Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Nov 16.
Published in final edited form as: J Couns Dev. 2022 Mar 9;100(4):352–363. doi: 10.1002/jcad.12430

Sensation Seeking and Adolescent Drinking: Do Protective Behavioral Strategies Lower Risk?

Diana M Doumas a,b, G Michael Russo a,b, Raissa Miller a,b, Susan Esp b,c, Nadine R Mastroleo d, Rob Turrisi e
PMCID: PMC10653376  NIHMSID: NIHMS1941085  PMID: 37974903

Abstract

Using a cross-sectional design, we examined protective behavioral strategies (PBS) as a moderator of the relationship between sensation seeking and hazardous drinking and alcohol-related consequences among high school seniors (N = 212). Hierarchical regression analyses indicated sensation seeking was a significant predictor of binge drinking (β = .65, p < .001), pre-partying (β = .71, p < .001), gaming (β = .75, p < .001), and alcohol-related consequences (β = .69, p < .001). Further, PBS moderated these relationships such that among high sensation seeking adolescents, PBS use was associated with better outcomes, including lower levels of binge drinking (β = −.37, p < .01), pre-partying (β = −.44, p < .01), gaming (β = −.31, p < .05), and alcohol-related consequences (β = −.53, p < .001). We discuss counseling implications, including assessment and harm reduction strategies focusing on PBS to reduce hazardous drinking among high sensation seeking adolescents.

Keywords: Adolescents, alcohol use, hazardous drinking, sensation seeking, protective behavioral strategies


Underage drinking is a significant problem in the United States, with 58.5% of adolescents reporting alcohol use by their senior year (Johnston et al., 2019). Among high school students, seniors have the highest rates of alcohol use, with 30.2% of seniors reporting alcohol use in the past 30 days, 13.8% reporting binge drinking in the past two weeks, and 42.9% reporting being drunk at least once in their lifetime (Johnston et al., 2019). Further, hazardous drinking is more prevalent among older high school students, with the highest rates of risky drinking practices (i.e., pre-partying and drinking games) occurring among 17 – 19-year-olds (Borsari et al., 2013; Zamboanga et al., 2016). Researchers have also found that risky patterns of drinking established in adolescence are predictive of heavy drinking (Kenny et al., 2010) and alcohol-related problems (Kenny et al., 2010; Scaglione et al., 2015) in college and young adulthood (Enstad et al., 2019), as well as with alcohol dependence later in life (Marshall, 2014).

Adolescent alcohol use is associated with a wide range of negative socio-emotional consequences. For example, adolescent alcohol use is associated with academic problems (Rasberry et al., 2017), poor school attendance (Patte et al., 2017), and failure to complete high school (Kelly et al., 2015; Wang & Fredricks, 2014). Further, among adolescents, alcohol consumption is associated with being a target of violence, including school bullying (Doumas, Midgett, & Johnston, 2017; Johnston et al., 2017), dating violence (Miller et al., 2007), and unwanted sexual activity (Arata et al., 2003), attempting suicide (Miller et al., 2007), and drinking and driving (Borsari et al., 2013). Researchers have also found that substance use increases depression and suicidality for adolescents who have experienced interpersonal trauma (Terrell et al., 2020). Finally, hazardous drinking practices (e.g., pre-partying and gaming) in high school are associated with extreme drinking (e.g., 8+/10+ drinks for females and males, respectively; Fairlie et al., 2015) having a hangover, and passing out (Borsari et al., 2013).

Adolescence is a time of dynamic brain reorganization that makes adolescents particularly vulnerable to the negative effects of alcohol (Crews et al., 2019; Spear, 2018). Adolescent alcohol consumption is associated with deficits in sleep, cognitive flexibility, attention, memory, verbal learning, visuospatial ability, and psychomotor speed (Hanson et al., 2011; Nguyen-Louie et al., 2015; Spear, 2018). Neurologically, alcohol consumption disrupts healthy adolescent brain development by way of inhibiting neurogenesis, varying grey and white matter volumes, altering connections between frontal and limbic brain regions, inducing neuroinflammation, and initiating whole-brain changes in electroencephalogram (EEG) activity (Spear, 2018). These changes occur specifically in the range of theta wave (4–6 Hz) activity bandwidth (Rangaswamy et al., 2003), which has been associated with cortical decay (Coutin-Churchman et al., 2006; Saletu-Zyhlarz et al., 2004), suggesting alcohol misuse may lead to difficulties with decision-making and impulse control.

Sensation Seeking

One explanation for adolescent alcohol use is enhanced sensation seeking (Romer et al., 2017). Sensation seeking is defined as a biologically based personality trait manifested as the tendency to seek out novel, exciting stimuli, including risk-taking behavior (Zuckerman 1979; 1994). Adolescents’ desire to explore the environment and engage in new learning is an adaptive function of development and likely results, in part, from increases in dopaminergic activation that naturally occur during adolescence (Romer et al., 2017). Although it is developmentally adaptive for adolescents to engage in some sensation seeking behavior, when paired with impulsivity (e.g., developmental deficits in cognitive control), it can lead to problematic behaviors, such as alcohol use (Khurana et al., 2018; Meeus et al., 2021; Romer, et al., 2017).

Researchers have found that among adolescents, sensation seeking is associated with higher levels of alcohol use (MacPherson et al., 2010; Sznitman & Engel-Yeger, 2017), binge drinking, and alcohol-related consequences (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017). Further, sensation seeking and impulsivity levels are higher among individuals aged 16 and up convicted of DWI/DWUI compared to those without DWI/DWUI convictions who report similar levels of alcohol use (Curran et al., 2010). Sensation seeking has also been identified as a promising marker for identifying adolescents who are at risk for initiating binge drinking (Sargent et al., 2010). Sensation seeking is also associated with early onset (Jensen et al., 2017; Malmberg et al., 2012) and higher levels of substance use (Bekman et al., 2010), as well as problems associated with substance use (Ortin et al., 2012). This is particularly important as early initiation of alcohol use is associated with neurodevelopmental changes in cognition, brain structure, and function (Lisdahl et at. 2013), increased risk of alcohol use and binge drinking later in adolescence (Aiken et al., 2017), and alcohol-related problems in young adulthood (Enstad et al., 2019).

Protective Behavioral Strategies

Researchers have examined protective factors to minimize risks associated with adolescent alcohol use. For example, researchers have identified protective behavioral strategies (PBS) as a way to buffer adolescents from alcohol-related consequences (Martens et al., 2005). PBS include strategies that occur before, during, or after drinking, including creating plans to stop or slow down drinking, changing how one drinks to avoid rapid alcohol consumption, and behaving in ways that decrease dangerous consequences and promote harm reduction (Martens et al., 2005). Researchers conducting studies with college students have found that using PBS is associated with lower levels of negative alcohol outcomes (Araas & Adams, 2008; Borden et al., 2011; Kenny & LaBrie, 2013; Zeigler-Hill et al., 2012). Further, among college students, using PBS moderates the relationship between self-regulation (D’Lima et al., 2012) and negative urgency (e.g., acting rashly when distressed) and alcohol-related consequences (Weaver et al., 2012); students with low levels of self-regulation and high levels of negative urgency who use PBS have fewer alcohol-related consequences than students who are less likely to use PBS.

Although some researchers have identified the buffering effects of PBS for college students with traits associated with impulsivity (i.e., self-regulation and negative urgency), we could find only one study examining the relationship between PBS and alcohol use among high sensation seeking adolescents (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017). Results from this study indicated that among high sensations seeking adolescents, PBS use was inversely associated with binge drinking and alcohol-related consequences (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017). Although this study provides support for the use of PBS to minimize high-risk drinking and the associated consequences, the study included several limitations. First, the sample included non-drinkers. Because non-drinkers, by definition, do not use PBS, research conducted specifically with adolescents who report alcohol use is warranted. Additionally, researchers used the Protective Behavioral Strategy Scale (PBSS; Martens et al., 2005) to measure PBS. A revised version of the scale is now available and captures additional behaviors related to harm reduction and has improved internal consistency (Treloar et al., 2015). Finally, the researchers assessed hazardous drinking using one measure (i.e., binge drinking). Researchers have found that adolescents engage in other hazardous drinking behaviors including pre-partying (e.g., consuming alcohol prior to attending a planned event) and gaming (e.g., playing drinking games that often include rapid alcohol consumption), practices that are related to higher levels of alcohol use and are predictive of heavy alcohol use and alcohol-related problems in college (Kenney et al., 2010).

The Role of the Counselor

Adolescence is a critical time to identify and intervene in alcohol misuse (Curtis et al., 2014). Counselors can be a source of support for adolescents in many areas of their lives, including concerns regarding alcohol use (Brooks & McHenry, 2015). Counselors may also be the first professional contact for an adolescent engaging in alcohol misuse, particularly in the schools (Burrow-Sanchez & Lopez, 2009). Additionally, researchers have found that screening, assessment, and brief interventions are effective for reducing alcohol use among adolescents (Mitchell et al., 2013; Patton et al., 2014). Thus, counselors in school and community settings are well-positioned to address adolescent alcohol use through assessment and the integration of harm reduction strategies into treatment planning decisions. Because the prevalence rates of alcohol use and heavy drinking increase thorough adolescence, counselors working with this age group need to be able to identify risk factors for adolescent alcohol use, including sensation seeking traits. Counselors are also in a unique position to be able to provide and design treatment plans that include strategies high-risk adolescents can use to minimize the negative consequences associated with hazardous drinking. Thus, it is important for counselors to understand the relationship between sensations seeking and hazardous drinking and the impact PBS can have on reducing hazardous drinking and the negative associated consequences for high sensation seeking adolescents.

The Current Study

The purpose of this study was to extend the literature by examining the moderating effects of PBS on the relationship between sensation seeking and hazardous drinking and alcohol-related consequences among high school seniors who report alcohol use. We selected high school seniors as they are at greatest risk for hazardous drinking and the negative associated outcomes among school-aged students. To address the limitations in prior research, we only included high school seniors who reported alcohol use, we utilized the revised PBSS scale (PBSS-20; Treloar et al., 2015), and we included three measures of hazardous drinking (i.e., binge drinking, pre-partying, and gaming), as well as alcohol-related consequences. We hypothesized that 1) sensation seeking would be positively related to hazardous drinking and alcohol-related consequences and 2) PBS would moderate this relationship, such that among high sensation seeking adolescents, those using PBS would report lower levels of hazardous drinking and alcohol-related consequences than adolescents using fewer PBS.

Method

Research Design

We used a cross-sectional design to examine the relationships between sensation seeking, PBS, hazardous drinking, and alcohol-related consequences. All study procedures were approved by the University Institutional Review Board and the School District Research Board.

Participants

Participants in the current study were high school seniors recruited from two high schools in the Northwest. Consent forms were sent to parents of all students (N = 867), including those who were 18 years old, per School District Research Board policy. A total of 342 (39.4%) parents provided consent. Among students with parental consent, those who were in attendance at school (N = 330) were given an opportunity to participate. Among these students, 311 (48.8% male, 51.2% female) provided assent. Of these, 68.2% (N = 212) reported alcohol use in the past year and met inclusion criteria for the study. Participant ages ranged from 15 to 18 (M = 17.15, SD = 0.47). Participants were primarily White (85.8%), with 4.7% Hispanic, 3.8% Asian, 1.4% Black, 1.9 % American Indian/Alaskan Native, and 2.4% other. Among participants, 60.4% reported living with both parents, 18.9% reported living primarily with their mother, 7.5% reported living primarily with their father, and 13.2% reported other for their living situation.

Procedure

A member of the research team contacted the principals at two of the largest high schools within the capitol district in a northwest region of the United States. The schools contacted all parents of seniors via letter by mail at their permanent addresses provided by the registrar’s office. The letter included a parental consent form and a project-addressed, stamped envelope for parents to return signed consent forms indicating permission (yes/no) for their adolescent to participate in the study. The letter also contained a phone number and email address so that parents could ask questions prior to signing the consent form. The school sent reminder letters to the student’s home address and gave student reminder letters to give to their parents.

A member of the research team recruited students during a common core class period. Students with parental consent met at the school’s computer lab to participate in the study. A member of the research team described the study and invited the students to participate. Students logged onto the survey website where they read a welcome screen explaining the research and were asked for their assent to participate. Once students gave assent by clicking “Agree”, they were directed to begin the survey, which took approximately 20 minutes to complete. Students who did not have parental consent remained in their classroom with their teacher and completed an alternative exercise. Students who did not provide assent went back to their classrooms where they joined the other students and teacher. Incentives included $100 for school supplies and a pizza or bagel “party” for classrooms that achieved a 60% parental consent return rate.

Measures

Demographic Survey

Participants completed a brief demographic questionnaire with questions about age, gender, and race/ethnicity.

Binge Drinking

We defined frequency of binge drinking as consuming 5+ drinks in a row for males and 3+ for females in a 2 hour period during the last 2 weeks. The 5/3 quantity has been identified as appropriate for adolescents aged 16–17 based on BAC levels for this age group (Donovan, 2009).

Pre-partying

We defined pre-partying as “drinking before you go out to your planned destination (e.g., party, bar, or concert) at which more alcohol may or may not be consumed.” Participants responded to the following question: “On occasions when you pre-partied, how many drinks did you typically consume overall (includes drinks consumed during and after pre-partying)?” This measure has been used in research with high school students (Kenny et al., 2010).

Gaming

We defined gaming as “games where drinking is part of the known rules, or where chugging is involved. The object of the game is either to avoid drinking or to show that you can drink a lot. A secondary aim is to get others to drink a lot.” Participants responded to the following question: “On occasions when you played drinking games, how many drinks did you typically consume overall (includes drinks consumed during and after playing drinking games)?” This measure has been used in research with high school students (Kenny et al., 2010).

Alcohol-Related Consequences

We assessed alcohol-related consequences using the Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index (RAPI; White & Labouvie, 1989). The RAPI is a 23-item self-administered screening tool for assessing adolescent problem drinking. Participants were asked “How many times have the following scenarios happened to you while you were consuming alcohol or as a result of your drinking in the past 30 days.” Responses were measured on a 5-point scale ranging from never to more than 10 times. A total consequence score was created by summing the 23 items. The RAPI has good internal consistency (α = .92) (White & Labouvie, 1989) and test-retest reliability (r = .83, .86, and .88 for 1 month, 6 months, and 1 year, respectively) (Miller et al., 2002). The RAPI has been correlated significantly (r = .49 - .67) with several drinking variables (White & Labouvie, 1989). Coefficient alpha for this sample was α = .90.

Sensation Seeking

We used the Zuckerman–Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ; Zuckerman et al., 1993) to assess sensation seeking. The ZKPQ is a 40-item forced choice questionnaire designed to measure five personality traits. For the purposes of this study, we used the 19-item Impulsive Sensation Seeking (ImpSS) subscale of the ZKPQ. The ImpSS items reflect a lack of planning (e.g. “I usually think about what I am going to do before doing it” reverse scored), a tendency to act impulsively (e.g., “I often do things on impulse”), a need for excitement and thrills (e.g. “I like to have new and exciting experiences and sensations even if they are a little frightening), a preference for unpredictability (e.g. “I enjoy getting into new situations where you can’t predict how things will turn out”), and a need for novelty and change (e.g. “I tend to change interests frequently”). The ImpSS scale has been psychometrically validated, with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .77 - .82 (Zuckerman et al., 1993) and has been used in research with high school seniors (α = .77) (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017). Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was α = .76.

Protective Behavioral Strategies (PBS)

We used the Protective Behavioral Strategy Scale-20 (PBSS-20; Treloar et al., 2015) to assess PBS. The PBSS-20 is a 20-item measure used to assess cognitive–behavioral strategies used to reduce risky drinking. Respondents rate the degree to which they engage in certain behaviors when consuming alcohol using a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). The PBSS-20 scale has been psychometrically validated, with research demonstrating test-retest reliability (r = .67), content validity, and criterion-related validity (Treloar et al., 2015). The measure assesses three types of protective behaviors: Stopping/Limiting Drinking (e.g., “Determine not to exceed a set number of drinks,” “Stop drinking at a predetermined time,”), Manner of Drinking (e.g., “Avoid drinking games,” “Drink slowly, rather than gulp or chug,” “Avoid pre-gaming”), and Serious Harm Reduction (e.g., “Use a designated driver,” “Know where your drink has been at all times,” and “Only go out with people you know and trust”). All items are summed to create a total PBS scale. Cronbach’s alpha for this sample was α = .98.

Power Analysis

We conducted an a priori power analysis using the G*Power 3.1.3 program (Faul et al., 2007) for a hierarchical regression with three test variables and two control variables. Results indicated a sample size of 77 is needed for power of 0.80 to detect a medium effect size with an alpha level of .05. Thus, our final sample size of 212 is greater than the needed size to provide adequate power for our analyses.

Statistical Analyses

We conducted all analyses using SPSS version 25. We examined the outcome and predictor variables for outliers and adjusted outliers to 3.3 SD above the mean before conducting analyses (Tabachnik & Fidell, 2007). We conducted bivariate correlations among predictor and dependent variables prior to conducting the main regression analyses (see Table 1). Although several of the correlations were significant at p < .01, the variance inflation factor (VIF) ranged between 1.0 – 5.6, with corresponding tolerance levels ranging from .18 - .99. The VIF is below the rule of thumb of VIF < 10 (Norman & Streiner, 2008), suggesting acceptable levels of multicollinearity among the predictor variables.

Table 1.

Means, Standard Deviations, and Bivariate Correlations for Alcohol Variable, Sensation Seeking, and Protective Behavioral Strategies

Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Binge Drinking
2. Pre-Partying Drinks .66**
3. Gaming Drinks .51** .65**
4. Alcohol-Related Consequences .48** .56** .37**
5. Sensation Seeking .30** .32** .32** .25**
6. Protective Behavioral Strategies −.06 −.11 −.12 −.09 −.01
M 1.12 3.78 3.58 4.34 28.14 66.61
SD 1.48 4.35 4.47 5.82 3.90 18.81

We also examined demographic differences in drinking variables, sensation seeking, and protective behavioral strategies. Gender was significantly related to binge drinking, with females reporting higher levels of binge drinking (M = 1.32, SD = 1.51) than males (M = 0.86, SD = 1.40), t(211) = −2.26, p < .03, Cohen’s d = .32. This is consistent with current prevalence rates of binge drinking among high school seniors in the US, with 23.3% of females reporting binge drinking compared to 21.5% of males (Center for Disease Control [CDC], 2019). Race/ethnicity was dichotomized into White and participants of color due to low frequency of participants reporting several race/ethnicity categories. Race/ethnicity was significantly related to binge drinking, pre-partying, and gaming, with White participants reporting higher levels of binge drinking (M = 1.23, SD = 1.54) than participants of color, (M = 0.43, SD = 0.68), t(211) = −2.76, p < .01, Cohen’s d = .67, White participants reporting higher levels of pre-partying (M = 4.03, SD = 4.35) than participants of color, (M = 2.20, SD = 4.10), t(211) = −2.15, p < .03, Cohen’s d = .43, and White participants reporting higher levels of gaming (M = 3.90, SD = 4.60) than participants of color, (M = 1.57, SD = 3.00), t(211) = −2.68, p < .01, Cohen’s d = .59. Thus, we included gender and race/ethnicity as control variables in the regression analyses. Means and standard deviations for drinking variables, sensation seeking, and protective behavioral strategies are presented in Table 1.

Our aim was to test whether the relationship between sensation seeking and adolescent drinking behavior is moderated by PBS. To test this aim, we conducted a series of hierarchical regression analyses, with interaction effects used to test for moderation. We mean centered all predictor variables to reduce problems of multicollinearity introduced into equations containing interaction terms (Aiken & West, 1991). We created the interaction term by computing the product of sensation seeking and PBS. Gender and race/ethnicity were entered on Step 1, sensation seeking and PBS were entered on Step 2, and the sensation seeking x PBS interaction term was entered on Step 3 to examine the moderating effects of PBS. We used simple slopes to examine the direction and degree of significant interactions (Aiken & West, 1991). All analyses were conducted at p < .05. We calculated effect size using the R2 with .01 considered small, .09 considered medium, and .25 considered large (Cohen, 1969). We controlled for Type 1 error by using the Holm-Bonferroni procedure (Holm, 1979). Table 2 presents regression model results.

Table 2.

Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Hazardous Drinking

Variable B 95% CI for B
SE B β R 2 ΔR2
LL UL

Frequency of Binge Drinking

Step 1 .06** .06**
 Gender 0.49 0.09 0.89 0.20 .16*
 Race/Ethnicity 0.81 0.25 1.37 0.28 .19**

Step 2 .19*** .15***
 Sensation Seeking 1.93 1.12 2.72 0.41 .65***
 PBS 0.46 1.38 5.14 0.34 .13
 Sensation Seeking x PBS −1.15 −2.05 0.25 0.46 −.37**

Pre-Partying Drinks Consumed

Step 1
 Gender −0.12 −0.05 4.84 0.60 −.01 .01 .01
 Race/Ethnicity 1.82 0.14 3.50 0.85 .15*

Step 2 .16*** .16***
 Sensation Seeking 6.18 3.78 8.58 1.23 .71***
 PBS 1.73 −0.26 3.71 1.00 .16
 Sensation Seeking x PBS −3.97 −6.67 −1.28 1.37 −.44**

Gaming Drinks Consumed

Step 1 .05** .05**
 Gender −1.09 0.82 5.79 0.61 −.12
 Race/Ethnicity 2.29 0.58 3.99 0.87 .18**

Step 2
 Sensation Seeking 5.13 2.64 7.61 1.26 .57*** .15*** .12***
 PBS 0.99 −1.07 3.04 1.04 .09
 Sensation Seeking x PBS −2.91 −5.70 −0.12 1.42 −.31*

Note. N = 122. SE = standard error. CI = confidence interval.

*

p < .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001.

Results

Hazardous Drinking

Binge Drinking

The full regression equation was statistically significant for binge drinking, F(5, 206) = 10.67, p < .001. Examination of the adjusted R2 indicates sensation seeking and PBS accounted for 19% of the variance in binge drinking. As seen on Step 2, sensation seeking was a statistically significant predictor of binge drinking (p < .001). As hypothesized, higher levels of sensation seeking were associated with higher levels of binge drinking. Further, as seen on Step 3, the interaction between sensation seeking and PBS was statistically significant (p < .01). Examination of the slopes in Figure 1 indicates that among high sensation seeking participants, those using more PBS reported lower levels of binge drinking than those using fewer PBS.

Figure 1: Means for Hazardous Drinking by Sensation Seeking and PBS.

Figure 1:

Figure 1:

Note. Simple slopes are shown depicting the direction and degree of the significant interaction testing moderator effects for frequency of binge drinking, total number of drinks consumed on pre-partying occasions, and total number of drinks consumed on gaming occasions. When controlling for gender and race/ethnicity, among participants with high sensation seeking, those using more PBS reported less hazardous drinking than those using fewer PBS. The highest levels of hazardous drinking are reported by high sensation seeking students who report using PBS.

Pre-Partying

The full regression equation was statistically significant for pre-partying, F(5, 206) = 8.91, p < .001. Examination of the adjusted R2 indicates sensation seeking and PBS accounted for 16% of the variance in pre-partying. As seen on Step 2, sensation seeking was a statistically significant predictor of the number drinks consumed on pre-partying occasions (p < .001). As hypothesized, higher levels of sensation seeking were associated with more drinks consumed on pre-partying occasions. Further, as seen on Step 3, the interaction between sensation seeking and PBS was statistically significant (p < .01). Examination of the slopes in Figure 1 indicates that among high sensation seeking participants, those using more PBS reported fewer drinks consumed on pre-partying occasions than those using fewer PBS.

Gaming

The full regression equation was statistically significant for gaming, F(5, 206) = 8.30, p < .001. Examination of the adjusted R2 indicates sensation seeking and PBS accounted for 15% of the variance in gaming. As seen on Step 2, sensation seeking was a statistically significant predictor of the number drinks consumed on gaming occasions (p < .001). As hypothesized, higher levels of sensation seeking were associated with more drinks consumed on gaming occasions. Further, as seen on Step 3, the interaction between sensation seeking and PBS was statistically significant (p < .05). Examination of the slopes in Figure 1 indicates that among high sensation seeking participants, those using more PBS reported fewer drinks consumed on gaming occasions than those using fewer PBS.

Alcohol-Related Consequences

The full regression equation was statistically significant for alcohol-related consequences, F(5, 206) = 6.06, p < .001. Examination of the adjusted R2 indicates sensation seeking and PBS accounted for 13% of the variance in alcohol-related consequences. As seen on Step 2, sensation seeking was a statistically significant predictor of alcohol-related consequences (p < .001). As hypothesized, higher levels of sensation seeking were associated with higher levels of alcohol-related consequences. Further, as seen on Step 3 the interaction between sensation seeking and PBS was statistically significant (p < .001). Examination of the slopes in Figure 2 indicates that among participants with high sensation seeking, those using more PBS reported fewer alcohol-related consequences than those using fewer PBS.

Figure 2: Means for Alcohol-Related Consequences by Sensation Seeking and PBS.

Figure 2:

Note. Simple slopes are shown depicting the direction and degree of the significant interaction testing moderator effects for alcohol-related consequences. When controlling for gender and race/ethnicity, among participants with high sensation seeking, those using more PBS reported fewer alcohol-related consequences than those using fewer PBS. The highest levels of alcohol-related consequences are reported by high sensation seeking students who report using PBS.

Discussion

This study examined the moderating effects of PBS on the relationship between sensation seeking, hazardous drinking, and alcohol-related consequences among high school seniors. Findings from this study indicated sensation seeking was a statistically significant predictor of both hazardous drinking (i.e., binge drinking, pre-partying, and gaming) and alcohol-related consequences. Effect sizes for all of the outcomes were in the medium to large range, suggesting the importance of the association of sensation seeking to high-risk drinking and the associated negative outcomes. These findings are consistent with research indicating high sensation seeking among adolescents is associated with high rates of substance use (MacPherson et al., 2010), early initiation of alcohol use (Doumas et al., 2019), greater severity of substance use (Bekman et al., 2010; Ortin et al., 2012; Urban et al., 2008), binge drinking (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017; Sargent et al., 2010), and alcohol-related consequences (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017).

Results from the current study also indicate that among high sensation seeking adolescents, those using PBS reported less hazardous drinking and fewer alcohol-related consequences than those not using PBS. These findings replicate prior research conducted with high school seniors demonstrating PBS moderates the relationship between sensation seeking and binge drinking and alcohol-related consequences (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017). The current findings also extend the literature by demonstrating this relationship among a sample of drinkers (vs including non-drinkers) and using the PBSS-20 (Treloar et al., 2015) as the assessment measure. Results are also consistent with research suggesting PBS serve a protective function for college students with low-self regulation (D’Lima et al., 2012) and high levels of negative urgency (Weaver et al., 2012). Thus, results support prior research indicating PBS serve as a buffer against hazardous drinking and the negative alcohol outcomes associated with high sensation seeking.

We also extended the literature by examining two additional measures of hazardous drinking (i.e., pre-partying and gaming). Including these variables in addition to binge drinking is important as both pre-partying and gaming are associated with extreme drinking (Fairlie et al., 2015). Gaming is also associated with high-risk behaviors, such as “chugging” alcohol, posing immediate and serious health risks to adolescents engaging in this practice (Zamboanga et al., 2014). Although binge drinking can lead to serious consequences for adolescents, the risk is exacerbated when adolescents consume additional alcohol prior to attending social events or play drinking games that involve risky practices that include rapidly consuming large amounts of alcohol. Although research suggests playing drinking games is common among adolescents, the best indicators of risk include both total amount of alcohol consumed and psychological variables, such as impulsivity (Borsari et al., 2013). Thus, using PBS to reduce risky drinking practices, as well as the amount of alcohol consumed, is an important harm reduction strategy that can reduce negative outcomes associated with hazardous drinking among high sensation seeking adolescents.

Implications for Counseling

Findings from this study have important implications for counselors. Results indicate high sensation seeking is related to hazardous alcohol use among adolescents, including binge drinking, pre-partying, and gaming. Coupled with research indicating sensation seeking is associated with early age of drinking initiation (Jensen et al., 2017; Malmberg et al., 2012), it is important for school and community counselors to identify adolescents with high sensation seeking traits. High sensation seeking is associated with several behaviors that can help counselors identify high risk students. For example, high sensation seeking is associated with impulsive behavior, including engaging in high-risk activities, such as substance use, high-risk sexual activities, fighting, and extreme sports (Roberti, 2004) and drinking and driving (Curran et al., 2010). High sensation seeking is also related to a propensity for boredom, which can include affective (e.g., depressive affect), cognitive (e.g., inattention), and behavioral (e.g., disruptive behavior) components (Freund et al., 2021). School and community counselors who become aware of these indicators can offer further assessment of adolescents using tools such as the ZKPQ (Zuckerman et al., 1993).

Because sensation seeking is considered to be a biologically driven personality trait (Zuckerman, 1979; 1994), rather than trying to change sensation seeking tendencies, it may be more effective to use harm reduction strategies to teach high sensation seeking youth to minimize hazardous drinking and the associated negative consequences. Specifically, results of this study indicate that using PBS moderates the relationship between sensation seeking and hazardous alcohol use. Thus, treatment planning for counselors can include teaching high sensation seeking adolescents to use PBS to reduce harm associated with alcohol use. When designing treatment plans for high sensation seeking adolescents, focusing on strategies specific to changing how one drinks, such as avoiding drinking games, shots, and gulping/chugging, avoiding mixing different types of alcohol, and not trying to out-drink others, may be most beneficial (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017). In contrast, strategies that are designed to stop or limit drinking, including planning ahead of time not to exceed a certain amount of drinks, stopping drinking at a certain time, or leaving a party at a predetermined time may be less effective for high sensation seeking adolescents (Doumas, Miller, & Esp, 2017). Treatment planning can also include harm reduction strategies such as motivational enhancement approaches (Tevyaw & Monti, 2004) to work with adolescents to discuss how they have used PBS in the past and what new strategies they are willing to try in the future. Counselors can also plan to use technology based programs that include assessment and feedback related to PBS (e.g., eCHECKUP TO GO; http://www.echeckuptogo.com/), as research supports the efficacy of this approach in reducing alcohol use (Doumas, Esp, et al., 2017) and alcohol-related consequences (Doumas et al., 2019) among adolescents who report hazardous drinking, as well as for adolescents who report early initiation of alcohol use (Doumas et al., 2016).

Limitations and Directions for Future Research

This study adds to the limited literature examining PBS as a moderator of the relationship between sensation seeking and alcohol outcomes among adolescents. We do, however, note several limitations. First, due to a primarily White sample form the Northwest region, generalizability is limited. Second, information in this study was obtained through self-report. Self-reported alcohol use, however, is common practice in studies examining alcohol use among adolescents and researchers have demonstrated the reliability and validity of self-reported use in this age group (Flisher et al., 2004; Lintonen et al., 2004). We did, however, take specific steps to minimize social desirability and response bias, including providing assurances regarding confidentiality. Finally, we used a cross-sectional research design, limiting our ability to determine the direction of the relationships of interest. Although sensation seeking is presumed to be a biologically-based personality trait, thus preceding alcohol use, it is possible that sensation seeking can also be an outcome of epigenetic factors such as psychological (Babad et al., 2019; Kerig, 2019) or physical (i.e., head injury; O’Jile et al., 2004; Liebel et al., 2021) trauma. Similarly, some researchers have suggested that similar epigenetic factors are associated with substance misuse (Jones et al., 2021) and that the relationship between epigenetic factors and addiction may be mediated by sensation seeking behavior (Hamilton & Nestler, 2019). Thus, we recommend future research using a longitudinal design to explore the nature of the relationship between sensation seeking and hazardous alcohol use in this age group.

Conclusion

This study extends the literature suggesting that PBS may buffer high sensation seeking adolescents from hazardous drinking and the associated negative consequences among high school seniors. These findings suggest school and community counselors can use brief personality-targeted interventions to motivate and support high sensation seeking adolescents to use skills to reduce excessive and rapid drinking.

Table 3.

Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analyses for Alcohol-Related Consequences

Variable B 95% CI for B
SE B β R 2 ΔR2
LL UL

Step 1 .00 .00
 Gender 0.29 −1.31 1.89 0.81 .02
 Race/Ethnicity 0.87 −1.41 3.14 1.15 .05

Step 2
 Sensation Seeking 8.04 4.73 11.35 1.68 .69*** .13*** .11***
 PBS 2.14 −0.60 4.88 1.39 .15
 Sensation Seeking x PBS −6.44 −10.17 −2.72 1.89 −.53***

Note. N = 122. SE = standard error. CI = confidence interval.

*

p < .05

**

p < .01

***

p < .001.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Andrea Pikes, April Watts, Beverly Glouser, and Dayna Showalter for assistance with participant recruitment and data collection. This work was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism of the National Institutes of Health under Grant Number R21AA023880. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health

References

  1. Aiken A, Clare PJ, Wadolowski M, Hutchinson D, Najman JM, Slade T, Bruno R, McBride N, Kypri K, & Mattick RP (2017). Age of alcohol initiation and progression to binge drinking in adolescence: a prospective cohort study. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 40(1), 100–110. 10.1111/acer.13525 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aiken LS, & West SG (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  3. Araas TE, & Adams TB (2008). Protective behavioral strategies and negative alcohol related consequences in college students. Journal of Drug Education, 38(3), 211–224. 10.2190/DE.38.3.b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arata CM, Stafford J, & Tims MS (2003). High school drinking and its consequences. Adolescence, 38(151), 567–579. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Babad S, Zwilling A, Carson KW, Fairchild V, Razak S, Robinson G, & Nikulina V (2019). Risk-taking propensity and sensation seeking in survivors of adverse childhood experiences. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(19–20), NP10670–NP10687. 10.1177/0886260519876035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bekman NM, Cummins K, & Brown SA (2010). Affective and personality risk and cognitive mediators of initial adolescent alcohol use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71(4), 570–580. 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.570 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Borden LA, Martens MP, McBride MA, Sheline KT, Bloch KK, & Dude K (2011). The role of college students’ use of protective behavioral strategies in the relation between binge drinking and alcohol-related problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 25(2), 346–351. 10.1037/a0022678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Borsari B, Zamboanga BL, Correia C, Olthuis J,V., Van Tyne K., Zadworny Z., Grossbard J,R., & Horton N,J. (2013). Characterizing high school students who play drinking games using latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 38(10), 2532–2540. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brooks F, & McHenry B (2105). A contemporary approach to substance use disorders and addiction counseling (2nd ed.).Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  10. Burrow-Sanchez J & Lopez A (2009). Identifying substance abuse issues in high schools. A national survey of high school counselors. Journal of Counseling and Development, 87 (1), 72–79. 10.1002/j.1556-6678.2009.tb00551.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2019). Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS). https://www.cdc.gov/healthyyouth/data/yrbs/index.htm
  12. Chein J, Albert D, O’Brien L, Uckert K, & Steinberg L (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Science, 14(2), 1–16. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cohen J (1969). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cousijn J, Luijten M, & Feldstein Ewing SF (2018). Adolescent resilience to addiction: A social plasticity hypothesis. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 2(1), 69–78. 10.1016/S2352-4642(17)30148-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Coutin-Churchman P, Moreno R, Añez Y, & Vergara F (2006). Clinical correlates of quantitative EEG alterations in alcoholic patients. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(4), 740–751. 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Curran MF, Fuertes JN, Alfonso VC, & Hennessy JJ (2010). The association of sensation seeking and impulsivity to driving while under the influence of alcohol. Journal of Addictions & Offender Counseling, 30(2), 84–98. 10.1002/j.2161.2010.tb00059.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Curtis BL, McLellan AT, & Gabellini BN (2014). Translating SBIRT to public school settings: An initial test of feasibility. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 46, 15–21. 10.1016/j.jsat.2013.08.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Davidow JY, Foerde K, Galvan A, & Shohamy D (2016). The upside to reward sensitivity: The hippocampus supports enhanced reinforcement learning in adolescence. Neuron, 92(1), 93–99. 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.08.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. D’Lima GM, Pearson MR, & Kelley ML (2012). Protective behavioral strategies as a mediator and moderator of the relationship between self-regulation and alcohol-related consequences in first-year college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26(2), 330–337. 10.1037/a0026942 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Donovan JE (2009). Estimated blood alcohol concentrations for child and adolescent drinking and their implications for screening instruments. Pediatrics, 123(6), 975–981. 10.1542/peds.2008-0027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Doumas DM, & Esp S (2019). Reducing alcohol-related consequences among high school seniors: Efficacy of a brief, web-based intervention. Journal of Counseling & Development, 97(1), 53–61. 10.1002/jcad.12235 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Doumas DM, Esp S, Flay B, & Bond L (2017). A randomized controlled trial testing the efficacy of a brief online alcohol intervention for high school seniors. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 78(5), 706–715. 10.15288/jsad.2017.78.706 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Doumas DM, Hausheer R, & Esp S (2016). Age of drinking initiation as a moderator of the efficacy of a brief, web-based personalized feedback alcohol intervention. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 25(6), 591–597. 10.1080/1067828X.2016.1171185 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Doumas DM, Midgett A, & Johnston AD (2017). Substance use and bullying victimization among middle and high school students: Is positive school climate a protective factor?. Journal of Addictions & Offender Counseling, 38(1), 2–15. 10.1002/jaoc.12025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Doumas DM, Miller R, & Esp S (2017). Impulsive sensation seeking, binge drinking, and alcohol-related consequences: Do protective behavioral strategies help high risk adolescents? Addictive Behaviors, 64, 6–12. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Doumas DM, Turrisi R, Miller R, Esp S, & Flay B (2019). Age of drinking initiation as a mediator of the relationship between sensation seeking and heavy drinking among high school seniors. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 28(1), 32–38. 10.1080/1067828X.2018.1561579 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Enstad F, Evans-Whipp T, Kjeldsen A, Toumbourou JW, & Von Soest T (2019). Predicting hazardous drinking in late adolescence/young adulthood from early and excessive adolescent drinking - a longitudinal cross-national study of Norwegian and Australian adolescents. BMC Public Health, 19(1), 1–12. 10.1186/s12889-019-7099-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fairchild AJ, & MacKinnon DP (2009). A general model for testing mediation and moderation effects. Prevention Science, 10, 87–99. 10.1007/s11121-008-0109-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Fairlie AM, Maggs JL, & Lanza ST (2015). Prepartying, drinking games, and extreme drinking among college students: A daily-level investigation. Addictive Behaviors, 42(1), 91–95. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Flisher AJ, Evans J, Muller M, & Lombard C (2004). Brief report: Test-retest reliability of self-reported adolescent risk behavior. Journal of Adolescence, 27(2), 207–212. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2001.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. French MT, & Maclean JC (2006). Underage alcohol use, delinquency, and criminal activity. Health Economics, 15(12), 1261–1281. 10.1002/hec.1126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Freund VA, Schulenberg JE & Maslowsky J (2021). Boredom by sensation-seeking interactions during adolescence: Associations with substance use, externalizing behavior, and internalizing symptoms in a US national sample. Prevention Science, 22, 555–566. 10.1007/s11121-020-01198-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hamilton PJ, & Nestler EJ (2019). Epigenetics and addiction. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 59(1), 128–136. 10.1016/j.conb.2019.05.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hanson KL, Medina KL, Padula CB, Tapert SF, & Brown SA (2011). Impact of adolescent alcohol and drug use on neuropsychological functioning in young adulthood: 10-year outcomes. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 20(2), 135–154. 10.1080/1067828X.2011.555272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Holm S (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6(2), 65–70. [Google Scholar]
  36. Jensen M, Chassin L, & Gonzales NA (2017). Neighborhood moderation of sensation seeking effects on adolescent substance use initiation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46, 1953–1967. 10.1007/s10964-017-0647-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Johnston LD, Miech RA, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE, & Patrick ME (2019). Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use 1975–2018: Overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan. http://www.monitoringthefuture.org/pubs/monographs/mtf-overview2018.pdf [Google Scholar]
  38. Johnston AD, Doumas DM, Midgett A, & Moro RR (2017). Gender differences in the relationship between bullying victimization and substance use among high school students. Journal of Child and Adolescent Counseling, 3(1), 30–43. 10.1080/23727810.2017.1280749 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jones DE, Park JS, Gamby K, Bigelow TM, Mersha TB, & Folger AT (2021). Mental Health Epigenetics: A Primer with Implications for Counselors. Professional Counselor, 11(1), 102–121. 10.15241/dej.11.1.102 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kelly AB, Evans-Whipp TJ, Smith R, Chan GC, Toumbourou JW, Patton GC, Hemphill SA, Hall WD, & Catalano RF (2015). A longitudinal study of the association of adolescent polydrug use, alcohol use and high school non-completion. Addiction, 110(4), 627–635. 10.1111/add.12829 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kenney SR, & LaBrie JW (2013). Use of protective behavioral strategies and reduced alcohol risk: Examining the moderating effects of mental health, gender, and race. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27(4), 997–1009. 10.1037/a0033262 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kenney SR, Hummer JF, & LaBrie JW (2010). An examination of prepartying and drinking game playing during high school and their impact on alcohol-related risk upon entrance into college. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(9), 999–1011. 10.1007/s10964-009-9473-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kerig PK (2019). Linking childhood trauma exposure to adolescent justice involvement: The concept of posttraumatic risk-seeking. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 26(3), e12280. 10.1111/cpsp.12280 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Khurana A, Romer D, Betancourt LM, & Hurt H (2018). Modeling trajectories of sensation seeking and impulsivity dimensions from early to late adolescence: Universal trends or distinct sub-groups? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47(1), 1922–2005. 10.1007/s10964-018-0891-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Liebel SW, Edwards KA, & Broglio SP (2021). Sensation-seeking and impulsivity in athletes with sport-related concussion. Current Psychiatry Reports, 23(4), 1–6. 10.1007/s11920-021-01232-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lintonen T, Ahlstrom S, & Metso L (2004). The reliability of self-reported drinking in adolescence. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 39(4), 362–368. 10.1093/alcalc/agh071 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. MacPherson L, Magidson JF, Reynolds EK, Kahler CW, & Lejuez CW (2010). Changes in sensation seeking and risk-taking propensity predict increases in alcohol use among early adolescents. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 34(8), 1400–1408. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01223.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Malmberg M, Kleinjan M, Vermults AA, Overbeek G, Monshouwer K, Lammer J, & Engels RCM (2012). Do substance use risk personality dimensions predict the onset of substance use in early adolescence? A variable- and person-centered approach. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(11), 1512–1525. 10.1007/s10964-012-9775-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Marshall EJ (2014). Adolescent alcohol use: risks and consequences. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 49(2), 160–164. 10.1093/alcalc/agt180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Martens MP, Ferrier AG, Sheehy MJ, Corbett K, Anderson DA, & Simmons A (2005). Development of the Protective Behavioral Strategies Survey. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 66(5), 698–705. 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Meeus W, Vollebergh W, Branje S, Crocetti E, Ormel J, Crone EA, & Becht A (2021). On imbalance of impulse control and sensation seeking in adolescent risk: An intra-individual developmental test of systems and maturational imbalance models. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 50(1), 827–840. 10.1007/s10964-021-01419-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Miller ET, Neal DJ, Roberts LJ, Baer JS, Cressler SO, Metrick J, & Marlatt GA (2002). Test-retest reliability of alcohol measures: Is there a difference between internet-based assessment and traditional methods? Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 16, 56–63. 10.1037/0893-164X.16.1.56 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Miller JW, Naimi TS, Brewer RD, & Jones SE (2007). Binge drinking and associated health risk behaviors among high school students. Pediatrics, 119(1), 76–85. 10.1542/peds.2006-1517 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mitchell SG, Gryczynski J, O’Grady KE, & Schwartz RP (2013). SBIRT for adolescent drug and alcohol use: Current status and future directions. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 44(5), 463–472. 10.1016/j.jsat.2012.11.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nguyen-Louie TT, Castro N, Matt GE, Squeglia LM, Brumback T, & Tapert SF (2015). Effects of emerging alcohol and marijuana use behaviors on adolescents’ neuropsychological functioning over four years. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 76(5), 738–748. 10.15288/jsad.2015.76.738 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Norman GR, & Streiner DL (2008). Biostatistics: The bare essentials (3rd ed.). Hamilton, B.C. Decker, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  57. O’Jile JR, Ryan LM, Parks-Levy J, Betz B, & Gouvier WD (2004). Sensation seeking and risk behaviors in young adults with and without a history of head injury. Applied Neuropsychology, 11(2), 107–112. 10.1207/s15324826an1102_7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ortin A, Lake AM, Kleinman M, & Gould MS (2012). Sensation seeking as risk factor for suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in adolescence. Journal of Affective Disorders, 143, 214–222. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Patte KA, Qian W, & Leatherdale ST (2017). Marijuana and alcohol use as predictors of academic achievement: A longitudinal analysis among youth in the COMPASS study. Journal of School Health, 87(5), 310–318. 10.1111/josh.12498 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Patton R, Deluca P, Kaner E, Newbury-Birch D, Phillips T, & Drummond C (2014). Alcohol screening and brief intervention for adolescents: the how, what and where of reducing alcohol consumption and related harm among young people. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 49(2), 207–212. 10.1093/alcalc/agt165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rangaswamy M, Porjesz B, Chorlian DB, Wang K, Jones KA, Bauer LO, & Reich T (2002). Beta power in the EEG of alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry, 52(8), 831–842. 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01362-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Rasberry CN, Tiu GF, Kann L, McManus T, Michael SL, Merlo CL, Lee SM, Bohm MK, Annor F, & Ethier KA (2017). Health-related behaviors and academic achievement among high school students—United States, 2015. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report (MMWR). https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/volumes/66/wr/mm6635a1.htm#contribAff [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  63. Romer D, Reyna VF, & Satterwaite TD, (2017). Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk taking: Placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(1), 19–34. 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Roberti JW (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality, 38(3), 256–279. 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00067-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Saletu-Zyhlarz GM, Arnold O, Anderer P, Oberndorfer S, Walter H, Lesch OM, & Saletu B (2004). Differences in brain function between relapsing and abstaining alcohol-dependent patients, evaluated by EEG mapping. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 39(3), 233–240. 10.1093/alcalc/agh041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Sargent JD, Tanski S, Stoolmiller M, & Hanewinkel R (2010). Using sensation seeking to target adolescents for substance use interventions. Addiction, 105(3), 506–514. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02782.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Scaglione NM, Mallett KA, Turrisi R, Reavy R, Cleveland MJ, & Ackerman S (2015). Who will experience the most alcohol problems in college? The roles of middle and high school drinking tendencies. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39(10), 2039–2046. 10.1111/acer.12846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Shaw T, Cross D, Thomas LT, & Zubrick SR (2015). Bias in student survey findings for active parental consent procedures. British Educational Research Journal, 41(2), 229–243. 10.1002/berj.3137 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Smith DC, Boel-Studt S, & Cleeland L (2009). Parental consent in adolescent substance abuse treatment outcome studies. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 37(3), 298–306. 10.1016/j.jsat.2009.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Spear LP (2018). Effects of adolescent alcohol consumption on the brain and behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19(1), 197–214. 10.1038/nrn.2018.10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Stephenson MT, Hoyle RH, Palmgreen P, & Slater MD (2003). Brief measures of sensation seeking for screening and large-scale surveys. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 72(3), 279–286. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2003.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sznitman S, & Engel-Yeger B (2017). Sensation seeking and adolescent alcohol use: Exploring the mediating role of unstructured socializing with peers. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 52(3), 396–401. 10.1093/alcalc/agx008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Tabachnick BG, & Fidell LS (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Allyn and Bacon. [Google Scholar]
  74. Terrell KR, Zeglin RJ, Palmer RE, Moore MJ, & Barr EM (2021). Interpersonal trauma in high school: Substance use as a moderator of depression and suicidality. Journal of Child and Adolescent Counseling, 7(1), 1–12. 10.1080/23727810.2020.1835411 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tevyaw TOL, & Monti PM (2004). Motivational enhancement and other brief interventions for adolescent substance abuse: foundations, applications and evaluations. Addiction, 99, 63–75. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00855.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Treloar H, Martens MP, & McCarthy DM (2015). The Protective Behavioral Strategies Scale-20: Improved content validity of the Serious Harm Reduction subscale. Psychological Assessment, 27(1), 340–346. 10.1037/pas0000071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wang MT, & Fredricks JA (2014). The reciprocal links between school engagement, youth problem behaviors, and school dropout during adolescence. Child Development, 85(2), 722–737. 10.1111/cdev.12138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Weaver CC, Martens MP, & Smith AE (2012). Do protective behavioral strategies moderate the relationship between negative urgency and alcohol-related outcomes among intercollegiate athletes? Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 73(3), 498–503. 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.498 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. White HR, Labouvie EW (1989). Towards the assessment of adolescent problem drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 50(1), 30–37. 10.15288/jsa.1989.50.30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zamboanga BL, Olthuis JV, Kenney SR, Correia CJ, Van Tyne K, Ham LS, & Borsari B (2014). Not just fun and games: a review of college drinking games research from 2004 to 2013. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(3), 682–695. 10.1037/a0036639 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Zamboanga BL, Tomaso CC, Cloutier RM, Blumenthal H, Kenney SR, & Borsari B (2016). Drinking game participation among high school and incoming college students. Journal of Addictions Nursing, 24(1), E1. 10.1097/01.JAN.0000481966.33116.a5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Zeigler-Hill V, Madson MB, & Ricedorf A (2012). Does self-esteem moderate the associations between protective behavioral strategies and negative outcomes associated with alcohol consumption?. Journal of Drug Education, 42(2), 211–227. 10.2190/DE.42.2.f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Zuckerman M (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  84. Zuckerman M (1994). Behavioral expression and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  85. Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM, Joireman J, Teta P, & Kraft M (1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality: The big three, the big five, and the alternative five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 757–768. 10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.757 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES