Abstract
Pain is a complex experience involving sensory, emotional, and cognitive aspects, and multiple networks manage its processing in the brain. Examining how pain transforms into a behavioral response can shed light on the networks’ relationships and facilitate interventions to treat chronic pain. However, studies using high spatial and temporal resolution methods to investigate the neural encoding of pain and its psychophysical correlates have been limited. We recorded from intracranial stereo-EEG (sEEG) electrodes implanted in sixteen different brain regions of twenty patients who underwent psychophysical pain testing consisting of a tonic thermal stimulus to the hand. Broadband high-frequency local field potential amplitude (HFA; 70–150 Hz) was isolated to investigate the relationship between the ongoing neural activity and the resulting psychophysical pain evaluations. Two different generalized linear mixed-effects models (GLME) were employed to assess the neural representations underlying binary and graded pain psychophysics. The first model examined the relationship between HFA and whether the patient responded "yes" or "no" to whether the trial was painful. The second model investigated the relationship between HFA and how painful the stimulus was rated on a visual analog scale. GLMEs revealed that HFA in the inferior temporal gyrus (ITG), superior frontal gyrus (SFG), and superior temporal gyrus (STG) predicted painful responses at stimulus onset. An increase in HFA in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), SFG, and striatum predicted pain responses at stimulus offset. Numerous regions, including the anterior cingulate cortex, hippocampus, IFG, MTG, OFC, and striatum, predicted the pain rating at stimulus onset. However, only the amygdala and fusiform gyrus predicted increased pain ratings at stimulus offset. We characterized the spatiotemporal representations of binary and graded painful responses during tonic pain stimuli. Our study provides evidence from intracranial recordings that the neural encoding of psychophysical pain changes over time during a tonic thermal stimulus, with different brain regions being predictive of pain at the beginning and end of the stimulus.
Introduction
Pain is a complex and multi-faceted experience encompassing sensory, emotional, and cognitive aspects [1]. The ability to feel pain is a crucial defense mechanism managed by several processing networks. Unlike other sensory modalities, pain does not appear to have a single primary brain region responsible for initial processing, as shown by multiple studies [2–5]. When pain persists without an apparent cause, it becomes a chronic condition that can devastate one’s quality of life. Pain arising without a noxious stimulus suggests a disruption in the connection between the stimulus and psychophysical correlates. Understanding this interaction could offer valuable information for managing chronic pain.
Much of what is known about pain processing has resulted from studies that induce pain in healthy volunteers using brief, millisecond-long stimuli with infrared lasers [6–8]. Such work has revealed that pain networks comprise regions such as the somatosensory, insular, and prefrontal cortices [9–13]. Noninvasive imaging studies inducing tonic pain with thermodes suggest that ongoing pain activates similar brain regions as brief experimental stimuli [14–16]. However, it’s thought that experimental tonic pain also involves the medial prefrontal cortex and related projections, such as the anterior cingulate cortex [17], nucleus accumbens [18], hippocampus, periaqueductal gray, globus pallidus, and subthalamic nucleus [19–21]. The involvement of these regions during experimental tonic pain may indicate a shift from sensory to emotional and cognitive encoding-based processes [1]. The spatiotemporal dynamics of these emotional and cognitive encoding-based processes are crucial in understanding how pain is shaped.
We sought to understand the psychological aspects of painful experiences with high spatiotemporal resolution recordings of neuronal population activity in human participants and statistical modeling of two types of psychophysical ratings. We hypothesized that regions involved in somatosensation and perception would have increased gamma activity if the subject later evaluated that stimulus as painful. At the end of the stimulus, we hypothesized that high gamma activity would dissipate from regions involved at the onset of the trial and instead would develop in regions involved in cognitive-emotional processing. Our results revealed that the inferior, middle, and superior temporal gyrus (ITG, MTG, STG) predicted binary and numerical psychophysical correlates. Other regions, such as the cingulate cortex, orbitofrontal, hippocampus, and amygdala, had HFA amplitudes associated with higher numerical ratings of pain. We describe the utility of understanding how these regions interact and propose future studies of interest.
Methods and materials
Patient selection
Patients undergoing routine intracranial monitoring with sEEG for localization of epileptic foci were screened for inclusion in the study between 2020 and 2022. A total of 20 patients (12 female, 8 male) met the inclusion criteria and were included in the final data analysis. The sample size was determined from an a priori power analysis with an assumed effect size (f = 0.625), type I error probability (α = 0.05), and power threshold (0.80). The age range of the patient population was 21–66 years (mean 38 ± 10 years), and all patients had a diagnosis of drug-resistant epilepsy as determined by a multidisciplinary conference at our institution. All patients underwent sEEG implantation for clinical purposes. Clinical data was used to determine likely seizure onset zones to help guide clinical treatment. Seven patients included in the study had onset zones within the left hemisphere. Four were within the left hippocampal region, one within the temporal pole, one in the orbitofrontal region, and one broadly in the mesial temporal structures. Four patients had onset zones within the right hemisphere, including two patients with a mesial temporal onset, one within the central median nucleus of the thalamus/pars opercularis area, and one within the right insula. Six patients had either broad ictal onset, multifocal epileptic zones, or bitemporal lobe epileptogenicity. The onset zone was inconclusive for three patients. Several steps were taken to avoid gathering data with epileptiform activity. Patients were not asked to participate if they had a clinical seizure or seizure-rescue medication, such as a benzodiazepine, within several hours. Additional signal processing steps were taken to avoid including data with potential seizure-like activity and are outlined in the preprocessing section.
Participation in the research study described was obtained through informed written consent under a protocol approved (IRB_00122256) by the University of Utah Institutional Review Board. To be eligible for the study, participants must have been 18 years of age or older, able to give informed consent, without any nerve damage in their arms or hands, able to communicate during the task, and not have any serious medical conditions such as bleeding disorders or cancer. Patients were excluded if their seizures interfered with data collection.
Psychophysical pain task
Our research group recently developed and fabricated a thermoelectric device compatible with intracranial electrodes. The device and integrated software have been validated as a psychophysical pain task in healthy human subjects [22]. The task consists of four main events (Fig 1A), which occur in a single trial: 1) The participant places their hand on the device for 10 seconds, 2) the participant removes their hand from the device, 3) the participant responds to whether the trial was perceived as painful, and 4) the participant rates the perceived pain and heat intensity on a scale of 0–10. Each of these events is time-locked to the ongoing intracranial recording. The start and end of each trial are time-locked using a capacitive touch sensor under the device’s surface. The patient used their dominant hand unless an intravenous access site was on that hand.
The thermoelectric device also includes built-in software that implements a psychophysical algorithm to estimate pain thresholds (QUEST Psychtoolbox, [23]). The algorithm incorporates the participant’s responses after each trial. This enables adaptation to the most likely pain threshold temperature. Participants completed at least 20 trials to allow adequate sampling for signal processing. The full description of the psychophysical pain task and its capability is reported elsewhere [22].
Electrode localization
The study utilized electrodes designed for sEEG. The locations of the electrodes were identified using each patient’s structural magnetic resonance imaging coregistered with postoperative high-resolution computed tomography using the LeGUI software package (https://github.com/Rolston-Lab/LeGUI) [24]. The Brainnetome atlas was used within LeGUI to label the anatomical region based on the electrode locations (http://atlas.brainnetome.org) [25]. The electrode labels were then converted to regional gyri by only keeping the first part of the alphanumerical atlas label. The distinction between the right and left hemispheres was also removed. For example, the Brainnetome label "Amyg_L_2_1" was collapsed to "Amyg." Electrode locations were not included if they were within white matter or if at least three unique patients did not have electrodes in the same region.
Sixteen total areas were represented by 3 to 20 unique patients (Fig 1B). The number of patients and the average number of electrodes representing each brain area is shown in Table 1. The electrodes sampling the 16 brain regions were visualized in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using NeuroMArVL (https://github.com/mangstad/neuromarvl, where cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation of the MNI space were performed with the FreeSurfer image analysis suite (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) [26].
Table 1. Brain regions sampled across twenty patients.
region of interest | unique patients | unique electrode contacts | mean electrodes per patient ± standard deviation |
---|---|---|---|
Amy, amygdala | 11 | 44 | 4.0 ± 2.3 |
CG, cingulate cortex | 20 | 51 | 2.6 ± 1.7 |
FuG, fusiform gyrus | 9 | 16 | 1.8 ± 0.8 |
Hipp, hippocampus | 18 | 115 | 6.4 ± 4.3 |
IFG, inferior frontal gyrus (ventrolateral prefrontal cortext) | 17 | 94 | 5.5 ± 3.5 |
INS, insular cortex | 12 | 30 | 2.5 ± 2.1 |
ITG, inferior temporal gyrus | 6 | 19 | 3.2 ± 1.6 |
MFG, middle frontal gyrus (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex) | 17 | 116 | 6.8 ± 3.8 |
MTG, middle temporal gyrus | 19 | 140 | 7.4 ± 3.7 |
OFC, orbitofrontal cortex | 18 | 190 | 10.6 ± 8.1 |
PrG, precentral gyrus | 3 | 15 | 5.0 ± 1.0 |
SFG, superior frontal gyrus | 13 | 32 | 2.5 ± 1.6 |
STG, superior temporal gyrus | 11 | 36 | 3.3 ± 3.8 |
Str, striatum | 10 | 41 | 4.1 ± 2.4 |
Tha, thalamus | 5 | 19 | 3.8 ± 1.1 |
pSTS, posterior superior temporal sulcus | 3 | 9 | 3.0 ± 2.6 |
The unique Brainnetome regions that were sampled, along with the number of unique patients that had electrodes in these regions is shown. The total number of unique contacts in these regions is also described.
Data collection and preprocessing
Neurophysiological data were recorded using a 128-channel data acquisition system (NeuroPort, Blackrock Microsystems, Salt Lake City, UT). Recorded data were bandpass filtered online at 0.3–250 Hz and sampled at 1 kHz. Reference channels were chosen for each patient consisting of a single intracranial depth electrode contact located in the white matter without artifact or epileptiform activity. The reference electrode was selected after electrode location labeling. An intracranial reference in white matter was chosen based on the improved signal quality over a scalp or skin reference.
Intracranial recordings were referenced using the common median, which is more robust to large amplitude transient events than the common average reference [27]. Data were segmented into epochs of -1000 to 1000 ms to facilitate analysis relative to specific trial events. These events included: 1) initiation of stimulus delivery upon participant’s hand placement on the device and 2) removal of the participant’s hand from the device. After removing their hand from the device, participants responded via a desktop touchscreen to whether the stimulus was painful. Participants subsequently provided a numerical rating of perceived pain intensity on a scale of 0–10 (visual analog scale, VAS), which was not time-locked to the intracranial recordings. Both pain evaluation metrics were utilized as response variables in mixed-effects modeling.
Epochs with 60-Hz noise >100 μV2/Hz, transients >300 μV/ms, variance <1 μV2 and voltages equal to the amplifier saturation voltage (±6553 μV) were identified as artifacts and removed. Epileptiform discharges/noise spikes were identified as outliers if the epoch contained a value that was more than three median absolute deviations from the median. Channels were also visually inspected for artifacts after this automated analysis. While it is possible that data from a seizure onset zone or brain region with epileptiform activity was included, it is very unlikely as the electrode contact recording this activity would have likely been excluded included in our analysis. Trials containing such activity were removed per the method described and if there were not enough trials with non-epileptiform activity for a representative electrode, then it was removed from the analysis. Signals were downsampled to 500 Hz for subsequent analyses.
sEEG recordings allow evaluation of local neural activation through changes in amplitude within the broadband high-gamma frequency range (HFA; 70–150 Hz) in the recorded local field potential (LFP) [28–30]. HFA is highly correlated with both blood-oxygen-level-dependent fMRI and population firing rates. It is useful for bridging the gap between human neuroimaging results and findings from nonhuman primate electrophysiology studies [31]. HFA was isolated through bandpass filtering the LFP between 70–150 Hz using a 4th-order zero-phase Butterworth filter. The absolute value of the Hilbert transform was applied to isolate the amplitude of the HFA component. The signal was smoothed using a moving average filter with a 300 ms fixed window. The epochs for each channel were normalized against baseline activity, using the mean and standard deviation from -3 to -2 s relative to the trial event onset. The time-locked epochs were z-scored using the mean and standard deviation to represent the relevant event being modeled.
Experimental design and statistical analyses
To investigate the relationship between HFA and painful experience, we used sliding-window generalized linear mixed-effects (GLME) models. Trials from all patients were separated by brain region (see Electrodes for brain region labeling). We focused our analyses on the first and last seconds when the hand was exposed to the stimulus. We also evaluated the second after the hand was removed, right before they registered their binary choice. We chose a time window of 500 ms with a 250 ms overlapping sliding window to capture the likely physiological HFA of painful stimuli [4].
Two different types of GLME models were employed. The first model examined the relationship between HFA and whether the patient responded "yes" or "no" to whether the trial was painful (binary response). The fixed effect in this model was the mean HFA amplitude in one of the 16 brain regions during the 500 ms window, while the random effects were trial temperature and patient identity. A binomial distribution was used because the response variable was binary. The Wilkinson notation for this model was:
(1) |
In Eq (1), A represents the HFA amplitude for the particular brain region, B was the trial temperature on the thermoelectric device, and C was the patient identity. A random intercept and slope model was specified to account for variation in the fixed effect across subjects and temperature.
The second model evaluated the relationship between HFA and the numerical pain rating on a scale from 0 to 10. As in the first model, the fixed effect was the mean HFA amplitude in one of the 16 brain regions during a 500 ms window. As previously mentioned, the random effects were trial temperature and patient identity. This response variable was a numerical pain value between 0 and 10.
Statistical significance for each model was determined by a permutation test, where the model was re-evaluated 1,000 times with patients’ responses randomly permuted. The 95% range of the pseudo-t-statistics was used to determine the bounds of t-statistics associated with randomness. The true model was considered significant if the real t-statistic did not fall within these bounds. For the significantly predictive windows, an observed probability, P, was determined by adding the number of pseudo-t-statistics greater than the real t-statistic, multiplying by two to account for the two-sided distribution, and then dividing by the 1000 total observations. The smallest possible P-value was ≤0.002. Heatmaps of the real t-statistics were generated for each brain region and time window to visualize the models that significantly predicted the response variable.
Evaluating model accuracy and predictions
We evaluated the accuracy of statistical models to predict the relationship between high-frequency activity (HFA) and pain response. The accuracy of the models was evaluated by comparing the real and predicted (fitted conditional) responses for each GLME. The error percentage was calculated as the absolute difference between the real and predicted responses that were most likely not painful, divided by the total number of trials and multiplied by 100. A lower error percentage indicates a more accurate model. We made scatterplots of the fitted conditional VAS responses versus the real VAS responses for the brain regions to visualize the model’s accuracy.
Lastly, we evaluated the fixed-effects coefficients for all GLME models that predicted either the binary or VAS pain response for at least a one-time window. The coefficients were evaluated to understand how the response variable would change given a change in the mean z-scored HFA by one unit.
Code accessibility
Code used for preprocessing and analyses is available upon request.
Results
We utilized intracranial recordings to analyze changes in HFA associated with thermal pain perception across a distributed brain network. GLME models examined the relationship between HFA changes and self-reported pain. Results showed significant changes in HFA amplitude in multiple brain regions, including the prefrontal cortex, lateral temporal cortex, and others, during the time windows surrounding the four trial events of the task.
Relationship between broadband gamma and the binary pain response
We first evaluated the mean HFA amplitude during the first second the subjects’ hand was on the device (Fig 2C, "first second"). We found that the mean HFA amplitude in the ITG, superior frontal gyrus (SFG), and STG was significantly higher when subjects responded that the stimulus was painful (response = "yes"). An example of how the z-scored mean HFA for the "yes" responses was different from the "no" responses is shown in Fig 2A. In this instance, the mean HFA amplitude in the STG for the "yes" responses had a similar shape to the "no" responses. However, the "yes" maximum amplitude was greater than the "no" responses at the onset of the stimulus.
Subjects held their hand in place on the surface of the thermoelectric device for 10 seconds before removing it. We assessed which brain regions predicted the binary response at the end of the stimulus compared to the start by looking at the average HFA amplitude when subjects took their hand off the device (Fig 2C, "last second"). The results showed that increased HFA in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) predicted "yes" responses after hand removal from the device. The SFG and the striatum also predicted this response at different time windows. An increase in HFA in the STG predicted "no" responses. While the HFA associated with "yes" responses was mostly higher in amplitude than "no" responses, it should be noted that visualizing the z-scored mean HFA across time, as in Fig 2A and 2B, does not account for interaction variables, such as temperature and patient identification.
We calculated an error metric for each brain region to evaluate the accuracy of each model’s prediction. The metric was the difference between the actual response and the response predicted by the model, with a lower error indicating a more accurate model. We only reported the accuracy for the predictive time windows from the analysis in Fig 2C. The model of the SFG was the most accurate at the start of the stimulus (Fig 3A). The STG had a slightly higher error. The STG was less accurate during the first 0 to 500 ms than the ITG. The STG was the least accurate of all models during the first second of the stimulus. The striatum was the least accurate when subjects removed their hand from the device but slightly less accurate than the STG. Over time, the accuracy of the STG improved, but the error of the striatum increased. The lowest error was found in the SFG, while the highest was in the striatum.
Lastly, we evaluated the relationship between the HFA and the binary response variables by looking at the estimated coefficient of the HFA models. This coefficient represented how much the response ("yes" or "no") variable changed when the HFA changed by one unit (Fig 4A). The ITG had the largest coefficient in the first 500 ms of the stimulus, with a value of 0.12, which means that one unit increase in the HFA would result in a 12% increase in the proportion of "yes" responses. The SFG had the next biggest coefficient, and the superior temporal gyrus had the smallest coefficient.
In the last second of the stimulus, the model coefficient for the SFG remained stable. However, the coefficient peaked right when the hand was taken off the device. The STG coefficient was negative throughout the trial. In contrast, it was slightly positive during the first second of the stimulus. The coefficient for the striatum was highest at the end of the stimulus and remained slightly higher than the SFG coefficient. During the final time window, the OFC had the largest model coefficient. At the trial onset, the highest overall coefficient for the mixed-effect model with the binary response variable was from the striatum. The second highest was from the SFG from 0:500 ms relative to the end of the stimulus.
Relationship between broadband gamma and the numerical rating of pain
We used mixed-effects models to study the relationship between the z-scored mean high-frequency activity (HFA) amplitude and the numerical pain ratings the subjects gave in response to the stimulus. The models were similar to the one used to evaluate the relationship between HFA and the binary pain response. However, in this case, the response variable was the numerical pain score participants assigned to each trial. During the first second of the stimulus, when each participant’s hand was on the device, we found that several brain areas had a significant relationship between HFA amplitude and the subjects’ pain rating (Fig 2F). The t-statistic was positive for all the predictive regions, meaning that the z-scored mean HFA amplitudes were associated with increased pain ratings on the VAS.
Several regions uniquely predicted VAS response in the first second of the stimulus, including the cingulate gyrus, hippocampus, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), MTG (Fig 2D), OFC, and striatum. The SFG and the STG were also predictive in the first second of the stimulus. Two cortical areas demonstrated a relationship between the mean HFA amplitude and pain ratings at the beginning and right after the stimulus—the IFG (Fig 2E) and the STG. However, many of the same brain regions were not involved when the stimulus ended. The amygdala was uniquely involved in the relationship between HFA and pain rating. Similarly, the fusiform gyrus was involved in three-time windows.
We tested the accuracy of our model by looking at the predicted and actual pain ratings on a scatter plot. As the pain stimulus was mild, most pain ratings were between 0 and 4, as shown in Fig 3B and 3C. When the real pain rating was around one, the predicted response was around one. However, when the actual pain rating was two, the predicted response seemed lower than two. This relationship suggests that the model may be under-predicting the pain score, but it could also be due to the limited number of higher real pain ratings.
To see how the HFA amplitude affects future predictions of pain, we looked at the model coefficients for each predictive region (Fig 4B). During the first second of the stimulus, the biggest to smallest model coefficients were the striatum, SFG, hippocampus, MTG, OFC, STG, IFG, and the cingulate gyrus. The hippocampus, STG, MTG, and cingulate gyrus had increased coefficients in the first second. The coefficient for the SFG decreased over time. Although many regions had low coefficients, none were negative, which means increased HFA was always linked to increased VAS ratings.
We compared the models for the different brain regions for the period right after the hand was removed from the device. The fusiform gyrus had the largest coefficient, with the peak occurring at the end of the stimulus. The IFG was the next largest, followed by the amygdala and the STG. The peak model coefficient for the IFG was observed at 250 to 750 ms after the end of the stimulus.
Discussion
Mapping neural activity associated with ongoing pain with high spatial and temporal resolution is especially valuable and currently limited. Prior studies have used laser-based or thermode stimuli paired with noninvasive imaging or electroencephalography (EEG). Noninvasive imaging has provided valuable network-level information on whole-brain activity. However, it is limited in its temporal resolution. EEG can measure brain activity with a better temporal resolution but has limited spatial resolution of deep cerebral structures. Our approach involved acquiring recordings using intracranial stereo-EEG (sEEG) during a psychophysical pain testing paradigm to evaluate temporal changes in numerous regions involved in detecting, processing, and responding to painful stimuli [22]. We used advanced statistical modeling to understand what regions underwent neuronal activations at the beginning of the stimulus, how those regions changed at the end, and how these neuronal activations were related to how the pain was reported.
Traditional models propose that a stimulus produces a percept and triggers a behavioral response [32]. However, recent research indicates that this linear sequence may not apply to pain perception. May et al. argue that behavioral responses influence the perception of painful stimuli [33]. This suggests that the cognitive and emotional aspects of pain processing are not separate from sensory perception but are integrated to create the subjective experience of pain. Schulz et al. have demonstrated that the subjective intensity of pain differs significantly from objective stimulus intensity and brief pain stimuli [21]. Chronic pain is a subjective experience that persists for extended periods. Therefore, in this study, we focused on the neural activity associated with the subjective experience of persistent pain.
Although there is limited research on temporal lobe abnormalities in chronic pain, it is believed that these areas are responsible for assigning emotional value to short-term memories associated with painful experiences [34]. Specifically, the STG plays a role in pain processing by monitoring the discrepancy between pain expectation and perception, anticipation, and expression [35–37]. In an fMRI study, Schwedt et al. [38] demonstrated that migraine patients may exhibit atypical connectivity between the MTG and a range of subcortical and cortical areas. Patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain disorders are similarly affected. For example, pain duration and intensity in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain disorders correlate with decreased gray matter volume in the STG and MTG [39, 40]. Interestingly, recent evidence has shown that the STG plays a causal role in forming biased pain memories, as virtual lesions using transcranial magnetic stimulation reduced biased pain unpleasantness [41]. The MTG and STG regions might contribute to the emotional response of a painful stimulus, leading to dysfunction in patients with chronic pain over time. To better understand where the pain changes are occurring within the MTG and STG, future analyses should use high resolution methods like sEEG. This is especially important since the STG is typically associated with speech and hearing, so a more precise approach can provide greater spatial specificity.
In our study, HFA at the onset of the stimulus in the ITG, SFG, and STG were significantly higher when participants responded that the stimulus was painful (“yes”). All three GLME models for these regions had a relatively low error. Since temporal gyrus components were predictive in both response types, we hypothesize that the temporal gyrus may represent more experiential aspects of pain.
At the end of the trial, increased STG HFA was associated with an increase in “no” responses, opposite to the trial start. We suggest that this change may reflect behavioral learning of the tonic stimulus or indicate a decrease in the neural processing demand, completion of the task, or the disengagement of attention from the task [42, 43]. Alternatively, it could also indicate inhibition or suppression of the brain region’s activity by other regions in the brain. All the coefficients for the predictive VAS models were positive, meaning that increased HFA was always linked to increased VAS ratings. An increase in HFA in more than one brain region near the same time suggests synchronized neural activity between these regions. The synchronized activity may indicate that these regions are involved in the same cognitive task or communicating. It would be valuable to study the connectivity of ongoing pain to understand the relationship between these neural features.
Other prefrontal cortical regions predicted both responses at the beginning and end of the stimuli. Prior groups have established the involvement of prefrontal cortices (e.g., IFG, MFG, SFG) in pain’s affective and cognitive dimensions [4, 5, 19, 44]. Recent studies also suggest that the medial prefrontal cortex encodes the subjective perception of ongoing pain and that gamma oscillations in this region encode subjection pain perception [21, 45]. Time-frequency analyses in patients with chronic back pain revealed that ongoing pain intensity is reflected in prefrontal gamma oscillations [46]. A recent study using EEG and a tonic pain stimulus in healthy patients observed a widespread increase of gamma power correlated with subjective pain intensity [47]. Another study supported this finding, which found that gamma oscillations selectively encoded the subjective perception of tonic pain in the medial prefrontal cortex [21]. It is currently unclear whether the narrowband gamma activity occurs in the same regions or if the gamma responses are a true reflection of the neuronal populations in the medial PFC.
We show that the SFG and IFG encoded subjective perception at the beginning and end of the tonic stimulus. The prefrontal cortex receives ascending, nociceptive input and controls top-down pain [20, 48, 49]. Based on these regions exhibiting HFA at the start and end of the stimuli associated with the psychophysical correlates, we may have also seen this phenomenon. However, causal evidence of this mechanism in humans experiencing tonic pain is still needed.
More brain regions exhibited HFA activity parametrically related to the VAS response. At the beginning of the trial, HFA in the cingulate gyrus, hippocampus, orbitofrontal cortex, and striatum showed significant relationships with increased VAS ratings. By the end of the trial, HFA in the fusiform gyrus and amygdala were also associated with increased VAS ratings. While all these regions have previously shown involvement in pain processing, no one has shown a parametric relationship between behavioral responses and HFA during ongoing pain in these brain regions using intracranial electrodes. These findings suggest that the perception and psychophysical pain correlates depend on how patients are asked to evaluate it. The different brain regions involved in the VAS predictions highlight the importance of recognizing that brain dynamics can shift by changing just one aspect of the stimulus-perception-behavior relationship.
In this study, we utilized sEEG to study the spatiotemporal dynamics of tonic pain processing. While sEEG has great spatial and temporal benefits, several limitations exist. Electrode placement was determined based on the clinical hypothesis about the seizure onset zone, and there are innate differences in the sampled locations between patients. We combined electrode laterality and broadened specific electrode labels to obtain adequate statistical power for group-level analyses. Future analyses may use our findings to study the described regions with higher resolution. We also did our best to limit experimentation with patients receiving pain medication within 4 hours, despite pain medication being commonly indicated for patients with sEEG. Additionally, pain sensitivity may differ from one patient to another (at baseline and after surgical electrode placement), and sEEG is associated with lower pain medication use than other intracranial recording techniques [50]. To account for this, we used a maximum-likelihood adaptive procedure within the psychophysical testing paradigm to optimize the stimulus intensity for each patient. We also accounted for potential differences in stimulus intensity perception in our GLME models by including temperature as a random effect related to the HFA slope.
Our study sheds light on the spatiotemporal dynamics of neural activity associated with psychophysical pain evaluation. Our findings indicate that depending on the psychometric variable in question, different brain regions may be involved in processing pain. These regions may exhibit distinct patterns of activity based on the reference frame (beginning or end of the stimulus). Additional research examining the temporal dynamics during the evolution of pain will enhance our comprehension of how the relationship between these regions changes as pain becomes a conscious perception.
Data Availability
Data cannot be shared publicly because of HIPPA. De-identified data would be made available from the University of Utah for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data where a data sharing agreement can be put into place. Data requests can be sent to Dr. Ann Johnson, email: ann.johnson@hsc.utah.edu, office Phone: 801-213-7156, 385-419-0712.
Funding Statement
JDR was supported by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (K23 NS114178). RMC was supported by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke T32 NS115723. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Tan LL, Kuner R. Neocortical circuits in pain and pain relief. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. Nature Publishing Group; 2021. pp. 458–471. doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00468-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Seymour B. Pain: A Precision Signal for Reinforcement Learning and Control. Neuron. 2019;101: 1029–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.055 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Caston RM, Smith EH, Davis TS, Rolston JD. The Cerebral Localization of Pain: Anatomical and Functional Considerations for Targeted Electrical Therapies. J Clin Med. 2020. doi: 10.3390/jcm9061945 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bastuji H, Frot M, Perchet C, Magnin M, Garcia-Larrea L. Pain networks from the inside: Spatiotemporal analysis of brain responses leading from nociception to conscious perception. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37: 4301–4315. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kucyi A, Davis KD. The dynamic pain connectome. Trends in Neurosciences. Elsevier Current Trends; 2015. pp. 86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.11.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Markman T, Liu CC, Chien JH, Crone NE, Zhang J, Lenz FA. EEG analysis reveals widespread directed functional interactions related to a painful cutaneous laser stimulus. J Neurophysiol. 2013;110: 2440–2449. doi: 10.1152/jn.00246.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kim JH, Chien JH, Liu CC, Lenz FA. Painful cutaneous laser stimuli induce event-related gamma-band activity in the lateral thalamus of humans. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113: 1564–1573. doi: 10.1152/jn.00778.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Liu CC, Chien JH, Chang YW, Kim JH, Anderson WS, Lenz FA. Functional Role of Induced Gammy Oscillatory Responses in Processing Noxious and Innocuous Sensory Events in Humans. Neuroscience. 2015;310: 389–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.09.047 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Mancini F, Zhang S, Seymour B. Computational and neural mechanisms of statistical pain learning. Nat Commun. 2022;13: 1–4. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-34283-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wager TD, Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, Roy M, Woo C-W, Kross E. An fMRI-Based Neurologic Signature of Physical Pain. New England Journal of Medicine. 2013;368: 1388–1397. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Davis KD, Pope GE, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ. Neural correlates of prickle sensation: a percept-related fMRI study. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5: 1121–1122. doi: 10.1038/nn955 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Asad ABA, Seah S, Baumgartner R, Feng D, Jensen A, Manigbas E, et al. Distinct BOLD fMRI responses of capsaicin-induced thermal sensation reveal pain-related brain activation in nonhuman primates. PLoS One. 2016;11: 1–22. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0156805 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Xu A, Larsen B, Baller EB, Scott JC, Sharma V, Adebimpe A, et al. Convergent neural representations of experimentally-induced acute pain in healthy volunteers: A large-scale fMRI meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;112: 300–323. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.01.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Schreckenberger M, Siessmeier T, Viertmann A, Landvogt C, Buchholz H. The unpleasantness of tonic pain is. 2005; 1175–1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Owen DG, Clarke CF, Ganapathy S, Prato FS, St. Lawrence KS. Using perfusion MRI to measure the dynamic changes in neural activation associated with tonic muscular pain. Pain. 2010;148: 375–386. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wasan AD, Loggia ML, Chen LQ, Napadow V, Kong J, Gollub RL. Neural correlates of chronic low back pain measured by arterial spin labeling. Anesthesiology. 2011;115: 364–374. doi: 10.1097/ALN.0b013e318220e880 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Marusak HA, Thomason ME, Peters C, Zundel C, Elrahal F, Rabinak CA. You say ‘prefrontal cortex’ and I say ‘anterior cingulate’: meta-analysis of spatial overlap in amygdala-to-prefrontal connectivity and internalizing symptomology. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6: 1–10. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baliki MN, Geha PY, Fields HL, Apkarian AV. Predicting Value of Pain and Analgesia: Nucleus Accumbens Response to Noxious Stimuli Changes in the Presence of Chronic Pain. Neuron. 2010;66: 149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15: 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ong WY, Stohler CS, Herr DR. Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Pain Processing. Molecular Neurobiology. Humana Press Inc.; 2019. pp. 1137–1166. doi: 10.1007/s12035-018-1130-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Schulz E, May ES, Postorino M, Tiemann L, Nickel MM, Witkovsky V, et al. Prefrontal Gamma Oscillations Encode Tonic Pain in Humans. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25: 4407–4414. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Caston RM, Davis TS, Smith EH, Rahimpour S, Rolston D. A novel thermoelectric device integrated with a psychophysical paradigm to study pain processing in human subjects. J Neurosci Methods. 2023;386: 109780. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2022.109780 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Watson AB. QUEST: A general multidimensional bayesian adaptive psychometric method. J Vis. 2017. doi: 10.1167/17.3.10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Davis TS, Caston RM, Philip B, Charlebois CM, Anderson DN, Weaver KE, et al. LeGUI: A Fast and Accurate Graphical User Interface for Automated Detection and Anatomical Localization of Intracranial Electrodes. Front Neurosci. 2021;15. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.769872 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Fan L, Li H, Zhuo J, Zhang Y, Wang J, Chen L, et al. The Human Brainnetome Atlas: A New Brain Atlas Based on Connectional Architecture. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26: 3508–3526. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Fischl B, Van Der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, Halgren E, Ségonne F, Salat DH, et al. Automatically Parcellating the Human Cerebral Cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2004;14: 11–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Rolston JD, Gross RE, Potter SM. Common median referencing for improved action potential detection with multielectrode arrays. Proceedings of the 31st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society: Engineering the Future of Biomedicine, EMBC 2009. 2009; 1604–1607. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2009.5333230 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 28.Ray S, Maunsell JHR. Different origins of gamma rhythm and high-gamma activity in macaque visual cortex. PLoS Biol. 2011;9. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000610 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Bartoli E, Bosking W, Chen Y, Li Y, Sheth SA, Beauchamp MS, et al. Functionally Distinct Gamma Range Activity Revealed by Stimulus Tuning in Human Visual Cortex. Current Biology. 2019;29: 3345–3358.e7. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Buzsáki G, Anastassiou CA, Koch C. The origin of extracellular fields and currents-EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13: 407–420. doi: 10.1038/nrn3241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Miller KJ. Broadband spectral change: Evidence for a macroscale correlate of population firing rate? Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30: 6477–6479. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6401-09.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Romo R. Conversion of sensory signals into perceptions, memories and decision. Prog Neurobiol. 2013;103: 1–2. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.May ES, Tiemann L, Schmidt P, Nickel MM, Wiedemann N, Dresel C, et al. Behavioral responses to noxious stimuli shape the perception of pain. Sci Rep. 2017;7: 1–8. doi: 10.1038/srep44083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Godinho F, Magnin M, Frot M, Perchet C, Garcia-Larrea L. Emotional modulation of pain: Is it the sensation or what we recall? Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26: 11454–11461. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2260-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.De Pauw R, Coppieters I, Caeyenberghs K, Kregel J, Aerts H, Lenoir D, et al. Associations between brain morphology and motor performance in chronic neck pain: A whole-brain surface-based morphometry approach. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40: 4266–4278. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24700 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Palermo S, Benedetti F, Costa T, Amanzio M. Pain anticipation: An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36: 1648–1661. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22727 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Budell L, Kunz M, Jackson PL, Rainville P. Mirroring pain in the brain: Emotional expression versus motor imitation. PLoS One. 2015;10: 1–20. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107526 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Schwedt TJ, Chong CD, Peplinski J, Ross K, Berisha V. Persistent post-traumatic headache vs. migraine: an MRI study demonstrating differences in brain structure. Journal of Headache and Pain. 2017;18: 1–8. doi: 10.1186/s10194-017-0796-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gerstner G, Ichesco E, Quintero A, Schmidt-Wilcke T. Changes in regional gray and white matter volume in patients with myofascial-type temporomandibular disorders: a voxel-based morphometry study. J Orofac Pain. 2011;25: 99–106. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Coppieters I, Meeus M, Kregel J, Caeyenberghs K, De Pauw R, Goubert D, et al. Relations Between Brain Alterations and Clinical Pain Measures in Chronic Musculoskeletal Pain: A Systematic Review. Journal of Pain. 2016;17: 949–962. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2016.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Houde F, Martel M, Coulombe-Lévêque A, Harvey MP, Auclair V, Mathieu D, et al. Perturbing the activity of the superior temporal gyrus during pain encoding prevents the exaggeration of pain memories: A virtual lesion study using single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2020;169: 107174. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107174 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Fu B, Wen SN, Wang B, Wang K, Zhang JY, Liu SJ. Acute and chronic pain affects local field potential of the medial prefrontal cortex in different band neural oscillations. Mol Pain. 2018;14: 3–8. doi: 10.1177/1744806918785686 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Madhavan R, Millman D, Tang H, Crone NE, Lenz FA, Tierney TS, et al. Decrease in gamma-band activity tracks sequence learning. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;8: 1–16. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Salomons T V., Johnstone T, Backonja MM, Shackman AJ, Davidson RJ. Individual differences in the effects of perceived controllability on pain perception: Critical role of the prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19: 993–1003. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.993 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hashmi JA, Baliki MN, Huang L, Baria AT, Torbey S, Hermann KM, et al. Shape shifting pain: Chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits. Brain. 2013;136: 2751–2768. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt211 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.May ES, Nickel MM, Ta Dinh S, Tiemann L, Heitmann H, Voth I, et al. Prefrontal gamma oscillations reflect ongoing pain intensity in chronic back pain patients. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40: 293–305. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Peng W, Hu L, Zhang Z, Hu Y. Changes of spontaneous oscillatory activity to tonic heat pain. PLoS One. 2014;9: 1–11. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kummer KK, Mitrić M, Kalpachidou T, Kress M. The medial prefrontal cortex as a central hub for mental comorbidities associated with chronic pain. Int J Mol Sci. 2020;21. doi: 10.3390/ijms21103440 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.De Freitas RL, Salgado-Rohner CJ, Biagioni AF, Medeiros P, Hallak JEC, Crippa JAS, et al. NMDA and AMPA/kainate glutamatergic receptors in the prelimbic medial prefrontal cortex modulate the elaborated defensive behavior and innate fear-induced antinociception elicited by GABAA receptor blockade in the medial hypothalamus. Cerebral Cortex. 2014;24: 1518–1528. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Scoville JP, Joyce E, Hunsaker J, Reese J, Wilde H, Arain A, et al. Stereotactic Electroencephalography Is Associated with Reduced Pain and Opioid Use When Compared with Subdural Grids: A Case Series. Operative Neurosurgery. 2021;21: 6–13. doi: 10.1093/ons/opab040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data cannot be shared publicly because of HIPPA. De-identified data would be made available from the University of Utah for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data where a data sharing agreement can be put into place. Data requests can be sent to Dr. Ann Johnson, email: ann.johnson@hsc.utah.edu, office Phone: 801-213-7156, 385-419-0712.