Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2023 Nov 20;18(11):e0294741. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294741

Socioeconomic disparities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Sweden: An intersectional ecological niches analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (IEN-AIHDA)

Christoffer Hornborg 1,2,3,*, Rebecca Axrud 2, Raquel Pérez Vicente 2, Juan Merlo 2,4
Editor: Lakshit Jain5
PMCID: PMC10659213  PMID: 37983221

Abstract

We aimed (i) to gain a better understanding of the demographic and socioeconomical distribution of ADHD risk in Sweden; and (ii) to contribute to the critical discussion on medicalization, i.e., the tendency to define and treat behavioural and social problems as medical entities. For this purpose, we analysed the risk of suffering from ADHD in the whole Swedish population aged between 5 and 60 years, across 96 different strata defined by combining categories of gender, age, income, and country of birth. The stratified analysis evidenced considerable risk heterogeneity, with prevalence values ranging from 0.03% in high income immigrant women aged 50–59, to 6.18% in middle income immigrant boys aged 10–14. Our study questions the established idea that behavioural difficulties conceptualized as ADHD should be primarily perceived as a neurological abnormality. Rather, our findings suggest that there is a strong sociological component behind how some individuals become impaired and subject to medicalization.

Introduction

In August 2022, The Moderate Party of Sweden proposed that children in vulnerable suburbs around the capital Stockholm should be offered rapid tests for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as a preventive measure against exclusion and gang crime [1]. Without questioning the intentions of the proposal, there is an all the more important observation to make. Rather than addressing the social fabric surrounding children that grow up under adverse psychosocial circumstances, the proposal illustrated how the discourse of ‘neuropsychiatry’ has become more and more hegemonic in how to make sense of human behavior and social problems.

According to meta-analyses, the number of ADHD diagnoses has increased considerably all over the world [2]. Figs 1 and 2 summarizes data from the Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare [3], illustrating that from 2001 to 2018, the prevalence of ADHD diagnoses in open specialized care increased 36 times in boys and 112 times in girls between 5 and 19 years of age. This massive increase in the number of ADHD diagnoses during the last two decades is a notable epidemiological phenomenon [4]. Arguably, the threshold of who are considered to meet the criteria for a medical disorder has become lower, where parents, schools, and society in general seem to have become more inclined to use ADHD as an explanatory model when children display problematic or undesirable behaviour. In the dominant discourse on ADHD symptoms, the general emphasis is put on an underlying biological disturbance [5]. This is despite the fact that a tangible number of children who receive an ADHD diagnosis are subject to social disadvantage and stressful environments that may impact their development and well-being [6].

Fig 1. Prevalence (%) of ADHD diagnosis in the population 5 to 60 year-old by sex and calender year.

Fig 1

Fig 2. Prevalence (%) of ADHD diagnosis in the population 5 to 19 year-old by sex and calender year.

Fig 2

The ontology of ADHD

Historically, the debate about ADHD has been rather polarized. In psychiatry and the natural sciences, ADHD has been conceptualized as a universally occurring medical condition that exists regardless of societal conditions and cultural settings [7]. Within this paradigm, researchers have sought to investigate neurobiological components associated with possible functional [8, 9], structural [10, 11], and neurotransmitter [1214] alterations in various regions of the brain. Accordingly, ADHD has been treated as a natural kind, i.e., as a universal entity that exists independently of human systems of classification [15]. On the other hand, sociologists [16] and critical psychiatrists [17] have emphasised how societal and cultural processes have led to an expanded use of the diagnosis, and the scientific integrity of ADHD has also been questioned [18]. Others [19] have nuanced the tension by emphasizing how considering a diagnosis as a societal and cultural process, does not suggest that the diagnosis is not real in a specific context. Furthermore, the controversies around ADHD have been addressed as a problem of reification, i.e., the belief that if something is given a name, we have acquired an explanation [20]. Te Meerman and coworkers thus reminds us that “The descriptive classification Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is often mistaken for a disease entity that explains the causes of inattentive and hyperactive behaviors, rather than merely describing the existence of such behaviors”. Accordingly, there is a risk that not only scientists, but also laymen and politicians tend to regard ADHD as an entity, rather than a mere name for observed behaviors. Such a point of departure does not diminish the suffering and seriousness of the symptoms, but when it comes to scientific understanding, problems of reification can illustrate the fallacies that systematically run through psychiatric thinking. As an example, in an analysis of the increased prescription of pharmacological treatment of ADHD, The Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare [21] stated that the proportion of people receiving a diagnosis eventually will be reflected in how many that actually have ADHD in the population, and when that happens, the curve for new users of medications will flatten out. The fallacy in this sort of thinking is that the diagnosis is conceptualized as a thing one simply has or not. There is a risk that an excessively narrow perspective on epidemiology will end up in a discussion about whether ADHD is over- or underdiagnosed. Such a focus assumes that there exists an objective prevalence, but since most traits and behaviours, including those applied to define ADHD, are continuously distributed within the population, the prevalence is ultimately dependent on social consensus regarding the boundaries of what fits into the diagnosis and what does not. In other words, there is no ‘natural’ prevalence to be found, regardless of scientific rigour. ADHD as a category is so heterogenous, multi-factorial, and even ambiguous [22], that it could implicate an all too broad spectrum of idiopathic symptoms of suffering, depending on the intentions and bias of its advocates.

According to meta-analyses [23, 24], there has been no evidence to suggest an increase in the number of individuals who meet criteria for ADHD in the past three decades. Within the general Swedish population, there are no indications that ‘ADHD traits’ have increased, despite the obvious rise in the number of diagnoses. This discrepancy of course raises questions. Has society become better in discovering more cases of a valid disease entity, among individuals who have previously been undiagnosed? Or has it changed the view on what should be considered a disorder [25, 26]? Psychiatric diagnoses are flexible objects that arise, evolve, and even disappear, in line with historical processes [27, 28]. Something that started out as a label for hyperactive children, nowadays serve as a hypothetical cause for a tremendously broad range of behavioral and cognitive features, in both children and adults: difficulties with concentration, aggression, carelessness, burn-out syndrome, addiction, apathy, impulsivity, antisocial behavior, and so on–basically a majority of psychological attributes associated with impaired functioning. To treat ADHD as a more or less biological disease entity causing an impairment, can be a logical fallacy of reification, prohibiting a more nuanced understanding of human functioning. Therefore, it is important to map the contextual factors that are associated with individuals that become diagnosed. This is where sociological and philosophical perspectives have the potential to enrich the understanding of psychiatric epidemics such as ADHD [16, 27, 2931].

Ecological niches as a conceptual framework

Rather than seeking to establish the objective prevalence of ADHD, Hornborg and Merlo [25] have advocated that the focus should be to identify the way diagnoses and prevalence are moulded by how societal, economic, and ideological currents change the assumptions, conceptual apparatus and explanatory models of clinicians and researchers. What type of social and cultural forces may have increased the prevalence and in which settings do ADHD occur? One way of understanding social and psychiatric epidemiology on the basis of these issues is by applying the philosophy of Ian Hacking [27]. Instead of perceiving controversial diagnoses as either medically valid or merely socially constructed, Hacking sought to understand the contextual factors that allowed the diagnosis to arise and thrive over time and space. To do this he used the concept of ecological niche, a metaphor derived from biology. Just as biological organisms only will survive in a niche with the right conditions, the same applies to certain types of diseases. For example, the diagnosis of dyslexia would not flourish in a society without a written language. It is possible to argue that the symptoms of ADHD have always existed, but the idea of an ecological niche helps to illuminate in which conditions they become classified as a medical diagnosis. Hacking’s metaphor is epidemiologically relevant since it acknowledges both a disorder (analogous to the organism) and its context (defined by the factors that forms the ecological niche in which the organism thrives), and it can be particularly useful in social epidemiology. In drastic epidemiological changes, it is of great importance for research to study the contextual forces that underly the act of diagnosing. This can be done by investigating vehicles on a macro level, such as the influence of the pharmaceutical industry, an increased biological focus within psychiatry, transitions from ICD to DSM and the role of the Internet [32], i.e., factors that affect the entire way research is conceptualized, as well as the general perception of laymen and media. But it can also be done by examining how different micro niches, domains within society, is affected differently by the changing perceptions of what constitutes a disorder, acknowledging how the act of turning human behaviour into disease always takes place within a particular context. If the national ADHD prevalence has a considerable heterogeneity in the distribution of diagnoses, this could be framed as an existence of smaller ecological niches in which demographical and socioeconomic factors condition the setting where diagnoses occur. This approach is analogous to the traditional study of inequalities in health where socioeconomic differences are investigated with an intersectional approach [33].

Intersectional analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (AIHDA)

Intersectionality was originally a theory that mainly applied to qualitative methodology. However, an intersectional quantitative approach is being increasingly utilized in epidemiology and public health [3337] as it provides an ideal framework for understanding the demographic and socioeconomic heterogeneity in diagnoses existing within a community. The Oxford English Dictionary 2015 defines intersectionality as “The interconnected nature of social categorizations such as race, class, and gender, regarded as creating overlapping and interdependent systems of discrimination or disadvantage”. Thereby, the demographic, and socioeconomic dimensions that condition health and disease risk need to be considered as interlocked rather than as unidimensional. By using intersectionality, numerous intersectional groups (i.e., strata) can be defined by a combination of several demographic and socioeconomic dimensions (e.g., age, gender, income, and country of birth). In this way, the intersectional perspective provides an improved mapping of disadvantage that better illustrates the heterogeneous socioeconomic distribution of health in the community. Such intersectional strata could be conceptualized as micro niches that facilitate or hinder the construction of ADHD diagnoses by providing different thresholds for dichotomizing traits and behaviours. For instance, many of the questions included in self-assessment forms for ADHD such as ASRS-v1.1 [38] can be interpreted differently in different intersectional contexts. Also, schoolteachers and legal guardians from different niches may have different expectations that condition the evaluation of the symptomatology [3941]. The influence of intersectional niches could be perceived as an addition of influences from separate social dimensions or as an interaction where the propensity of receiving an ADHD diagnosis is larger or lower than the sum of the influences of separate social dimensions.

The present study develops an intersectional perspective into an intersectional ecological niche (IEN) framework. The IEN framework benefits from intersectionality by allowing an improved use of interlocked socioeconomic dimensions, but simultaneously allows the considerations of whatever other relevant ecological factors that would not traditionally be included in a formal intersectional analysis. Thereby, the IE framework gains flexibility when it comes to investigating overlapping and interdependent systems of discrimination or disadvantage. Utilizing the concept of ecological niches is a way of counteracting essentialist narratives and developing a more constructionist framework on how disorders such as ADHD flourish due to contextual factors. Instead of conceptualizing ADHD as a universally occurring entity, it is approached as something that emerges more or less in a given context. Accordingly, the IEN-AIHDA framework that we propose may be a way of addressing the emergence of mental illnesses both in societies and in individuals [cf. 42].

By applying a methodology denominated multilevel analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (MAIHDA) [36], the individual is considered as nested within intersectional ecological niches. Such a methodology can be applied by using both formal multilevel regression [43, 44] as well as traditional regression analyses [33]. MAIHDA provides a detailed mapping of the risk of ADHD diagnosis in the population and allows an evaluation to which degree micro niches are with accuracy able to discriminate individuals with ADHD from individuals without ADHD [36, 45]. Traditional methodologies assign the same average value (i.e., probability) to all the members in the group (i.e., intersectional niche) without considering the individual heterogeneity around the group average and the overlapping between categories [46]. If the discriminatory accuracy is low, the benefit of focusing on specific intersectional niches for understanding differences in ADHD diagnosis can be discussed.

Aim

The aim of this study is twofold. Firstly, we want to obtain a better understanding of the demographic and socioeconomic distribution of ADHD diagnosis in the Swedish population. We hypothesised that different intersectional niches (i.e., contexts, strata) may promote or hinder the risk of getting an ADHD diagnose in Sweden. That is, the same individual would receive or not receive the diagnosis if he/she was exposed to a different niche. If this is true, differences between the intersectional niches would be high. Secondly, we want to contribute to the ongoing critical discussion on medicalization, i.e., the tendency to increasingly define and treat behavioural and social problems as medical entities. Our analysis of intersectional niches is therefore grounded in a nominalist point of departure, highlighting the importance of critically examining current discourses on disease categories such as ADHD.

  • How is the prevalence of ADHD diagnoses distributed across intersectional niches defined by gender, age, country of birth, and income?

  • What is the discriminatory accuracy of the intersectional niches for classifying individuals regarding the existence of an ADHD diagnosis?

  • In what way can the findings contribute to the ongoing discussion about ADHD as a historically situated diagnostic trend of epidemic proportions?

Methods

Databases

We used a database composed of three linked registers covering the whole country of Sweden: The Register of the Total Population (TPR) [47], the Longitudinal Integration Database for Health Insurance and Labour Market Studies (LISA) administered by Statistics Sweden [48], and The National Patient Register (NPR) [49] administered by the National Board of Health and Welfare [50]. We performed a record linkage using the unique 12-digit social security number (SSN) assigned to each individual residing in Sweden. In order to pseudonymize the research database, Statistics Sweden in coordination with The National Board of Health and Welfare replaced the SSN with an arbitrary serial number before delivering files. The information is registered without formal individual consent. However, while the data is confidential, the Swedish authorities delivers pseudonymize information for research purposes after evaluation.

The TPR [47] registers data on all Swedish residents regarding socioeconomic information about gender, civil status, migration, country of birth and family relationship as well as general demographics for those who stayed in Sweden for at least 12 months. The LISA database is administered by Statistics Sweden [48] and combines information on all Swedish residents above the age of 15 from several registers, including the labour market, social sectors, and educational sectors. It includes status on living conditions, demographics, and socioeconomic factors, like income, education, and employment. The NPR [50, 51] records more than 99% of inpatient and approximately 80% of specialized outpatient diagnoses from hospitals in Sweden. At the time of the study, all diagnoses were coded according to the International Classification of Diseases, 10th version (ICD-10). The NPR collects data from both private and public health care providers with an 85–95% positive predictive value on inpatient diagnosis [49]. Data from primary care is not included in the NPR.

Study population

According to the TPR, 9.420.128 individuals resided in Sweden at the baseline date of December 31st, 2010 (Fig 3). When comparing data with the Death Register, we excluded 5.630 individuals that had passed away but with delayed registration, making the correct number of individuals 9.414.498. Based on the TPR, 444.596 individuals that had stayed in Sweden for less than 5 years before baseline were excluded, since recently arrived immigrants might have incomplete register information. Finally, we excluded 2.882.109 individuals older than 60 or younger than 5 years, leaving the database containing information on 6.093.423 individuals.

Fig 3. Flow chart informing on inclusion and exclusion criteria, as well as number of individuals included in the study population.

Fig 3

Assessment of the outcome variable

The outcome variables used were presence or absence of ADHD diagnosis between 2005 and 2010. We defined ADHD diagnosis as any variant of International Classification of Diseases, 10th version (ICD-10) codes F90.0-F90.8.

Assessment of demographic and socioeconomic variables

Gender was binary coded as either man or woman according to legal status. We categorized age at the baseline date into eight intervals: 5–9, 10–14, 15–19, 20–24, 25–29, 30–39, 40–49 and 50–59 years of age. It is known that an ADHD diagnosis is more common in boys than girls [5257] and studies have also concluded that it is more common amongst 10–14-year-old children [58, 59]. Furthermore, we used information from Statistics Sweden to classify country of birth into a binary variable. We labelled individuals born in Sweden as ‘natives’ and individuals born outside of Sweden as ‘immigrants’.

In order to achieve accurate measures of the individuals’ income, the income variable was created using the individualized cumulative disposable income of the household. The disposable income is the income that remains after taxes has been accounted for. We computed a variable for the years 2000, 2005, and 2010, and divided the total disposable income of a family by the number of individuals in that family. The different consumption weights of adults and children of different ages were used according to a specific formula [60]. We sorted groups by 25th quantiles using the TPR in the years 2000, 2005, and 2010. Thereafter, were added the scores from the three years ranging from 1 to 25 and obtained values from three (lowest cumulative income) to 75 (highest cumulative income). Finally, we categorized the cumulative income into three groups (high, middle, and low) by tertiles. The 1002 individuals with missing values of income during 2000 or 2005 were assigned the tertile values of the year 2010. No individuals in our study population had missing income data in that particular year.

Assessment of intersectional niches

As a way of including the intersectional niches, we created a multi-categorical variable consisting of 96 strata. The strata were created by combining the two categories of gender, the eight categories of age, the two categories of country of birth and the three categories of cumulative income. We used a reference stratum constructed of 10-14-year-old women born in Sweden with high income for comparison. This choice was somewhat arbitrary but based on previous studies indicating the characteristics of the individuals with the lowest ADHD risk.

Statistical analysis

The first step in our analysis was to map the prevalence (i.e., absolute risk) of ADHD across the intersectional niches by graphical stratified analyses. Thereafter, we modelled the binary ADHD variable as the dependent variable. We applied Cox proportional hazard regressions with a constant follow-up time equal to one. In this way, we formally attained prevalence ratios (PR) or relative risks [61].

We performed six different models. Model 1 included only age, model 2 only gender, model 3 only income, and model 4 only country of birth. In model 5 all four single variables were entered simultaneously. Finally, in model 6, we included the same information as in model 5 but with the multi-categorical variable with the stratum of 10–14-year-old native women with high income as reference. In all models we calculated PR with 99% instead of 95% confidence intervals (CI) to counteract the multiple comparisons problem.

For each model, we quantified its discriminatory accuracy (DA) by means of the area under the receiver operator characteristics curve (AUC) [45]. The AUC is calculated by plotting the true positive fraction (i.e., sensitivity) against the false positive fraction (i.e., 1 –specificity) for different binary classification thresholds of the predicted probability of ADHD diagnosis. Thus, the AUC measures the accuracy of the information provided by the variables in the model used to discriminate individuals with ADHD from those without ADHD. The AUC obtains a value between 0.5 and 1, where 1 indicates perfect discrimination and 0.5 means that the studied variables have no discriminatory accuracy at all. Rather than only evaluating the differences between absolute risks of strata values, the DA also assessed the overlapping of the individual risk predictions (based on the intersectional niches) between individuals with and without ADHD. There is no fully established practical guideline for the interpretation of the size of the AUC as a measure of discriminatory accuracy when analysing IEN inequalities. However, based on the classification provided by Hosmer and Lemeshow [62] we defined the discriminatory accuracy as (i) ‘absent or very low’ (AUC = 0.5–0.6) corresponding with ‘absent or very low’ strata inequalities, (ii)‘poor’ (AUC >0.6–≤ 0.7), corresponding with ‘small’ strata inequalities, (iii) ‘acceptable’ (AUC >0.7– ≤ 0.8), corresponding with ‘large’ strata inequalities, (iv) ‘excellent’ (AUC >0.8–≤ 0.9) or (v) ‘outstanding’ (AUC > 0.9–1) corresponding with ‘very large’ strata inequalities.

We further calculated the gradual change in the AUC value (Δ-AUC) between the models. The Δ-AUC quantifies the improvement in the discriminatory accuracy obtained by a model, in relation to a reference model [63]. The categorical intersectional variable in model 6 allows us to capture possible interaction of effects between the variables that define the strata so if any interaction exists, the discriminatory accuracy of model 6 in comparison with model 5 will increase and the Δ-AUC be positive [33].

All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) version 24 and Stata v15 (StataCorp, College Station, TX).

Results

Out of all 6.093.423 individuals registered in our database, 54.181 (0.9%) were diagnosed with ADHD during the study period. Table 1 shows that the prevalence of ADHD diagnoses increases with age from 0.72% among 5–9-year-old children until the age of 15–19 when it reaches its maximum of 2.32%, and thereafter decreases with age reaching its lowest value of 0.2% among people 50–59 years old. Overall, ADHD is more frequent in men and in native Swedes than in women and immigrants. Also, there is a clear income gradient with the highest prevalence in the low-income group. Figs 4 and 5 state the prevalence in the different strata using two different approaches, in order to improve our understanding of the underlying heterogeneity in the distribution of ADHD diagnoses across different IE niches. The principal finding is that such heterogeneity is considerable. When observing the stratified analysis of the 96 groups, the risk of ADHD is higher in most immigrant strata than in native strata, especially in males. However, Table 2, which does not contain stratified information, indicates that immigrants have a lower ADHD risk than natives. This could illustrate the so-called Simpsons paradox [64], which describes that when combining strata, a trend can disappear, or show the opposite outcome in comparison to unidimensional analysis.

Table 1. Number of individuals (N), number of individuals with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (n) and prevalence of ADHD in Sweden between 2005 and 2010.

  n N Prevalence (%)
Sweden 54 181 6 093 423 0.89
Age (years) 5–9 3502 485 595 0.72
10–14 9742 460 644 2.11
15–19 13694 589 363 2.32
20–24 8279 581 506 1.42
25–29 4594 502 999 0.91
30–39 6804 1 102 084 0.62
40–49 5342 1 239 052 0.43
50–59 2224 1 132 180 0.20
Gender Female 18556 2 991 959 0.62
Male 35625 3 101 464 1.15
Country of birth Immigrant 3709 727 362 0.51
Native 50472 5 366 061 0.94
Income Low 33299 2 101 824 1.58
Middle 16371 2 218 311 0.74
High 4511 1 773 288 0.25

Fig 4. Prevalence (absolute risk) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) by gender, age, country of birth and low (L), middle (M) and high (H) income in the study cohort.

Fig 4

Fig 5. Prevalence (absolute risk) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), black circles across the 96 intersectional strata defined by gender, country of birth, age and low (L), middle (M) and high (H) income.

Fig 5

The lines crossing the circles are 99% confidence intervals. The lines between the strata show the income gradient across each age interval.

Table 2. Prevalence ratios a.

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6
N (99.0%CI)
Age (years) 5–9 0.34 (0.32–0.36) 0.38 (0.36–0.40)
10–14 Ref Ref
15–19 1.10 (1.06–1.14) 1.03 (1.00–1.07)
20–24 0.67 (0.65–0.70) 0.65 (0.62–0.67)
25–29 0.43 (0.41–0.45) 0.50 (0.48–0.53)
30–39 0.29 (0.28–0.30) 0.44 (0.42–0.46)
40–49 0.20 (0.20–0.21) 0.28 (0.27–0.29)
50–59 0.09 (0.09–0.10) 0.14 (0.13–0.15)
Gender Female Ref Ref
Male 1.85 (1.81–1.90) 1.93 (1.89–1.98)
Income Low 6.23 (5.98–6.49) 4.56 (4.37–4.76)
Middle 2.90 (2.78–3.03) 2.40 (2.30–2.51)
High Ref Ref
Country of birth Swedish Ref Ref
Immigrant 0.54 (0.52–0.57) 0.57 (0.54–0.59)
AUC 0.76 (0.76–0.76) 0.57 (0.57–0.58) 0.67 (0.66–0.67) 0.53 (0.52–0.53) 0.76 (0.76–0.76) 0.77 (0.77–0.77)

aPrevalence Ratios (PR) and 99% confidence intervals (CI) indicating the association between age (Model 1), gender (Model 2), income (Model 3) and country of birth (Model 4) in separate analyses and in analyses including all those variables together as specific variables (Model 5) and as a multicategorical variable with 96 strata (Model 6). The table also informs on the area under the ROC curve and 99% confidence intervals (CI) obtained in models 1 to 6.

The tables and graphs demonstrating the strata or IE niches, present a similar pattern as in Table 1 concerning the prevalence of different age intervals as well as a higher prevalence in men than women. However, according to the strata seen in Figs 4 and 5, the income gradient appears to show a different pattern for immigrants compared to native Swedes. The immigrants with low income show the lowest prevalence amongst the income groups until the age of 24, thereafter the income gradients disappear, and the prevalence appears to be low in all three income groups. In contrast, the native Swedish groups present an income gradient across all age intervals and show a lower prevalence amongst individuals with high income. Observing the native Swedes, the absolute risk of having an ADHD diagnosis amongst young boys (10–19 years old) with a low income compared to the boys in the high-income groups is almost three times higher. The outcome is similarly much higher in immigrant boys when comparing them to native Swedish boys, in the high- and middle-income groups.

Table 2 displays the PR indicating the association of ADHD and age (Model 1), gender (Model 2), income (Model 3) and country of birth (Model 4) in separate analyses. It also displays the variables stated above put together as one specific variable (Model 5), and as multi-categorical variable with 96 strata (Model 6). The results of model 6 are displayed in Table 3 and show the prevalence or absolute risk for the 10 highest and 10 lowest strata out of all 96 that are seen in the supplementary information (S1 Table). These strata are compared to the reference 10-14-year-old women born in Sweden with high income.

Table 3. Number of individuals (N) and prevalence or Absolute Risk (AR) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in the 96 intercategorical strata as well as prevalence ratios (PR), obtained in model 6.

Only the 10 lowest and 10 highest PR are shown.

N AR (99% CI) PR (99% CI)
Female 50–59 High Immigrant 25 934 0.03 (0.01–0.04) 0.09 (0.03–0.22)
Female 50–59 High Swedish 218 100 0.05 (0.04–0.05) 0.15 (0.11–0.21)
Male 50–59 High Immigrant 20 962 0.09 (0.04–0.09) 0.24 (0.13–0.46)
Male 50–59 High Swedish 235 261 0.09 (0.07–0.09) 0.24 (0.18–0.32)
Female 50–59 Middle Immigrant 29 176 0.12 (0.07–0.12) 0.32 (0.20–0.53)
Female 30–39 High Swedish 156 217 0.12 (0.10–0.12) 0.33 (0.25–0.45)
Female 50–59 Low Immigrant 38 058 0.13 (0.08–0.13) 0.35 (0.23–0.54)
Female 40–49 High Swedish 152 524 0.13 (0.11–0.13) 0.36 (0.27–0.48)
Female 5–9 High Swedish 57 851 0.13 (0.10–0.14) 0.37 (0.26–0.54)
Male 40–49 High Swedish 236 364 0.16 (0.14–0.16) 0.44 (0.34–0.57)
Reference: Female 10–14 High Swedish 37 267 0.36 (0.28–0.37) Ref
Male 10–14 Middle Swedish 81 227 2.77 (2.63–2.81) 7.71 (6.13–9.70)
Male 5–9 Middle Immigrant 1 002 2.79 (1.63–3.24) 7.77 (4.55–13.27)
Female 15–19 High Immigrant 1 229 3.17 (2.03–3.60) 8.83 (5.52–14.10)
Male 20–24 High Immigrant 1 318 3.26 (2.13–3.68) 9.07 (5.78–14.25)
Male 15–19 Low Swedish 154 354 3.92 (3.80–3.96) 10.91 (8.71–13.66)
Male 10–14 Low Swedish 105 259 4.22 (4.06–4.27) 11.74 (9.37–14.72)
Male 10–14 High Immigrant 1 449 5.04 (3.67–5.59) 14.01 (9.63–20.38)
Male 15–19 Middle Immigrant 2 397 5.59 (4.45–6.04) 15.55 (11.35–21.30)
Male 15–19 High Immigrant 1 282 5.62 (4.09–6.27) 15.62 (10.72–22.76)
Male 10–14 Middle Immigrant 1 959 6.18 (4.86–6.73) 17.18 (12.44–23.73)

When comparing the 96 strata, the difference in absolute risk, AR, is striking. 10-14-year-old immigrant boys had an AR of 6.18, which is more than 200 times as high as that of 50-59-year-old immigrant women (AR = 0.03). As illustrated in Table 3, the risk was highest among middle- and high-income immigrant boys between the age of 10 and 19, followed by low-income native boys in the same age group. For full information, see S1 and S2 Tables.

The AUC-value, hence the DA, was calculated to 0.76 when all four variables were included in Model 5. Comparing this value with the models including only one variable at time showed no difference in age (Δ-AUC = 0.00; 0.76–0.76) but a difference compared to gender (Δ-AUC = 0.02), income (Δ-AUC = 0.09) and country of birth (Δ-AUC = 0.23). The DA for model 6 was calculated to be 0.77 which indicates a difference and a small interaction effect comparing it to model 5 (Δ-AUC = 0.01). Thus, the variables used in our study present mainly additive effects and only a small interactional effect.

Discussion

Adopting an IEN approach, this study provides a detailed stratified analysis that offers a better understanding of the ADHD ‘epidemic’ in Sweden in 2010. It shows that behind the overall national prevalence there is considerable demographic and socioeconomic heterogeneity in the distribution of ADHD diagnoses in the Swedish population. As expected, the risk of receiving a diagnosis had a peak in younger age groups and was higher amongst males. However, the stratified analysis illustrates how this risk is strikingly higher in young immigrants than in natives, especially in males. It is noteworthy that, overall, natives had a higher absolute risk of ADHD than immigrants, yet when performing the stratified analysis, many of the immigrant strata had a higher prevalence than the native strata. Also, while there is a typical income gradient in native Swedes (with a higher ADHD risk in low-income individuals), a different pattern is observed among immigrants, where the low-income group has lower risk than middle- and high-income groups.

A map of the distribution of ADHD diagnoses across numerous IE strata is useful for identifying contexts with a potential higher risk. However, a disadvantage is that this detailed information may promote the stigmatization of concrete IEN, such as 10–14 years old male immigrants from a middle-income background. An important reminder is therefore to interpret strata information from the viewpoint of discriminatory accuracy, i.e., the capacity of an IEN to classify with accuracy cases and non-cases within the population. The average immigrant teenage boy is obviously not likely to have behavioural problems associated with an ADHD diagnosis. In the same way, a diagnosis may occur in any strata of the population. However, some subjects are at much greater risk, and the intersectional grouping in this study provides a detailed picture of this uneven distribution.

Since observability is a crucial vector in the terminology of Hacking [65], Brossard [42] has proposed that behaviours associated with mental disorders are primarily role disturbances. A diagnosis therefore requires identification of how social roles are disturbed and such an observation is contingent on social networks in the specific context. This might explain why our study illustrates a thorough heterogeneity between different strata. Furthermore, prevalence discrepancies between natives and immigrants within the low-income-group may depend on differences between countries of origin, regarding attitudes and access to psychiatric treatment. In some countries, psychiatric care is not utilized to the same extent, and parents from other regions of the world could be expected to have differing expectations of the health care system. It is possible that psychiatric diagnoses such as ADHD may be considered more stigmatising in some cultures [66], and this could be particularly evident among people with less resources, i.e., low income. In addition, there could be quite some cultural variation in what is considered acceptable or standard childhood behaviour [67]. However, our study uses a very rough classification of country of birth, grouping all immigrants together, even though this is a highly heterogenous category. It is a limitation of this study that it was not possible to analyse how coming from a war afflicted country, trauma, education level, cultural differences, and so on, relates to our results. In future studies, we propose an analysis using a more granulated classification of country of birth. Since the study illustrated a strong overall link between ADHD and socioeconomic/demographic variables, future research could also benefit from expanding the analysis by incorporating other variables in the construction of strata, i.e., geographic area and/or specific care facility. The impact of the clinician can for example be a key factor behind varying prevalence. As commented in the introduction, the IEN-AIHDA framework is more flexible than an analysis formally based on intersectional theory only. An important limitation with the application of IEN-AIHDA in this study is that we were unable to choose the variables of interest a priori, since the information in the registers was limited. Future studies can contribute to the IEAN-AIHDA approach by adding new variables hypothesized to be relevant in the understanding of ADHD risk distribution.

So how should the massive heterogeneity in this study be interpreted in relation to other studies? Previous research has shown that lower family income is associated with stressful events [68], parenting style [69, 70], and family conflict [71]. Regarding ADHD, population-based studies have confirmed that there is a link between an increased risk of receiving a diagnosis and social problems such as childhood adversities and low family income [72, 73]. Such relationships between psychiatric disorders and social factors can be explained by either (i) a social drift hypothesis, i.e., that biologically caused illness increases the risk of drifting into a lower social class and decreases the chances for upward social mobility, or (ii) a social causation hypothesis, i.e., that social disadvantage increases the risk of developing psychiatric disorders [74]. When it comes to these socioeconomic disparities in ADHD, there is a risk that the social drift hypothesis will continue to appeal, since parent traits then can explain both social disadvantage and the child’s symptoms, without compromising the narrative of a neurological entity, characterized by ‘neural abnormalities’ and a strong genetic association [75]. The biological discourse associated with behaviors labeled as ADHD, risks portraying social conditions as a dependent variable rather than a cause of illness, i.e., that individuals to a greater extend end up in socioeconomically unfavorable conditions due to neuropsychiatric characteristics, rather than vice versa. However, our large study, utilizing stratified analysis showed that in the middle and high-income groups, immigrant boys have a much higher risk of ADHD than native boys. These findings support the notion that psychosocial factors should be more highlighted since it is unlikely that children to a greater extent have an immigrant background as a result of neuropsychiatric characteristics (cf. [76]). This opens for a more thorough discussion on how social factors needs to be recognized in the understanding of behaviours and symptoms associated with receiving an ADHD diagnosis [77, 78]. Accordingly, Miller and coworkers [78] have pointed out that if socioeconomic disadvantage causes disorders such as ADHD, explanations and interventions should not only consider individual characteristics. The tendency to address social problems by seeking to detect undiagnosed biological dysfunction among socially disadvantaged youth, as noted in the introduction, is therefor problematic.

While researchers have proposed to move beyond the dichotomy of genes versus environment, and instead develop models based on interplay and epigenetics [79], the aetiologic narrative of ‘ADHD symptoms’ arguably continues to be unbalanced, explaining a wide range of heterogeneous behaviors as a biological disease entity [6, 20, 80]. Te Meerman and coworkers [80], for example, found that academic textbooks were rather biased, overstating the results of twin studies, and not addressing the implications of disappointing molecular research. Such an emphasis on genetical coherence through twin studies tends to disguise the fact that environmental factors can be of great importance even for traits with a very high heritability (i.e., human height, which in Europe increased by 11 centimeters in just 100 years [81]). This is not to say that the different strata in this study provide a more coherent explanation on why individuals become diagnosed. Rather, it is important to acknowledge the complexity of diagnoses entirely based on behavioral problems, and to highlight that some psychiatric categories should be approached with care, as blunt clinical tools. In clinical practise, there is a risk that ADHD comes to serve as an umbrella term for people that suffer and display a wide variety of maladaptive symptoms: A trend that might be accentuated due to a reorientation from attention and hyperactivity, towards a broader clinical discourse on ‘executive difficulties’ or ‘self-regulation’. In line with how medicalization processes operate, this leads to an all too wide range of behavioral difficulties in everyday life residing within the same explanatory category.

It is highly important to consider to which extent ADHD is utilized as an explanation rather than a heterogenous description [20]. In an understanding of the ontology of ADHD, the core question is what it actually means to have ADHD. Is it to have been diagnosed? Is it to be positioned at the outskirts of a bell-shaped curve for a certain trait? Or is it to have difficulties in everyday life, i.e., stated as ‘functional impairment’? And perhaps most importantly, what are the relationships between these parameters? High-functioning adults can display tangible features of chaos, hyperactivity, inattentiveness, and impulsivity in their everyday life, without experiencing much suffering. People who have such traits to a lesser extent (but who also have childhood trauma, financial stress, low status in society, low self-esteem) arguably have a higher risk of fulfilling criteria D–problems with academic, social, and/or occupational functioning–and therefore being diagnosed with ADHD. In the end, a diagnosis is not a result of how much ‘ADHD traits’ a person has, but how well his or her life is working out. This functioning over time, i.e., adaptation to life, is apparently crucially intertwined with social variables. Such an awareness does not dismiss the fact that ADHD, as well as every other psychiatric category, contains a certain biological predisposition. But when it comes to understanding the increasing presence of ADHD in clinical discourse, in academic research, and in media, it is vital to distinguish between people’s behavioral characteristics on the one hand, and why something becomes a disability, on the other. Even if a trait such as activity level might have a substantial genetic basis, this is not the same thing as having a disease [80]. The genetic findings from twin studies may simply display that children at risk of receiving an ADHD diagnosis more often have certain temperament factors. However, this should not be conceptualized as an explanation, since the disorder is ultimately contingent on an environment that is not sufficiently adapted to the developmental needs of the specific individual. Such an interpretation is in line with the findings of this study, where context–intersectional ecological niche–had a large impact on prevalence.

There is an abundance of research that has investigated correlates between subjects diagnosed with ADHD and potential biological abnormalities. Studies have reported an association with inflammatory processes [82], hypothesized a connection to foods and inhalants [83], or highlighted ADHD as part of a broader biological dysfunction in stress-relevant mechanisms [84]. As a nuance to this research paradigm, it is an important reminder that a person’s psychiatric symptoms seldom can be categorized into those for which we can find a biological cause and those for which we can not. This is why the term idiopathic is not even included in the DSM. When trying to imitate somatic care by utilizing assessments and explanatory narratives centered around the body in order to find a biomedical explanation behind behavioral symptoms, it is not only a misunderstanding of the very nature of mental disorders, but also a way of shifting focus and resources from other modes of understanding. In a time when the number of ADHD diagnoses are hitting epidemic proportions, parallel to growing economic gaps, it is important to be sociologically observant of which people that are receiving these diagnoses, and what kind of distress is being subject to ‘psychiatrization’ [85, 86].

Conclusion

Our study analyses a large record linkage and nationwide database, which minimises the risk of selection bias that can be prominent in smaller studies. In summary, using IE-AIHDA we mapped prevalence of ADHD across numerous strata defined by demographics and socioeconomic categories. As far as we know, our study is pioneering in providing such information. We found a large heterogeneity in the risk for an ADHD diagnosis across those IE strata, and there is a need for further studies to understand the reason for these large differences in prevalence. For this purpose, the use of intersectional contexts and thinking of these contexts as ‘ecological niches’ provides a promising approach for illuminating the diagnostic trend as a process that is historically, sociologically, and culturally situated. The large differences between different social groups raises important questions. It is our hope that future studies aim to focus on identifying factors that increases the risk of labelling behavioural and social issues as ADHD. Research could also benefit from addressing contextual interpretation of symptoms and diagnosis, contradictions between different paradigms, and practical applications of the above insights.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Number of individuals (N) and prevalence or Absolute Risk (AR) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in the 96 intercategorical strata as well as prevalence ratios (PR).

Ranked.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Number of individuals (N) and prevalence or Absolute Risk (AR) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in the 96 intercategorical strata as well as prevalence ratios (PR).

Not ranked.

(XLS)

S1 Dataset. Minimal data set.

(DTA)

S1 File. Syntax.

(DO)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Moderaterna. (2022, 12 aug). Förslag: Snabbtest för ADHD i utsatta områden. https://moderaterna.se/stockholm/nyhet/forslag-snabbtest-for-adhd-i-utsatta-omraden/
  • 2.Thomas R., Sanders S., Doust J., Beller E., & Glasziou P. (2015). Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 135(4):e994–1001. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3482 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.National Board of Health and Welfare. (2019). Welfare SNBoHa. Statistical databases. https://www.socialstyrelsen.se/en/statistics-and-data/statistics/statistical-databases/
  • 4.Polyzoi M., Ahnemark E., Medin E., & Ginsberg Y. (2018). Estimated prevalence and incidence of diagnosed ADHD and health care utilization in adults in Sweden—a longitudinal population-based register study. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 14, 1149–61. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S155838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Faraone S. V., Banaschewski T., Coghill D., Zheng Y., Biederman J., Bellgrove M. A., et al. (2021). The World Federation of ADHD international consensus statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews, 128, 789–818. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Erlandsson S., Hornborg C., Sorbring E., & Dauman N. (2022). Is ADHD a way of conceptualizing long-term emotional stress and social disadvantage? Frontiers in Public Health, 10, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.966900 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Barkley R. A. (2002). International consensus statement on ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 5, 89–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Dickstein S. G., Bannon K., Castellanos F. X., & Milham M. P. (2006). The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 47(10), 1051–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01671.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cortese S., Kelly C., Chabernaud C., Proal E., Di Martino A., Milham M.P., et al. (2012). Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies. American journal of psychiatry, 169(10), 1038–55. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11101521 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Valera E. M., Faraone S. V., Murray K. E., & Seidman L. J. (2007). Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological psychiatry, 61(12), 1361–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ellison-Wright I., Ellison-Wright Z., & Bullmore E. (2008). Structural brain change in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder identified by meta-analysis. BMC psychiatry, 8:51. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-51 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Del Campo N., Chamberlain S.R., Sahakian B.J., & Robbins T. W. (2011). The roles of dopamine and noradrenaline in the pathophysiology and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological psychiatry, 69(12), e145–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Economidou D., Theobald D. E., Robbins T. W., Everitt B. J., & Dalley J. W. (2012). Norepinephrine and dopamine modulate impulsivity on the five-choice serial reaction time task through opponent actions in the shell and core sub-regions of the nucleus accumbens. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37(9), 2057–66. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.53 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Maltezos S., Horder J., Coghlan S., Skirrow C., O’Gorman R., Lavender T. J., et al. (2014). Glutamate/glutamine and neuronal integrity in adults with ADHD: a proton MRS study. Transl Psychiatry, 4, e373. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Brinkmann S. (2014). “Natural and human kinds”, in: Teo T. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Critical Psychology. Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 1220–1225. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Conrad P., & Potter D. (2000). From hyperactive children to ADHD adults: observations on the expansion of medical categories. Social Problems, 47(4), 559–582. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Timimi S., & Leo J. (Eds.). (2009). Rethinking ADHD: From brain to culture. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mills S. (2022). The scientific integrity of ADHD: A critical examination of the underpinning theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1062484 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Brinkmann S. (2016). Toward a cultural psychology of mental disorder: The case of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Culture & Psychology, 22(1), 80–93. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Te Meerman S., Freedman J. E., & Batstra L. (2022). ADHD and reification: Four ways a psychiatric construct is portrayed as a disease. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1055328 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.National Board of Health and Welfare. (2021). Förskrivningen av adhd-läkemedel fortsätter att öka. https://www.socialstyrelsen.se/globalassets/sharepoint-dokument/artikelkatalog/ovrigt /2021-6-7436.pdf
  • 22.Honkasilta J. & Koutsoklenis A. (2022). The (un)real existence of ADHD–criteria, functions, and forms of the diagnostic entity. Frontiers in Sociology. doi: 10.3389/fsoc.2022.814763 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Polanczyk G. V., Willcutt E. G., Salum G. A., Kieling C., & Rohde L. A. (2014). ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. International journal of epidemiology, 43(2), 434–42. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt261 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Polanczyk G. V., Salum G. A., Sugaya L. S., Caye A., & Rohde L. A. (2015). Annual research review: A meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 56(3), 345–65. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hornborg C., & Merlo J. (2018). Finns det en ADHD-epidemi i Sverige? Antropologiska och socialepidemiologiska reflektioner. Läkartidningen, 2018, 115. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hornborg C., & Erlandsson S. (2022).”The Ontology of ADHD: Discovery or Invention?”, in Maisel E. (ed.), Deconstructing ADHD: Mental disorder or social construct, 330–353. Cambridge: Ethics International Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hacking I. (1999). The social construction of what? Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Johannisson K. (2006).”Hur skapas en diagnos? Ett historiskt perspektiv”, in: Hallerstedt G. (Ed.), Diagnosens makt. Om kunskap, pengar och lidande, 29–41. Göteborg: Daidalos. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rhodes L. (1990). “Studying biomedicine as a cultural system”, in Johnson T. M. & Sargent C. (Eds.), Medical Anthropology: Contemporary Theory and Method (pp. 159–173). New York: Praeger. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Pérez-Álvarez M. (2017). The four causes of ADHD: Aristotle in the classroom. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 928. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00928 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Nilsson Sjöberg M., & Dahlbeck J. (2018). The inadequacy of ADHD: a philosophical contribution. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 23(1), 97–108. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Conrad P, & Bergey M. R. (2014). The impending globalization of ADHD: notes on the expansion and growth of a medicalized disorder. Social Science & Medicine, 122, 31–43. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.10.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wemrell M., Bennet L., & Merlo J. (2019). Understanding the complexity of socioeconomic disparities in type 2 diabetes risk: a study of 4.3 million people in Sweden. MJ Open Diabetes Research and Care, 97, e000749. doi: 10.1136/bmjdrc-2019-000749 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bauer G. (2014). Incorporating intersectionality theory into population health research methodology: challenges and the potential to advance health equity. Social Science & Medicine, 110, 10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.03.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Evans C. R., Williams D.R., Onnela J.P., & Subramanian S. V. (2017). A multilevel approach to modelling health inequalities at the intersection of multiple social identities. Social Science & Medicine. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Merlo J. (2018). Multilevel analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (MAIHDA) within an intersectional framework. Social Science & Medicine, 203, 74–80. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.12.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Persmark A., Wemrell M., Zettermark S., Leckie G., Subramanian S. V., & Merlo J. (2019). Precision public health: Mapping socioeconomic disparities in opioid dispensations at Swedish pharmacies by Multilevel Analysis of Individual Heterogeneity and Discriminatory Accuracy (MAIHDA). PLoS One, 14(8), e0220322. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0220322 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kessler R., Adler L., Ames M., Demler O., Faraone S., Hiripi E., et al. (2005). The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 245–256. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Harvey E. A., Fischer C., Weieneth J. L., Hurwitz S. D., & Sayer A. G. (2013). Predictors of discrepancies between informants’ ratings of preschool-aged children`s behavior: An examination of ethnicity, child characteristics and family functioning. Early Childhood Research, 28(4), 668–682. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.DuPaul G. J., Reid R., Anastopoulos A. D., Lambert M. C., Watkins M. W., & Power T. J. (2015). Parent and teacher ratings of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms: factors structure and normative data. Psychological Assessment, 28(2) 214–225. doi: 10.1037/pas0000166 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lawson G. M., Nissley-Tisiopinis J., Nahmais A., McConaughy S. H., & Eiraldi R. (2017). Do parent and teacher report of ADHD symptoms in children differ by SES and racial status? Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessments, 39, doi: 10.1007/s10862-017-9591-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Brossard B. (2019). Why mental disorders flourish and wither: Extending the theory of ecological niches. Social Science & Medicine, 237. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Axelsson Fisk S., Mulinari S., Wemrell M., Leckie G., Perez Vicente R., & Merlo J. (2018). Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease in Sweden: An intersectional multilevel analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy. SSM Popul Health, 4, 334–346. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2018.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Evans C. R., & Erickson N. (2019). Intersectionality and depression in adolescence and early adulthood: A MAIHDA analysis of the national longitudinal study of adolescent to adult health, 1995–2008. Social Science & Medicine, 220, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.10.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Pepe M. S., Janes H., Longton G., Leisenring W., & Newcomb P. (2004). Limitations of the odds ratio in gauging the performance of a diagnostic, prognostic, or screening marker. American Journal of Epidemiology, 159(9), 882–90. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwh101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Merlo J., Mulinari S., Wemrell M., Subramanian S. V., & Hedblad B. (2017). The tyranny of the averages and the indiscriminate use of risk factors in public health: The case of coronary heart disease. SSM Popul Health, 3, 684–98. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2017.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ludvigsson J. F., Almqvist C., Edstedt Bonamy A.-K., Ljung R., Michaëlsson K., Neovius M., et al. (2016). Registers of the Swedish total population and their use in medical research. European journal of epidemiology, 31(2), 125–36. doi: 10.1007/s10654-016-0117-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Statistics Sweden (2016a). Longitudinell integrationsdatabas för sjukförsäkrings- och arbetsmarknadsstudier (LISA). Available from: https://www.scb.se/LISA/
  • 49.Ludvigsson J. F., Andersson E., Ekbom A., Feychting M., Kim J.-L., Reuterwall C., et al. (2011). External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC public health, 11:450. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-450 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.National Board of Health and Welfare. (2022). The National Patient Register. https://www.socialstyrelsen.se/statistik-och-data/register/patientregistret/
  • 51.National Board of Health and Welfare. (2009). Kvalitet och innehåll i patientregistret. Utskrivningar från slutenvården 1964–2007 och besök i specialiserad öppenvård (exklusive primårvårdsbesök) 1997–2007. https://www.socialstyrelsen.se/globalassets/sharepoint-dokument/artikelkatalog/statistik/2009-125-15_200912515_rev2.pdf
  • 52.Scahill L., Schwab-Stone M., Merikangas K. R., Leckman J. F., Zhang H., & Kasl S. (1999). Psychosocial and clinical correlates of ADHD in a community sample of school-age children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(8), 976–84. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199908000-00013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Anselmi L., Menezes A. M. B., Barros F.C., Hallal P. C., Araújo C.L., Domingues M.R., et al. (2010). Early determinants of attention and hyperactivity problems in adolescents: the 11-year follow-up of the 1993 Pelotas (Brazil) birth cohort study. Cadernos de saude publica, 26(10), 1954–62. doi: 10.1590/s0102-311x2010001000012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Getahun D., Jacobsen S. J., Fassett M. J., Chen W., Demissie K., & Rhoads G. G. (2013). Recent trends in childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA pediatrics, 167(3), 282–8. doi: 10.1001/2013.jamapediatrics.401 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Reyes N., Baumgardner D. J., Simmons D. H., & Buckingham W. (2013). The potential for sociocultural factors in the diagnosis of ADHD in children. WMJ: official publication of the State Medical Society of Wisconsin, 112(1), 13–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Russell A. E., Ford T., & Russell G. (2015). Socioeconomic Associations with ADHD: Findings from a Mediation Analysis. PLoS One, 10(6), e0128248-e. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128248 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Sun S., Kuja-Halkola R., Faraone S. V., D’Onofrio B. M., Dalsgaard S., Chang Z., et al. (2019). Association of psychiatric comorbidity with the risk of premature death among children and adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. JAMA psychiatry. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.1944 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lindemann C., Langner I., Kraut A. A., Banaschewski T., Schad-Hansjosten T., Petermann U., et al. (2012). Age-specific prevalence, incidence of new diagnoses, and drug treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Germany. Journal of child and adolescent psychopharmacology, 22(4), 307–14. doi: 10.1089/cap.2011.0064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Vasiliadis H.-M., Diallo F. B., Rochette L., Smith M., Langille D., Lin E., et al. (2017). Temporal Trends in the Prevalence and Incidence of Diagnosed ADHD in Children and Young Adults between 1999 and 2012 in Canada: A Data Linkage Study. Canadian journal of psychiatry / Revue canadienne de psychiatrie, 62(12), 818–26. doi: 10.1177/0706743717714468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Statistics Sweden (2016b). Att jämföra inkomster för hushåll. Available from: https://www.scb.se/hitta-statistik/artiklar/2016/Att-jamfora-inkomster-for-hushall/
  • 61.Barros A. J. D, & Hirakata V. N. (2003). Alternatives for logistic regression in cross-sectional studies: an empirical comparison of models that directly estimate the prevalence ratio. BMC medical research methodology, 3, 21. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-3-21 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hosmer D.W., & Lemeshow S. (2000). Applied logistic regression [2nd ed]. New York: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Wemrell M., Mulinari S., & Merlo J. (2017). Intersectionality and risk for ischemic heart disease in Sweden: Categorical and anti-categorical approaches. Social Science & Medicine, 177, 213–22. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.01.050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Wagner C. H. (1982). Simpson’s Paradox in Real Life. The American Statistician, 36(1), 46–48. doi: 10.2307/2684093 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Hacking I. (1998). Mad Travellers: Reflections on the Reality of Transient Mental Illnesses. London: Free Association Books. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Johnsdotter S. (2008).”Kulturella aspekter av psykisk ohälsa”, in: Östman M. (Ed.), Migration och psykisk ohälsa, 123–164. Malmö: Malmö högskola. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Due C., Riggs D., & Augoustinos M. (2014). Research with children of migrant and refugee backgrounds: A review of child-centered research methods. Child Indicators Research, 7(1), 209–227. doi: 10.1007/s12187-013-9214-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Chen E., Martin A. D., & Matthews K. A. (2007). Trajectories of socioeconomic status across children’s lifetime predict health. Pediatrics, 120(2), e297–303. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Costello E. J., Compton S. N., Keeler G., & Angold A. (2003). Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: a natural experiment. Jama, 290(15), 2023–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Rutter M. (2003). Poverty and child mental health: natural experiments and social causation. Jama, 290(15), 2063–4. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Bradley R. H., & Corwyn R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual review of psychology, 53, 371–99. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Björkenstam E., Björkenstam C., Jablonska B., & Kosidou K. (2018). Cumulative exposure to childhood adversity, and treated attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a cohort study of 543 650 adolescents and young adults in Sweden. Psychological medicine, 48(3), 498–507. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001933 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Zhang N., Gao M., Yu J., Zhang Q., Wang W., Zhou C., et al. (2022). Understanding the association between adverse childhood experiences and subsequent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Brain and Behavior, 12, e32748. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2748 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Topor A., & Ljungqvist I. (2017). Money, social relationships and the sense of self: the consequences of an improved financial situation for persons suffering from serious mental illness. Community Mental Health Journal, 53, 823–31. doi: 10.1007/s10597-017-0146-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Yadav S.K., Bhat A.A., Hashem S., Nisar S., Kamal M., Syed N., et al. (2021). Genetic variations influence brain changes in patients with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Transl Psychiatry 11, 349. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01473-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Lehti V., Chudal R., Suominen A., Gissler M., Sourander A. (2016). Association between immigrant background and ADHD: a nationwide population-based case-control study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(8), 967–75. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Larsson H., Sariaslan A., Langström N., D’Onofrio B., & Lichtenstein P. (2014). Family income in early childhood and subsequent attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A quasi-experimental study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(5), 428–435. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Miller L. L., Gustafsson H. C., Tipsord J., Song M., Nousen E., Dieckmann N., et al. (2018). Is the association of ADHD with socio-economic disadvantage explained by child comorbid externalizing problems or parent ADHD? Journal of abnormal child psychology, 46, 951–963. doi: 10.1007/s10802-017-0356-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Nigg J. T., & Craver L. (2014). Commentary: ADHD and social disadvantage: an inconvenient truth? A reflection on Russell et al. (2014) and Larsson et al. (2014). Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 55(5), 446–447. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12237 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Te Meerman S., Batstra L., Hoekstra R., & Grietens H. (2017). Study books on ADHD genetics: balanced or biased? International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-being, 12, 1305590. doi: 10.1080/17482631.2017.1305590 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Hatton T. J. (2014). How have Europeans grown so tall? Oxford Economic Papers, 66(2), 349–372. doi: 10.1093/oep/gpt030 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Anand D., Colpo G. D., Zeni G., Zeni C. P., & Teixeira A. L. (2017). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and inflammation: What does current knowledge tell us? A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8:228. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00228 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Pelsser L. M. J., Buitelaar J. K., & Savelkoul H. F. J. (2009). ADHD as a (non) allergic hypersensitivity disorder: A hypothesis. Pediatr Allergy Immunol, 20, 107–112. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-3038.2008.00749.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Chang J. P., Mondelli V., Satyanarayanan S. K., Chiang Y., Chen H., Su K., et al. (2020). Cortisol, inflammatory biomarkers and neurotrophins in children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Taiwan. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 88, 105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Mills C. (2015). The psychiatrization of poverty: rethinking the mental health-poverty nexus. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(5), 213–222. [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Beeker T., Mills C., Bhugra D., te Meerman S., Thoma S., Heinze M., et al. (2021). Psychiatrization of society: a conceptual framework and call for transdisciplinary research. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 645556. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.645556 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Lakshit Jain

13 Sep 2023

PONE-D-23-22871Socioeconomic disparities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Sweden: An intersectional ecological niches analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (IEN-AIHDA)PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Hornborg,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

 

  • Kindly review the remarks and feedback provided by the reviewers attached

  • Please address the feedback provided by the reviewers that recommend Major concerns first

  • Overall, i enjoyed the context of the study and the research question, and would like to see a revised version 

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Oct 28 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Lakshit Jain, MD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf.

2. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

"Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

3. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

Reviewer #5: Partly

Reviewer #6: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #6: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #6: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #6: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: 1. The study includes whole Sweden population as a sample and that is the biggest strength of this study.

2. The core question in current world still remains, what is the reason for increased diagnosis of ADHD in recent years ? Whether its really more prevalent now compared to previous decades or just were undiagnosed so far? Author has made it clear that aim of this study is different but it can't be separated form the core issue and so this study should have comment on this question too.

Reviewer #2: Very important study on a topic that highly controversial on both sides of the Atlantic! It's a luxury to have access to such huge national database as it can inform a growing trend without having to worry about sampling biases.

The background well written; however, I wonder if there's a way to make it somewhat simpler to follow. It does go into more details which could be presented even more effectively during the discussion part.

I would also comment on the fact that demographic data often look at difference from one perspective- the patient's perspective. It is equally important to consider the impact of the clinician who is making the diagnosis of ADHD. As a clinician practicing in both community and university setting, I come across many individuals with complaints of "ADHD" that have had the diagnosis made by primary care providers (or pediatricians), who might have "met the criteria" if seen by specialists. I don't highlight this point to discuss who is better placed to make this diagnosis, but only to review how clinician access (High/ low social economical status, immigrant/ native populations) could also be a major player behind varying prevalence. If that does seem to be the case, perhaps training could also help bridge this gap. If this information is not readily available, perhaps it could be highlighted as a limitation.

Another factor to consider will be grouping "immigrant" population in one cohort. Immigrant population can vary highly from the standpoint of cultural differences in seeking mental health treatment, level of tolerance in managing these symptoms or to approach them with discipline.

The overriding principle of how the diagnosis of ADHD essentially describes symptoms and groups them to "create" a medical condition, is problematic in my opinion. Looking through the same lens, diagnosis of Major Depressive Disorder might not differ much. For example the same sentence that describes ADHD in the introduction section of the study as “I have concentration difficulties because of my ADHD” is as tautological as saying “I have concentration difficulties because of my concentration difficulties,” could be applied to depression as "I have depression because of my Major Depression" as saying, "I have depression because of my depression." I only highlight this point as such statements may divert readers away from reviewing the study and understanding what it shows.

Reviewer #3: Thank you for the opprotunity to read and review this study. The study presents a new approach called IEN to better understand the distribution of ADHD. The approach involves a detailed analysis that goes beyond the national prevalence rate and reveals a significant variation in ADHD prevalence across demographic and socioeconomic categories. By adopting an intersectional perspective, the study shows that ADHD prevalence is influenced by various factors such as age, gender, immigration status, and socioeconomic standing. The study also emphasizes the importance of cautious interpretation of strata information to avoid perpetuating stigmas.

Despite the valuable insights gained from the stratified analysis, the study acknowledges the potential for stigmatization due to the detailed information.

In addition, the study challenges previous research on the link between family income and ADHD and requires further exploration of potential explanations. Overall, the study's novel methodology, stratified analysis, intersectional perspective, and responsible data interpretation are commendable. However, addressing contextual interpretation, stigmatization concerns, cultural factors, practical applications of theory, and contradictions with existing research would further enhance the study's insights into the complex ADHD distribution landscape. (This was a suggestuion for future work.)

Reviewer #4: The paper presents a comprehensive analysis of ADHD prevalence in Sweden across different demographic and socioeconomic groups. Through a large-scale examination of over six million individuals, the authors unveil significant variations in ADHD risk based on age, gender, income, and country of birth. The paper had two primary aims: understanding the demographic and socioeconomic distribution of ADHD risk in Sweden, and contributing to the ongoing discourse on medicalization, particularly in relation to behavioral and social issues. I have the following comments/questions:

• The results provide valuable epidemiological information about the individuals diagnosed with ADHD. The authors do a great job of achieving aim 1

• Although I agree with sentiment expressed by the authors about considering sociodemographic factors and their effects of ADHD diagnosis, their data does not do them any favors from an explanatory standpoint. The wide paradoxical outcomes reported across different strata do not provide a coherent explanation of how these sociodemographic factors affect the diagnosis. However instead of acknowledging this complexity and accepting that based on only there results they cannot conclusively confirm or refute the claim that increasing prevalence of ADHD can be explained away by sociodemographic factors, it appears that they double down and try and prove a point their data does not support. Thus the discussion section would benefit from such an acknowledgment

Reviewer #5: This is an interesting study that compared the risk of suffering from ADHD across 96 strata in the Swedish population.

Strengths of the study:

-The methodology of the study is strong. Being a population level study that has access to a large database (that covers 99% of inpatient and 80% of outpatient diagnoses) makes the specific results extremely well generalizable for a western country.

-Statistical analysis was also well described and is a valuable addition to the literature.

A few concerns:

-The introduction section would benefit from a rewrite and significant editing. There were multiple long run-on sentences. The same concepts were explained multiple times in different subsections of the introduction (like the discussion about theories from Ian Hacking, these are explained at length in both the 'ecological niches as a conceptual framework' and 'intersectional analysis' sub-sections). All of this could be condensed into one introduction section.

A couple of examples of sentences that need editing:

1. page 2, first paragraph of introduction: "Without questioning the intentions of the proposal- people in these vulnerable areas do have a reduced tendency to seek this type of care - there is an all the more important observation to make." Could be broken down into easier and understandable language.

2. page 5, second paragraph: "Something that started out as a label for hyperactive children, ......"

-Alternatively, I would consider moving a lot of the introduction section into the discussion. Much of the concepts explained in the introduction belong much better in the discussion rather than setting up the study. This also fits in with the stated aims of the authors which include 1. the actual study looking at the distribution of ADHD and 2. a critical discussion on diagnosis and medicalization.

-In the discussion section, the authors highlight the differences between strata and attempt to explain these differences well. The roles of social context and understanding the ontology of ADHD are explained well and contribute to the discussion around this issue.

-However, the authors fail to offer a counter perspective to their arguments. As the authors point out in the introduction section, there have been investigations into function, structural and neurotransmitter alterations in the brains of those with ADHD. While the data may not be comprehensive and overwhelming indicative of one cause, there is enough underlying biological predisposition that cannot be dismissed entirely. While the social content that the authors describe can be potentially responsible for the increase in ADHD prevalence (the so called "epidemic"), this does not dismiss ADHD as an actual neurobiological condition. Rather it just helps us look at the parameters for diagnosis more critically. The clam the authors make around there being no 'natural' prevalence of ADHD that can be found is concerning. While our current tools/parameters for diagnosis are not ideal, this just seems to indicate that decades of scientific research into ADHD is pointless. This perspective is contradicted by clinical wisdom and also by the effectiveness of stimulant medication in treating ADHD symptoms (in the absence of other comorbidities). The authors do present a valuable perspective. However, this should be tempered and cognizant of other literature. The discussion section should be better balanced and appears rather skewed towards one side of this issue.

Overall, a good study, could be a much better manuscript with some work.

Reviewer #6: Please see attached world document for manuscript review edits and comments. There are no ethical concerns for the article. Authors conclude that ADHD's association to sociological component. I congratulate authors for same.

Reviewer #7: Your paper presents some intriguing ideas, but the explanation of the intersectional ecological niche framework requires further clarification. Additionally, the summary could benefit from some concise editing. While discussing your work, individuals acknowledged its strengths, yet they expressed uncertainty regarding its limitations. Providing a more detailed explanation of these limitations is crucial.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: VIMAL SATODIYA

Reviewer #2: Yes: Ankit Parmar, MD

Reviewer #3: Yes: Anil Bachu

Reviewer #4: No

Reviewer #5: No

Reviewer #6: No

Reviewer #7: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Lakshit Jain

17 Oct 2023

PONE-D-23-22871R1Socioeconomic disparities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Sweden: An intersectional ecological niches analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (IEN-AIHDA)PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Hornborg,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

ACADEMIC EDITOR: 

  • The authors have made several changes to the article and have adequately addressed the concerns raised by reviewers that evaluated the previous draft. This is reflected in 3 reviewers recommending that the article be accepted in current form.

  • However, a new reviewer (reviewer 8) has raised additional concerns and has recommended that the article undergo major revisions.

  • Kindly address the concerns raised by reviewer 8 and revise the article as you see fit.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 01 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Lakshit Jain, MD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #5: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #7: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #8: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

Reviewer #8: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

Reviewer #8: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

Reviewer #8: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

Reviewer #8: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: 1. Author has revised the manuscript with considering each reviewer comments and that has made the article better if not the best.

Reviewer #5: I appreciate the authors thoughtful response to my review.

The authors have sufficiently addressed my concerns and have introduced a more nuanced and balanced perspective.

The manuscript is much stronger as a result of the changes. I especially enjoyed reading the new discussion section. One of key points introduced in the revision is that interventions focused on ADHD should not just be limited to individual characteristics and rather focus on social conditions along with it. This is the highlight for this study.

Good job!

Reviewer #7: The authors made appropriate changes in this revision. The research presents very good insight and better understanding about the ADHD and the new methods.

Reviewer #8: 1. I would like to thank you for the opportunity to review this article. The authors have done a wonderful job gathering the data and the sample size is excellent. There are clear graphs that are self explanatory.

2. The articles shows the difference in prevalence rates in different gender, age, country of birth and income groups in Sweden. The low income group in immigrant population has the lowest prevalence whereas middle and high income group immigrant (boys) have much higher prevalence. The native Swedes Low income group have higher prevalence compared to high income group natives. The high and middle income immigrant group showed a totally different trend compared to low income immigrant group which cannot be explained. There is definitely a role of social disadvantage as stress and trauma definitely contribute to increase in prevalence of ADHD diagnosis. But that does not explain why the trend reversed. The author has tried to explain it by saying that it probably depends on people’s attitudes and access to psychiatric treatment. However the article does not explain why the trend reverses with increase of income in immigrant population. Perhaps it will be a good idea to understand where the immigrant population is coming from. Refugees from war afflicted countries with totally different cultures will have chronic stress, trauma and difficulty acculturing to the new country. Also children usually express frustration (due to difficulties with new language and culture) by showing agitation which can be confused as impulsivity and aggression. Perhaps getting information on where the immigrants came from, educational level, perception of mental health in immigrants, cultural differences from countries of origin, trauma and substance use in population will be a valuable information to obtain

SOLUTION: Perhaps the lack of above information can be included in the limitation of study

3. The article explains beautifully that incidence has increased but fails to address why is it so. The current research also takes inflammation into account for explaining increased prevalence. Perhaps the modern diet and modern day stresses in the childhood and prenatal period has something to do with it

SOLUTION: Perhaps the authors can also mention these findings in research paper. I am attaching a few reference articles for author.

References

a. Pelsser LM, Buitelaar JK, Savelkoul HF. ADHD as a (non) allergic hypersensitivity disorder: a hypothesis. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2009 Mar;20(2):107-12. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-3038.2008.00749.x. Epub 2008 Apr 24. PMID: 18444966.

b. Anand D, Colpo GD, Zeni G, Zeni CP and Teixeira AL (2017) Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder And Inflammation: What Does Current Knowledge Tell Us? A Systematic Review. Front. Psychiatry 8:228. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00228

c. Chang JP, etal Cortisol, inflammatory biomarkers and neurotrophins in children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Taiwan. Brain Behav Immun. 2020 Aug;88:105-113. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.05.017. Epub 2020 May 8. PMID: 32418647.

4. The current research also hints toward the role of genetics in ADHD. There are also neuroimaging studies describing the differences in brain structure of ADHD vs non ADHD children. So the author blaming it on medicalization of ADHD and considering it as behavioral and social problem is perhaps too simplistic and narrow of an explanation. ADHD also has neurobiological underpinnings to it.

SOLUTION: Please add it to conclusion section. There is a need for further understanding the genetic and neurological factors in addition to social correlates

References

a. Yadav SK et al. Genetic variations influence brain changes in patients with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Transl Psychiatry. 2021 Jun 5;11(1):349. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01473-w. PMID: 34091591; PMCID: PMC8179928.

b. Stevens HE, Scuderi S, Collica SC, Tomasi S, Horvath TL, Vaccarino FM. Neonatal loss of FGFR2 in astroglial cells affects locomotion, sociability, working memory, and glia-neuron interactions in mice. Transl Psychiatry. 2023 Mar 11;13(1):89. doi: 10.1038/s41398-023-02372-y. PMID: 36906620; PMCID: PMC10008554.

PLEASE CONSIDER ADDRESSING THESE ISSUES.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #5: No

Reviewer #7: No

Reviewer #8: Yes: JASLEEN KAUR MD

CONNECTICUT VALLEY HOSPITAL

MD PSYCHIATRY

FELLOWHIP: PSYCHOSOMATIC AND ADDICTION MEDICINE

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: ADHD REVIEW.docx

Decision Letter 2

Lakshit Jain

8 Nov 2023

Socioeconomic disparities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Sweden: An intersectional ecological niches analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (IEN-AIHDA)

PONE-D-23-22871R2

Dear Dr. Hornborg,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Lakshit Jain, MD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #5: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #7: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #8: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #9: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #10: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

Reviewer #8: Yes

Reviewer #9: Yes

Reviewer #10: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

Reviewer #8: Yes

Reviewer #9: Yes

Reviewer #10: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

Reviewer #8: Yes

Reviewer #9: Yes

Reviewer #10: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #5: Yes

Reviewer #7: Yes

Reviewer #8: Yes

Reviewer #9: Yes

Reviewer #10: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed during 1st revision and I don't have anything else to add or comment now on this article.

Reviewer #5: Authors have addressed all concerns. Manuscript is much stronger as a result. This will make a good addition to the literature.

Reviewer #7: In my opinion, the authors or best efforts in revising the paper based on the given suggestions. From my standpoint, the paper has been improved significantly and is now in a good state to be published. I appreciate your team work.

Reviewer #8: The authors have addressed all my concerns in the revised draft. The article is beautifully written and has a good sample size.

Reviewer #9: Thank you for the opportunity to review this piece on the socioeconomic disparities in ADHD. The authors have addressed the review comments appropriately. This article has clarified all comments including the pending comments from the reviewer satisfactorily. The paper brings a unique perspective to the etiology of ADHD and would add value to the existing literature on the topic. I recommend publishing this article as it is at this time.

Reviewer #10: The research paper aims to understand the socioeconomic disparities in ADHD risk in Sweden. The researchers analyzed the risk of ADHD across varied categories of age, gender, income, and country of birth. The findings challenge the conventional view of ADHD as primarily a neurological abnormality and suggest that there exist significant sociological factors determining how some individuals are more susceptible to ADHD. The argument that ADHD is not solely a neurological abnormality is valid and well substantiated with the results from the study which showed considerable risk heterogeneity across different demographic and socioeconomic categories. In summary, the research paper offers a compelling perspective on ADHD diagnoses, shifting the focus from a purely neurological standpoint to a sociological one.

The author has demonstrated a commendable commitment to addressing the concerns outlined in the previous review of the article. The revisions and improvements made have significantly enhanced the overall quality and clarity of the work. The paper now aligns better with academic standards and guidelines, and the arguments are more robustly supported by evidence and analysis. In particular, the author has successfully clarified points of contention, strengthened the rationale for their claims, and filled gaps in the research, thereby enriching the paper's contribution to the field. Moreover, the incorporation of specific data and empirical evidence, where needed, lends additional credibility to the assertions made in the article. The revisions also reflect a greater depth of understanding of the subject matter, providing readers with a more comprehensive and nuanced perspective. Overall, the author's responsiveness to the feedback is evident in the enhanced quality of the research, making it a more valuable and reliable addition to the academic discourse.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #5: No

Reviewer #7: No

Reviewer #8: Yes: Jasleen Kaur MD

Reviewer #9: No

Reviewer #10: Yes: Aditi Sharma

**********

Acceptance letter

Lakshit Jain

10 Nov 2023

PONE-D-23-22871R2

Socioeconomic disparities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in Sweden: An intersectional ecological niches analysis of individual heterogeneity and discriminatory accuracy (IEN-AIHDA)

Dear Dr. Hornborg:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Lakshit Jain

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Number of individuals (N) and prevalence or Absolute Risk (AR) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in the 96 intercategorical strata as well as prevalence ratios (PR).

    Ranked.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Number of individuals (N) and prevalence or Absolute Risk (AR) of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) in the 96 intercategorical strata as well as prevalence ratios (PR).

    Not ranked.

    (XLS)

    S1 Dataset. Minimal data set.

    (DTA)

    S1 File. Syntax.

    (DO)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ADHD REVIEW.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES