Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2022 Sep 22;53(12):4560–4576. doi: 10.1007/s10803-022-05742-5

Autism Traits and Cognitive Performance: Mediating Roles of Sleep Disturbance, Anxiety and Depression

Gaynor E McArthur 1, Eunro Lee 1, Robin Laycock 1,
PMCID: PMC10627948  PMID: 36138298

Abstract

Theories about autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have addressed cognitive deficits however few have examined how comorbid diagnoses, including sleep disturbance, anxiety and depression contribute to the underlying deficits. We investigated potential mediations of common ASD comorbidities in the relationship between sub-clinical autism traits and cognitive performance using an international community sample. Cognitive tasks assessed working memory [executive functioning (EF) theory], mental state attribution [theory of mind (ToM)], and global/local visual processing [weak central coherence (WCC) theory]. Structural equation modelling (SEM) demonstrated sleep disturbance and anxiety mediated the relationship of autism traits on measures of EF, but not WCC and ToM. This suggests that treating the symptoms of sleep disturbance and anxiety may lead to improvements in working memory.

Keywords: Autism traits, Anxiety, Sleep disturbance, Executive function, Theory of mind, Weak central coherence


Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a life-long, neurodevelopmental disorder presenting with repetitive and restrictive patterns of behaviour, and persistent deficits in social communication across more than one context (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Alongside deficits in social cognition there is evidence for difficulties in other cognitive processes, including executive functioning such as working memory, planning and cognitive and emotional regulation, as well anomalies in global processing with a tendency to focus more on detailed ‘local’ information within individuals with ASD as well as those with high autistic traits, a subclinical constellation of traits that is part of a broader autism phenotype (Cody et al., 2014; Richmond et al., 2013). Furthermore, comorbid diagnoses are prevalent in individuals with ASD, with sleep disturbance, anxiety and depression frequently also observed (Breslau, et al., 1996; Kerns et al., 2015; Liu et al., 2006). Currently, little is understood about how these varied comorbidities might contribute to or explain various cognitive difficulties in ASD.

Three prominent cognitive theories have been proposed to underpin ASD symptomologies. These theories seek to explain various features thought to be at the core of ASD, specifically with difficulties in social cognition explained by a deficit in theory of mind (ToM) (Gerlach and Starfelt, 2018; Jones et al., 2018; Premack & Woodruff, 1978); impairments in cognitive performance explained by the executive function (EF) deficit theory (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985; Cantio et al., 2016; Russell, 1997); and finally perceptual and cognitive biases towards detailed or local information processing has been explained by the weak central coherence (WCC) theory (Frith & Happé, 1994; Nuske & Bavin, 2011).

Premack and Woodruff (1978) defined ToM as the ability to impute mental states to oneself, and to understand that others have mental states that are different to one’s own. Baron-Cohen et al. (1985) further posited that individuals with ASD may have deficits in ToM after children with ASD did not employ ToM when tested on a belief question. The question involved guessing where ‘Sally’ would think the location of her marble might be after leaving it in a box, and after which it was secretly moved to another location. Individuals with ASD thought Sally would not go back to the original place where she left her marble, but would instead go to the current location, thus illustrating reduced capacity to understand Sally’s beliefs (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). Individuals with ASD demonstrate persistent deficits in social cognition, evidenced through a reduced capacity for ToM in individuals from a young age (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985; Smith et al., 2010). There are also suggestions of a relationship between ToM, and restrictive and repetitive behaviours observed in individuals with ASD (Jones et al., 2018). A basic level of ToM involves the ability to recognise emotions and to attribute mental states from facial expressions, which has been demonstrated to be impaired in individuals with ASD (Andreou & Skrimpa, 2020; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a, 2001b; Jones et al., 2018; Smith et al., 2010).

While ToM has been useful in furthering our understanding of communicative and social difficulties and to a lesser extent restrictive and repetitive behaviours in ASD (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a, 2001b; Peterson, 2014), there also exist a number of difficulties that are unrelated to social patterns of behaviour. In fact, it is evident that individuals with ASD face deficits in executive functioning, including in planning, working memory, cognitive flexibility, self-regulation and response inhibition (Demetriou et al., 2018; Happe et al., 2006; Hill, 2004; Johnston et al., 2019; Russell, 1997; Storch et al., 2013; De Vries & Geurts, 2015). Importantly, working memory deficits are likely to influence key areas such as reading, attention, intelligence, and reasoning ability (Wang et al., 2018). Several working memory studies have demonstrated that individuals with ASD performed worse particularly on spatial working memory tasks compared to verbal or auditory working memory tasks (Baltruschat et al., 2011; Bodner et al., 2019; Habib et al., 2019; Wang et al., 2017).

Finally, the WCC theory seeks to explain the observed local processing bias and suggests that individuals with ASD have trouble integrating information but also show ability to process local details more competently at the expense of global meaning (Burnette et al., 2005; Frith & Happe, 1994; Frith & Frith, 2001; Happe & Frith, 2006). A number of studies have provided evidence that substantiates a preference for local processing by individuals with ASD showing that WCC can impact perceptual and visuospatial processing, restricted event schemas (cognitive tools for social understanding), comprehension and inferential processing of spoken narratives and ToM (Booth & Happe, 2018; Frith, 2003; Frith & Happé, 2006; Happe, 1999; Loth et al., 2008; Neufeld et al., 2020; Nuske & Bavin, 2011).

One difficulty with investigations into social, cognitive and perceptual anomalies in ASD, is the heterogenous nature of the disorder. This difficulty is compounded by the common comorbidities often associated with ASD (Mannion & Leader, 2013; Matson & Goldin, 2013). The presence of such comorbidities may impact the relationship between the behavioural traits of ASD and various aspects of social and cognitive functioning.

Sleep problems have been found in 50–80% of children with ASD compared to 9–50% in age-matched typically developed children (Kotagal & Broomall, 2012). Sleep problems typically include insomnia, parasomnias (nightmares, wakes screaming, enuresis), hyposomnias, sleep related breathing, and movement disorders (Richdale & Schreck, 2009). Individuals with ASD who have poor sleep have been found to have problems with attention span, social interactions, and with increased severity of autistic symptoms such as oppositional behaviour, aggression, explosiveness, attention deficit, impulsivity hyperactivity, anxiety, depression and mood variability (Deserno et al., 2019; Malhi et al., 2019; Mayes & Calhoun, 2009).

A study by Salmela et al. (2019) investigated the relationship between autistic traits and sleep by examining subclinical autistic traits in the general population, and demonstrated how elevated levels of autistic traits were associated with shorter weekday sleep duration, when comorbid psychiatric symptoms such as anxiety and depression were controlled for. The results suggested that subclinical autism traits should be considered as a possible antecedent to affecting sleep (Rzepecka et al., 2011; Salmela et al., 2019).

Anxiety is another highly prevalent comorbidity in ASD, with varying levels of prevalence being noted, ranging from 11 to 84% of individuals with ASD estimated to have an anxiety disorder (White et al., 2009). Such anxiety was shown to predict poor executive functions in adolescents with ASD (Hollocks et al., 2014). Indeed it has been suggested that anxiety moderates the relationship between autistic symptom severity and quality of life (Smith et al., 2019). Previous research has also demonstrated a relationship between anxiety and autism traits in the general population (Salmela et al., 2019). While there is little research on how these factors together impact cognitive performance, several studies have shown the negative impacts of anxiety on cognitive performance in neurotypical individuals (Gayete et al., 2020; Shilton et al., 2019). It has been posited that anxiety restricts the capacity of working memory (Shackman et al., 2006) and increases a visual perceptual bias towards local information (Basso et al., 1996; Shilton et al., 2019).

Along with sleep disturbance and anxiety, studies have also shown depression to be a common psychiatric comorbidity in ASD, with 70% of individuals having experienced at least one episode of major depression and 50% having had a recurrent depressive disorder (Bitsika & Sharpley, 2015; Leyfer et al., 2006; Sturm & Kasari, 2019). Again, although limited research exists on how depression impacts on cognitive performance in ASD, a number of studies show the negative impacts of depression on cognitive performance in individuals without ASD. For example, individuals with depressed symptoms demonstrate impairments in executive function, including working memory, possibly due to competition between attempts to direct attentional resources to the task and away from the distracting effects of automatic negative thoughts (Christopher & MacDonald, 2005).

Consideration of comorbid anxiety and depression may be further complicated by any reported gender and/or sex differences found in the autism spectrum (Sedgewick et al., 2020). In fact there is now increased awareness that the expression of autism is different for females than males (Rynkiewicz & Łucka, 2018). In those with ASD, there is some mixed evidence for gender differences in terms of psychopathology, though in adults it appears that women are more often diagnosed with comorbid anxiety and depression than men (see Napolitano et al., 2022 for a review). When examining autistic traits in the general population similar patterns appear evident, with higher rates of anxiety in female adults with higher autistic traits (Kanne et al., 2009). Furthermore, other aspects of cognition, including working memory have been suggested to have a different relationship with autistic features depending on gender (Chouinard et al., 2019). In the case of theory of mind, there are some reports females in the general population having superior theory of mind compared with males (Giovagnoli, 2019), though this difference was small and non-significant when using the Reading the Mind in the Eyes task (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a, 2001b). Females compared to males with autism appear to show no differences in ToM, despite higher social reciprocity, indicating behavioural camouflaging (Livingston et al., 2019; Wood-Downie et al., 2021).

A wide-ranging literature has highlighted the complexity in understanding the social, cognitive and perceptual difficulties associated with ASD. For example, difficulties in working memory are associated with ASD and sleep (Calhoun et al., 2019) as well as anxiety and depression (MacNamara et al., 2019; Smith et al., 2018; Storch et al., 2013). In this regard, an understanding of EF deficits in ASD should take into account comorbid factors such as sleep, depression and anxiety. Similarly, there is some evidence that those with depression and anxiety have impairment in ToM (Burnette et al., 2005; Hezel & McNally, 2014; Washburn et al., 2016; Wolkenstein et al., 2011). In the neurotypical population, sleep deprivation has also been shown to impair judgement of facial emotions suggesting poor sleep may lead to impairment in the identification of social cues (Van der Helm et al., 2010).

Thus, the present study sought to examine the possibility that common comorbidities may mediate this relationship between ASD and core cognitive, social, and perceptual anomalies. As a first step to systematically control for the heterogeneity and common comorbidities in ASD, this study adopted the dimensional view of autism and examined autistic traits in the general population (Austin, 2005; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a, 2001b; Landy & Chouinard, 2016; Laycock et al., 2014). This approach of researching the broader autism spectrum suggests that autism traits are normally distributed throughout the general population, with those with a diagnosed ASD forming one end of this spectrum. A number of self-report surveys are now commonly used to measure quantitative autism traits in the non-clinical population (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a, 2001b; Hurley et al., 2007; Kanne et al., 2012), often referred to as the broader autism phenotype (Hurley et al., 2007). A large body of research highlights how neurotypical individuals with higher autism traits, but who do not meet criteria for an ASD diagnosis, are associated with similar comorbid psychiatric issues (Kanne et al., 2009), reduced social cognition (Poljac et al., 2013; Wallace et al., 2010), reduced executive function (Christ et al., 2010), and enhanced local-focussed perceptual processing (Cribb et al., 2016), as those with a diagnosed ASD. This dimensional approach thus provides research opportunities with larger sampling pools, control over other confounding variables such as intellectual functioning and offers higher practicality for ASD studies (Hyseni et al., 2019; Landry & Chouinard, 2016).

Specifically, this study aims to explore the relationship between autistic traits and (1) working memory performance, (2) mental state attribution from faces, and (3) global and local visual information processing, in an attempt to represent elements of three prominent theories of autistic impairment, namely executive function deficits, Theory of Mind deficits, and Weak Central Coherence, respectively. The impact of autistic traits on these cognitive variables will be examined by also testing the mediating roles of sleep disturbance, anxiety, and depression. To our knowledge no previous study in the field has explored this integral modelling in a single study design, and thus this empirical investigation should benefit research and practical intervention initiatives.

Method

Participants

From 243 adult participants recruited via social media (n = 39) and an international crowd sourcing platform (MicroWorkers; n = 204; Shapiro et al., 2013; Tompkins, 2019), 188 complete datasets from the general population were attained over a period of approximately 1 month. Demographic and descriptive information is provided in Table 1. Participants from Facebook were offered the option to enter a draw to win a $50AUD voucher, while MicroWorkers participants received a credit worth 2USD (Bennetts et al., 2019; Cahill et al., 2019).

Table 1.

Demographics and descriptive statistics of participants (N = 188)

Item Total (frequency)
Mean age years (SD) 30.49 (8.75)
Gender
 Male 108 (57%)
 Female 78 (41%)
 Othera 2 (2%)
Education attainment
 High school or below 51 (27%)
 Studying at university 36 (19%)
 Completed university 77 (41%)
 Masters or higher 24 (13%)
Country of residence
 Australia 30 (16%)
 Canada 14 (8%)
 New Zealand 4 (2%)
 United Kingdom 26 (14%)
 United States 111 (60%)
 Other 1 (.5%)
History of mental healthb: diagnosed 137 (74%)

Autism spectrum diagnoses

 Participant diagnosed

16 (9%)
 Family diagnosed 17 (9%)
Sleep disorder diagnosesc: diagnosed 15 (8%)

aDue to the small number of the other gender, these two cases were treated as missing gender in the later analysis to prevent statistical instability

bMental health diagnoses: ADHD, anxiety, anxiety/depression, anxiety/autism, borderline personality disorder, depression/PTSD, panic disorder/PTSD/depression, schizotypal, social anxiety disorder, obsessive compulsive, general anxiety disorder/social phobia, and non-diagnosed

CSleep narcolepsy, sleep apnoea, obstructive sleep disorder, insomnia/bruxism, insomnia, historical sleep maintenance disorder

Materials

Demographic Information

All participants were required to indicate their age, gender, level of education, handedness, and a brief history of health including mental health, sleep disorders and a question about personal and family history of having an ASD diagnosis.

Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ)

The AQ is a 50-item questionnaire that assesses quantitative autistic traits in the general non-clinical population. Participants indicated their level of agreement with statements relating to domains of social skills, communication, imagination, attention to detail, and attention switching (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001a, 2001b). Responses to a four-point Likert scale was scored using a binary scoring system as described by Baron-Cohen et al., (2001a, 2001b). Answers that endorsed the autistic trait scored one point. The reliability of the total AQ score was α = .70 in this sample.

Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21)

The DASS-21 was used to assess depression and anxiety symptoms experienced over the past week (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). The DASS-21 consists of 21 questions using a 4-point Likert scale ranging from ‘did not apply to me at all’ (0) to ‘applied to me very much or most of the time’ (3), such as “I couldn’t seem to experience any positive feeling at all”. The reliability of the DASS–21 was α = .93 for Depression and was α = .82 for Anxiety in this sample.

Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)

The PSQI questionnaire was used to assess sleep quality and quantity (Buysse et al., 1989). It contains 19 items in the seven component domains of subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbance, use of sleep medication and daytime dysfunction. Participants completed a four-point Likert scale that ranges from very good (0) to very bad (3). The component scores were combined to produce the Global Sleep Quality Score of between 0 and 27, with participants scoring 6 or greater considered to be poor sleepers. The reliability of the PSQI was α = .72 in this sample.

Corsi Block Tapping Task

For an index variable of Executive Function, the Corsi Block task was used to assess visuospatial working memory performance (Vandierendonck et al., 2004). An online version was developed for this study using the task builder provided in the Gorilla software. A similar digital version of this task has been successfully utilised previously (Brunetti et al., 2014). As Fig. 1 (top panel) illustrates, the screen presented participants with 9 black squares (‘blocks’) scattered on a white background. One at a time a single block changed to the colour blue for 500 ms before returning to the original black colour. Following a 250 ms interval, another block changed colour. Initially a two-block sequence was shown and participants were required to use the mouse to click on the same sequence of blocks previously presented. The sequence span increased after two repetitions regardless of participant accuracy until a sequence of 8 was presented. Performance was scored as the number of the blocks with the longest span correctly recalled (Brunetti et al., 2014). The Corsi Block Tapping task has been previously reported to have moderate test-rest reliability (White et al., 2018; Williams et al., 2005).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Top: Illustration of the online Corsi block tapping task presented to participants. Following a sequence of blocks that flashed a different colour, participants used a computer mouse to press the same sequence of blocks. Bottom: Stimuli used in the Navon Task: From left to right, Congruent, Global Incongruent, and Local Incongruent Stimuli for X (upper) and A (lower) target stimuli. Participants indicated whether there was an X or A present, which could be at either the global or local levels

Navon Task

For an index variable for Weak Central Coherence (WCC), the Navon task was used to assess a global precedence which refers to a bias towards cognitive processing the overall structure of a stimulus over its individual elements (Hayward et al., 2018; Navon, 1977). As illustrated in Fig. 1 (bottom panel), participants were presented with a central large global letter (H, N, A or X) which was created from the arrangement of smaller local letters (H, N, A or X). The small letters could be either congruent or incongruent with the large letters.

Participants were required to respond by a keyboard press whether an A or an X was present at either local or global level. Thus, on any trial there could be an A or X at the global but not the local level (global incongruent), or an A or X at the local but not the global level (local incongruent), or an A or X at both global and local levels (congruent) with 12 trials for each condition resulting in 36 trials. Reaction time for correct trials were analysed, and the mean reaction time score for each of the three conditions (global incongruent, local incongruent, and congruent) was calculated for each participant.

For statistical analysis, three final outcome variables were computed: (1) local interference (reaction time for global incongruent stimuli—reaction time for congruent stimuli) indicating the interference of task irrelevant incongruent local stimuli on global processing (2) global interference (reaction time for local incongruent stimuli—reaction time for congruent stimuli) indicating the interference of task irrelevant incongruent global stimuli on local processing (3) global precedence (reaction time for local incongruent stimuli—reaction time for global incongruent stimuli) for which a positive score indicates faster reaction time for global than local stimuli, whereas a negative score indicates faster reaction time local than global stimuli. The test re-rest reliability for a Navon task was recently reported to have an IntraClass Correlation (ICC) of .74 (Hedge et al., 2018).

Reading the Mind in the Eye Test

For an index variable of ToM, the revised version of the Eyes test was used to assess individual differences in social cognition (Baron-Cohen et al., 1997, 2001a, 2001b). The test included 36 photographs of faces showing only a rectangle area around the eyes with four mental state terms (e.g., playfulness, desire, worrying, cautiousness). Participants were required to select the corresponding description that best matched the expressed mental state in the photograph. Performance was determined by the sum total of correct responses. The reliability index for this study sample was Cronbach’s α = .83.

Procedure

In accordance with the university research ethics approval, participants completed the questionnaires and cognitive tasks online using the Gorilla Experiment platform (www.gorilla.sc) (Anwyl-Irvine et al., 2019). Participants provided online consent and completed a brief demographics survey. Next the three measures were completed in a randomized order, and subsequently the three cognitive tasks which were also randomised. Randomization used a Latin square design. The study was completed in 30 min on average.

Data Analysis Overview

Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 26 was used for all analyses. For the Navon task, participants with reaction time data faster than 200 ms or slower than 1500 ms on any congruence condition were treated as ingenuine responses and excluded from all analyses. This resulted in 43 cases excluded from the main data set. Outliers of the Navon task and Corsi Block task variables were also excluded while there were no outliers on the Eyes Test.

Autistic trait groups were established following a tertile split based on AQ scores from the current sample. This resulted in lowest tertile (AQ total score < 20, n = 69, 37%) and the highest tertile (AQ total score > 24, n = 62, 33%) constituting low and high AQ groups, respectively. For the main analysis the low AQ group had a mean AQ = 14.42 (SD = 3.15), and the high AQ group had a mean AQ = 29.27 (SD = 4.82). Thus while the AQ profile of our overall sample (M = 21.69, SD = 7.10) is higher than that reported in large population studies (e.g., Stevenson & Hart, 2017; M = 18.04, SD = 5.86), both AQ groups in the current study are significantly different from the sample mean in Stevenson et al. (p’s < .001), indicating good separation for high and low autistic tendency from the general population.

An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to test the mean differences between the high and low AQ groups for the PSQI, Depression, Anxiety, Corsi Block task, and the Eyes Test scores. For the Navon ANOVA the reduced sample was analysed (low AQ, n = 59; high AQ, n = 43).

Multiple regression analysis was also conducted with the total AQ scores as the explanatory variable using the total sample to examine the AQ impact on comorbidity symptoms and cognitive performance, while controlling for demographics, diagnosis of ASD in the participants and family members, other mental health comorbid diagnoses, and sleep disorders.

Finally, Structural Equation Modelling (SEM) with AMOS (Analysis of a Moment Structures) version 26 was used to test multiple mediation roles of sleep disturbance, anxiety and depression in explaining the impact of autistic traits upon cognitive performance measured with Corsi Block task, Navon task, and the Eyes Test. The direct effect and indirect effects through the mediators were tested for their significance (Bentler, 2007; Bentler & Yuan, 1999; Mackinnon, 2012; Rigdon, 1996). Given the potential for gender to influence these effects, gender was also included in the SEM analysis.

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Pearson Correlations

After data cleaning and excluding outliers, descriptive statistics and Pearson’s correlations were tested between AQ score and the three comorbidity variables of sleep disturbance, anxiety and depression, and the cognitive test variables of Eyes Test, CBBT, Navon task (global interference, local interference and global precedence, Table 2). The results of the Pearson correlations indicated that there were significant positive correlations observed between AQ score and depression, anxiety and sleep, while from the cognitive measures, significant negative correlations were observed between AQ score and the Eyes Test and the global precedence measure from the Navon task. Further advanced main analysis was therefore conducted to examine underlying relationships between these variables.

Table 2.

Pearson’s correlations between the autism quotient and study variables

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1 Autism quotient
2 Depression .46***
3 Anxiety .46*** .67***
4 Sleep disturbance .18* .42*** .40***
5 Eyes in the mind − .22** − .06 − .21** − .03
6 Corsi block task − .12 − .09 − .15* − .14 .32***
7 Global interference − .13 − .12 − .05 .06 .05 − .08
8 Local interference .13 − .02 − .01 − .06 − .09 .03 .23**
9 Global precedence − .21 − .08 − .04 .10 .11 − .09 .62*** − .62***
Mean 21.69 7.89 6.08 5.01 23.16 8.15 247.13 178.71 68.42
Standard deviation 7.09 5.64 4.12 2.59 6.30 2.30 145.95 145.95 181.55

N = 188, except correlations involving Navon conditions, n = 144

***p < .001, **p < .01, *p < .05

ANOVA and Multiple Regression Models: Differences Between Low and High AQ Groups

For the Corsi Block task, outliers with two or less than two correct responses were excluded, resulting in one participant from the low AQ group with only 1 correct response being excluded (representing > 3 SD from the group mean). ANOVA showed significant differences between the high and low autism trait groups for the Eyes Test, Corsi Block task, as well as for depression, anxiety, though not for sleep disturbance (Table 3).

Table 3.

ANOVA results comparing high and low AQ groups, and regression analysis of outcome variables explained by AQ scores

Dependent
variables
ANOVA (high vs low AQ) Regressiona (AQ total scores)
F p ηp2 Power β p R2 b [LCI,UCI]
Mind in the eyes 14.24 (1,129)  < .001 .10 .96 − .17 .02 .02

− .15

[− .30, .01]

Corsi block task 5.38 (1,125) .02 .04 .63 − .03 .17 .01

− .09

[− .27, .06]

Global interference 2.18 (1,100) .14 .02 .31 − .16 .08 .07

− 3.16

[− 6.70, 0.38]

Local interference 2.76 (1,100) .10 .03 .38 .09 .34 .04

1.75

[− 1.86, 5.36]

Global precedence 6.79 (1,100) .01 .06 .73 − .20 .03 .11

− 4.91

[− 9.26, − 0.56]

Depression 27.40 (1,129)  < .001 .18 0.99 .30 .00 .20

.38

[.19, .41]

Anxiety 35.10 (1,129)  < .001 .21 1.00 .36 .00 .27

.22

[.14, .29]

Sleep disturbance 0.43 (1,129) .52 .003 .10 .12 .17 .12

.04

[− .02, .09]

Significant results are in bold

aCovariates were controlled in the regression models and they included age, gender, education, mental health diagnosis, sleep disorder, ASD diagnosis for the participant and family members; LCI lower confidence intervals; UCI upper confidence intervals

For the Navon task, as seen in Fig. 2, the pattern of results was as expected with the low group showing more global interference on local processing, and less local interference on global processing than the high AQ group. However ANOVA showed no evidence for significant group differences on these measures (Table 3). To further examine this pattern of results, a mixed design ANOVA with Interference Level (Global vs Local interference) as the within subject factor, and AQ Group (high vs low) as the between subject factor was conducted. This analysis demonstrated a main effect of interference level [F(1,100) = 12.60, p = .001], no significant main effect of group [F(1,100) = 0.02, p = .884], and a significant interaction between interference level and group [F(1,100) = 6.79, p = .011]. Simple main effects were used to interpret this interaction, suggesting that while the low AQ group had significantly larger global than local interference (p < .001), the high AQ group did not show this difference (p = .536). When examining global precedence, a significant group difference was apparent indicating a larger global processing advantage in the low AQ group, when compared with the high AQ group (p < .01; see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Three outcome variables for the navon task for high and low AQ groups. Global interference represents the difference in reaction time between congruent navon stimuli and incongruent stimuli with the target present at the local level. Local interference represents the difference in reaction time between congruent navon stimuli and incongruent stimuli with the target present at the global level. Global precedence represents the difference in reaction time between target identification for global and local incongruent stimuli. The high AQ group demonstrated significantly lower global precedence when compared with the low AQ group. Error bars indicate Standard Error of the Mean. **p < .01

When participant age and gender, along with diagnosis of ASD and other comorbid disorders were controlled in multiple regression, the AQ total scores significantly explained only the Eyes Test, global precedence, depression, and anxiety (see Table 3).

SEM: Mediation of Comorbidities in the Impact of AQ on Cognitive Performance

In exploring mediation roles among the comorbidity variables upon each of the cognitive performance variables, a series of multiple mediation SEMs were tested. As presented in Fig. 3, the most parsimonious model included multiple mediation paths (1) from autism traits to cognitive performance outcome variables via sleep disturbance and in turn through anxiety or depression, (2) from autism traits to cognitive performance outcome variables via anxiety or depression. In total, 10 models (= 2 s mediators (Depression or Anxiety) × 5 dependent variables (Eyes, Corsi Block task, three Navon variables) were tested. Model fit was analysed with χ2 to test for any significant differences in the covariance-variance matrices between the data and the model. Non-significant χ2 indicates good fit, and overall, all models demonstrated good fit with the data as shown in Table 4. As a second step, the individual paths tested in the models are detailed in the following.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Final mediation SEM: the mediation effects of sleep disturbance, depression, and anxiety in the impact of autism traits on theory of mind, executive functioning and weak central coherence. Note. Error terms and correlations are omitted for simplicity

Table 4.

Mediation models: fit indices and proportions of explained variances in cognitive performance variables

Model path in explaining 5 dependent variables
All models also included a path from autism traits to anxiety/depression
Model fit χ2 RMSEA CFI TLI R2

Model 1

Autism traits sleep disturbance anxiety eyes in the mind

1.43

.00

CI .000, .093

1.00 1.10 .13

Model 2

Autism traits sleep disturbance depression eyes in the mind

.57

.00

CI .000, .052

1.00 1.10 .13

Model 3

Autism traits sleep disturbance anxiety Corsi block task

.20

.00

CI .000, .000

1.00 1.13 .06

Model 4

Autism traits sleep disturbance depression Corsi block task

.35

.00

CI .000, .000

1.00 1.13 .04

Model 5

Autism traits sleep disturbance anxiety global interference

3.41

.03

CI .000, .152

1.00 .97 .01

Model 6

Autism traits sleep disturbance depression global interference

2.00

.00

CI .000, .124

1.00 1.08 .01

Model 7

Autism traits sleep disturbance anxiety local interference

3.17

.02

CI .000, .145

1.00 .99 .04

Model 8

Autism traits sleep disturbance depression local interference

1.06

.00

CI .000, .092

1.00 1.15 .03

Model 9

Autism traits sleep disturbance anxiety global precedence

1.82

.00

CI .000, .119

1.00 1.07 .05

Model 10

Autism traits sleep disturbance depression global precedence

1.04

.02

CI .000, .092

1.00 1.14 .05

Non-significant χ2 indicates the models fit the data supporting the null hypothesis posing no significant difference in the covariance-variance matrices between the data and the model. df = 3. RMSEA: root mean square error of approximation, CFI: Comparative Fit Index, TLI: Tucker–Lewis Index, CI: 95% Confidence Interval. Sample sizes: eyes in the mind task (N = 186), Corsi block task (N = 177), global incongruence (N = 137), local incongruence (N = 142), global Precedence (N = 139)

In none of the models tested were all paths found to have significant relationships. One model however was suggestive of illuminating important relationships between the variables. Figure 4 presents the SEM coefficients and paths of the mediation model on working memory, measured with the Corsi Block task. All paths within this model were significant, except for the final path. Although statistically non-significant at the conventional alpha level (.05), there was the suggestion of full mediation effects of sleep and anxiety: autism traits explained the variance in working memory performance but only indirectly through sleep disturbance and anxiety. Specifically, autism traits significantly predicted sleep disturbance (p < .01) which in turn significantly explained anxiety (p < .01), though anxiety was associated with working memory performance with a non-significant relationship (p = .06). As detailed in Table 5, a one-unit increase in autism traits was associated with a 0.44-SD increase in anxiety and a 0.16 − SD increase in sleep disturbance which in turn indirectly decreased working memory performance with − 0.07 SD and − 0.01 SD respectively.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Standardised SEM coefficients: the mediation effects of sleep disturbance and anxiety in the impact of autism traits on executive functioning. Note. For the model fit, refer to Table 4. Sleep Disturbance: Good Sleep = 0, Poor Sleep = 1, Gender: male = 1, female = 2, p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, Error terms, correlations, and related coefficients are omitted for simplicity. Solid lines depict significant paths while dotted lines depict non-significant effects

Table 5.

SEM coefficients of the final model: mediation of anxiety and sleep disturbance in the impact of autism traits on working memory

Coefficients
Predictors and paths b SE β p
Autism traits → sleep disturbance 0.012* .005 .16 .029
Sleep disturbance → anxiety 2.303** .534 .27 .000
Autism traits → anxiety 0.267** .038 .44 .000
Anxiety → EF − 0.081 .043 − .16 .060
Autism traits → EF − 0.020 .026 − .07 .437
Gender → EF .59 .318 .14 .062
Autism traits→ anxiety→ EF − .02 .01 − .07 .101
Autism traits→ sleep disturbance→ anxiety .04* .02 .07 .019
Autism traits → sleep disturbance→ anxiety→ EF − .003 .003 − .01 .199

*p < .05, **p < .01, †p < .10, β: Standardised coefficient, b: unstandardized coefficient, SE: standard error. EF: executive functioning indexed by the working memory variable, Corsi Block Task total scores. Gender: male = 1, female = 2

The full mediation of sleep and anxiety was not observed either when examining the measures of ToM and WCC. Autistic traits directly explained local interference (b = .19, p = .044) without mediation of sleep disturbance and anxiety or depression in the models. Mental state attribution, the Eyes test scores (b = − .14, p = 0.073) and global precedence (b = .04, p = 0.064) demonstrated trends for being explained by autistic traits, but were not statistically significant. In contrast, global interference (b = − .03, p = .780) was not explained directly by autistic traits and any mediation of sleep disturbance and anxiety or depression was not significant in the models either.

Discussion

The present study investigated potential mediations of prevalent ASD comorbidities including sleep disturbance, anxiety and depression in the relationship between autism traits and cognitive performance using an international community sample from the general population. The dimensional approach to exploring the broader autism phenotype was used to maximise the research capacity with a large sample size and sophisticated statistical modelling. Results demonstrated that autism traits were linked to cognitive measures, and also to common comorbidities, though as will be discussed below, performance on a measure of working memory was mediated by both sleep and anxiety.

Autism Traits: Consistency and Complexities in Comorbid Symptoms and Cognitive Performance

Individuals with low and high autistic traits in the sample showed significant differences in mental state attribution as a ToM index; in executive function as measured by a spatial working memory task; and from a Navon global/local task when assessing global precedence, though not global or local interference, which were taken as measures of Weak Central Coherence. In addition, significant autistic trait group differences were established for anxiety and depression, while no group differences were found for sleep disturbance. These findings in general are supportive of previous findings of impairments in the autism spectrum in a range of social, cognitive and perceptual tasks (Baltruschat et al., 2011; Baron-Cohen et al., 1985; Plaisted et al., 1999; Smith et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2018), as well as difficulties in mental health (Leyfer et al., 2006). We did not replicate the previous finding of abnormalities in sleep linked to autism traits in the general population (Salmela et al., 2019). Importantly however, Salmela et al. showed an association between autisic traits and objective measure of sleep using wrist actigraphy recordings, whereas similar to the current findings these authors found no association between autism traits and self-reported sleep quality (also using the PSQI). These findings together converge with evidence that similar brain differences are found in high autism trait groups as in clinical populations with ASD (Focquaert & Vanneste, 2015) and are supportive of research utilising this dimensional approach to understand autistic traits, as a complement to clinical ASD studies (Landry & Chouinard, 2016).

Regarding the Navon task which was used as a measure of global/local processing and the WCC theory, results indicate significant differences for global precedence indicating that those with higher autism traits were slower to respond to global stimuli relative to local stimuli suggestive of a reduced bias for global processing. On the other hand the results established no group differences in local or global interference, while it should be noted that the group means were in the expected direction, with high AQ participants having greater local interference on global processing that the low AQ participants. The lack of significance on the local interference and global interference measures may be attributable to the online testing environment where reduced signal to noise is expected, particularly for a reaction time measure. Another complication in comparing Navon task results from different studies is the wide range of stimulus designs (e.g., size of local to global stimulus ratios, letters vs numbers vs arrows) (Hayward et al., 2018; Navon, 1977). Nevertheless, as can be seen in Fig. 2, and as suggested by a second analysis, low AQ participants demonstrated more global interference than they did local interference, whereas the degree of global and local interference was equivalent in the high AQ group, again suggested of a reduced global bias. These results thus broadly support the notion that autistic traits, similar to ASD findings (Plaisted et al., 1999) are associated with a bias against processing global information.

Notably, only mental state attribution, global precedence, depression, and anxiety were significantly explained by autistic traits in hierarchical regression models when the total AQ scores were modelled using the whole sample with demographic variables including presence of a mental disorder and ASD diagnoses controlled. These results, with the measure of working memory (Corsi block task) not significant, suggest more complex relationships between the variables which called for the subsequent SEM modelling with more sophisticated mediation analysis. While limited utility of using the AQ scores has been observed in the regression results, it is also suggested that the covariates of age, gender, education, mental health including ASD, and sleep disorders should be taken into account in future research designs (Ruzich et al., 2015).

Impacts of Comorbidities: Mediation Roles for explaining Working Memory?

For the main investigation, the SEM results demonstrated that although statistically non-significant, sleep disturbance and anxiety were suggested to fully mediate the impact of autism traits on working memory as an index of executive function. Specifically, SEM showed autism traits explained the variance in working memory measured with the Corsi Block task, but only indirectly through sleep disturbance and anxiety, suggesting a full mediation. Importantly, this finding did not reach statistical significance, likely due to the the large number of predictors variables included in the analysis. The large number of estimated coefficients overburdened the models with the relatively small sample size for the complexity of the multiple mediation paths. In understanding the convoluted nature of the interactions of neurological, psychological, and behavioural factors and symptoms in autism, even larger samples would be required to confirm these relationships. Furthermore, replication in clinical samples will also be important to confirm the relevance to those diagnosed with ASD. Interestingly, this mediation was not observed for depression. The suggestion of anxiety and sleep mediating the relationship between autism traits and executive function is consistent with recent research into the relationships between ASD and sleep with elevated levels of subclinical autistic traits shown to be associated with shorter weekday sleep duration, when comorbid psychiatric symptoms such as anxiety and depression were controlled for (Salmela et al., 2019). While the present study supported Salmela et al. (2019), where subclinical autism traits should be considered as a possible antecedent to affecting adolescent sleep, we now also suggest that anxiety was further explained by sleep disturbance and in turn impacted working memory performance.

Previous research has highlighted that in children with ASD, autism traits were positively associated with chronic stress hormone level (assessed from cortisol in hair), while in the same children cortisol was negatively associated with spatial working memory performance (Ogawa et al., 2017). Interestingly, Ogawa et al. (2017) found no similar relationships between cortisol, autism traits and a revised child-version of the Eyes task, which also seems to support the results of the present study. The maintenance of visuospatial information in working memory is understood to be reliant on a right-lateralised fronto-parietal network (Rottschy et al., 2012). Atypical development of structural connectivity within the frontoparietal network has been demonstrated in ASD (Lin et al., 2019), with a recent meta-analysis pointing to the importance of this network for underlying executive dysfunction in ASD (Ma et al., 2015; May & Kana, 2020).

Anxiety and sleep difficulties are both associated with impairments in cognitive functioning, including in working memory (Dai et al., 2020; Shackman et al., 2006). Similarly, while both anxiety and sleep deprivation have detrimental effects on frontoparietal networks (Ma et al., 2015, 2019), previous research have highlighted how individuals with ASD and sleep problems are more likely to suffer from anxiety and have difficulties with a number of cognitive and behavioural domains (Malhi et al., 2019; Mayes & Calhoun, 2009; Mughal et al., 2020). The current results thus build on these findings by suggesting that severity of autistic behaviours may predict anxiety, and in turn sleep difficulties, which finally may mediate working memory performance. A recent meta-analysis found broad difficulties in executive function in ASD compared with neurotypical control participants, though it was suggested that this may not achieve clinical sensitivity (Demetriou et al., 2018). However, our findings indicate that future research may need to investigate the causal influence of anxiety and sleep difficulties on executive functioning, to improve the understanding of the relationship between autistic behaviours and executive functioning.

Calhoun et al. (2020) have recently shown that in adolescents with ASD, those who had a sleep disturbance demonstrated a relationship between working memory and learning problems, while those without sleep disturbance did not show this relationship. The authors suggest that interventions may need to target both sleep and working memory. We suggest that anxiety may also be a critical target of intervention to allow improvement in both sleep and working memory, though future studies will need to confirm the nature of these mediating relationships in clinical populations.

Contrary to our hypotheses, sleep disturbance, anxiety and depression did not mediate the relationship between autism traits and performance of ToM (mental state attribution) and WCC (Navon task on global and local processing) measures. However, although both anxiety and ASD have been associated with a disturbance in ToM, there is some evidence that the nature of this disturbance may differ between conditions. For example it has been suggested that while those with ASD fail to use Theory of Mind to infer another’s mental state, anxiety has been associated with ‘over-mentalising’ (Washburn et al., 2016), in which mental states are inferred, though they are incorrectly made. This difference in ToM abilities, while anomalous in both clinical conditions, would clearly be expected to complicate the relationship between autism traits, anxiety and ToM.

The failure to establish mediating relationships in predicting measures of ToM and WCC may also be due to the online cognitive task settings and limited refinement in the stimuli. In particular the Navon task requires accurate measurement of motor reaction time which may be expected to have increased inter- and intra-participant variability. Although online cognitive testing has begun to be more common, with some recent validation studies (Wesnes et al., 2017), there are still technical difficulties in ensuring consistent conditions for all participants. More controlled laboratory studies with rigorous sampling should cross-validate the results of the present study.

Limitations and Future Research and Practice

One of the limitations in this study was that the PSQI questionnaire results may have been impacted due to an error in omission of a single item and subsequent default scoring of 0 for those responses. Due to the non-linear nature of scoring for the PSQI, this is not suggested to have altered the ability of this scale to detect sleep disturbance. Nevertheless, some caution is required in comparing PSQI scores to previous studies, and may have influenced the current findings on sleep disturbance in subtle ways. In addition further larger sample sizes will allow more comprehensive mediation models with all relevant covariates to be controlled for which was not available for the present study. Due to the limited scope of this study, subscales of AQ such as social skills, attention shift, and communication skills were not analysed which should be examined in future work. Similarly, sub factor analysis of sleep disturbance such as ‘Getting to Sleep Difficulty’ and ‘Activity and Enthusiasm Problem’ by providing more precise unidimensional measures may provide more clinically meaningful elaboration of the relationship between variables. Finally, it is important to caution that due to the sample being a non-diagnosed community sample, clinical implications may be different for diagnosed persons. It will be important to cross-validate these findings in a clinical population.

Conclusion

This study examined three major cognitive theories to understand the autistic symptoms with three common comorbidities. It is the first time where all three ASD theories have been tested in a single study design to assess how prominent comorbidities may impact the key cognitive symptomologies of ASD. In terms of methodology, the present study also demonstrated time and cost efficiencies through using an online experimental platform and benefits from using sub-clinical autism traits assessed through the AQ with heightened approachability, larger sampling pools, and higher practicality for ASD research.

While no significant mediation effects were found regarding the tasks expected to test WCC theory and ToM, the present results demonstrated that the comorbidities of sleep disturbance and anxiety are likely to play a mediating role in the impact of autistic traits on working memory. This finding thus adds evidence to the importance of EF theory in explaining that the deficits in executive functioning in autism may be indirect via the mediations of sleep disturbance and anxiety that were impacted by autistic traits. This suggests that treating the symptoms of sleep disturbance and anxiety may lead to improvements in working memory and possibly in other executive functions (Andersen et al., 2008; Baltruschat et al., 2011; Storch et al., 2013). Future research with different measures of executive function such as emotion regulation may expand the present findings, and determine how specific the mediating role of anxiety and sleep is to visuospatial working memory.

Author contributions

RL developed the stimuli and tasks. EL undertook the interpretation of data analyses. All authors conceived and designed the study, collected the data, contributed to interpretation of data analyses and contributed to the first draft. All authors provided feedback and contributed to the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by CAUL and its Member Institutions.

Declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This study was approved by the RMIT College of Science, Engineering and Health Human Ethics Committee and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards as laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

Consent to Participate

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5. American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen IM, Kaczmarska J, McGrew SG, Malow BA. Melatonin for insomnia in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Neurology. 2008;23(5):482–485. doi: 10.1177/0883073807309783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andreou M, Skrimpa V. Theory of mind deficits and neurophysiological operations in autism spectrum disorders: A review. Brain Sciences. 2020;10(6):393. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10060393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anwyl-Irvine AL, Massonnié J, Flitton A, Kirkham N, Evershed JK. Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods. 2019 doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01237-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Austin EJ. Personality correlates of the broader autism phenotype as assessed by the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;38(2):451–460. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.04.022. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baltruschat L, Hasselhorn M, Tarbox J, Dixon DR, Najdowski AC, Mullins RD, Gould ER. Addressing working memory in children with autism through behavioral intervention. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011;5(1):267–276. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1997;38(7):813–822. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-7610.1997.tb01599.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition. 1985;21(1):37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2001;42(2):241–251. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2001;31(1):5–17. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Basso MR, Schefft BK, Ris MD, Dember WN. Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1996;2(3):249–255. doi: 10.1017/S1355617700001193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bennetts SK, Hokke S, Crawford S, Hackworth NJ, Leach LS, Nguyen C, Nicholson JM, Cooklin AR. Using paid and free Facebook methods to recruit Australian parents to an online survey: An evaluation. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2019;21(3):e11206. doi: 10.2196/11206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bentler PM. On tests and indices for evaluating structural models. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;42(5):825–829. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.024. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bentler PM, Yuan KH. Structural equation modeling with small samples: Test statistics. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 1999;34(2):181–197. doi: 10.1207/S15327906Mb340203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bitsika V, Sharpley CF. Differences in the prevalence, severity and symptom profiles of depression in boys and adolescents with an autism spectrum disorder versus normally developing controls. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education. 2015;62(2):158–167. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2014.998179. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bodner KE, Cowan N, Christ SE. Contributions of filtering and attentional allocation to working memory performance in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2019;128(8):881. doi: 10.1037/abn0000471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Booth RD, Happé FG. Evidence of reduced global processing in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2018;48(4):1397–1408. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2724-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Breslau N, Roth T, Rosenthal L, Andreski P. Sleep disturbance and psychiatric disorders: A longitudinal epidemiological study of young adults. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;39(6):411–418. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00188-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brunetti R, Del Gatto C, Delogu F. eCorsi: Implementation and testing of the Corsi block-tapping task for digital tablets. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:939. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00188-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Burnette CP, Mundy PC, Meyer JA, Sutton SK, Vaughan AE, Charak D. Weak central coherence and its relations to theory of mind and anxiety in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2005;35(1):63–73. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1035-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, III, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Research. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cahill TJ, Wertz B, Zhong Q, Parlato A, Donegan J, Forman R, Manot S, Wu T, Xu Y, Cummings JJ, Norkunas Cunningham T, Wang C. The search for consumers of web-based raw DNA interpretation services: Using social media to target hard-to-reach populations. Journal of Medical Internet Research. 2019;21(7):e12980. doi: 10.2196/12980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Calhoun SL, Pearl AM, Fernandez-Mendoza J, Durica KC, Mayes SD, Murray MJ. Sleep disturbances increase the impact of working memory deficits on learning problems in adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03928-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cantio C, Jepsen JRM, Madsen GF, Bilenberg N, White SJ. Exploring ‘The autisms’ at a cognitive level. Autism Research. 2016;9(12):1328–1339. doi: 10.1002/aur.1630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Calhoun SL, Pearl AM, Fernandez-Mendoza J, Durica KC, Mayes SD, Murray MJ. Sleep Disturbances Increase the Impact of Working Memory Deficits on Learning Problems in Adolescents with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2020;50(5):1701–1713. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03928-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chouinard B, Gallagher L, Kelly C. He said, she said: Autism spectrum diagnosis and gender differentially affect relationships between executive functions and social communication. Autism. 2019;23(7):1793–1804. doi: 10.1177/1362361318815639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Christ SE, Kanne SM, Reiersen AM. Executive function in individuals with subthreshold autism traits. Neuropsychology. 2010;24(5):590. doi: 10.1037/a0019176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Christopher G, MacDonald J. The impact of clinical depression on working memory. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2005;10(5):379–399. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cody MW, Clerkin EM, Stevens ES, Gasser ML, Pasciuti ML, Teachman BA. Social anxiety disorder and global/local performance on a visuospatial processing task. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology. 2014;5(1):83–96. doi: 10.5127/jep.035013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cribb SJ, Olaithe M, Di Lorenzo R, Dunlop PD, Maybery MT. Embedded figures test performance in the broader autism phenotype: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2016;46(9):2924–2939. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2832-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dai C, Zhang Y, Cai X, Peng Z, Zhang L, Shao Y, Wang C. Effects of sleep deprivation on working memory: Change in functional connectivity between the dorsal attention, default mode, and fronto-parietal networks. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2020 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. de Vries M, Geurts H. Influence of autism traits and executive functioning on quality of life in children with an autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;45(9):2734–2743. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2438-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Demetriou EA, Lampit A, Quintana DS, Naismith SL, Song YJC, Pye JE, Hickie I, Guastella AJ. Autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular Psychiatry. 2018;23(5):1198–1204. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Deserno MK, Borsboom D, Begeer S, Agelink van Rentergem JA, Mataw K, Geurts HM. Sleep determines quality of life in autistic adults: A longitudinal study. Autism Research. 2019;12(5):794–801. doi: 10.1002/aur.2103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Focquaert F, Vanneste S. Autism spectrum traits in normal individuals: A preliminary VBM analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015;9:264. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Frith U. Autism: Explaining the enigma. Blackwell Publishing; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  37. Frith U, Frith C. The biological basis of social interaction. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2001;10(5):151–155. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00137. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Frith U, Happé F. Autism: beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition. 1994;50:115–132. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90024-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gayete S, Giné A, Miret M, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Haro JM, Olaya B. Cognitive function associated with different diagnoses of anxiety disorders over the lifespan: Results from a Spanish representative sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2020;75:102296. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gerlach C, Starrfelt R. Global precedence effects account for individual differences in both face and object recognition performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2018;25(4):1365–1372. doi: 10.3758/s13423-018-1458-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Giovagnoli AR. Theory of mind across lifespan from ages 16 to 81 years. Epilepsy & Behavior. 2019;100:106349. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.05.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Habib A, Harris L, Pollick F, Melville C. A meta-analysis of working memory in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(4):e0216198. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Happé F. Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1999;3(6):216–222. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01318-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Happé F, Booth R, Charlton R, Hughes C. Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain and Cognition. 2006;61(1):25–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Happé F, Frith U. The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36(1):5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Happé F, Frith U. The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused Cognitive Style in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36(1):5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hayward DA, Fenerci C, Ristic J. An investigation of global-local processing bias in a large sample of typical individuals varying in autism traits. Consciousness and Cognition. 2018;65:271–279. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hedge C, Powell G, Sumner P. The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods. 2018;50(3):1166–1186. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hezel DM, McNally RJ. Theory of Mind Impairments in social anxiety disorder. Behavior Therapy. 2014;45(4):530–540. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2014.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hill EL. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review. 2004;24(2):189–233. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hollocks MJ, Jones CR, Pickles A, Baird G, Happé F, Charman T, Simonoff E. The association between social cognition and executive functioning and symptoms of anxiety and depression in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research. 2014;7(2):216–228. doi: 10.1002/aur.1361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Hurley RS, Losh M, Parlier M, Reznick JS, Piven J. The broad autism phenotype questionnaire. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2007;37(9):1679–1690. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0299-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hyseni F, Blanken LM, Muetzel R, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H, White T. Autistic traits and neuropsychological performance in 6-to-10-year-old children: A population-based study. Child Neuropsychology. 2019;25(3):352–369. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2018.1465543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Johnston K, Murray K, Spain D, Walker I, Russell A. Executive function: Cognition and behaviour in adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2019;49(10):4181–4192. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04133-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Jones CRG, Simonoff E, Baird G, Pickles A, Marsden AJS, Tregay J, Happé F, Charman T. The association between theory of mind, executive function, and the symptoms of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research. 2018;11(1):95–109. doi: 10.1002/aur.1873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kanne SM, Christ SE, Reiersen AM. Psychiatric symptoms and psychosocial difficulties in young adults with autistic traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2009;39(6):827–833. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0688-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kanne SM, Wang J, Christ SE. The subthreshold autism trait questionnaire (SATQ): Development of a brief self-report measure of subthreshold autism traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2012;42(5):769–780. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1308-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kerns CM, Kendall PC, Zickgraf H, Franklin ME, Miller J, Herrington J. Not to be overshadowed or overlooked: Functional impairments associated with comorbid anxiety disorders in youth with ASD. Behavior Therapy. 2015;46(1):29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2014.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kotagal S, Broomall E. Sleep in children with autism spectrum disorder. Pediatric Neurology. 2012;47(4):242–251. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Landry O, Chouinard PA. Why we should study the broader autism phenotype in typically developing populations. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2016;17(4):584–595. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2016.12000. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Laycock R, Cross AJ, Dalle Nogare F, Crewther SG. Self-rated social skills predict visual perception: Impairments in object discrimination requiring transient attention associated with high autistic tendency. Autism Research. 2014;7(1):104–111. doi: 10.1002/aur.1336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Leyfer OT, Folstein SE, Bacalman S, Davis NO, Dinh E, Morgan J, Tager-Flusberg H, Lainhart JE. Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with autism: Interview development and rates of disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36(7):849–861. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0123-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lin HY, Perry A, Cocchi L, Roberts JA, Tseng WYI, Breakspear M, Gau SSF. Development of frontoparietal connectivity predicts longitudinal symptom changes in young people with autism spectrum disorder. Translational Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0418-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Liu X, Hubbard JA, Fabes RA, Adam JB. Sleep disturbances and correlates of children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2006;37(2):179–191. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0028-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Livingston LA, Colvert E, Bolton P, Happé F. Good social skills despite poor theory of mind: exploring compensation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2019;60(1):102–110. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12886. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Loth E, Gómez JC, Happé F. Event schemas in autism spectrum disorders: The role of theory of mind and weak central coherence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38(3):449–463. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0412-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH. The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1995;33(3):335–343. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ma N, Dinges DF, Basner M, Rao H. How acute total sleep loss affects the attending brain: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Sleep. 2015;38(2):233–240. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ma Z, Wang C, Hines CS, Lu X, Wu Y, Xu H, Li J, Wang Q, Pang M, Zhong Y, Zhang N. Frontoparietal network abnormalities of gray matter volume and functional connectivity in patients with generalized anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 2019;286:24–30. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. MacKinnon D. Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. Routledge; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  71. MacNamara A, Jackson TB, Fitzgerald JM, Hajcak G, Phan KL. Working memory load and negative picture processing: Neural and behavioral associations with panic, social anxiety, and positive affect. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 2019;4(2):151–159. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Malhi P, Kaur A, Singhi P, Sankhyan N. Sleep dysfunction and behavioral daytime problems in children with autism spectrum disorders: A comparative study. The Indian Journal of Pediatrics. 2019;86(1):12–17. doi: 10.1007/s12098-018-2731-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Mannion A, Leader G. Comorbidity in autism spectrum disorder: A literature review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2013;7(12):1595–1616. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.09.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  74. Matson JL, Goldin RL. Comorbidity and autism: Trends, topics and future directions. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2013;7(10):1228–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2013.07.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. May KE, Kana RK. Frontoparietal network in executive functioning in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research. 2020;13(10):1762–1777. doi: 10.1002/aur.2403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Variables related to sleep problems in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2009;3(4):931–941. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2009.04.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Mughal R, Hill CM, Joyce A, Dimitriou D. Sleep and cognition in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD) and children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Brain Sciences. 2020;10(11):863. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10110863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Napolitano A, Schiavi S, La Rosa P, Rossi-Espagnet MC, Petrillo S, Bottino F, Tagliente E, Longo D, Lupi E, Casula L, Valeri G. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: Diagnostic, neurobiological, and behavioral features. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2022 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.889636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Navon D. Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology. 1977;9(3):353–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Neufeld J, Hagström A, Van’t Westeinde A, Lundin K, Cauvet É, Willfors C, Isaksson J, Lichtenstein P, Bölte S. Global and local visual processing in autism—A co-twin-control study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2020;61(4):470–479. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Nuske HJ, Bavin EL. Narrative comprehension in 4–7-year-old children with autism: Testing the weak central coherence Account. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. 2011;46(1):108–119. doi: 10.3109/13682822.2010.484847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Ogawa S, Lee YA, Yamaguchi Y, Shibata Y, Goto Y. Associations of acute and chronic stress hormones with cognitive functions in autism spectrum disorder. Neuroscience. 2017;343:229–239. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Peterson C. Theory of mind understanding and empathic behavior in children with autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 2014;39:16–21. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Plaisted K, Swettenham J, Rees L. Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1999;40(5):733–742. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Poljac E, Poljac E, Wagemans J. Reduced accuracy and sensitivity in the perception of emotional facial expressions in individuals with high autism spectrum traits. Autism. 2013;17(6):668–680. doi: 10.1177/1362361312455703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Premack D, Woodruff G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1978;1(4):515–526. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00076512. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  87. Preti A, Vellante M, Petretto DR. The psychometric properties of the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: An item response theory (IRT) analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2017;22(3):233–253. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2017.1300091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Richdale AL, Schreck KA. Sleep problems in autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, nature, & possible biopsychosocial aetiologies. Sleep Medicine Reviews. 2009;13(6):403–411. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Richmond LL, Thorpe M, Berryhill ME, Klugman J, Olson IR. Individual differences in autistic trait load in the general population predict visual working memory performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2013;66(6):1182–1195. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.734831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Rigdon EE. CFI versus RMSEA: A comparison of two fit indexes for structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal. 1996;3(4):369–379. doi: 10.1080/10705519609540052. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB. Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. NeuroImage. 2012;60(1):830–846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Russell JE. Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford University Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  93. Ruzich E, Allison C, Smith P, Watson P, Auyeung B, Ring H, Baron-Cohen S. Measuring autistic traits in the general population: A systematic review of the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Molecular Autism. 2015;6(1):2. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-6-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Rynkiewicz A, Łucka I. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in girls. Co-occurring psychopathology. Sex differences in clinical manifestation. Psychiatria Polska. 2018;52(4):629–639. doi: 10.12740/PP/OnlineFirst/58837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Rzepecka H, McKenzie K, McClure I, Murphy S. Sleep, anxiety and challenging behaviour in children with intellectual disability and/or autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2011;32(6):2758–2766. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Salmela L, Kuula L, Merikanto I, Räikkönen K, Pesonen AK. Autistic traits and sleep in typically developing adolescents. Sleep Medicine. 2019;54:164–171. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.09.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Sedgewick F, Leppanen J, Tchanturia K. Gender differences in mental health prevalence in autism. Advances in Autism. 2020;7(3):208–224. doi: 10.1108/AIA-01-2020-0007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Shackman AJ, Sarinopoulos I, Maxwell JS, Pizzagalli DA, Lavric A, Davidson RJ. Anxiety selectively disrupts visuospatial working memory. Emotion. 2006;6(1):40. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Shapiro DN, Chandler J, Mueller PA. Using Mechanical Turk to study clinical populations. Clinical Psychological Science. 2013;1(2):213–220. doi: 10.1108/AIA-01-2020-0007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  100. Shilton AL, Laycock R, Crewther SG. Different effects of trait and state anxiety on global-local visual processing following acute stress. Cognition, Brain, Behavior. 2019;23(3):155–170. [Google Scholar]
  101. Shackman AJ, Sarinopoulos I, Maxwell JS, Pizzagalli DA, Lavric A, Davidson RJ. Anxiety selectively disrupts visuospatial working memory. Emotion. 2006;6(1):40–61. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Smith IC, Ollendick TH, White SW. Anxiety moderates the influence of ASD severity on quality of life in adults with ASD. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2019;62:39–47. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2019.03.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Smith MJL, Montagne B, Perrett DI, Gill M, Gallagher L. Detecting subtle facial emotion recognition deficits in high-functioning autism using dynamic stimuli of varying intensities. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(9):2777–2781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Hinderliter AL, Watkins LL, Hoffman BM, Sherwood A. Microvascular endothelial function and neurocognition among adults with major depressive disorder. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2018;26(10):1061–1069. doi: 10.24193/cbb.2019.23.09. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Stevenson JL, Hart KR. Psychometric properties of the autism-spectrum quotient for assessing low and high levels of autistic traits in college students. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2017;47(6):1838–1853. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3109-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Storch EA, Arnold EB, Lewin AB, Nadeau JM, Jones AM, De Nadai AS, Jane Mutch P, Selles RR, Ung D, Murphy TK. The effect of cognitive-behavioral therapy versus treatment as usual for anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorders: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;52(2):132–142. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Sturm A, Kasari C. Academic and psychosocial characteristics of incoming college freshmen with autism spectrum disorder: The role of comorbidity and gender. Autism Research. 2019;12(6):931–940. doi: 10.1002/aur.2099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Tompkins, K. A. (2019). A comparison of mechanical turk with traditional recruitment settings for conducting psychotherapy research [Doctoral dissertation, University of Alaska Anchorage]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing
  109. Van Der Helm E, Gujar N, Walker MP. Sleep deprivation impairs the accurate recognition of human emotions. Sleep. 2010;33(3):335–342. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.3.335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Vandierendonck A, Kemps E, Fastame MC, Szmalec A. Working memory components of the Corsi blocks task. British Journal of Psychology. 2004;95(1):57–79. doi: 10.1348/000712604322779460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Wallace S, Sebastian C, Pellicano E, Parr J, Bailey A. Face processing abilities in relatives of individuals with ASD. Autism Research. 2010;3(6):345–349. doi: 10.1002/aur.161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Wang Y, Zhang YB, Liu LL, Cui JF, Wang J, Shum DH, van Amelsvoort T, Chan RC. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychology Review. 2017;27(1):46–61. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9336-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Wang Z, Jing J, Igarashi K, Fan L, Yang S, Li Y, Jin Y. Executive function predicts the visuospatial working memory in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Autism Research. 2018;11(8):1148–1156. doi: 10.1002/aur.1967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Washburn D, Wilson G, Roes M, Rnic K, Harkness KL. Theory of mind in social anxiety disorder, depression, and comorbid conditions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2016;37:71–77. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wesnes KA, Brooker H, Ballard C, McCambridge L, Stenton R, Corbett A. Utility, reliability, sensitivity and validity of an online test system designed to monitor changes in cognitive function in clinical trials. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2017;32(12):e83–e92. doi: 10.1002/gps.4659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. White N, Forsyth B, Lee A, Machado L. Repeated computerized cognitive testing: Performance shifts and test–retest reliability in healthy young adults. Psychological Assessment. 2018;30(4):539. doi: 10.1037/pas0000503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. White SW, Oswald D, Ollendick T, Scahill L. Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Clinical Psychology Review. 2009;29(3):216–229. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.01.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Williams LM, Simms E, Clark CR, Paul RH, Rowe D, Gordon E. The test-retest reliability of a standardized neurocognitive and neurophysiological test battery: “Neuromarker”. International Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;115(12):1605–1630. doi: 10.1080/00207450590958475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Wolkenstein L, Schönenberg M, Schirm E, Hautzinger M. I can see what you feel, but I can’t deal with it: Impaired theory of mind in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011;132(1–2):104–111. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Wood-Downie H, Wong B, Kovshoff H, Mandy W, Hull L, Hadwin JA. Sex/gender differences in camouflaging in children and adolescents with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2021;51:1353–1364. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04615-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES