Abstract
Background
Studies that examined sex differences in first-episode patients consistently show that males compared to females have poor premorbid adjustment, earlier age of onset, worse clinical characteristics, and poorer outcomes. However, little is known about potential mediators that could explain these sex differences.
Methods
Our sample consisted of 137 individuals with first episode schizophrenia (males, n = 105; 77%) with a mean age of 22.1(s.d. = 4.1) years and mean education of 12.5(s.d. = 1.7) years. At entry, patients were within 2 years of their first psychotic episode onset. Baseline assessments were conducted for premorbid adjustment, symptoms, cognitive functioning, insight, and at 6-months for role and social functioning.
Results
Males as compared to females had poorer premorbid adjustment across several key developmental periods (p < 0.01), an earlier age of onset [M = 20.3(3.3) v. 22.8(5.6), p = 0.002], more negative symptoms (p = 0.044), poorer insight (p = 0.031), and poorer baseline and 6-month role (p = 0.002) and social functioning (p = 0.034). Several of these variables in which males showed impairment were significant predictors of 6-month role and social functioning. Premorbid adjustment and insight mediated the relationship between sex and role and social functioning at 6-months, but not negative symptoms.
Discussion
Males compared to females were at lower levels across several key premorbid and clinical domains which are strongly associated with functional outcome supporting the hypothesis that males might have a more disabling form of schizophrenia. The relationship between sex with role and social functioning was mediated through premorbid adjustment and insight suggesting pathways for understanding why females might have a less disabling form of schizophrenia.
Key words: Age of onset, first episode schizophrenia, functional outcome, insight, mediation analysis, neurocognition, premorbid adjustment, sex differences
Introduction
The general conclusion from studies of multi-episode schizophrenia is that males compared to females appear to be at greater risk of developing schizophrenia and have a more severe form of the illness with regard to onset, symptoms, and psychosocial functional outcome (Abel, Drake, & Goldstein, 2010; Aleman, Kahn, & Selten, 2003). Understandably, researchers have turned to the initial course of schizophrenia to further examine early sources that contribute to sex differences. First episode studies also show that males compared with females have an earlier age of illness onset, lower levels of premorbid adjustment, lower baseline social and role functioning, more negative symptoms, and overall poorer course and outcome (Ochoa, Usall, Cobo, Labad, & Kulkarni, 2012). Researchers are interested in understanding sex differences in social and role functioning in search of ways to better understand prognosis and to improve outcomes.
Studies published thus far have generally not addressed possible mediation pathways that could explain better outcomes observed in females. This process involves examining the literature for possible mediators such as age of onset, premorbid adjustment, neurocognition and social cognition, and negative symptoms that have been found to been associated with outcomes in schizophrenia. Mediation analyses can then examine whether those predictors of outcome are mediator of the relationship between sex and functional outcomes. One study is an example of this approach, finding that females had better verbal memory compared to males and that a sequence of verbal memory and negative symptoms mediated functioning assessed at one year (Buck et al., 2020).
Better premorbid functioning in schizophrenia has been associated with a better prognosis, so sex differences in premorbid adjustment might have implications for short and long-term functional outcome (Addington & Addington, 2005; Brill et al., 2009; Minor et al., 2015; Stoffelmayr, Dillavou, & Hunter, 1983). One recent study suggested that the more favorable premorbid and baseline functional characteristics of women predicted better outcomes during the first three years of follow-up when treatment was delivered in early intervention services (EIS) (Ayesa-Arriola et al., 2020). Despite the age of onset and premorbid functional differences, generally these studies of first episode schizophrenia (FES) a term we use for brevity that includes individuals with schizophreniform and schizoaffective disorder, found few or no sex differences in demographic variables, most types of symptoms, and functioning at study entry. However, the findings are mixed for duration of untreated psychosis (Chang et al., 2011; Hui et al., 2016; Thorup et al., 2007, 2014). Given its prominent role, premorbid adjustment might be a potential mediator of the relationship between sex and functional outcome.
While there are few sex differences reported in positive symptoms, males reportedly have higher levels of negative symptoms while females show higher levels of mood symptoms such as depression and anxiety (Thorup et al., 2014). In fact, Cotton et al. (2009) found that women presented with higher levels of affective symptoms in general than did men. In contrast, Szymanski et al. (1995) found in individuals with schizophrenia admitted for the first time, that women compared to men presented with more anxiety, but also illogical thinking, inappropriate affect, and bizarre behavior. However, in one study no gender differences were found (Barajas, Ochoa, Obiols, & Lalucat-Jo, 2015). In any event, a review of FES studies concluded that males generally had more negative symptoms and females had more mood symptoms while there were little or no differences found in positive symptoms (Ochoa et al., 2012). Given the prominent role that negative symptoms play in functional outcome, negative symptoms might be a potential mediator of the relationship between sex and functional outcome.
The literature on cognitive deficits in FES is comparable with that on multi-episode patients showing that cognitive deficits are present (Mesholam-Gately, Giuliano, Goff, Faraone, & Seidman, 2009) and that there is a relationship between neurocognitive and social cognitive functioning and outcome in first episode patients (Fu, Czajkowski, Rund, & Torgalsbøen, 2017; Horan et al., 2012; Milev, 2005). Further, FES studies show that males perform better in the domain of visual perception while females perform better in domains such as verbal learning and memory (Fu et al., 2017; Hui et al., 2016; Pu et al., 2019; Torgalsbøen, Mohn, & Rund, 2014; Yang, Gao, Xiong, & Zhang, 2021). In one of the largest studies of sex differences in FES, late onset male patients were found to be more impaired than females on measures of verbal memory, executive functions, and other areas of cognitive performance (Ayesa-Arriola et al., 2020). Studies suggest that the superior social cognitive performance of females is not present in individuals with FES (Danaher, Allott, Killackey, Hester, & Cotton, 2018; Navarra-Ventura et al., 2018; Verdaguer-Rodríguez et al., 2021) for a review see Mote and Kring (2016). In the general population, women on average perform somewhat better on verbal tasks while men perform somewhat better on visuospatial tasks (Kern et al., 2008a, 2008b; Weiss et al., 2003). Several studies that examined neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia did not include a healthy control group and/or did not correct for sex differences in the general population. Whether these effects are present in schizophrenia to a larger extent than in the general population is not clear (Lewine, Thurston-Snoha, & Ardery, 2006). Thus, whether the conclusions regarding sex differences in neurocognition for schizophrenia patients reflect the impact of schizophrenia or general population sex differences is difficult to determine.
There is still much to be learned about sex differences in insight for individuals with first episode psychotic disorders. In a prospective study using structured assessments, FES women as compared with men showed better insight into psychotic illness (McEvoy et al., 2006). However, additional studies of FES individuals that also used structured assessments did not find significant sex differences in global awareness of having a mental disorder (Ayesa-Arriola et al., 2011; Keshavan, Rabinowitz, DeSmedt, Harvey, & Schooler, 2004; Mutsatsa, Joyce, Hutton, & Barnes, 2006). The possibility that there are sex differences in insight early in the course of schizophrenia is important because a large body of evidence in schizophrenia links better insight with lower levels of symptoms and better functioning. Better insight among females, if present, could be a good prognostic sign and help explain the better course, outcomes, and recovery rates seen in females. Given the prominent role for insight in functional outcome, insight might be a potential mediator of the relationship between sex and functional outcome.
We hypothesized that FES males compared to females have poorer premorbid adjustment, an earlier age of onset, poorer insight into their illness, poorer baseline role and social functioning, and higher baseline levels of negative symptoms. Further, we planned to examine these variables as potential mediators of the relationship between sex and functional outcome to provide additional information regarding pathways by which sex influences early functional outcome in schizophrenia.
Methods
Participants
The patient sample consisted of 137 individuals with schizophrenia, schizophreniform, or schizoaffective disorder-depressed type, including 105 males (77%) and 32 females (23%). The mean age at study entry was 22.0 (s.d. = 4.1) years, the mean education was 12.5 (1.7) years, and most patients were single (n = 131; 86%). All participants received outpatient psychiatric treatment at the UCLA Aftercare Research Program and were participants in the fourth phase of the Developmental Processes in Schizophrenic Disorders Project (Nuechterlein et al., 2020; Subotnik et al., 2015). This study was approved by the UCLA Office for Human Research Protections, and all participants gave written informed consent. Diagnosis was established by trained and certified research assessment staff through the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-IV) plus supplementary information from family members and treating professionals.
Entry criteria were: (1) an onset of psychotic illness within 2 years of study entry; (2) a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder-depressed type, or schizophreniform disorder; (3) 18 to 45 years of age; (4) no evidence of a known neurological disorder; (5) no evidence of significant and habitual drug abuse or alcoholism in the 6 months prior to study entry and that the psychosis was substance-induced; (6) premorbid IQ not less than 70; (7) sufficient fluency in English to avoid invalidating research measures; and (8) treatment with risperidone was not contraindicated.
Normal comparison subjects were recruited through the use of flyers and advertisements which were placed in community sites and local newspapers. Normal comparison participants were chosen on the basis of age, sex, race/ethnicity, handedness, parental education, and community of residence that were comparable to those of the patient sample. Potential normal comparison subjects were assessed using the SCID-IV, sections of SCID-II for Personality Disorders, and the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS). The eligibility criteria included: no major psychiatric disorder except for a single episode of major depression, no personality disorder, no family history of a major psychotic disorder, and no current of substance use or history substance dependence.
Procedures
Study Entry was defined as the time point in which the patient enrolled in the study. Baseline occurred about 2–3 months after study entry and the 6-month point occurred subsequent to the baseline assessment.
Measures
Psychiatric history, premorbid adjustment, symptoms, and functioning
UCLA Psychiatric and Social History Schedule (P&SH). Demographic and premorbid history data were collected at study entry using a comprehensive form developed by the study team. P&SH data that were collected included age, sex, marital status, race, age of onset, patient and parental years of education, first appearance of prodromal and psychotic symptoms, and treatment history as well as additional psychiatric history variables.
Cannon-Spoor Premorbid Adjustment Scale (PAS) (Cannon-Spoor, Potkin, & Wyatt, 1982). The PAS domains include four developmental periods: (1) Childhood (up to age 11); (2) Early adolescence (ages 12 to 15); (3) Late adolescence (ages 17 to 18); and (4) Adulthood (age 19 and above). The PAS also includes five domains of psychosocial adjustment: (1) Sociability and withdrawal; (2) Peer relationships; (3) Scholastic performance; (4) Adaptation to school; and (e) Social-sexual aspects of life. A semi-structured interview accompanies the PAS items which are rated from 0 to 6 based on an interview with the patient and from all sources of information, e.g., the patient's parents, and school and medical records. The PAS was administered at baseline. We examined the summary scores for developmental periods for this study. Higher scores indicate more severe developmental delay.
Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) (Ventura, Green, Shaner, & Liberman, 1993a) The BPRS was used to examine severity levels in positive, negative, depression-anxiety, and manic symptoms (Ventura et al., 1993b). Each BPRS rater achieved a median Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) of 0.80 or higher across all BPRS items compared with a set of criterion ratings and participated in a quality assurance program. The BPRS is separated into factors (Ventura, Nuechterlein, Subotnik, Gutkind, & Gilbert, 2000) that include: Positive Symptoms (Unusual Thought Content, Hallucinations, and Conceptual Disorganization), Negative Symptoms (Blunted Affect, Emotional Withdrawal, and Motor Retardation), and Depression-Anxiety (Depression, Anxiety, Guilt). The BPRS ratings were administered at Study Entry.
Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) (Andreasen, 1984). The SANS is a 25-item measure that is widely used to assess two negative symptom domains: (1) Expressive Symptoms, which consisted of Affective Flattening (blunted affect) and Alogia, and (2) Experiential Symptoms, which consisted of Avolition/Apathy and Anhedonia/Asociality. The SANS was administered every 3 months by raters who were trained to criterion levels of ICC = 0.75 or higher on either the Global Items or all SANS items (Ventura et al., 1993a).
Global Functioning Scale: Role (GFS: Role) and Global Functioning Scale: Social (GFS: Social) (Cornblatt et al., 2007; Niendam, Bearden, Johnson, & Cannon, 2006). The GFS: Role is a 10-point rating scale that measures a combination of the quantity and quality of work/school functioning. The GFS: Social is a 10-point rating scale that measures a combination of the quantity and quality of social interactions with family and friends. Both versions of the GFS ratings were completed at study entry and at 6 months based on their ongoing interactions with the patients and family members. Raters were individual therapists who were trained raters, with mean ICC of ICC = 0.91 for GRS: Role and ICC = 0.76 for the GFS: Social.
Neurocognition and social cognition
MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) was administered at Baseline to provide a standardized assessment of cognitive functioning (Nuechterlein et al., 2008). This battery assesses seven domains: Speed of Processing, Attention/Vigilance, Working Memory, Verbal Learning, Visual Learning, Reasoning and Problem Solving, and Social Cognition. The dependent variable was the MCCB Overall Composite Score and the individual domain scores for which we used a scaled T scores based on a community sample (Kern et al., 2008b). Due to the focus on sex differences, we did not use T scores corrected for gender and age.
The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT) Part III uses video clips depicted individuals either lying or expressing sarcasm (McDonald, Flanagan, Rollins, & Kinch, 2003). After each item (video clip and hint), the participants were asked four questions meant to gauge their ability to make inferences about the characters' actions, intentions, thoughts and feelings. A total score was calculated across lie and sarcasm items (Kern et al., 2008a).
Relationships Across Domains (RAD) is a 75-item paper-and-pencil assessment of an individual's perception of various aspects of relationships. The RAD has good internal consistency in schizophrenia patients and normal controls (Sergi et al., 2009). The RAD reflects the ability of individuals to use their implicit knowledge of four relational models to comprehend social relationships and infer the behavior of social partners. The RAD contains 25 vignettes. After each vignette, participants were asked yes-no questions that evaluate their implicit knowledge of the relational model. The dependent measure was the total score across the four relational models.
Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test 2.0 (MSCEIT) is a self-report measure consisting of 141 items and 8 subscales that evaluate four components (branches) of emotion processing (Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2002). The MSCEIT has been shown to demonstrate good reliability and discriminant validity in schizophrenia (Eack et al., 2010; Eack, Greeno, Christian-Michaels, Dennis, & Anderson, 2009; Kee et al., 2009). The MISCEIT contains four branches: Identifying Emotions, Using Emotions, Understanding Emotions, and Managing Emotions. In this study we used a global measure from the MSCEIT, Emotional IQ (EIQ).
Insight
Scale to Assess Unawareness of Mental Disorder (SUMD) (Amador, Strauss, Yale, & Gorman, 1993) is a multi-dimensional scale based on a semi-structured interview. The SUMD items and subscales define specific components of insight – General Unawareness of (Having a) Mental Disorder, Unawareness of the Achieved Effects of Medication, Unawareness of Symptoms, Misattribution of Symptoms, and Unawareness of the Social Consequences of (Having a) Mental Disorder. To reduce the number of relationships examined, we chose one primary variable to approximate overall insight, i.e., General Unawareness of (Having a) Mental Disorder. This primary item represents an individual's lack of understanding that the symptoms or experiences of the prior psychotic episode at study entry were part of a mental illness. SUMD items are scored from 1 (aware) to 5 (unaware) so higher scores indicate greater unawareness. Additional components of unawareness were examined in secondary analyses after the primary variable of general unawareness of mental disorders was analyzed for a sex difference.
Statistical approach
We conducted t test, ANOVA, and Chi-square analyses to examine sex differences between male and female first-episode schizophrenia patients on variables collected during Study Entry, Baseline, and at 6 months after baseline.
There were significant relationships among several potential predictor variables such as premorbid adjustment, negative symptoms, and insight measured at baseline in relationship to role and social functioning at 6 months. Mediation analyses (Model One) were used to evaluate whether the relationship between sex and role functioning (school and work) was mediated by these predictor variables (Fig. 1a). We also examined (Model Two) the indirect effect of premorbid adjustment, negative symptoms, or insight on the relationship between sex and social functioning (friends and family) (Fig.1b). Analyses were conducted on functioning at the 6-month point. We followed the well-established procedures and conceptual understanding for mediation provided by Baron and Kenny (1986) and using bootstrap confidence intervals for the indirect effects of the mediators (Preacher & Hayes, 2004).
Results
Demographic information and a statistical analysis of sample characteristics can be found in Table 1 which contains a list of variables, means, standard deviations, F or t test values, and p values.
Table 1.
Variables of interest | Total sample mean (s.d.) % | Males (n = 105) | Females (n = 32) | F, t, or χ2 Statistic | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age at study entry | 22.0 (4.1) | 21.5 (3.2) | 23.8 (5.7) | 8.45 | 0.004 |
Education | 12.5 (1.7) | 12.4 (1.6) | 12.8 (1.9) | 1.57 | 0.212 |
Marital status | Single 133 (96%) Married 4 (4%) |
104 (99%) 1 (1%) |
29 (91%) 3 (9%) |
χ2 4.94 |
0.026 |
Premorbid adjustmenta | |||||
Childhood | 1.40 (0.95) | 1.52 (0.94) | 1.03 (0.92) | 6.13 | 0.015 |
Early adolescence | 1.64 (0.96) | 1.76 (0.94) | 1.28 (0.93) | 5.67 | 0.019 |
Late adolescence | 1.91 (1.0) | 2.05 (1.0) | 1.48 (1.0) | 6.90 | 0.010 |
Adulthood | 2.32 (1.4) | 2.54 (1.4) | 1.56 (1.2) | 8.90 | 0.004 |
Total premorbid adjustment score | 1.85 (0.84) | 2.01 (0.81) | 1.34 (0.76) | 13.6 | 0.000 |
Age of onsetb | 20.9 (4.1) | 20.3 (3.3) | 22.8 (5.6) | 9.52 | 0.002 |
Duration of psychotic symptomsb (months) | 8.11 (8.7) | 8.57 (9.1) | 6.42 (6.7) | 1.24 | 0.266 |
Total time Ill (including prodrome)b (months) | 12.18 (11.1) | 12.73 (10.8) | 10.14 (12.2) | 1.02 | 0.315 |
Duration of untreated psychosis (DUP)b | 20.1 (22.5) | 18.5 (20.4) | 24.6 (27.2) | 1.63 | 0.204 |
BPRS symptomsc | |||||
Negative | 2.28 (1.0) | 2.38 (1.1) | 1.94 (0.9) | 4.11 | 0.044 |
Positive | 3.60 (1.5) | 3.57 (1.5) | 3.69 (1.5) | 0.16 | 0.684 |
Mood | 2.63 (1.2) | 2.57 (1.2) | 2.81 (1.3) | 0.80 | 0.372 |
SANS negative symtomsd | |||||
Expressive | 2.04 (1.1) | 2.17 (1.1) | 1.60 (1.1) | 4.72 | 0.032 |
Experiential | 2.89 (1.0) | 3.00 (0.9) | 2.54 (1.2) | 3.57 | 0.062 |
SANS Total | 2.48 (0.9) | 2.58 (0.9) | 2.08 (1.1) | 5.40 | 0.022 |
Global daily functioninge | |||||
Role (work and school) baseline | 4.21 (2.2) | 3.85 (2.0) | 5.35 (2.4) | 9.66 | 0.002 |
Role six months | 5.27 (2.5) | 4.75 (2.4) | 6.95 (2.0) | 13.0 | <0.001 |
Social (friends and family) baseline | 5.95 (1.8) | 5.75 (1.8) | 6.62 (1.6) | 4.58 | 0.034 |
Social six months | 6.32 (1.8) | 6.09 (1.9) | 7.05 (1.6) | 4.07 | 0.047 |
Insight into mental disorder (SUMD)f | |||||
General unawareness of (having a) mental disorder | 2.62 (1.5) | 2.81 (1.5) | 2.00 (1.2) | 5.76 | 0.018 |
Unawareness of the benefits of medication | 2.25 (1.5) | 2.47 (1.6) | 1.58 (0.98) | 7.07 | 0.009 |
Unawareness of past delusions | 3.75 (2.0) | 4.04 (2.1) | 2.92 (1.3) | 5.98 | 0.016 |
Misattribution of past delusions | 2.82 (1.9) | 3.06 (2.0) | 2.04 (1.5) | 5.49 | 0.080 |
Unawareness of past hallucinations | 4.23 (2.6) | 4.43 (2.6) | 2.60 (2.7) | 1.81 | 0.181 |
Misattribution of past hallucinations | 2.99 (2.6) | 3.13 (2.6) | 2.56 (2.8) | 0.84 | 0.361 |
Unawareness of the social consequences (of having) a mental disorder | 1.90 (1.5) | 2.09 (1.6) | 1.29 (0.9) | 5.16 | 0.025 |
Cannon-Spoor Premorbid Adjustment Scale.
Psychiatric and Social History Form.
Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS).
Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS)
Global Functioning Scale: Social and Role.
Scale to assess unawareness of mental disorder; for GFS analyses at 6 months, total n = 84, n = 60 males; n = 24 females.
We found that males compared to females had lower levels of premorbid adjustment across the four key developmental periods including Childhood (p = 0.015), Early Adolescence (p = 0.019), Late Adolescence (p = 0.010), and Adulthood (p = 0.004), and had an earlier age of onset, M = 21.7 (s.d. = 3.2) v. 23.7 (s.d. = 4.8), p = 0.004 (Table 1). In keeping with the analyses of premorbid adjustment, we found at study entry that males were rated lower than females in Role Functioning (p = 0.002) and Social Functioning (p = 0.034) and at 6 months (Table 1). Symptom assessments at study entry indicated that males as compared to females exhibited higher levels of negative symptoms (p = 0.044), but that there were no differences between the two groups in positive symptoms (p = 0.684) or mood symptoms (p = 0.372). In addition, we found that at baseline females had better insight than males (p = 0.018; Table 1) in that they scored lower on Unawareness of Having a Mental Disorder. Also, secondary analyses revealed that females were significantly lower (better insight) than males on Unawareness of the Benefits of Medication, Unawareness of Past Delusions, and Unawareness of the Social Consequences of Having a Mental Disorder (Table 1).
Because sex differences in some cognitive domains tend to be present in the general population, we examined cognitive functioning on the MCCB at baseline through Diagnosis (Schizophrenia v. Healthy Comparison) × Sex (Male v. Female) ANOVAs (Table 2). We found no significant Diagnosis × Sex interactions for the Overall Composite Score or any domain score. Further, there were no statistically significant Diagnosis × Sex interactions at baseline on any of the measures of social cognition. As expected, both male and female patients performed more poorly compared to their respective control group.
Table 2.
Variables of interest | Patients total sample Mean (s.d.) | Patients males Mean (s.d.) | Patients females Mean (s.d.) | Healthy controls total sample Mean (s.d.) | Control males Mean (s.d.) | Control females Mean (s.d.) | F ratio/p value for Group x sex interaction |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neurocognition | |||||||
MCCBa overall | |||||||
Composite | 43.52 (14.1) | 43.0 (13.5) | 44.59 (15.5) | 52.5 (10.3) | 52.0 (10.5) | 53.2 (10.2) | 449 / 0.50 |
Speed of processing | 39.5 (11.2) | 39.8 (11.4) | 38.5 (11.1) | 53.7 (9.4) | 53.2 (9.3) | 54.5 (9.7) | 0.54 / 0.46 |
Attention/Vigilance | 38.7 (11.6) | 39.2 (11.2) | 37.4 (12.9) | 51.7 (9.7) | 52.2 (9.5) | 50.9 (10.1) | 0.01 / 0.90 |
Working memory | 46.5 (13.4) | 47.7 (13.2) | 42.9 (13.7) | 52.5 (11.5) | 53.1 (12.0) | 51.7 (10.9) | 0.69 / 0.40 |
Verbal learning | 39.6 (9.6) | 39.7 (8.9) | 39.4 (11.0) | 50.8 (9.9) | 49.7 (9.5) | 52.6 (10.3) | 0.97 / 0.32 |
Visual learning | 44.3 (10.4) | 44.9 (10.4) | 42.7 (10.4) | 49.9 (9.8) | 49.3 (11.0) | 51.0 (7.8) | 1.35 / 0.24 |
Reasoning problem | |||||||
Solving | 47.3 (8.9) | 48.4 (8.8) | 44.1 (8.5) | 51.7 (9.4) | 52.3 (9.0) | 50.8 (10.1) | 0.89 / 0.34 |
Social cognition | 37.3 (12.9) | 37.1 (12.7) | 37.8 (14.0) | 51.5 (8.9) | 50.4 (9.1) | 53.2 (8.4) | 0.29 /.58 |
Social cognition | |||||||
TASITb | 22.0 (4.2) | 22.3 (5.2) | 21.0 (4.1) | 25.9 (3.7) | 26.9 (4.0) | 24.5 (2.7) | 0.374 / 0.54 |
RADc | 47.8 (8.8) | 47.5 (9.1) | 48.8 (8.0) | 59.8 (5.2) | 60.1 (4.6) | 59.3 (6/1) | 0.564 / 0.45 |
MSCEITd | 88.2 (15.2) | 88.1 (15.1) | 88.3 (15.8) | 100.3(12.1) | 101.6 (12.5) | 98.2 (11.5) | 0.462 / 0.50 |
MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB) T scores are uncorrected for age and gender.
The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT).
Relationships Across Domains (RAD).
Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test 2.0 (MSCEIT); Patients: MCCB n = 98, TASIT n = 82; RAD; n = 83; MSCEIT n = 86; Controls: MCCB n = 92, TASIT n = 39, RAD n = 52, MSCEIT n = 45.
Given the significant sex differences in premorbid adjustment, and baseline negative symptoms and insight we conducted correlational analyses which indicated that better total premorbid adjustment was predictive of better role and social functioning at 6 months (Table 3). Lower negative symptoms as measured by the SANS were correlated with higher role functioning at 6 months and higher social functioning at 6 months (Table 3). In addition, correlational analyses indicated that better insight into having a mental illness at baseline was predictive of better role and social functioning 6 months later.
Table 3.
Role and social functioning | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | 6 Month | |||
Role | Social | Role | Social | |
Premorbid adjustment1 | ||||
Childhood | −0.18+ | −0.15 | −0.33** | −0.06 |
Early adolescence | −0.16 | −0.29** | −0.32** | −0.16 |
Late adolescence | −0.12 | −0.29** | −0.28* | −0.25* |
Adulthood | −0.29* | −0.22* | −0.37** | −0.37** |
Premorbid adjustment (Total) | −0.25* | −0.32** | −0.47** | −0.32** |
Age of onset | 0.15 | 0.08 | 0.13 | 0.17 |
Negative symptoms (at baseline) | ||||
BPRS negative symptoms | −0.21* | −0.32** | −0.10 | −0.13 |
SANS expressive | −0.22* | −0.49** | −0.28* | −0.43** |
SANS experiential | −0.45** | −0.64** | −0.36** | −0.52** |
SANS total | −0.38** | −0.64** | −0.37** | −0.55** |
Insight at study entry | ||||
General unawareness (of having) a mental disorder | −0.14+ | −0.28** | −0.38* | −0.45** |
Unawareness of benefits of medication | −0.18 | −0.23* | −0.21* | −0.34** |
Unawareness of past delusions | −0.11 | −0.30* | −0.28* | −0.34* |
Unawareness of the social consequences (of having) a mental disorder | −0.15 | −0.29* | −0.28* | −0.19+ |
*p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; + p < 0.10; n = 60 males; n = 24 females.
Mediation analysis
We conducted analyses to determine if the relationship between sex and role functioning (work or school) at 6 months was mediated by any of three potential mediators: premorbid adjustment, negative symptoms, and insight (see Fig. 1 for model). The mediation path (indirect effect) through premorbid adjustment was significant (95% CI 0.22–1.4). After taking the mediation path through premorbid adjustment into account, sex does still have a direct effect on the outcome (95% CI 0.42–2.94) so premorbid adjustment is considered a partial mediator. The mediation path (indirect effect) through insight was significant (95% CI 0.00–0.74). After taking the mediation path through insight into account, sex does still have a direct effect on the outcome (95% CI 0.80–3.22) so insight was considered a partial mediator. However, the mediation path (indirect effect) through negative symptoms was not significant (95% CI −0.17 to 0.76).
We also conducted analyses to determine if the relationship between sex and social functioning at 6 months was mediated by any of the same three potential mediators (see Fig. 1b for model). The mediation path (indirect effect) through premorbid adjustment was significant (95% CI 0.02–0.86). After taking the mediation path through premorbid adjustment into account, sex does not have a direct effect on the outcome (95% CI −0.040 to 2.09) so premorbid adjustment is a full mediator. The mediation path (indirect effect) of insight was significant (95% CI 0.074–0.927). After taking the mediation path through insight into account, sex does not have a direct effect on the social functioning (95% CI −0.29 to 1.52) so insight is considered a full mediator. However, the mediation path (indirect effect) through negative symptoms was not significant (95% CI −0.15 to 0.96).
Discussion
Our analyses are consistent with prior studies showing robust sex differences in a rigorously defined cohort of FES patients across several developmental, clinical, insight, and functional domains. Male individuals compared to females had lower levels of premorbid adjustment, an earlier age of onset, higher levels of negative symptoms, and lower levels of social and work functioning. In contrast, there were no sex differences in positive or mood symptoms. At study entry, females had better insight into having a mental illness and better insight into the benefits of medication and the social consequences of a disorder. Differences in premorbid adjustment, negative symptoms, and insight significantly predicted role and social functioning at 6 months for the entire sample. One key finding of our study is that premorbid adjustment and insight were mediators of the relationship between sex and functional outcome (role and social), while negative symptoms were not. Specifically, the mediation analyses indicate that pathways from the patient's sex to role and social functioning (school and work) go through premorbid adjustment and insight, suggesting that higher levels of functioning observed in females can partially be explained by the influence of sex on premorbid adjustment and insight. Although both males and females with schizophrenia performed more poorly than did healthy controls on measures of neurocognition and social cognition, no statistically significant diagnosis by sex interactions were found in cognitive performance.
The important role of premorbid functioning is consistent with prior studies which show an association with a better prognosis in first episode patients (Albert et al., 2011; Amminger et al., 2011). In addition, most studies have found that sex differences in premorbid functioning predict better functioning in women compared to men up to 3 years (Ayesa-Arriola et al., 2020). In addition, individuals with schizophrenia with higher levels of premorbid functioning generally demonstrate better treatment response to medications compared to individuals with lower levels (Hatzimanolis et al., 2020; Klein & Rosen, 1973; Rabinowitz et al., 2011; Strous et al., 2004).
Males typically have an earlier age of onset (Immonen, Jaaskelainen, Korpela, & Miettunen, 2017; Miettunen, Immonen, McGrath, Isohanni, & Jääskeläinen, 2019; Neill et al., 2020) and this finding has been extended to first episode patients, for a review see Ochoa (Ochoa et al., 2012). That is consistent with the view that males have a more severe form of the illness compared to females (Abel et al., 2010; Immonen et al., 2017; Rabinowitz, Levine, & Hafner, 2006). Indeed, the earlier the onset of symptoms, the less time an individual has to develop socially or academically before functional impairment occurs due to schizophrenia.
The observation of a relationship in our sample between greater baseline insight and higher role and social functioning at 6-months suggests that insight is an important predictor of the early course of schizophrenia. The finding that insight is a mediator of the relationship between premorbid adjustment and role functioning is important because there are sex differences in insight early in the course of schizophrenia and better insight is linked to lower levels of symptoms and better functioning (Cobo et al., 2016, 2020; Pousa et al., 2017; Ramu, Kolliakou, Sanyal, Patel, & Stewart, 2019). Insight into one's illness indicates the ability to reflect on one's experiences, which is likely related to the ability to understand the experiences of others as well. These Theory of Mind skills are related to better insight (Subotnik et al., 2020). Thus, the better awareness of a mental disorder in females at baseline might help to explain their better course and functional outcome. The implication is that EIS for people with schizophrenia might focus more attention for males on the development of insight, as this may lead to better functional outcome.
Broader issues in sex-related cultural stereotypes in expectations of males compared to females might interact with the clinical mediators we identified to influence the course of the illness. Congruent with gender stereotype theory, adolescent men may feel greater pressure to leave home and begin a career at an earlier age than do females (Hentschel, Heilman, & Peus, 2019). Also, several investigators have hypothesized that females tend to possess stereotypically female gender traits, such as acceptance of their illness and treatment adherence, while males tend to exhibit more socially adverse and self-destructive behaviors and attitudes (Morgan, Castle, & Jablensky, 2008; Riecher-Rössler & Häfner, 2000). The greater insight into having a mental illness that we found in females may represent an aspect of this acceptance of illness. On a related topic, whether socioeconomic or cultural influences on gender identity are a stronger influence than sex at birth on premorbid adjustment and insight into illness needs more investigation (Lewine et al., 2006).
Biological factors may also act as a mediator which could be an additional influence on the more favorable course observed in females compared with males with schizophrenia. In fact, the level of estrogen has been suggested as a protective factor (da Silva & Ravindran, 2015; Falkenberg & Tracy, 2014; Gogos et al., 2015; Hafner et al., 1994; Salem & Kring, 1998). The estrogen hypothesis postulates that the vulnerability threshold for schizophrenia is mediated by the estrogen levels in women until menopause (Abel et al., 2010; Gogos et al., 2015; Olsen et al., 2008). Estrogen is known to protect against prenatal complications and excessive synaptic pruning and may also have a antipsychotic-like effect by impacting post-synaptic dopaminergic signal transduction and reducing dopamine-mediated psychotic symptoms (Seeman, 2012). Hence, the modulating role of estrogen may be an additional factor in the later onset, better role and social functioning, and lower levels of negative symptoms observed in the early phase of illness.
We did not find any Diagnosis by Sex interactions in neurocognitive or social cognitive performance. Some prior studies that found sex differences in cognition among schizophrenia patients did not include a healthy control group and did not use cognitive scores that were corrected for sex differences in the general population. Thus, many of the prior reports of sex differences in cognitive performance among schizophrenia patients might be due to general population differences rather than to the impact of schizophrenia.
The study had some limitations. Some patients may not accurately recall how long ago their psychotic symptoms began. We believe that our use of parental and professional reports as well as medical records to minimize this issue. As is the case with many studies of individuals with schizophrenia, our sample size for females is smaller than for males. However, we were able to detect significant sex differences with this sample size of females. Because we received most of our referrals from hospitals at which patients were acutely ill, sample selection may favor those patients who required a hospitalization. Men or women who never needed hospitalization may therefore be underrepresented. Our sample consists of individuals who are willing to participate in clinical research. However, how these factors would differentially impact participation of males v. females to produce the sex differences we found is not obvious.
To summarize, in examining sex differences between males and female individuals with schizophrenia, we found many elements of consistency with the prior literature which indicates that males have poorer premorbid adjustment, an earlier age of onset, higher negative symptoms, and lower levels of role and social functioning. We found that predictors of functioning, such as premorbid adjustment and insight partially mediated the relationship between sex and role functioning and fully mediated the relationship between sex and social functioning. These findings underscore that males are more impaired than females and have prognostic implications for the impact of sex on the early course of schizophrenia.
References
- Abel, K. M., Drake, R., & Goldstein, J. M. (2010). Sex differences in schizophrenia. International Review of Psychiatry, 22(5), 417–428. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2010.515205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Addington, J., & Addington, D. (2005). Patterns of premorbid functioning in first episode psychosis: Relationship to 2-year outcome. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112(1), 40–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00511.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albert, N., Bertelsen, M., Thorup, A., Petersen, L., Jeppesen, P., Le Quack, P., … Nordentoft, M. (2011). Predictors of recovery from psychosis analyses of clinical and social factors associated with recovery among patients with first-episode psychosis after 5 years. Schizophrenia Research, 125(2–3), 257–266. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aleman, A., Kahn, R. S., & Selten, J. P. (2003). Sex differences in the risk of schizophrenia: Evidence from meta-analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(6), 565–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.6.565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amador, X., Strauss, D., Yale, S., & Gorman, J. (1993). Assessment of insight in psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150(6), 873–879. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.6.873 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amminger, G. P., Henry, L. P., Harrigan, S. M., Harris, M. G., Alvarez-Jimenez, M., Herrman, H., … McGorry, P. D. (2011). Outcome in early-onset schizophrenia revisited: Findings from the early psychosis prevention and intervention centre long-term follow-up study. Schizophrenia Research, 131(1–3), 112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen, N. C. (1984). The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, IA: The University of Iowa. [Google Scholar]
- Ayesa-Arriola, R., de la Foz, V. O., Setien-Suero, E., Ramirez-Bonilla, M. L., Suarez-Pinilla, P., Son, J. M., … Crespo-Facorro, B. (2020). Understanding sex differences in long-term outcomes after a first episode of psychosis. NPJ Schizophrenia, 6(1), 33. doi: 10.1038/s41537-020-00120-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ayesa-Arriola, R., Rodriguez-Sanchez, J. M., Morelli, C., Pelayo-Teran, J. M., Perez-Iglesias, R., Mata, I., … Crespo-Facorro, B. (2011). Insight dimensions in first-episode psychosis patients: Clinical, cognitive, pre-morbid and socio-demographic correlates. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 5(2), 140–149. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7893.2010.00249.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barajas, A., Ochoa, S., Obiols, J. E., & Lalucat-Jo, L. (2015). Gender differences in individuals at high-risk of psychosis: A comprehensive literature review. TheScientificWorldJournal, 2015, 430735. doi: 10.1155/2015/430735 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brill, N., Levine, S., Reichenberg, A., Lubin, G., Weiser, M., & Rabinowitz, J. (2009). Pathways to functional outcomes in schizophrenia: The role of premorbid functioning, negative symptoms and intelligence. Schizophrenia Research, 110(1–3), 40–46. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.02.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buck, G., Lavigne, K. M., Makowski, C., Joober, R., Malla, A., & Lepage, M. (2020). Sex differences in verbal memory predict functioning through negative symptoms in early psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(6), 1587–1595. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cannon-Spoor, H. E., Potkin, S. G., & Wyatt, R. J. (1982). Measurement of premorbid adjustment in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 8, 470–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang, W. C., Hui, C. L., Tang, J. Y., Wong, G. H., Lam, M. M., Chan, S. K., & Chen, E. Y. (2011). Persistent negative symptoms in first-episode schizophrenia: A prospective three-year follow-up study. Schizophrenia Research, 133(1–3), 22–28. doi: S0920-9964(11)00485-3 [pii] 10.1016/j.schres.2011.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cobo, J., Labad, J., Pousa, E., Nieto, L., Ochoa, S., Usall, J., … Ruiz, A. I. (2020). Exploring the relationship of insight with psychopathology and gender in individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders with structural equation modelling. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 23(5), 643–655. doi: 10.1007/s00737-020-01031-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cobo, J., Nieto, L., Ochoa, S., Pousa, E., Usall, J., Banos, I., … Ruiz, A. I. (2016). Insight and gender in schizophrenia and other psychoses. Psychiatry Research, 243, 268–277. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornblatt, B. A., Auther, A. M., Niendam, T., Smith, C. W., Zinberg, J., Bearden, C. E., & Cannon, T. D. (2007). Preliminary findings for two new measures of social and role functioning in the prodromal phase of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(3), 688–702. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotton, S. M., Lambert, M., Schimmelmann, B. G., Foley, D. L., Morley, K. I., McGorry, P. D., & Conus, P. (2009). Gender differences in premorbid, entry, treatment, and outcome characteristics in a treated epidemiological sample of 661 patients with first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 114(1–3), 17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Danaher, H., Allott, K., Killackey, E., Hester, R., & Cotton, S. (2018). An examination of sex differences in neurocognition and social cognition in first-episode psychosis. Psychiatry Research, 259, 36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- da Silva, T. L., & Ravindran, A. V. (2015). Contribution of sex hormones to gender differences in schizophrenia: A review. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 18, 2–14. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2015.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eack, S. M., Greeno, C. G., Christian-Michaels, S., Dennis, A., & Anderson, C. M. (2009). Case managers’ perspectives on what they need to do their job. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 32(4), 309–312. doi: F53W683PH4583872 [pii] 10.2975/32.4.2009.309.312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eack, S. M., Greeno, C. G., Pogue-Geile, M. F., Newhill, C. E., Hogarty, G. E., & Keshavan, M. S. (2010). Assessing social-cognitive deficits in schizophrenia with the Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(2), 370–380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falkenberg, J., & Tracy, D. K. (2014). Sex and schizophrenia: A review of gender differences. Psychosis, 6(1), 61–69. doi: 10.1080/17522439.2012.733405. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fu, S., Czajkowski, N., Rund, B. R., & Torgalsbøen, A. K. (2017). The relationship between level of cognitive impairments and functional outcome trajectories in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 190, 144–149. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogos, A., Sbisa, A. M., Sun, J., Gibbons, A., Udawela, M., & Dean, B. (2015). A role for estrogen in schizophrenia: Clinical and preclinical findings. International Journal of Endocrinology, 2015, 615356. doi: 10.1155/2015/615356 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hafner, H., Maurer, K., Loffler, W., Fatkenheuer, B., an der Heiden, W., Riecher-Rossler, A., … Gattaz, W. F. (1994). The epidemiology of early schizophrenia. Influence of age and gender on onset and early course. The British journal of psychiatry. Supplement, 164(23), 29–38. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8037899. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatzimanolis, A., Stefanatou, P., Kattoulas, E., Ralli, I., Dimitrakopoulos, S., Foteli, S., … Stefanis, N. C. (2020). Familial and socioeconomic contributions to premorbid functioning in psychosis: Impact on age at onset and treatment response. European Psychiatry, 63(1), e44. doi: 10.1192/j.eurpsy.2020.41 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hentschel, T., Heilman, M. E., & Peus, C. V. (2019). The multiple dimensions of gender stereotypes: A current look at men's and women's characterizations of others and themselves. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horan, W. P., Green, M. F., DeGroot, M., Fiske, A., Hellemann, G., Kee, K., … Nuechterlein, K. H. (2012). Social cognition in schizophrenia, part 2: 12–month stability and prediction of functional outcome in first-episode patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 865–872. doi: sbr001 [pii] 10.1093/schbul/sbr001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hui, C. L., Leung, C. M., Chang, W. C., Chan, S. K., Lee, E. H., & Chen, E. Y. (2016). Examining gender difference in adult-onset psychosis in Hong Kong. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 10(4), 324–333. doi: 10.1111/eip.12167 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Immonen, J., Jaaskelainen, E., Korpela, H., & Miettunen, J. (2017). Age at onset and the outcomes of schizophrenia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 11(6), 453–460. doi: 10.1111/eip.12412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kee, K. S., Horan, W. P., Salovey, P., Kern, R. S., Sergi, M. J., Fiske, A. P., … Green, M. F. (2009). Emotional intelligence in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 107(1), 61–68. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=18805674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kern, R. S., Green, M. F., Fiske, A. P., Kee, K. S., Lee, J., Sergi, M. J., … Nuechterlein, K. H. (2008a). Theory of mind deficits for processing counterfactual information in persons with chronic schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 39(4), 645–654. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003966 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kern, R. S., Nuechterlein, K. H., Green, M. F., Baade, L. E., Fenton, W. S., Gold, J. M., … Marder, S. R. (2008b). The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, part 2: Co-norming and standardization. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(2), 214–220. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=18172018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keshavan, M. S., Rabinowitz, J., DeSmedt, G., Harvey, P. D., & Schooler, N. (2004). Correlates of insight in first episode psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 70(2–3), 187–194. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=15329295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein, D. F., & Rosen, B. (1973). Premorbid asocial adjustment and response to phenothiazine treatment among schizophrenic inpatients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 29(4), 480–485. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1973.04200040034006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewine, R. R., Thurston-Snoha, B.-J., & Ardery, R. (2006). Sex, gender, and neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(8), 1362–1372. doi: 10.1080/13803390500473738 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2002). Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) user's manual. Toronto: Multi-Health System Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- McDonald, S., Flanagan, S., Rollins, J., & Kinch, J. (2003). TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 18(3), 219–238. doi: 10.1097/00001199-200305000-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEvoy, J. P., Johnson, J., Perkins, D., Lieberman, J. A., Hamer, R. M., Keefe, R. S., … Sharma, T. (2006). Insight in first-episode psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 36(10), 1385–1393. doi: S0033291706007793 [pii] 10.1017/S0033291706007793 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mesholam-Gately, R. I., Giuliano, A. J., Goff, K. P., Faraone, S. V., & Seidman, L. J. (2009). Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology, 23(3), 315–336. doi: 10.1037/a0014708 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miettunen, J., Immonen, J., McGrath, J. J., Isohanni, M., & Jääskeläinen, E. (2019). The age of onset of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. In de Girolamo G., McGorry P. D., & Sartorius N. (Eds.), Age of onset of mental disorders (pp. 55–73). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Milev, P. (2005). Predictive values of neurocognition and negative symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia: A longitudinal first-episode study with 7-year follow-up. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(3), 495–506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minor, K. S., Friedman-Yakoobian, M., Leung, Y. J., Meyer, E. C., Zimmet, S. V., Caplan, B., … Keshavan, M. S. (2015). The impact of premorbid adjustment, neurocognition, and depression on social and role functioning in patients in an early psychosis treatment program. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 49(5), 444–452. doi: 10.1177/0004867414565473 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan, V. A., Castle, D. J., & Jablensky, A. V. (2008). Do women express and experience psychosis differently from men? Epidemiological evidence from the Australian national study of low prevalence (Psychotic) disorders. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 42(1), 74–82. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mote, J., & Kring, A. M. (2016). Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: Does sex matter? World Journal of Psychiatry, 6(2), 257–268. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v6.i2.257 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mutsatsa, S. H., Joyce, E. M., Hutton, S. B., & Barnes, T. R. (2006). Relationship between insight, cognitive function, social function and symptomatology in schizophrenia: The West London first episode study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 256(6), 356–363. doi: 10.1007/s00406-006-0645-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navarra-Ventura, G., Fernandez-Gonzalo, S., Turon, M., Pousa, E., Palao, D., Cardoner, N., & Jodar, M. (2018). Gender differences in social cognition: A cross-sectional pilot study of recently diagnosed patients with schizophrenia and healthy subjects. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 63(8), 538–546. doi: 10.1177/0706743717746661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neill, E., Tan, E. J., Toh, W. L., Selvendra, A., Morgan, V. A., Rossell, S. L., & Castle, D. J. (2020). Examining which factors influence age of onset in males and females with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 223, 265–270. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.08.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niendam, T. A., Bearden, C. E., Johnson, J. K., & Cannon, T. D. (2006). Global functioning scale: Role (GFS: Role). rating scale. Los Angeles: University of California, Los Angeles. [Google Scholar]
- Nuechterlein, K. H., Green, M. F., Kern, R. S., Baade, L. E., Barch, D. M., Cohen, J. D., … Marder, S. R. (2008). The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, part 1: Test selection, reliability, and validity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(2), 203–213. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=18172019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nuechterlein, K. H., Ventura, J., Subotnik, K. L., Gretchen-Doorly, D., Turner, L. R., Casaus, L. R., … Medalia, A. (2020). A randomized controlled trial of cognitive remediation and long-acting injectable risperidone after a first episode of schizophrenia: Improving cognition and work/school functioning. Psychological Medicine, 52(8), 1517–1526. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720003335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ochoa, S., Usall, J., Cobo, J., Labad, X., & Kulkarni, J. (2012). Gender differences in schizophrenia and first-episode psychosis: A comprehensive literature review. Schizophrenia Research and Treatment, 2012, 916198. doi: 10.1155/2012/916198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen, L., Hansen, T., Jakobsen, K. D., Djurovic, S., Melle, I., Agartz, I., … Werge, T. (2008). The estrogen hypothesis of schizophrenia implicates glucose metabolism: Association study in three independent samples. BMC Medical Genetics, 9, 39. doi: 10.1186/1471-2350-9-39 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pousa, E., Ochoa, S., Cobo, J., Nieto, L., Usall, J., Gonzalez, B., … Usall, J. (2017). A deeper view of insight in schizophrenia: Insight dimensions, unawareness and misattribution of particular symptoms and its relation with psychopathological factors. Schizophrenia Research, 189, 61–68. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.02.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(4), 717–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pu, C., Qiu, Y., Zhou, T., Yang, F., Lu, Z., Wang, C., … Yu, X. (2019). Gender differences of neurocognitive functioning in patients with first-episode schizophrenia in China. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 95, 152132. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2019.152132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabinowitz, J., Levine, S. Z., & Hafner, H. (2006). A population based elaboration of the role of age of onset on the course of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 88(1–3), 96–101. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabinowitz, J., Napryeyenko, O., Burba, B., Martinez, G., Neznanov, N. G., Fischel, T., … Schreiner, A. (2011). Premorbid functioning and treatment response in recent-onset schizophrenia: Prospective study with risperidone long-acting injectable. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 31(1), 75–81. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e31820568c6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramu, N., Kolliakou, A., Sanyal, J., Patel, R., & Stewart, R. (2019). Recorded poor insight as a predictor of service use outcomes: Cohort study of patients with first-episode psychosis in a large mental healthcare database. BMJ Open, 9(6), e028929. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-028929 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riecher-Rössler, A., & Häfner, H. (2000). Gender aspects in schizophrenia: Bridging the border between social and biological psychiatry. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica.102 Supplementum, 102(407), 58–62. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.00011.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salem, J. E., & Kring, A. M. (1998). The role of gender differences in the reduction of etiologic heterogeneity in schizophrenia. Clinical Psychological Review, 18(7), 795–819. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00008-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeman, M. V. (2012). Treating schizophrenia at the time of menopause. Maturitas, 72(2), 117–120. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2012.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sergi, M. J., Fiske, A. P., Horan, W. P., Kern, R. S., Kee, K. S., Subotnik, K. L., … Green, M. F. (2009). Development of a measure of relationship perception in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 166(1), 54–62. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=19193447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoffelmayr, B. E., Dillavou, D., & Hunter, J. E. (1983). Premorbid functioning and outcome in schizophrenia: A cumulative analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51(3), 338. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.51.3.338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strous, R. D., Alvir, J. M., Robinson, D., Gal, G., Sheitman, B., Chakos, M., & Lieberman, J. A. (2004). Premorbid functioning in schizophrenia: Relation to baseline symptoms, treatment response, and medication side effects. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30(2), 265–278. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007077 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Subotnik, K. L., Casaus, L. R., Ventura, J., Luo, J. S., Hellemann, G. S., Gretchen-Doorly, D., … Nuechterlein, K. H. (2015). Long-acting injectable risperidone for relapse prevention and control of breakthrough symptoms after a recent first episode of schizophrenia. A randomized clinical trial. Jama Psychiatry, 72(8), 822–829. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0270 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Subotnik, K. L., Ventura, J., Hellemann, G. S., Zito, M. F., Agee, E. R., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (2020). Relationship of poor insight to neurocognition, social cognition, and psychiatric symptoms in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 220, 164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.03.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szymanski, S., Lieberman, J. A., Alvir, J. M., Mayerhoff, D., Loebel, A., Geisler, S., … Cooper, T. (1995). Gender differences in onset of illness, treatment response, course, and biologic indexes in first-episode schizophrenic patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152(5), 698–703. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.5.698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorup, A., Albert, N., Bertelsen, M., Petersen, L., Jeppesen, P., Le Quack, P., … Nordentoft, M. (2014). Gender differences in first-episode psychosis at 5-year follow-up – two different courses of disease? Results from the OPUS study at 5–year follow-up. European Psychiatry, 29(1), 44–51. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2012.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorup, A., Petersen, L., Jeppesen, P., Ohlenschlaeger, J., Christensen, T., Krarup, G., … Nordentoft, M. (2007). Gender differences in young adults with first-episode schizophrenia spectrum disorders at baseline in the Danish OPUS study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195(5), 396–405. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000253784.59708.dd [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torgalsbøen, A.-K., Mohn, C., & Rund, B. R. (2014). Neurocognitive predictors of remission of symptoms and social and role functioning in the early course of first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 216(1), 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ventura, J., Green, M. F., Shaner, A., & Liberman, R. P. (1993a). Training and quality assurance with the brief psychiatric rating scale: “The drift busters.”. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 3(4), 221–244. [Google Scholar]
- Ventura, J., Lukoff, D., Nuechterlein, K. H., Liberman, R. P., Green, M. F., & Shaner, A. (1993b). Appendix 1: Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) expanded version (4.0): Scales, anchor points, and administration manual. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 3, 227–243. [Google Scholar]
- Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K. H., Subotnik, K. L., Gutkind, D., & Gilbert, E. A. (2000). Symptom dimensions in recent-onset schizophrenia and mania: A principal components analysis of the 24-item brief psychiatric rating scale. Psychiatry Research, 97, 129–135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdaguer-Rodríguez, M., López-Carrilero, R., Ferrer-Quintero, M., García-Mieres, H., Díaz-Cutraro, L., Grasa, E., … Peláez, T. (2021). Are there gender differences in social cognition in first-episode psychosis? Women, 1(4), 204–211. doi: 10.3390/women1040018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss, E., Siedentopf, C. M., Hofer, A., Deisenhammer, E. A., Hoptman, M. J., Kremser, C., … Delazer, M. (2003). Sex differences in brain activation pattern during a visuospatial cognitive task: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in healthy volunteers. Neuroscience Letters, 344(3), 169–172. 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00406-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang, M., Gao, S., Xiong, W., & Zhang, X. Y. (2021). Sex-differential associations between cognitive impairments and white matter abnormalities in first episode and drug-naive schizophrenia. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 15(5), 1179–1187. doi: 10.1111/eip.13059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]