Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2023 Aug 16;153:105361. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2023.105361

Toward objective characterizations of suicide risk: A narrative review of laboratory-based cognitive and behavioral tasks

Steven J Lamontagne a, Paloma K Zabala a, Carlos A Zarate Jr a, Elizabeth D Ballard a
PMCID: PMC10592047  NIHMSID: NIHMS1928381  PMID: 37595649

Abstract

Although suicide is a leading cause of preventable death worldwide, current prevention efforts have failed to substantively mitigate suicide risk. Suicide research has traditionally relied on subjective reports that may not accurately differentiate those at high versus minimal risk. This narrative review supports the inclusion of objective task-based measures in suicide research to complement existing subjective batteries. The article: 1) outlines risk factors proposed by contemporary theories of suicide and highlights recent empirical findings supporting these theories; 2) discusses ongoing challenges associated with current risk assessment tools and their ability to accurately evaluate risk factors; and 3) analyzes objective laboratory measures that can be implemented alongside traditional measures to enhance the precision of risk assessment. To illustrate the potential of these methods to improve our understanding of suicide risk, the article reviews how acute stress responses in a laboratory setting can be modeled, given that stress is a major precipitant for suicidal behavior. More precise risk assessment strategies can emerge if objective measures are implemented in conjunction with traditional subjective measures.

Keywords: suicide, objective measures, laboratory tasks, risk mitigation, stress response

1.0. Introduction

Suicide is a global public health concern and a leading cause of death worldwide, contributing to 700,000 deaths annually (World Health Organization, 2014). In the United States, suicide was among the top nine leading causes of death in 2021 for individuals aged 10 to 64, accounting for over 48,000 deaths (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2023). An even greater number of American adults have attempted or thought about suicide, with 1.7 million people attempting and 12.3 million seriously considering suicide in 2021 (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2022).

Although suicide is within the top 10 causes of preventable mortality worldwide (Naghavi, 2019), current prevention efforts have largely been unsuccessful. Such prevention strategies rely on our ability to understand suicidal states as well as identify those at highest risk for future attempts. Currently, most suicide research is conducted within the confines of subjective assessment, mostly through patient self-report questionnaires. Various issues surrounding the subjective reporting of suicidal states have recently generated concerns about the reliability and validity of such methods, particularly when used in isolation (Blanchard & Farber, 2020; Hom et al., 2017; Maple et al., 2020). In addition to these concerns, most of the knowledge about suicide risk factors is derived from subjective measures of suicide history. These measures, particularly single-item assessments that are commonly used in research and clinical practice, can lead to the misclassification of prior suicidal thoughts and behaviors (STBs) (Hom, Joiner, & Bernert, 2016). For example, self-report scales have generated poor reliability in differentiating low from high-risk individuals (Velupillai et al., 2019). Such lack of specificity likely arises from a multitude of risk factors in these models (e.g., psychiatric symptoms, age, sex) that pertain to many people with no history of STBs (Chan et al., 2016; Franklin et al., 2017). Thus, improvements in research methodology are urgently needed to better conceptualize suicide risk. In this context, the National Action Alliance for Suicide Prevention’s Research Prioritization Task Force (2014) outlined a series of aspirational goals necessary to advance suicide research. Three of the main objectives address the critical need to improve research methodology:

a. Characterize suicide risk using effective assessments:

“Determine the degree of suicide risk (e.g., imminent, near term, long-term) through feasible and effective screening and assessment approaches.”

b. Use reliable tools to identify high-risk patients:

“Distinguish suicidal ideation from imminent risk for suicidal behavior by developing and implementing reliable clinical tools.”

c. Model suicide precipitants in a laboratory setting:

“Know what leads to, or protects against, suicidal behavior and learn how to change those things to prevent suicide. Find clear targets for intervention through better understanding of the interplay among biological, psychological, and social risk factors, and understand how to model these effects scientifically.”

As the Task Force goals underscore, little progress has been made to advance these objectives despite several decades of research, necessitating a major shift in suicide research frameworks. This article offers a comprehensive narrative review examining each of the aforementioned action items and offering an analysis of recent research that has significantly advanced our understanding of suicide. In particular, the review: 1) examines suicide risk factors within the context of contemporary theories of suicide, with an emphasis on recent findings that support various fundamental elements within these theories, as well as current gaps in the literature; 2) reviews ongoing challenges and limitations associated with standard risk assessment tools; 3) analyzes objective laboratory-based measures of suicide risk, arguing that researchers should use these methods in conjunction with traditional self-report measures to enhance our understanding of suicide risk; and 4) highlights the prospect of modeling acute stress responses in suicide research laboratories, given that stress is one of the most consequential precipitants for STBs. Broadly, the review underscores the many challenges associated with risk assessment and invites future studies to implement new approaches that center around objective assessments of risk. In keeping with the Action Alliance mission, such strategies will help to characterize suicide risk, allowing for improved risk mitigation and prevention efforts.

Nuances in suicide-related terminology exist across the research field and in different parts of the world. For the purposes of this review, we delineate between suicidal ideation (i.e., thoughts about killing oneself with or without intent) and behaviors (i.e., intentional self-harm with suicidal intent). Unless otherwise stated, suicide attempts are characterized by any non-fatal suicidal behavior, distinct from nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) (i.e., intentional self-harm without suicidal intent). For a review on the distinction between suicide attempts and NSSI, see (Kapur et al., 2013).

2.0. Characterizing suicide risk: Who is most vulnerable and when?

The current state of suicide research methodology should be contemplated within a theoretical context that characterizes those at highest risk for STBs. Across several decades, theories have emerged to explain suicide risk in various contexts. Early theories of suicide implicated dysfunctional social cohesion (i.e., the strength of connectedness and solidarity among social groups) and social integration (i.e., a reliance on others to achieve fulfillment) as the predominant risk factors for STBs or death by suicide, with little consideration given to individual factors (Durkheim, 1897). According to Durkheim (1987), suicide can emerge from excessive social integration, whereby self-sacrifice preserves a greater societal good (i.e., the “altruistic suicide hypothesis”). Evolutionary accounts of suicide expand these ideas, viewing suicide as an adaptation under certain conditions that ensures the survival of larger communities and, ultimately, the species as a whole (e.g., self-sacrificial behavior in ants or bees to ensure survival of the colony) (De Catanzaro, 1986). More recent theories suggest that suicide in modern humans is a “derangement” of this evolutionary adaptation, representing a fundamental miscalculation that one’s death is of greater worth to others than their own life (e.g., a perception of emotional or financial burden) (Joiner et al., 2016). Other early theories defined suicide risk based on internally directed experiences, describing the suicidal state as a “psychache” (Shneidman, 1976, 1993), an “aversive self-awareness” (Baumeister, 1990), and a “symptom of hopelessness depression” (Abramson et al., 1989).

More recent, prevailing theories of suicide adopt a multifactorial approach that accounts for complex interactions between various risk factors proposed by these classic theories. These also include interactions with neurobiological correlates of suicide. As an example, social disconnection can initiate downstream biological consequences, like activation of inflammatory pathways (Slavich & Irwin, 2014), which has been linked to suicidal ideation and behavior (for a systematic review, see Serafini et al., 2020). Dysfunctional neurotransmitter systems are also associated with suicide risk, such as abnormalities in serotonin, norepinephrine, glutamate, and opioid receptor function (for a review, see Mann & Rizk, 2020). These factors are particularly relevant in the diathesis-stress model of suicide, which posits that suicidal behavior is a product of an interaction between proximal risk factors (e.g., acute stress exposure) and distal factors (e.g., neurobiological correlates of suicide-related traits). Recent conceptualizations of suicide risk delineate these complex dynamics along a continuum of risk that aims to characterize the transition from ideation to behavior.

In line with early theories, recent studies found that social and cultural factors can contribute to suicide risk (perhaps to a lesser extent compared to the presence of any mental disorder, see Favril et al., 2022), particularly in the way that individuals perceive themselves within broader contexts. Cultural risk factors account for wide demographic variability in suicide rates; these include gender role expectations, societal discrimination against race, access to lethal means, suicide-related stigmatization, and income inequality (Stack, 2000). These factors strongly influence the types of stressors that precede suicide attempts, as well as one’s likelihood of expressing STBs (Chu et al., 2010). Suicide risk is heightened among ethnic or sexual minority groups that experience acculturative stressors (e.g., discrimination, victimization, stereotyping), with even higher risk conferred among those who identify as sexual and ethnic minorities (Meyer et al., 2008; Walls et al., 2008). Social support can buffer these stressors, particularly an increased sense of belonging and connectedness with cultures of origin (Chu et al., 2010). According to the Interpersonal Theory of Suicide (Joiner, 2005; Van Orden et al., 2010), high suicide risk is defined by the co-occurrence of thwarted belongingness (i.e., an unmet need for social connectedness) and perceived burdensomeness (i.e., an unmet need for social competence, driven by the perception that one is a burden on others) in conjunction with both a desire and capability for suicide. In this model, capability can develop along a continuum from habituation to provocative experiences, self-injury, or pain, each of which can be invoked by repeated exposure to suicide, including family history or one’s own prior attempts, or by chronic stress exposure (e.g., military combat or childhood maltreatment). This theory is supported by several empirical studies linking thwarted belongingness and perceived burdensomeness to greater suicide risk (for a systematic review and meta-analysis, see Chu et al., 2017). Notably, however, these studies yielded small-to-moderate effects (Chu et al., 2017; Joiner et al., 2009).

Ideation-to-action frameworks center around multidimensional influences that increase an individual’s risk for suicide. These models attribute the propensity to act on suicidal thoughts to a variety of factors, including dispositional (e.g., genetics, low pain sensitivity), acquired (e.g., habituation to pain, fear, or death), and practical (e.g., access to lethal means) (Klonsky & May, 2015). A biopsychosocial model of suicide similarly integrates genetic, psychological, clinical, cultural/sociological, and environmental factors to determine risk (Lutz et al., 2017; Turecki et al., 2014, 2016, 2019). In particular, distal factors such as genetics or early life adversity are thought to predispose individuals to STBs, whereas proximal factors such as acute stress and substance use are thought to precipitate suicide attempts (Turecki et al., 2019). Critically, distal factors appear to be indirectly linked to suicide risk via intermediate phenotypes, like personality traits, negative affect, impulsive-aggression, or cognitive deficits (Turecki et al., 2014, 2016, 2019). The relative degree of suicide risk can also depend on interactions between demographic variables and the presence of specific endophenotypes related to suicide risk. As an example, impulsivity, aggression, and novelty-seeking are linked to a greater risk for suicide among younger individuals, but these factors become less important in determining risk with increasing age (McGirr et al., 2008). Notably, acute stress exposure interacts with existing diatheses (i.e., genetic or biological vulnerabilities) to increase one’s propensity to shift from suicide ideation to suicide attempt (O’Connor, 2011; O’Connor & Kirtley, 2018). In the Integrated Motivational-Volitional model of suicide, social and cultural factors can act like diatheses (O’Connor & Kirtley, 2018). For instance, socially prescribed perfectionism—a perception of unrealistically high expectations—is a vulnerability factor that might interact with acute social stressors, such as interpersonal conflicts, to increase suicide risk through a sense of defeat and heightened sensitivity to negative stimuli (O’Connor & Kirtley, 2018; Smith et al., 2018).

Interpersonal and multidimensional theories of suicide appear to yield large inter-individual risk variabilities. That is, the contribution of any combination of risk factors varies widely between individuals, impeding the ability to characterize individual risk. Furthermore, these models offer insight into which individuals are prone to STBs but less about when suicide is likely to occur or the likelihood that any individual will experience suicidal thoughts or attempt suicide at all. Suicide-specific diagnostic categories have recently been proposed to characterize the temporal dynamics of suicide risk, including: 1) Suicide Crisis Syndrome, which includes cognitive and affective responses to real or perceived threats, and 2) Acute Suicidal Affective Disturbance, which reflects interactions between acute psychological states (e.g., overarousal) and theory-driven predictors of suicide (e.g., social alienation) (Rogers et al., 2017). These diagnoses aim to identify those at high risk for imminent suicide crises by identifying the acute and rapidly increasing symptoms that precede STBs.

Alternatively, the fluid vulnerability theory of suicide (Rudd, 2006) characterizes temporal dynamics along a continuum of risk, positing that suicide follows a nonlinear time course along two dimensions: 1) chronic or stable properties that persist over time, like genetics or trauma exposure (i.e., a baseline dimension), and 2) dynamic properties, like cognition, emotion, behavior, and physiology that fluctuate in response to external forces (i.e., an acute dimension). Related to the aforementioned theories of suicide, stress plays a significant role in this model. Acute stress exposure is thought to unlock predisposing suicide-specific vulnerabilities (i.e., diatheses), with lower stress tolerance in those with a greater number of predispositions (Bryan et al., 2020). Because stress promotes deviation from a set point—one’s homeostatic point of equilibrium—stress-induced alterations within the acute dimension could lead to an imminent shift toward a high-risk state (Bryan et al., 2020). Indeed, several studies have reported temporal associations between the onset of an acute stress experience and suicide attempts or death by suicide (Feskanich et al., 2002; Gradus et al., 2010). This can be attributed to a failure of biological responses to acute stressors, increasing proximal risk for STBs (for a comprehensive review, see Miller & Prinstein, 2019). Typically, self-regulatory mechanisms and coping strategies will counteract emotional distress caused by acute stress exposure, thereby promoting homeostatic balance. In contrast, dysfunctional self-regulatory processes invite maladaptive responses to acute stressors (e.g., substance use) that can interact with various dynamic factors (e.g., low mood, social isolation) to induce an imminent suicide crisis (Bryan et al., 2020; Rudd, 2006).

Relatedly, the Cognitive Model of Suicidal Behavior proposes that stress activates suicide schemas such as hopelessness and perceptions of unbearability, leading to maladaptive cognitive processing (e.g., attentional fixation on suicide-related cues) that could immediately precede a suicide crisis (Wenzel & Beck, 2008). In this context, suicide attempts emerge when such cognitive-emotional dysregulation exceeds one’s threshold of tolerance (Wenzel & Beck, 2008). Although stressful life events across the lifespan can heighten suicide risk (McLaughlin et al., 2012; Serafini et al., 2015), various protective factors—for instance, connectedness (Klonsky & May, 2015) and social support (Turecki et al., 2019)—have been shown to mitigate risk.

Each of these theories offers important insights into the characteristics of suicide risk, but many of these constructs are not thoroughly captured by current research methods. In some cases, suicide risk factors are difficult or impossible to measure objectively. Nevertheless, objective measures have long been used to evaluate many risk factors described in contemporary theories of suicide, including cognitive or behavioral disposition. Subjective measures of suicide risk continue to be the gold standard in research, but many challenges with this approach have recently been discussed.

3.0. Current challenges with risk assessment

Accurate patient reporting is fundamental to our ability to identify which individuals are at highest risk for suicide. Unfortunately, self-reported and clinician-rated data can be unreliable for several reasons. Indeed, multiple intra- and inter-personal factors have been identified as significant barriers to the disclosure of STBs (Maple et al., 2020); as a key example, fear of involuntary hospitalization is among the most cited reasons for concealing or minimizing STBs (Blanchard & Farber, 2020; Hom et al., 2017; Ganzini et al., 2013). One study found that almost half of patients who conceal STBs in therapy would have responded more honestly without the threat of hospitalization (Blanchard & Farber, 2020). People with prior suicide attempts have also been subjected to stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination (Sheehan et al., 2016), discouraging the accurate reporting of suicidal states and deterring opportunities for therapeutic intervention. The internalization of these negative public perceptions, or self-stigma, could also prevent accurate disclosure of STBs (Ganzini et al., 2013).

Relying on self-reported experiences with suicide also invites concerns about the validity of participant accounts, even when participants respond truthfully. Explicit self-judgments are often not congruent with unconscious internal states, including affective or cognitive processes (Wilson, 2009), thus markedly diminishing the value of subjective measures of suicide risk1. Relatedly, participants might accurately report current suicidal states, but the fact that these can fluctuate rapidly over time may further complicate these efforts. For example, suicidal thoughts and intent might be absent during clinical assessment but become acutely activated during subsequent stress exposure (Nock et al., 2009). This requires rapid, in-the-moment assessments of transient changes in suicide risk, which current self-report inventories are not designed to address. Although ecological momentary assessments offer insights into important temporal dynamics associated with suicidal states, these measures do not resolve the aforementioned concerns about the reliability or validity of patient reporting; for further exploration of this issue, see (Ballard et al., 2021). Finally, limitations associated with risk assessment strategies and clinician training can impede the ability to adequately evaluate suicide risk, posing concerns about the validity of clinician-rated data (for a review of these challenges, see (Fowler, 2012)).

Patient and clinician reports continue to offer the best avenue for suicide prediction efforts. Despite several decades of research, however, very little progress has been made to predict suicide with clinically relevant accuracy (Franklin et al., 2017). Suicide prediction models rely heavily— and often exclusively—on explicit self-report measures that have relatively poor predictive value; for a systematic review of risk assessment scales and their predictive power, see (Chan et al., 2016). A logical conclusion emerging from these challenges is that suicide simply cannot be predicted with clinically meaningful accuracy (e.g., Mulder et al., 2016). This begs the question of whether this notion is valid or whether improved methodology might enhance predictive power within existing models. Recent findings suggest that using a composite approach that combines various sources of patient self-report and electronic health record data (Nock et al., 2022) may show promise. In one assessment of such models, the positive predictive value, while still relatively low (above 20%-30%), far exceeded that of any modality alone (Nock et al., 2022), suggesting that future efforts should focus on a multi-modal assessment of suicide risk for prediction. Currently, there is a dearth of robust evidence supporting the notion that objective measures, such as laboratory-based tasks, could enhance the predictive capacity of these models. Thus, it remains unknown whether a composite approach that includes task-based outcomes has the potential for clinical utility beyond a research setting.

4.0. Objective task-based measures to characterize and identify suicide risk

Self-insight, particularly self-knowledge about internal states, rarely converges with objective criteria such as behavioral patterns or prospective outcomes. Studies have found that the average correspondence between self-perception and laboratory behavior is low to moderate (Vazire & Carlson, 2010). Literature from social psychology suggests that self-evaluations of internal states must be corroborated with independent measures, namely implicit measures or nonverbal behavior (Wilson, 2004, 2009). In this context, behavioral tasks that examine suicide risk are imperative for assessing real-time objective responses in light of concerns associated with self-report. Towards this goal, laboratory-based tasks could easily be incorporated into clinical trials to characterize the continuum of suicide risk (Ballard et al., 2021). Below, we review suicide-specific laboratory tasks as well as tasks that assess suicide indirectly through relevant endophenotypes (for a summary, see Table 1).

Table 1.

Summary of objective task-based measures used to identify and characterize suicide risk.

Task Description Outcomes
Implicit Cognitions
 Suicide or Life-Death Implicit
 Association Task (S/LD-IAT)
  • Assesses the degree to which individuals self-associate with death- or suicide-related semantic stimuli. Faster reaction times for self-death associations reflect a stronger implicit bias toward death/suicide.

Attentional Biases
 Suicide Stroop Task
  • Measures attentional fixation on suicide-related semantic cues by assessing response latencies when naming the font color of words that represent suicide compared to negatively valanced or neutral words. Greater response latencies (i.e., slower reaction times) reflect a diminished ability to disengage from suicide- relevant stimuli.

  • Assesses attentional allocation to neutral, negative, or positive emotionally valanced stimuli via response latencies to words, images, or faces. Faster response times for congruent trials (i.e., when the probe and emotional stimulus are paired) indicate an attentional bias toward emotional as opposed to neutral stimuli.

  • Greater response latencies to suicide-related words in those with prior suicide attempts (Becker et al., 1999; Stewart et al., 2017; Tavakoli et al., 2021; Williams & Broadbent, 1986), particularly those with recent attempts (Cha et al., 2010).

  • Some have reported concerns surrounding this task’s ability to characterize suicide risk; for instance, the effect sizes are small (Richard-Devantoy et al., 2016a), the task performs close to chance in differentiating attempters from non-attempters (Wilson et al., 2019), and it might not outperform traditional self-report measures in characterizing risk (Chung & Jeglic, 2016).

  • Neural responses to angry faces differentiated suicide attempters from non-suicidal controls in several regions of the prefrontal cortex (Jollant et al., 2008).

  • Among attempters, an increase in the positive association between theta power and suicide ideation was observed for angry versus happy faces in the extended amygdala-hippocampal region; the opposite pattern was found in non-attempters (Gilbert et al., 2022). No behavioral effects emerged in this task (Gilbert et al., 2022).

  • Suicide ideators are more sensitive to negatively valanced stimuli than to positively valanced stimuli, as indexed by larger N1 amplitudes to fearful and sad faces (Lin et al., 2022).

 Dot Probe Task
Response Inhibition and Impulsivity
 Go/No-Go Task
  • Measures inhibition of prepotent responses through participants’ ability to respond to a designated target on a screen (Go trials) and to inhibit responding to an intermittent non-target (No-Go trials).

  • Measures the propensity to devalue temporally distant rewards as an index of impulsive decision-making. Greater discounting is a reflection of immediate versus delayed gratification.

 Delay Discounting Task
Interpersonal Difficulties
 Cyberball Task
  • Social exclusion is manipulated using a computerized ball-toss game. Participants experience either inclusion (i.e., the ball is equally distributed between the participant and other players) or exclusion (i.e., the ball is predominantly distributed between the other players, but not the participant).

  • Compared with non-suicidal clinical samples and healthy volunteers, suicide attempters showed decreased oxytocin levels during exclusion phases (Chu et al., 2020). Attempters reported no desire for emotional support following exclusion (Chu et al., 2020).

  • Attempters showed reduced posterior insular activity during exclusion relative to non-attempters and healthy volunteers (Olié et al., 2017).

Physiological Sensitivity
 Pain Sensitivity Tasks
  • Objectively measure pain sensitivity by administering cold water or mild electrical shocks to participants’ hands or fingers, with endurance time as the index of pain tolerance.

  • Measures aversion via electromyographic eye blink responses to a startle probe while viewing emotionally valanced images. Potentiated eye-blink response is a proxy measure of increased aversion to these stimuli.

  • Those with prior suicide attempts showed elevated pain tolerance relative to psychiatric controls and healthy volunteers (Orbach et al., 1996a,b, 1997), with exceptions (e.g., Rabasco & Andover, 2020).

  • Compared to patients hospitalized for accident-related injuries, those hospitalized for suicide attempts had significantly higher pain tolerance to electric shocks (Orbach et al., 1996a).

  • Perceived negative life stress was positively associated with pain tolerance in attempters, but the opposite pattern was found in accident victim and control groups (Orbach et al., 1996a).

  • Compared to suicide ideators and those who attempted suicide only once, those with multiple attempts showed greater startle potentiation to unpleasant images (Hazlett et al., 2016). Effects were not observed in response to equally arousing pleasant images or self-reported picture valence (Hazlett et al., 2016).

  • Attempters did not differ from suicide ideators or healthy volunteers in psychophysiological reactivity to suicide-specific images (Smith et al., 2010).

  • Elevated startle potentiation to predictable threats found in depressed inpatients with a prior suicide attempt relative to depressed inpatients with no history of suicide attempt (Ballard et al., 2014).

 Affectively Modulated Startle Reflex Task

4.1. Implicit cognitions about death or suicide

Mounting evidence supports a role for suicide or life-death implicit association tasks (hereafter, S/LD-IAT) in characterizing suicidal states beyond patient or clinician reports and well-known risk factors such as prior attempts or depression (Nock et al., 2010). Derived from social psychology (Greenwald et al., 1998), the S/LD-IAT assesses the degree to which individuals self-associate with death- or suicide-related semantic stimuli as a measure of implicit cognition about suicide or death (Nock et al., 2010). Specifically, the computer task evaluates differences in response latencies between the pairing of the self with life-related words (e.g., breathing, survive, alive) versus death-related words (e.g., lifeless, deceased, suicide) (Nock et al., 2010). Faster reaction times for self-death associations reflect a stronger implicit bias toward death/suicide (Nock et al., 2010).

Original findings by Nock and colleagues (2010) showed a significantly stronger self-death/suicide association among those with a prior suicide attempt compared with psychiatrically distressed individuals with no prior attempts. These findings have since been replicated in various samples (Barnes et al., 2017; Glenn et al., 2017). Self-associations with death/suicide have been found to prospectively predict a future suicide attempt (Barnes et al., 2017; Ellis et al., 2016; Glenn et al., 2017; Nock & Banaji, 2007; Nock et al., 2010; Tello et al., 2020). However, contrary findings suggest that the S/LD-IAT has limited long-term predictive capacity (Harrison et al., 2018; Rath et al., 2021). Nevertheless, in some studies, the predictive power of the S/LD-IAT exceeded that of traditional measures (Nock et al., 2010; Tello et al., 2020), suggesting it might be implemented alongside existing batteries in both research and clinical settings.

Recent studies have also examined the neural underpinnings associated with implicit associations with death or suicide in healthy volunteers. In a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) version of the task, greater activation was observed in the bilateral insula for self-death as compared with self-life associations (Ballard et al., 2019). This suggests that the salience network might be implicated in dissociating self-referential associations with death versus life. Relatedly, enhanced gamma oscillations were observed in the amygdala and anterior insula for self-death compared with self-life associations (Ballard, Gilbert et al., 2020). Gamma connectivity estimates among these regions also differentiated healthy volunteers from a small group of individuals (N=4) experiencing a suicide crisis, defined as attempt or ideation with intent over the prior two weeks. Future studies are needed to evaluate these differential activation patterns along the continuum of suicide risk with larger samples, thereby elucidating contrasts between healthy volunteers and at-risk groups.

Related paradigms such as the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (D-IRAP) (Barnes-Holmes et al., 2010; Hussey et al., 2016) and the Suicide Affect Misattribution Procedure (S-AMP) (Tucker et al., 2018) have demonstrated similar patterns of implicit death-related cognitions in those with a history of STBs (Hussey et al., 2016; Wells et al., 2020). However, these tasks have neither been widely implemented nor studied extensively in the context of suicide. In the S-AMP, participants with a history of suicide ideation (but no recent ideation) showed an implicit bias for suicide, suggesting that automatic associations with suicide possess trait-like characteristics that persist beyond resolution of suicide ideation (Wells et al., 2020). Major concerns surrounding the validity of AMP tasks have recently been raised (Cummins et al., 2020), suggesting that findings drawn from the S-AMP should be interpreted with caution.

4.2. Suicide-related attentional biases

As described previously, maladaptive cognitive processing could trigger a suicide crisis through attentional fixation on suicide-relevant information, as, for instance, in the Cognitive Model of Suicidal Behavior (Wenzel & Beck, 2008). Various cognitive tasks have been developed to characterize processing biases along the continuum of suicide risk. One prominent example is the Suicide Stroop Task, which measures participants’ response latencies when naming the font color of words that represent suicide (e.g., funeral, death) compared to negatively valanced (e.g., stupid, alone) or neutral (e.g., paper, pencil) words. Greater response latencies (i.e., slower reaction times) reflect a diminished ability to disengage from emotionally relevant stimuli (i.e., suicide-relevant information), which is thought to be driven by impaired top-down attentional control (Feng et al., 2018). Individuals with prior suicide attempts show greater response latencies to suicide-related words (Becker et al., 1999; Stewart et al., 2017; Tavakoli et al., 2021; Williams & Broadbent, 1986), particularly those with recent attempts (Cha et al., 2010). Other studies do not show this behavioral effect, reporting that the Stroop effect does not outperform traditional self-report measures that differentiate those with prior STBs from those with none (Chung & Jeglic, 2016). Of note, a recent meta-analysis compared 233 suicide attempters and 768 non-attempters and found a significant attentional bias to suicide-related—but not negatively valanced—words in suicide attempters, but effect sizes were small (Richard-Devantoy et al., 2016a). This is likely a consequence of the task’s low internal consistency, low concurrent validity, and poor classification accuracy (Wilson et al., 2019). Nevertheless, the Suicide Stroop Task performs close to chance in differentiating attempters from non-attempters (Wilson et al., 2019). Collectively, these findings suggest that the Suicide Stroop Task’s ability to characterize suicide risk, while promising, is somewhat mixed. More studies are needed to systematically analyze the value of this task beyond traditional subjective measures.

To a lesser extent, attentional biases toward emotional faces have also been studied in the context of suicide. Dot probe tasks are widely used to study emotional attentional biases in depression and accurately discriminate depressed from non-depressed samples (for a meta-analysis, see Peckham et al., 2010). In this task, response latencies to words, images, or faces are measured as indices of attentional allocation to either neutral, negative, or positive emotionally valanced stimuli (MacLeod et al., 1986). Faster response times for congruent trials—that is, when the probe and emotional stimulus are paired—indicate an attentional bias toward emotional as opposed to neutral stimuli. Although this task has rarely been implemented in the context of suicide, interesting neural patterns have emerged in recent studies. For instance, one fMRI study found that neural responses to angry faces differentiated suicide attempters from non-suicidal controls in several regions of the prefrontal cortex, reflecting reduced attention allocation to positively valanced stimuli (Jollant et al., 2008). An electrophysiology study found no behavioral differences between suicide attempters and non-attempters on dot probe reaction times, but groups showed differing patterns of neural responses to negative stimuli (Gilbert et al., 2022). Among attempters, an increase in the positive association between theta power and suicide ideation was observed for angry versus happy faces in the extended amygdala-hippocampal region; the opposite pattern was found in non-attempters (Gilbert et al., 2022). In another study, those with suicide ideation were found to be more sensitive to negatively valanced stimuli than to positively valanced stimuli, as indexed by larger N1 amplitudes to fearful and sad faces (Lin et al., 2022). Thus, the dot probe task might differentially characterize suicide risk on the basis of neural responses to negative emotional stimuli. Future studies should use functional imaging to validate this effect in larger samples. However, it should be noted that concerns have been raised about the reliability of dot probe tasks (Chapman et al., 2019; Price et al., 2015; Schmukle, 2005). For recent suggestions on how researchers might improve the reliability of the task—which may vary based on data analytic methods or sample characteristics (e.g., patients compared to healthy participants)—we refer the reader to a recent review by Price and colleagues (Price et al, 2015).

4.3. Response inhibition and impulsive decision-making

Deficits in impulse control are thought to underlie the transition from suicide ideation to suicide attempt, particularly impaired response inhibition (Klonsky & May, 2015). Several behavioral tasks have been used to characterize impulsivity along the continuum of suicide risk. The Go/No-Go task measures participants’ ability to respond to a designated target on a screen (Go trials) and to inhibit responding to an intermittent non-target (No-Go trials) (Gordon & Caramazza, 1982; Keilp et al., 2005). The stop signal task is similar, but participants must inhibit responding on Go trials when a “stop” signal emerges on the screen (Logan & Cowan, 1984). Both tasks measure inhibition of prepotent responses and have successfully differentiated suicide attempters from non-attempters. Specifically, attempters show higher false alarm rates (i.e., failure to inhibit responding to a No-Go target) (Richard-Devantoy et al., 2012, 2016b; Westheide et al., 2008). Furthermore, stop-signal reaction times differentiated impulsive attempters from both non-impulsive attempters and non-attempters (Wojnar et al., 2009). Self-reported trait impulsivity (Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; Barratt, 1959) did not differentiate these groups (Wojnar et al., 2009). This implies that these cognitive tasks are more sensitive to state impulsivity than self-report measures, which generally capture trait impulsivity. Furthermore, increased Go/No-Go miss rates—that is, failure to respond to a Go target—predicted subsequent suicide attempts among those with severe suicide ideation and past year attempts (Myers et al., 2022). In that study, computational modeling revealed no general slowing of stimulus encoding or motor responses, nor a response bias towards No-Go trials (Myers et al., 2022). This suggests that those who would soon attempt suicide had reduced decisional efficiency (Myers et al., 2022), which is a neurocognitive deficit linked to several clinical disorders (Weigard & Sripada, 2021). Thus, Go/No-Go miss rate might be an important indicator of proximal suicide risk. Corroborating this sentiment, past-week attempts were associated with greater commission errors on the Go/No-Go task compared to more distal attempts (>1 year prior) (Interian et al., 2020). Notably, these groups were not differentiated by traditional clinical assessments (e.g., prior attempts, suicide ideation, mood disturbances) (Interian et al., 2020), underscoring an added benefit to the objective measurement.

Impulsive decision-making is also associated with STBs, particularly suicide attempts. Delay Discounting tasks measure the propensity to devalue temporally distant rewards, even when long-term benefits outweigh immediate ones (Rachlin et al., 1991). In these tasks, impulsive decision-making is indexed by greater discounting as a reflection of immediate versus delayed gratification. High delay discounting has been associated with prior suicide attempts in several studies (Bryan & Bryan, 2021; Dombrovski et al., 2011; Liu et al., 2012; Mathias et al., 2011), with some exceptions (e.g., Bridge et al., 2015). In these contexts, delay discounting is thought to represent a decision to forgo all future rewards (e.g., prospective goals, prosperity, self-preservation) in favor of immediate relief (e.g., to eliminate severe distress). By this logic, delay discounting should characterize abrupt suicide attempts that involve minimal planning or forethought. In support of this notion, high delay discounting—defined as preference for immediate rewards—is associated with low-lethality attempts whereas low delay discounting (i.e., willingness to delay future rewards) is associated with high-lethality and planned suicide attempts (Dombrovski et al., 2011). As a screening tool, these tasks might identify those at highest risk for imminent suicide attempts, allowing for swift intervention during periods of ambivalence or acute crisis, such as experiencing major life stressors. Related tasks that evaluate risky decision-making, such as the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), have reported similar results, with suicide attempters demonstrating impaired decision-making compared to both clinical samples and healthy volunteers (Gorlyn et al., 2013; Jollant et al., 2005). As a caveat, IGT performance rarely correlates with cognitive abilities in other domains (Toplak et al., 2010). In fact, IGT decision-making was not correlated with self-reported impulsivity (Jollant et al., 2007) or performance on response inhibition tasks (Richard-Devantoy et al., 2013), suggesting these may be distinct processes. Instead, decision-making impairments in suicide attempters might reflect dysfunctional valuation of risk under conditions of uncertainty (Jollant et al., 2010). Nevertheless, performance on these tasks offers insight into decision-making characteristics that could identify at-risk subgroups, such as suicide attempters that used violent versus non-violent methods.

4.4. Interpersonal difficulties and physiological sensitivity: Desire and acquired capacity

As described above, the Interpersonal Theory of Suicide posits that social difficulties such as perceived burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness suffice to activate a desire for suicide (Joiner, 2005; Van Orden et al., 2010). Social exclusion can be measured objectively using the Cyberball game (Williams et al., 2000), which adapts a face-to-face paradigm of ostracism (Williams & Sommer, 1997). In this task, participants play a computerized ball-toss game with two confederate or simulated players where they experience either inclusion (i.e., the ball is equally distributed between the participant and other players) or exclusion (i.e., the ball is predominantly distributed between the other players, but not the participant). Ostracized participants subsequently report lower levels of belonging, self-esteem, and meaningful existence, as well as a reduced sense of need (Williams, Cheung, & Choi, 2000; Zadro et al., 2004). This effect is amplified in those with chronic depression (Seidl et al., 2020), but few studies have objectively manipulated social belonging in people at risk for suicide. Some studies using the Cyberball game have highlighted dissociable biological patterns depending on suicide risk. Compared with non-suicidal clinical samples and healthy volunteers, suicide attempters showed decreased oxytocin levels during exclusion phases (Chu et al., 2020), perhaps denoting maladaptive neurobiological responses to social rejection. Indeed, unlike control groups, attempters reported no desire for emotional support following exclusion (Chu et al., 2020), further implicating indifference to subsequent social connectedness. Interestingly, attempters also showed reduced posterior insular activity during exclusion relative to non-attempters and healthy volunteers (Olié et al., 2017). Given that this region is implicated in the neural basis for the “acquired capability” for suicide (Deshpande et al., 2016), future studies should evaluate its relevance to prospective suicide risk.

According to the Interpersonal Theory of Suicide, the desire for death only translates to suicidal behavior in the presence of “acquired capability” for suicide, particularly a heightened degree of pain insensitivity and fearlessness. Based on this notion, a lack of fear or aversion toward physical harm (e.g., elevated pain tolerance) increases risk for suicide attempts. Although interoceptive processing is extremely subjective, physiological pain sensitivity tasks offer objective insights into pain tolerance. In these tasks, cold water or mild electrical shocks are administered to participants’ hands or fingers, with endurance time as the index for pain tolerance. Relative to psychiatric controls and healthy volunteers, individuals with prior suicide attempts showed elevated pain tolerance (Orbach et al., 1996a,b, 1997) (for an exception, see Rabasco & Andover, 2020). Elevated pain thresholds have also been observed in individuals with borderline personality disorder, which is commonly associated with STBs (Ludäscher et al., 2007). One study found that, compared to individuals hospitalized for accident-related injuries, those hospitalized for suicide attempts had significantly higher pain tolerance to electric shocks (Orbach et al., 1996a). Furthermore, in that study, perceived negative life stress was positively associated with pain tolerance in the suicide attempt group, but the opposite pattern was found in the accident victim and control groups (Orbach et al., 1996a). These findings suggest that stress might interact with pain tolerance to determine suicide risk, but this has yet to be investigated prospectively. These findings might also reflect dissociative processes that alter pain perception in suicide attempters (Orbach et al., 1996b). This small body of literature offers an important empirical basis for acquired capability theories of suicide, but further replication in independent research groups is necessary.

Dysregulated fear responses are also implicated in the acquired capability for suicide. The affectively modulated startle reflex task is an objective measure of aversion that examines participants’ electromyographic eye blink responses to a startle probe while they view emotionally valanced images (Lang et al., 1990). Potentiated eye-blink response is a proxy measure of increased aversion to these stimuli. Although very few studies have used this paradigm to test suicide directly, emerging findings are worth considering. Compared to suicide ideators and those who attempted suicide only once, individuals with multiple attempts showed greater startle potentiation to unpleasant images (Hazlett et al., 2016). These individuals did not show higher startle reflex to equally arousing pleasant images, implicating potentiated arousal to negatively valanced stimuli rather than generalized heightened arousal (Hazlett et al., 2016). Critically, suicide ideators and attempters did not differ on self-reported picture valance (Hazlett et al., 2016), suggesting that this task evaluates emotional processing beyond subjective assessment. Others have found that attempters did not differ from suicide ideators or healthy volunteers in psychophysiological reactivity to suicide-specific images; however, self-reported fearlessness was highest among attempters (Smith et al., 2010). In other words, subjective accounts are more consistent with the acquired capability of suicide, specifically to reduced fear aversion and/or increased habituation to fearful stimuli with prior attempts. Thus, it is possible that introspective self-insights surrounding fear more accurately reflect the principles of Interpersonal Theory than psychophysiological responses. Furthermore, similar tasks have measured startle potentiation to both uncertain and predictable threats (NPU-threat task; Schmitz & Grillon, 2012). Depressed inpatients with a prior suicide attempt showed elevated startle potentiation to predictable, but not unpredictable, threats relative to depressed inpatients with no history of suicide attempt (Ballard et al., 2014). This suggests that distinct mechanisms might underlie suicide ideation, given that current and lifetime ideation is positively associated with startle potentiation to uncertain threats (Lieberman et al., 2020). Future studies should examine the precise neural underpinnings associated with these diverging response patterns in suicide risk.

5.0. Modeling suicide precipitants in a laboratory setting: Stress manipulations in suicide research as a future direction

Stress is ubiquitous in various theories of suicide, often mediating the effect of well-known risk factors on STBs (Cohen et al., 2022). As outlined above, contemporary theories differ in how they describe stress as a risk factor for suicide, as well as the types of stressors that increase risk (e.g., interpersonal, psychosocial, physical). Across theories, however, stress emerges as a major precipitant in the development of suicide ideation and impacts the relationship between ideation and action. Specifically, stress is thought to interact with temporal dynamics along the continuum of suicide risk, governing the likelihood and imminence of a suicide crisis. In this context, stress, as well as individual coping mechanisms in response to stressors (see Okechukwu et al., 2022), can powerfully influence one’s capability for suicide and shape the progression from suicide ideation to attempt. Numerous studies have reported associations between life stress and suicide ideation or attempts (e.g., Coope et al., 2015; Feskanich et al., 2002; Gradus et al., 2010; Liu et al., 2019; Stewart et al., 2019; Wang et al., 2015). This extensive body of literature is beyond the scope of this review; however, several limitations that impede causal inferences about stress can be attributed to concerns raised here. For instance, these studies can be prone to recall bias because they rely on retrospective reports of prior stressful events, which can range from recent to a very distant past. The effects of these subjective accounts are then evaluated using traditional self-reported measures of suicide risk. Recent studies have directly manipulated stress responses in a laboratory setting to characterize associations between stress reactivity, psychological traits, and suicide risk (e.g., Alacreu-Crespo et al., 2022). Few studies, however, have tested whether abnormal stress responses alter objective suicide risk. Because stress mediates the relationship between common risk factors (e.g., depression) and suicide attempts (Cohen et al., 2022; Merida-Lopez et al., 2018; Moskowitz et al., 2013), information about stress response could improve our understanding of dynamic fluctuations in suicide risk over time.

5.1. Examining stress-related effects on suicide risk using objective tasks: Does acute stress alter implicit or attentional biases toward death- or suicide-related cues?

As described previously, implicit biases toward suicide or death currently offer the most robust objective markers for characterizing suicide risk. Accordingly, future studies could investigate the real-time effects of acute stress induction on various laboratory-based paradigms that evaluate cognitive biases toward suicide-related cues. Although suicide-specific studies are lacking, some evidence exists for acute laboratory-based stress in modulating implicit and attentional biases. Acute psychological stress, for instance, increases the strength of implicit self-associations with anxiety in an implicit association task (Sato & Kawahara, 2012). Of note, this effect might vary depending on the type of stressor, given that a different psychosocial stressor did not elicit the same effect (Schmukle & Egloff, 2004). Relatedly, acute physical stress increased attentional bias to negatively valanced stimuli in a dot probe task, particularly among trauma-exposed individuals (Blekić et al., 2021). Although these methods require replication in suicide research, the study suggests that those with prior traumatic experiences might be more susceptible to stress-induced attentional biases toward negative or threatening stimuli. These findings are also in line with longitudinal studies showing associations between war exposure and attentional biases toward threat stimuli (Wald et al., 2013). Physical stress was also found to differentially affect men and women’s attentional biases in a non-clinical sample, with greater post-stress biases toward threatening faces in women than men (Carr et al., 2016). Other forms of stress have produced opposing patterns, whereby attentional biases to threatening faces were suppressed after acute psychosocial stress (Jiang et al., 2017), in contrast to the aforementioned effects observed after physical stress. As described above, those with prior STBs showed altered physical responses such as pain insensitivity (Orbach et al., 1996a, b), as well as interpersonal difficulties such as maladaptive responses to social rejection (Chu et al., 2020). It is therefore possible that stress could increase suicide risk via different mechanisms based on stressor type—for instance, reactivity to physical versus psychosocial stressors.

Evidence also suggests that acute stress increases attentional responses to self-relevant stimuli. In heavy alcohol consumers, for example, a laboratory psychosocial stressor increased attentional bias for alcohol-related cues and enhanced subjective alcohol cravings (Field & Powell, 2007). Interestingly, the effect was specific to those who used alcohol as a coping mechanism (Field & Powell, 2007). Given that STBs are thought to reflect maladaptive coping strategies in the face of distress (Bazrafshan et al., 2014), future studies should examine whether acute stress similarly enhances biases toward suicide-related cues in at-risk individuals. This would help identify which individuals are at greatest risk for a proximal suicide crisis. A similar psychological stressor increased neural responses to smoking cues among smokers, mainly in regions involved with cognitive control (Dagher et al., 2009). This suggests that acute stress enhances the salience of self-relevant cues, which might translate to those with prior STBs to the extent that they self-identify with suicide-related cues. In these contexts, difficulties disengaging from aversive stimuli might reflect stress-induced perturbations within brain regions that subserve attentional control. Indeed, stress impedes top-down regulation of subcortical structures such as the amygdala, leading to a reactive and reflexive state that could promote maladaptive cognitive processing and irrational behaviors (Datta & Arnsten, 2019).

By elucidating the mechanisms underlying the relationship between stress and implicit or attentional biases, researchers could better understand the characteristics of dynamic suicide risk. Studies could also evaluate how acute stress interacts with existing traits or diatheses to influence maladaptive cognitions linked to suicide risk. For instance, recent studies found that interactions between acute physical stress and trait impulsivity influenced choice behavior in a decision-making task (Raio et al., 2020). Another study found that acute psychosocial stress interacted with state rumination in biasing attention toward emotional information (LeMoult et al., 2013). Future studies might examine the indirect effects of acute stress on these objective measures. For instance, stress-induced inflammation is linked to attentional biases toward negative information (for a review, see Maydych, 2019), and both are associated with elevated suicide risk. Negative preconscious attentional biases can also predict cortisol stress reactivity patterns (Fox et al., 2010). As an example, attentional biases toward negative stimuli in a visual probe task predicted cortisol responsivity to subsequent psychosocial stress, but self-reported depression, anxiety, and neuroticism did not (Fox et al., 2010). These findings implicate bidirectional associations between biological stress responses and suicide-related risk factors. Although these research questions are interesting, they have yet to be investigated in the context of suicide and require further consideration in that context.

6.0. Conclusion

Suicide research is evolving alongside novel methods that capture rapid changes in risk (Ballard et al., 2021). Traditional approaches to risk assessment, which largely include self-report scales, have generated few advances over several decades of research (Franklin et al., 2017). In accordance with the research agenda proposed by the Action Alliance (2014), the current review offers promising new directions for suicide research. By objectively evaluating suicide risk, future studies will improve the precision of risk categorization. With this approach, we can better understand how precipitating factors—like stress exposure—interact with well-known risk factors to determine one’s likelihood for suicide, as well as the imminence of a future attempt. Collectively, this narrative review highlights a need for a fundamental shift in suicide research methodology. Although it may not substitute for traditional research methods such as subjective reports, complementary objective methods are likely to provide more precise risk assessment, ultimately yielding more successful efforts to mitigate suicide risk.

Highlights.

  • Suicide research relies primarily on subjective reports, which have been unreliable

  • Laboratory tasks can complement self-report to enhance precision of risk assessment

  • Existing tasks show promising directions for objective characterizations of risk

  • Future studies might use stress paradigms to model suicide precipitants in the lab

  • A multimodal approach to risk assessment can improve suicide prevention strategies

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the 7SE research unit and staff for their support. Ioline Henter (NIMH) provided invaluable editorial assistance.

Funding

Funding for this work was provided by the Intramural Research Program at the National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health (IRP-NIMH-NIH; ZIAMH002927). The NIMH had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication. This work was completed as part of the authors’ official duties as Government employees. The views expressed do not necessarily reflect the views of the NIH, the Department of Health and Human Services, or the United States Government.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Declaration of Interest

Dr. Zarate is listed as a co-inventor on a patent for the use of ketamine in major depression and suicidal ideation; as a co-inventor on a patent for the use of (2R,6R)-hydroxynorketamine, (S)-dehydronorketamine, and other stereoisomeric dehydroxylated and hydroxylated metabolites of (R,S)-ketamine metabolites in the treatment of depression and neuropathic pain; and as a co-inventor on a patent application for the use of (2R,6R)-hydroxynorketamine and (2S,6S)-hydroxynorketamine in the treatment of depression, anxiety, anhedonia, suicidal ideation, and post-traumatic stress disorders. He has assigned his patent rights to the U.S. government but will share a percentage of any royalties that may be received by the government. All other authors have no conflict of interest to disclose, financial or otherwise.

1

As a caveat, some have argued that inaccurate explicit measures of internal states do not reflect unconscious or inaccessible processes but, instead, reflect peoples’ tendency to over- or under-present themselves explicitly (Olson et al., 2007). In support of this notion, implicit and explicit self-evaluations converge when people make rapid explicit responses, thus restricting the influence of conscious self-reflection (Koole et al., 2001). While this may explain the discrepancy between explicit and implicit measures, it does not rectify issues surrounding the reliability of self-reported data during risk assessment – that is, the accurate reporting of internal states.

References

  1. Abramson LY, Alloy LB, Hogan ME, Whitehouse WG, Gibb BE, Hankin BL, & Cornette MM (2002). The hopelessness theory of suicidality. In Suicide Science (pp. 17–32). Springer, Boston, MA. [Google Scholar]
  2. Abramson LY, Metalsky GI, & Alloy LB (1989). Hopelessness depression: A theory-based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96(2), 358. [Google Scholar]
  3. Alacreu-Crespo A, Guillaume S, Sénèque M, Olié E, & Courtet P (2020). Cognitive modelling to assess decision-making impairments in patients with current depression and with/without suicide history. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 50–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alacreu-Crespo A, Hidalgo V, Girod C, Olié E, & Courtet P (2022). The impulsiveness level influences the salivary cortisol response and social stress sensitivity in suicidal patients. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 156, 159–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ballard ED, Gilbert JR, Fields JS, Nugent AC, & Zarate CA Jr (2020). Network changes in insula and amygdala connectivity accompany implicit suicidal associations. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 577628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ballard ED, Gilbert JR, Wusinich C, & Zarate CA Jr (2021). New methods for assessing rapid changes in suicide risk. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 598434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Ballard ED, Ionescu DF, Voort JLV, Slonena EE, Franco-Chaves JA, Zarate CA Jr, & Grillon C (2014). Increased fear-potentiated startle in major depressive disorder patients with lifetime history of suicide attempt. Journal of Affective Disorders, 162, 34–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Ballard ED, Reed JL, Szczepanik J, Evans JW, Yarrington JS, Dickstein DP, … & Zarate CA Jr (2019). Functional imaging of the implicit association of the self with life and death. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 49(6), 1600–1608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barnes-Holmes D, Barnes-Holmes Y, Stewart I, & Boles S (2010). A sketch of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and the Relational Elaboration and Coherence (REC) model. The Psychological Record, 60(3), 527–542. [Google Scholar]
  10. Barnes SM, Bahraini NH, Forster JE, Stearns-Yoder KA, Hostetter TA, Smith G, … & Nock MK (2017). Moving beyond self-report: Implicit associations about death/life prospectively predict suicidal behavior among veterans. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 47(1), 67–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barratt ES (1959). Anxiety and impulsiveness related to psychomotor efficiency. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 9(3), 191–198. [Google Scholar]
  12. Baumeister RF (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychological Review, 97(1), 90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bazrafshan MR, Jahangir F, Mansouri A, & Kashfi SH (2014). Coping strategies in people attempting suicide. International Journal of High Risk Behaviors & Addiction, 3(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Beck AT, & Steer RA (1991). Manual for the Beck scale for suicide ideation. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  15. Beck AT, Kovacs M, & Weissman A (1979). Assessment of suicidal intention: the Scale for Suicide Ideation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47(2), 343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, & Erbaugh J (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4(6), 561–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Becker ES, Strohbach D, & Rinck M (1999). A specific attentional bias in suicide attempters. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187(12), 730–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Belsher BE, Smolenski DJ, Pruitt LD, Bush NE, Beech EH, Workman DE, … & Skopp NA (2019). Prediction models for suicide attempts and deaths: a systematic review and simulation. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(6), 642–651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Blanchard M, & Farber BA (2020). “It is never okay to talk about suicide”: Patients’ reasons for concealing suicidal ideation in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 30(1), 124–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Blekić W, Rossignol M, Wauthia E, & Felmingham KL (2021). Influence of acute stress on attentional bias toward threat: How a previous trauma exposure disrupts threat apprehension. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 170, 20–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bridge JA, Reynolds B, McBee-Strayer SM, Sheftall AH, Ackerman J, Stevens J, … & Brent DA (2015). Impulsive aggression, delay discounting, and adolescent suicide attempts: effects of current psychotropic medication use and family history of suicidal behavior. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 25(2), 114–123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Bryan CJ, & Bryan AO (2021). Delayed reward discounting and increased risk for suicide attempts among US adults with probable PTSD. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 81, 102414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bryan CJ, Butner JE, May AM, Rugo KF, Harris JA, Oakey DN, … & Bryan AO (2020). Nonlinear change processes and the emergence of suicidal behavior: A conceptual model based on the fluid vulnerability theory of suicide. New Ideas in Psychology, 57, 100758. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carr AR, Scully A, Webb M, & Felmingham KL (2016). Gender differences in salivary alpha-amylase and attentional bias towards negative facial expressions following acute stress induction. Cognition and Emotion, 30(2), 315–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Carter G, Milner A, McGill K, Pirkis J, Kapur N, & Spittal MJ (2017). Predicting suicidal behaviours using clinical instruments: systematic review and meta-analysis of positive predictive values for risk scales. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 210(6), 387–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2023). National Center for Health Statistics. National Vital Statistics System, Mortality 2018-2021 on CDC WONDER Online Database, released in 2023. Data are from the Multiple Cause of Death Files, 2018-2021, as compiled from data provided by the 57 vital statistics jurisdictions through the Vital Statistics Cooperative Program. Accessed at http://wonder.cdc.gov/mcd-icd10-expanded.html on June 13, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  27. Cha CB, Najmi S, Park JM, Finn CT, & Nock MK (2010). Attentional bias toward suicide-related stimuli predicts suicidal behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(3), 616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cha CB, O'Connor RC, Kirtley O, Cleare S, Wetherall K, Eschle S, … & Nock MK (2018). Testing mood-activated psychological markers for suicidal ideation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(5), 448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Chan MK, Bhatti H, Meader N, Stockton S, Evans J, O'Connor RC, … & Kendall T (2016). Predicting suicide following self-harm: systematic review of risk factors and risk scales. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(4), 277–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Chapman A, Devue C, & Grimshaw GM (2019). Fleeting reliability in the dot-probe task. Psychological Research, 83(2), 308–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Chu C, Buchman-Schmitt JM, Stanley IH, Hom MA, Tucker RP, Hagan CR, … & Joiner TE Jr (2017). The interpersonal theory of suicide: A systematic review and meta-analysis of a decade of cross-national research. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 1313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Chu C, Hammock EA, & Joiner TE (2020). Unextracted plasma oxytocin levels decrease following in-laboratory social exclusion in young adults with a suicide attempt history. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 121, 173–181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Chu JP, Goldblum P, Floyd R, & Bongar B (2010). The cultural theory and model of suicide. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 14(1-4), 25–40. [Google Scholar]
  34. Chung Y, & Jeglic EL (2016). Use of the modified emotional Stroop task to detect suicidality in college population. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 46(1), 55–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Chung Y, & Jeglic EL (2017). Detecting suicide risk among college students: A test of the predictive validity of the modified emotional Stroop task. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 47(4), 398–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Cohen LJ, Mokhtar R, Richards J, Hernandez M, Bloch-Elkouby S, & Galynker I (2022). The Narrative-Crisis Model of suicide and its prediction of near-term suicide risk. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 52(2), 231–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Coope C, Donovan J, Wilson C, Barnes M, Metcalfe C, Hollingworth W, … & Gunnell D (2015). Characteristics of people dying by suicide after job loss, financial difficulties and other economic stressors during a period of recession (2010–2011): A review of coroners’ records. Journal of Affective Disorders, 183, 98–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Cummins J, Hussey I, & Hughes S (2019). The AMPeror’s new clothes: Performance on the affect misattribution procedure is mainly driven by awareness of influence of the primes. [Preprint]. PsyArXiv. 10.31234/osf.io/d5zn8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dagher A, Tannenbaum B, Hayashi T, Pruessner JC, & McBride D (2009). An acute psychosocial stress enhances the neural response to smoking cues. Brain Research, 1293, 40–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Datta D, & Arnsten AF (2019). Loss of prefrontal cortical higher cognition with uncontrollable stress: molecular mechanisms, changes with age, and relevance to treatment. Brain Sciences, 9(5), 113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. De Catanzaro D. (1986). A mathematical model of evolutionary pressures regulating self-preservation and self-destruction. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 16(2), 166–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Deshpande G, Baxi M, Witte T, & Robinson JL (2016). A neural basis for the acquired capability for suicide. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 7, 125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Dombrovski AY, Szanto K, Siegle GJ, Wallace ML, Forman SD, Sahakian B, … & Clark L (2011). Lethal forethought: delayed reward discounting differentiates high-and low-lethality suicide attempts in old age. Biological Psychiatry, 70(2), 138–144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Dougherty DM, Mathias CW, Marsh DM, Papageorgiou TD, Swann AC, & Moeller FG (2004). Laboratory measured behavioral impulsivity relates to suicide attempt history. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 34(4), 374–385. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Durkheim E (1951). Suicide: A study in sociology [1897]. Translated by Spaulding JA and G. Simpson. Glencoe, IL: The Free Press. [Google Scholar]
  46. Ellis TE, Rufino KA, & Green KL (2016). Implicit measure of life/death orientation predicts response of suicidal ideation to treatment in psychiatric inpatients. Archives of Suicide Research, 20(1), 59–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Favril L, Yu R, Uyar A, Sharpe M, & Fazel S (2022). Risk factors for suicide in adults: systematic review and meta-analysis of psychological autopsy studies. BMJ Ment Health, 25(4), 148–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Feng C, Becker B, Huang W, Wu X, Eickhoff SB, & Chen T (2018). Neural substrates of the emotion-word and emotional counting Stroop tasks in healthy and clinical populations: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. NeuroImage, 173, 258–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Feskanich D, Hastrup JL, Marshall JR, Colditz GA, Stampfer MJ, Willett WC, & Kawachi I (2002). Stress and suicide in the Nurses' health study. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 56(2), 95–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Field M, & Powell H (2007). Stress increases attentional bias for alcohol cues in social drinkers who drink to cope. Alcohol & Alcoholism, 42(6), 560–566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Fowler JC (2012). Suicide risk assessment in clinical practice: pragmatic guidelines for imperfect assessments. Psychotherapy, 49(1), 81–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Fox E, Cahill S, & Zougkou K (2010). Preconscious processing biases predict emotional reactivity to stress. Biological Psychiatry, 67(4), 371–377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Franklin JC, Ribeiro JD, Fox KR, Bentley KH, Kleiman EM, Huang X, … & Nock MK (2017). Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: A meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 143(2), 187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Ganzini L, Denneson LM, Press N, Bair MJ, Helmer DA, Poat J, & Dobscha SK (2013). Trust is the basis for effective suicide risk screening and assessment in veterans. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 28(9), 1215–1221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gilbert JR, Gerner JL, Burton CR, Nugent AC, Zarate CA Jr, & Ballard ED (2022). Magnetoencephalography biomarkers of suicide attempt history and antidepressant response to ketamine in treatment-resistant major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 312, 188–197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Glenn CR, Kleiman EM, Coppersmith DD, Santee AC, Esposito EC, Cha CB, … & Auerbach RP (2017). Implicit identification with death predicts change in suicide ideation during psychiatric treatment in adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(12), 1319–1329. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gordon B, & Caramazza A (1982). Lexical decision for open-and closed-class words: Failure to replicate differential frequency sensitivity. Brain and Language, 15(1), 143–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gorlyn M, Keilp JG, Oquendo MA, Burke AK, & Mann JJ (2013). Iowa Gambling Task performance in currently depressed suicide attempters. Psychiatry Research, 207(3), 150–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Gradus JL, Qin P, Lincoln AK, Miller M, Lawler E, Sørensen HT, & Lash TL (2010). Acute stress reaction and completed suicide. International Journal of Epidemiology, 39(6), 1478–1484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Greenwald AG, McGhee DE, & Schwartz JL (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Harrison DP, Stritzke WG, Fay N, & Hudaib AR (2018). Suicide risk assessment: Trust an implicit probe or listen to the patient?. Psychological Assessment, 30(10), 1317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hazlett EA, Blair NJ, Fernandez N, Mascitelli K, Perez-Rodriguez MM, New AS, … & Goodman M (2016). Startle amplitude during unpleasant pictures is greater in veterans with a history of multiple-suicide attempts and predicts a future suicide attempt. Psychophysiology, 53(10), 1524–1534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hom MA, Joiner TE Jr, & Bernert RA (2016). Limitations of a single-item assessment of suicide attempt history: Implications for standardized suicide risk assessment. Psychological Assessment, 28(8), 1026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hom MA, Stanley IH, Podlogar MC, & Joiner TE Jr (2017). “Are you having thoughts of suicide?” Examining experiences with disclosing and denying suicidal ideation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 73(10), 1382–1392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Horesh N. (2001). Self-report vs. computerized measures of impulsivity as a correlate of suicidal behavior. Crisis: The Journal of Crisis Intervention and Suicide Prevention, 22(1), 27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Hussey I, Barnes-Holmes D, & Booth R (2016). Individuals with current suicidal ideation demonstrate implicit “fearlessness of death”. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 51, 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Interian A, Myers CE, Chesin MS, Kline A, Hill LS, King AR, … & Keilp JG (2020). Towards the objective assessment of suicidal states: Some neurocognitive deficits may be temporally related to suicide attempt. Psychiatry Research, 287, 112624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Jiang C, Buchanan TW, Yao Z, Zhang K, Wu J, & Zhang L (2017). Acute psychological stress disrupts attentional bias to threat-related stimuli. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Joiner TE (2005). Why people die by suicide. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  70. Joiner TE, Hom MA, Hagan CR, & Silva C (2016). Suicide as a derangement of the self-sacrificial aspect of eusociality. Psychological review, 123(3), 235–254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Joiner TE Jr, Van Orden KA, Witte TK, Selby EA, Ribeiro JD, Lewis R, & Rudd MD (2009). Main predictions of the interpersonal–psychological theory of suicidal behavior: Empirical tests in two samples of young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(3), 634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Jollant F, Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Astruc B, Torres S, Verdier R, … & Courtet P (2005). Impaired decision making in suicide attempters. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(2), 304–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Jollant F, Guillaume S, Jaussent I, Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Castelnau D, … & Courtet P (2007). Psychiatric diagnoses and personality traits associated with disadvantageous decision-making. European Psychiatry, 22(7), 455–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Jollant F, Lawrence NS, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, Fullana MA, Drapier D, … & Phillips ML (2008). Orbitofrontal cortex response to angry faces in men with histories of suicide attempts. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(6), 740–748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Jollant F, Lawrence NS, Olie E, O'Daly O, Malafosse A, Courtet P, & Phillips ML (2010). Decreased activation of lateral orbitofrontal cortex during risky choices under uncertainty is associated with disadvantageous decision-making and suicidal behavior. Neuroimage, 51(3), 1275–1281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kapur N, Cooper J, O'Connor RC, & Hawton K (2013). Non-suicidal self-injury v. attempted suicide: new diagnosis or false dichotomy?. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(5), 326–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Keilp JG, Sackeim HA, & Mann JJ (2005). Correlates of trait impulsiveness in performance measures and neuropsychological tests. Psychiatry Research, 135(3), 191–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Kessler RC, Bossarte RM, Luedtke A, Zaslavsky AM, & Zubizarreta JR (2020). Suicide prediction models: a critical review of recent research with recommendations for the way forward. Molecular Psychiatry, 25(1), 168–179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Klonsky ED, & May AM (2015). The three-step theory (3ST): A new theory of suicide rooted in the “ideation-to-action” framework. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 8(2), 114–129. [Google Scholar]
  80. Koole SL, Dijksterhuis A, & Van Knippenberg A (2001). What's in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(4), 669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, & Cuthbert BN (1990). Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97(3), 377. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Large M, Kaneson M, Myles N, Myles H, Gunaratne P, & Ryan C (2016). Meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies of suicide risk assessment among psychiatric patients: heterogeneity in results and lack of improvement over time. PloS One, 11(6), e0156322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. LeMoult J, Arditte KA, D'Avanzato C, & Joormann J (2013). State rumination: Associations with emotional stress reactivity and attention biases. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 4(5), 471–484. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Lieberman L, Petrey K, Shankman SA, Phan KL, & Gorka SM (2020). Heightened reactivity to uncertain threat as a neurobehavioral marker of suicidal ideation in individuals with depression and anxiety. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 155, 99–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Lin L, Liu Y, Mo J, Wang C, Liu T, Xu Z, … & Wu X (2022). Attentional bias to emotional facial expressions in undergraduates with suicidal ideation: an ERP study. Archives of Suicide Research, 1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Liu BP, Zhang J, Chu J, Qiu HM, Jia CX, & Hennessy DA (2019). Negative life events as triggers on suicide attempt in rural China: a case-crossover study. Psychiatry Research, 276, 100–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Liu RT, Vassileva J, Gonzalez R, & Martin EM (2012). A comparison of delay discounting among substance users with and without suicide attempt history. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26(4), 980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Logan GD, Cowan WB, & Davis KA (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(2), 276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Ludäscher P, Bohus M, Lieb K, Philipsen A, Jochims A, & Schmahl C (2007). Elevated pain thresholds correlate with dissociation and aversive arousal in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 149(1-3), 291–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Lutz PE, Mechawar N, & Turecki G (2017). Neuropathology of suicide: recent findings and future directions. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(10), 1395–1412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. MacLeod C, Mathews A, & Tata P (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Mann JJ, & Rizk MM (2020). A brain-centric model of suicidal behavior. American journal of psychiatry, 177(10), 902–916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Maple M, Frey LM, McKay K, Coker S, & Grey S (2020). “Nobody hears a silent cry for help”: Suicide attempt survivors’ experiences of disclosing during and after a crisis. Archives of Suicide Research, 24(4), 498–516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mathias CW, Dougherty DM, James LM, Richard DM, Dawes MA, Acheson A, & Hill-Kapturczak N (2011). Intolerance to delayed reward in girls with multiple suicide attempts. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 41(3), 277–286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Maydych V. (2019). The interplay between stress, inflammation, and emotional attention: relevance for depression. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. McGirr A, Renaud J, Bureau A, Seguin M, Lesage A, & Turecki G (2008). Impulsive-aggressive behaviours and completed suicide across the life cycle: a predisposition for younger age of suicide. Psychological Medicine, 38(3), 407–417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. McLaughlin J, O'carroll RE, & O'connor RC (2012). Intimate partner abuse and suicidality: a systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(8), 677–689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Mérida-López S, Extremera N, & Rey L (2018). Understanding the links between self-report emotional intelligence and suicide risk: does psychological distress mediate this relationship across time and samples?. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Meyer IH, Dietrich J, & Schwartz S (2008). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders and suicide attempts in diverse lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations. American Journal of Public Health, 98(6), 1004–1006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Miller AB, & Prinstein MJ (2019). Adolescent suicide as a failure of acute stress-response systems. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Moreno M, Gutiérrez-Rojas L, & Porras-Segovia A (2022). Implicit cognition tests for the assessment of suicide risk: A systematic review. Current Psychiatry Reports, 24(2), 141–159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Moskowitz A, Stein JA, & Lightfoot M (2013). The mediating roles of stress and maladaptive behaviors on self-harm and suicide attempts among runaway and homeless youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(7), 1015–1027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Mulder R, Newton-Howes G, & Coid JW (2016). The futility of risk prediction in psychiatry. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 209(4), 271–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Myers CE, Dave CV, Callahan M, Chesin MS, Keilp JG, Beck KD, … & Interian A (2022). Improving the prospective prediction of a near-term suicide attempt in veterans at risk for suicide, using a go/no-go task. Psychological Medicine, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Naghavi M. (2019). Global, regional, and national burden of suicide mortality 1990 to 2016: systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. BMJ, 364, 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. National Action Alliance for Suicide Prevention: Research Prioritization Task Force. (2014). A prioritized research agenda for suicide prevention: An action plan to save lives. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health and the Research Prioritization Task Force. [Google Scholar]
  107. Nock MK (2009). Why do people hurt themselves? New insights into the nature and functions of self-injury. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 78–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Nock MK, & Banaji MR (2007). Prediction of suicide ideation and attempts among adolescents using a brief performance-based test. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(5), 707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Nock MK, Millner AJ, Ross EL, Kennedy CJ, Al-Suwaidi M, Barak-Corren Y, … & Kessler RC (2022). Prediction of suicide attempts using clinician assessment, patient self-report, and electronic health records. JAMA Network Open, 5(1), e2144373–e2144373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Nock MK, Park JM, Finn CT, Deliberto TL, Dour HJ, & Banaji MR (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind: Implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21(4), 511–517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Nock MK, Prinstein MJ, & Sterba SK (2009). Revealing the form and function of self-injurious thoughts and behaviors: A real-time ecological assessment study among adolescents and young adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(4), 816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. O'Connor RC, & Kirtley OJ (2018). The integrated motivational–volitional model of suicidal behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1754), 20170268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. O’Connor RC (2011). Towards an integrated motivational–volitional model of suicidal behaviour. International Handbook of Suicide Prevention: Research, Policy and Practice, 1, 181–98. [Google Scholar]
  114. Okechukwu FO, Ogba KT, Nwufo JI, Ogba MO, Onyekachi BN, Nwanosike CI, & Onyishi AB (2022). Academic stress and suicidal ideation: moderating roles of coping style and resilience. BMC Psychiatry, 22(1), 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Olié E, Jollant F, Deverdun J, de Champfleur NM, Cyprien F, Le Bars E, … & Courtet P (2017). The experience of social exclusion in women with a history of suicidal acts: a neuroimaging study. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Olson MA, Fazio RH, & Hermann AD (2007). Reporting tendencies underlie discrepancies between implicit and explicit measures of self-esteem. Psychological Science, 18(4), 287–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Orbach I, Mikulincer M, King R, Cohen D, & Stein D (1997). Thresholds and tolerance of physical pain in suicidal and nonsuicidal adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(4), 646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Orbach I, Palgi Y, Stein D, Har-Even D, Lotem-Peleg M, Asherov J, & Elizur A (1996b). Tolerance for physical pain in suicidal subjects. Death Studies, 20(4), 327–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Orbach I, Stein D, Palgi Y, Asherov J, Har-Even D, & Elizur A (1996a). Perception of physical pain in accident and suicide attempt patients: Self-preservation vs self-destruction. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30(4), 307–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Owens C, Hansford L, Sharkey S, & Ford T (2016). Needs and fears of young people presenting at accident and emergency department following an act of self-harm: secondary analysis of qualitative data. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 208(3), 286–291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Peckham AD, McHugh RK, & Otto MW (2010). A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depression and Anxiety, 27(12), 1135–1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Price RB, Kuckertz JM, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Silk JS, Ryan ND, … & Amir N (2015). Empirical recommendations for improving the stability of the dot-probe task in clinical research. Psychological Assessment, 27(2), 365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Rabasco A, & Andover MS (2020). The interaction of dissociation, pain tolerance, and suicidal ideation in predicting suicide attempts. Psychiatry Research, 284, 112661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Rachlin H, Raineri A, & Cross D (1991). Subjective probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55(2), 233–244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Raio CM, Konova AB, & Otto AR (2020). Trait impulsivity and acute stress interact to influence choice and decision speed during multi-stage decision-making. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Randall JR, Rowe BH, Dong KA, Nock MK, & Colman I (2013). Assessment of self-harm risk using implicit thoughts. Psychological Assessment, 25(3), 714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Rath D, Teismann T, Schmitz F, Glaesmer H, Hallensleben N, Paashaus L, … & Forkmann T (2021). Predicting suicidal behavior by implicit associations with death? Examination of the death IAT in two inpatient samples of differing suicide risk. Psychological Assessment, 33(4), 287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Richard-Devantoy S, Ding Y, Lepage M, Turecki G, & Jollant F (2016b). Cognitive inhibition in depression and suicidal behavior: a neuroimaging study. Psychological Medicine, 46(5), 933–944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Richard-Devantoy S, Ding Y, Turecki G, & Jollant F (2016a). Attentional bias toward suicide-relevant information in suicide attempters: A cross-sectional study and a meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 196, 101–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Richard-Devantoy S, Jollant F, Kefi Z, Turecki G, Olié JP, Annweiler C, … & Le Gall D (2012). Deficit of cognitive inhibition in depressed elderly: a neurocognitive marker of suicidal risk. Journal of Affective Disorders, 140(2), 193–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Richard-Devantoy S, Olié E, Guillaume S, Bechara A, Courtet P, & Jollant F (2013). Distinct alterations in value-based decision-making and cognitive control in suicide attempters: toward a dual neurocognitive model. Journal of affective disorders, 151(3), 1120–1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Rogers ML, Galynker I, Yaseen Z, DeFazio K, & Joiner TE (2017). An overview and comparison of two proposed suicide-specific diagnoses: Acute suicidal affective disturbance and suicide crisis syndrome. Psychiatric Annals, 47(8), 416–420. [Google Scholar]
  133. Rudd MD (2006). Fluid vulnerability theory: A cognitive approach to understanding the process of acute and chronic suicide risk. Ellis PT (Ed.), Cognition and suicide: theory, research and therapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  134. Sato H, & Kawahara JI (2012). Assessing acute stress with the Implicit Association Test. Cognition & Emotion, 26(1), 129–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Schmitz A, & Grillon C (2012). Assessing fear and anxiety in humans using the threat of predictable and unpredictable aversive events (the NPU-threat test). Nature Protocols, 7(3), 527–532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Schmukle SC (2005). Unreliability of the dot probe task. European Journal of Personality, 19(7), 595–605. [Google Scholar]
  137. Seidl E, Padberg F, Bauriedl-Schmidt C, Albert A, Daltrozzo T, Hall J, … & Jobst A (2020). Response to ostracism in patients with chronic depression, episodic depression and borderline personality disorder a study using Cyberball. Journal of Affective Disorders, 260, 254–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Serafini G, Muzio C, Piccinini G, Flouri E, Ferrigno G, Pompili M, … & Amore M (2015). Life adversities and suicidal behavior in young individuals: a systematic review. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 24(12), 1423–1446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Serafini G, Parisi VM, Aguglia A, Amerio A, Sampogna G, Fiorillo A, … & Amore M (2020). A specific inflammatory profile underlying suicide risk? Systematic review of the main literature findings. International journal of environmental research and public health, 17(7), 2393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Sheehan LL, Corrigan PW, Al-Khouja MA, & Stigma of Suicide Research Team. (2016). Stakeholder perspectives on the stigma of suicide attempt survivors. Crisis, 26, 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Shneidman ES (1976). A psychologic theory of suicide. Psychiatric Annals, 6(11), 51–66. [Google Scholar]
  142. Shneidman ES (1993). Suicide as psychache: A clinical approach to self-destructive behavior. New York, NY: Aronson. [Google Scholar]
  143. Slavich GM, & Irwin MR (2014). From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: a social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychological bulletin, 140(3), 774–815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Smith MM, Sherry SB, Chen S, Saklofske DH, Mushquash C, Flett GL, & Hewitt PL (2018). The perniciousness of perfectionism: A meta-analytic review of the perfectionism–suicide relationship. Journal of Personality, 86(3), 522–542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Smith PN, Cukrowicz KC, Poindexter EK, Hobson V, & Cohen LM (2010). The acquired capability for suicide: A comparison of suicide attempters, suicide ideators, and non-suicidal controls. Depression and Anxiety, 27(9), 871–877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Sohn MN, McMorris CA, Bray S, & McGirr A (2021). The death-implicit association test and suicide attempts: a systematic review and meta-analysis of discriminative and prospective utility. Psychological Medicine, 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Stack S. (2000). Suicide: a 15-year review of the sociological literature part I: cultural and economic factors. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 30(2), 145–162. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Stewart JG, Glenn CR, Esposito EC, Cha CB, Nock MK, & Auerbach RP (2017). Cognitive control deficits differentiate adolescent suicide ideators from attempters. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 78(6), 3157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Stewart JG, Shields GS, Esposito EC, Cosby EA, Allen NB, Slavich GM, & Auerbach RP (2019). Life stress and suicide in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(10), 1707–1722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2022). Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2021 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (HHS Publication No. PEP22-07-01-005, NSDUH Series H-57). Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. https://www.samhsa.gov/data/report/2021-nsduh-annual-national-report [Google Scholar]
  151. Swann AC, Dougherty DM, Pazzaglia PJ, Pham M, Steinberg JL, & Moeller FG (2005). Increased impulsivity associated with severity of suicide attempt history in patients with bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(9), 1680–1687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Tavakoli P, Jerome E, Boafo A, & Campbell K (2021). Attentional bias deficits in adolescent suicide attempters during an emotional Stroop task: an ERP study. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 1695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Taylor TL, Hawton K, Fortune S, & Kapur N (2009). Attitudes towards clinical services among people who self-harm: systematic review. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 194(2), 104–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Tello N, Harika-Germaneau G, Serra W, Jaafari N, & Chatard A (2020). Forecasting a fatal decision: Direct replication of the predictive validity of the Suicide–Implicit Association Test. Psychological Science, 31(1), 65–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Toplak ME, Sorge GB, Benoit A, West RF, & Stanovich KE (2010). Decision-making and cognitive abilities: A review of associations between Iowa Gambling Task performance, executive functions, and intelligence. Clinical psychology review, 30(5), 562–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Tucker RP, Wingate LR, Burkley M, & Wells TT (2018). Implicit association with suicide as measured by the suicide affect misattribution procedure (s-amp) predicts suicide ideation. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 48(6), 720–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Turecki G. (2014). The molecular bases of the suicidal brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(12), 802–816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Turecki G, & Brent DA (2016). Suicide and suicidal behaviour. The Lancet, 387(10024), 1227–1239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Turecki G, Brent DA, Gunnell D, O’Connor RC, Oquendo MA, Pirkis J, & Stanley BH (2019). Suicide and suicide risk. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 5(1), 1–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Van Orden KA, Witte TK, Cukrowicz KC, Braithwaite SR, Selby EA, & Joiner TE Jr (2010). The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psychological Review, 117(2), 575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Vazire S, & Carlson EN (2010). Self-knowledge of personality: Do people know themselves?. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(8), 605–620. [Google Scholar]
  162. Velupillai S, Hadlaczky G, Baca-Garcia E, Gorrell GM, Werbeloff N, Nguyen D, … & Dutta R (2019). Risk assessment tools and data-driven approaches for predicting and preventing suicidal behavior. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Wald I, Degnan KA, Gorodetsky E, Charney DS, Fox NA, Fruchter E, … & Bar-Haim Y (2013). Attention to threats and combat-related posttraumatic stress symptoms: prospective associations and moderation by the serotonin transporter gene. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(4), 401–408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Walls NE, Freedenthal S, & Wisneski H (2008). Suicidal ideation and attempts among sexual minority youths receiving social services. Social Work, 53(1), 21–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Wang Y, Sareen J, Afifi TO, Bolton SL, Johnson EA, & Bolton JM (2015). A population-based longitudinal study of recent stressful life events as risk factors for suicidal behavior in major depressive disorder. Archives of Suicide Research, 19(2), 202–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Weigard A, & Sripada C (2021). Task-General Efficiency of Evidence Accumulation as a Computationally Defined Neurocognitive Trait: Implications for Clinical Neuroscience. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 1(1), 5–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Wells TT, Tucker RP, Kraines MA, Smith LM, & Unruh-Dawes E (2020). Implicit bias for suicide persists after ideation resolves. Psychiatry Research, 285, 112784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Wenzel A, & Beck AT (2008). A cognitive model of suicidal behavior: Theory and treatment. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(4), 189–201. [Google Scholar]
  169. Werntz AJ, Steinman SA, Glenn JJ, Nock MK, & Teachman BA (2016). Characterizing implicit mental health associations across clinical domains. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 52, 17–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Westheide J, Quednow BB, Kuhn KU, Hoppe C, Cooper-Mahkorn D, Hawellek B, … & Wagner M (2008). Executive performance of depressed suicide attempters: the role of suicidal ideation. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 258(7), 414–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Williams JMG, & Broadbent K (1986). Distraction by emotional stimuli: Use of a Stroop task with suicide attempters. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 25(2), 101–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Williams KD, & Sommer KL (1997). Social ostracism by coworkers: Does rejection lead to loafing or compensation?. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(7), 693–706. [Google Scholar]
  173. Williams KD, Cheung CK, & Choi W (2000). Cyberostracism: effects of being ignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(5), 748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Wilson TD (2004). Strangers to ourselves. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  175. Wilson TD (2009). Know thyself. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(4), 384–389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Wojnar M, Ilgen MA, Czyz E, Strobbe S, Klimkiewicz A, Jakubczyk A, … & Brower KJ (2009). Impulsive and non-impulsive suicide attempts in patients treated for alcohol dependence. Journal of Affective Disorders, 115(1-2), 131–139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Woodford R, Spittal MJ, Milner A, McGill K, Kapur N, Pirkis J, … & Carter G (2019). Accuracy of clinician predictions of future self-harm: a systematic review and meta-analysis of predictive studies. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 49(1), 23–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. World Health Organization. (2014). Preventing Suicide: A Global Imperative. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
  179. Zadro L, Williams KD, & Richardson R (2004). How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(4), 560–567. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES