Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Nov 16;18(11):e0294134. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294134

Let’s talk about PFAS: Inconsistent public awareness about PFAS and its sources in the United States

T Allen Berthold 1, Audrey McCrary 1, Stephanie deVilleneuve 1,*, Michael Schramm 1
Editor: Linglin Xie2
PMCID: PMC10653490  PMID: 37971973

Abstract

The presence of per- and polyfluoroalkyl substances (PFAS) in U.S. drinking water has recently garnered significant attention from the media, federal government, and public health professionals. While concerns for PFAS exposure continue to mount, the general public’s awareness and knowledge of the contaminant has remained unknown. This exploratory study sought to fill this data gap by administering a nationwide survey in which the awareness of PFAS and community contamination, awareness of PFAS containing products and intentions to change product use, and awareness and concern about PFAS in drinking water were assessed. The results indicated that almost half the respondents had never heard of PFAS and do not know what it is (45.1%). Additionally, 31.6% responded that they had heard of PFAS but do not know what it is. A large portion of respondents (97.4%) also responded that they did not believe their drinking water had been impacted by PFAS. Demographic association did not influence knowledge of PFAS or levels of concern with PFAS in drinking water. The strongest predictor of PFAS awareness was awareness due to known community exposure. The respondents aware of community exposure were more likely to have knowledge of PFAS sources, change their use of items with potential PFAS contamination, and answer that their drinking water sources were also contaminated with PFAS. Based on the received responses, PFAS information and health risks need to be better communicated to the public to help increase awareness. These efforts should also be coordinated between government agencies, utilities, the research community, and other responsible entities to bolster their effectiveness.

Introduction

Over the past two decades, per- and polyfluoroalkyl substances (PFAS) have caught the attention of international researchers and governments as a rapidly emerging environmental contaminant. Often referred to by the news media as “forever chemicals,” PFAS are a group of thousands of synthetic fluorinated chemical compounds that degrade slowly in the environment due to their extremely strong carbon-fluorine bond [1,2]. Since the late 1940s, PFAS have been produced and utilized in a wide range of industrial processes and consumer products because they are incredibly stable, non-reactive, and hydrophobic [3]. Examples of these products include food packaging, non-stick cookware, household upholstery, personal care products, and cleaning supplies. PFAS are also an integral component of fire extinguishing foams, or aqueous film-forming foams (AFFFs), used frequently in emergency response events and firefighting training activities [3,4].

A result of the extensive sources and high mobility of PFAS, in conjunction with their resistance to degradation, is that they have been bioaccumulating in soil, water, and air over time. This has consequently led to nearly all populations in developed countries having detectable levels of PFAS in their blood serum [5,6]. The widespread human exposure to PFAS can be partially attributed to its growing presence in surface and ground water sources used for public drinking water supplies and private drinking water wells. A recent United States Geological Service study found that at least half of the nation’s tap water supply is exposed to some amount of PFAS chemicals [7]. Drinking water is considered one of the dominant routes of exposure to PFAS for populations across the globe, particularly in communities that are near contaminated waters [810]. This has caused concern from public health professionals because exposure to PFAS has been linked to negative health effects such as cancer, irregular hormone development, liver damage, weakened immune systems, and reproductive harm [11].

While scientists continue to dissect the complexity of PFAS exposure in humans, the U.S. government’s response to regulating PFAS levels in drinking water has, until recently, been limited. In 2016, the United States Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) established the lifetime health advisory level for exposure to perfluorooctanoic acid (PFOA) and perfluorooctane sulfonate (PFOS) types of PFAS from drinking water at 70 ppt [12]. Andrews and Naidenko [13] estimate 0.4–1 million people are exposed to 70 ppt combined PFAS in drinking water systems and as much as 18–80 million or 8%-22% of the US population at 10 ppt. Cadwallader et al. [14] provides slightly higher estimates of 0.93 to 1.96 million people at 70 ppt and national level mean exposures of 4.7–5.2 ppt using Bayesian mixed models and the UCMR3 dataset. In March 2023, the EPA proposed an enforceable National Primary Drinking Water Regulation (NPDWR) to establish Maximum Contaminant Levels for six PFAS variants [15]. The newly proposed NPDWR seeks to reduce the enforceable maximum contaminant levels from 70 ppt to 4 ppt [15]. If finalized, this regulation will help reduce the levels of PFAS in drinking water, monitor for the presence of PFAS, and better notify the public of the levels of PFAS in their local water systems. The EPA has also allocated billions of dollars from the Infrastructure Investment and Jobs Act to improve states’ drinking water systems, including the addition of PFAS detection and monitoring. In addition to the United States government’s response to the growing threat of PFAS, multiple large corporations have been sued by communities across the country seeking damages and liability for PFAS clean up in municipal water supplies. One of the major corporations in these lawsuits is 3M, who reached a $10.3 billion settlement in June 2023 in which they will pay out money over 13 years to any cities and counties that want to test and clean up PFAS in their public water supplies [16]. Chemical manufacturers Chemours, DuPont, and Corteva also reached a settlement in June 2023 to pay $1.18 billion to remove PFAS from public drinking water systems [17]. Obsekov et al. [18] estimated the financial burden of broader health impacts of PFAS to be between $5.5 and $62.6 billion, rendering regulatory intervention and adoption of alternatives to PFAS the more economically viable alternative to continued PFAS use and exposure.

An increase in awareness of risks of environmental contaminants generally leads to changes in social stigma that spurs political, economic, and regulatory changes [19]. A key factor in the transition from overall awareness to meaningful behavioral and policy changes is the level of concern about the impact of pollutants on human health [20]. Public awareness of numerous environmental contaminants such as asbestos, lead, chlorinated hydrocarbons (e.g., PCBs, DDT, dioxins), and their effects on health have led to regulatory and voluntary changes in the use of these contaminants in manufactured products [2123]. The rapidly evolving scientific understanding of impacts of PFAS on human health likely contributes to the unawareness and uncertainty of the general public and slow pace of regulatory intervention [24,25].

Risk perception and avoidance research is primarily derived from public health studies concerned with disease mitigation. For example, social consequences, perceptions, and behavioral changes related to smoking and tobacco consumption have been investigated heavily since the landmark study by Wynder and Graham [26], which was the first to link smoking to lung cancer [27]. More recently, copious studies on COVID-19 risk perception and behavioral adaptations by the public were generated during the global pandemic [28]. The abundance of literature on these and other public health topics over the last century has allowed different frameworks of public perception to be extended into other fields of study, including climate change, pollution, food safety, and even nuclear energy, to name a few [2932]. In these studies across disciplines, one common theme is concluded from data: awareness, knowledge, and personal experience have a positive effect on behavioral changes. Existing research on public perceptions of PFAS has not fully investigated these factors, therefore the extension of these conclusions to the subject of PFAS is not currently established.

Internationally, concern and behaviors surrounding PFAS exposure have been sparsely studied. In Italy, concern about and perceived risk of detrimental health effects from PFAS exposure were elevated for mothers who had children, a wider social network, higher trust in scientific sources and social media, and were not employed full-time [33]. In Girardi et al. [33], the presence of social networking and trust in scientific information was a key predictor of an increase in subjective knowledge about PFAS. Communities in Australia expressed concern about the uncertainty surrounding PFAS, including its impact on long-term health outcomes and socio-economic impacts of contamination in localized areas [34]. Awareness of PFAS contamination, their magnitude, and potential impacts are still underdeveloped within both government agencies and general populations in Asia [35]. However, a single study focused on culinary preferences in India found 61.9% of the surveyed respondents were not aware of PFAS presence in non-stick cookware and intention to use non-stick cookware declined after information was given about the potential leaching of PFAS from this source [36].

The existing literature on public perceptions of PFAS primarily consists of studies about the experience of residents who have been directly affected by industrial PFAS contamination. Wickham and Shriver [25] found that scientific uncertainty led to mixed messaging from government agencies which increased anxiety and concern around acute PFAS contamination in North Carolina communities. Other stressors in affected communities include uncertainty about PFAS exposure pathways, timing of health effects, and financial burdens from decontamination of sources and medical treatments [37]. Many community members reported hearing about PFAS contamination through local news, neighbors, or incidental interaction with government responses [37]. The few, broader studies on public interaction with information about PFAS have noted an acceleration in published news articles and social media posts within the last decade, with a substantial surge occurring in the last two years [38,39]. However, it is unclear whether this increase in information is equivalent to an increase in awareness and action for the general population, who may not have personal experience with direct PFAS contamination [40]. Studies have not yet characterized the perceptions of PFAS across different communities that comprise the broader U.S. landscape.

Given the gap in research on awareness, concerns, and behaviors related to PFAS for the general public in the U.S., we designed this study to assess the population’s: (1) awareness of PFAS and community contamination, (2) awareness of PFAS containing products and intentions to change product use, and (3) awareness and concern about PFAS in drinking water. The survey was done using a nationally representative sample, so that the following results could be generalized for the broader U.S. population. This work was designed to provide a baseline measurement of these parameters so that the impact of future social and regulatory changes regarding the use of PFAS can be correctly discerned and accurately measured.

Methods

Survey instrument

The target population was the general U.S. population aged 18 or older. The survey was distributed in April 2023 through Qualtrics online panels. Panels were continuously sampled until a nationally representative sample was obtained. To approximate a representative sample of the U.S. population, panels were recruited using gender, age, and race/ethnicity quotas. The total sample size was 1,100 respondents and was estimated to be representative of the U.S. population within a ±3% margin of error at the 95% confidence level [41]. A summary of the demographic profile of sample respondents is included in the (S1 Table).

Qualtrics performed quality control checks to ensure response validity, including attention checks, survey duration checks, and IP address checks to prevent duplicate responses. Surveys that failed attention and speed checks, provided invalid answers, or did not meet representative demographic criteria were excluded. The Texas A&M University Institutional Review Board (TAMU IRB) reviewed the study protocol and survey instrument prior to distribution. TAMU IRB deemed the study to be exempt from formal review. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants in the first question of the survey instrument.

To determine awareness of PFAS, respondents were asked if they had heard of PFAS and their level of confidence in their knowledge about it. To assess community exposure, participants were also asked if, to the best of their knowledge, their community had been exposed to PFAS. To assess familiarity with sources of PFAS, survey participants were asked to rate their familiarity with 13 different potential items that might be contaminated with or cause PFAS contamination and their intentions to change use of those items. Although the use of PFAS compounds is much more extensive across industrial and consumer products [42], the items included in this survey were intended to be consistent with item categories currently summarized in U.S. Environmental Protection Agency reports and action plans [43]. To explore awareness and concern about PFAS contamination specifically in drinking water, we asked survey participants for their primary source of drinking water, if their primary source of drinking water had been impacted by PFAS, and their level of concern about PFAS in drinking water. Additionally, respondents were asked to estimate what percentage of the U.S. population they thought had been exposed to PFAS. Questions used for non-demographic variables are included in S2 Table.

Survey analysis

Although we applied sampling quotas, the returned marginal population levels did not completely match recent national-level statistics. Prior to analysis, individual survey responses were weighted so that marginal proportions of the survey more closely matched national level benchmarks from the 5-year 2021 American Community Survey (ACS) [44] on sex/gender, age group, race/ethnicity, and education level (Table 1). Weights on gender were developed by re-coding “female” and “other” responses as “non-male” because the ACS only provides binary response options for sex. Using this approach, responses from both “female” and “other” respondents have the same marginal weight. Kennedy et al. [45] provide substantial discussion on the treatment of sex and gender in survey adjustment. Due to small subpopulation sample sizes within the race/ethnicity variable, race/ethnicity were recoded as white or Caucasian and non-white categories. Weights were developed by poststratification raking using the American National Election Study (ANES) weighting algorithm implemented in the anesrake R package [46,47].

Table 1. Unadjusted and adjusted survey profile with target marginal population benchmarks derived from the 2021 American Community Survey [44].

Characteristic Unweighted N Unweighted % Target % Weighted N Weighted %
Age
    18:24 125 11.4 11.9 130.6 11.9
    25:34 192 17.5 17.7 195.1 17.7
    35:44 204 18.5 16.6 183.1 16.6
    45:54 198 18.0 16.3 179.2 16.3
    55:64 171 15.5 16.8 184.4 16.8
    65+ 208 18.9 20.7 227.6 20.7
    No answer 2 0.2 - - -
Education
    Some high school 47 4.3 7.8 85.8 7.8
    High school graduate or GED 418 38.0 49.4 543.7 49.4
    Associate’s degree 178 16.2 8.3 91.3 8.3
    Bachelor’s degree 246 22.4 19.4 213.7 19.4
    Master’s degree 132 12.0 8.3 91.3 8.3
    Doctorate or terminal degree 28 2.5 1.3 14.7 1.3
    Other 40 3.6 5.4 59.5 5.4
    No answer 11 1.0 - - -
Race/Ethnicity
    White 723 65.7 62.4 686.3 62.4
    Non-white 373 33.9 37.6 413.7 37.6
    No answer 4 0.4 - - -
Sex/Gender
    Male 529 48.1 49.0 539.1 49.0
    Not Male 569 51.7 51.0 560.9 51.0
    No answer 2 0.2 - - -

To explore factors associated with an individual’s understanding of PFAS, two different proportional odds models [48] were developed relating: 1) self-described knowledge of PFAS (4 responses ranging from “I’ve never heard of it, and don’t know what it is” to “I’m confident I know what it is”); and 2) awareness of potential sources of PFAS (5 responses ranging from “Not at all familiar” to “Extremely familiar”; to sex/gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, and awareness of community exposure to PFAS. To explore factors associated with intended behavior change, a proportional odds model was developed relating intention to change use of items associated with PFAS (5 responses ranging from “Will never change” to “Have already changed”) to the same dependent variables.

Additionally, the probability that an individual was aware of PFAS impacting their drinking water was explored using a logistic regression model relating awareness of PFAS contamination in drinking water (dummy variable) to sex/gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, drinking water source, and awareness of community exposure to PFAS. A final model evaluated the factors associated with an individual’s level of concern about PFAS in their drinking water using a proportional odds model to fit level of concern (5 responses ranging from “Not at all concerned” to “Extremely concerned”) to sex/gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, drinking water source, and awareness of PFAS contamination in drinking water.

Model results are presented as odds-ratios (with approximate p-values calculated by comparing the t-value against the standard normal distribution). Marginal effects are also presented as population-level predicted probabilities for a given predictor estimated using observed values [49]. Confidence intervals (95%) were derived using a parametric bootstrap as implemented in the svyEffects R package [50]. All models were fit using the survey package in R version 4.2.1 [51,52].

Results

Most respondents reported no knowledge of (41.1%) or were unsure (47.4%) if their community had been exposed to PFAS (Table 2). Only 11.5% responded that they knew their community has been exposed to PFAS. When asked to describe knowledge level about PFAS, 45.1% responded that they have never heard of it and do not know what it is. An additional 31.6% responded they have heard of PFAS, but do not know what PFAS is. On average, respondents estimated that 54.2% of the U.S. population had been exposed to PFAS.

Table 2. Population level estimates of responses for PFAS knowledge, awareness of community exposure, sources of drinking water, awareness of drinking water contamination, and concern about drinking water contamination.

Question Percent Response, (SE)
What is your main source of drinking water?
    Unfiltered tap water 27.9 (1.5)
    Filtered tap water 37.6 (1.6)
    Bottled/prepackaged water 34.2 (1.6)
    Other 0.3 (0.1)
To your knowledge, has your primary source of drinking water been impacted by PFAS?
    No 97.4 (0.5)
    Yes 2.6 (0.5)
How concerned are you about PFAS in your drinking water?
    Not at all concerned 23.1 (1.4)
    Slightly concerned 17.8 (1.2)
    Moderately concerned 24.3 (1.4)
    Very concerned 19.1 (1.3)
    Extremely concerned 15.7 (1.2)
To your knowledge, has your community been exposed to PFAS?
    Yes 11.5 (1.0)
    No 41.1 (1.6)
    Not sure 47.4 (1.6)
How would you describe your knowledge about PFAS as an environmental contaminant?
    I’ve never heard of it, and don’t know what it is 45.1 (1.6)
    I’ve heard of it or seen it somewhere, but don’t know what it is 31.6 (1.5)
    I think I know what it is 17.2 (1.2)
    I’m confident I know what it is 6.2 (0.8)

Most individuals said they use unfiltered (27.9%) or filtered (37.6%) tap water as their main source of drinking water. A large majority of people responded that, to their knowledge, their drinking water had not been impacted by PFAS (97.4%). When asked about their level of concern about PFAS in drinking water, 23.1% had no concerns, 17.8% and 24.3% were slightly or moderately concerned. Fewer people responded that they were extremely concerned (15.7%) or very concerned (19.1%) about PFAS in their drinking water.

On average, 47.6% (SE = 0.6%) of respondents were “Not at all familiar” with potential sources of PFAS included in the survey (Table 3). The probability of response decreased for increasing levels of familiarity across all potential PFAS sources with only 7.2% (SE = 0.3%) of respondents, on average across all sources, responded “Extremely familiar.”

Table 3. Population-level estimates of percent responses to awareness of different potential sources of PFAS contamination.

Percent Response
Sources Not at all familiar1 Slightly familiar1 Moderately familiar1 Very familiar1 Extremely familiar1
Drinking water 45.8 (1.6) 19.7 (1.3) 17.1 (1.2) 9.2 (0.9) 8.2 (0.9)
Waterways near waste disposal sites 45.2 (1.6) 18.3 (1.3) 20.0 (1.3) 10.7 (1.0) 5.9 (0.8)
Soils near waste disposal sites 46.3 (1.6) 20.2 (1.3) 17.5 (1.2) 10.1 (0.9) 5.9 (0.8)
Dairy products 51.1 (1.6) 16.3 (1.2) 15.3 (1.2) 10.0 (0.9) 7.3 (0.8)
Fresh produce 50.3 (1.6) 14.5 (1.1) 16.1 (1.2) 11.7 (1.0) 7.4 (0.8)
Freshwater fish 48.7 (1.6) 16.4 (1.2) 17.6 (1.2) 11.4 (1.0) 5.9 (0.7)
Seafood 48.8 (1.6) 15.3 (1.2) 17.7 (1.2) 9.9 (1.0) 8.3 (0.9)
Food packaging 48.1 (1.6) 16.2 (1.2) 16.9 (1.2) 11.8 (1.0) 7.0 (0.8)
Non-stick cookware 47.0 (1.6) 16.9 (1.2) 16.8 (1.2) 12.8 (1.1) 6.5 (0.8)
Personal hygiene products 46.6 (1.6) 14.7 (1.1) 18.2 (1.3) 12.6 (1.0) 8.0 (0.9)
Household products 45.0 (1.6) 16.0 (1.2) 18.7 (1.3) 12.3 (1.0) 7.9 (0.9)
Fire extinguishing foam 50.7 (1.6) 14.7 (1.1) 15.8 (1.2) 11.7 (1.0) 7.1 (0.9)
Fertilizers from wastewater plants 45.9 (1.6) 17.2 (1.2) 17.0 (1.2) 11.9 (1.0) 8.1 (0.9)

1Percent Responses (Standard Error).

Individual intention to change product usage due to PFAS contamination was less certain. Across all items, most individuals responded, “Not sure” (Mean = 24.6%, SE = 0.9%) or “Might change” (Mean = 27.9%, SE = 0.4%; Table 4). While the mean response rate across product categories for individuals that have already changed product use was only 11.8% (SE = 0.7%), the drinking water item stood out with 18.9% (SE = 1.3%) of respondents indicating they have already changed use of drinking water.

Table 4. Population level estimates of percent responses rating intention to change products because of potential for PFAS contamination.

Percent Response
Sources Will never change1 Not sure1 Might change1 Planning to change1 Have already changed1
Drinking water 15.8 (1.2) 18.4 (1.3) 26.5 (1.4) 20.5 (1.3) 18.9 (1.3)
Waterways near waste disposal sites 13.9 (1.1) 26.7 (1.5) 27.9 (1.4) 20.5 (1.3) 11.0 (1.0)
Soils near waste disposal sites 15.7 (1.2) 28.1 (1.5) 26.4 (1.4) 19.8 (1.3) 10.0 (1.0)
Dairy products 17.8 (1.3) 22.8 (1.4) 29.8 (1.5) 18.6 (1.2) 11.0 (1.0)
Fresh produce 18.4 (1.3) 22.5 (1.4) 28.8 (1.5) 19.9 (1.3) 10.4 (1.0)
Freshwater fish 17.3 (1.2) 25.2 (1.4) 29.3 (1.5) 17.9 (1.2) 10.4 (1.0)
Seafood 18.1 (1.3) 24.6 (1.4) 28.0 (1.4) 19.7 (1.3) 9.6 (0.9)
Food packaging 15.7 (1.2) 23.6 (1.4) 27.7 (1.4) 21.0 (1.3) 11.9 (1.0)
Non-stick cookware 15.0 (1.2) 23.2 (1.4) 27.9 (1.4) 18.8 (1.3) 15.0 (1.1)
Personal hygiene products 15.4 (1.2) 23.8 (1.4) 28.9 (1.5) 20.2 (1.3) 11.6 (1.0)
Household products 15.1 (1.2) 22.0 (1.4) 29.8 (1.5) 21.5 (1.3) 11.5 (1.0)
Fire extinguishing foam 17.4 (1.2) 29.7 (1.5) 25.7 (1.4) 16.6 (1.2) 10.7 (1.0)
Fertilizers from wastewater plants 14.2 (1.1) 28.6 (1.4) 26.0 (1.4) 19.5 (1.3) 11.7 (1.0)

1Percent Responses (Standard Error).

Factors associated with self-described PFAS knowledge and products

We did not find evidence for any association between sex, race/ethnicity, or education with self-described knowledge about PFAS (Table 5). With the sample size used in the current study we were not able to incorporate the sub-populations and develop a model that would converge. There was not strong evidence for the influence of age on PFAS knowledge among most of the age brackets (Table 5). However, there is evidence to support that individuals in the 35:44 age bracket will respond with a higher self-assessed knowledge level (OR = 1.63, p = 0.039; Table 5) than someone in the reference bracket (18:24). There was also strong evidence that people aware of PFAS exposure in their communities self-report higher levels of knowledge about PFAS. People aware of PFAS exposure in their communities are 3.57 times and 4.35 times more likely to respond with a higher self-assessed knowledge level than those responding “No” (OR = 0.28, p < 0.001; Table 5) or “Not sure” (OR = 0.23, p <0.001; Table 5) to awareness of PFAS contamination in their communities.

Table 5. Odds ratios, confidence intervals, and approximate p-values from (Model 1) the proportional odds model relating covariates to self-described knowledge levels about PFAS; (Model 2) the logistic regression model relating covariates with awareness of drinking water contamination from PFAS; and (Model 3) the proportional odds model relating covariates with levels of concerns about PFAS in drinking water.

Model 1: Knowledge level of PFAS Model 2: Knowledge of drinking water contamination Model 3: Level of concern about PFAS
Characteristic OR1 95% CI1 p-value OR1 95% CI1 p-value OR1 95% CI1 p-value
Sex/Gender
    Male
    Female 0.99 0.76, 1.29 >0.9 0.66 0.25, 1.75 0.4 1.08 0.84, 1.38 0.5
    Other 0.88 0.19, 3.38 0.8 0.00 0.00, 0.00 <0.001 0.67 0.26, 1.77 0.4
Age
    18:24
    25:34 1.38 0.87, 2.18 0.2 1.30 0.12, 14.0 0.8 0.93 0.63, 1.39 0.7
    35:44 1.63* 1.03, 2.59 0.039 0.72 0.07, 7.45 0.8 1.21 0.80, 1.82 0.4
    45:54 1.03 0.64, 1.67 0.9 1.64 0.13, 21.2 0.7 1.30 0.86, 1.96 0.2
    55:64 1.09 0.66, 1.80 0.7 4.47 0.29, 69.7 0.3 0.80 0.50, 1.28 0.3
    65+ 0.94 0.57, 1.57 0.8 0.80 0.06, 10.1 0.9 0.74 0.47, 1.16 0.2
Race/Ethnicity
    White
    Non-white 0.95 0.71, 1.27 0.7 0.58 0.19, 1.72 0.3 1.04 0.78, 1.39 0.8
Education
    Some high school
    High school/GED 0.65 0.34, 1.23 0.2 0.74 0.05, 10.7 0.8 0.67 0.34, 1.30 0.2
    Associate’s degree 0.89 0.46, 1.76 0.7 0.90 0.05, 15.0 >0.9 0.54 0.27, 1.09 0.084
    Bachelor’s degree 0.98 0.50, 1.92 >0.9 1.46 0.13, 17.1 0.8 0.67 0.34, 1.35 0.3
    Master’s degree 1.04 0.51, 2.12 >0.9 1.54 0.14, 16.5 0.7 1.01 0.49, 2.08 >0.9
    Doctorate or terminal degree 1.38 0.57, 3.32 0.6 1.61 0.08, 34.2 0.8 1.05 0.39, 2.84 >0.9
    Other 1.56 0.61, 3.94 0.4 2.20 0.15, 31.3 0.6 0.93 0.39, 2.20 0.9
Community PFAS Exposure
    Yes
    No 0.28*** 0.19, 0.41 <0.001 0.01*** 0.00, 0.09 <0.001
    Not Sure 0.23*** 0.16, 0.35 <0.001 0.12** 0.03, 0.49 0.003
Drinking Water Source
    Unfiltered tap water
    Filtered tap water 0.28* 0.10, 0.79 0.016 1.28 0.95, 1.73 0.10
    Bottled/prepackaged water 0.44 0.17, 1.19 0.11 1.44* 1.05, 1.99 0.025
    Other 0.00*** 0.00, 0.00 <0.001 1.73 0.69, 4.34 0.2
Drinking Contaminated by PFAS
    No
    Yes 4.27 2.20, 8.31 <0.001

1 OR = Odds Ratio, CI = Confidence Interval.

*p<0.05

**p<0.01

***p<0.001.

There was no difference in marginal predicted response probabilities between people that were unsure if their community had been exposed to PFAS or said their community had not been exposed to PFAS for responses to self-assessed knowledge about PFAS (Fig 1). People that were aware that their community had been exposed to PFAS had between a 10.8% to 11.6% higher probability of responding they were confident of their knowledge of PFAS compared to the remaining groups. They also had a 15.8% to 17.9% higher probability of responding they thought that they knew what PFAS was compared to the remaining groups. Conversely, someone that is aware of community PFAS exposure was much less likely to respond that they had never heard of PFAS and did not know what it was (20%) compared to those that said their community has not been exposed (46%) or did not know (51%).

Fig 1. Average marginal effects (left) and contrasted effects (right) of awareness of community PFAS exposure on self-assessed knowledge of PFAS.

Fig 1

Horizontal lines indicate the 95% confidence intervals of the marginal predicted probabilities and contrasts in marginal predicted probabilities across the population.

Awareness of community PFAS exposure also shows strong associations with familiarity of potential PFAS sources and intentions to change use of items with potential for PFAS contamination (Figs 2 and 3). On average, 46% of respondents that were unaware and 46.5% of those that were not sure if their communities were contaminated by PFAS responded they were “not familiar at all” with specific sources of PFAS contamination. This decreased to averages of 6% and 4% for the “extremely familiar” response. On average, people that stated their communities were contaminated by PFAS had a lower probability (18%) of responding that they were “not familiar at all” and higher probability (20%) of being “extremely familiar” with PFAS sources compared to the other two groups.

Fig 2. Average marginal effects of awareness of community PFAS exposure on the response probability for familiarity with different products associated with PFAS contamination.

Fig 2

Horizontal lines are the 95% confidence intervals of the marginal predicted probability across the population.

Fig 3. Average marginal effects of awareness of community PFAS exposure on the response probability for intention to change use of different products associated with PFAS contamination.

Fig 3

Horizontal lines are the 95% confidence intervals of the marginal predicted probability across the population.

Respondents who answered no or were unsure of community PFAS contamination were on average more likely to say they will never change their use of items (17% and 17%) compared to those aware of PFAS contamination in their community (8%; Fig 3). Those aware of community PFAS contamination were also more likely on average to have already changed use of items (22%) relative to the other two groups (11% for the “no” group and 10% for the “unsure” group).

PFAS and drinking water contamination

There was not strong evidence that age, race, or education are predictive of an individual’s awareness of PFAS contamination in their drinking water (Table 5). There was some evidence supporting correlations with the gender variable, with individuals identifying as other having 100% lower odds of responding that they know that their water is contaminated with PFAS (OR = 0, p < 0.001; Table 5) than individuals identifying male. There was strong evidence that awareness of community PFAS exposure was associated with knowledge that drinking water sources were contaminated with PFAS. The odds that an individual aware of community PFAS exposure indicated their drinking water was contaminated with PFAS was 100 times greater than those that responded they were unaware of community PFAS exposure (OR = 0.01, p < 0.001; Table 5) and 8.3 times lower than those that were uncertain of PFAS exposure (OR = 0.12, p = 0.003; Table 5). There was also evidence for associations between the types of drinking water sources used by an individual and their awareness of their drinking water being contaminated by PFAS. Individuals with unfiltered tap water had 3.57 times the odds of being aware that their drinking water was contaminated than those with filtered tap water (OR = 0.28, p = 0.016; Table 5).

There was not strong evidence that sex/gender, age, race/ethnicity, or education were associated with levels of concern with PFAS contamination in drinking water (Table 5). There was some evidence of associations between source of drinking water and levels of concern about PFAS contamination with users of bottled/prepackaged water having 1.44 times the odds of higher levels of concern about PFAS contamination in drinking water compared to those that use unfiltered tap water (p = 0.025; Table 5). There was strong evidence of associations between awareness of PFAS contamination in drinking water and concern about PFAS in drinking water. An individual that was aware of PFAS contamination in their drinking water had 4.27 times higher odds of reporting a higher level of concern about PFAS contamination that someone that was unaware of contamination (p = <0.001; Table 5). Marginal predicted probabilities show that an individual aware of PFAS contamination in their drinking water had a 27.0% higher probability of being “Extremely concerned” about PFAS contamination and 6.0% higher probability of being “Very concerned” (Fig 4). Conversely, individuals who said their drinking water was not contaminated by PFAS were more likely to respond that they were “Not at all concerned”, “Slightly concerned”, or “Moderately concerned”.

Fig 4. Average marginal effects (left) and contrasts in effects (right) of awareness of drinking water PFAS contamination on level of concerns about PFAS contamination in drinking water.

Fig 4

Horizontal lines indicate the 95% confidence intervals of the marginal predicted probabilities and contrasts in marginal predicted probabilities across the population.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this study was the first measure of awareness of PFAS within the general U.S. population. Overall, only about half of the respondents stated they were aware of PFAS as an environmental contaminant, while 76% of respondents stated they did not know what PFAS are. Despite these gaps, most respondents stated that they had some level of concern about PFAS in their drinking water. Those who were the most concerned with PFAS contaminating their drinking water were also those who indicated their primary source of drinking water had been contaminated. Community exposure appears to be the strongest predicting factor regarding the level of public knowledge and awareness of PFAS and its sources.

Individuals who responded that they were aware of PFAS contamination in their community generally reported higher perceived knowledge of PFAS as an environmental contaminant. This relationship is consistent with findings by Liu and Yang [40], who concluded that an increase in perceived personal relevance of PFAS boosts the information-seeking behaviors of individuals to reach a level of sufficient, or useful, knowledge. However, in other studies, individuals who experienced acute contamination in their communities still expressed uncertainty about the practical aspects of PFAS knowledge, including pathways of exposure, health risks, and potential mitigation strategies [25,37]. While improved awareness and knowledge of PFAS is logical for communities that have faced acute contamination, it appears that the flood of information about PFAS as a contaminant may not translate into practical understanding of PFAS exposure and the health implications in everyday life. Our results show that awareness and knowledge of PFAS is underdeveloped in the general population, which could mean the broader public either has not reached the threshold where PFAS is relevant enough to prompt information-seeking behaviors, or existing communications about PFAS are not translating the personal relevance of the abundance of exposure pathways or the long-term health implications effectively.

In communities exposed to industrial PFAS contamination, uncertainty about the chemical stems from the conflicting information presented by local government, state agencies, and PFAS manufacturers responding to the situation [25,37]. Likewise, the broader population also faces uncertainty about where to find practical information for PFAS and details about their impact on public health [38]. Currently, there is no definitive answer for who is responsible for communicating the risks of PFAS to the general public, which can make individuals and communities feel isolated from the scientific and regulatory discourse [38]. Distrust in political institutions and the opaque use of jargon by PFAS manufacturers limits the effectiveness of their involvement in public outreach [25]. Some findings suggest scientific institutions, such as environmental agencies, universities, and national or state research institutes, may be the best authority for communicating about PFAS because they are more trusted to provide timely and actionable information [53]. Conflicting information about PFAS will continue to stifle public awareness, and in turn, limit effective action and regulatory policy until more cohesive and decisive messaging is adopted. A discussion about which institutions have responsibility for PFAS messaging and honest feedback about the effects of PFAS could help create a unified communication strategy, so that social discourse about PFAS is unambiguous, honest, and reliable–to the benefit of the general public.

Regardless of who sends the message, entities who communicate to the public must be clear about what is known about PFAS so individuals, health professionals, and communities can make educated decisions to minimize exposure [54]. On average, nearly half of survey respondents were completely unfamiliar with various consumer products as sources of PFAS. Again, those who were aware that their community had been affected by PFAS were more likely to say they had greater familiarity with everyday sources of PFAS. A survey by Shin et al. [20] found that chemicals in consumer products were the most common concern related to environmental health risk. Our survey revealed that while 55% of the population may have heard of PFAS, just 23% felt they understood PFAS as an environmental contaminant, meaning most of the population did not know about its uses, risks, or extent as a chemical present in consumer products. Similarly, Dong and Yang [53] found that respondents felt they had just a quarter of the sufficient knowledge needed to make informed decisions about the risks of PFAS to their personal health. Insufficient knowledge is a clear detriment for the adoption of behaviors that reduce personal risk [55]. However, knowledge must have subjective context to affect behavioral intentions and outcomes of individuals [56]. The knowledge presented about PFAS in products must not just be broadcast, but also be unambiguous, relevant, and actionable, so that more consumers can make informed decisions about their level of exposure.

The intention to change the use of consumer products containing PFAS was (again) closely linked to awareness of PFAS exposure within the respondent’s community and familiarity with PFAS sources. Individuals who are more aware of the risks of exposure may be more motivated to seek information and act on it [57,58]. Other literature has shown perceived social responsibility, which can be influenced through direct primary contacts (friends, family, coworkers) or mass media, to be a critical factor in risk avoidant behaviors at an individual level as well [28,31,59]. However, there may be other factors acting as barriers to action for those not intending to change usage habits, such as the costs associated with avoiding PFAS in drinking water and food products (such as replacing cookware or installing filters) and the perceived efficacy of remediation [58]. Citizens in communities with severe exposure to PFAS contamination have cited lack of resources, including financial and technical assistance, as a limiting factor for avoiding PFAS-contaminated water [37]. Increasing awareness to encourage knowledge-seeking behavior and spur changes in intention may be helpful, but these intentions may not be achievable for many individuals, especially those in low-income populations. More research into the efficacy of financial and technical assistance to address PFAS in both acutely contaminated communities and the broader population may help uncover effective solutions. Furthermore, regulatory intervention to reduce the baseline environmental presence of PFAS may help narrow the gap between contamination and remediation.

In our study, those who responded that they were aware their community had been exposed to PFAS were also significantly more likely to have knowledge of contamination in drinking water. Though only 2.6% of respondents believed their primary source of drinking water had been exposed to PFAS, the presence of PFAS in drinking water in the U.S. is quite extensive, with an estimated 45% of all drinking water samples containing at least one type of PFAS, according to a recent study by Smalling et al. [7]. In communities near contaminated sites, approximately 75% of PFAS exposure comes from drinking water alone [8]. Our results indicate that awareness of community PFAS contamination may largely be attributed to known contamination of drinking water supplies, even though dietary exposure is the major contributor of population PFAS exposure in the U.S. [60,61]. The increased awareness of PFAS in communities affected by acute drinking water contamination may be attributed to heightened local media attention and warnings from drinking water suppliers and city governments. However, the results also show that 20% of the respondents who are aware of PFAS exposure in their drinking water also described themselves as having limited knowledge of PFAS. Therefore, it is important to emphasize education efforts in areas where community exposure is high. Dietary exposure to PFAS, as well as exposure through dust inhalation and consumer goods, is likely unreported at the community level because it is rarely traced to point source contamination and the innumerable pathways of exposure leave the responsibility of communicating the risks uncertain. If known community exposure is the key to PFAS knowledge and awareness, municipalities and private water suppliers should prioritize funds to test for PFAS contamination in drinking water systems. This would enable these entities to provide the public with accurate, real-time data that creates a personal link between the consumers and PFAS exposure, likely leading them to want to know more. Manufacturers of PFAS products should also clearly label their products with the presence of the chemical, so that consumers can be aware of their exposure frequency and make adjustments in use as desired.

While the results indicate there are no strong connections between individual PFAS awareness in drinking water and demographics such as race, education level, and age, certain groups are more likely to be exposed to PFAS from contaminated drinking water. For example, Liddie [62] found community water systems with higher concentrations of PFAS were more likely to serve greater proportions of Hispanic/Latino and non-Hispanic black populations. A study conducted at a PFAS contaminated water supply in Paulsboro, New Jersey found that perfluorononanoic acid (PFNA) blood serum levels were higher in older residents compared to younger, and higher in males compared to females [63]. Other studies support the findings that males typically have higher concentrations of PFAS in their blood serum than females [5,44,64,65] and that PFAS exposure from drinking water increases in magnitude with age [66]. Lower concentrations of PFAS in female serum is likely due to elimination from menstrual blood loss and PFAS transfer during breastfeeding [67]. Greater concentrations in older populations may be due to higher cumulative exposure or changes in susceptibility [66]. Due to the lack of PFAS awareness from drinking water across all demographic groups, entities aimed at increasing public awareness should spend more resources targeting groups that are likely to be exposed to greater concentrations of PFAS from drinking water such as minorities, males, and older populations. While increasing the awareness of all groups is ideal, prioritizing demographics with higher exposure may help change the behaviors of those who are the most vulnerable to significant health risks.

Within the study design and subsequent dataset there were a few limitations that could have influenced certain outcomes. One limitation is that race/ethnicity sub-populations effects cannot be excluded because race/ethnicity was collapsed into “white” and “non-white” categories. For example, all the respondents identifying as “Native Hawaiian or Other Pacific Islander” (unweighted n = 3) responded that they had never heard of or knew what PFAS were. Some sub-populations might be more likely to answer that they have less knowledge about PFAS. Another limitation was the absence of geographic targets or weighting in the analysis. This can skew the results to be over or under targeting areas with PFAS contamination relative to the overall population. Additionally, the use of an online survey instrument creates inherent bias against portions of the U.S. population who may not have internet access. An estimated 7%, or nearly 23 million Americans, do not use the internet [68]. Therefore, the sample obtained from this survey may not be generalizable to this portion of the population that is not accessible via internet-based survey instruments.

Conclusion

This study used an exploratory public survey to identify large discrepancies in awareness about PFAS, its sources, and the adoption of behavioral change to avoid PFAS exposure among the U.S. population. Through the analysis it became clear that greater PFAS awareness, knowledge, and willingness to change behavior is associated with communities that have known PFAS drinking water contamination. The inconsistent public awareness about PFAS indicates that improved efforts in educating the public still need to be undertaken by the U.S. government, utilities, universities, state extension services, and other scientific institutions with high public trust. While there is widely available information regarding PFAS sources and negative health effects, an overwhelming majority of the U.S. population are still completely unaware of what PFAS are, even if they have heard of them. The rapidly evolving scientific understanding of PFAS has led to uncertain messaging to the public which can impact their overall awareness. The widespread nature of the presence of PFAS in humans is also not common knowledge amongst the American people as only a small fraction of respondents were aware that their primary source of drinking water had been impacted, despite studies that have determined otherwise. This study builds on a growing body of evidence that improved messaging and communication about PFAS, its sources, and its health risks are needed. As scientific understanding of the health impacts and scope of PFAS exposure increases, coordinated efforts are also required among government agencies, the research community, and utilities to develop and evaluate the effectiveness of public messaging efforts.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Unadjusted demographic characteristics of all survey respondents.

(PDF)

S2 Table. Questions and response options used in survey instrument.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Ed Rhodes, of the Texas Water Resources Institute, for his insight regarding the draft of the manuscript.

Data Availability

All raw data files are available from the Zenodo database. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.8132987.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

Decision Letter 0

Linglin Xie

15 Aug 2023

PONE-D-23-21851Let's talk about PFAS: Inconsistent public awareness about PFAS and its sources in the United StatesPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. deVilleneuve,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 29 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Linglin Xie

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well. 

3. Please include a copy of Table 6 which you refer to in your text on page 18.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information. 

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The article has measured the awareness of PFAS within the general US population. However, there are major questions noticed.

1. The online distribution of surveys could already contribute bias to represent the general US population on socioeconomics, education, and other factors. Please discuss this potential bias.

2. One of the major finding of the article is recognized community exposure as predicting factor regarding the level of public knowledge and awareness of PFAS. Although a higher awareness of PFAS could increase information-seeking behavior, what is the application of this finding in promoting the general population awareness of PFAS?

3. Detailed validation would be beneficial to look into the respondent who reported “yes” to the “Has your community been exposed to PFAS?” Are they from similar communities? Has anyone reported “no” to this question from the same communities?

Reviewer #2: In this study, Berthold et al. evaluated the general public’s awareness of PFAS contamination. They found that about half of respondents had never heard of PFAS. The respondents who are aware of PFAS community exposure were more likely to have knowledge of PFAS. They conclude that PFAS information and health risks need to be better communicated to the public to help increase awareness. This study provides a general picture of public awareness of PFAS contamination, which would have effects on social and regulatory changes in the use of PFAS. I have several questions as following:

1. The authors mentioned that the quality control checks were performed by Qualtrics. Could the authors provide detailed information about how the quality control was performed. How many surveys were received, how many surveys were used in the study. If some surveys were not used, please explain the reasons. Please provide the URL of Qualtrics used in the method part.

2. In this study, the author found that almost half of the respondents had never heard of PFAS and do not know what it is. Whether it is because the areas where these people live are not contaminated by PFAS or because they lack the education on PFAS contamination?

3. For the people who are aware of community exposure to PFAS, do the authors know how they learned about this information. This can help to better promote the PFAS information.

4. It is reasonable that people who are aware of PFAS exposure are more likely to know more about PFAS. As mentioned in the study, the percentage of people who are aware of PFAS exposure but have limited knowledge about PFAS was 20%. It is important for this population to learn about PFAS since they live in the area where PFAS exposure is higher. The author should discuss this point.

5. In the discussion, could the author add several sentences about how to increase the public awareness about PFAS contamination in drinking water, especially in higher exposure area?

6. There are some typos, such as line 339 “that”.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Nov 16;18(11):e0294134. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294134.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


11 Sep 2023

Journal Requirements:

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming.

Authors’ response: We have updated the style of the manuscript to meet PLOS ONE’s requirements.

2. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well.

Authors’ response: We have made the suggested additions to clarify IRB approval methods and consent requirements. The changes are reflected in the Methods section, lines 166-171, and reads as follows:

“The Texas A&M University Institutional Review Board (TAMU IRB) reviewed the study protocol and survey instrument prior to distribution. TAMU IRB deemed the study to be exempt from formal review. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants in the first question of the survey instrument.”

3. Please include a copy of Table 6 which you refer to in your text on page 18.

Authors’ response: The reference to Table 6 on page 18 was a typo. We have corrected the text to refer to Table 5.

4. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly.

Authors’ response: As suggested, we have added captions for the Supporting Information files and updated the in-text citations to match.

Comments to the Author:

Reviewer #1:

1. The online distribution of surveys could already contribute bias to represent the general US population on socioeconomics, education, and other factors. Please discuss this potential bias.

Authors’ response: We agree that there is general inherent bias in conducting internet research, as it excludes a portion of the population without internet access from the potential sample selection. We have added the following text in the Discussion section to acknowledge this limitation:

“Additionally, the use of an online survey instrument creates inherent bias against portions of the U.S. population who may not have internet access. An estimated 7%, or nearly 23 million Americans, do not use the internet [68]. Therefore, the sample obtained from this survey may not be generalizable to this portion of the population that is not accessible via internet-based survey instruments.”

2. One of the major finding of the article is recognized community exposure as predicting factor regarding the level of public knowledge and awareness of PFAS. Although a higher awareness of PFAS could increase information-seeking behavior, what is the application of this finding in promoting the general population awareness of PFAS?

Authors’ response: The application of this finding for which methods would best raise awareness of PFAS in the general population is outside of the scope of this study. Our instrument was designed to be exploratory in nature since the scientific community has no base measure of awareness within the U.S. population. The data we collected from our respondents is meant to inform the need for future work, which could focus on the determining the best way to apply this finding. We broadly discuss potential avenues for raising public awareness with support from other studies throughout the manuscript, but we feel it is outside of the scope of our data to make definitive conclusions on how best to promote PFAS awareness.

3. Detailed validation would be beneficial to look into the respondent who reported “yes” to the “Has your community been exposed to PFAS?” Are they from similar communities? Has anyone reported “no” to this question from the same communities?

Authors’ response: This is a good idea for a new line of inquiry with this data. However, spatial validation at this scale was not pursued in this manuscript because it is not possible to replicate multiple answers at the ZIP code scale with the sample size of 1,100 responses. Spatial analysis was not the primary goal of this study and would be better pursued in a follow-up study that addresses these details with different research questions.

Reviewer #2: In this study, Berthold et al. evaluated the general public’s awareness of PFAS contamination. They found that about half of respondents had never heard of PFAS. The respondents who are aware of PFAS community exposure were more likely to have knowledge of PFAS. They conclude that PFAS information and health risks need to be better communicated to the public to help increase awareness. This study provides a general picture of public awareness of PFAS contamination, which would have effects on social and regulatory changes in the use of PFAS. I have several questions as following:

1. The authors mentioned that the quality control checks were performed by Qualtrics. Could the authors provide detailed information about how the quality control was performed. How many surveys were received, how many surveys were used in the study. If some surveys were not used, please explain the reasons. Please provide the URL of Qualtrics used in the method part.

Authors’ response: Thank you for this suggestion. We have added statements to clarify what type of quality control checks Qualtrics uses. As stated in the manuscript, the total sample size was 1,100 surveys using a demographic quota to obtain a statistically representative sample of the general U.S. population. We cannot provide the number of surveys sent out in total, because Qualtrics did not provide that data. The URL of the Qualtrics survey could not be added because it is no longer active.

2. In this study, the author found that almost half of the respondents had never heard of PFAS and do not know what it is. Whether it is because the areas where these people live are not contaminated by PFAS or because they lack the education on PFAS contamination?

Authors’ response: This would be a good research question for a future study. What we know from our study is that people who are both unknowledgeable about PFAS and unaware of contamination in their community are unaware of PFAS overall. The survey instrument used does not delineate the cause of PFAS unawareness, therefore we believe it would be outside the scope of our study and data to attempt to address this question.

3. For the people who are aware of community exposure to PFAS, do the authors know how they learned about this information. This can help to better promote the PFAS information.

Authors’ response: Thank you for this suggestion. This would have been good additional data to collect. However, that information was not collected with this study and is beyond the scope of our analysis. This is a question that could be pursued in a follow-up study that specifically samples communities with exposure. Other studies that investigate sources of information in exposed communities are discussed in lines 133-135 of the Introduction. This is briefly revisited in lines 443-445 of the Discussion.

4. It is reasonable that people who are aware of PFAS exposure are more likely to know more about PFAS. As mentioned in the study, the percentage of people who are aware of PFAS exposure but have limited knowledge about PFAS was 20%. It is important for this population to learn about PFAS since they live in the area where PFAS exposure is higher. The author should discuss this point.

Authors’ response: This is a great point and should be included in the text. Subsequently, we added the following statement to our manuscript in lines 446-448 of the Discussion:

“However, the results also show that 20% of the respondents who are aware of PFAS exposure in their drinking water also described themselves as having limited knowledge of PFAS. Therefore, it is important to emphasize education efforts in areas where community exposure is high”.

5. In the discussion, could the author add several sentences about how to increase the public awareness about PFAS contamination in drinking water, especially in higher exposure area?

Authors’ response: This is not within the scope of this exploratory study. However, our intention is for this manuscript to inform future research studies focused on increasing awareness of PFAS exposure.

6. There are some typos, such as line 339 “that”.

Authors’ response: Thank you for pointing this out. We have corrected grammar and typos where needed.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Linglin Xie

26 Oct 2023

Let's talk about PFAS: Inconsistent public awareness about PFAS and its sources in the United States

PONE-D-23-21851R1

Dear Dr. deVilleneuve,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Linglin Xie

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: All the comments have been addressed with explanation and edition. There is no further questions discovered from the manuscript.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Linglin Xie

7 Nov 2023

PONE-D-23-21851R1

Let’s talk about PFAS: Inconsistent public awareness about PFAS and its sources in the United States

Dear Dr. deVilleneuve:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Linglin Xie

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Unadjusted demographic characteristics of all survey respondents.

    (PDF)

    S2 Table. Questions and response options used in survey instrument.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All raw data files are available from the Zenodo database. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.8132987.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES