramaturgical origins of emotional labor theory1,6.

Secondly, this study also attempts to explore the uncharted role of self-consciousness as a potential precursor to dancers’ use of emotional labor strategies. Self-consciousness, an important but intricate personality trait32, affects how one focuses attention during self-presentation1418. Our research demonstrated that in general, emotional labor strategies, particularly those involving expression of genuine feelings from the heart, appeared to fall short in dancers who were inclined to direct more attention towards their outer image, suggesting that PuSC might be a potential hindrance to the brewing, generation, and modification of inner emotions. On the contrary, people who were accustomed to monitoring and evaluating their internal states (i.e., with higher PrSC) seemed to be better at engendering the expected feelings by will and hence more capable of adopting different emotional labor strategies. These results offer a preliminary understanding of how personality traits influence emotional regulation in the context of performance arts, revealing the complex dynamics of internal emotional regulation among dancers.

Thirdly, our results confirmed several preceding studies in the flow literature. We discerned positive impacts of DA and ENFE on flow experience, while SA had no such effects. This observation is in line with a previous study that directly associated emotional labor strategies with flow59, as well as two other studies that approached this topic indirectly57,58. Additionally, our insights that PrSC fosters flow experience, while PuSC fails to do so, are congruent with earlier empirical outcomes6164. In light of the limited availability of relevant prior research, our study might stand as a substantial contribution to reinforcing these findings.

Lastly, thanks to the unique opportunity brought by online dance classes due to COVID-19, we were allowed to pioneer the examination of dancers' emotional regulation and expression while they danced solo. The differences between this environment and traditional performances on stage under observation could introduce a novel dimension to the dance research literature.

Recognizing the exploratory nature of our study, we advise caution in interpreting and generalizing our findings. The dynamics of emotional labor in the realm of performance arts are intricate, and what our observations provide is merely an preliminary comprehension. Future research, ideally with more varied and representative samples, is crucial to further validate our findings and to arrive at more robust conclusions.

Practical implications

While the findings warrant cautious interpretation, this research still offers tentative practical implications. Firstly, in the realm of dance education, our study underscores the importance of focusing more on the inner self, rather than persistently responding to the evaluations of spectators, as a means to alleviate tension and bolster a sense of control on stage. Moreover, delving into the gestation and creation of authentic emotions through the Stanislavski acting method is highlighted as a more conducive and sustainable approach, especially for the well-being of dancers. Secondly, our study reinforces the scientific basis for viewing dance as a form of emotional regulation training to combat stress78, suggesting that a deeper immersion in the role of the dance performance is likely to yield more favorable mental health outcomes. Lastly, our research points to online dance training as a viable alternative to traditional in-person classes, particularly under the constraints of a pandemic or various other accessibility issues, given the observed similarities in dancers’ emotional regulation across both settings. Nonetheless, as these insights are preliminary and await further validation, practitioners are advised to incorporate them with discretion.

Limitations and future research direction

Nevertheless, our study inevitably had some limitations that should be addressed by future research. The first one related to our inability to recruit more dancers as participants. Though the sample size was passable compared to other dance research47 given the scarcity of such participants, future replication is needed to assess the studied variables and relationships employing a more extensive and diverse sample of dancers. A larger sample would provide more statistical power, possibly testifying some of the hypotheses that we were unable to support in the current study.

The second one related to the cross-sectional nature of the data collected at a single time point. This prevents determining directionality and causal relationships between the variables. Though we hypothesized and tested a model implying certain causal sequences, the true directionality cannot be established without longitudinal or experimental data. For instance, flow experience could also plausibly influence dancers’ emotional labor strategies. Our mediation analyses should therefore be considered exploratory rather than confirmatory. Future research using longitudinal or experimental designs is essential to ascertain the causal mechanisms underlying the associations found.

Third, as we implemented a between-subject design to examine emotional labor strategies with and without being watched by spectators, there might be confounding factors uncontrolled between the two samples affecting the results, even if the dancing skills of the dancers were deemed on the same level by experts. Given that existing studies on online dancing during COVID-19 are really rare79,80, there is still much room for future research to continue to look into emotional labor strategies under such a unique condition of dancing in contrast to that on the stage. Though the closure of schools as a result of the pandemic has ended in China, dance students might practice dancing alone in front of a mirror or camera at ordinary times, which is also an important dance scene worthy of study.

Fourth, as this was the first study on dancers’ emotional labor strategies, merely using quantitative analyses was rough and unable to unveil the full picture, especially in terms of the distinctions between dancers’ use of emotional labor strategies and other professionals’ emotional labor strategy adoption. One important distinction was that normally, employees in service industries are asked to suppress negative emotions and to activate and express positive emotions; however, dancers are more often than not required to experience and deliver negative emotions according to the performance settings. This vast divergence might fundamentally alter the nature of emotional labor strategies and their effects on vocational well-being. Therefore, it is critical for future studies to explore the valence of expected emotions that the dancers are trying to labor. And the qualitative approach can be a more detailed exploratory attempt on this issue.

Finally, using flow alone as the outcome variable could not represent the overall performance of dancers. For one, as flow is only a subjective experience inside, dancers high in the flow state does not necessarily dance better as evaluated by the spectators or dance experts. This might account for the inconsistency between the present study and previous ones which found that PuSC was more beneficial for skillful performance under public pressure28,29. Thus, future studies adopting objective indicators of dancers’ performance are needed. For another, common negative indicators of mental health such as burnout, anxiety, and depression will also be needed to form and present a more complete picture of dancers’ well-being under emotional labor81.

Conclusions

In this exploratory study, we ventured into the relatively unexplored domain of dancers’ emotional labor strategies as well as their antecedents in terms of self-consciousness and their outcomes in the form of flow experience. The preliminary findings primarily derived from a theoretical approach were summed as follows.

  1. The dancers in our study were found to adopt a comprehensive use of the three strategies: surface acting, deep acting, and expression of naturally felt emotions, among which, deep acting was the most frequent.

  2. In the condition of dancing on the stage under observation, our data suggested that expression of naturally felt emotions mediated the positive effect of private self-consciousness and the negative effect of public self-consciousness on flow experience, while surface and deep acting did not appear to function as a mediator in this context.

  3. In scenarios where dancers performed without an audience, our data revealed that both deep acting and expression of naturally felt emotions mediated the positive impact of private self-consciousness on flow state, whereas public self-consciousness seemed disfunctional.

Further research, preferably with larger samples and more diverse contexts, is essential to validate and extend these initial observations.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, X.L. and R.G.; methodology, Y.L. and R.G.; software, Y.L.; validation, Y.L., J.D. and R.G.; formal analysis, Y.L.; investigation, X.L. and J.T.; resources, X.L. and J.T.; data curation, R.G.; writing—original draft preparation, X.L. and J.T.; writing—review and editing, R.G.; visualization, Y.L.; supervision, J.D.; project administration, R.G.; funding acquisition, X.L. and J.D. All authors have read and agreed to the current version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by grants from the Disciplinary Co-construction Project of Guangdong Philosophy and Social Sciences in 2018 during the 13th Five-Year Plan (GD18XYS30) and the Research Project of South China Normal University (ZDPY1401).

Data availability

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author because of privacy and ethical restrictions.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Jifang Duan, Email: duanjifang@163.com.

Ruixiang Gao, Email: ruixianggao@m.scnu.edu.cn.

References

  • 1.Hochschild AR. The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling. University of California Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Morris JA, Feldman DC. The dimensions, antecedents, and consequences of emotional labor. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1996;21(4):986–1010. doi: 10.2307/259161. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barsade SG, Brief AP, Spataro SE. The affective revolution in organizational behavior: the emergence of a paradigm. In: Greenberg J, editor. Organizational Behavior: The State of the Science. 2. Erlbaum; 2003. pp. 3–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hülsheger UR, Schewe AF. On the costs and benefits of emotional labor: A meta-analysis of three decades of research. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2011;16(3):361–389. doi: 10.1037/a0022876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Yang C, Chen A. Emotional labor: A comprehensive literature review. Hum. Syst. Manag. 2021;40:479–501. doi: 10.3233/HSM-200937. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Grove SJ, Fisk RP. Impression management in services marketing: A dramaturgical perspective. In: Giacalone RA, Rosenfeld P, editors. Impression Management in the Organization. Erlbaum; 1989. pp. 427–438. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Konstantin Stanislavsky. Encyclopedia Britannica. https://www.britannica.com/biography/Konstantin-Stanislavsky. Accessed 15 March 2023.
  • 8.Konijn E. Actors and emotions: A psychological perspective. Theatre Res Int. 1995;20(2):132–140. doi: 10.1017/S0307883300008373. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Tian M. Mei Lanfang and Stanislavsky: The (de)construction of an intercultural myth on the international stage. Theatre Res Int. 2020;45(3):264–280. doi: 10.1017/S0307883320000267. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Grandey AA. Emotional regulation in the workplace: A new way to conceptualize emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2000;5:95–110. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ashforth BE, Humphrey RH. Emotional labor in service roles: The influence of identity. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1993;18:88–115. doi: 10.2307/258824. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Diefendorff JM, Croyle MH, Gosserand RH. The dimensionality and antecedents of emotional labor strategies. J. Vocat. Behav. 2005;66(2):339–359. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2004.02.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Grandey AA, Gabriel AS. Emotional labor at a crossroads: Where do we go from here? Ann. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 2015;2:323–349. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032414-111400. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ding Z, Liu Q. Online social behavior and online altruistic behavior: The role of self-esteem and public self-consciousness. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 2020;36(02):175–183. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2020.02.06. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lewis BP, Linder DE. Thinking about choking? Attentional processes and paradoxical performance. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1997;23:937e944. doi: 10.1177/0146167297239003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mesagno C, Harvey JT, Janelle CM. Self-presentation origins of choking: Evidence from separate pressure manipulations. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2011;33:441459. doi: 10.1123/jsep.33.3.441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Meng, L. (2015). Attentional Bias towards Emotional Information in Individuals with Different Levels of Public/Private Self-Consciousness. Master of Thesis, Xinan University, Chongqing, China (in Chinese).
  • 18.Fenigstein A, Scheier MF, Buss AH. Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1975;43(4):522–527. doi: 10.1037/h0076760. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Fenigstein A. Self-consciousness, self-attention, and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1979;37:75–86. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.37.1.75. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Schlenker BR, Weigold MF. Self-consciousness and self-presentation: Being autonomous versus appearing autonomous. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1990;59(4):820–828. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.4.820. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Fenigstein A. Private and public self-consciousness. In: Leary MR, Hoyle RH, editors. Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. The Guilford Press; 2009. pp. 495–511. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Morita T, Asada M, Naito E. Gray-matter expansion of social brain networks in individuals high in public self-consciousness. Brain Sci. 2021;11:374. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11030374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.White K, Peloza J. Self-benefit versus other-benefit marketing appeals: Their effectiveness in generating charitable support. J. Mark. 2009;73(4):109–124. doi: 10.1509/jmkg.73.4.109. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Chen, H. (2016). The Effect of Public Self-awareness and Past Temporal Distance on Moral Psychological License. Master of Thesis, Hunan Normal University, Changsha, China (in Chinese).
  • 25.Gangestaad SW, Snyder M. Self-monitoring: Appraisal and reappraisal. Psychol. Bull. 2000;126:530–555. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.4.530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bono JE, Vey MA. Personality and emotional performance: Extraversion, neuroticism, and self-monitoring. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2007;12(2):177–192. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.12.2.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mesmer-Magnus JR, DeChurch LA, Wax A. Moving emotional labor beyond surface and deep acting: A discordance-congruence perspective. Organ. Psychol. Rev. 2012;2(1):6–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Baumeister RF. Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1984;46:610620. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Geukes K, Mesagno C, Hanrahan SJ, Kellmann M. Testing an interactionist perspective on the relationship between personality traits and performance under public pressure. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2012;13:243–250. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.12.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wicklund RA. The influence of self-awareness on human behavior: The person who becomes self-aware is more likely to act consistently, be faithful to societal norms, and give accurate reports about himself. Am. Sci. 1979;67(2):187–193. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ahadzadeh AS, Ong FS, Wu SL, Deng R. Private self-consciousness and self-monitoring on Instagram: The mediating effect of internal locus of control and self-concept. J. Psychol. 2021;155(3):334–355. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2021.1884035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hart W, Tortoriello GK, Richardson K. Profiling public and private self-consciousness on self-presentation tactic use. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2019;147:53–57. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kammeyer-Mueller JD, Rubenstein AL, Long DM, Odio MA, Buckman BR, et al. A meta-analytic structural model of dispositional affectivity and emotional labor. Pers. Psychol. 2013;66:47–90. doi: 10.1111/peps.12009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Yin H, Huang S, Chen G. The relationships between teachers’ emotional labor and their burnout and satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. Educ. Res. Rev. 2019;28:100283. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100283. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Judge TA, Woolf EF, Hurst C. Is emotional labor more difficult for some than for others? A multi-level, experience-sampling study. Pers. Psychol. 2009;62:57–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2008.01129.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gabriel AS, Diefendorff JM. Emotional labor dynamics: A momentary approach. Acad. Manag. J. 2015;58:1804–1825. doi: 10.5465/amj.2013.1135. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kong H, Jeon J-E. Daily emotional labor, negative affect state, and emotional exhaustion: Cross-level moderators of affective commitment. Sustainability. 2018;10:1967. doi: 10.3390/su10061967. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Côté S, Morgan LM. A longitudinal analysis of the association between emotion regulation, job satisfaction, and intentions to quit. J. Organ. Behav. 2002;23(8):947–962. doi: 10.1002/job.174. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hülsheger UR, Lang JWB, Maier GW. Emotional labor, strain, and performance: Testing reciprocal relationships in a longitudinal panel study. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2010;15(4):505–521. doi: 10.1037/a0021003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Xiong W, Huang M, Okumus B, Leung XY, Cai X, Fan F. How emotional labor affect hotel employees’ mental health: A longitudinal study. Tour. Manag. 2022;94:104631. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2022.104631. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Harmat L, Anderssen FØ, Ullén F, Wright J, Sadlo G. Flow Experience: Empirical Research and Applications. Springer; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Csikszentmihalyi M. Beyond Boredom and Anxiety. Jossey-Bass Publishers; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Abuhamdeh S. Investigating the “flow” experience: Key conceptual and operational issues. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Tse DCK, Nakamura J, Csikszentmihalyi M. Living well by “flowing” well: The indirect effect of autotelic personality on well-being through flow experience. J. Posit. Psychol. 2021;16(3):310–321. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2020.1716055. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Csikszentmihalyi M. Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology. Springer; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Csikszentmihalyi M. Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. J. Leisure Res. 1990;24(1):93–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Arslan İ, Altan-Atalay A. Too perfectionist to flow: The roles of perfectionistic strivings and perfectionistic concerns for flow in dance. Curr. Psychol. 2022 doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03364-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Festinger L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford University Press; 1957. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hobfoll SE. Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am. Psychol. 1989;44:513–524. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Brotheridge CM, Lee RT. Testing a conservation of resources model of the dynamics of emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2002;7(1):57–67. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.7.1.57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Liu Y, Prati LM, Perrew’e PL, Ferris GR. The relationship between emotional resources and emotional labor: An exploratory study. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2008;38(10):2410–2439. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00398.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Ma S, Huang M. Emotional labor: Surface acting and deep acting, which one is better? Acta Psychol. Sin. 2006;38(02):262–270. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Liao H, Yan A. The connotation of emotional labor. Chin. J. Manag. 2015;12(02):306–312. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implication for affect, relationships, and well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003;85(2):348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Côté S. A social interaction model of the effects of emotion regulation on work strain. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2005;30(3):509–530. doi: 10.5465/amr.2005.17293692. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Salanova M, Bakker AB, Llorens SJ. Flow at work: Evidence for an upward spiral of personal and organizational resources. J. Happiness Stud. 2006;7(1):1–22. doi: 10.1007/s10902-005-8854-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Mäkikangas A, Bakker AB, Aunola K, Demerouti E. Job resources and flow at work: Modelling the relationship via latent growth curve and mixture model methodology. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2010;83(3):795–814. doi: 10.1348/096317909X476333. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lilius JM. Recovery at work: Understanding the restorative side of “depleting” client interactions. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2012;37:569–588. doi: 10.5465/amr.2010.0458. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Xanthopoulou D, Bakker AB, Oerlemans WG, Koszucka M. Need for recovery after emotional labor: Differential effects of daily deep and surface acting. J. Organ. Behav. 2018;39:481–494. doi: 10.1002/job.2245. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wang M, Liao H, Zhan Y, Shi J. Daily customer mistreatment and employee sabotage against customers: Examining emotion and resource perspectives. Acad. Manag. J. 2011;54:312–334. doi: 10.5465/amj.2011.60263093. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Buss AH. Self-consciousness and Social Anxiety. W. H. Freeman and Company; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kawaguchi A, Morita T, Okamoto Y, Nakaaki S, Kawaguchi T, et al. Neural basis of self-reflection on self-face image in patients with social anxiety disorder. Anxiety Disord. Res. 2018;10:29–44. doi: 10.14389/jsad.10.1_29. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003;84(4):822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Newman DB, Nezlek JB. Private self-consciousness in daily life: Relationships between rumination and reflection and well-being, and meaning in daily life. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2019;136:184–189. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.039. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric Theory. 3. McGraw Hill; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Wen Z, Liu H. Analyses of Mediating and Moderating Effects: Methods and Applications. Educational Science Publishing House; 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Xu, X. (2021). The Occupation of Art Performers from the Perspective of Positive Psychological Research on Mental Health. Master of Thesis, Harbin Conservatory of Music, Harbin, China (in Chinese).
  • 68.Fenigstein A, Scheier MF, Buss AH. Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1975;43:522–527. doi: 10.1037/h0076760. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Yin H. Adaptation and validation of the teacher emotional labour strategy scale in China. Educ. Psychol. 2012;32:451–465. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2012.674488. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Liu W, Liu X, Ji L, Watson JC, Zhou C, Yao J. Chinese translation of the flow state scale-2 and the dispositional flow scale-2: Examination of factorial validity and reliability. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 2012;43:153–175. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Barrett P. Structural equation modelling: Adjudging model fit. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2007;42:815–824. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Zapf D. Emotion work and psychological well-being: A review of the literature and some conceptual considerations. Hum. Resour. Manag. Rev. 2002;12(2):237–268. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Zammuner VL, Lotto L, Galli C. Regulation of emotions in the helping professions: Nature, antecedents, and consequences. In: Morrow L, Verins I, Willis E, editors. Mental Health and Work: Issues and Perspectives. Auseinet; 2002. pp. 217–231. [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 1998;2(3):271–299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Gross JJ. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1998;74:224–237. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Richards JM, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2000;79(3):410–424. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Grandey AA, Dickter DN, Sin H. The customer is not always right: Customer aggression and emotion regulation of service employees. J. Organ. Behav. 2004;25:397–418. doi: 10.1002/job.252. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Liu D, Sun F, Zhu Y, Jia C, Mao Y, Liu B. Fitness dance counteracts female Ph.D. candidates’ stress by affecting emotion regulation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2022;19:14627. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192214627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Rugh R, Humphries A, Tasnim N, Basso JC. Healing minds, moving bodies: Measuring the mental health effects of online dance during the COVID-19 pandemic. Res. Dance Educ. 2022 doi: 10.1080/14647893.2022.2078297. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Humphries A, Tasnim N, Rugh R, et al. Acutely enhancing affective state and social connection following an online dance intervention during the COVID-19 social isolation crisis. BMC Psychol. 2023;11:13. doi: 10.1186/s40359-022-01034-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Liu X, He T, Yu S, Duan J, Gao R. The effects of emotional labor on work strain and nonwork strain among dancers: A Person-centered approach. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2023;16:3675–3685. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S426659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author because of privacy and ethical restrictions.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES