Abstract
Impulsive choice has enduring trait-like characteristics and is defined by preference for small immediate rewards over larger delayed ones. Importantly, it is a determining factor in the development and persistence of substance use disorder (SUD). Emerging evidence from human and animal studies suggests frontal cortical regions exert influence over striatal reward processing areas during decision-making in impulsive choice or delay discounting (DD) tasks. The goal of this study was to examine how these circuits are involved in decision-making in animals with defined trait impulsivity. To this end, we trained adolescent male rats to stable behavior on a DD procedure and then re-trained them in adulthood to assess trait-like, conserved impulsive choice across development. We then used chemogenetic tools to selectively and reversibly target corticostriatal projections during performance of the DD task. The prelimbic region of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) was injected with a viral vector expressing inhibitory designer receptors exclusively activated by designer drugs (Gi-DREADD), and then mPFC projections to the nucleus accumbens core (NAc) were selectively suppressed by intra-NAc administration of the Gi-DREADD actuator clozapine-n-oxide (CNO). Inactivation of the mPFC-NAc projection elicited a robust increase in impulsive choice in rats with lower vs. higher baseline impulsivity. This demonstrates a fundamental role for mPFC afferents to the NAc during choice impulsivity and suggests that maladaptive hypofrontality may underlie decreased executive control in animals with higher levels of choice impulsivity. Results such as these may have important implications for the pathophysiology and treatment of impulse control, SUDs, and related psychiatric disorders.
Subject terms: Reward, Motivation
Introduction
Humans and animals are required to make decisions between immediate and delayed rewards in order to survive (i.e., foraging theory). Deciding between rewards of different delays to receipt is referred to as intertemporal choice and is marked by various gradations of choice impulsivity, or the propensity to choose rewards available now over those available later. The process underlying impulsive choice is commonly referred to as delay discounting (DD), as individuals discount the value of rewards as a function of the delay to their delivery [1, 2]. For instance, most people would consider a free large pizza today more valuable than five free large pizzas in a year and a wide body of research demonstrates DD of rewards by both humans and animals [3, 4]. Excessive levels of DD, however, result in impulsive choices and are associated with several psychiatric disorders, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, impulse control disorders, and substance use disorder (SUD) [3, 5–9]. Further, choice impulsivity may directly contribute to the pathophysiology of these disorders (e.g., immediate drug use vs. family and job stability). For instance, steeper discounting as delays to reward increase predicts enhanced acquisition and escalation of cocaine self-administration in rats and is associated with greater severity of SUD symptoms and reduced success of recovery in humans [10–13]. Despite this, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying DD remain unclear.
DD has both trait-like (enduring) and state-like (unfixed) characteristics which may underlie both innate and acquired vulnerabilities to SUD [14, 15]. Among its trait-like characteristics, DD is heritable [16–18] and has robust test-retest reliability [14, 19–22], and subjects demonstrate similar levels of discounting across tasks [23, 24] and reinforcers [14, 25]. Conversely, DD is also readily altered by short-term manipulations, such as drug administration [26–28]. Even with the robust trait-like characteristics of DD, comparisons of DD across the lifespan have yielded mixed results. While several studies show a decline in DD as individuals age from adolescence to adulthood and from young adulthood to older adulthood [29–36], other research demonstrates no difference in DD across the lifespan [32, 34, 37–40], or even an increase in DD [41]. Further, very few reports test the same subjects across development, making it difficult to determine if DD is a stable lifelong trait within subjects and thus if DD phenotypes (e.g., high vs. low levels of choice impulsivity) convey similar risk to the development of SUD across life stages.
Human studies suggest a role for corticostriatal circuits in DD. For instance, corticostriatal activity is shown to be inversely related to choice impulsivity [42–45], with greater DD associated with reduced frontal cortical control over both ventral and dorsal striatum [46–48]. Yet animal research interrogating corticostriatal circuits during DD has not provided a clear conclusion. Several investigations show that lesions or pharmacological manipulation of the striatum disrupts DD in rodents [49–58]; in particular, lesions of the nucleus accumbens core (NAc), the accumbens subregion shown to be involved in processing delay to reinforcement, increase delay aversion [50, 59]. However, it is unclear how frontal cortical regions may influence striatal processing during DD. Human research demonstrates a role for both orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [60, 61] and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) [42, 45] in intertemporal choice. Notably, OFC is conserved in rodents, while human ACC aligns most with rodent medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) [62–64]. Rodent research illustrates that the role of OFC in DD is complex; showing either decreased DD following inactivation, increased DD after inactivation, differences based on subregion (i.e., medial versus lateral OFC), or no effect [65–71]. In rodent studies, DD is enhanced by prefrontal 6-OH-DA dopamine depletion in juvenile rats [72], pharmacological disruption of dopaminergic signaling in mPFC in adult rats [73], or viral over-expression of D1 receptors in mPFC [74]. However, while Churchwell and colleagues (2009) showed that pharmacological inactivation of mPFC increased impulsive choice on a single delay T-maze procedure, several studies show that complete lesions of the mPFC do not affect DD in rats during a multi-delay instrumental choice procedure [50, 75–77]. Therefore, while a wide body of research has observed cortical and striatal contributions to DD, a causal role of corticostriatal projections in control over impulsive choice has not been established.
Consequently, the current study had two main goals: (1) to assess DD in adolescence and adulthood in the same subjects to directly compare intertemporal choice across the lifespan and (2) to investigate the role of corticostriatal projections in trait-like DD. To accomplish this, adolescent male rats were trained daily on a DD task in which delay to the larger reinforcer increases across the session. Stable DD behavior was recorded in adolescence and again in adulthood to allow comparison of impulsive choice across developmental stages in the same animals and determine potential trait-like choice impulsivity. Following stable DD behavior in adulthood, rats received bilateral infusion of an inhibitory DREADD virus (Gi-DREADD) in the prelimbic region of the mPFC and bilateral guide cannulae aimed at the NAc. Systemic delivery of the Gi-DREADD actuator clozapine-n-oxide (CNO) allowed for the assessment of temporary mPFC inactivation on DD behavior, while intra-NAc infusion of CNO allowed for the interrogation of mPFC-NAc projections in impulsive choice.
Materials and methods
Animals
Subjects were male Long-Evans rats bred at the University of Maryland School of Medicine. Many studies investigating sex differences in intertemporal choice show that male and female rodents and humans exhibit similar levels of DD [78, 79], have conserved corticostriatal activity during task performance [47], and express indistinguishable test-retest reliability [41, 80]. Adolescent rats (PND 30–55) were group-housed, and adult rats (PND 55+) were pair-housed until stereotaxic surgery in adulthood (PND ~ 120–130); they were then singly housed to protect surgical implants. When littermates were used, they were distributed across experimental and control groups. All housing rooms were maintained at 24 °C and 40–50% humidity under a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 0700 h). Experimental procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at University of Maryland School of Medicine.
Apparatus
DD task
In adolescence and adulthood, food was removed from the homecage the evening before initiation of training or re-testing to facilitate acquisition of the task. Afterwards, daily food pellets earned on the task were supplemented up to 16 ± 2 g of chow after the session. This feeding schedule resulted in adolescent animals maintaining a typical free-feeding body weight throughout adolescence (based on feeding regimen by [29] as well as growth charts published by Charles River Laboratories) and adult animals maintaining 85 ± 5% free-feeding body weight.
Figure 1A illustrates the study timeline. During adolescence and early adulthood, rats were tested in 2 sessions per day. Following re-stabilization on the DD task after surgery, rats were thereafter tested once per day. Initially, adolescents underwent nosepoke training under a fixed ratio 1 (FR1) schedule of reinforcement during 45-min sessions; in addition to response-contingent pellets delivered by a nosepoke at either port, a single, noncontingent pellet was delivered every 5 min. Once nosepoking produced ≥50 reinforcers in both sessions, rats were trained to discriminate among two ports that produced 1 (“small”) vs. 3 (“large”) pellets under an FR1 schedule of reinforcement until they allocated ≥80% of responses to the large alternative port.
Rats then progressed to a within-session increasing delay procedure [2, 28]. Sessions consisted of 4 blocks of 2 sample trials followed by 12 choice trials (Fig. 1B). The start of each trial was signaled by houselight and active nosepoke port(s) light illumination. In the 2 sample trials, rats were exposed to both choice alternatives in random order. If the left port was signaled, a response in that port produced a single pellet immediately (i.e., small-immediate alternative). If the right port was signaled, a response in that port produced three pellets after a delay of T-s (i.e., large-delayed alternative) during which the stimulus light above the port flashed (2 Hz) until pellet delivery. Choice trials allowed rats to choose between the small-immediate and large-delayed alternatives. During the first block, the delay (T) to the large-delayed alternative was 0-s, and it increased across successive trial blocks (5, 10, and 20-s). Trials were separated by a 45-s compensating inter-trial interval (ITI) that ensured equal trial spacing regardless of the alternative chosen. Sessions terminated following completion of all trials or 60 min, whichever occurred first. Changes in delay gradient shape were quantified using the area-under-the-curve (AUC) method where a decrease in AUC indicated an increase in impulsive choice [81]. Stable discounting behavior (i.e., less than 10% change in AUC for 3 consecutive sessions with no successive increases or decreases in AUC across the 3 sessions) was averaged in adolescence (~PND 50–53) and again in adulthood following stereotaxic surgery but prior to chemogenetic testing (PND ~ 150–153) (n = 16).
Virus injection and cannula implantation surgery
Under isoflurane anesthesia, rats (n = 16) were bilaterally transduced with 0.5 µl/side of pAAV-CaMKIIa-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry (gift from Bryan Roth, University of North Carolina Vector Core, Chapel Hill, NC) [82] into the prelimbic region of mPFC (+2.7 AP, ±0.5 ML, −2.5 DV relative to skull surface at Bregma) to primarily target excitatory projection neurons [83]. Bilateral guide cannulae (26 G, Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) were targeted at the NAc (+1.3 AP, ±1.4 ML, −5 DV), and a stainless steel obdurator (33 G, Plastics One) was inserted in each cannula [83]. Projections of the prelimbic cortex project preferentially to the NAc core [84–86]. An additional group of animals was implanted with guide cannula (as above) but not transduced with virus in order to control for stereotaxic implantation and to examine the intrinsic pharmacological effects of CNO (NIDA Drug Supply Program, RTI International, Research Triangle Park, NC). Rats were administered topical antibiotic and analgesic ointment as well as injectable analgesics for 48 h following surgery and were allowed at least 7 days of recovery.
Systemic and intracranial pharmacology
Following surgery, rats were allowed to restabilize on the DD task. In separate sessions, the same group of rats underwent systemic (1.5–3.0 mg/kg, IP; n = 9) and intra-NAc (1.5 μg/side, IC; n = 16) injections of CNO or saline vehicle (n = 14). Doses of CNO and vehicle were counterbalanced between animals within each block. Two rats did not complete vehicle injections due to blocked IC cannulae. After each test session animals were given at least 3 sessions to re-establish stable discounting behavior. To determine if CNO infusion itself in the absence of Gi-DREADD transduction affects impulsive choice, a separate cohort of rats received bilateral cannula aimed at the NAc but no transfection with Gi-DREADD virus (n = 10). Once they achieved stable DD performance in adulthood, the effects of intra-NAc CNO were assessed. For IP injections, CNO or vehicle was delivered in a volume of 1 ml/kg 30 min before testing. For IC infusions, bilateral infusion cannulae (33 G, 1.5 mm projection) were connected to a 5 µl Hamilton syringe via PE50 tubing back-filled with CNO. Infusion cannulae were inserted through the guide cannulae 1 min prior to CNO delivery, and CNO was delivered in a volume of 0.5 µl/side over 2 min using a motorized syringe pump. Infusion cannulae were left in place for 5 min after infusion.
Histology
After behavioral experiments, animals were anesthetized with a fatal dose of pentobarbital and transcardially perfused. Brains were extracted, sectioned into 40 µm coronal sections, and viewed using a fluorescent confocal microscope to confirm hM4Di-mCherry expression in cell bodies of the mPFC and terminals in the NAc, as well as cannula placement in the NAc. Rats included in data analysis had viral expression confined to the prelimbic cortex [83] and bilateral NAc cannulae placement within a confined range of the targeted NAc coordinates (+/− 0.25 mm AP, +/− 0.3 mm ML, and + 0.25 mm DV) (Fig. 2).
Slice electrophysiology
To verify inhibitory function of the DREADD virus following CNO, a subset of rats were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane, and their brains were rapidly removed and submerged in ice-cold, modified artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF; in mM: 194 sucrose, 30 NaCl, 4.5 KCl, 1 MgCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 1.2 NaH2PO4, and 10 D-glucose). 250 µm thick coronal slices were made containing the NAc (Leica VT 1200 vibratome, Leica Biosystems, Deer Park, IL) and stored in 95% oxygen, 5% carbon dioxide (carbogen)-bubbled aCSF (in mM: 124 NaCl, 4.5 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 1 MgCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 1.2 NaH2PO4, and 10 D-glucose) at 32 °C for 30 min. Hemisected slices were placed in a recording chamber with aCSF containing 50 µM picrotoxin at 30 ± 1 °C. Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were made from NAc medium spiny neurons (MSNs) using boroscillate glass micropipettes (World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, FL) in the 2–5 MΩ range filled with a high CsMeSO3 internal solution (in mM: 120 CsMeSO3, 5 NaCl, 10 tetraethylammonium-Cl, 10 HEPES, 5 QX-314, 1.1 EGTA, 0.3 Na-GTP, and 4 Mg-ATP). Cells were voltage clamped at −60 mV using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices, San Jose, CA), and excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs) were evoked every 20 s using a concentric bipolar stimulating electrode (World Precision Instruments) located approximately 100 µm from the cell. Signals were filtered at 2 kHz and digitized at 10 kHz. Following a stable 5-min baseline period, aCSF containing either 10 µM CNO or vehicle was applied to the slices. Cells were discarded from analysis if the series resistance changed more than 15% throughout the recording.
Data analysis
Results
Delay discounting in adolescence and adulthood is positively correlated
To examine impulsive choice across development, performance on the DD task during adolescence and again in adulthood was assessed. Fig. 1C shows delay gradients for the same animals in adolescence and adulthood. A significant main effect of delay [F(3,45) = 207.207, P < .001, ηp2 = 0.932] illustrates that animals robustly discounted delayed rewards across adolescence and adulthood. Interestingly, animals exhibited greater discounting of delayed rewards in adulthood as compared to adolescence [main effect of developmental stage F(1,15) = 8.251, P = 0.012, ηp2 = 0.355; delay x developmental stage interaction F(3,45) = 3.321, P = 0.028, ηp2 = 0.181]. Post-hoc comparisons between groups at each delay showed that rats chose significantly fewer rewards at the 20-s delay [t(15) = 3.372, P = 0.004, Cohen’s d = 0.843] but exhibited similar levels of discounting at the other delays [corrected α = 0.0125; 0-s, t(15) = −0.019, P = 0.985, Cohen’s d = 0.005; 5-s, t(15) = 0.498, P = 0.626, Cohen’s d = 0.124; 10-s, t(15) = 2.578, P = 0.021, Cohen’s d = 0.644]. Paired-samples t-test revealed a significant difference between AUC in adolescence and adulthood [Fig. 1D; t(15) = 2.983, P = 0.009, Cohen’s d = 0.746]. Fig. 1E illustrates a significant positive correlation between delay gradient AUC in adolescence and adulthood [r = 0.85, P < 0.0001], evidencing that, although DD tends to increase as training continues into adulthood, each animal maintains relative stability of choice impulsivity across development.
Chemogenetic inactivation of mPFC does not affect impulsive choice
In adulthood, rats transduced with Gi-DREADD in the mPFC received systemic (IP) injection of vehicle and each of 2 doses of CNO to determine if DREADD-induced inactivation of mPFC altered impulsive choice. There was no significant change in DD task performance from baseline following IP vehicle or CNO at either dose [Fig. 3A; F(3,24) = 0.909, P = 0.452, ηp2 = 0.102], suggesting no effect of mPFC inactivation on impulsive choice. These data align with previous work showing no effect of mPFC lesion or inactivation on DD [50, 75, 76].
CNO-mediated inactivation of excitatory mPFC-NAc projections increases impulsive choice
In order to directly assess the role of excitatory projections from mPFC to NAc in impulsive choice, vehicle or CNO was microinfused bilaterally into the NAc to selectively inactivate mPFC afferents (Fig. 3B). During baseline and following intra-NAc vehicle, selection of the large-delayed reinforcer decreased as delays increased [main effect of delay F(3,39) = 75.258, P < 0.000, ηp2 = 0.853]. However, vehicle did not significantly alter impulsive choice at any delay relative to baseline [no main effect of treatment, F(1,13) = 0.026, P = 0.875, ηp2 = 0.002; no treatment x delay interaction, F(3,39) = 0.791, P = 0.507, ηp2 = 0.057] and did not affect AUC between sessions [Fig. 3C; t(13) = 0.489, P = 0.633, Cohen’s d = 0.131]. Interestingly, intra-NAc CNO robustly increased impulsive choice across the session, as shown by a decrease in AUC [Fig. 3E; t(15) = 3.578, P = 0.003, Cohen’s d = 0.895]. All animals decreased their choice of the large-delayed reinforcer as delays increased, and CNO similarly decreased choice of the large reinforcer at each delay (Fig. 3D) [main effect of delay, F(3,45) = 199.354, P < 0.000, ηp2 = 0.930]. However, administration of CNO significantly reduced choice of the large-delayed reinforcer compared to baseline [main effect of CNO, F(1,15) = 15.280, P = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.505; no delay x CNO interaction, F(3,45) = 2.742, P = 0.54, ηp2 = 0.155]. Critically, planned comparisons confirmed that CNO had no effect on choice of the large reinforcer at the 0 s delay [t(15) = 0.253, P = 0.507, Cohen’s d = 0.170], demonstrating no disruption in preference for the large-delayed reinforcer over the small-immediate reinforcer or the ability of animals to perform the task. There was also no difference observed following CNO treatment at the 5-s delay [corrected α = 0.0125, t(15) = 2.681, P = 0.017, Cohen’s d = 0.670]. However, at the two longest delays, CNO-induced inactivation of mPFC-NAc excitatory projections significantly reduced choice of the large-delayed reinforcer [corrected α = 0.0125; 10-s, t(15) = 2.846, P = 0.012, Cohen’s d = 0.711; 20-s, t(15) = 3.091, P = 0.007, Cohen’s d = 0.773]. Finally, CNO did not affect consumption of food pellets or time to complete the task (data not shown).
No-virus control animals exhibited normal DD gradients (Fig. 3F), decreasing their choice of the larger reinforcer as the delay increased [F(3,27) = 57.184, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.864]. CNO infusion had no effect on DD performance [no main effect of treatment, F(1,9) = 2.715, P = 0.134, ηp2 = 0.232; no difference in AUC, t(9) = 0.261, P = 0.800, Cohen’s d = 0.083], suggesting that CNO-induced increases in impulsive choice in experimental rats cannot be attributed to non-specific actions of intra-NAc CNO or its metabolites [87, 88].
Trait impulsivity determines degree of change in impulsive choice following silencing of PFC-NAc projections
A large body of research indicates stable subpopulations in impulsive choice and suggests differences in underlying neurobiology [89–92]. To explore individual differences in the contributions of the mPFC-NAc pathway to DD, the change in proportion of large-alternative choices following CNO was assessed as a function of trait level of impulsivity or proportion of large-alternative choices during baseline (the average of the three sessions preceding CNO test day) (Fig. 3G). Pearson’s correlation indicates that animals with lower levels of impulsivity, i.e., those with higher baseline choice of the large alternative at the 10-s and 20-s delays, exhibit larger decreases in these choices following intra-NAc CNO (5-s: r = −0.2830, R2 = 0.08009, P = 0.288; 10-s: r = −0.5745, R2 = 0.3300, P = 0.020; 20-s: r = −0.5664, R2 = 0.3208, P = 0.028). Additionally, constraint lines (Fig. 3G, black lines) indicate that the majority of animals did not reach a floor in their % large choices following intra-NAc CNO treatment. Altogether these data demonstrate that mPFC-NAc excitatory projections mediate impulse control during the DD task differentially in animals with higher vs. lower levels of trait choice impulsivity.
Ex vivo confirmation of Gi-DREADD expression and function
Electrically-evoked EPSCs were measured using whole-cell patch-clamp recordings from NAc MSNs and compared to baseline following vehicle and CNO (Fig. 4A, B) [main effect of treatment: F(2,22) = 11.288, P < 0.001, η2 = 0.530]. Post-hoc analyses showed that while EPSC amplitude following vehicle did not differ from baseline [t(12) = 1.026, P = 0.163, Cohen’s d = 0.548], EPSC amplitude was significantly reduced following CNO treatment when compared to both baseline and vehicle [corrected α = 0.0167; t(14) = 7.917, P < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 3.990; t(14) = 2.752, P = 0.008, Cohen’s d = 1.387]. To confirm a CNO-induced reduction in the probability of presynaptic vesicular release, paired-samples t-test revealed a significant reduction in PPR following CNO (Fig. 4C, D) [t(8) = 2.437, P = 0.0408, Cohen’s d = 0.812]. Altogether, these data support inhibitory function of CNO-mediated Gi-DREADD activation on mPFC afferents to NAc.
Discussion
We utilized a DD task to assess impulsive choice across development and to interrogate the functional role of the mPFC and its afferents to the NAc in impulsive decision-making. First, we found that DD performance in adolescence was positively correlated with performance in adulthood, suggesting relative phenotypic delay tolerance or aversion throughout the lifespan. Second, specific Gi-DREADD-mediated inactivation of mPFC-NAc projections, but not global inactivation of the mPFC, reversibly increased DD. Finally, increases in impulsive choice due to mPFC-NAc inactivation were negatively associated with characteristic baseline levels of delay discounting; namely, animals with lower levels of choice impulsivity were more affected by silencing mPFC input to the NAc than animals with higher levels. These data together suggest that phenotypic intertemporal choice may be, in part, mediated by mPFC input to the NAc.
Relative stability of DD from adolescence to adulthood aligns with a wide body of research demonstrating long-term, stable “trait-like” levels of impulsive choice in humans and animals [14, 19, 20, 41]. Although it is possible that DD behavior observed here is not the result of trait-like choice impulsivity but rather due to fixed patterns of responding on the task established through extended training, the propensity for DD behavior to become fixed or habitual over training is currently unexplored. Additionally, while our animals displayed relative stability over development (i.e., rats that were more/less impulsive during adolescence were more/less impulsive during adulthood), results did not support absolute stability across developmental stages (i.e., rats were overall less impulsive during adolescence than in adulthood). Previous direct comparisons of DD between adolescents and adults also yielded mixed results. While a number of studies show a tendency for adolescents to exhibit more impulsive choice [29, 30, 33, 34, 93], other research demonstrates no differences [34, 37–40]. Interestingly, McClure and colleagues (2014) found that rats in early adolescence (PND 28–42) were less impulsive than when tested again in the transition from late adolescence to early adulthood (PND 58–64) [41, 94–96]. Disparate findings among previous work and present findings may be attributable to individual differences between cohorts [97]. The use of a within-subjects design in the current study allowed for sensitive comparisons of DD across development to uncover an increase in impulsive choice in adulthood that would not have reached significance with a between-subjects design. Of course, it is possible that extended experience with the reinforcer altered its value, reducing choice of the large reinforcer at longer delays in adulthood. However, it is important to note that animals were motivated to earn reinforcers, completed each session, and consumed all earned pellets. Altogether, these data evidence relative trait-like levels of impulsive choice within subjects and provide a framework to investigate neural systems contributing to intertemporal choice.
Here we show that chemogenetic silencing of the mPFC using a Gi-DREADD under the CaMKII promoter had no effect on DD performance, replicating the results of previous lesion and pharmacological inactivation studies [50, 75, 76]. However, present results also indicate that Gi-DREADD-induced inhibition of the mPFC-NAc pathway reliably increases impulsive choice. Thus, while global inhibition of the CaMKII-expressing neurons of the prelimbic mPFC had no effect on DD, selective attenuation of afferents from the prelimbic cortex to the NAc core resulted in robust increases in DD. Other published studies demonstrate similar phenomena. For example, Giertler and colleagues (2003) showed that intra-NAc infusion of amphetamine significantly reduced reaction time in a conditioned lever release task, while temporary inactivation of the NAc with lidocaine had no effect on reaction times [98]. Similarly, disruption of dopamine signaling in the mPFC with intra-mPFC infusion of a D2 receptor agonist or antagonist is shown to increase impulsive choice, while the current study and others show no effect of mPFC inactivation [50, 75, 76]. There are at least two potential possibilities through which this may occur: (1) complete disruption of mPFC activity allows for other regions to control behavioral output, and (2) mPFC activity during DD promotes both impulsive and self-controlled choices through different efferent pathways and thus complete inhibition has no net effect on behavior. While the current study was not designed to test these hypotheses, previous research supports a combination of these possibilities. Indeed, brain regions other than mPFC are shown to similarly exert control over the NAc during DD. For instance, inhibition of OFC-NAc projections increases impulsive choice [99], suggesting an alternative pathway that may be relied upon to maintain self-control following mPFC inactivation. Also, while no published studies evidence an mPFC efferent pathway that promotes impulsive choice, Li and colleagues (2020) found that two parallel mPFC pathways mediate impulsive action in mice. They show that optogenetic inhibition of mPFC projections to the subthalamic nucleus (STN) severely impairs performance on a go/no-go task (i.e., increases impulsive action), whereas inactivation of mPFC projections to the lateral hypothalamus improves performance [100]. Thus, it remains a target for future research to determine how mPFC inactivation and mPFC-NAc pathway inactivation differentially affect DD.
Downstream of the cortex, minor projections of the prelimbic cortex target the accumbens shell subregion of the ventral striatum, but the vast majority preferentially target the accumbens core [84–86]. The core promotes learning and selection of delayed rewards [50, 101, 102], and its lesioning or inactivation results in steeper delay discounting [50, 103, 104]. Permanent lesions of the accumbens shell, however, have not affected intertemporal choice [103, 104]. Recent studies outline differentially patterned dopamine release in both the core and shell subregions during a DD task [78], and reversibly inactivating the core or the shell during a T-maze DD task resulted in waiting impairments [105]. Therefore, there may be a mediating role for the shell in addition to the core in DD. In the present study, we cannot entirely rule out inactivation of Gi-DREADD-expressing afferents targeting the shell from the prelimbic cortex. However, given that the majority of prelimbic afferents project to the core and only a proportion of all prelimbic afferents expressed Gi-DREADD, we believe our CNO injections targeting the core were acting upon terminals in this subregion. Dissecting the contributions of accumbens subregions in choice impulsivity is an opportunity for future investigation.
The observed increased preference for the small-immediate reward most likely reflects an intolerance to delay following mPFC-NAc inhibition. Intertemporal choice requires neural computations of reward size, delay duration, and preference for a small-immediate or large-delayed reward [106]. Here we show that inhibition did not affect preference for the large reward at the 0-s delay, suggesting that this pathway did not affect discrimination of reward size or preference for a larger reward when there was no delay to receipt. Preserved preference for the large reward at the 0-s delay also indicates that mPFC-NAc inhibition did not affect baseline motivation for food [107]. Additionally, rats completed all test sessions and ate all pellets, suggesting that motivation for food rewards remained intact across the test session. As delays progressed across the session, CNO decreased choice of the large reward to a similar degree at both the 10- and 20-s delay. Thus, it is possible that mPFC-NAc projections exert control over intolerance to longer delays, rather than intolerance to delay based on computations of reward value and delay length. Future research using additional delays or an adjusting delay procedure may provide further evidence of the precise role of this pathway. It is also possible that corticostriatal silencing reduced choice for the large-delayed reward by increasing subjective overestimation of the delay until reward, shifting the DD curve down at both 10-s and 20-s delays [108]. Humans and animals with poor interval timing also demonstrate greater impulsive choice [41, 106, 109–111]. However, rodent studies using lesions or temporary inactivation of mPFC or NAc do not support a causal role for these structures in time estimation required for DD [52, 112, 113]. Thus, these data align with previous research showing that both regions integrate information about reward size and delay to reward receipt [78, 114–118], and extend these findings to show that activity of projections from mPFC to NAc promote self-control during intertemporal choice.
mPFC-NAc inhibition significantly reduced choice of the large-delayed reward to a greater extent in rats with lower levels of baseline impulsivity, suggesting reliance on this pathway for control over DD may be stronger in animals with greater delay tolerance. In support of this, research demonstrates individual differences in DD in both humans and animals, with high and low impulsive subjects exhibiting differing structural and functional neurobiology [97, 119]. Neuroimaging studies show that individuals with high levels of impulsive choice have reduced corticostriatal functional connectivity and structural integrity of white matter compared to low impulsive individuals [46–48]. The current data align with these findings, highlighting a role for corticostriatal projections in DD and substantiate a mechanistic role for this projection in impulsive choice. Rodent studies have not specifically interrogated mPFC-NAc pathway during DD, although several studies have delineated neurobiological differences in both mPFC and NAc in animals differing in trait impulsivity. For instance, high impulsive rats have reduced D2 mRNA expression in prelimbic cortex and D2 receptor availability in the NAc [120–122], as well as blunted NAc dopamine release during DD [116] compared to low impulsive counterparts. Interestingly, similar dopaminergic dysfunction is observed in SUD, and it is hypothesized to result in decreased valuation of natural rewards (e.g., food) as well as increased impulsive choice through enhanced salience of immediate rewards [120, 123, 124]. The current data substantiate a role for mPFC-NAc projections in control over intertemporal decision-making and suggest that this pathway may be recruited to a greater degree in self-controlled versus impulsive individuals. Nevertheless, the current data cannot rule out the possibility that the observed greater increases in DD in rats exhibiting lower levels of impulsive choice at baseline may be due to greater parametric space for downshifts in discounting curves compared to animals exhibiting higher impulsive choice. However, the majority of animals did not reach a floor in their % large choices following CNO silencing of mPFC-NAc projections.
It is important to note that the DD behavior we analyze here may have been shaped by the design of the task, such as the use of cues or the order of delay presentation. Similarly, task design may have influenced the effects of neurobiological manipulations on behavior. In the current experiments, when rats responded for the delayed reward a cue light above the nosepoke port flashed at 2 Hz throughout the duration of the delay until reward delivery. Such delay-spanning cues are reported to enhance learning in a DD task, and with over-training they can begin to function as a conditioned reinforcer [65, 125, 126]. Zeeb and colleagues (2010) examined how the use of delay cues alters OFC involvement in DD. Briefly, they found that when the delay was cue-signaled, pharmacological inactivation of the OFC increased impulsive choice in low-impulsive animals; however, when the delay was unsignaled, OFC inactivation decreased impulsive choice in high-impulsive animals [65]. Cardinal et al. (2001) found no effects of mPFC lesions on DD using an unsignaled delay procedure, and here we replicated these results using a task in which the delay to reward was cue-signaled, suggesting that effects of mPFC inactivation may not be influenced by delay-spanning cues [50]. It remains a potential target for future research to determine how mPFC-NAc inactivation affects DD in an unsignaled DD procedure.
Like delay-spanning cues, the order in which delays are presented can also affect intertemporal choice. In our experiments, each session contained four blocks of trials with delay progressively increasing across blocks (i.e., ascending delays). Human research suggests that presenting delays in an ascending order results in greater discounting of rewards over delays as compared to descending order; however, behavior in both procedures is correlated [127, 128]. Rodent studies are mixed, with some reports showing differences in intertemporal choice as a function of the order of delay presentation [129, 130] and others showing no effect of delay order on behavior [131]. A number of rodent studies, however, do show that amphetamine administration, either systemically or directly into the NAc, decreases impulsive choice when delays are ascending but increases impulsive choice when delays are descending [54, 132, 133]. The authors of these studies argue that amphetamine reduces cognitive flexibility, biasing an animal to perseverate on their initial choice. When the initial choice was for a larger reinforcer at a short delay (as in ascending delays) choice across the session is biased towards choice of the larger reinforcer. If, in our investigation, chemogenetic inhibition of mPFC-NAc projections similarly reduced cognitive flexibility, we would assume this would decrease DD in our ascending procedure. However, instead we show greater discounting of the delayed reward. Thus, this suggests that decreased activity in this pathway results in reduced control over DD and greater impulsive choice.
Finally, a growing body of human and animal research presents conflicting data on sex differences in intertemporal choice [22, 134]. For instance, studies in human subjects either show that men exhibit greater control over delay discounting [135–138], that women exhibit greater control [139], or that both men and women discount delayed rewards at similar rates [139, 140]. Further, a recent meta-analysis of 28 papers shows no significant differences in delay DD between men and women [79]. Undoubtedly, in rodents, intertemporal choice has been more well-characterized in males, due to decades of using male rodents as “standard” in preclinical research. In studies that have compared DD behavior in male and female rodents, several studies do not substantiate sex differences in intertemporal choice [29, 78, 141]. Those studies that have identified sex differences in DD show that males are more impulsive than females when using a procedure in which delays are presented at random across the session [80], and females are more impulsive than males when using an adjusting delay procedure in which delays decrease after responses on the small/immediate lever and increase after responses on the large/delayed lever [142]. Further, evidence suggests that female rats express greater DD following amphetamine administration [141], and female rats bred to be low saccharin (LoS) preferring similarly exhibit greater DD than LoS male rats [142]. Thus, while sex differences may not be found at baseline levels of intertemporal choice, they may be unveiled by experimental manipulations and within subgroups of subjects. In the current study, we used male rats to examine baseline DD behavior across adolescence to adulthood and to determine a role for mPFC-NAc projections in intertemporal choice. We found that DD behavior was highly correlated in adolescence and adulthood evidencing relative stability and that mPFC-NAc inhibition increased impulsive choice in male rats. Based on previous research using similar within-session increasing delay procedures [78, 141] we expect we would observe similar effects in females; however, this remains an objective of future research.
In conclusion, here we show that impulsive choice assessed with DD (1) is conserved from adolescence to adulthood in rats and (2) requires mPFC-NAc pathway activation. These findings have clinical relevance as excessive DD is positively correlated with SUD, ADHD, and impulse control disorders [3, 5–9]. Further, mPFC, NAc, and projections between these structures are shown to be involved in these disorders. For instance, both ACC and NAc exhibit altered size and thickness in individuals with SUD [143], and reduced functional connectivity in ACC-NAc projections is correlated with greater relapse vulnerability [144]. Further, in rodents, optogenetic or chemogenetic silencing of the mPFC-NAc pathway attenuates both drug- and cue-primed reinstatement of drug seeking [145–147]. Therefore, these data provide integral knowledge for the understanding the neural underpinnings of DD and identify the mPFC-NAc pathway as a potential target for the treatment of excessive impulsive choice across the lifespan.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
The authors are grateful for the support and technical expertise of Iness Gildish, Autumn Bows, and Brian Donnellan.
Author contributions
JMW, NEZ, and JFC conceived of and designed the study; JMW, NEZ, VMA, HMD, and MHP carried out the experiments; JRS, LYZ, BNM, and JFC provided essential intellectual input on study design, analysis, and data interpretation; JMW, NEZ, and JRS analyzed the data; JMW and NEZ wrote the initial draft of the manuscript; all authors contributed to, edited, and approved of the final manuscript.
Funding
This work supported by National Institute on Drug Abuse grants R01 DA022340 and R01 DA042595 (JFC), F32 DA039690 (JMW), F32 DA043967 and K99 DA047419 (NEZ), and National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism grant F31 AA024683A (MHP).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Jennifer M. Wenzel, Natalie E. Zlebnik.
Contributor Information
Jennifer M. Wenzel, Email: jenwenzel@sandiego.edu
Natalie E. Zlebnik, Email: natalie.zlebnik@medsch.ucr.edu
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41386-023-01604-5.
References
- 1.Chung SH, Herrnstein RJ. Choice and delay of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1967;10:67–74. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1967.10-67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Evenden JL, Ryan CN. The pharmacology of impulsive behaviour in rats VI: the effects of ethanol and selective serotonergic drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 1999;146:413–21. doi: 10.1007/PL00005486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Lempert KM, Steinglass JE, Pinto A, Kable JW, Simpson HB. Can delay discounting deliver on the promise of RDoC? Psychol Med. 2019;49:190–99. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718001770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vanderveldt A, Oliveira L, Green L. Delay discounting: pigeon, rat, human–does it matter? J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2016;42:141–62. doi: 10.1037/xan0000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mitchell SH. Linking delay discounting and substance use disorders: genotypes and phenotypes. Perspect Behav Sci. 2019;42:419–32. doi: 10.1007/s40614-019-00218-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Amlung M, Marsden E, Holshausen K, Morris V, Patel H, Vedelago L, et al. Delay discounting as a transdiagnostic process in psychiatric disorders: a meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:1176–86. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1999;128:78–87. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mies GW, de Water E, Wiersema JR, Scheres A. Delay discounting of monetary gains and losses in adolescents with ADHD: contribution of delay aversion to choice. Child Neuropsychol. 2019;25:528–47. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2018.1508563. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Petry NM, Casarella T. Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999;56:25–32. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(99)00010-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Anker JJ, Perry JL, Gliddon LA, Carroll ME. Impulsivity predicts the escalation of cocaine self-administration in rats. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2009;93:343–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.05.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Athamneh LN, Freitas Lemos R, Basso JC, Tomlinson DC, Craft WH, Stein MD, et al. The phenotype of recovery II: The association between delay discounting, self-reported quality of life, and remission status among individuals in recovery from substance use disorders. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2022;30:59–72. doi: 10.1037/pha0000389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kraplin A, Hofler M, Pooseh S, Wolff M, Kronke KM, Goschke T, et al. Impulsive decision-making predicts the course of substance-related and addictive disorders. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2020;237:2709–24. doi: 10.1007/s00213-020-05567-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Perry JL, Larson EB, German JP, Madden GJ, Carroll ME. Impulsivity (delay discounting) as a predictor of acquisition of IV cocaine self-administration in female rats. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2005;178:193–201. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1994-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Haynes JM, Galizio A, Frye CCJ, Towse CC, Morrissey KN, Serang S, et al. Discounting of food and water in rats shows trait- and state-like characteristics. J Exp Anal Behav. 2021;115:495–509. doi: 10.1002/jeab.677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Odum AL, Baumann AAL. Delay discounting: state and trait variable. In: Madden G, Bickel W, editors. The behavioral and neurological science of discounting. Washington, DC: APA Books; 2010. p. 39–65.
- 16.Anokhin AP, Golosheykin S, Grant JD, Heath AC. Heritability of delay discounting in adolescence: a longitudinal twin study. Behav Genet. 2011;41:175–83. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9384-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Anokhin AP, Grant JD, Mulligan RC, Heath AC. The genetics of impulsivity: evidence for the heritability of delay discounting. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:887–94. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.10.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Isen JD, Sparks JC, Iacono WG. Predictive validity of delay discounting behavior in adolescence: a longitudinal twin study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;22:434–43. doi: 10.1037/a0037340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Odum AL. Delay discounting: trait variable? Behav Process. 2011;87:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Odum AL, Becker RJ, Haynes JM, Galizio A, Frye CCJ, Downey H, et al. Delay discounting of different outcomes: review and theory. J Exp Anal Behav. 2020;113:657–79. doi: 10.1002/jeab.589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Peterson JR, Hill CC, Kirkpatrick K. Measurement of impulsive choice in rats: same- and alternate-form test-retest reliability and temporal tracking. J Exp Anal Behav. 2015;103:166–79. doi: 10.1002/jeab.124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Weafer J, Baggott MJ, de Wit H. Test-retest reliability of behavioral measures of impulsive choice, impulsive action, and inattention. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013;21:475–81. doi: 10.1037/a0033659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Craig AR, Maxfield AD, Stein JS, Renda CR, Madden GJ. Do the adjusting-delay and increasing-delay tasks measure the same construct: delay discounting? Behav Pharm. 2014;25:306–15. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Epstein LH, Richards JB, Saad FG, Paluch RA, Roemmich JN, Lerman C. Comparison between two measures of delay discounting in smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2003;11:131–8. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.11.2.131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Madden GJ, Petry NM, Badger GJ, Bickel WK. Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and monetary rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1997;5:256–62. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.5.3.256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.de Wit H. Acute administration of d-amphetamine decreases impulsivity in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:813–25. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00343-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Diller JW, Saunders BT, Anderson KG. Effects of acute and repeated administration of caffeine on temporal discounting in rats. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2008;89:546–55. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Smethells JR, Carroll ME. Discrepant effects of acute cocaine on impulsive choice (delay discounting) in female rats during an increasing- and adjusting-delay procedure. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2015;232:2455–62. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-3874-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Doremus-Fitzwater TL, Barreto M, Spear LP. Age-related differences in impulsivity among adolescent and adult Sprague-Dawley rats. Behav Neurosci. 2012;126:735–41. doi: 10.1037/a0029697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Green L, Myerson J, Ostaszewski P. Discounting of delayed rewards across the life span: age differences in individual discounting functions. Behav Process. 1999;46:89–96. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(99)00021-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Leverett S, Garza C, Seaman K. The effect of delay duration on delay discounting across adulthood. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2022;77:467–71. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbab198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lukkes JL, Thompson BS, Freund N, Andersen SL. The developmental inter-relationships between activity, novelty preferences, and delay discounting in male and female rats. Dev Psychobiol. 2016;58:231–42. doi: 10.1002/dev.21368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Olson EA, Hooper CJ, Collins P, Luciana M. Adolescents’ performance on delay and probability discounting tasks: contributions of age, intelligence, executive functioning, and self-reported externalizing behavior. Pers Individ Dif. 2007;43:1886–97. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.06.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pinkston JW, Lamb RJ. Delay discounting in C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice: adolescent-limited and life-persistent patterns of impulsivity. Behav Neurosci. 2011;125:194–201. doi: 10.1037/a0022919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Roesch MR, Bryden DW, Cerri DH, Haney ZR, Schoenbaum G. Willingness to wait and altered encoding of time-discounted reward in the orbitofrontal cortex with normal aging. J Neurosci. 2012;32:5525–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0586-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Simon NW, LaSarge CL, Montgomery KS, Williams MT, Mendez IA, Setlow B, et al. Good things come to those who wait: attenuated discounting of delayed rewards in aged Fischer 344 rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2010;31:853–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hammerslag LR, Belagodu AP, Aladesuyi Arogundade OA, Karountzos AG, Guo Q, Galvez R, et al. Adolescent impulsivity as a sex-dependent and subtype-dependent predictor of impulsivity, alcohol drinking and dopamine D(2) receptor expression in adult rats. Addict Biol. 2019;24:193–205. doi: 10.1111/adb.12586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Macedo I, Fernandes C, Barbosa F, Marques-Teixeira J. Delay discounting in aging: the influence of cognitive and psychological variables. Behav Neurosci. 2022;136:392–403. doi: 10.1037/bne0000518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Robertson SH, Rasmussen EB. Effects of a cafeteria diet on delay discounting in adolescent and adult rats: alterations on dopaminergic sensitivity. J Psychopharmacol. 2017;31:1419–29. doi: 10.1177/0269881117735750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Steward T, Mestre-Bach G, Fernandez-Aranda F, Granero R, Perales JC, Navas JF, et al. Delay discounting and impulsivity traits in young and older gambling disorder patients. Addict Behav. 2017;71:96–103. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.McClure J, Podos J, Richardson HN. Isolating the delay component of impulsive choice in adolescent rats. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014;8:3. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Chen Z, Becker B, Qin P, Lei W, Chen J, Liu P, et al. Neural networks during delay discounting as trans-disease marker: a meta-analytical review. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;139:62–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science. 2004;306:503–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Peters J, Buchel C. Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron. 2010;66:138–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Schmaal L, Goudriaan AE, van der Meer J, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ. The association between cingulate cortex glutamate concentration and delay discounting is mediated by resting state functional connectivity. Brain Behav. 2012;2:553–62. doi: 10.1002/brb3.74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hampton WH, Alm KH, Venkatraman V, Nugiel T, Olson IR. Dissociable frontostriatal white matter connectivity underlies reward and motor impulsivity. NeuroImage. 2017;150:336–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Peper JS, Mandl RC, Braams BR, de Water E, Heijboer AC, Koolschijn PC, et al. Delay discounting and frontostriatal fiber tracts: a combined DTI and MTR study on impulsive choices in healthy young adults. Cereb Cortex. 2013;23:1695–702. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Wang Q, Lv C, He Q, Xue G. Dissociable fronto-striatal functional networks predict choice impulsivity. Brain Struct Funct. 2020;225:2377–86. doi: 10.1007/s00429-020-02128-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bezzina G, Cheung TH, Asgari K, Hampson CL, Body S, Bradshaw CM, et al. Effects of quinolinic acid-induced lesions of the nucleus accumbens core on inter-temporal choice: a quantitative analysis. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2007;195:71–84. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0882-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Cardinal RN, Pennicott DR, Sugathapala CL, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Impulsive choice induced in rats by lesions of the nucleus accumbens core. Science. 2001;292:2499–501. doi: 10.1126/science.1060818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.da Costa Araujo S, Body S, Hampson CL, Langley RW, Deakin JF, Anderson IM, et al. Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core on inter-temporal choice: further observations with an adjusting-delay procedure. Behav Brain Res. 2009;202:272–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.04.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Galtress T, Kirkpatrick K. The role of the nucleus accumbens core in impulsive choice, timing, and reward processing. Behav Neurosci. 2010;124:26–43. doi: 10.1037/a0018464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Moschak TM, Mitchell SH. Partial inactivation of nucleus accumbens core decreases delay discounting in rats without affecting sensitivity to delay or magnitude. Behav Brain Res. 2014;268:159–68. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.03.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Orsini CA, Mitchell MR, Heshmati SC, Shimp KG, Spurrell MS, Bizon JL, et al. Effects of nucleus accumbens amphetamine administration on performance in a delay discounting task. Behav Brain Res. 2017;321:130–36. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Steele CC, Peterson JR, Marshall AT, Stuebing SL, Kirkpatrick K. Nucleus accumbens core lesions induce sub-optimal choice and reduce sensitivity to magnitude and delay in impulsive choice tasks. Behav Brain Res. 2018;339:28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tedford SE, Persons AL, Napier TC. Dopaminergic lesions of the dorsolateral striatum in rats increase delay discounting in an impulsive choice task. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0122063. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Valencia-Torres L, Olarte-Sanchez CM, da Costa Araujo S, Body S, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E. Nucleus accumbens and delay discounting in rats: evidence from a new quantitative protocol for analysing inter-temporal choice. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2012;219:271–83. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2459-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Yates JR, Bardo MT. Effects of intra-accumbal administration of dopamine and ionotropic glutamate receptor drugs on delay discounting performance in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2017;131:392–405. doi: 10.1037/bne0000214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, Marbini HD, Toner AJ, Bussey TJ, Robbins TW, et al. Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behavior by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:566–87. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Zhang R, Chen Z, Xu T, Feng T. The neural basis underlying the relation between the action identification level and delay discounting: The medial and orbital frontal cortex functional connectivity with the precuneus. Int J Psychophysiol. 2021;159:74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Sellitto M, Ciaramelli E, di Pellegrino G. Myopic discounting of future rewards after medial orbitofrontal damage in humans. J Neurosci. 2010;30:16429–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2516-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Seamans JK, Lapish CC, Durstewitz D. Comparing the prefrontal cortex of rats and primates: insights from electrophysiology. Neurotox Res. 2008;14:249–62. doi: 10.1007/BF03033814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Laubach M, Amarante LM, Swanson K, White SR. What, if anything, is rodent prefrontal cortex? eNeuro. 2018;5:ENEURO.0315-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 64.Heilbronner SR, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Quirk GJ, Groenewegen HJ, Haber SN. Circuit-based corticostriatal homologies between rat and primate. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;80:509–21. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Zeeb FD, Floresco SB, Winstanley CA. Contributions of the orbitofrontal cortex to impulsive choice: interactions with basal levels of impulsivity, dopamine signalling, and reward-related cues. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2010;211:87–98. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-1871-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Winstanley CA, Theobald DE, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW. Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4718–22. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5606-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Stopper CM, Green EB, Floresco SB. Selective involvement by the medial orbitofrontal cortex in biasing risky, but not impulsive, choice. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:154–62. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Mobini S, Body S, Ho MY, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, Deakin JF, et al. Effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2002;160:290–8. doi: 10.1007/s00213-001-0983-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Mar AC, Walker AL, Theobald DE, Eagle DM, Robbins TW. Dissociable effects of lesions to orbitofrontal cortex subregions on impulsive choice in the rat. J Neurosci. 2011;31:6398–404. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6620-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kheramin S, Body S, Ho MY, Velázquez-Martinez DN, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, et al. Role of the orbital prefrontal cortex in choice between delayed and uncertain reinforcers: a quantitative analysis. Behav Process. 2003;64:239–50. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00142-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Jo S, Kim KU, Lee D, Jung MW. Effect of orbitofrontal cortex lesions on temporal discounting in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2013;245:22–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Freund N, MacGillivilray HT, Thompson BS, Lukkes JL, Stanis JJ, Brenhouse HC, et al. Sex-dependent changes in ADHD-like behaviors in juvenile rats following cortical dopamine depletion. Behav Brain Res. 2014;270:357–63. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.05.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Yates JR, Perry JL, Meyer AC, Gipson CD, Charnigo R, Bardo MT. Role of medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal monoamine transporters and receptors in performance in an adjusting delay discounting procedure. Brain Res. 2014;1574:26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Sonntag KC, Brenhouse HC, Freund N, Thompson BS, Puhl M, Andersen SL. Viral over-expression of D1 dopamine receptors in the prefrontal cortex increase high-risk behaviors in adults: comparison with adolescents. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2014;231:1615–26. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3399-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Feja M, Koch M. Ventral medial prefrontal cortex inactivation impairs impulse control but does not affect delay-discounting in rats. Behav Brain Res. 2014;264:230–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Deziel RA, Tasker RA. Effects of endothelin-induced prefrontal cortical lesions on delay discounting in the rat. Behav Neurosci. 2017;131:11–19. doi: 10.1037/bne0000179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Churchwell JC, Morris AM, Heurtelou NM, Kesner RP. Interactions between the prefrontal cortex and amygdala during delay discounting and reversal. Behav Neurosci. 2009;123:1185–96. doi: 10.1037/a0017734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Sackett DA, Moschak TM, Carelli RM. Prelimbic cortical neurons track preferred reward value and reflect impulsive choice during delay discounting behavior. J Neurosci. 2019;39:3108–18. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Doidge JL, Flora DB, Toplak ME. A meta-analytic review of sex differences on delay of gratification and temporal discounting tasks in ADHD and typically developing samples. J Atten Disord. 2021;25:540–61. doi: 10.1177/1087054718815588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Panfil K, Bailey C, Davis I, Mains A, Kirkpatrick K. A time-based intervention to treat impulsivity in male and female rats. Behav Brain Res. 2020;379:112316. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2019.112316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Myerson J, Green L, Warusawitharana M. Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. J Exp Anal Behav. 2001;76:235–43. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.76-235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Armbruster BN, Li X, Pausch MH, Herlitze S, Roth BL. Evolving the lock to fit the key to create a family of G protein-coupled receptors potently activated by an inert ligand. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:5163–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0700293104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 7th ed. London: Academic Press. 2013.
- 84.Voorn P, Vanderschuren LJ, Groenewegen HJ, Robbins TW, Pennartz CM. Putting a spin on the dorsal-ventral divide of the striatum. Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:468–74. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Gorelova N, Yang CR. The course of neural projection from the prefrontal cortex to the nucleus accumbens in the rat. Neuroscience. 1997;76:689–706. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(96)00380-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Montaron MF, Deniau JM, Menetrey A, Glowinski J, Thierry AM. Prefrontal cortex inputs of the nucleus accumbens-nigro-thalamic circuit. Neuroscience. 1996;71:371–82. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00455-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Roth BL. DREADDs for neuroscientists. Neuron. 2016;89:683–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Gomez JL, Bonaventura J, Lesniak W, Mathews WB, Sysa-Shah P, Rodriguez LA, et al. Chemogenetics revealed: DREADD occupancy and activation via converted clozapine. Science. 2017;357:503–07. doi: 10.1126/science.aan2475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Tian L, Liu X, Mei X, Cui R, Li X. The role of dopamine D1- and D2-like receptors related to muscarinic M1 receptors in impulsive choice in high-impulsive and low-impulsive rats. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2019;176:43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2018.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Moreno M, Azocar V, Verges A, Fuentealba JA. High impulsive choice is accompanied by an increase in dopamine release in rat dorsolateral striatum. Behav Brain Res. 2021;405:113199. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Marusich JA, Bardo MT. Differences in impulsivity on a delay-discounting task predict self-administration of a low unit dose of methylphenidate in rats. Behav Pharm. 2009;20:447–54. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e328330ad6d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Anker JJ, Zlebnik NE, Gliddon LA, Carroll ME. Performance under a Go/No-go task in rats selected for high and low impulsivity with a delay-discounting procedure. Behav Pharm. 2009;20:406–14. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3283305ea2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Stein JS, Johnson PS, Renda CR, Smits RR, Liston KJ, Shahan TA, et al. Early and prolonged exposure to reward delay: effects on impulsive choice and alcohol self-administration in male rats. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013;21:172–80. doi: 10.1037/a0031245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Spear LP. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000;24:417–63. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00014-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Sengupta P. The laboratory rat: relating its age with human’s. Int J Prev Med. 2013;4:624–30. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.McCutcheon JE, Marinelli M. Age matters. Eur J Neurosci. 2009;29:997–1014. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06648.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Keidel K, Rramani Q, Weber B, Murawski C, Ettinger U. Individual differences in intertemporal choice. Front Psychol. 2021;12:643670. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.643670. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Giertler C, Bohn I, Hauber W. The rat nucleus accumbens is involved in guiding of instrumental responses by stimuli predicting reward magnitude. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:1993–6. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02904.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Bezzina G, Body S, Cheung TH, Hampson CL, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E, et al. Effect of disconnecting the orbital prefrontal cortex from the nucleus accumbens core on inter-temporal choice behaviour: a quantitative analysis. Behav Brain Res. 2008;191:272–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.03.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Li B, Nguyen TP, Ma C, Dan Y. Inhibition of impulsive action by projection-defined prefrontal pyramidal neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2020;117:17278–87. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2000523117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Cardinal RN, Cheung TH. Nucleus accumbens core lesions retard instrumental learning and performance with delayed reinforcement in the rat. BMC Neurosci. 2005;6:9. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-6-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Cardinal RN, Parkinson JA, Lachenal G, Halkerston KM, Rudarakanchana N, Hall J, et al. Effects of selective excitotoxic lesions of the nucleus accumbens core, anterior cingulate cortex, and central nucleus of the amygdala on autoshaping performance in rats. Behav Neurosci. 2002;116:553–67. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.116.4.553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Basar K, Sesia T, Groenewegen H, Steinbusch HW, Visser-Vandewalle V, Temel Y. Nucleus accumbens and impulsivity. Prog Neurobiol. 2010;92:533–57. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Pothuizen HH, Jongen-Relo AL, Feldon J, Yee BK. Double dissociation of the effects of selective nucleus accumbens core and shell lesions on impulsive-choice behaviour and salience learning in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:2605–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Feja M, Hayn L, Koch M. Nucleus accumbens core and shell inactivation differentially affects impulsive behaviours in rats. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2014;54:31–42. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Peters J, Buchel C. The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15:227–39. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Kelley AE, Baldo BA, Pratt WE, Will MJ. Corticostriatal-hypothalamic circuitry and food motivation: integration of energy, action and reward. Physiol Behav. 2005;86:773–95. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.08.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Wittmann M, Paulus MP. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Baumann AA, Odum AL. Impulsivity, risk taking, and timing. Behav Process. 2012;90:408–14. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Marshall AT, Smith AP, Kirkpatrick K. Mechanisms of impulsive choice: I. Individual differences in interval timing and reward processing. J Exp Anal Behav. 2014;102:86–101. doi: 10.1002/jeab.88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.van den Broek MD, Bradshaw CM, Szabadi E. Performance of normal adults on the matching familiar figures test. Br J Clin Psychol. 1987;26:71–2. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1987.tb00730.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Buhusi CV, Reyes MB, Gathers CA, Oprisan SA, Buhusi M. Inactivation of the medial-prefrontal cortex impairs interval timing precision, but not timing accuracy or scalar timing in a peak-interval procedure in rats. Front Integr Neurosci. 2018;12:20. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2018.00020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Narayanan NS, Horst NK, Laubach M. Reversible inactivations of rat medial prefrontal cortex impair the ability to wait for a stimulus. Neuroscience. 2006;139:865–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.11.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Kim S, Hwang J, Lee D. Prefrontal coding of temporally discounted values during intertemporal choice. Neuron. 2008;59:161–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Kim S, Hwang J, Seo H, Lee D. Valuation of uncertain and delayed rewards in primate prefrontal cortex. Neural Netw. 2009;22:294–304. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2009.03.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Moschak TM, Carelli RM. Impulsive rats exhibit blunted dopamine release dynamics during a delay discounting task independent of cocaine history. eNeuro. 2017;4:ENEURO.0119-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 117.Roesch MR, Singh T, Brown PL, Mullins SE, Schoenbaum G. Ventral striatal neurons encode the value of the chosen action in rats deciding between differently delayed or sized rewards. J Neurosci. 2009;29:13365–76. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2572-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Saddoris MP, Sugam JA, Stuber GD, Witten IB, Deisseroth K, Carelli RM. Mesolimbic dopamine dynamically tracks, and is causally linked to, discrete aspects of value-based decision making. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77:903–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.10.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Galtress T, Garcia A, Kirkpatrick K. Individual differences in impulsive choice and timing in rats. J Exp Anal Behav. 2012;98:65–87. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2012.98-65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Barlow RL, Gorges M, Wearn A, Niessen HG, Kassubek J, Dalley JW, et al. Ventral striatal D2/3 receptor availability is associated with impulsive choice behavior as well as limbic corticostriatal connectivity. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018;21:705–15. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyy030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Laguna A, Lajud N, Juarez J, Sanz-Martin A. Chronic early-life stress increases cognitive impulsivity and D2 immunoreactivity in the nucleus accumbens of adult rats. Dev Psychobiol. 2022;64:e22259. doi: 10.1002/dev.22259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Simon NW, Beas BS, Montgomery KS, Haberman RP, Bizon JL, Setlow B. Prefrontal cortical-striatal dopamine receptor mRNA expression predicts distinct forms of impulsivity. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;37:1779–88. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Volkow ND, Wang GJ, Fowler JS, Telang F. Overlapping neuronal circuits in addiction and obesity: evidence of systems pathology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363:3191–200. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Thanos PK, Fowler JS. Similarity between obesity and drug addiction as assessed by neurofunctional imaging: a concept review. J Addict Dis. 2004;23:39–53. doi: 10.1300/J069v23n03_04. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Mazur JE. Choice, delay, probability, and conditioned reinforcement. Anim Learn Behav. 1997;25:131–47. doi: 10.3758/BF03199051. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Williams BA, Dunn R. Preference for conditioned reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1991;55:37–46. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1991.55-37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Robles E, Vargas PA, Bejarano R. Within-subject differences in degree of delay discounting as a function of order of presentation of hypothetical cash rewards. Behav Process. 2009;81:260–3. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2009.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Robles E, Vargas PA. Parameters of delay discounting assessment: number of trials, effort, and sequential effects. Behav Process. 2008;78:285–90. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2007.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Fox AT, Hand DJ, Reilly MP. Impulsive choice in a rodent model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Res. 2008;187:146–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Sjoberg E, Ottasen HM, Wilner RG, Johansen EB. Previous experience with delays affects delay discounting in animal model of ADHD. Behav Brain Funct. 2023;19:4. doi: 10.1186/s12993-022-00199-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Slezak JM, Anderson KG. Effects of variable training, signaled and unsignaled delays, and d-amphetamine on delay-discounting functions. Behav Pharm. 2009;20:424–36. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3283305ef9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Tanno T, Maguire DR, Henson C, France CP. Effects of amphetamine and methylphenidate on delay discounting in rats: interactions with order of delay presentation. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2014;231:85–95. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3209-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Maguire DR, Henson C, France CP. Effects of amphetamine on delay discounting in rats depend upon the manner in which delay is varied. Neuropharmacology. 2014;87:173–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2014.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Orsini CA, Setlow B. Sex differences in animal models of decision making. J Neurosci Res. 2017;95:260–69. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Logue AW, Anderson YD. Higher-education administrators: when the future does not make a difference. Psychol Sci. 2001;12:276–81. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Reynolds B, Richards JB, de Wit H. Acute-alcohol effects on the Experiential Discounting Task (EDT) and a question-based measure of delay discounting. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2006;83:194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Smith CL, Hantula DA. Methodological considerations in the study of delay discounting in intertemporal choice: a comparison of tasks and modes. Behav Res Methods. 2008;40:940–53. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.4.940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Beck RC, Triplett MF. Test-retest reliability of a group-administered paper-pencil measure of delay discounting. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2009;17:345–55. doi: 10.1037/a0017078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Kirby KN, Marakovic NN. Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon Bull Rev. 1996;3:100–4. doi: 10.3758/BF03210748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Mischel W, Underwood B. Instrumental ideation in delay of gratification. Child Dev. 1974;45:1083–8. doi: 10.2307/1128098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Eubig PA, Noe TE, Floresco SB, Sable JJ, Schantz SL. Sex differences in response to amphetamine in adult Long-Evans rats performing a delay-discounting task. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2014;118:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.12.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Perry JL, Nelson SE, Anderson MM, Morgan AD, Carroll ME. Impulsivity (delay discounting) for food and cocaine in male and female rats selectively bred for high and low saccharin intake. Pharm Biochem Behav. 2007;86:822–37. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Oh H, Gosnell S, Nguyen T, Tran T, Kosten TR, Salas R. Cingulate cortex structural alterations in substance use disorder psychiatric inpatients. Am J Addict. 2021;30:72–79. doi: 10.1111/ajad.13093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Camchong J, Macdonald AW, 3rd, Mueller BA, Nelson B, Specker S, Slaymaker V, et al. Changes in resting functional connectivity during abstinence in stimulant use disorder: a preliminary comparison of relapsers and abstainers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;139:145–51. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.03.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Kearns AM, Siemsen BM, Hopkins JL, Weber RA, Scofield MD, Peters J, et al. Chemogenetic inhibition of corticostriatal circuits reduces cued reinstatement of methamphetamine seeking. Addict Biol. 2022;27:e13097. doi: 10.1111/adb.13097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Kerstetter KA, Wunsch AM, Nakata KG, Donckels E, Neumaier JF, Ferguson SM. Corticostriatal afferents modulate responsiveness to psychostimulant drugs and drug-associated stimuli. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:1128–37. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Stefanik MT, Moussawi K, Kupchik YM, Smith KC, Miller RL, Huff ML, et al. Optogenetic inhibition of cocaine seeking in rats. Addict Biol. 2013;18:50–3. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00479.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.