Skip to main content
Neuropsychopharmacology logoLink to Neuropsychopharmacology
. 2023 Jul 12;48(12):1707–1715. doi: 10.1038/s41386-023-01649-6

Associations between childhood ethnoracial minority density, cortical thickness, and social engagement among minority youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis

Benson S Ku 1,, Meghan Collins 2, Deidre M Anglin 3,4, Anthony M Diomino 5, Jean Addington 6, Carrie E Bearden 7, Kristin S Cadenhead 3, Tyrone D Cannon 2,8, Barbara A Cornblatt 9,10, Benjamin G Druss 11, Matcheri Keshavan 12, Daniel H Mathalon 13, Diana O Perkins 14, William S Stone 12, Ming T Tsuang 5, Scott W Woods 6, Elaine F Walker 15
PMCID: PMC10579230  PMID: 37438421

Abstract

An ethnoracial minority density (EMD) effect in studies of psychotic spectrum disorders has been observed, whereby the risk of psychosis in ethnoracial minority group individuals is inversely related to the proportion of minorities in their area of residence. The authors investigated the relationships among area-level EMD during childhood, cortical thickness (CT), and social engagement (SE) in clinical high risk for psychosis (CHR-P) youth. Data were collected as part of the North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study. Participants included 244 ethnoracial minoritized (predominantly Hispanic, Asian and Black) CHR-P youth and ethnoracial minoritized healthy controls. Among youth at CHR-P (n = 164), lower levels of EMD during childhood were associated with reduced CT in the right fusiform gyrus (adjusted β = 0.54; 95% CI 0.17 to 0.91) and right insula (adjusted β = 0.40; 95% CI 0.05 to 0.74). The associations between EMD and CT were significantly moderated by SE: among youth with lower SE (SE at or below the median, n = 122), lower levels of EMD were significantly associated with reduced right fusiform gyrus CT (adjusted β = 0.72; 95% CI 0.29 to 1.14) and reduced right insula CT (adjusted β = 0.57; 95% CI 0.18 to 0.97). However, among those with greater SE (n = 42), the associations between EMD and right insula and fusiform gyrus CT were not significant. We found evidence that lower levels of ethnic density during childhood were associated with reduced cortical thickness in regional brain regions, but this association may be buffered by greater levels of social engagement.

Subject terms: Translational research, Risk factors

Introduction

The relationship between adverse experiences and brain morphology has been documented in numerous studies in the past decades [13]. This relationship has been demonstrated in numerous experimental studies with animals [47] and, more recently, similar findings have been reported in studies of humans [5, 8, 9]. Exposure to stress appears to have a cumulative effect on gray matter morphology in humans, such that increased stress is linked with reduced thickness and volume in cortical regions. This has recently been demonstrated in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis (CHR-P), as well as healthy controls [10]. In fact, there is growing evidence that different types of stressors (e.g., threat versus deprivation) during childhood may impact distinct regions of the brain [11, 12]. While the relationship between early adversity and psychopathology is well established [13], the underlying biological mechanisms (e.g., changes in brain structure) that link early adversity, protective factors, and cortical thickness among ethnoracial minority youth at CHR-P have yet to be examined.

It has long been known that ethnoracial minority individuals in the U.S. and Europe experience greater stressful life events compared to white people [14, 15]. Greater life event stress may partially explain the higher rates of certain mental illnesses, including psychosis, among ethnoracial minorities, especially those who reside in predominantly white communities [16, 17]. It has been shown that ethnoracial minorities who live in neighborhoods with greater proportions of other ethnoracial minorities, referred to as ethnoracial minority density (EMD), have a reduced risk of developing psychopathology, including psychotic disorders [18, 19]. These findings may be explained by lower exposure to perceived discrimination and other stressors as well as greater engagement in social networks [17, 2022].

In our previous study, we found that among ethnoracial minority youth at CHR-P, higher levels of EMD during childhood predicted lower perceived discrimination at baseline, which in turn predicted remission of psychosis-risk symptoms within two-year follow-up [23]. It is possible that the inverse relationship between childhood EMD and psychosis-risk may be partially explained by discrimination related to ethnoracial minority status and other adverse social experiences, which in turn impacts select cortical regions [24, 25]. The Stimulation Discrepancy Deprivation (SDD) model of psychosis categorizes different types of structural exposures along with their proposed influences on different neural systems [26]. The SDD model proposes that EMD falls under “social discrepancy exposure” such that chronic exposure to lower levels of EMD and greater discrepancy from peers in culture and appearance may increase feelings of being ‘different’ for ethnoracial minority children, as they develop and become aware of social cues and social hierarchy in respect to their minority status [27]. Their enhanced ethnoracial minority status in context of living in areas with fewer people who also share a minority status may predispose them to greater perceived discrimination, lack of social cohesion, lower sense of belonging and support, and less robust social networks [27].

The SDD model posits these social stressors and circumstances may impact certain “social brain” regions from the oxytocinergic system, which has been linked to social attachment, empathy, and social threat [26, 28, 29]. Studies have found intranasal delivery of oxytocin to modulate the evaluation of socially relevant faces through the fusiform gyrus [30, 31]. Administration of intranasal oxytocin has also been shown to decrease activity in the anterior insula in response to social versus nonsocial positive stimuli compared with placebo treatment [32]. Other neural regions that have been shown to be impacted by oxytocin in The SDD model include the orbitofrontal cortex [33], anterior cingulate cortex [34], and posterior cingulate cortex [26, 35]. These brain regions also process social norm compliance [36], conflict monitoring [37], and social connection [26, 38]. Interestingly, loss of gray matter in these regions has also been shown to be associated with conversion to psychosis, although results have been mixed across studies [3948].

In sum, exposure to lower levels of childhood EMD may predispose ethnoracial minority youth to experience greater social stress [25, 49], which may in turn lead to gray matter loss of social brain regions [4]. In addition, recent evidence suggests that engaging in desirable social activities may buffer the deleterious effects of early social adversity on gray matter volume [50]. Therefore, it is possible that social engagement also moderates the association between lower levels of EMD and cortical thickness among ethnoracial minority youth. To our knowledge, no study has explored the relationships among childhood EMD, cortical thickness, and social engagement. The present study examines these relations in samples of ethnoracial minoritized CHR-P and healthy participants, with EMD indexed as the proportion of residents who did not identify as white non-Hispanics in the area. We predict the following: (1) Lower levels of childhood EMD will specifically be associated with reduced cortical thicknesses of the “social brain” regions as suggested from The SDD Model, which include the fusiform gyrus, insula, rostral anterior cingulate, lateral orbitofrontal, medial orbitofrontal, and posterior cingulate at baseline. We also predict that there will be greater effects on the right as compared to the left hemisphere based on prior evidence from animal models and human studies suggesting that early-life environmental stressors appear to have a greater impact on brain morphometry in the right hemisphere [5154]. (2) Social engagement will moderate the relationships between EMD and selected brain regions. Specifically, it is hypothesized that social engagement will reduce the adverse impacts of lower levels of EMD on cortical thickness among ethnoracial minorities. In addition to testing the above hypotheses, we will test the potential moderating effects of demographic and clinical characteristics including age, sex, and CHR-P status.

Methods and Materials

Participants

Baseline data were collected as part of the North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study-Phase 2 (NAPLS2), a multi-site consortium that recruited help-seeking youth at CHR-P and healthy controls. CHR-P individuals met the Criteria of Prodromal Syndromes, which is based on the Structured Interview for Psychosis Risk Syndromes (SIPS) [55]. The aims and recruitment methods have been described in prior literature [56, 57]. The participants in the present sample include those who identified with a non-White ethnoracial or Hispanic group, had complete data on childhood residence (cities/towns) in the USA suitable for geocoding, and whose structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data passed quality assurance metrics. Table 1 lists characteristics of the sample and a comparison of these characteristics between participants included and those excluded due to missing data is shown in Table S1. The Institutional Review Boards of each study site reviewed and approved the study protocol and consent forms, and procedures complied with the ethical standards of relevant committees.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic characteristics.

Total (n = 244) Ethnoracial minority youth at CHR-P (n = 164) Ethnoracial minority healthy youth (n = 80) Pa
N (%) N (%) N (%)
Age 0.229
   12.0 to 16.0 85 (34.8) 60 (36.6) 25 (31.3)
   16.1 to 20.0 88 (36.1) 62 (37.8) 26 (32.5)
   20.1 to 35.0 71 (29.1) 42 (25.6) 29 (36.3)
Sex 0.208
   Male 139 (57.0) 98 (59.8) 41 (51.3)
   Female 105 (43.0) 66 (40.2) 39 (48.8)
Race/Ethnicityb 0.675
   White Hispanic 35 (14.3) 25 (15.2) 10 (12.5)
   non-White Hispanic 62 (25.4) 45 (27.4) 17 (21.3)
   Black non-Hispanic 83 (34.0) 55 (33.5) 28 (35)
   Asian non-Hispanic 37 (15.2) 23 (14) 14 (17.5)
   Other non-Hispanic 27 (11.1) 16 (9.8) 11 (13.8)
Family history of mental illnesses <0.001
   Yes 143 (58.6) 112 (68.3) 31 (38.8)
   No 101 (41.4) 52 (31.7) 49 (61.3)
Highest paternal education 0.138
   High school or greater 214 (94.3) 138 (92.6) 76 (97.4)
   Less than high school 13 (5.7) 11 (7.4) 2 (2.6)
Highest maternal education 0.37
   High school or greater 229 (95.0) 152 (95.0) 77 (97.5)
   Less than high school 10 (4.1) 8 (5.0) 2 (2.5)
Living Below the Poverty Level During Childhood 0.148
   Yes 53 (21.7) 40 (24.4) 13 (16.2)
   No 191 (78.3) 124 (75.6) 67 (83.8)
Social Engagementc 0.024
   0 4 (1.6) 2 (1.2) 2 (2.5)
   1 18 (7.4) 17 (10.4) 1 (1.3)
   2 25 (10.2) 19 (11.6) 6 (7.5)
   3 51 (20.9) 37 (22.6) 14 (17.5)
   4 84 (34.4) 47 (28.7) 37 (46.3)
   5 62 (25.4) 42 (25.6) 20 (25)
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Area-level Characteristics During Childhood
   Percentage of residents who lived in owner-occupied housing 55.5 (13.1) 55.4 (12.7) 55.7 (13.8) 0.855
   Percentage of residents who are unemployed 6.3 (1.8) 6.3 (1.8) 6.2 (1.7) 0.946
   Percentage of residents who lived under the poverty line 12.1 (4.9) 12.3 (4.9) 11.8 (5.0) 0.463
   Percentage of residents who completed high school or obtained GED 25.1 (4.7) 24.8 (4.7) 25.8 (4.8) 0.141
   Percentage of residents who identify as ethnoracial minority 37.9 (18.6) 38.7 (18.6) 36.1 (18.5) 0.307

aP value reflects the chi-square and independent-samples t tests of sociodemographic characteristics between ethnoracial minority youth at CHR-P and healthy youth for categorical and continuous variables, respectively.

bInformation regarding race and ethnicity were self-reported and collected at baseline. For racial identification, respondents selected among several options, including First Nations, East Asian, Southeast Asian, South Asian, Black, Central/South American, West/Central Asian and Middle East, White, Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander and Interracial. Participants were then asked about Hispanic ethnicity. In this study, we consolidated racial identification and Hispanic ethnicity into categories of the US Census: White Hispanic, non-White Hispanic, Black non-Hispanic, Asian non-Hispanic, and Other non-Hispanic.

cSocial engagement was calculated by the total count of five items from the Desirable Social Activities subscale of Life Event Stress scale. These items include: (1) involvement in church or synagogue, club, neighborhood, or other organization; (2) took a vacation; (3) took up a new hobby, sport, craft, or recreational activity; (4) acquired a pet; and (5) made new friends.

Instruments

Demographic and clinical variables were obtained from self-report and interview-based measures during baseline assessments and included age, sex, race and ethnicity, family history of mental illnesses, the towns where participants spent the longest time during childhood, and social engagement. Family history of mental illnesses included bipolar disorder, depression, psychosis, and schizophrenia among first- or second-degree relatives [58, 59].

Area-level variables

Towns where individuals lived for the longest time during childhood were linked to the primary county 5-digit Federal Information Processing Standards (FIPS) codes [60]. Then, 1990 county-level characteristics were linked to these FIPS codes for those born between 1985 and 1994, whereas census data from 2000 were used for individuals born between 1995 and 2000 [61]. Censuses from these two time periods were chosen to capture the area (county) characteristics during childhood for each participant. Area-level EMD was defined as percentage of all residents who did not self-identify their race/ethnicity as White non-Hispanic as done in prior literature [19, 62]. Other definitions of ethnic or group density (including the proportion of others belonging to the same ethnoracial group) were not used in this study due to missing area-level characteristics regarding specific ethnoracial minority groups, including those who identify as Interracial, for example. Area-level Socioeconomic Deprivation was defined as the average z-score of the percentage of residents living below the poverty level, percentage of residents who did not complete high school and/or obtain GED, percentage of residents in the civilian labor force who were unemployed, and percentage of owner-occupied housing units (reversed) as done in prior literature [63, 64].

Social engagement

Social engagement was calculated using the total count of five items from the Desirable Social Activities subscale of Life Event Stress (LES) scale as used in prior studies [50, 65, 66] and this score ranges from zero to five. The LES scale was assessed at baseline regarding events that have occurred in the participant’s life. These items include: (1) Involvement in church or synagogue, club, neighborhood, or other organization; (2) Took a vacation; (3) Took up a new hobby, sport, craft, or recreational activity; (4) Acquired a pet; and (5) Made new friends.

Imaging data acquisition and processing

Cortical thickness of all brain regions was obtained at baseline for this study. Five sites used Siemens scanners, which used 12-channel head coils, and three sites used GE scanners, which used 8-channel head coils. All scanners had a magnetic field strength of 3-Tesla. Sequence parameters were optimized for each scanner manufacturer, software version, and coil configuration according to the ADNI protocol [67]. To extract regional parcels for cortical thicknesses, automated surface-based cortical reconstruction, cortical parcellation, and subcortical segmentation were carried out using the Freesurfer software suite version 5.3 [68, 69]. Regional parcels for cortical thicknesses were extracted using a gyral and sulcal pattern-based Desikan atlas with 34 parcels in each hemisphere [70].

Data analysis

A priori 12 cortical ROIs were derived from prior literature on The SDD Model [26] and included the fusiform gyrus, insula, lateral orbitofrontal cortex, medial orbitofrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, rostral anterior cingulate cortex of the right and left hemispheres. For Aim 1, we first calculated the Pearson’s correlations between EMD and cortical thicknesses of these 12 ROIs, adjusting for multiple comparisons using the False Discovery Rate (FDR). In this study, the outcomes were cortical brain ROIs, and we did not include subcortical structures that may have been relevant (e.g., amygdala). However, we did assess for possible structural effects outside the pre-hypothesized anatomical regions, and they were significant if they survived multiple comparisons correction across the whole brain. Then, each significant association between EMD and ROI was tested using a generalized linear mixed (GLM) model with the inclusion of covariates. Fixed factors included age, sex, CHR-P status, family history of mental illnesses, and area-level deprivation. Random factors included unique counties and sites. Variance inflation factors of less than or equal to five was used to rule out multicollinearity in these models. We tested whether the association between EMD and each ROI may differ based upon age, sex, and CHR-P status. Each significant association between EMD and ROI in the adjusted multi-level model was further tested with their respective interaction terms. For each significant interaction term, we analyzed the relationships between EMD and cortical thicknesses by subgroups. For Aim 2, we assessed social engagement as a potential moderator. Each significant association between EMD and ROI in the adjusted multi-level model was further tested with the interaction term, EMD-by-social engagement. For each significant interaction term, we conducted a secondary analysis to further analyze the significant interactions observed. We stratified the sample into subgroups of participants based upon the median value of the total social engagement score and assessed the association between EMD and ROI.

R version 4.2.1 was used for statistical analyses.

Results

Sample characteristics

This study (N = 244; mean (SD) age, 18.2 (3.4) years) included 139 males (57.0%). There were 164 (67.2%) youth at CHR-P. Other participant sociodemographic information is presented in Table 1. Childhood EMD was positively correlated with cortical thicknesses of the right fusiform gyrus (Pearson’s r = 0.183; FDR-adjusted p < 0.05) and right insula (Pearson’s r = 0.177; FDR-adjusted p < 0.05) (Fig. 1). Childhood EMD was not significantly associated with cortical thicknesses of other brain regions (Table S2).

Fig. 1. Associations between childhood EMD and cortical thickness of ROIs selected a priori from The Stimulation Discrepancy Deprivation Model (n = 244).

Fig. 1

A Pearson correlations between childhood EMD and cortical thicknesses of selected brain regions. B False discovery rate (FDR)-corrected maps indicate that lower levels of EMD during childhood are significantly associated with lower cortical thicknesses of the right fusiform and right fusiform. ACC anterior cingulate, OFC orbitofrontal, PCC posterior cingulate cortex. All significant associations (FDR-adjusted p < 0.05) are shown in bold.

Generalized linear mixed models predicting cortical thicknesses of fusiform gyrus and insula

In the main analysis, lower levels of childhood EMD were associated with reduced cortical thickness of the right fusiform gyrus (adjusted β = 0.31; 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.59; p = 0.041) and the right insula (adjusted β = 0.30; 95% CI = 0.06 to 0.53; p = 0.019). To test whether these associations would differ by age, sex, and CHR-P status, six additional GLM models were constructed assessing each interaction term with childhood EMD for the right fusiform gyrus and right insula (Table S3). Only the interaction term, CHR-P status-by-childhood EMD was significantly associated with right fusiform gyrus cortical thickness (adjusted β = 0.25; 95% CI = 0.01 to 0.50; p = 0.040), suggesting that there may be differences in the association between childhood EMD and cortical thickness between healthy youth and those at CHR-P. Stratifying this association by healthy youth and those at CHR-P demonstrated that the associations between childhood EMD and right fusiform gyrus (adjusted β = 0.54; 95% CI = 0.17 to 0.91; p = 0.006) and right insula (adjusted β = 0.40; 95% CI = 0.05 to 0.74; p = 0.028) were only significant among youth at CHR-P, but not among healthy youth (Table 2).

Table 2.

Generalized linear mixed models of childhood ethnoracial minority density predicting right fusiform and right insula cortical thicknesses among minorities stratified by healthy youth and those at clinical high-risk for psychosis.

Right Fusiform among
CHR-P ethnoracial minority youth
(Model A; n = 164)
Right Insula among
CHR-P ethnoracial minority youth
(Model B; n = 164)
Right Fusiform among
healthy ethnoracial minority youth
(Model C; n = 80)
Right Insula among
healthy ethnoracial minority youth
(Model D; n = 80)
Parameters β SE 95% CI p β SE 95% CI p β SE 95% CI p β SE 95% CI p
Intercept 0.86 0.47 −0.06 to 1.78 0.071 1.57 0.46 0.67 to 2.48 <0.001 1.61 0.53 0.57 to 2.65 0.003 1.67 0.52 0.65 to 2.69 0.002
Age −0.05 0.02 0.10 to 0.00 0.046 −0.08 0.02 0.12 to0.03 0.001 −0.08 0.03 0.13 to0.02 0.006 −0.09 0.03 0.14 to0.03 0.003
Female Sex −0.28 0.14 −0.56 to 0.01 0.058 −0.22 0.15 −0.51 to 0.07 0.139 0.11 0.20 −0.28 to 0.50 0.582 −0.41 0.19 −0.79 to −0.02 0.041
Family History of Mental Illnesses 0.08 0.15 −0.22 to 0.38 0.599 −0.07 0.15 −0.37 to 0.24 0.665 −0.14 0.20 −0.53 to 0.25 0.475 0.26 0.19 −0.12 to 0.64 0.188
Area-level Socioeconomic Deprivation −0.41 0.22 −0.84 to 0.02 0.068 −0.34 0.21 −0.75 to 0.06 0.103 −0.16 0.21 −0.57 to 0.24 0.432 −0.35 0.20 −0.75 to 0.05 0.091
Area-level Ethnoracial Minority Density 0.54 0.19 0.17 to 0.91 0.006 0.40 0.18 0.05 to 0.74 0.028 0.12 0.17 −0.21 to 0.44 0.485 0.26 0.16 −0.06 to 0.58 0.119
Pseudo-R2 (fixed effects) 14.6% 14.2% 10.7% 24.5%

CHR-P clinical high risk for psychosis.

All models used generalized linear mixed multivariate models with the following fixed effects (age, female sex, family history of mental illnesses, and area-level deprivation) and random effects (counties and sites).

Model A tested the associations between all fixed effects and right fusiform cortical thickness among CHR-P ethnoracial minorities (n = 164).

Model B tested the associations between all fixed effects and right insula cortical thickness among CHR-P ethnoracial minorities (n = 164).

Model C tested the associations between all fixed effects and right fusiform cortical thickness among healthy ethnoracial minorities (n = 80).

Model D tested the associations between all fixed effects and right insula cortical thickness among healthy ethnoracial minorities (n = 80).

All significant associations (p < 0.05) are shown in bold.

Moderating effect of social engagement

Among ethnoracial minority youth at CHR-P, the interaction term EMD-by-social engagement was significantly associated with cortical thickness of the right fusiform gyrus (adjusted β = −0.15; 95% CI = −0.26 to −0.04; p = 0.010) and right insula (adjusted β = −0.14; 95% CI = −0.26 to −0.03; p = 0.018). To further analyze the significant interactions observed, we stratified the sample into two subgroups of participants, those whose total social engagement score was at or below (n = 122) and above (n = 42) four, the median social engagement score. Among those with lower social engagement, lower levels of EMD were significantly associated with reduced cortical thickness of the right fusiform gyrus (adjusted β = 0.72; 95% CI = 0.29 to 1.14; p = 0.002) and right insula (adjusted β = 0.57; 95% CI = 0.18 to 0.97; p = 0.007). Conversely, among those with higher social engagement, EMD was not significantly associated with cortical thickness of the right fusiform gyrus (adjusted β = 0.03; 95% CI = −0.61 to 0.67; p = 0.921) or right insula (adjusted β = 0.29; 95% CI = −0.85 to 0.29; p = 0.338) (Table 3). Best-fit lines of the associations between EMD and cortical thicknesses of the right fusiform gyrus and right insula for each subgroup is shown in Fig. 2.

Table 3.

Generalized linear mixed models of childhood ethnoracial minority density predicting right fusiform and right insula cortical thicknesses among minorities at clinical high-risk for psychosis stratified by social engagement.

Right Fusiform and Greater SE
(Model E; n = 42)
Right Fusiform and Lower SE
(Model F; n = 122)
Right Insula and Greater SE
(Model G; n = 42)
Right Insula and Lower SE
(Model H; n = 122)
Parameters β SE 95% CI p β SE 95% CI p β SE 95% CI p β SE 95% CI p
Intercept 1.53 1.01 −0.45 to 3.50 0.138 0.84 0.54 −0.22 to 1.90 0.124 2.09 0.94 0.24 to 3.94 0.033 1.69 0.52 0.66 to 2.71 0.002
Age −0.08 0.05 −0.18 to 0.03 0.164 −0.05 0.03 −0.10 to 0.00 0.069 −0.11 0.05 −0.21 to −0.01 0.039 −0.09 0.03 −0.14 to −0.03 0.002
Female Sex −0.65 0.31 −1.26 to −0.04 0.043 −0.16 0.17 −0.49 to 0.18 0.355 −0.31 0.30 −0.89 to 0.28 0.315 −0.21 0.17 −0.54 to 0.13 0.233
Family History of Mental Illnesses 0.21 0.33 −0.44 to 0.87 0.524 0.05 0.18 −0.30 to 0.40 0.768 0.16 0.31 −0.46 to 0.77 0.624 −0.09 0.18 −0.44 to 0.26 0.613
Area-level Socioeconomic Deprivation 0.02 0.38 −0.74 to 0.77 0.966 −0.54 0.25 −1.04 to −0.05 0.035 0.32 0.36 −0.38 to 1.02 0.375 −0.55 0.24 −1.01 to −0.09 0.024
Area-level Ethnoracial Minority Density 0.03 0.33 −0.61 to 0.67 0.921 0.72 0.22 0.29 to 1.14 0.002 −0.28 0.29 −0.85 to 0.29 0.338 0.57 0.20 0.18 to 0.97 0.007
Pseudo-R2 (fixed effects) 17.8% 18.1% 14.9% 19.0%

SE social engagement, CHR-P clinical high risk for psychosis.

Social engagement was dichotomized into higher and lower based upon its median value of 4.

All models used generalized linear mixed multivariate models with the following fixed effects (age, female sex, clinical high risk for psychosis status, family history of mental illnesses, and area-level deprivation) and random effects (counties and sites).

Model E tested the associations between all fixed effects and right fusiform cortical thickness among CHR-P minorities with greater social engagement (n = 42).

Model F tested the associations between all fixed effects and right fusiform cortical thickness among CHR-P minorities with lower social engagement (n = 122).

Model G tested the associations between all fixed effects and right insula cortical thickness among CHR-P minorities with greater social engagement (n = 42).

Model H tested the associations between all fixed effects and right insula cortical thickness among CHR-P minorities with lower social engagement (n = 122).

All significant associations (p < 0.05) are shown in bold.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Best-fit lines for the associations between childhood ethnoracial minority density and cortical thicknesses by social engagement among minority youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis (n = 164).

Discussion

The current study builds upon prior studies on ethnic density effect in psychosis and finds neurobiological correlates of exposure to EMD during childhood. In addition, we showed that the associations between childhood EMD and right insula and fusiform gyrus cortical thicknesses were significant among minority youth at CHR-P and not among those who are healthy, although it is possible that we could not detect significant associations between EMD and cortical thicknesses due to the relatively small sample size of healthy participants. Among youth at CHR-P, we found that social engagement moderated the association between childhood EMD and right insula and right fusiform gyrus cortical thicknesses. Those who are more socially engaged already have consistently high cortical thickness regardless of levels of EMD. However, for those who are less socially engaged, levels of EMD are correlated with cortical thickness and this subgroup may benefit from social support and greater levels of EMD during childhood. This highlights the importance of not only social engagement, but also of having those protective connectedness cultural factors during childhood in the event there is less social engagement in a person’s life trajectory.

It is possible that ethnoracial minority youths growing up in communities with greater number of people who share similar minority status may feel less of a “minority” in their local communities [71] and therefore, they may experience less perceived discrimination [72]. Conversely, for minority youths who grew up in communities with fewer people who share similar minority status, their minority status may be accentuated, which may impact their ability to “fit in” to social groups [71]. Perhaps, this chronic experience of being an “outsider” or othered may abnormally activate the fusiform gyrus [72] and insula [73, 74], as abnormal activation has been shown to be associated with cortical thinning over time [75].

In accordance with our prediction, we found that EMD exposure during childhood was associated with cortical thickness in regions of the right hemisphere. Prior literature suggests that greater plasticity during younger age would lead to early recruitment of the right hemisphere [54, 76, 77]. Perhaps, early exposure to the social environment may result in greater changes to the gray matter of certain social brain regions in the right hemisphere.

The fusiform gyrus is particularly sensitive to human faces [31, 78, 79]. Studies have shown that this area of the brain is activated during social categorization tasks. In these experiments, participants are asked to distinguish between different social groups, such as males and females, or people of different races [8085]. Other studies have found that the fusiform gyrus is more active when participants have to update their own or others' social status [86, 87]. The frequent activation of the fusiform gyrus during explicit social categorization may indicate its role in modulating social perception from the top down. It is possible that the initial role of the fusiform gyrus in discriminating between social categories and identifying significant social cues based on pre-existing expectations and attention to social context may be relevant to the processing of socially relevant environmental exposures.

The right insula has been shown to play a role in distinguishing between in-group and out-group membership, which may arise from its role in connecting the mirror neuron system and the limbic system [8891]. This brain region has also been specifically shown to demonstrate strong outgroup bias. Numerous studies have found that the insula exhibits greater activation in response to various social categories and tasks. These include racial and political outgroups [9296], unfamiliar faces [97], dominant faces [98], and the ability to discriminate against minimal outgroups [86].

It is possible that hyperactivation of the fusiform gyrus and insula from heightened social threat of being an ethnoracial minority may lead to dendritic shrinkage and progressive cortical gray matter reduction of these brain regions over time [99], which have been shown to be associated with conversion to psychosis [43, 100, 101]. However, greater social engagement may moderate this effect by helping to minimize the perception of being in an out-group. Through engaging with other people in the community (i.e., making new friends, being part of a community organization or church), people of different backgrounds can come together, which might attenuate some of the in-group/out-group differences based on other characteristics and partially mitigate the psychological and neurobiological impact of ethnoracial minority status [24, 102].

An alternative interpretation of the significant moderation of social engagement on the association between EMD and cortical thickness is that social engagement may be differentiating those who have a more severe illness. Since one of the early premorbid changes and indicators of psychotic disorders is impaired social functioning, it may be possible that the association between lower EMD and reduced cortical thickness may be more evident among the group of CHR-P who are more ill. Future research should explore whether social engagement and its interaction with EMD predicts conversion to psychosis.

This study has several limitations. First, this study has a modest sample size and has a cohort design. Prospective longitudinal studies with large sample size are warranted to explore the causal directionality of EMD, cortical thickness, and social engagement as well as sensitive developmental exposure time periods in which EMD and social engagement might have their greatest impact on cortical thickness. Larger studies that include more ethnoracial minorities may also allow for the investigation of ethnoracial differences in the relationship between EMD and cortical thickness as well as investigating the relationship between area-level own group ethnic density and cortical thickness. It is important to note that in our study, EMD refers to the proportion of ethnoracial minorities and not the proportion of those who belong to the same ethnoracial group. Nevertheless, it is possible that social support from one’s own as well as other ethnoracial minority groups may impact cortical thickness. Second, this study identified a priori cortical brain regions based on the SDD model. However, it is possible that other subcortical brain regions (i.e. hippocampus and amygdala) may be impacted by social stress via the HPA axis [50]. Future research should examine the potential impact of EMD on other subcortical brain areas. Third, this study did not identify which part of the insula or fusiform gyrus may have been impacted by EMD. Future studies should investigate the relationship of EMD and specific sub-regions (e.g., anterior and posterior regions). Fourth, the assessment of childhood exposures at the county level was limited by availability of data provided. It is possible exposure to ethnic density may be more important at home versus at school, for example [103]. The non-specificities of both exposure and outcome measures may explain the modest effect sizes of the correlation between EMD and cortical thicknesses. For example, ethnic density was measured at the county level (as opposed to the more granular neighborhood or school levels) and cortical thickness of insula includes both the anterior and posterior insula. Follow-up studies assessing more specific area-level measurements of EMD and sub-regions of the brain are warranted. Lastly, the measurement of social engagement used in this study was limited, and it is still unclear which components of participant social engagement and what period (e.g., childhood or adolescence) might be most important in buffering the deleterious effects of adverse social environment on cortical thickness.

Conclusions

This study provides novel evidence for the associations between childhood EMD and right fusiform gyrus and right insula cortical thickness, with more prominent effects among minority youth at CHR-P. Furthermore, social engagement moderates this relationship as those with greater social engagement already seem to exhibit greater cortical thickness regardless of levels of ethnic density. These findings suggest there are aspects of the social environment that may have protective effects on neurobiological markers of conversion to psychosis. Further research is needed to better understand mechanisms linking environment, brain, and behavior.

Supplementary information

Supplemental Material (31.1KB, docx)

Author contributions

The authors confirm contribution to the paper as follows: study conception and design: BK, JA, CB, KC, TC, BC, BD, MK, DM, DP, WS, MT, SW, EW; data collection: BK, JA, CB, KC, TC, BC, MK, DM, DP, WS, MT, SW, EW; analysis and interpretation of results: BK, MC, DA, AD; draft manuscript preparation: BK, MC, DA, AD, BD, EW; or revise critically for important intellectual content: JA, CB, KC, TC, BC, MK, DM, DP, WS, MT, SW. All authors reviewed the results, approved the final version of the manuscript, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

This work was supported in part by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) grants U01 MH081902 to TDC, P50 MH066286 to CEB, U01 MH081857 to BAC, U01 MH82022 to SWW, U01 MH066134 to JA, U01 MH081944 to KSC, R01 U01 MH066069 to DOP, R01 MH076989 to DHM, U01 MH081988 to EFW, and K23 MH129684 to BSK.

Competing interests

TDC has served as a consultant for Boehringer-Ingelheim Pharmaceuticals and Lundbeck A/S. DHM has served as a consultant for Aptinyx, Boehringer-Ingelheim Pharmaceuticals, Cadent Therapeutics, and Greenwich Biosciences. DOP has served as a consultant for Sunovion and Alkermes, has received research support from Boehringer-Ingelheim, and has received royalties from American Psychiatric Association Publishing. SWW has received investigator-initiated research support from Pfizer and sponsor-initiated research support from Auspex and Teva; he has served as a consultant for Biomedisyn (unpaid), Boehringer-Ingelheim, and Merck and as an unpaid consultant to DSM-5; he has been granted a patent for a method of treating prodromal schizophrenia with glycine; and he has received royalties from Oxford University Press. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41386-023-01649-6.

References

  • 1.Belleau EL, Treadway MT, Pizzagalli DA. The impact of stress and major depressive disorder on hippocampal and medial prefrontal cortex morphology. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;85:443–53. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.09.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sanacora G, Yan Z, Popoli M. The stressed synapse 2.0: pathophysiological mechanisms in stress-related neuropsychiatric disorders. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2022;23:86–103. doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00540-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lenart-Bugla M, Szcześniak D, Bugla B, et al. The association between allostatic load and brain: a systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2022;145:105917. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Alleva E, Santucci D. Psychosocial vs. “physical” stress situations in rodents and humans. Physiol Behav. 2001;73:313–20. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00498-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sandi C, Haller J. Stress and the social brain: behavioural effects and neurobiological mechanisms. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:290–304. doi: 10.1038/nrn3918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hodges T, McCormick C. Stress and social development in adolescence in a rodent model, in Routledge International Handbook of Social Neuroendocrinology. London, Routledge: Taylor & Francis Group; 2018. pp. 479–503. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Patel D, Kas MJ, Chattarji S, et al. Rodent models of social stress and neuronal plasticity: relevance to depressive-like disorders. Behavioural Brain Research. 2019;369:111900. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.111900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ulmer Yaniv A, Salomon R, Waidergoren S, et al. Synchronous caregiving from birth to adulthood tunes humans’ social brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2021;118:e2012900118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2012900118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Shaw GA, Dupree JL, Neigh GN: Adolescent maturation of the prefrontal cortex: Role of stress and sex in shaping adult risk for compromise [Internet]. Genes, Brain and Behavior 2020; 19 [cited 2023 Apr 5] Available from: 10.1111/gbb.12626. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 10.Aberizk K, Collins MA, Addington J, et al. Life event stress and reduced cortical thickness in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis and healthy control subjects. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2022;7:171–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.LoPilato AM, Goines K, Addington J, et al. Impact of childhood adversity on corticolimbic volumes in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2019;213:48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.01.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Machlin L, Egger HL, Stein CR, et al. Distinct associations of deprivation and threat with alterations in brain structure in early childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2023;S0890-8567(23):00061–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2023.02.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Copeland WE, Keeler G, Angold A, et al. Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress in childhood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:577–84. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hatch SL, Dohrenwend BP. Distribution of traumatic and other stressful life events by race/ethnicity, gender, SES and age: a review of the research. Am J Community Psychol. 2007;40:313–32. doi: 10.1007/s10464-007-9134-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Muscatell KA, Alvarez GM, Bonar AS, et al. Brain–body pathways linking racism and health. American Psychologist. 2022;77:1049–60. doi: 10.1037/amp0001084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Faris REL, Dunham HW. Mental disorders in urban areas: an ecological study of schizophrenia and other psychoses. Oxford, England: Univ. Chicago Press; 1939. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Kirkbride JB, Errazuriz A, Croudace TJ, et al. Incidence of schizophrenia and other psychoses in England, 1950-2009: a systematic review and meta-analyses. PLoS One. 2012;7:e31660. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bécares L, Dewey ME, Das-Munshi J. Ethnic density effects for adult mental health: systematic review and meta-analysis of international studies. Psychol Med. 2018;48:2054–72. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717003580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Baker SJ, Jackson M, Jongsma H, et al. The ethnic density effect in psychosis: a systematic review and multilevel meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2021;219:632–43. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2021.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Veling W. Ethnic minority position and risk for psychotic disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2013;26:166–71. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32835d9e43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Crush E, Arseneault L, Jaffee SR, et al. Protective factors for psychotic symptoms among poly-victimized children. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2018;44:691–700. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Anglin DM, Ereshefsky S, Klaunig MJ, et al. From womb to neighborhood: a racial analysis of social determinants of psychosis in the United States. AJP. 2021;178:599–10. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20071091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Anglin DM, Espinosa A, Addington J, et al. Childhood Area-Level Ethnic Density in the U.S. and Psychosis Risk among Ethnoracial Minoritized Individuals in the NAPLS2 Cohort. JAMA Psychiatry. 2023; In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 24.Bécares L, Nazroo J, Stafford M. The buffering effects of ethnic density on experienced racism and health. Health Place. 2009;15:670–8. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Das-Munshi J, Becares L, Dewey ME, et al. Understanding the effect of ethnic density on mental health: multi-level investigation of survey data from England. BMJ. 2010;341:c5367. doi: 10.1136/bmj.c5367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Vargas T, Conley RE, Mittal VA. Chronic stress, structural exposures and neurobiological mechanisms: A stimulation, discrepancy and deprivation model of psychosis. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2020;152:41–69. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2019.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kearns A, Whitley E. Perceived neighborhood ethnic diversity and social outcomes: context-dependent effects within a postindustrial city undergoing regeneration. J Urban Aff. 2018;40:186–208. doi: 10.1080/07352166.2017.1343632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Heinrichs M, Baumgartner T, Kirschbaum C, et al. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:1389–98. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00465-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Norman GJ, Hawkley LC, Cole SW, et al. Social neuroscience: The social brain, oxytocin, and health. Social Neurosci. 2012;7:18–29. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2011.568702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Petrovic P, Kalisch R, Singer T, et al. Oxytocin attenuates affective evaluations of conditioned faces and amygdala activity. J Neurosci. 2008;28:6607–15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4572-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Petrovic P, Kalisch R, Pessiglione M, et al. Learning affective values for faces is expressed in amygdala and fusiform gyrus. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2008;3:109–18. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Yao S, Zhao W, Geng Y, et al. Oxytocin facilitates approach behavior to positive social stimuli via decreasing anterior insula activity. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;21:918–25. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyy068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Chen Y, Becker B, Zhang Y, et al. Oxytocin increases the pleasantness of affective touch and orbitofrontal cortex activity independent of valence. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2020;39:99–10. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2020.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Li X-H, Matsuura T, Xue M, et al. Oxytocin in the anterior cingulate cortex attenuates neuropathic pain and emotional anxiety by inhibiting presynaptic long-term potentiation. Cell Reports. 2021;36:109411. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Riem MME, Van IJzendoorn MH, Tops M, et al. Oxytocin effects on complex brain networks are moderated by experiences of maternal love withdrawal. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2013;23:1288–95. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Spitzer M, Fischbacher U, Herrnberger B, et al. The neural signature of social norm compliance. Neuron. 2007;56:185–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Adolphs R. The social brain: neural basis of social knowledge. Annu Rev Psychol. 2009;60:693–16. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Eisenberger NI. The neural bases of social pain: evidence for shared representations with physical pain. Psychosomatic Med. 2012;74:126–35. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182464dd1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Pantelis C, Velakoulis D, McGorry PD, et al. Neuroanatomical abnormalities before and after onset of psychosis: a cross-sectional and longitudinal MRI comparison. The Lancet. 2003;361:281–8. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12323-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Borgwardt S, Mcguire P, Aston J, et al. Reductions in frontal, temporal and parietal volume associated with the onset of psychosis. Schizophrenia Res. 2008;106:108–14. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Fornito A, Yung AR, Wood SJ, et al. Anatomic abnormalities of the anterior cingulate cortex before psychosis onset: an MRI study of ultra-high-risk individuals. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;64:758–65. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sun D, Phillips L, Velakoulis D, et al. Progressive brain structural changes mapped as psychosis develops in ‘at risk’ individuals. Schizophrenia Res. 2009;108:85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Takahashi T, Wood SJ, Yung AR, et al. Progressive gray matter reduction of the superior temporal gyrus during transition to psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:366. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Jung WH, Kim JS, Jang JH, et al. Cortical thickness reduction in individuals at ultra-high-risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Bull. 2011;37:839–49. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ziermans TB, Schothorst PF, Schnack HG, et al. Progressive structural brain changes during development of psychosis. Schizophrenia Bull. 2012;38:519–30. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Cannon TD, Chung Y, He G, Sun D, Jacobson A, van Erp TGM, et al. Progressive reduction in cortical thickness as psychosis develops: a multisite longitudinal neuroimaging study of youth at elevated clinical risk. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:147–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.05.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Collins MA, Ji JL, Chung Y, et al.: Accelerated cortical thinning precedes and predicts conversion to psychosis: The NAPLS3 longitudinal study of youth at clinical high-risk [Internet]. Mol Psychiatry 2022; [cited 2023 Mar 10] Available from: https://www.nature.com/articles/s41380-022-01870-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 48.Del ReEC, Stone WS, Bouix S, et al. Baseline cortical thickness reductions in clinical high risk for psychosis: brain regions associated with conversion to psychosis versus non-conversion as assessed at one-year follow-up in the Shanghai-at-risk-for-psychosis (SHARP) study. Schizophr Bull. 2021;47:562–74. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Shell AM, Peek MK, Eschbach K. Neighborhood Hispanic composition and depressive symptoms among Mexican-descent residents of Texas City, Texas. Soc Sci Med. 2013;99:56–63. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ku BS, Aberizk K, Addington J, et al. The association between neighborhood poverty and hippocampal volume among individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis: the moderating role of social engagement. Schizophr Bull. 2022;48:1032–42. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbac055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Toga AW, Thompson PM. Mapping brain asymmetry. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:37–48. doi: 10.1038/nrn1009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Taylor RL, Cooper SR, Jackson JJ, et al. Assessment of neighborhood poverty, cognitive function, and prefrontal and hippocampal volumes in children. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3:e2023774. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.23774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ku BS, Addington J, Bearden CE, et al. The associations between area-level residential instability and gray matter volumes from the North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS) consortium. Schizophr Res. 2022;241:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2021.12.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Esteves M, Ganz E, Sousa N, et al. Asymmetrical brain plasticity: physiology and pathology. Neuroscience. 2021;454:3–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.01.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.McGlashan T, Walsh B, Woods S: The Psychosis-Risk Syndrome: Handbook for Diagnosis and Follow-Up [Internet]. Oxford University Press, 2010. Available from https://books.google.com/books?id=FXM\_utb3D8EC.
  • 56.Addington J, Cadenhead KS, Cornblatt BA, et al. North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS 2): Overview and recruitment. Schizophrenia Research. 2012;142:77–82. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Addington J, Piskulic D, Liu L, et al. Comorbid diagnoses for youth at clinical high risk of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research. 2017;190:90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.03.043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.NIMH Genetics Initiative. Family Interview for Genetic Studies. Rockville: National Institute of Mental Health; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Georgopoulos G, Stowkowy J, Liu L, et al. The role of a family history of psychosis for youth at clinical high risk of psychosis. Early Intervent Psychiatry. 2019;13:251–6. doi: 10.1111/eip.12471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.US Cities Database [Internet]. Simple Maps Available from: Simplemaps.Com.
  • 61.Selected housing characteristics, 1995–2000 American Community Survey 5-year estimates. US Census Bureau, 2002.
  • 62.Bécares L, Shaw R, Nazroo J, et al. Ethnic density effects on physical morbidity, mortality, and health behaviors: a systematic review of the literature. Am J Public Health. 2012;102:e33–66. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Nagendra A, Halverson TF, Pinkham AE, et al. Neighborhood socioeconomic status and racial disparities in schizophrenia: An exploration of domains of functioning. Schizophrenia Res. 2020;224:95–101. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.09.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Karcher NR, Schiffman J, Barch DM. Environmental risk factors and psychotic-like experiences in children aged 9–10. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;60:490–500. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Falcón LM, Todorova I, Tucker K. Social support, life events, and psychological distress among the Puerto Rican population in the Boston area of the United States. Aging Mental Health. 2009;13:863–73. doi: 10.1080/13607860903046552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Rodríguez-Galán MB, Falcón LM. Patterns of Social Activity Engagement Among Older Hispanics and Their Relationship to Sociodemographic and Health Variables. Activit Adapt Aging. 2010;34:251–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.MRI Acquisition [Internet]. ADNI 2021; Available from: http://adni.loni.usc.edu/methods/mri-tool/mri-analysis/.
  • 68.Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. NeuroImage. 1999;9:179–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Fischl B. Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2004;14:11–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage. 2006;31:968–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Finney N, Jivraj S. Ethnic group population change and neighbourhood belonging. Urban Stud. 2013;50:3323–41. doi: 10.1177/0042098013482497. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Bécares L, Cormack D, Harris R. Ethnic density and area deprivation: neighbourhood effects on Māori health and racial discrimination in Aotearoa/New Zealand. Soc Sci Med. 2013;88:76–82. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Bach P, Frischknecht U, Bungert M, et al. Effects of social exclusion and physical pain in chronic opioid maintenance treatment: fMRI correlates. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2019;29:291–305. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.11.1109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Mwilambwe-Tshilobo L, Spreng RN. Social exclusion reliably engages the default network: a meta-analysis of Cyberball. Neuroimage. 2021;227:117666. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Putcha D, Brickhouse M, O’Keefe K, et al. Hippocampal hyperactivation associated with cortical thinning in Alzheimer’s disease signature regions in non-demented elderly adults. J Neurosci. 2011;31:17680–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4740-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Hull R, Vaid J. Bilingual language lateralization: A meta-analytic tale of two hemispheres*. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1987–2008. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Keenan JP, Thangaraj V, Halpern AR, et al. Absolute pitch and planum temporale. Neuroimage. 2001;14:1402–8. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Platek SM, Wathne K, Tierney NG, et al. Neural correlates of self-face recognition: an effect-location meta-analysis. Brain Research. 2008;1232:173–84. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Jung S, Kim J-H, Kang N-O, et al. Fusiform gyrus volume reduction associated with impaired facial expressed emotion recognition and emotional intensity recognition in patients with schizophrenia spectrum psychosis. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2021;307:111226. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2020.111226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.O’Doherty J, Winston J, Critchley H, et al. Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:147–55. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00145-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty J, et al. Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science. 2004;303:1157–62. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Van Bavel JJ, Packer DJ, Cunningham WA. The neural substrates of in-group bias: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:1131–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02214.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Van Bavel JJ, Packer DJ, Cunningham WA. Modulation of the fusiform face area following minimal exposure to motivationally relevant faces: evidence of in-group enhancement (not out-group disregard) J Cognit Neurosci. 2011;23:3343–54. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Marsh LE, de C Hamilton AF. Dissociation of mirroring and mentalising systems in autism. NeuroImage. 2011;56:1511–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Farrow TFD, Jones SC, Kaylor-Hughes CJ, et al. Higher or lower? The functional anatomy of perceived allocentric social hierarchies. NeuroImage. 2011;57:1552–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.069. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Rilling JK, Goldsmith DR, Glenn AL, et al. The neural correlates of the affective response to unreciprocated cooperation. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1256–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Zink CF, Tong Y, Chen Q, et al. Know your place: neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron. 2008;58:273–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.01.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Augustine J. Circuitry and functional aspects of the insular lobe in primates including humans. Brain Res Rev. 1996;22:229–44. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(96)00011-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Iacoboni M, Dapretto M. The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006;7:942–51. doi: 10.1038/nrn2024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Bud Craig AD. How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, et al. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:189–95. doi: 10.1038/nn1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Phelps EA, O’Connor KJ, Cunningham WA, et al. Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. J Cognit Neurosci. 2000;12:729–38. doi: 10.1162/089892900562552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Richeson JA, Baird AA, Gordon HL, et al. An fMRI investigation of the impact of interracial contact on executive function. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6:1323–8. doi: 10.1038/nn1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Richeson JA, Todd AR, Trawalter S, et al. Eye-gaze direction modulates race-related amygdala activity. Group Processes Intergroup Relat. 2008;11:233–46. doi: 10.1177/1368430207088040. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Kaplan JT, Aziz-Zadeh L, Uddin LQ, et al. The self across the senses: an fMRI study of self-face and self-voice recognition. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci. 2008;3:218–23. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Falk EB, Spunt RP, Lieberman MD. Ascribing beliefs to ingroup and outgroup political candidates: neural correlates of perspective-taking, issue importance and days until the election. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2012;367:731–43. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Ida Gobbini M, Leibenluft E, Santiago N, et al. Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces. NeuroImage. 2004;22:1628–35. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Chiao JY, Adams RB, Tse PU, et al. Knowing Who’s Boss: fMRI and ERP Investigations of Social Dominance Perception. Group Processes Intergroup Relat. 2008;11:201–14. doi: 10.1177/1368430207088038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.McEwen BS, Morrison JH. The brain on stress: vulnerability and plasticity of the prefrontal cortex over the life course. Neuron. 2013;79:16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Smieskova R, Fusar-Poli P, Aston J, et al. Insular volume abnormalities associated with different transition probabilities to psychosis. Psychol Med. 2012;42:1613–25. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.ENIGMA Clinical High Risk for Psychosis Working Group. Jalbrzikowski M, Hayes RA, et al. Association of Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging Measures With Psychosis Onset in Individuals at Clinical High Risk for Developing Psychosis: An ENIGMA Working Group Mega-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2021;78:753. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.0638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Bhugra D, Becker MA. Migration, cultural bereavement and cultural identity. World Psychiatry. 2005;4:18–24. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Ku BS, Addington J, Bearden CE, et al. Associations Between Childhood Area-Level Social Fragmentation Maladaptation to School and Social Functioning Among Healthy Youth and Those at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2023; In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material (31.1KB, docx)

Articles from Neuropsychopharmacology are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES