Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2023 Nov 7;14:1250268. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1250268

Brain signatures of chronic gut inflammation

Caitlin V Hall 1,2,3,*, Graham Radford-Smith 3,4,5, Emma Savage 1, Conor Robinson 1, Luca Cocchi 1,3, Rosalyn J Moran 2
PMCID: PMC10661239  PMID: 38025434

Abstract

Gut inflammation is thought to modify brain activity and behaviour via modulation of the gut-brain axis. However, how relapsing and remitting exposure to peripheral inflammation over the natural history of inflammatory bowel disease (IBD) contributes to altered brain dynamics is poorly understood. Here, we used electroencephalography (EEG) to characterise changes in spontaneous spatiotemporal brain states in Crohn’s Disease (CD) (n = 40) and Ulcerative Colitis (UC) (n = 30), compared to healthy individuals (n = 28). We first provide evidence of a significantly perturbed and heterogeneous microbial profile in CD, consistent with previous work showing enduring and long-standing dysbiosis in clinical remission. Results from our brain state assessment show that CD and UC exhibit alterations in the temporal properties of states implicating default-mode network, parietal, and visual regions, reflecting a shift in the predominance from externally to internally-oriented attentional modes. We investigated these dynamics at a finer sub-network resolution, showing a CD-specific and highly selective enhancement of connectivity between the insula and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), regions implicated in cognitive-interoceptive appraisal mechanisms. Alongside overall higher anxiety scores in CD, we also provide preliminary support to suggest that the strength of chronic interoceptive hyper-signalling in the brain co-occurs with disease duration. Together, our results demonstrate that a long-standing diagnosis of CD is, in itself, a key factor in determining the risk of developing altered brain network signatures.

Keywords: gut-brain, EEG, neuroimaging, microbiome, inflammation, IBD

Introduction

Immune dysfunction and accompanying systemic inflammation is thought to play a key role in the development of mood and affective symptoms (1, 2). As part of this mechanism, the presence of pro-inflammatory cytokines is communicated to the central nervous system (CNS) via peripheral activation of receptors expressed on vagal afferents, or the production of molecular intermediates at the blood–brain interface (c.f., circumventricular organs and the choroid plexus) (3). The brain recognises inflammation as a molecular signal of sickness, inducing changes at the neurophysiological and neurotransmitter level within brainstem, limbic and prefrontal regions (3, 4). Together, these neural responses generate a repertoire of “sickness behaviours” that includes social avoidance, anhedonia, fatigue, and depressed mood (1, 5, 6). The brain-cytokine response has been demonstrated in healthy adults administered lipopolysaccharides (LPS) (7) or typhoid vaccination (3, 8), who show transient alterations to cognitive-affective regions (involving the thalamus, amygdala, insula, and anterior cingulate), and a symptom profile that includes anxiety, poor mood, and impaired memory. These effects, however, embody the response of the resilient and adaptive CNS to an acute perturbation. Recent work investigating repeated exposure to immunogenic substances over an extended timeframe suggests more pervasive and enduring brain network abnormalities in chronic inflammation (9, 10).

Inflammatory bowel disease (IBD), a chronic, relapsing, and remitting intestinal disease, provides a unique and ecologically valid model to study the effects of inflammation chronicity on the brain (11). While IBD can occur at any age, disease incidence peaks in early adulthood (between 15 and 30 years) such that individuals experience a number of acute and recurrent inflammatory events that can endure for decades (12). As inflammation emerges within the gastrointestinal (GI) tract, the disease is well-placed to exert influence over the gut-brain axis (13). The gut-brain axis describes the communication pathways that exist between the gut and its microbiota and the brain. These pathways have not been fully elucidated, but are thought to involve complex, bidirectional links through vagal, immunological, and humoral pathways (14). Additionally, microbes produce and regulate gut peptides, hormones, short-chain-fatty acids, neurotransmitters, and inflammatory cytokines, all of which modulate CNS function. Together, these neurophysiological and neurochemical pathways are now recognised to play a role in regulating stress, mood, and anxiety-related behaviours (15). One of the key mechanisms involved in the gut-brain axis in IBS is the presence of inflammation. That is, the physical proximity of inflammation to the intestinal epithelium – a putative gut-brain interface – allows neural-related changes to be conceptualised as dysfunctions to vagal, immune, microbial, or endocrine signalling pathways. Alongside the mechanisms by which inflammation reaches and impacts the brain, an important research endeavour is focused on identifying specific brain regions affected by chronic inflammation, and how this can manifest behaviourally.

Neuroimaging work has provided initial insights into altered functional brain connectivity underpinning IBD pathophysiology, and suggests that a re-organisation of large-scale brain networks, rather than localised deficits, more clearly recapitulates disease-related changes (1620). Specifically, there is a growing consensus that individuals with IBD exhibit alterations to default-mode network (DMN) activity (17, 20). The DMN comprises a set of brain regions – largely involving the posterior cingulate cortex, ventromedial, anteromedial and dorsal prefrontal cortex, temporal pole, precuneus, and middle temporal gyrus – that exhibit coherent neural activity during rest, and deactivation during externally oriented cognitive tasks (21). Alongside its involvement in social, cognitive and affective processes, the network plays a critical role in endogenous thought, such as rumination, and self-referential processing (22, 23). Abnormal patterns of activation and deactivation within the DMN has been linked to the development of neuropsychiatric disorders, including depression (24) and anxiety (25). In IBD, functional connectivity changes have been reported between key regions of the DMN, including the posterior cingulate cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and precuneus regions (17, 20). Aberrant connectivity between nodes of the salience network (SN) (16, 19), including the anterior cingulate and insula cortex, further supports the possibility that IBD individuals experience altered interoceptive processing of visceral sensations (e.g., nociceptive, inflammatory, or microbial-related stimuli) (26). In a recent meta-analysis exploring the association between the human gut microbiota and functional connectivity, the highest level of consistency was found for the DMN, frontoparietal (FPN), and SN [particularly the insula and anterior cingulate cortex (27)]. Given the relationship between the DMN and SN in anxiety and depression, the reported alterations in patients with IBD may be of substantial clinical importance. Critically, these results are reported in quiescent IBD (17, 18, 2830), further supporting the argument that acute inflammation alone cannot account for the observed neural and behavioural impairments (9, 30, 31).

Among the two main IBD diagnoses, brain and behavioural abnormalities have more consistently been reported in Crohn’s Disease (CD) as opposed to Ulcerative Colitis (UC) (16, 17, 19, 20, 28, 29, 32). Despite overlapping symptoms, CD is thought to exhibit a more pervasive and severe disease expression attributed in part to the extent of affected anatomical sites, transmural involvement, and genetic and immune factors involved (33, 34). Moreover, while the microbiome in UC cannot be differentiated from controls following successful treatment, dysbiosis (imbalance) in CD persists long after remission and responds poorly to faecal microbiota transplantation (3537). Despite well-defined heterogeneity between UC and CD – with the latter thought to express a more chronic and systemic disease profile – only a limited number of studies (38, 39) have directly compared IBD sub-groups in the context of whole-brain signatures.

In this study, we investigated whether CD and UC were associated with alterations to spontaneous brain state dynamics. To do this, we fit a Hidden Markov Model (HMM) to resting-state electroencephalography (EEG) data which describes brain dynamics as a sequence of transient and distinct patterns of power and phase-coupling within and between brain regions, respectively. We further explored these brain dynamics at a sub-network resolution, showing differential patterns of effective connectivity that are specific and selective to CD. Our results converge on the suggestion that long-term exposure to chronic gut inflammation confers a higher risk of altered brain and behavioural signatures, with the extent of these effects related to disease duration.

Methods

Participants

The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute (P3436). Written informed consent was obtained for all participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Twenty-eight healthy controls (34 ± 11 years; 16 female), 40 CD (43 ± 13 years; 20 female), and 30 UC (42 ± 11 years; 21 female) participants were recruited from the Brisbane (Australia) metropolitan area by gastroenterologist (GRS) and accredited practicing dietitian (CVH) (Supplementary Table S1). Exclusion criteria are presented in Supplementary Note 1. Study requirements involved (I) general health and clinical questionnaires; (II) neurocognitive assessments; (III) a resting state EEG recording; and (IV) a stool sample collected at home.

General health and clinical questionnaires

A general health (Brisbane Health Area Survey) and diet questionnaire (Traditional Mediterranean Diet Adherence, TMD) was administered to all participants, while an additional clinical questionnaire was administered to individuals with IBD only (Supplementary Note 1).

Neurocognitive assessments

Neurocognitive assessments were performed by a clinical psychologist and accredited practicing dietitian, and were used to rule out previous or current history of a neurological or psychiatric illness (excluding anxiety-related disorders or depression). Assessments of anxiety and depression included the Hamilton and Montgomery Anxiety (HAM-A), Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS), Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (42-item) (DASS), and Generalized Anxiety Disorder (7-item) (GAD-7).

Sample collection and processing

Participants were provided with a stool nucleic acid collection and preservation tube (Norgen Biotek Corp., Thorold, Ontario, Canada) and were instructed to collect the sample within a window of 48 h before/after the study session. Each stool sample was labelled and stored in a -80°C freezer until sample processing. Tissue homogenization was performed using tubes containing 1.4 mm ceramic beads (Precellys Lysing Kit). DNA was extracted from samples and quantitated using Nanodrop 2000 (Thermo Scientific). PCR amplification was performed on the V3–V4 hypervariable region of the 16S rRNA gene, and sequenced on a MiSeq sequencer (Australian Genome Research Facility, Melbourne).

16S data processing and analysis

Demultiplexed fastq files were processed using default settings within QIIME2 2020.21 (40). Amplicon Sequence Variants (ASVs) were generated by denoising with DADA2 (41). For taxonomic structure analysis, taxonomy was assigned to ASVs using a pre-trained Naïve Bayes classifier and the q2-feature-classifier plugin against the Greengenes 13_8 99% 16S rRNA gene sequencing database. Samples were rarefied to a read depth of 2,200 for diversity analyses. ANCOVA was used to test for group differences in Shannon diversity and Chao1 measures accounting for the effects of age, sex, and body mass index (BMI). Beta-diversity, assessed using unweighted UniFrac distance (42), was used to compare groups, controlling for age, sex, and BMI using qiime2 plugins PERMANOVA and adonis. The metagenomic functional contribution of each sample was predicted using the computational modelling approach, Phylogenetic Investigation of Communities by Reconstruction of Unobserved States 2.0 (PICRUSt2 v2.2.0-b) (43), using the MetaCyc Metabolic Pathway Database (44). The multivariate statistical framework, MaAsLin2 (45), implemented in R, was used to assess the relationship between group membership with (i) microbial abundance (collapsed at genus level) and (ii) functional pathway abundance. Covariates, including sex, age and BMI, were included as fixed effects. Features were included in if they had at least 10% non-zero values (across samples) and a minimum relative abundance threshold of 0.0001, both validated parameter settings in MaAsLin2. Significant features were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Benjamini-Hochberg FDR procedure, with corrected values of p < 0.05 and q < 0.25 considered statistically significant.

Resting-state EEG recordings

Participants were fitted with a 64-channel EEG cap (Ant Neuro – EEGgo sports system), configured to the 10–20 international system. Signals were processed online using EEGgo with a sampling frequency of 2,000 Hz. Scalp impedance was reduced to a maximum of 20 kΩ in all electrodes with the application of conductive gel. EEG activity was processed online using eego software. All electrodes were referenced to the CPz electrode. Prior to recordings, participants were reminded to keep their eyes open and fixate on a white crosshair against a black background. Participants were encouraged to breathe and blink normally, and relax head and neck muscles to minimize signal artifacts. Resting-state signals were recorded continuously for 4 min.

EEG pre-processing

EEG data was pre-processed offline using EEGlab software (v2019.1) in MATLAB (vR2018b). The data were downsampled to 250 Hz. EEG signals were visually inspected, and excessively noisy channels were removed before signals were re-referenced to the common average reference (excluding EOG, M1 and M2 electrodes). Signals were band-pass filtered into a frequency band of 1–45 Hz, and epoched into 5 s segments. Epochs were manually inspected and removed if they contained large artifacts that would otherwise not be detected by independent components analysis (ICA) (e.g., strong muscle artifacts). Artifacts that were characteristic of cardiac, ocular or minor muscular movements were subsequently removed using ICA (InfoMax) (46). As the HMM is sensitive to noise, a fairly stringent approach was adopted to remove potential sources of signal artifact. This approach represents a necessary trade-off to ensure that the HMM is inferred on neurobiologically meaningful data and not spurious noise sources (47). As such, if more than 20 ICs were marked as artefactual, the original time series prior to ICA was re-inspected for additional sources of artifact. If more than 50% of epochs were removed, or more than 20 ICs were excluded after the second ICA run, recordings were excluded from the analysis. Recordings from 11 subjects (2 HC, 3 CD, and 6 UC) were not included in the HMM. Subsequent processing and analysis of EEG data were performed using toolboxes and software packages found within the Oxford Centre for Human Brain Activity (OHBA) Software Library (OSL) and SPM12. For source reconstruction, the forward model was generated using a symmetric boundary element method (BEM) and the inverse model was performed using a Linearly Constrained Minimum Variance (LCMV) vector beamformer. A 44-region weighted parcellation of the entire cortex was adapted from previous work (4850). Thirty-eight parcels were constructed from an ICA of fMRI data from the Human Connectome Project, while the remaining six parcels corresponded to the anterior and posterior precuneus, bilateral intraparietal sulci, and bilateral insula cortex. The inclusion of the insula cortex – specific to our analyses – was based on previous work supporting the contribution of this region to interoceptive processing in chronic and inflammatory conditions, including IBD (17, 18, 5153). Time-courses were extracted by taking the first principal component, with voxel contributions weighted by the parcellation. Symmetric multivariate spatial leakage (volume conduction) correction was applied (49).

Time-delay embedded-HMM

We adopted the TDE-HMM implemented within the HMM-MAR MATLAB toolbox2 (47, 48). We used stochastic variational Bayes (48) to infer the TDE-HMM parameterized with 6 states and 41 time lags (corresponding to a window length of 160 ms) (Supplementary Figure S1) using 500 training cycles and initialization parameters according to previously established procedures (48, 5456). Prior to HMM inference, we concatenated time series across subjects from all three groups, producing a full dataset to obtain a common set of brain states across all participants. This approach facilitated a direct comparison of spatial and temporal statistics across groups (56, 57). Supplementary Note 2 provides a full description of the TDE-HMM and Supplementary Figure S2 provides an overview of the analysis pipeline.

From the HMM we calculated the (subject-specific) temporal properties of each state using three parameters: (I) fractional occupancy, the proportion of total time spent in a state (K × 1); (II) interval time, the length of time between consecutive visits to the same state (K × 1); and (III) dwell time, the average length of time spent in a state before transitioning to another state (K × 1). We also computed subject-specific transition probability matrices representing the probabilities of transitioning from one state, to every other state (K × K). ANCOVA was used to test for significant differences in fractional occupancy, dwell times, and interval times between groups, controlling for the effects of age and sex. Permutation testing was used to reject the null hypothesis of equality between groups. As implemented in previous work (57), for each state we generated 5,000 permutations by shuffling group labels among participants. We then repeated ANCOVAs on the permuted values, therefore generating an empirical null distribution of F-statistics for each state and temporal measure (fractional occupancy, dwell times, and interval times). We ascribed statistical significance (p < 0.05) to the temporal values by assessing the proportion of null statistics that were greater than or equal to the value of the statistic computed for the non-permuted data. For significant ANCOVAs, Tukey’s HSD post-hoc paired t-tests were used to identify where differences were expressed between groups. The Network-based Statistic (NBS) (58) was used to perform inference on the transition probability matrices between the three groups, again including age and sex as covariates. We used an F-test with the primary statistic threshold set to 3.0, and performed a total of 5,000 permutations (family-wise error rate controlled at 5%).

Candidate cortical regions

Using state-specific coherence values averaged across subjects, we calculated the eigenvector centrality (EC) measure for each region. EC calculates the centrality (degree) of each node and weights this according to the EC of the nodes that it connects to van den Heuvel and Sporns (59). EC was performed using the eigenvector_centrality_und function within the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (60). The top 10% of EC scores taken from a single hemisphere were used to inform regions for a DCM.

Dynamic causal modelling

We used dynamic causal modelling (DCM) for cross-spectral densities (CSD) to selectively isolate those differences observed in the networks above (61, 62). Specifically, we modelled the extrinsic (between-region) effective connectivity strengths between candidate regions. We adopted the convolution based local field potential (LFP) neural mass model which describes source activity as the result of interactions between populations of inhibitory interneurons, excitatory spiny stellate cells, and excitatory pyramidal cells (63). The data to which the DCM was fit comprised the processed time series. For each subject, we specified and estimated a single model with a fully-connected network of 7 regions. To obtain the most robust estimates, we then re-estimated the DCM using an updated prior parameter space using the posteriors from an exemplar subject (Supplementary Figure S3). For each subject, we selected the iteration with the best fit (as assessed by free energy). One-way MANCOVA (Wilks’ Lambda) was used to assess group differences in the forward and backward connectivity parameters. Univariate tests were corrected for multiple comparisons (pFWE < 0.05, Bonferroni corrected). A multiple regression model was used to assess the contributions of behavioural (non-clinical) variables to effective connectivity strengths.

Results

Resting-state EEG recordings and 16S rRNA profiles were analysed for 40 CD, 30 UC, and 28 healthy participants. Demographic, behavioural, and clinical characteristics are presented in Supplementary Table S1. As IBD is a heterogeneous disease, we observed differences in participant’s medication use, surgical history, and disease duration between UC and CD groups (Supplementary Table S1). As this study was designed as a naturalistic assessment of IBD brain state dynamics, we did not exclude participants taking specific medications or surgical procedures. IBD and healthy control (HC) participants were matched in terms of general demographics with the exception of age, and the Hamilton and Montgomery Anxiety (HAM-A) scores (Supplementary Table S1).

Establishing distinct microbiota signatures in CD and UC

We first used 16S rRNA sequencing to compare microbiota profiles between the three groups. Our results show a significant difference in beta (unweighted UniFrac) and alpha (Shannon effective species and Chao1 index) diversity measures in CD, compared to HC and UC (Figures 1A,B). While not reaching statistical significance, UC showed a trend towards lower alpha diversity and distinct beta diversity profiles compared to HC. Multivariate analyses also revealed a number of significant taxonomic and functional differences in CD and to a lesser extent, in UC (Figures 1C,D, enlarged visualisation shown in Supplementary Figure S4). The microbiota results converge in supporting the existence of a perturbed and heterogeneous microbial profile in CD (36). It is important to note that the small subset of CD participants exhibiting mild (n = 3) or moderate (n = 1, later excluded for poor quality EEG data) disease activity were not outliers in terms of their diversity scores (i.e., were distributed within the normal range for CD). Together, the clinical and microbiota results demonstrate clear distinctions between CD and UC sub-groups, providing a strong motivation to perform brain assessments in each group independently. Full statistical results for this assessment can be found in Supplementary Note 3.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Comparison of microbiota results between Crohn’s Disease (CD), Ulcerative Colitis (UC), and healthy control individuals (HC). Results from (A) beta (unweighted Unifrac) and (B) alpha diversity (Shannon effective species and Chao1 index) measures show significant differences between CD and UC, and CD and HC, assessed using one-way ANCOVAs. Multivariate analyses performed using MaAslin2 revealed significant differences in (C) taxonomic abundance (genus resolution) and (D) functional pathways in CD and to a lesser extent, in UC, when compared to HC. Enlarged figures for (C,D) are presented in Supplementary Figure S4. All microbiota assessments were controlled for the effects of age, sex, and BMI. * denotes p < 0.05; *** denotes p < 0.0005.

Brain states expressed during resting-state EEG

The TDE-HMM posits that a time series can be decomposed into a number of discrete and recurrent hidden brain states, comprising several regions that co-activate together, such that at each time point, only one state is active (64) (Supplementary Note 2). Results showed that resting-state EEG data was best described by six short-lived and recurring brain states, each with unique spatial, spectral, and temporal profiles (Figures 2AF; Supplementary Figure S5; Supplementary Note 2). Our state selection is consistent with previous studies modelling M/EEG dynamics using the TDE-HMM, ranging between six and 16 states (47, 48, 65, 66). The spatial maps of power (i.e., the amount of activity) and coherence networks (i.e., the level of synchronisation or coupling between two regions) were averaged across a wideband frequency range (1–30 Hz). Power maps correspond to the mean power within each region and state (z-scored) and coherence networks show functional connections that are stronger (p < 0.01) compared to all other possible between-region connections for that state. Our spatial maps share characteristics with previous M/EEG HMM studies, including a bilateral pattern of activity for some, but not all states (47, 48), and strong increases in power often accompanying increases in coherence (48).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Brain states identified using Hidden Markov Modelling represent networks of power and spectral coherence. (A-F) Left panel shows wideband (1–30 Hz) power maps (top) and coherence networks (bottom) displayed for each state. Power maps are relative to the state average (z-scored) where blue colours reflect power that is lower than the state average and red/yellow colours reflect power that is higher than the average within that state. Coherence networks show statistically significant (p < 0.01) connections that stand out from a background level of connectivity within that state. Nodes are coloured based on which fMRI association map/s they anatomically correspond to, and the size of each node reflects the centrality (degree) score. (A.I–III) Comparison of temporal statistics between healthy controls (HC), Crohn’s Disease (CD) and Ulcerative Colitis (UC) individuals for each state, after adjusting for age and sex. Fractional occupancy (%) represents the proportion of overall time spent in a state; interval time (ms) represents the length of time between consecutive visits to the same state; and dwell time (ms) is the length of each state visit. Permutation tests were performed to assess the null hypothesis of equality in temporal measures between groups and Tukey’s HSD post-hoc tests were used to identify where significant pair-wise differences were expressed. * denotes pFWE < 0.05; ** denotes pFWE < 0.005.

Brain states correspond to resting-state association maps

We quantified the functional overlap between the HMM states with established resting-state association networks from the meta-analysis database, Neurosynth (67). Specifically, we assessed the spatial overlap (voxel-wise correlation) between our power maps (z-scored, unthresholded) with canonical maps of prefrontal, parietal, sensorimotor, visual, DMN, and temporal fMRI association maps (Supplementary Figure S6). For ease of interpretation, states were named according to the spatial patterns of activation to which they were most strongly correlated. States 1 (prefrontal) and 2 (integrated prefrontal) were defined by higher and lower power in prefrontal and visual regions respectively, with more extensive prefrontal coherence in State 2. States 3 (right sensorimotor-parietal) and 5 (left sensorimotor) were characterised by higher power in right and left sensorimotor regions, respectively, with coherence patterns closely following power in State 3. State 4 (visual) was characterised by high power and coherence in visual regions, while State 6 (DMN-parietal) reflected power and coherence in regions associated with DMN and parietal regions. Each state also exhibits frequency-specific differences in power and coherence, which can be visualized as an average across regions over the full spectrum (1–30 Hz) (Supplementary Figure S5). There is a strong distinction between the DMN-parietal, characterised by power in the slower frequencies (delta/theta) and the visual state, characterised by stronger power in the alpha frequency. All states exhibit higher coherence within the alpha frequency band, with the strongest occurring in right sensorimotor-parietal, visual, and DMN-parietal states.

Temporal brain state dynamics are differentially expressed in IBD

At each time point in the time series, the HMM estimates the probability that each brain state is active, referred to as the state time course. The state time courses estimated from the HMM were used to investigate between-group differences in three temporal statistics: (a) fractional occupancy, the proportion of overall time spent in a state; (b) dwell time, the length of each state visit; and (c) interval time, the length of time between consecutive visits to the same state. One-way ANCOVAs identified a significant main effect of group on fractional occupancy in the visual (F (2,82) = 4.31, p = 0.018) and DMN-parietal (F(2,82) = 7.40, p = 0.002) states, and a main effect of group on interval times in the DMN-parietal (F(2,82) = 4.41, p = 0.001) (Figures 2D,F). Relative to HC, individuals with CD resided for less time overall in the visual state (pFWE = 0.04) (Figures 2D.I), but spent a longer time overall (pFWE = 0.002) and had shorter interval times between consecutive visits to the DMN-parietal state compared to UC (pFWE = 0.01) (Figures 2D.I,II). While HC spent more time in the visual state compared to UC, and less time in the DMN-parietal state compared to CD, these effects did not survive Bonferroni correction (UC, pFWE = 0.08; CD, pFWE = 0.06).

We next used the probabilities associated with the state time courses to identify significant between-group differences in the transitions between brain states [Network-based Statistic (58), pFWE < 0.05] (Figure 3). Bold, coloured lines indicate transitions included in the significant NBS component while thin black lines show the top 20% most probable state transitions for each group (Figures 3B,D,F). Firstly, individuals with UC were more likely to transition to the prefrontal state compared to HC and CD (Figures 3E,F). Secondly, individuals with CD and UC were more likely to transition from the left sensorimotor to the integrated prefrontal state, while the inverse was true for HC. Finally, HC individuals were more likely to transition to the visual state, specifically from the DMN-parietal or right sensorimotor-parietal states.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Representation of the transition probabilities between the six brain states in the three groups. (A) We computed subject-specific transition probability matrices representing the likelihood of transitioning from one state, to every other state (K × K). Diagonal matrix elements represent self-transitions (i.e., the probability of remaining in that state) and were set to zero to aid visualisation. (B) Directed transition diagram showing the top 20% most probable state transitions, where each arrow represents a transition. The thin black lines do not represent significant between-group differences, but represent transitions that were more probable on average for that group. Bold, coloured lines indicate a significantly higher probability of this transition in that group. The network-based statistics (NBS) was used to identify significant between-group differences in state transitions (pFWE < 0.05). (C–F) Same as (A,B) but for Crohn’s Disease and Ulcerative Colitis. State 1: Prefrontal (Pre), State 2: integrated prefrontal (IntPre), State 3: right sensorimotor-parietal (RSen-Par), State 4: visual (Vis), State 5: left sensorimotor (LSen), and State 6: DMN-parietal (DMN-Par). Healthy control (HC), Crohn’s Disease (CD), and Ulcerative Colitis (UC).

Taken together, our results suggest that: (a) CD and UC individuals spent less time in, and are less likely to transition to the visual state; (b) individuals with UC are more likely to transition to, and may spend more time in the prefrontal state (although not reaching significance); and (c) individuals with CD reside for longer in, and spent less time between consecutive visits to the DMN-parietal state.

Altered connectivity patterns between key brain regions differentiating groups

To identify the key drivers of these differences, we performed a refined sub-network analysis on communication between specific nodes within the visual and DMN-parietal states. We identified seven candidate regions exhibiting higher influence within each state’s spatial profile (See Candidate Regions in Methods). The posterior precuneus (Pprec), medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and left inferior parietal lobule (IPL) were identified within the DMN-parietal state, while the inferior occipital gyrus (IOG), mid occipital gyrus (MOG), and left insula (insula) were identified within the visual state (Figure 4A; Supplementary Table S2). The posterior cingulate (PCC) had strong involvement within both states, supporting previous work recognising its “flexible” participation across a number of dynamic networks and associated cognitive processes (68, 69). Using the time series from each candidate region, we calculated the strength of directed (effective) connectivity, using dynamic causal modelling (DCM) (Figure 4B) (See DCM in Methods and Supplementary Figure S2 for details). Taking the expected values of the estimated connectivity parameters from all subjects, we identified a significant multivariate association between backward connectivity parameters and group membership (Wilks’ Lambda = 0.16, F (84, 82) = 1.52, pFWE = 0.03). Univariate F tests identified a significant difference between groups in the connectivity from the left insula to mPFC (F (2,84) = 8.57, pFWE = 0.017) (Figures 4C,D). Specifically, individuals with CD showed significantly stronger connectivity from the left insula to mPFC compared to HC (p = 3.63 × 10−4) and UC (p = 0.03). There were no significant differences between UC and HC for any connections.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Targeted analyses of effective brain connectivity (Dynamic Causal Modelling, DCM). (A) Candidate regions were selected from the HMM brain states for a DCM analysis. (B) The Local Field Potential (LFP) convolution-based neural mass model was selected, modelling three subpopulations with five intrinsic connections. Extrinsic afferents are conceptualised as (A) forward connections arriving at the input spiny stellate population; (B) backward connections arriving at both the output pyramidal and interneuron populations. (C) Extrinsic (between-region) efferents project from the output pyramidal population to distant targets. (C,D) Results from MANCOVA post-hoc tests, showing significantly stronger effective connectivity from the left insula to the mPFC in CD individuals. (E) Multiple regression in CD group testing whether disease duration and behavioural symptoms predict the strength of left insula to mPFC connectivity. * denotes p < 0.05; *** denotes p < 0.0005.

Insula to mPFC connectivity linked to disease duration in CD

Our results demonstrated a highly selective enhancement of connectivity between the left insula to mPFC in the CD group. With the exception of three individuals with mild disease activity, all CD individuals were in clinical remission. Thus, these findings provide support to our hypothesis that between-group connectivity differences may be driven by chronic, rather than acute inflammation. Our final aim was to specifically test whether inter-individual variability in the strength of insula to mPFC connectivity in CD was linked to long-standing disease features, thus testing for a more pronounced relationship of how CD chronicity (disease duration) links to depression, anxiety, and stress scores (DASS-42). The DASS-42 is based on a dimensional, rather than categorical, assessment of psychological symptoms, and provides higher inter-subject variability in sub-clinical populations. While the HAM-A and MADRS, and HADS-A and HADS-D, tend to produce anxiety and depression scores that are highly correlated, the DASS-42 is able to more clearly distinguish between anxiety and depression (70). Critical to our study, the anxiety scale assesses key components of interoceptive processing, including autonomic arousal, situational anxiety, and the subjective experience of anxious affect. While results did not show an overall multivariate relationship, we did find a significant independent regression coefficient linking longer CD duration (adjusted for participant age) with stronger insula to mPFC connectivity (β = 0.01, t (34) = 2.19, p = 0.036) (Figure 4E).

Discussion

In this study we assessed whether IBD – a model of chronic, relapsing, and remitting systemic inflammation – is associated with alterations in the spatiotemporal dynamics of spontaneous brain states. In particular, we directly compared CD and UC to delineate whether known distinctions in clinical, microbiome, and physical manifestations of gut inflammation also extends to variability in brain dynamics. Our findings extend upon previous work by showing a CD-specific brain signature implicating regions involved in cognitive-interoceptive appraisal mechanisms. The HMM assessment converges with these findings at a broader scale, demonstrating that IBD individuals exhibit alterations in the temporal properties of brain states supporting computations linking internal and external milieus. Together, our study supports a description of IBD as a dysfunction of the gut-brain axis, moving away from clinical definitions that compartmentalise effects in the gut from the CNS.

Our sub-network DCM dissected the relevant properties of the global brain state assessment. Specifically, this analysis provided a refined interpretation of global neuronal dynamics grounded in physiological and biophysical properties of the brain. These results showed a highly selective enhancement of connectivity from the insula to the mPFC in CD individuals (Figure 4C). The insula is a key interoceptive hub, thought to be responsible for integrating information from the internal and external milieu to generate an awareness of the current emotional and internal state (7173). During rest, information about the internal milieu likely emerges from gastrointestinal and cardiorespiratory stimuli before converging in the NTS and higher cortical regions, including the insula (26). Anatomically, the insula shares afferent and efferent connections with the mPFC (74) which together provide a contextual evaluation of emotional and affective states (75). The finding that CD individuals exhibit stronger bottom-up signalling from the insula to mPFC converges with a model describing altered interoceptive processing. As a function of persistent worry and rumination over anticipated visceral discomfort, many patients with GI disorders develop strong and rigid beliefs (i.e., hyperprecise priors) about the state of the body (76, 77). While the perception of abdominal pain in a healthy individual may not be considered alarming, the same signal may elicit hypervigilance in IBD. The perceived hypervigilance to visceral sensations has previously been cast within a predictive coding framework (78, 79). That is, the persistent inability to accurately detect afferent viscerosensory signals may produce a mismatch between top-down predicted states and the actual interoceptive input reaching the insula and prefrontal regions. This hypothesis is in line with a recent fMRI study showing altered interoceptive processing in CD to uncertainty about anticipated visceral discomfort, compared to controls (29). The tendency of an individual to overestimate the likelihood of a future aversive bodily state provides a conceptual bridge between altered interoception, and the development of clinical anxiety and depression (78, 80). While a confluence of factors are likely to contribute to the high prevalence of anxiety and depression in IBD, models describing the persistence and reinforcement of negative biases towards self-relevant information is thought to be a key contributor. As such, psychological interventions such as mindfulness and meditation have been put forth as adjuvant treatment approaches in IBD to modulate the brain’s response to future aversive interoceptive stimuli (81, 82).

Recent work has demonstrated that long-term exposure to recurrent systemic inflammation impacts brain and behavioural responses in a more permanent and pervasive way as opposed to a single inflammatory event (9, 10). Both CD and UC participants were either in clinical remission or had a mild disease course, with no difference in cardiovascular risk compared to healthy participants. Our results strongly suggest that brain dynamic alterations do not represent the effects of acute inflammation or vascular events, but suggests a more permanent network reconfiguration. In our study, we showed that insula to mPFC hyper-connectivity strengthens with disease duration in CD (Figure 4E). The persistent and chronic effects from repeated exposure to inflammation are likely to result in a confluence of behavioural, biological, and neurophysiological changes, including alterations to interoceptive processing (e.g., heightened sensitivity to visceral inflammatory or nociceptive signals) (18, 29), hyper-activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (83), functional changes to the gut-brain interface, or altered serotonergic and glutamatergic neurotransmission (1, 84). In this study, we did not observe a relationship between effective connectivity and behavioural symptoms. However, it is possible that altered insula-mPFC hyper-connectivity represents a vulnerability towards developing psychological symptoms. Our results provide a strong motivation to pursue longitudinal assessments – monitoring fluctuations in inflammatory activity, medication use, symptoms, surgical procedures, and behaviour – to identify the causal mechanisms contributing to altered network signatures in long-standing CD.

Our results suggest that a diagnosis of CD is, in itself, a key factor in determining the risk of developing altered brain network signatures. Previous work suggests that UC and CD exhibit distinct disease processes (33, 34, 36). UC is described as a mucosal disease with an acute onset, while CD is considered a chronic and systemic disease with a long premorbid phase and transmural involvement (85). Systemic involvement in CD may also be reflected in the higher prevalence of extra-intestinal manifestations (86), with one study attributing low bone density to chronic and long-standing exposure to cytokines selectively in CD, but not in UC (85). Moreover, emerging work suggests that neurological effects related to IBD follows a differential pattern of involvement between sub-groups (87). That is, UC appears to exhibit extra-intestinal manifestations mostly in the peripheral nervous system, while CD is more closely associated with effects in the CNS (87). These observations are in line with a previous structural MRI (sMRI) and resting-state fMRI study comparing CD and UC sub-groups, showing that neural changes in CD may be more pronounced in patients exhibiting extra-intestinal manifestations (39). However in contrast to our results, this study, as well as another using near infrared spectroscopy (38), found that UC exhibited more pronounced neural changes overall compared to CD. Longitudinal and adequately powered studies will be critical to disentangle the nuanced alterations between CD and UC reported in this current study, and in previous work. Our results also showed that diversity, taxonomic, and functional microbiota profiles in CD are significantly different from UC and HC, despite the absence of major inflammatory activity (Figure 1). The failure to restore eubiosis in CD may indirectly serve as a marker of chronicity, representing an epiphenomenon caused by repeated inflammation and extensive bowel damage, as well as a risk factor for recurrent relapse (3537).

Our results suggest that anxiety and depression-related symptoms in IBD reflect altered interoceptive processing of visceral sensations, and a preference to engage in repetitive, self-monitoring and rumination behaviours. As such, the value of integrative clinical approaches, including cognitive-behavioural therapy, interoceptive exposure therapy, mindfulness, and meditation may represent adjuvant treatments in IBD and other visceral-related conditions. Given the finding that disease duration in CD was associated with perturbed interoceptive networks, these approaches may be beneficial early on in the disease process, even in the absence of current anxiety and depressive symptoms. This underscore the broader significance of early diagnosis, and both rapid and effective control of gut and systemic inflammation in IBD patients.

A number of caveats need to be considered when interpreting the results from this study. The cross-sectional design and modest sample size are recognised as limitations. For example, our microbiota assessments of alpha and beta diversity did not detect significant differences between UC and HC. While these results are consistent with results from previous longitudinal 16S rRNA studies showing only diversity differences in CD compared to HC, but not in UC (36, 88), our relatively modest sample size may have resulted in a type II error (i.e., the non-detection of smaller effects in UC individuals). Secondly, our cross-sectional design does not allow us to disentangle the relative contribution of long-standing GI symptoms versus chronic inflammation to observed brain-related effects. However, recent works comparing UC to a control group with irritable bowel syndrome (GI symptoms without underlying inflammation) provides further support that these changes are more specifically driven by chronic gut inflammation, rather than long-term GI symptoms (53, 89). IBD is a heterogeneous disease, and even within CD and UC there is large variability in terms of surgical procedures, medication use, genetics, and inflammatory history. However, the main focus of this study was to characterise the large-scale brain effects from chronic, recurrent and relapsing gut inflammation within an ecologically valid and naturalistic setting. For example, a key source of heterogeneity was medication use in IBD. Specifically, there was a higher proportion of UC individuals taking aminosalcylates, analgesics, and corticosteroid medication (Supplementary Table S1). However, the fact that the CD group appear to be less reliant on medications overall – specifically analgesics – highlights that they have well-controlled symptoms. Taken together, this further strengthens the interpretation of our CD results, supporting the idea that hyper-connectivity from the insula to mPFC is more closely linked to disease chronicity, rather than acute inflammation or symptom flare-ups. Our study provides the initial impetus to pursue future targeted work, including a focus towards creating larger, longitudinal databases including multimodal neuroimaging, clinical, behavioural, and metagenomics data. While most IBD participants were in clinical remission, a number of participants were taking biologic agents, anti-inflammatory, or immunomodulatory medication. This suggests that some participants had experienced an acute inflammatory event at some point prior to the study. As we do not have longitudinal data about previous disease activity, we cannot directly assess their contribution to observed brain alterations. Unlike DCM, the TDE-HMM is a statistical method that is not grounded on biophysical models of neural activity. When inferring the HMM we recognize, like previous authors (48), that there is no biological “ground truth” with regards to the number of brain states selected. Instead, varying the number of states simply offers different resolutions (spatiotemporal detail) to study brain dynamics. Selecting six states represented a necessary trade-off, allowing us to examine brain states that overlap with established fMRI resting state maps but in the process, limiting our ability to detect more subtle dynamics. Our HMM brain states were inferred from resting-state data. Future investigations could extend this work by assessing how external task-related demands modulate spatiotemporal dynamics in DMN and visual networks in CD and UC.

There is converging evidence showing the effects of acute inflammation on brain activity and behaviour (3, 7, 8). However, there remains a large gap in understanding how chronic and repeated exposure to systemic inflammation engenders change in spontaneous whole-brain dynamics. Using an ecologically valid model of peripheral inflammation, we demonstrate that CD individuals exhibit alterations in brain states and patterns of effective connectivity supporting computations within internal, interoceptive mental states. Our results provide motivation to pursue longitudinal assessments evaluating the impact of mood and affective disorders on the natural history of IBD, and vice versa. Understanding the extent and nature of gut-brain dysfunctions in IBD will help to optimise the monitoring and management of behavioural symptoms and critically, to prevent a gut disease from progressing to a comorbid psychiatric or neurodegenerative illness.

Data availability statement

The data presented in the study are deposited in the NCBI repository, accession number PRJNA1028828, and are available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/bioproject/PRJNA1028828/.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by QIMR Research Ethics Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

CH, RM, and LC: conceptualization and methodology. CH and RM: formal analysis. CH, CR, ES, and GR-S: data curation. CH, GR-S, ES, CR, LC, and RM: writing – original draft, and review and editing. GR-S and LC: funding acquisition. RM and LC: supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank L. Simms for their contributions to stool preparation.

Funding Statement

LC acknowledges support from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC, APP1099082 and APP1138711).

Footnotes

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1250268/full#supplementary-material

References

  • 1.Dantzer R, O’Connor JC, Freund GG, Johnson RW, Kelley KW. From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2008) 9:46–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2297, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bullmore E. The inflamed mind: a radical new approach to depression. Picador: Short Books Ltd; (2018). [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Harrison NA, Brydon L, Walker C, Gray MA, Steptoe A, Dolan RJ, et al. Neural origins of human sickness in interoceptive responses to inflammation. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 66:415–22. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.007, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Goehler LE, Gaykema RPA, Hansen MK, Anderson K, Maier SF, Watkins LR. Vagal immune-to-brain communication: a visceral chemosensory pathway. Auton Neurosci. (2000) 85:49–59. doi: 10.1016/S1566-0702(00)00219-8, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Dantzer R, Kelley KW. Twenty years of research on cytokine-induced sickness behavior. Brain Behav Immun. (2007) 21:153–60. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2006.09.006, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dantzer R, Kelley KW. Stress and immunity: an integrated view of relationships between the brain and the immune system. Life Sci. (1989) 44:1995–2008. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(89)90345-7, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Reichenberg A, Yirmiya R, Schuld A, Kraus T, Haack M, Morag A, et al. Cytokine-associated emotional and cognitive disturbances in humans. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2001) 58:445–52. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Harrison NA, Brydon L, Walker C, Gray MA, Steptoe A, Critchley HD. Inflammation causes mood changes through alterations in subgenual cingulate activity and mesolimbic connectivity. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 66:407–14. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.015, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bell JA, Kivimäki M, Bullmore ET, Steptoe A, Bullmore E, Vértes PE, et al. Repeated exposure to systemic inflammation and risk of new depressive symptoms among older adults. Transl Psychiatry. (2017) 7:e1208. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.155 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Schrepf A, Kaplan CM, Ichesco E, Larkin T, Harte SE, Harris RE, et al. A multi-modal MRI study of the central response to inflammation in rheumatoid arthritis. Nat Commun. (2018) 9:2243. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04648-0, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Collins SM. Interrogating the gut-brain axis in the context of inflammatory bowel disease: a translational approach. Inflamm Bowel Dis. (2020) 26:493–501. doi: 10.1093/ibd/izaa004, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Johnston RD, Logan RFA. What is the peak age for onset of IBD? Inflamm Bowel Dis. (2008) 14:S4–5. doi: 10.1002/ibd.20545 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gracie DJ, Hamlin PJ, Ford AC. The influence of the brain–gut axis in inflammatory bowel disease and possible implications for treatment. Lancet Gastroenterol Hepatol. (2019) 4:632–42. doi: 10.1016/S2468-1253(19)30089-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cussotto S, Sandhu KV, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. The neuroendocrinology of the microbiota-gut-brain axis: a behavioural perspective. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2018) 51:80–101. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2018.04.002, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cryan JF, Dinan TG. Mind-altering microorganisms: the impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2012) 13:701–12. doi: 10.1038/nrn3346, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kornelsen J, Wilson A, Labus JS, Witges K, Mayer EA, Bernstein CN. Brain resting-state network alterations associated with Crohn’s disease. Front Neurol. (2020) 11:48. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00048, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Thomann AK, Griebe M, Thomann PA, Hirjak D, Ebert MP, Szabo K, et al. Intrinsic neural network dysfunction in quiescent Crohn’s disease. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:11579. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11792-y, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Thomann AK, Reindl W, Wüstenberg T, Kmuche D, Ebert MP, Szabo K, et al. Aberrant brain structural large-scale connectome in Crohn’s disease. Neurogastroenterol Motil. (2019) 31:e13593. doi: 10.1111/nmo.13593, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bao C, Liu P, Liu H, Jin X, Shi Y, Wu L, et al. Difference in regional neural fluctuations and functional connectivity in Crohn’s disease: a resting-state functional MRI study. Brain Imaging Behav. (2018) 12:1795–803. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-9850-z, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hou J, Mohanty R, Nair VA, Dodd K, Beniwal-Patel P, Saha S, et al. Alterations in resting-state functional connectivity in patients with Crohn’s disease in remission. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:7412. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-43878-0, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2008) 1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Andrews-Hanna JR. The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist. (2012) 18:251–70. doi: 10.1177/1073858411403316, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2014) 1316:29–52. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12360, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wise T, Marwood L, Perkins AM, Herane-Vives A, Joules R, Lythgoe DJ, et al. Instability of default mode network connectivity in major depression: a two-sample confirmation study. Transl Psychiatry. (2017) 7:e1105. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.40 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Coutinho JF, Fernandesl SV, Soares JM, Maia L, Gonçalves ÓF, Sampaio A. Default mode network dissociation in depressive and anxiety states. Brain Imaging Behav. (2016) 10:147–57. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9375-7, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mayer EA. Gut feelings: the emerging biology of gut-brain communication. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:453–66. doi: 10.1038/nrn3071, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Mulder D, Aarts E, Arias Vasquez A, Bloemendaal M. A systematic review exploring the association between the human gut microbiota and brain connectivity in health and disease. Mol Psychiatry. (2023) 21:1–25. doi: 10.1038/s41380-023-02146-4, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Clarke G, Kennedy PJ, Groeger JA, Quigley EMM, Shanahan F, Cryan JF, et al. Impaired cognitive function in Crohn’s disease: relationship to disease activity. Brain Behav Immun Health. (2020) 5:100093. doi: 10.1016/j.bbih.2020.100093, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rubio A, Pellissier S, Van Oudenhove L, Ly HG, Dupont P, Tack J, et al. Brain responses to uncertainty about upcoming rectal discomfort in quiescent Crohn’s disease – a fMRI study. Neurogastroenterol Motil. (2016) 28:1419–32. doi: 10.1111/nmo.12844, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hou J, Dodd K, Nair VA, Rajan S, Beniwal-Patel P, Saha S, et al. Alterations in brain white matter microstructural properties in patients with Crohn’s disease in remission. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:2145. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-59098-w, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.van Langenberg DR, Yelland GW, Robinson SR, Gibson PR. Cognitive impairment in Crohn’s disease is associated with systemic inflammation, symptom burden and sleep disturbance. United European Gastroenterol J. (2017) 5:579–87. doi: 10.1177/2050640616663397, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Fan Y, Bao C, Wei Y, Wu J, Zhao Y, Zeng X, et al. Altered functional connectivity of the amygdala in Crohn’s disease. Brain Imaging Behav. (2020) 14:2097–106. doi: 10.1007/s11682-019-00159-8, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Shanahan F. Pathogenesis of ulcerative colitis. Lancet. (1993) 342:407–11. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(93)92818-E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ni J, Wu GD, Albenberg L, Tomov VT. Gut microbiota and IBD: causation or correlation? Nat Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol. (2017) 14:573–84. doi: 10.1038/nrgastro.2017.88, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Magro DO, Santos A, Guadagnini D, de Godoy FM, Silva SHM, Lemos WJF, et al. Remission in Crohn’s disease is accompanied by alterations in the gut microbiota and mucins production. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:13263. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-49893-5, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Pascal V, Pozuelo M, Borruel N, Casellas F, Campos D, Santiago A, et al. A microbial signature for Crohn’s disease. Gut. (2017) 66:813–22. doi: 10.1136/gutjnl-2016-313235, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vermeire S, Joossens M, Verbeke K, Wang J, Machiels K, Sabino J, et al. Donor species richness determines faecal microbiota transplantation success in inflammatory bowel disease. J Crohn’s Colitis. (2016) 10:387–94. doi: 10.1093/ecco-jcc/jjv203, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Fujiwara T, Kono S, Katakura K, Abe K, Takahashi A, Gunji N, et al. Evaluation of brain activity using near-infrared spectroscopy in inflammatory bowel disease patients. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:402. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18897-4, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Thomann AK, Schmitgen MM, Kmuche D, Ebert MP, Thomann PA, Szabo K, et al. Exploring joint patterns of brain structure and function in inflammatory bowel diseases using multimodal data fusion. Neurogastroenterol Motil. (2021) 33:e14078. doi: 10.1111/nmo.14078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Bolyen E, Rideout JR, Dillon MR, Bokulich NA, Abnet CC, Al-Ghalith GA, et al. Reproducible, interactive, scalable and extensible microbiome data science using QIIME 2. Nat Biotechnol. (2019) 37:852–7. doi: 10.1038/s41587-019-0209-9, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Callahan BJ, McMurdie PJ, Rosen MJ, Han AW, Johnson AJA, Holmes SP. DADA2: high-resolution sample inference from Illumina amplicon data. Nat Methods. (2016) 13:581–3. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3869, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lozupone C, Knight R. UniFrac: a new phylogenetic method for comparing microbial communities. Appl Environ Microbiol. (2005) 71:8228–35. doi: 10.1128/AEM.71.12.8228-8235.2005, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Langille MGI, Zaneveld J, Caporaso JG, McDonald D, Knights D, Reyes JA, et al. Predictive functional profiling of microbial communities using 16S rRNA marker gene sequences. Nat Biotechnol. (2013) 31:814–21. doi: 10.1038/nbt.2676, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Caspi R, Billington R, Fulcher CA, Keseler IM, Kothari A, Krummenacker M, et al. The MetaCyc database of metabolic pathways and enzymes. Nucleic Acids Res. (2018) 46:D633–9. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkx935, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Mallick H, Rahnavard A, McIver LJ, Ma S, Zhang Y, Nguyen LH, et al. Multivariable association discovery in population-scale meta-omics studies. PLoS Comput Biol. (2021) 17:e1009442. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009442 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bell AJ, Sejnowski TJ. An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution. Neural Comput. (1995) 7:1129–59. doi: 10.1162/neco.1995.7.6.1129, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Quinn AJ, Vidaurre D, Abeysuriya R, Becker R, Nobre AC, Woolrich MW. Task-evoked dynamic network analysis through hidden Markov modeling. Front Neurosci. (2018) 12:603. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00603, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Vidaurre D, Abeysuriya R, Becker R, Quinn AJ, Alfaro-Almagro F, Smith SM, et al. Discovering dynamic brain networks from big data in rest and task. NeuroImage. (2018) 180:646–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.077, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Colclough GL, Brookes MJ, Smith SM, Woolrich MW. A symmetric multivariate leakage correction for MEG connectomes. NeuroImage. (2015) 117:439–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.071, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Colclough GL, Woolrich MW, Tewarie PK, Brookes MJ, Quinn AJ, Smith SM. How reliable are MEG resting-state connectivity metrics? NeuroImage. (2016) 138:284–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Baliki MN, Geha PY, Apkarian AV, Chialvo DR. Beyond feeling: chronic pain hurts the brain, disrupting the default-mode network dynamics. J Neurosci. (2008) 28:1398–403. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4123-07.2008, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Neeb L, Bayer A, Bayer K-E, Farmer A, Fiebach JB, Siegmund B, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation in inflammatory bowel disease patients modifies resting-state functional connectivity: a RCT. Brain Stimul. (2019) 12:978–80. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.03.001, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Turkiewicz J, Bhatt RR, Wang H, Vora P, Krause B, Sauk JS, et al. Altered brain structural connectivity in patients with longstanding gut inflammation is correlated with psychological symptoms and disease duration. Neuroimage Clin. (2021) 30:102613. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102613, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Vidaurre D, Quinn AJ, Baker AP, Dupret D, Tejero-Cantero A, Woolrich MW. Spectrally resolved fast transient brain states in electrophysiological data. NeuroImage. (2016) 126:81–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.047, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Vidaurre D, Smith SM, Woolrich MW. Brain network dynamics are hierarchically organized in time. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2017) 114:12827–32. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1705120114, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Meer JNV, Breakspear M, Chang LJ, Sonkusare S, Cocchi L. Movie viewing elicits rich and reliable brain state dynamics. Nature. Communications. (2020) 11:5004. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-18717-w, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kottaram A, Johnston LA, Cocchi L, Ganella EP, Everall I, Pantelis C, et al. Brain network dynamics in schizophrenia: reduced dynamism of the default mode network. Hum Brain Mapp. (2019) 40:2212–28. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24519, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Zalesky A, Fornito A, Bullmore ET. Network-based statistic: identifying differences in brain networks. NeuroImage. (2010) 53:1197–207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. Network hubs in the human brain. Trends Cogn Sci. (2013) 17:683–96. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage. (2010) 52:1059–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Moran RJ, Stephan KE, Seidenbecher T, Pape H-C, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal models of steady-state responses. NeuroImage. (2009) 44:796–811. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.048, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Moran RJ, Kiebel SJ, Stephan KE, Reilly RB, Daunizeau J, Friston KJ. A neural mass model of spectral responses in electrophysiology. NeuroImage. (2007) 37:706–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.032, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Moran R, Pinotsis D, Friston K. Neural masses and fields in dynamic causal modeling. Front Comput Neurosci. (2013) 7:57. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00057, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Vidaurre D, Hunt LT, Quinn AJ, Hunt BAE, Brookes MJ, Nobre AC, et al. Spontaneous cortical activity transiently organises into frequency specific phase-coupling networks. Nat Commun. (2018) 9:2987. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05316-z, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Baker AP, Brookes MJ, Rezek IA, Smith SM, Behrens T, Probert Smith PJ, et al. Fast transient networks in spontaneous human brain activity. elife. (2014) 3:e01867. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01867, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Van Schependom J, Vidaurre D, Costers L, Sjøgård M, D’Hooghe MB, D’Haeseleer M, et al. Altered transient brain dynamics in multiple sclerosis: treatment or pathology? Hum Brain Mapp. (2019) 40:4789–800. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24737, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods. (2011) 8:665–70. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Leech R, Braga R, Sharp DJ. Echoes of the brain within the posterior cingulate cortex. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:215–22. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3689-11.2012, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Leech R, Sharp DJ. The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain J Neurol. (2014) 137:12–32. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt162, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Antony MM, Bieling PJ, Cox BJ, Enns MW, Swinson RP. Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the depression anxiety stress scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychol Assess. (1998) 10:176–81. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.176 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Craig AD. How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Fullana MA, Harrison BJ, Soriano-Mas C, Vervliet B, Cardoner N, Avila-Parcet A, et al. Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Mol Psychiatry. (2016) 21:500–8. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Nguyen VT, Breakspear M, Hu X, Guo CC. The integration of the internal and external milieu in the insula during dynamic emotional experiences. NeuroImage. (2016) 124:455–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.078, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Öngür D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex. (2000) 10:206–19. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.206, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. (2011) 15:85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Tumati S, Paulus MP, Northoff G. Out-of-step: brain-heart desynchronization in anxiety disorders. Mol Psychiatry. (2021) 26:1726–37. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01029-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.McCormick JB, Hammer RR, Farrell RM, Geller G, James KM, Loftus EV, et al. Experiences of patients with chronic gastrointestinal conditions: in their own words. Health Qual Life Outcomes. (2012) 10:25. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-10-25, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Paulus MP, Stein MB. An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 60:383–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Critchley Hugo D, Harrison NA. Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron. (2013) 77:624–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.008, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Paulus MP, Stein MB. Interoception in anxiety and depression. Brain Struct Funct. (2010) 214:451–63. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0258-9, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Cotton S, Humenay Roberts Y, Tsevat J, Britto MT, Succop P, McGrady ME, et al. Mind–body complementary alternative medicine use and quality of life in adolescents with inflammatory bowel disease. Inflamm Bowel Dis. (2010) 16:501–6. doi: 10.1002/ibd.21045, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Paulides E, Boukema I, van der Woude CJ, de Boer NKH. The effect of psychotherapy on quality of life in IBD patients: a systematic review. Inflamm Bowel Dis. (2021) 27:711–24. doi: 10.1093/ibd/izaa144, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Mawdsley JE, Rampton DS. Psychological stress in IBD: new insights into pathogenic and therapeutic implications. Gut. (2005) 54:1481–91. doi: 10.1136/gut.2005.064261, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Müller N, Schwarz MJ. The immune-mediated alteration of serotonin and glutamate: towards an integrated view of depression. Mol Psychiatry. (2007) 12:988–1000. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002006, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Ardizzone S, Bollani S, Bettica P, Bevilacqua M, Molteni P, Bianchi PG. Altered bone metabolism in inflammatory bowel disease: there is a difference between Crohn’s disease and ulcerative colitis. J Intern Med. (2000) 247:63–70. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2796.2000.00582.x, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Levine JS, Burakoff R. Extraintestinal manifestations of inflammatory bowel disease. Gastroenterol Hepatol (N Y). (2011) 7:235–41. PMID: [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Zois CD, Katsanos KH, Kosmidou M, Tsianos EV. Neurologic manifestations in inflammatory bowel diseases: current knowledge and novel insights. J Crohn’s Colitis. (2010) 4:115–24. doi: 10.1016/j.crohns.2009.10.005, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Halfvarson J, Brislawn CJ, Lamendella R, Vázquez-Baeza Y, Walters WA, Bramer LM, et al. Dynamics of the human gut microbiome in inflammatory bowel disease. Nat Microbiol. (2017) 2:17004. doi: 10.1038/nmicrobiol.2017.4, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Hong JY, Labus JS, Jiang Z, Ashe-Mcnalley C, Dinov I, Gupta A, et al. Regional neuroplastic brain changes in patients with chronic inflammatory and non-inflammatory visceral pain. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e84564. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084564, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in the study are deposited in the NCBI repository, accession number PRJNA1028828, and are available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/bioproject/PRJNA1028828/.


Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES