Abstract
The mammalian hippocampal formation (HF) is organized into domains associated with different functions. These differences are driven in part by the pattern of input along the hippocampal long axis, such as visual input to the septal hippocampus and amygdalar input to temporal hippocampus. HF is also organized along the transverse axis, with different patterns of neural activity in the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex. In some birds, a similar organization has been observed along both of these axes. However, it is not known what role inputs play in this organization. We used retrograde tracing to map inputs into HF of a food-caching bird, the black-capped chickadee. We first compared two locations along the transverse axis: the hippocampus and the dorsolateral hippocampal area (DL), which is analogous to the entorhinal cortex. We found that pallial regions predominantly targeted DL, while some subcortical regions like the lateral hypothalamus (LHy) preferentially targeted the hippocampus. We then examined the hippocampal long axis and found that almost all inputs were topographic along this direction. For example, the anterior hippocampus was preferentially innervated by thalamic regions, while posterior hippocampus received more amygdalar input. Some of the topographies we found bear resemblance to those described in the mammalian brain, revealing a remarkable anatomical similarity of phylogenetically distant animals. More generally, our work establishes the pattern of inputs to HF in chickadees. Some of these patterns may be unique to chickadees, laying the groundwork for studying the anatomical basis of these birds’ exceptional hippocampal memory.
Keywords: Chickadee, bird, food-caching, hippocampus, entorhinal, topography, inputs, RRID
Graphical Abstract
This study cataloged the inputs of the hippocampal formation of a food-caching bird, the black-capped chickadee. The chickadee hippocampal formation receives input from the pallium, subpallium, diencephalon, midbrain, and brainstem, and these inputs were largely topographic along the transverse and long hippocampal axes. Many of these input gradients were similar to those found in the mammalian brain, revealing an unexpected anatomical similarity between phylogenetically distant animals.
Introduction
The hippocampus is necessary for memory formation in species from every class of vertebrates (Scoville and Milner, 1957; Krushinskaya, 1966; Rodríguez et al., 2002; Inés Sotelo et al., 2022). Different types of memory rely on the hippocampus. For example, rodents need their hippocampus for both ‘spatial’ memory (e.g., location of a hidden platform in the Morris water maze) and ‘emotional’ memory (e.g., link of a tone with a foot shock) (Morris et al., 1982; Kjelstrup et al., 2002; Maren and Holt, 2004). These distinct functions are anatomically organized along the dorso-ventral (DV) “long” axis of the hippocampus (Moser and Moser, 1998; Fanselow and Dong, 2010; Strange et al., 2014). The dorsal hippocampus is needed for spatial memory tasks (Cimadevilla et al., 2000; Barbosa et al., 2012), and neural recordings there have shown spatially tuned cells (Jung et al., 1994; Kjelstrup et al., 2008). In contrast, the ventral hippocampus has been implicated in a variety of emotional tasks, and neurons there exhibit far less spatial tuning (Henke, 1990; Kjelstrup et al., 2002).
In non-mammalian species, the hippocampus is also involved in both spatial and emotional behaviors (Krushinskaya, 1966; Sherry and Vaccarino, 1989; Reis et al., 1999; Damphousse et al., 2022). Anatomical organization of these functions seems to be similar to that in mammals. In birds, the hippocampal long axis extends in an antero-posterior (AP) direction, with anterior hippocampus likely homologous to the rodent dorsal hippocampus (Smulders, 2017; Herold et al., 2019; Payne et al., 2021; Damphousse et al., 2022). Anterior hippocampus contains spatially tuned neurons and is preferentially involved in spatial tasks, whereas posterior hippocampus is less spatially tuned and is involved in stress-related functions.
What drives these functional differences across the hippocampus? In mammals, functional organization is driven in part by the anatomy of inputs. For example, head direction signals from the anterior thalamus preferentially target the dorsal HF, where they likely contribute to spatial representations (Prasad and Chudasama, 2013). The amygdala, which has been implicated in emotionally driven behaviors, innervates ventral hippocampus and likely contributes to the emotionally linked deficits of ventral-hippocampal lesions (Van Groen and Wyss, 1990). However, it is unknown whether a similar organization of inputs along the long axis exists in non-mammals.
Further functional segmentation of the hippocampal formation (HF) occurs along the transverse axis. In mammals, there are major differences in input between the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex. For example, cortical inputs preferentially innervate the entorhinal cortex (Burwell and Amaral, 1998) while hypothalamic inputs preferentially innervate the hippocampus (Swanson and Cowan, 1975; Hahn and Swanson, 2012). These anatomical differences may have functional consequences, possibly driving differences in spatial representations between regions (O’Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971; Hafting et al., 2005). Birds also have an anatomical organization along the transverse axis, with a possible segregation into hippocampus-like and entorhinal-like regions. Although the exact boundary these regions have been debated (Sherry et al., 1989; Barnea and Nottebohm, 1994; Kahn et al., 2003; Atoji and Wild, 2005; Puelles et al., 2019), recent work has suggested the dorsolateral hippocampal formation (DL) as a functional analog of the medial entorhinal cortex (Applegate et al., 2023). Whereas the avian hippocampus contains place cells, (DL) contains many entorhinal-like firing patterns, including grid-like cells (Agarwal et al., 2021; Payne et al., 2021; Applegate et al., 2023). Again, it is unknown how the patterns of inputs into these regions compare to mammals.
We decided to study the organization of inputs to HF in a food-caching bird, the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). These animals are memory specialists, capable of remembering hidden locations of thousands of food items scattered throughout their environment (Pravosudov, 1985; Stevens and Krebs, 1986). These memories require the hippocampus (Krushinskaya, 1966; Sherry and Vaccarino, 1989). Previous studies have examined avian inputs to HF (Kraniak and Siegel, 1978; Casini et al., 1986; Székely and Krebs, 1996; Atoji et al., 2002; Atoji and Wild, 2004), but no anatomical tracing of HF has been done in a food-caching bird. In addition to analyzing inputs along the two axes of HF, we were interested in what may be unique to food-caching species. The hippocampus of food-caching birds is hypertrophic, roughly three times larger than the hippocampus of non-food-caching birds (Krebs et al., 1989; Garamszegi and Eens, 2004). There could be interesting differences in connectivity as well, further motivating the study of inputs to HF in food-caching birds.
Materials & methods
Experimental model and subjects
All animal procedures were approved by the Columbia University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and carried out in accordance with the US National Institutes of Health guidelines. The subjects were 22 black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus) collected from multiple sites in New York State using Federal and State scientific collection licenses. Subjects were at least 4 months old at the time of the experiment, but age was not determined more precisely.
Chickadees are not visibly sexually dimorphic; therefore, all surgical procedures were performed blindly to sex. Sex was determined post-mortem. Seven female and 14 male birds were used for retrograde tracing, and one male was used to create a 3D rendering of the brain.
Prior to experiments, all birds were housed in an aviary in groups of 1–3, on a ‘winter’ light cycle (9 h:15 h light:dark). Birds were given an ad-libitum supply of a small-bird diet (Small Bird Maintenance Diet, 0001452, Mazuri or Organic High Potency Fine Bird foods, HPF, Harrison’s Bird Foods). Upon transfer to the lab for experiments, birds had primary flight feathers trimmed and were individually housed.
Surgery
Surgeries were performed on birds after 2–16 months (9.6 ± 1.0, mean ± SEM) in captivity, using the same procedures as in our prior work (Applegate et al., 2023). For all surgical procedures, birds were anesthetized with 1–2% isoflurane in oxygen. Feathers were removed from the surgical site, and the skin was treated with Betadine. Birds were then placed into a stereotaxic apparatus for the duration of the surgery. The head was rotated to an angle appropriate for each experiment (Table 1), measured as the angle between the groove in the bone at the base of the upper beak mandible and the horizontal table surface. Throughout the surgery, saline was injected subcutaneously to prevent dehydration (~0.4 mL every 30–60 min). After the surgery, an analgesic (buprenorphine, 0.05 mg/kg) was injected intraperitoneally.
Table 1:
Location index | Region | Coordinate (mm) | Number of annotated injections | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
AP | ML | DV | Head angle | Pallial | Non-pallial | ||
1 | Hp | −1.0 | 1.9 | 0.4 | 90° | 4 | 3 |
2 | 1.0 | 0.4 | 0.4 | 75° | 5 | 4 | |
3 | 3.0 | 0.4 | 0.4 | 75° | 6 | 4 | |
4 | 5.0 | 0.4 | 1.0 | 75° | 6 | 3 | |
5 | DL | 2.0 | 2.3 | 0.4 | 75° | 5 | 4 |
6 | 3.5 | 1.7 | 0.4 | 75° | 4 | 4 | |
7 | 5.5 | 0.7 | 0.3 | 75° | 4 | 4 |
Coordinates and the number of annotated injections for all injection locations. Antero-posterior (AP) and medio-lateral (ML) coordinates are relative to lambda. Dorso-ventral (DV) coordinate is the depth relative to the brain surface. The number of pallial and non-pallial annotations is sometimes different due to birds in which the same wavelength of fluorophore was used bilaterally. In this case, only pallial regions were annotated,since they do not project contralaterally.
3D brain reconstruction
To visualize the 3D arrangement of brain structures, we constructed a template chickadee brain. In one bird, stiff wire (Malin Co., 0.006” music wire) was inserted horizontally from the lateral wall of the brain toward the midline to create two visible tracts. The bird was immediately perfused (see below), and following tissue fixation the brain was sliced sagittally. Slices were annotated (Adobe Illustrator) and aligned using custom MATLAB code, with the two lesions used for alignment.
Anatomical tracing experiments
For retrograde tracing, we used cholera toxin subunit B (CTB in 1X PBS, 0.4% weight/volume) fluorescently conjugated to Alexa Fluor in one of three different wavelengths (488 nm, 555 nm, and 647 nm; C34775, C34776, C34778, respectively, Invitrogen). We made injections into 1–4 locations per animal, using different wavelengths and sometimes different hemispheres for different locations in individual birds.
Injection coordinates for different brain regions are given in Table 1. The majority of the injection locations (2–7) were made at a 75° head angle. However, to access the very posterior Hp (location 1), the brain was rotated to a 90° head angle. If multiple injections were performed, lambda was relocated for each injection.
Hp injections (locations 1–4) were chosen to be evenly spaced along the AP axis. DL injections (locations 5–7) were chosen to be roughly at the centers of those portions of DL that were retrogradely labeled by Hp injections at locations 2–4 (Applegate et al., 2023). In other words, locations 2–4 were topographically matched to locations 5–7. For location 1, the corresponding portion of DL was too small for reliable injections and was omitted from this study.
Histology and histological imaging
All perfusions were performed 7 days after surgery. Animals were administered ketamine and xylazine (10 mg/kg and 4 mg/kg respectively), then transcardially perfused with 1X PBS followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. The brains were extracted and stored in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS for 2 days. They were then sectioned coronally into 100 |im-thick slices (Leica VT1000S vibratome). Sections were stained with fluorescent DAPI (300 nM in 1X PBS, D1306, Invitrogen) and mounted in Vectashield mounting medium (H-1400–10, Vector Laboratories). Sections were then imaged using a AZ100 Multizoom Slide Scanner (Nikon), using filters for DAPI and all injected fluorophores.
Quantification and statistical analysis
Quantification of labeled cells in anatomical tracing
All data will be uploaded to Dryad prior to publication. To visualize brain areas in 3D stereotaxic coordinates, we needed to align the images of each coronal section to the reference chickadee brain (described above). First, we found the coronal section corresponding to the center of one of the CTB injections. Since injections were at known stereotaxic coordinates, and the coronal sections were each 100 μm thick, we used this reference point to assign an AP position to each section. Next, we manually annotated the dorsal and ventral endpoints of the midline in each section. These were used to rotate the image of each section to make the midline vertical. The midline was assigned to be at 0 mm ML. The hemisphere ipsilateral to the injection was designated as positive ML, and the contralateral hemisphere was designated as negative ML. We then annotated the dorsal brain surface by using the MATLAB functions imbinarize and bwboundaries. This brain surface was used to manually align the sections dorso-ventrally to the reference brain. Finally, small manual corrections were made dorso-ventrally and medial-laterally to ensure that the reconstructed brain surface was smooth.
To quantify the density of retrograde labeling, we developed a procedure for automatically counting the number of labeled cell bodies. We decided to use cell counts instead of raw fluorescence because in many cases fluorescence of the neuropil was influenced by anterograde labeling of axons from injection site. For example, MS and NDB had strongly labeled fibers of passage in addition to the retrogradely labeled soma. We detected labelled cells using a standard Difference-of-Gaussians algorithm for blob detection. We used a series of five sigma values, logarithmically spaced from 2.5 to 12.5 μm - corresponding to blob sizes of 5 to 25 μm. The image of a brain section was smoothed with Gaussians of each of these sigma values. Successive pairs of smoothed images were subtracted from one another to compute four difference-of-Gaussians, and these differences were stacked into a 3-dimensional matrix. Local maxima exceeding 0.02 in this matrix were detected as the centers of the blobs. If two detected blobs were overlapping, the one with the lower value was eliminated.
Slices were then annotated manually to assign detected cells to regions of interest. The boundaries of these brain regions were determined by comparing the DAPI-stained images to published atlases from other species (Karten and Hodos, 1967; Stokes et al., 1974; Puelles et al., 2019). We found 15 regions outside of HF in which there was retrograde labeling resulting from every injection in at least one of the coordinates (Table 1). These regions were annotated and analyzed. Labeled cells in other regions were never observed following more than one injection in one bird. These were usually very small numbers of scattered cells and were omitted from analysis.
Ipsilateral and contribution input
Data from our initial set of birds confirmed that, like other pallial connections in the bird brain (Reiner et al., 2005), pallial inputs into HF were exclusively ipsilateral. In subsequent birds, we therefore occasionally used the same wavelength of the fluorophore on both hemispheres (7 chickadees). These animals were used only for quantifying pallial inputs into HF.
To compare the density of input from the ipsilateral and contralateral hemispheres, we computed the following lateralization score () for each input brain region:
Eq. 1 |
Where is the number of retrogradely labeled cells in the ipsilateral hemisphere, and is the number in the contralateral hemisphere. For every brain region, we calculated the average across all CTB injections, regardless of their location in HF.
Linear mixed-effects models
We used a linear mixed-effects model to examine how the number of HF-projecting cells in each brain region varied with the coordinate of the injection in HF. This model was fit separately for every input region. Models were fit to the base-10 logarithm of the cell counts, with 1 added to all counts to prevent taking a logarithm of 0:
Eq. 2 |
where is the number of labeled cells and is the adjusted cell count following injection . Here, indexes across all injections across all birds, for which the input region was annotated.
We considered two fixed effects in the model. The first effect was denoted using variable (for transverse axis) and specified whether the injection was into or into . The second effect was denoted using variable (for long axis) and specified the AP location of the injection in mm. For Hp injections, had a value of −1, 1, 3, or 5 (locations 1–4, Table 1). For DL injections (locations 5–7, Table 1), was assigned values of 1, 3, and 5, respectively, based on the topographically matched Hp location.
In addition to the two fixed effects, we also considered two random effects. The first random effect was the identity of the bird. This was used to correct for any uncontrolled factors that could affect the count of cells across birds, such as age or the quality of the perfusion. This random effect was assigned coefficients for each of the 21 birds used in the experiment. The second random effect was the wavelength of fluorophore used for the injection. This was modeled since the different fluorophores had different signal-to-noise ratios of fluorescence against the surrounding tissue, which could have resulted in systematic differences in cell counts. This random effect was assigned coefficients , , and for the three different wavelengths of the fluorophore that we used.
For each injection , variable specified the identity of the bird , and variable specified the wavelength used (488, 555, or 647). Cell counts were then modeled as
Eq. 3 |
Here, is a fixed offset, is the observation error for injection , is the coefficient modeling the effect of the transverse axis location (Hp or DL), and is the coefficient modeling the effect of the long axis coordinate. We used the MATLAB function fitlme to fit the coefficients, measure their confidence intervals, and determine whether they significantly differed from zero. These coefficients and p-values are reported in figures 8e and 9e, respectively.
Next, for each input brain region we asked whether there was an axis along which cells preferentially projected to different parts of the HF long axis. Here, we only considered brain regions that had projections to at least three of our four locations along the long axis. This excluded PMI and TnA.
First, we looked for topographic organization of input along the three stereotaxic axes, using a linear mixed-effects model. For this analysis, we only considered injections that labeled at least one cell in the brain region being analyzed. We fit the following linear mixed-effects models to predict how the mean AP coordinate , mean ML coordinate and mean DV coordinate varied between injections into different parts of the HF long axis.
Eq. 4 |
Eq. 5 |
Eq. 6 |
Here, coefficients , , model the effect of the transverse axis, which we did not analyze. Coefficients , , model the effect of the long axis. In other words, the vector is the direction of the topographic gradient connecting the brain region to the HF long axis.
Next, we projected the location of each labeled cell onto this vector . For each injection , we referred to the mean projected coordinate as . This allowed us to compute the center of each retrogradely labeled region along the topographic axis. We used a fourth model for this coordinate
Eq. 7 |
Here, models the effect of the transverse axis, which we did not analyze, and models the effect of the long axis. In other words, is the change in the coordinate along the topographic axis of the brain region, for every mm shift in injection coordinate along the HF long axis. We normalized by the overall extent of the brain region along the topographic axis. This extent was estimated using a bird with retrograde labeling from locations 1 and 3, representing almost the entire extent of the structure. Following this normalization, could be expressed in percent change for every mm shift along the HF long axis.
Finally, we noted that the direction of the topographic vector v is arbitrary: it is possible to obtain the same model fit by negating both and . We therefore multiplied by −1 if the dot product of v with the AP axis (1,0,0) was <0. In other words, was positive if the more anterior parts of the region projected to the more anterior parts of HF.
Results
Organization of the avian HF
HF in chickadees consists of Hp, DL, and CDL (Figure 1a). Hp is located in the dorsomedial telencephalon and is clearly defined by a lower cell density compared to the surrounding tissue. DL is situated directly lateral to Hp and has been delineated in our previous work by retrograde tracing from Hp (Applegate et al., 2023). We decided to consider inputs into Hp and DL, since these regions appear to be analogous to the mammalian hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex, respectively. CDL is situated further laterally and is a thin layer extending onto the posterior and lateral surfaces of the brain. Although CDL may also be part of an entorhinal-like region, most of this structure in chickadees is exceedingly small (often 1–2 cell bodies thick) and was not considered in this study.
We used the retrograde tracer cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) to map inputs into HF. We selected injection coordinates that included both Hp and DL, and that spanned most of the AP extent of these structures. For Hp injections, we chose four antero-posterior coordinates at −1A, 1A, 3A, and 5A (mm anterior to lambda, Figure 1b, Table 1). Injections at all four coordinates appeared to span both the dorso-medial (DM) and the V-shaped subregions of HF described in other species (Figure 1c) (Atoji and Wild, 2004, 2006). As previously reported, these injections labeled different portions of DL, with anterior DL most strongly innervating the anterior Hp (Applegate et al., 2023).
For DL, we therefore chose locations that were topographically matched to the Hp injections (Figure 1b–c, Table 1). Hp coordinates 1A, 3A, and 5A topographically corresponded to DL coordinates at 2A, 3.5A, and 5.5A, respectively. Injections into the fourth coordinate at - 1A labeled an extremely thin posterior tail of DL. This tail was too small for reliable CTB injections and was omitted from the study.
For each of the resulting seven locations (four in Hp and three in DL), we made injections in at least three chickadees (Table 1). In some birds, injections into multiple coordinates were made in different wavelengths of CTB. For every injection, we identified all retrogradely labeled cells using a blob detection algorithm. These cells were classified into different input regions using atlases of other avian species (Karten and Hodos, 1967; Stokes et al., 1974; Puelles et al., 2019). In the next three sections, we will examine all inputs, regardless of which of the seven coordinates they innervated. Afterwards, we will examine the topographic patterning of these inputs to different locations.
Pallial inputs to HF
We first analyzed inputs to HF from the pallium. These inputs consisted of four distinct nuclei, some of which were large and irregularly shaped. We visualized these inputs in 3D (Figure 2). The most dorsal of these structures was in the hyperpallium, while the other three were in the nidopallium.
The hyperpallial nucleus was HD (Figure 3a). HD is a portion of the visual Wulst constrained dorso-ventrally by two lamina: LFM and LFS (Atoji et al., 2017). In chickadees, HD extended medially to within a few hundred microns of the ventricle and the lateral surface of the brain. It extended roughly 2.5 mm AP. Some prior work has subdivided HD into a medial portion called HD and a lateral portion called HL (lateral hyperpallium) (Atoji et al., 2002; Atoji and Wild, 2005). However, we observed no cytoarchitectural or projection differences along this ML direction, so for the sake of all analyses we classified these cells as HD.
The nidopallial nuclei were all in the lateral part of the structure (NL). NL is typically subdivided into frontal, intermediate, and caudal portions based on position relative to the entopallium. The first nucleus we observed was in the frontal potion of NL (NFL, Figure 3b). This extremely anterior population occupied the ventro-lateral edge of the brain. The other two nuclei were in the intermediate portion of NL (NIL) and occupied different ML positions (Figure 3c). We therefore called these nuclei NILl and NILM. NILl was bordered ventrally and laterally by the brain surface and dorsally by LM. NILM was situated more medially, and in its anterior portion spanned the entire extent from LM to the ventral brain surface. In its posterior-most extent, it was constrained dorsally by the entopallium and ventrally by the surface of the brain (Figure 3d). Here, it formed a tight nucleus spanning only a few hundred microns ML and DV.
Subpallial inputs to HF
We next examined the subpallial inputs to HF. The first two of these were portions of the septum. The anterior-most septal population was NDB (Figure 4a). This nucleus extended about 1 mm AP. In its medial portion, NDB was confined within the fiber tract FDB. At its posterior extent, NDB extended laterally beyond FDB, but remained at roughly the same ventral position. In the posterior-dorsal part of the septum, we also observed a population of labeled cells in MS (Figure 4b). These cells were usually only sparsely labelled, as has been observed in pigeons (Atoji et al., 2002; Atoji and Wild, 2004; Montagnese et al., 2004).
The last subpallial region was TnA in the amygdala (Figure 4c). TnA, also sometimes called the pallial medial amygdalar nucleus (Abell±n et al., 2009), was located in the posterior medio-ventral telencephalon, immediately dorsal to OM. We observed labelled cells distributed over 0.5 mm AP. Note that while in other species TnA appears more laterally, its position relative to OM is consistent with our observations (Leutgeb et al., 1996; Cheng et al., 1999a).
Non-telencephalic inputs to HF
We next examined the diencephalic input to HF. In the thalamus we observed four nuclei, all of which have been previously described as inputs to HF (Casini et al., 1986). Going from anterior to posterior, the first of these was SPC (Figure 5a). It was the largest thalamic nucleus, forming a long thin (~100 μm) sheet that stretched from 1.7A to 0.5A along the DTB. The second and third nuclei were DLM and PMI, respectively (Figure 5b–c). Both extended about 0.5 mm AP and were about 1 mm across in cross-section. The most posterior thalamic nucleus was SRt (Figure 5d). It was bordered laterally by the medial extent of OM and was also the most ventral thalamic population we observed. SRt extended AP for about 300 p,m and was elongated in the ML direction (0.5 mm by 1 mm in cross-section). In the hypothalamus, we observed a lateral population of labeled cells (LHy, Figure 5e). These cells were constrained to the dorso-lateral hypothalamus,and extended for about 800 μm AP.
Next, we observed one midbrain population in the VTA (Figure 6a). This population extended for about 800 μm AP, was bounded ventrally by the brain surface and medially by the midline. As in other species, VTA appeared near the oculomotor nerve (NIII) in coronal sections (Reiner et al., 2004).
Finally, we observed two populations in the hindbrain. The more anterior of these was LC (Figure 6b). This nucleus formed a thin sheet (<100 μm) on the midline that extended roughly 700 μm AP and 1 mm DV. The second population was LoC (Figure 6c). This input was extremely posterior and extended roughly 700 μm AP. LoC occupied the dorsal portion of the brainstem and surrounded the FLM fiber bundle. At the anterior portion of LoC, cells were closer to the midline (within ~500 μm) while at the posterior extent, cells were distributed up to 1 mm from the midline.
Ipsilateral and contralateral contribution of inputs
In the preceding sections, we cataloged the inputs to the chickadee HF. These consisted of four pallial inputs (HD, NFL, NILM, NILl), three subpallial inputs (MS, NDB, TnA), five diencephalic inputs (SPC, DLM, PMI, SRt, LHy), and three brainstem inputs (VTA, LC, LoC). In the mammalian hippocampal circuit, the majority of input is from the ipsilateral hemisphere, though there are some contralateral projections (Goldowitz et al., 1975; Swanson and Cowan, 1977; Burwell and Amaral, 1998). Birds lack a corpus callosum, and in other avian circuits there is even less connection with the contralateral hemisphere than in mammals (Letzner et al., 2016; Wittek et al., 2021). This distinction is meaningful, likely accounting for some extreme lateralization of sensory and motor brain functions in birds (Graves and Goodale, 1977; Clayton, 1993; Vu et al., 1998; Long and Fee, 2008; Martinho et al., 2015; Martinho and Kacelnik, 2016). We were therefore motivated to quantify the relative ipsilateral and contralateral contribution of inputs into HF.
We found striking differences in contralateral input to HF from different brain regions. Some regions, like DLM, projected almost exclusively to the ipsilateral HF (Figure 7a–b). Other regions, like LoC, had similar numbers of neurons projecting to the ipsilateral and contralateral hemispheres (Figure 7c–d). To quantify these differences, we calculated a lateralization score ( 5iat) for each input area that ranged from −1 for a completely contralateral input to +1 for a completely ipsilateral input (Figure 7e).
These scores had interesting patterns for regions from the same part of the brain (Figure 7e). Like in most avian circuits, there was no contralateral input from any of the pallial regions (Nottebohm et al., 1976; Schmidt et al., 2004). In contrast, subpallial and diencephalic areas provided some contralateral input, though they also projected primarily ipsilaterally. The most notable contralateral projections among these areas were from MS and SPC. The brainstem regions had the most balanced projections to the two hemispheres. Neither of the hindbrain inputs (LC and LoC) showed significant differences between contralateral and ipsilateral labeling. Collectively, our results indicate a hippocampal circuit that has only a few regions for cross-hemispheric routing of information.
Topography along the transverse axis of HF
We next asked how projections from these input areas differed between Hp and DL. Our prior work has already described some differences in the relative density of pallial inputs to these structures (Applegate et al., 2023). Here, we provide a more detailed analysis of the different pallial subregions, and we also analyze all of the non-pallial inputs.
We observed differences in projections to DL and to Hp. To quantify these observations, we applied a linear mixed-effects model (Gaalso innervates rat hippocampus more than the entorhinal cortex ecki and Burzykowski, 2013), which measured the difference in the number of cells labeled by DL and Hp injections after compensating for other factors that could affect these numbers. The factors we considered were the AP position of the injection, the wavelength of the fluorophore, and the identity of the bird. Note that the number of labeled cells is not a perfect measure of the connection strength, since it can be influenced by factors like fibers of passage or the efficacy of CTB take-up.
Some inputs preferentially innervated DL. For example, only the ventral portion of HD projected to Hp, while the entire DV extent of HD projected to DL (Figure 8a). This difference led to nearly a 3-fold increase in the number of labeled cells (p<0.001, linear mixed-effects model, Figure 8b). In some other brain regions, there were more direct inputs to Hp than to DL. For example, LHy had a markedly weaker projection to DL, with several injections labeling no cells (Figure 8c–d, p<0.01).
Across all of the brain regions, the pallial nuclei HD and NILL had stronger projections to DL (Figure 8e). These results are consistent with the mammalian literature, where the entorhinal cortex receives more direct cortical input than the hippocampus (Burwell and Amaral, 1998). Only LHy and LC preferentially innervated Hp (Figure 8e). This result is also consistent with observations in rodents. Rodent LHy primarily sends connections to the subiculum in the hippocampus, and not to the entorhinal cortex (Hahn and Swanson, 2012). The rodent medial raphe nucleus, where LC is located, also innervates rat hippocampus more than the entorhinal cortex (Köhler and Steinbusch, 1982). These results are further evidence of the analogy between DL and the entorhinal cortex and suggest additional similarities in organization between the mammalian and avian circuits.
Topography along the long axis of HF
We next asked whether input regions exhibited topography of their connections along the HF long axis. We considered two possible forms of topography. First, an input region could preferentially target the anterior or posterior HF. In this case, anterior and posterior injections would retrogradely label different numbers of cells. Second, different portions of the same input region could project to different sections of the HF long axis. In this case, the coordinates of retrogradely labeled cells would be different between anterior and posterior injections.
We first looked at how the number of labeled cells varied with injection location. Again, we applied a linear mixed-effects model to quantify this effect. In some regions, anterior HF injections resulted in more labeled cells. For example, thalamic input from PMI was exclusively limited to the anterior HF (Figure 9a–b, p<0.001). In contrast, amygdalar input from TnA exclusively targeted the posterior HF (Figure 9c–d, p<0.001). We found that the majority of input regions preferentially targeted either the anterior or the posterior HF (Figure 9e). As before, interesting patterns emerged across divisions of the brain. For example, all pallial and all thalamic areas preferentially innervated the anterior HF. In contrast, two of the subpallial regions, TnA and MS, were the only inputs that preferentially targeted the posterior HF.
Finally, we asked whether different portions of these regions projected to different parts of the long axis. We observed some regions with noticeable differences; for example, anterior and posterior HD projected to more anterior and posterior parts of HF, respectively (Figure 10a–b). To quantify this phenomenon, we again used a linear mixed-effects model. We determined the relationship between the location of the HF injections (along the AP axis) and the average stereotaxic coordinate of retrogradely labeled cells. This model quantified the magnitude of this topographic gradient as the amount of shift in the position of labeled cells for every millimeter shift in the injection location. We also quantified the direction of this gradient in stereotaxic coordinates.
Using this approach, we found that different portions of HD indeed projected to different parts of the HF long axis (Figure 10c, p<0.001) with roughly a 330 μm shift for every mm shift in injection location. Repeating this procedure for other brain regions revealed other interesting topographies. For example, anterior LC preferentially targeted the more posterior HF locations (Figure 10d–f, p<0.005) with roughly a 40 μm shift per mm shift in injection location. To compare topographies across regions, we normalized the magnitude of each topographic gradient by the size of the brain structure along the direction of the gradient. We expressed these coefficients as the percent change in position for every mm change in injection location (Figure 10g–h).
About half of the input regions exhibited a significant topographic gradient. Again, some of these differences suggest interesting parallels to the mammalian circuit. Two of the pallial regions had a topographic gradient with the HF long axis. These types of patterns are common in mammalian cortical regions. For example, the orbitofrontal cortex is connected topographically to the lateral entorhinal cortex (Kondo and Witter, 2014). Some subcortical regions also had mammalian-like organization. As in our results, anterior-dorsal LHy in rodents preferentially innervates the septal hippocampus, while posterior-ventral LHy preferentially innervates the temporal hippocampus (Hahn and Swanson, 2012).
Discussion
In this study we used retrograde tracing to map inputs into the chickadee HF. By varying our injection location, we examined how inputs differ along both the transverse and the long axes of HF. Input regions were found in several parts of the brain and were generally similar to those described in other avian species and in mammals. We discovered that many of these inputs were topographic, with some striking similarities to the mammalian hippocampal system. These kinds of similarities are particularly surprising given the major changes in neural architecture between these species, including the lack of a layered cortex or a corpus callosum in birds. In addition to the similarities, we found some notable differences between chickadees and other species. These unique features of the anatomy will be particularly interesting to examine in the future, as they may contribute to the specialized memory functions of food-caching birds.
Scope of our anatomical tracing
Our injections spanned almost the entire volume of HF in chickadees. We therefore think that we have identified nearly all inputs into this structure. There are two notable exceptions. First, we did not inject into CDL. The relationship between this structure and HF is a matter of debate. Some studies have considered this CDL to be separate from HF (Atoji and Wild, 2004), while others have marked it as the lateral-most portion of HF (Puelles et al., 2019). We previously found that in chickadees this region is connected to Hp along topographically organized bands, which are continuous with topographic bands in DL (Applegate et al., 2023). This organization of DL/CDL is similar to that of the entorhinal cortex, with CDL occupying an anatomical position similar to the lateral entorhinal cortex. In this paper, we therefore considered CDL to be part of HF. If CDL is not part of HF, it should be counted as a fifth pallial input into HF, in addition to the four we described.
Like the lateral entorhinal cortex, CDL is associated with the olfactory system in other avian species and receives input from the piriform cortex (Atoji and Wild, 2005). In chickadees, CDL and the rest of the olfactory system are exceedingly small (Bang and Cobb, 1968) and could not be studied with our tracing protocols. This is consistent with chickadee behavior, as they are not known to use olfaction for any memory tasks (Sherry et al., 1981; Herz et al., 1993). However, if CDL is to be considered part of HF, piriform cortex may be an additional vestigial olfactory input not reported here.
In addition, our injections did not reach the most ventral and the most lateral portions of Hp at the intermediate long-axis coordinates - i.e. sections where Hp had the largest extent in the transverse plane. Work in other species has subdivided Hp into the V-shaped and DM regions (Szekely, 1999; Atoji and Wild, 2004, 2006). Although it is difficult to match anatomical locations across species, our injections appeared to span both the V-shaped region and DM. However, they excluded the ventral-most part of the V-shaped region and lateral-most part of DM. The V-shaped region participates almost exclusively in the intrinsic connectivity within Hp. Only MS has been identified as an extrinsic input into this region (Szekely, 1999; Atoji and Wild, 2005). CDL also provides input to the V-shaped region (Atoji and Wild, 2005; Rook et al., 2023), but as discussed above, we consider this to be an intrinsic hippocampal connection. Whether any additional medio-lateral organization exists within DM is unknown and remains to be addressed by future studies.
Inputs to the chickadee HF
Almost nothing is known in chickadees about the inputs we identified, but some information is available from other species. All pallial nuclei have been described to various extent in the literature. HD is a region of the thalamofugal visual pathway and has been observed as an input to HF in other species (Casini et al., 1986; Atoji et al., 2002; Kahn et al., 2003; Atoji and Wild, 2004; Herold et al., 2019). It is involved in visual memory (Shimizu and Hodos, 1989; Watanabe, 2003; Maekawa et al., 2006) and receives input from other portions of the visual Wulst and from the visual thalamic areas (Medina and Reiner, 2000; Atoji et al., 2017). NL has also been implicated in visual processing as part of the tectofugal pathway (Alpár and Tömböl, 1998; Watanabe, 2003), but may also participate in processing other sensory modalities (Brauth et al., 1987; Brauth and McHale, 1988; Hall et al., 1993). Although NFL has been detected as an input to Hp (Casini et al., 1986; Atoji et al., 2002), neither of the NIL nuclei (NILM and NILl) have been described in other species. However, it is possible that they overlap with the temporo-parieto-occipital area (TPO), which has been described as an input to DL (Behroozi et al., 2017).
Subpallial inputs in chickadees included two septal nuclei (NDB and MS) and an amygdalar nucleus TnA. Both septal nuclei are known as inputs to HF in other avian species (Casini et al., 1986; Atoji et al., 2002). In mammals, the septum provides cholinergic and GABAergic input to HF and has multiple functions in HF processing (Stewart and Fox, 1990; Kaifosh et al., 2013; Petersen and Buzsáki, 2020; Schlesiger et al., 2021). Less is known in birds, although the septum in pigeons does play a role in spatial memory (Peterson and Bingman, 2011). TnA is also a known input into the hippocampus (Cheng et al., 1999b). It has been implicated by prior avian studies in a variety of social and emotional behaviors, from social feeding to abnormal fear responses (Cheng et al., 1999b; Atoji et al., 2002).
The role of the four thalamic regions we identified is largely unknown. SPC, DLM, and SRt all receive visual and somatosensory input, while PMI is considered an auditory structure (Bagnoli and Burkhalter, 1983; Martin Wild et al., 1993; Atoji and Wild, 2006; Miceli et al., 2006; Atoji et al., 2017). A region called DLM is part of the song system (Bottjer et al., 1989), but it is unclear if that structure is the same as the HF-projecting population we labeled. The sole hypothalamic structure we found, LHy, has been implicated in both birds and mammals in feeding and food-hoarding behaviors (Herberg and Blundell, 1967; Kuenzel, 1972). LHy has been described as an input to HF in other avian species (Casini et al., 1986; Atoji et al., 2002).
Finally, we observed one input from the midbrain (VTA) and two inputs from the hindbrain (LoC and LC). VTA has been functionally implicated in many studies across both birds and mammals as a dopaminergic region involved in reward signaling (Schultz et al., 1997; Gadagkar et al., 2016). LC forms a part of the medial raphe nucleus and has been described as a serotonergic input to HF (Casini et al., 1986; Krebs et al., 1991), while LoC is a noradrenergic center (Erichsen et al., 1991; Mello et al., 1998). All of these have been previously described as inputs to HF (Casini et al., 1986; Atoji et al., 2002).
Lateralization of the hippocampal circuit
The chickadee hippocampal circuit was extremely lateralized. All of the pallial inputs and the majority of subpallial and thalamic inputs in our data were predominantly ipsilateral. This anatomical segregation likely explains lateralized learning in certain memory tasks. Although chickadees have some binocular overlap of their visual field (Moore et al., 2013), they use monocular vision for most perceptual tasks (Martin, 2009). When chickadees cache food with one eye covered by an eye patch, they are unable to find their caches shortly afterwards if the patch is moved to the other eye (Sherry et al., 1981; Clayton, 1993). This indicates that some crucial aspect of cache memory is not shared between hemispheres. Such a pattern of ‘ipsilateral learning’ is not unique to chickadees. In other visual tasks, like reversal learning, homing in pigeons, and imprinting in ducklings, there is also a lack of interocular transfer of learned information (Graves and Goodale, 1977; Martinho et al., 2015; Martinho and Kacelnik, 2016).
Interestingly, there is evidence that this transfer does happen after longer periods of time. For example, chickadees in the eye-patch experiment are able to find caches after 24 h (Clayton, 1993). It is unknown how this offline transfer happens. Our analysis has identified some input regions with particularly strong contralateral projections, which could play a role in this process. Another possible mechanism is via the hippocampal commissure (Atoji et al., 2002; Letzner et al., 2016). This connection between the right and left hippocampi targets primarily the V-shaped region, which was only partially included in the present study.
Comparison to mammalian circuitry
Organization of inputs into HF had notable similarities between chickadees and mammals. One of these similarities was in the transverse axis, which separates Hp from DL in birds and from the entorhinal cortex in mammals. As in rodents, we found that hypothalamic inputs preferentially targeted Hp (Hahn and Swanson, 2012). In contrast, pallial inputs preferentially targeted DL, much like they target the entorhinal cortex in mammals (Burwell and Amaral, 1998). Previous work has demonstrated numerous anatomical and physiological similarities of DL and the entorhinal cortex (Atoji and Wild, 2004; Abellán et al., 2014; Applegate et al., 2023). Our findings add further evidence of the equivalence between these regions across species.
In addition, we found similarities in the topography of inputs with the HF long axis. This topography was notable because it might contribute to the known organization of functions along this axis. We found that thalamic and some pallial inputs preferentially innervated the anterior HF in chickadees, much like they innervate the dorsal hippocampus in rodents. As in rodents, some of these inputs are associated with the visual system (Karten et al., 1973; Miceli et al., 2006; Atoji et al., 2017), which is critically important for spatial computations. These inputs may contribute to the selective abundance of spatially tuned cells in the anterior and dorsal hippocampi of chickadees and rodents, respectively. In contrast, we found a highly selective innervation of the posterior HF by the amygdala. This is equivalent to the amygdalar inputs into the rodent ventral hippocampus (Van Groen and Wyss, 1990; Petrovich et al., 2001). The rodent ventral hippocampus is involved in emotional and stress-related behaviors, and a similar function has been proposed for this region in birds (Bouillé and Baylé, 1973; Smulders, 2017). Overall, the topographies we found support the idea that inputs play a major role in the functional organization of the long axis, in both birds and mammals.
How might all these similarities between birds and mammals emerge? One possibility is that both the transverse and the long axes are derived from a common amniote precursor, and that these gradients of input have been preserved over millions of years (Witter et al., 2017; Herold et al., 2019). Indeed, similar transcription factors are expressed along the transverse axes across species (Abellán et al., 2014; Tosches et al., 2018). Conversely, similar topographies could have emerged through convergence. A functionally segmented hippocampus may confer some advantage to the animal. Theoretical work has shown that having related but distinct functional regions can aide in computation (Rolls and Webb, 2014).
Our results also point to an interesting anatomical difference between avian and mammalian hippocampal circuits. In mammals, the difference between inputs to the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex is dramatic. Nearly thirty cortical regions project to the entorhinal cortex, while only two of them send weak inputs directly to the hippocampus (Dolorfo and Amaral, 1998). In chickadees, the difference between Hp and DL was more subtle. All DL-projecting regions also targeted Hp, albeit often less strongly. This result suggests a less pronounced specialization of HF subregions in birds.
Specialization in a food-caching species
In addition to many similarities, we found notable differences between chickadees and other avian species. One difference was the input from the nidopallium. Work in other species has identified a relatively small input into HF from the frontal part of the nidopallium, NFL (Atoji and Wild, 2004). We found a much more expansive input region that also included parts of the NIL and that was highly structured into several distinct nuclei. Parts of NL receive input from the tectrofugal visual pathway and possibly other sensory pathways (Hall et al., 1993; Alpár and Tömböl, 1998; Watanabe, 2003). Other than this, not much is known about any of the regions we identified. Given that these inputs appear to be enhanced in chickadees, it will be interesting to investigate their possible role in food-caching behaviors.
The topographic patterns we describe also appear to be partly different from prior work. Some of our results, such as the topography of NDB and TnA, were previously reported in pigeons (Kraniak and Siegel, 1978; Casini et al., 1986; Atoji et al., 2002; Herold et al., 2019). However, most inputs to the avian HF were previously described without any topography. Most strikingly, some of the clearest topographic patterns we observed, such as the AP gradient of input in HD, have been explicitly described as absent in pigeons (Herold et al., 2019). Some other topographies appear to be stronger in chickadees. For example, TnA has been described as innervating the posterior half or posterior two-thirds of Hp in several studies (Casini et al., 1986; Atoji et al., 2002; Herold et al., 2019). In our data, TnA only innervated the posterior-most quarter of HF. What might be driving these differences between species?
One possibility is that some topographic patterns are harder to observe in other birds. In chickadees, we took advantage of the large hippocampus to study multiple injection coordinates, spaced 2 mm apart. We also quantified the location of every labeled cell and compensated for possible sources of noise like inter-individual variability. Without this quantification, or in an animal with a smaller hippocampus and fewer injection coordinates, it may be hard to detect some of the topographies.
Another possibility is that HF in chickadees is segregated into functional domains more strongly than in other species. Chickadee hippocampus needs to support an immense memory load to remember not only the location of caches, but also their content and other salient features (Cowie et al., 1981; Sherry and Vaccarino, 1989; Clayton and Dickinson, 1998). It is possible that increased computational demands are facilitated by specialized domains similar to those in rodents (Risold and Swanson, 1996; Fanselow and Dong, 2010; Strange et al., 2014). In chickadees some inputs, like PMI and TnA, project to completely non-overlapping parts of HF. It remains to be seen what role some of these extreme topographies play in the hippocampal specialization of food-caching birds.
Acknowledgements
We thank Stephanie Hale, David Scheck, and Luke Hammond for technical assistance; the Black Rock Forest Consortium, Timothy Green, and Jennifer Scribner, for field site help. Imaging was performed with support from the Zuckerman Institute’s Cellular Imaging platform. This work was supported by the Beckman Foundation Young Investigator Award, the New York Stem Cell Foundation — Robertson Neuroscience Investigator Award, NIH Director’s New Innovator Award (DP2-AG071918), NIH training grant (T32 EY013933, to MCA), NSF Graduate Research Fellowship Program (to MCA).
Abbreviations of anatomical terms
- Cb
Cerebellum
- CDL
Caudal dorsolateral hippocampal formation
- DL
Dorsolateral hippocampal formation
- DLM
Nucleus dorsolateralis anterior thalami, pars medialis
- DM
Dorsomedial hippocampal formation
- DTB
Diencephalo-telencephalic boundary
- E
Entopallium
- FDB
Fascicle of the diagonal band of Broca
- FLM
Fasciculus longitudinalis medialis
- HD
Hyperpallium densocellulare
- HF
Hippocampal formation
- Hp
Hippocampus
- Hy
Hypothalamus
- LC
Nucleus linearis caudalis
- LFM
Lamina frontalis suprema
- LFS
Lamina frontalis superior
- LHy
Lateral hypothalamus
- LM
Lamina mesopallialis
- LoC
Locus coeruleus
- LPS
Lamina pallio-subpallialis
- m
Midline
- MS
Medial septum
- NDB
Nucleus of the diagonal band of Broca
- NFL
Fronto-lateral nidopallium
- NIII
Oculomotor nerve
- NIL
Lateral intermediate nidopallium
- NILL
Lateral intermediate nidopallium,lateral portion
- NILM
Lateral intermediate nidopallium, medial portion
- NL
Lateral nidopallium
- OM
Tractus occipitomesencephalicus
- PMI
Nucleus paramedianus interni thalami
- S
Septum
- SPC
Nucleus superficialis parvocellularis
- SRt
Nucleus subrotundus
- TnA
Nucleus taeniae
- V
Lateral ventricle
- VTA
Ventral tegmental area
Data availability statement
The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in dryad at doi:10.5061/dryad.xwdbrv1jq
References
- Abellán A, Desfilis E, Medina L. 2014. Combinatorial expression of Lef1, Lhx2, Lhx5, Lhx9, Lmo3, Lmo4, and Prox1 helps to identify comparable subdivisions in the developing hippocampal formation of mouse and chicken. Front Neuroanat [Internet] 8:59. Available from: http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?eid=2-s2.0-84904043729&partnerID=tZ0tx3y1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abellán A, Legaz I, Vernier B, Rétaux S, Medina L. 2009. Olfactory and amygdalar structures of the chicken ventral pallium based on the combinatorial expression patterns of LIM and other developmental regulatory genes. J Comp Neurol 516:166–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agarwal A, Sarel A, Derdikman D, Ulanovsky N, Gutfreund Y. 2021. Spatial coding in the hippocampus of flying owls. bioRxiv [Internet]:2021.10.24.465553. Available from: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.10.24.465553v1%0Ahttps://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.10.24.465553v1.abstract [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alpár A, Tömböl T. 1998. Telencephalic connections of the visual system of the chicken: Tracing the interrelation of the efferents of the visual Wulst and the hyperstriatum ventrale. Ann Anat 180:529–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Applegate MC, Gutnichenko KS, Mackevicius EL, Aronov D. 2023. An entorhinal-like region in food-caching birds. bioRxiv. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atoji Y, Sarkar S, Wild JM. 2017. Differential projections of the densocellular and intermediate parts of the hyperpallium in the pigeon (Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 526:146–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atoji Y, Wild JM. 2004. Fiber connections of the hippocampal formation and septum and subdivisions of the hippocampal formation in the pigeon as revealed by tract tracing and kainic acid lesions. J Comp Neurol 475:426–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atoji Y, Wild JM. 2005. Afferent and efferent connections of the dorsolateral corticoid area and a comparison with connections of the temporo-parieto-occipital area in the pigeon (Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 485:165–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atoji Y, Wild JM. 2006. Anatomy of the avian hippocampal formation. Rev Neurosci 5:3–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atoji Y, Wild JM, Yamamoto Y, Suzuki Y. 2002. Intratelencephalic connections of the hippocampus in pigeons (Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 447:177–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bagnoli P, Burkhalter A. 1983. Organization of the afferent projections to the Wulst in the pigeon. J Comp Neurol 214:103–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bang BG, Cobb S. 1968. The size of the olfactory bulb in 108 species of birds. Auk 85:55–61. [Google Scholar]
- Barbosa FF, de Oliveira Pontes IM, Ribeiro S, Ribeiro AM, Silva RH. 2012. Differential roles of the dorsal hippocampal regions in the acquisition of spatial and temporal aspects of episodic-like memory. Behav Brain Res [Internet] 232:269–277. Available from: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnea A, Nottebohm F. 1994. Seasonal recruitment of hippocampal neurons in adult free-ranging black-capped chickadees. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A [Internet] 91:11217–21. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7972037%5Cnhttp://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=PMC45198%5Cnhttp://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=45198&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behroozi M, Ströckens F, Stacho M, Güntürkün O. 2017. Functional connectivity pattern of the internal hippocampal network in awake pigeons : a resting-state fMRI study. Brain Behav Evol. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottjer SW, Halsema KA, Brown SA, Miesner EA. 1989. Axonal connections of a forebrain nucleus involved with vocal learning in zebra finches. J Comp Neurol 279:312–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouillé C, Baylé JD. 1973. Effects of limbic stimulations or lesions on basal and stress-induced hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical activity in the pigeon. Neuroendocrinology 13:264–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brauth SE, McHale CM. 1988. Auditory pathways in the Budgerigar: II. Intratelencephalic Pathways. Brain Behav Evol:193–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brauth SE, McHale CM, Brasher CA, Dooling RJ. 1987. Auditory pathways in the budgerigar. I. Thalamo-telencephalic projections. Brain Behav Evol:174–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burwell RD, Amaral DG. 1998. Cortical afferents of the perirhinal, postrhinal, and entorhinal cortices. J Comp Neurol 205:179–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casini G, Bingman VP, Bagnoli P. 1986. Connections of the pigeon dorsomedial forebrain studied with WGA-HRP and 3H-proline. J Comp Neurol 245:454–470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng M-F, Chaiken M, Zuo M, Miller H. 1999a. Nucleus Taenia of the amygdala of birds: anatomical and functional studies in Ring Doves (Streptopelia risoria) and European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Brain Behav Evol 53:243–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng M-FF, Chaiken M, Zuo M, Miller H. 1999b. Nucleus taenia of the amygdala of birds: Anatomical and functional studies in ring doves (Streptopelia risoria) and European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Brain Behav Evol 53:243–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cimadevilla JM, Fenton AA, Bures J. 2000. Functional inactivation of dorsal hippocampus impairs active place avoidance in rats. Neurosci Lett 285:53–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clayton N. 1993. Lateralization and unilateral transfer of spatial memory in marsh tits. J Comp Physiol A 171:799–806. [Google Scholar]
- Clayton NS, Dickinson A. 1998. Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature [Internet] 395:272–274. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9751053%5Cnhttp://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v395/n6699/pdf/395272a0.pdf [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cowie RJ, Krebs JR, Sherry DF. 1981. Food storing by marsh tits. Anim Behav 29:1252–1259. [Google Scholar]
- Damphousse CC, Miller N, Marrone DF. 2022. Functional dissociation along the rostrocaudal axis of Japanese quail hippocampus. PLoS One [Internet] 17:e0277414. Available from: 10.1371/journal.pone.0277414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolorfo CL, Amaral DG. 1998. Entorhinal cortex of the rat: Topographic organization of the cells of origin of the perforant path projection to the dentate gyrus. J Comp Neurol 398:25–48. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erichsen JT, Bingman VP, Krebs JR. 1991. The distribution of neuropeptides in the dorsomedial telencephalon of the pigeon (Columba livia): A basis for regional subdivisions. J Comp Neurol 314:478–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fanselow MS, Dong HW. 2010. Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron [Internet] 65:7–19. Available from: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gadagkar V, Puzerey PA, Chen R, Baird-Daniel E, Farhang AR, Goldberg JH. 2016. Dopamine neurons encode performance error in singing birds. Science 354:1278–1282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gałecki A, Burzykowski T. 2013. Linear Mixed-Effects Models: basic concepts and examples. In: Mixed-effects models in S and S-Plus. . p 3–56. [Google Scholar]
- Garamszegi LZ, Eens M. 2004. The evolution of hippocampus volume and brain size in relation to food hoarding in birds. Ecol Lett 7:1216–1224. [Google Scholar]
- Goldowitz D, White WF, Steward O, Lynch G, Cotman C. 1975. Anatomical evidence for a projection from the entorhinal cortex to the contralateral dentate gyrus of the rat. Exp Neurol 47:433–441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graves JA, Goodale MA. 1977. Failure of interocular transfer in the pigeon (Columba livia). Physiol Behav 19:425–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Groen T, Wyss JM. 1990. Extrinsic projections from area CA1 of the rat hippocampus: Olfactory, cortical, subcortical, and bilateral hippocampal formation projections. J Comp Neurol 302:515–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser MB, Moser EI. 2005. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436:801–806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn JD, Swanson LW. 2012. Connections of the lateral hypothalamic area juxtadorsomedial region in the male rat. J Comp Neurol 520:1831–1890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall WS, Cohen PL, Brauth SE. 1993. Auditory projections to the anterior telencephalon. Brain Behav Evol. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henke PG. 1990. Hippocampal pathway to the amygdala and stress ulcer development. Brain Res Bull 25:691–695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herberg LJ, Blundell JE. 1967. Lateral hypothalamus: hoarding behavior elicited by electrical stimulation. Science 155:349–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herold C, Schlömer P, Mafoppa-Fomat I, Mehlhorn J, Amunts K, Axer M. 2019. The hippocampus of birds in a view of evolutionary connectomics. Cortex 118:165–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herz RS, Zanette L, Sherry DF. 1993. Spatial cues for cache retrieval by black-capped chickadees. Anim Behav [Internet] 48:343–351. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0003347284712474 [Google Scholar]
- Inés Sotelo M, Bingman VP, Muzio RN. 2022. The medial pallium and the spatial encoding of geometric and visual cues in the terrestrial toad, Rhinella arenarum. Neurosci Lett 786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jung MW, Wiener SI, McNaughton BL. 1994. Comparison of spatial firing characteristics of units in dorsal and ventral hippocampus of the rat. J Neurosci 14:7347–7356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahn MC, Hough GE, Ten Eyck GR, Bingman VP. 2003. Internal connectivity of the homing pigeon (Columba livia) hippocampal formation: An anterograde and retrograde tracer study. J Comp Neurol 459:127–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaifosh P, Lovett-Barron M, Turi GF, Reardon TR, Losonczy A. 2013. Septo-hippocampal GABAergic signaling across multiple modalities in awake mice. Nat Neurosci 16:1182–1184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karten HJ, Hodos W. 1967. A stereotaxic atlas of the brain of the pigeon: Columba livia. Johns Hopkins Press. [Google Scholar]
- Karten HJ, Hodos W, Nauta WJ, Revzin AM. 1973. Neural connections of the “visual wulst” of the avian telencephalon. Experimental studies in the piegon (Columba livia) and owl (Speotyto cunicularia). J Comp Neurol 150:253–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kjelstrup KB, Solstad T, Brun VH, Hafting T, Leutgeb S, Witter MP, Moser EI, Moser MB. 2008. Finite scale of spatial representation in the hippocampus. Science 321:140–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kjelstrup KG, Tuvnes FA, Steffenach HA, Murison R, Moser EI, Moser MB. 2002. Reduced fear expression after lesions of the ventral hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99:10825–10830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Köhler C, Steinbusch H. 1982. Identification of serotonin and non-serotonin-containing neurons of the mid-brain raphe projecting to the entorhinal area and the hippocampal formation. A combined immunohistochemical and fluorescent retrograde tracing study in the rat brain. Neuroscience 7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kondo H, Witter MP. 2014. Topographic organization of orbitofrontal projections to the parahippocampal region in rats. J Comp Neurol 522:772–793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraniak PF, Siegel A. 1978. Efferent connections of the hippocampus and adjacent regions in the pigeon. Brain Behav Evol 15:373–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs JR, Erichsen JT, Bingman VP. 1991. The distribution of neurotransmitters and neurotransmitter-related enzymes in the dorsomedial telencephalon of the pigeon (Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 314:467–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krebs JR, Sherryt DF, Healy SD, Perryt VH, Vaccarino AL. 1989. Hippocampal specialization of food-storing birds. Neurobiology 86:1388–1392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krushinskaya N. 1966. Some complex forms of feeding behavior of nutcracker Nucifraga caryocatactes, after removal of old cortex. J Evol Biochem Physiol:563–568. [Google Scholar]
- Kuenzel WJ. 1972. Dual hypothalamic feeding system in a migratory bird, Zonotrichia albicollis. Am J Psychiatry 223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Letzner S, Simon A, Güntürkün O. 2016. Connectivity and neurochemistry of the commissura anterior of the pigeon (Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 524:343–361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leutgeb S, Husband S, Riters LV., Shimizu T, Bingman VP. 1996. Telencephalic afferents to the caudolateral neostriatum of the pigeon. Brain Res 730:173–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long MA, Fee MS. 2008. Using temperature to analyse temporal dynamics in the songbird motor pathway. Nature 456:189–194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maekawa F, Komine O, Sato K, Kanamatsu T, Uchimura M, Tanaka K, Ohki-hamazaki H. 2006. Imprinting modulates processing of visual information in the visual wulst of chicks. BMC Neurosci 13:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maren S, Holt WG. 2004. Hippocampus and pavlovian fear conditioning in rats: muscimol infusions into the ventral, but not dorsal, hippocampus impair the acquisition of conditional freezing to an auditory conditional stimulus. Behav Neurosci 118:97–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin GR. 2009. What is binocular vision for? A birds’ eye view. J Vis 9:1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin Wild J, Karten HJ, Frost BJ. 1993. Connections of the auditory forebrain in the pigeon (columba livia). J Comp Neurol 337:32–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinho A, Biro D, Guilford T, Gagliardo A, Kacelnik A. 2015. Asymmetric visual input and route recapitulation in homing pigeons. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinho A, Kacelnik A. 2016. Swapping mallards: monocular imprints in ducklings are unavailable to the opposite eye. Anim Behav [Internet] 122:99–107. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016Zj.anbehav.2016.10.010 [Google Scholar]
- Medina L, Reiner A. 2000. Do birds possess homlogues of mammalian primary visual, somatosensory and motor cortices? Trends Neurosci 23:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mello C V, Pinaud R, Ribeiro S. 1998. Noradrenergic system of the zebra finch brain: Immunocytochemical study of dopamine-P-hydroxylase. J Comp Neurol 400:207–228. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miceli D, Réperant J, Medina M, Volle M, Rio JP. 2006. Distribution of ganglion cells in the pigeon retina labeled via retrograde transneuronal transport of the fluorescent dye rhodamine β-isothiocyanate from the telencephalic visual Wulst. Brain Res 1098:94–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montagnese CM, Székely AD, Ádám Á, Csillag A. 2004. Efferent connections of septal nuclei of the domestic chick (Gallus domesticus): an anterograde pathway tracing study with a bearing on functional circuits. J Comp Neurol 469:437–456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore BA, Doppler M, Young JE, Fernández-Juricic E. 2013. Interspecific differences in the visual system and scanning behavior of three forest passerines that form heterospecific flocks. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sensory, Neural, Behav Physiol 199:263–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris RGM, Garrud P, Rawlins JNPNP, O’Keefe J. 1982. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature [Internet] 297:681–683. Available from: https://www.nature.com/articles/297681a0.pdf [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moser MB, Moser EI. 1998. Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8:608–619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nottebohm F, Stokes TM, Leonard CM. 1976. Central control of song in the canary, Serinus canarius. J Comp Neurol 165:457–486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. 1971. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res 34:171–175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne HL, Lynch GF, Aronov D. 2021. Neural representations of space in the hippocampus of a food-caching bird. Science 373:343–348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen PC, Buzsáki G. 2020. Cooling of medial septum reveals theta phase lag coordination of hippocampal cell assemblies. Neuron 107:731–744.e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson RM, Bingman VP. 2011. Septal area lesions impair spatial working memory in homing pigeons (Columba livia). Neurobiol Learn Mem [Internet] 96:353–360. Available from: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.06.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrovich GD, Canteras NS, Swanson LW. 2001. Combinatorial amygdalar inputs to hippocampal domains and hypothalamic behavior systems. Brain Res Rev 38:247–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prasad JA, Chudasama Y. 2013. Viral tracing identifies parallel disynaptic pathways to the hippocampus. J Neurosci 33:8494–8503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pravosudov VV. 1985. Search for and storage of food by Parus cinctus lapponicus and Parus montanus borealis (Paridae). Zool Zhurnal (Journal Zool 64:1036–1043. [Google Scholar]
- Puelles L, Martinez-de-la-Torre M, Martinez S, Watson C, Paxinos G. 2019. The chick brain in stereotaxic coordinates and alternate stains. 2nd ed. Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Reiner A, Perkel DJ, Bruce LL, Butler AB, Csillag A, Kuenzel W, Medina L, Paxinos G, Shimizu T, Striedter G, Wild M, Ball GF, Durand S, Guturkun O, Lee DW, Mello CV., Powers A, White SA, Hough G, Kubikova L, Smulders TV., Wada K, Dugas-Ford J, Husband S, Yamamoto K, Yu J, Siang C, Jarvis ED. 2004. Revised nomenclature for avian telencephalon and some related brainstem nuclei. J Comp Neurol 473:377–414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reiner A, Yamamoto K, Karten HJ. 2005. Organization and evolution of the avian forebrain. Anat Rec - Part A Discov Mol Cell Evol Biol 287:1080–1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reis F, Schenka AA, Melo LL, Ferrari EAM. 1999. Role of the hippocampus in contextual memory after classical aversive conditioning in pigeons (C. Livia). Brazilian J Med Biol Res 32:1127–1131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Risold PY, Swanson LW. 1996. Structural evidence for functional domains in the rat hippocampus. Science 272:1484–1486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodríguez F, López JC, Vargas JP, Broglio C, Gómez Y, Salas C. 2002. Spatial memory and hippocampal pallium through vertebrate evolution: Insights from reptiles and teleost fish. Brain Res Bull 57:499–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Webb TJ. 2014. Finding and recognizing objects in natural scenes: Complementary computations in the dorsal and ventral visual systems. Front Comput Neurosci 8:1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rook N, Stacho M, Schwarz A, Bingman VP, Güntürkün O. 2023. Neuronal circuits within the homing pigeon hippocampal formation. J Comp Neurol:790–813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schlesiger MI, Ruff T, MacLaren DAA, Barriuso-Ortega I, Saidov KM, Yen TY, Monyer H. 2021. Two septal-entorhinal GABAergic projections differentially control coding properties of spatially tuned neurons in the medial entorhinal cortex. Cell Rep [Internet] 34:108801. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.celrep.2021.108801 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF, Ashmore RC, Vu ET. 2004. Bilateral control and interhemispheric coordination in the avian song motor system. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1016:171–186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. 1997. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 275:1593–1599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scoville WB, Milner B. 1957. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry [Internet] 20:11–21. Available from: http://jnnp.bmj.com/cgi/doi/10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sherry DF, Krebs JR, Cowie RJ. 1981. Memory for the location of stored food in marsh tits. Anim Behav 29:1260–1266. [Google Scholar]
- Sherry DF, Vaccarino AL. 1989. Hippocampus and memory for food caches in black-capped chickadees. Behav Neurosci 103:308–318. [Google Scholar]
- Sherry DF, Vaccarino AL, Buckenham K, Herz RS. 1989. The hippocampal complex of food-storing birds. Brain Behav Evol 34:308–317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimizu T, Hodos W. 1989. Reversal learning in pigeons: effects of selective lesions of the wulst. Behav Neurosci 103:262–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smulders T V. 2017. The avian hippocampal formation and the stress response. Brain Behav Evol 46:1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevens TA, Krebs JR. 1986. Retrieval of stored seeds by Marsh Tits Parus palustris in the field. Ibis (Lond 1859) 128:513–525. [Google Scholar]
- Stewart M, Fox SE. 1990. Do septal neurons pace the hippocampal theta rhythm? Trends Neurosci 13:163–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stokes TM, Leonard CM, Nottebohm F. 1974. The telencephalon, diencephalon, and mesencephalon of the canary, Serinus canaria, in stereotaxic coordinates. J Comp Neurol 156:337–374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strange BA, Witter MP, Lein ES, Moser EI. 2014. Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nat Rev Neurosci [Internet] 15:655–669. Available from: 10.1038/nrn3785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson LW, Cowan WM. 1975. Hippocampo-hypothalamic connections: Origin in subicular cortex, not Ammon’s horn. Science 189:303–304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson LW, Cowan WM. 1977. An autoradiographic study of the organization of the efferet connections of the hippocampal formation in the rat. J Comp Neurol 172:49–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Székely AD. 1999. The avian hippocampal formation: Subdivisions and connectivity. Behav Brain Res 98:219–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Székely AD, Krebs JR. 1996. Efferent connectivity of the hippocampal formation of the zebra finch (Taenopygia guttata): An anterograde pathway tracing study using Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol 368:198–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tosches MA, Yamawaki TM, Naumann RK, Jacobi AA, Tushev G, Laurent G. 2018. Evolution of pallium, hippocampus, and cortical cell types revealed by single-cell transcriptomics in reptiles. Science 360:881–888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vu ET, Schmidt MF, Mazurek ME. 1998. Interhemispheric coordination of premotor neural activity during singing in adult zebra finches. J Neurosci 18:9088–9098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watanabe S. 2003. Effects of Wulst and ectostriatum lesions on repeated acquisition of spatial discrimination in pigeons. Cogn Brain Res 17:286–292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wittek N, Matsui H, Behroozi M, Otto T, Wittek K, Sari N, Stoecker S, Letzner S, Choudhary V, Peterburs J, Güntürkün O. 2021. Unihemispheric Evidence Accumulation in Pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn 47:303–316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witter MP, Kleven H, Flatmoen AK. 2017. Comparative contemplations on the hippocampus. Brain Behav Evol 90:15–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are openly available in dryad at doi:10.5061/dryad.xwdbrv1jq