Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2023 Nov 22;13:20484. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-47430-z

Validation of a modified visual analogue scale to measure user-perceived comfort of a lower-limb exoskeleton

Mohammed Mohammed El Husaini 1, Axl Maberry 1, Anne E Martin 1,
PMCID: PMC10665473  PMID: 37993504

Abstract

User perceived exoskeleton comfort is likely important for device acceptance, but there is currently no validated instrument to measure it. The Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) is an existing tool to measure subjective human feedback by asking the user to mark a point on a line with each end of the line representing an opposing anchor statement. It can be modified to show the previous response, allowing the subject to directly indicate if the current condition is better or worse than the previous one. The goal of this study was to determine how well the modified VAS could measure user-perceived comfort as the exoskeleton control parameters were varied. To validate the survey, 14 healthy subjects walked in a pair of ankle exoskeletons with approximately ten distinct sets of control parameters tested in a prescribed order. Each set of control parameters was tested twice. After each trial, user-perceived comfort was measured using a two-question VAS survey. The repeatability coefficient was approximately 40 mm, similar to the total range of responses. The results were also inconsistent, with relative rankings between consecutive pairs of conditions matching for approximately 50% of comparisons. Thus, as tested, the VAS was not repeatable or consistent. It is possible that subject adaptation within the trial and over the course of the experiment may have impacted the results. Additional work is needed to develop a repeatable method to measure comfort and to determine how perceived comfort varies as subjects’ gain exoskeleton experience.

Subject terms: Biomedical engineering, Mechanical engineering

Introduction

Exoskeletons have the potential to enhance rehabilitation in healthcare settings and to reduce injury in industrial settings. As the population ages, gait-related problems are expected to increase1. These problems tend to reduce quality of life1 and increase the risk of secondary conditions2, motivating the development of novel rehabilitation methods such as exoskeletons3,4. In physically-demanding industries, exoskeletons can be used to reduce muscular effort5. This in turn may reduce the likelihood of injury and the associated costs6. Given the high rates (> 20%) of injury in the construction industry7, nursing8, and other fields with highly-loaded repetitive movements9, there is a great need for such assistive devices.

For exoskeletons to be adopted, they need to be effective as well as meet many subjective criteria such as being comfortable and not generating negative social perceptions1014. However, subjective criteria are only just beginning to be studied, particularly for lower-limb exoskeletons. While there is more work on comfort and user perception for upper-limb and back exoskeletons, there is not a standard, validated method to measure (dis)comfort15. Much of the work measuring comfort has focused on the physical design of the device11,16 and ignored how the exoskeleton controller impacts comfort. A few groups have optimized for user preference in lower-limb exoskeleton controllers1721 and prosthetic stiffness2227, but these studies did not directly consider comfort. While comfort is likely an aspect of preference, it is not necessarily equivalent to it, although comfort and preference are often assumed to be synonymous in the literature e.g.,18,28,29. In contrast to the previous studies, our study directly asked how comfortable the exoskeleton felt.

Unfortunately, measuring subjective feelings is difficult. Although the person may know exactly how they feel, communicating that information can be difficult due to the difficulty in verbalizing subjective evaluations of comfort24,26,30. Thus, methods such as Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) surveys15,20,3033 are used to measure subjective feelings. A standard VAS utilizes two opposing anchor statements that describe the measured variable34. They are typically presented as a horizontal line with a length of 100 mm35, and the subject responds by marking a point on the line. The response is then analyzed as a continuous numerical value. The VAS can be modified to include the previous response, thereby allowing the subject to make a direct comparison34,36,37. This technique allows the data to be analyzed as pairwise comparisons between consecutive conditions. However, very few studies have used VAS surveys for lower-limb exoskeletons20.

Instead, some studies have used variations on forced choice comparisons to find the most preferred exoskeleton controller or prosthesis setting17,18,21,23,24,26,27. These methods produce pairwise rankings but do not produce an absolute measure of the feeling. Other studies have had subjects self-tune device parameters to specify their most preferred condition19,22,25,28. This method does not produce a measure of the relevant subjective feeling; it only finds the associated optimal parameters. In theory, a modified VAS should measure both the relative comparison between consecutive conditions and provide an absolute value of comfort34, providing the most flexibility for later data analysis. Standard VAS surveys can reliably measure footwear comfort31 and pain32,3842 but have not yet been validated for lower-limb exoskeleton comfort.

A valid measurement tool needs to produce a similar response when the same condition is tested on the same person multiple times43. In other words, it needs to be repeatable. This is typically quantified using the repeatability coefficient (RC), with lower values indicating better repeatability43,44. The RC for pain using a standard 100 mm VAS scale is generally 12–20 mm42,45,46. The primary objective of this study was to measure the RC of a VAS for exoskeleton comfort.

Traditional validation methods for the VAS implicitly assume that the subject’s internal perception of the phenomena does not change as the experiment progresses. This may not be a valid assumption when measuring exoskeleton comfort, which would then make the test–retest approach unsuitable. Because most subjects will have very limited prior experience with exoskeletons, they may not have a good sense of what the maximum anchor feels like at the start of the experiment. As they gain more experience, the interpretation of the maximum anchor could change, causing responses to drift and lowering the repeatability of the survey30,36. Further, as the experiment progresses, subjects may become better at walking with the exoskeleton47,48, changing which controller feels most comfortable. If subjects become fatigued, perceived comfort could systematically decrease as the experiment progresses. There is some evidence that adaptation occurs when measuring footwear comfort because shoes worn prior to a comfort measurement affect the results31, implying that perceived comfort changed over time. Thus, the data needs to be checked for potential user adaptation to help interpret the validation results.

Therefore, the primary objective of this paper was to quantify the repeatability of a modified VAS survey that measured the user-perceived comfort of an ankle exoskeleton controller. To further investigate the consistency of the survey, the VAS responses were converted into relative rankings between consecutive pairs of experimental trials, and the consistency of the rankings were quantified. Because these analysis techniques assume that the user’s internal perception of comfort does not change over the course of the experiment, preliminary qualitative results on potential user adaptation are given.

Results

In this study, young healthy adults walked in a pair of ankle exoskeletons for approximately 20 3-min trials. After each trial, subjects answered a two-question 100 mm VAS survey that showed the response for the previous trial. The first question (Q1) was: “How comfortable are you?” and the second question (Q2) was: “How helpful is the exoskeleton at toe-off?”. Each subject experienced approximately 10 unique exoskeleton control parameters in a prescribed order—all 10 parameter sets were tested once in the first cycle and then all 10 parameter sets were tested a second time in the second cycle. Prior to the experiment, subjects had not used an exoskeleton. Our analysis aimed to determine if user responses were repeatable between the two cycles.

Initial analysis

The two VAS questions were strongly correlated with a Pearson correlation coefficient of 0.79. Correlations for most subjects were similar, with correlations ranging from 0.56 to 0.92, except for a single subject with a correlation of -0.10 (Supplemental Fig. 1).

The median range of VAS responses was 40 mm for Q1 and 42 mm for Q2, although there was significant between-subject variability (ranges of 13–87 mm for Q1 and 23–100 mm for Q2, Supplemental Table 1). Thus, most subjects did not utilize the entire range of the VAS.

Subject answers were not consistent between cycles. When each cycle’s data was plotted separately, 11 out of the 14 subjects had substantially different comfort contours between cycle 1 and 2 (Fig. 1, Supplemental Fig. 2). For 6 subjects, their most comfortable parameters shifted to more moderate parameter values in cycle 2. The remaining 5 subjects with changes all altered their preferences differently. When the VAS score was plotted versus trial number, there were no consistent trends such as a consistent decrease in comfort as the experiment progressed (Supplemental Fig. 3).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Comparison of normalized Q1 answers (Eq. 4) between cycles for one representative subject. The exoskeleton control parameters are k (effective stiffness) and m (rate of reference position change). For this subject, they found low k most comfortable in cycle 1. In cycle 2, they found moderate k and moderate m most comfortable. In 6 out of 14 subjects, the area of maximum comfort shifted towards more moderate k and m in the second cycle compared to the first cycle.

Repeatability

When all subjects were analyzed together, the RC was 39 mm for Q1 and 46 mm for Q2 (Fig. 2, Table 1). When each subject was analyzed individually, the median (minimum–maximum) RC was 33 (13–76) mm and 36 (17–85) mm for Q1 and Q2 respectively. Most subjects’ individual RC encompassed the majority and, in some cases, the entirety of their range of responses for each question (Supplemental Table 1).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Difference in VAS scores between cycles (Δ) versus mean VAS response (μ) per condition and subject. Each dot color represents a different subject. There were significant deviations among responses signifying poor repeatability. The repeatability coefficient of Q1 was slightly smaller than Q2, indicating that Q1 had better repeatability.

Table 1.

Results from the repeatability analysis when all subjects were analyzed together.

RC (mm) μΔ (mm) σΔ (mm) Δ<10mm (%) Δ<20mm (%) 90% threshold (mm)
Q1 39 5 19 47 75 32
Q2 46 4 23 40 65 38

The table contains the repeatability coefficient (RC), signed mean difference between cycles (μΔ), standard deviation of the difference between cycles (σΔ), percent of trials with between cycle differences (Δ) less than 10 mm and 20 mm, and the 90% threshold. The 90% threshold was defined as the difference magnitude for which 90% of responses had absolute differences less than the threshold. Although the (RC) for both questions was large, 75% of the differences between cycles (Δ) for Q1 were below 20 mm, implying that the VAS survey could measure a signal but not as well as for pain.

The 90% threshold of approximately 35 mm was similar to the RC as expected (Table 1). The percentages of trials with between cycle differences less than 10 mm and 20 mm were 47% and 75% for Q1 respectively. Consistent with the RC, these percentages were larger than the corresponding percentages of 40% and 65% for Q2. Both questions had a small positive bias μΔ, indicating that subjects generally rated sets of parameters slightly higher during the first cycle than in the second cycle. Due to the large RC for both questions, the bias was insignificant.

Relative comfort consistency

The relative comfort consistency between consecutive trials was 48% for Q1 and 46% for Q2 (Table 2). When the relative ranking changed between cycles, it had an approximately equal chance of moving to either of the two other options.

Table 2.

The number of trials for each relative ranking arranged by cycle.

Question 1 Question 2
Cycle 2 Cycle 1 Cycle 2 Cycle 1
Higher No change Lower Higher No change Lower
Higher 20 13 12 Higher 26 9 14
No change 12 21 4 No change 10 15 11
Lower 14 10 19 Lower 15 8 17

The relative rankings were not consistent between trials, as indicated by the large number of trials in the off-diagonal table cells.

Discussion

This work evaluated how consistently a modified VAS survey could measure user-perceived comfort of an ankle exoskeleton controller. The VAS instrument allowed explicit comparisons by showing the previous response. While multiple studies have shown that a VAS can measure footwear comfort31 and general pain32,3842, it did not produce repeatable measurements for exoskeleton comfort. The RC of 40 mm was more than twice as large as the RC for pain42,45,46. Further, the response range had a similar magnitude as the RC. Together, this indicates that individual subjects did not give similar absolute scores for identical control parameters. In addition, when comparing relative comfort scores between consecutive trials, the relative rankings were not consistent between cycles. This indicates that individual subjects did not consistently find one set of exoskeleton control parameters more comfortable than another set.

The lack of consistency is somewhat surprising since previous research has indicated that people can reliably choose which exoskeleton or prosthesis condition they prefer1719,2224,26,28. While comfort and preference are often thought of as synonymous, comfort is likely only one factor that influences preference. Thus, one possibility is that preference is primarily driven by factors other than comfort, such as metabolic cost or stability, so that preference and comfort are not strongly correlated. Future work is needed to better understand what factors influence comfort and preference. Another possibility is that adaptation plays a large role in perceived comfort. Multiple studies have shown that subjects adapt to walking with an exoskeleton, including preferring more assistance as the experiment progresses19,47. In this study, many subjects’ most comfortable control parameters shifted to more moderate values as they gained experience. Thus, subject adaptation could explain both the inconsistent relative responses and the low repeatability. This adaptation can occur during an individual trial, as well as across the entire experiment.

In this work, within trial adaptation was supported by statements from some subjects. They stated that at first, they might have felt a difference between changes to the set of control parameters, but then they could no longer tell by the end of the trial. Thus, it appears that requiring subjects to walk for several minutes in each condition may reduce the differences in perceived comfort as subjects learn to walk well with different controller parameters. In the preference studies, subjects only took a few steps before giving their response19,27, which may have improved the results. One way to resolve the issue of within-trial adaptation is to reduce the duration of the trial. However, this would make measuring other biological signals more difficult, particularly steady-state metabolic cost. Another approach could be introducing the survey during the trial, which would require the subject to answer the survey as they walk. This could result in subjects stumbling or losing balance as they focus on the survey. These approaches would also only give the initial impression of comfort, not how comfortable the control condition is after extended use. Additional studies investigating how within trial adaptation influence comfort are needed. Alternatively, larger changes in control parameters between trials may improve the repeatability by creating more of a difference between conditions. It is possible that subjects could not feel a difference between some conditions49,50, although if this occurred, subjects should have given the same VAS score for both trials. If the survey repeatability is only acceptable for large changes in control parameters, it would indicate that large changes in perceived comfort could be measured, but not small differences.

Adaptation over the duration of the experiment also may have occurred. As discussed in the introduction, this can change the perceived maximum (or minimum) and cause drift over time. If this occurred, the responses would not be repeatable, but the relative consistency between consecutive trials should have been high. Since this was not observed, it is unlikely that this occurred. Subjects could also adapt to the physical design of the exoskeleton or become fatigued. If either of these occurred and were the primary factor driving comfort, we would expect the mean comfort for the first cycle to be substantially different than for the second cycle. We would also expect to see a general trend of increasing or decreasing comfort scores as a function of trial number regardless of how the control parameters changed between trials. However, neither of these phenomena were observed, so it does not appear perceived comfort systematically increased or decreased over the course of the experiment.

Finally, the experimental protocol could influence adaptation and the perception of comfort. Much of the previous work that validated preference had subjects self-select their most preferred condition after freely exploring the parameter space19,22,28. It is possible that the initially diverse exploration prior to fine-tuning quickly got subjects used to the device and the range of possibilities, making it easier for them to identify their most preferred condition. If this is true, then performing a third cycle in this experiment and calculating the repeatability between the second and third cycles could give more consistent responses. Alternatively, introducing familiarization trials with several diverse control parameters prior to data collection may improve repeatability in less time than performing a full third cycle. It is also possible that introducing a control condition before each new set of parameters31 could lead to more consistent answers between cycles. In theory, it would improve the subjects’ ability to recall and make comparisons while answering the survey since they would always compare their experience to the same condition. However, this would halve the number of parameters that could be tested in a given time, making it difficult to explore the exoskeleton control space fully.

The validation methods used in this paper assumed that an individual’s perceived comfort could be fully defined by the control parameters. However, if the perception of comfort changed during the course of the experiment, then that assumption was violated, and the statistical tools used were inappropriate. If adaption did occur, then comfort needs to be analyzed differently, such as with a dynamic model that includes a time component. Both the large RC and the inconsistent relative responses partially support a dynamic interpretation of comfort, although the standard statistical methods used here cannot be used to fully asses if this is the case. Thus, the possible temporal nature of comfort should be investigated further using methods from dynamic systems.

Overall, the VAS yielded poor validation results for measuring comfort. The repeatability analysis yielded a RC of 39 mm for the combined data of Q1, which was significantly larger than the RC of 12–20 mm for surveys designed to measure pain42,45,46. In addition, under 50% of the relative responses between trials were the same between cycles. The results imply that the VAS was unable to measure comfort reliably. However, this could be due to subject adaptation both within a trial and over the course of the experiment. Much more additional work is needed to determine if and how perceived comfort varies as subjects’ gain exoskeleton experience.

Methods

Experimental design

14 healthy subjects (12 males, 2 females, mean ± SD, age 22.6 ± 3.0 years, mass 70.1 ± 12.1 kg, and height 172 ± 11.3 cm) consented to participate in the study. The subjects had not used an exoskeleton prior to the experiment and had no neuromuscular or neurological pathologies, injuries, or illnesses affecting gait. Individuals with significant femoral retroversion (duck feet posture) were excluded from the study since it caused frequent exoskeleton collisions. The experiment was approved by the Pennsylvania State University Institutional Review Board (STUDY00012828) and conducted in accordance to relevant guidelines and regulations. All subjects provided informed consent prior to starting the experiment.

For each trial, the subject walked on a treadmill for 3-min wearing a pair of ankle exoskeletons. To account for the weight of the exoskeletons, a slow treadmill speed of v=0.0033h, where h was the subject’s height in meters, was used51. The exoskeleton control parameters changed every trial. After each trial, a two-question VAS survey was presented on an electronic tablet to collect user-perceived comfort responses from the subject. In addition, kinematic, EMG, and metabolic signals were recorded but not used for this analysis.

The 3.6 kg custom-built exoskeletons had a single pneumatically-powered degree of freedom at the ankle to allow plantarflexion and dorsiflexion52 (Fig. 3). To allow gait phase detection, each exoskeleton was equipped with two force sensitive resistor (FSR) sensors, one at the ball of the foot and one at the heel. Voltage thresholds for the FSRs were set manually for each subject to achieve a binary (high/low) state for each sensor to indicate if that portion of the foot was loaded. The exoskeletons were controlled using an impedance controller:

u=kR(θ-mRt), 1

where kR was the virtual stiffness, θ was the current ankle angle, mR was the ramp input slope, and t was time. Time was reset to zero at the start of each phase. The desired ankle angle mRt saturated at a phase-dependent reference position (Table 3). The control parameters (kR, mR) were normalized based on subject height and mass since it was expected that subject size would affect the magnitude of their most comfortable stiffness and ramp slope:

k=1000kRmsgh 2
m=mRhg, 3

where ms is subject mass, h is subject height, and g is gravitational acceleration. k was scaled by a factor of 1000 to make the values easier to work with. The physical limits for the control parameters were found by pilot testing various parameter combinations multiple times to ensure the exoskeletons were operational for the entire trial duration. The final range for each parameter was k0,50,6Nm/deg and m0,500,120deg/s.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

One of the two one-degree-of-freedom ankle exoskeletons. As the pneumatic cylinder actuates, it changes the ankle angle.

Table 3.

Mapping between gait phase, FSR sensor readings, and desired angle saturation limit.

Gait phase Initial stance Middle stance Toe-off Swing
Front sensor Low High High Low
Back sensor High High Low Low
Saturation limit 0 0 -20 0

A negative angle indicates plantarflexion.

Subjects completed at least twenty 3-min trials during the 3-h experiment. During these trials, a set of 10 control parameters were each tested twice following a roughly spiral pattern (Fig. 4). The first and second time a parameter set was tested will be referred to as the first and second cycles respectively. If there was time, subjects completed 2 additional trials for an 11th set of control parameters. Subjects were given 15-min to rest after every 7 trials to mitigate the effects of fatigue on comfort perception. The frequency and duration of the rest breaks was determined during pilot testing.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Tested control conditions. The filled circles indicate the first and second cycle parameters. The open circle is the extra set of parameters tested if there was enough time after the second cycle was completed. The numbers represent the order in which the parameters were tested.

After each trial, a two-question VAS survey was used to measure user-perceived comfort. For the first trial, a standard 100 mm VAS was used. Starting with the second trial, the 100 mm VAS displayed the subject’s response for the previous trial. This allowed the subject to more easily place their answer relative to their last response (Fig. 5)34. Pilot testing indicated that this had the potential to improve response consistency by assisting subjects in recalling their experience34,36,53. Prior to displaying the VAS questions, the subject was asked a preliminary question about whether he/she felt a difference between the current and previous trials. If the subject answered no, the VAS survey was skipped, and his/her previous responses were assigned for the current trial.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Example VAS question and response. The blue handle indicated the previous response and could not be moved. The red handle was moveable, and the subject was free to place it anywhere on the line.

The VAS survey consisted of two questions: The first question (Q1) was: “How comfortable are you?” and the second question (Q2) was: “How helpful is the exoskeleton at toe-off?”. Before starting the experiment, toe-off was explained to subjects as: “One of the questions includes the term toe-off. Toe-off describes the portion of the step just before your foot leaves the ground. During this stage, you push off the ground using the ball of your foot and your toes. Hence the term, toe-off.” Q2 was meant to illicit a response more specific to changes in m since the exoskeleton assistance at toe-off is directly related to the magnitude of m. Therefore, we anticipated that Q2 would have less variability since Q2 was more specific than Q1. If Q1 yielded inconsistent responses but the questions were correlated, Q2 could be used as a proxy for Q1. Additional questions, including those focusing on a single body part, were considered during pilot testing. The two chosen questions appeared to perform the best.

When interpreting the correlation between questions, a “halo effect” (order bias54) could cause subsequent questions on a survey to depend on the first question posed55. The halo effect could lead to false conclusions about the correlation between questions since they would be answered similarly. This phenomenon was encountered during pilot testing, possibly due to mental fatigue, especially toward the end of the experiment. To mitigate this, the questions were asked in a random order54.

Initial analysis

Prior to validating the survey, several preliminary measures were determined. The correlation between questions was found for all subjects together and for each subject individually. The correlation was quantified using the Pearson correlation coefficient. In addition, the range of responses was found for both questions.

Two types of plots were created to qualitatively analyze how comfort changed as the experiment progressed. In one set of plots, comfort was estimated across the range of tested control parameters using linear interpolation. Specifically, Matlab’s fit function was used with the linearinterp option. These plots were created for each subject and cycle individually and for each subject when combining the data from both cycles. To aid in visualization, the experimental data was scaled linearly using min/max feature scaling:

Qscaled=Q-QminQmax-Qmin×100. 4

where Qscaled was the scaled response for that set of control parameters, Q was the original response, and Qmin and Qmax were the minimum and maximum responses over all experimental trials for that subject. These plots allowed qualitative analysis of how the most comfortable control parameters changed as the experiment progressed. In the second set of plots, the VAS answers were plotted versus trial number. The plots were visually checked for consistent trends. For example, consistently decreasing VAS scores may indicate that the subject was becoming fatigued and therefore found walking in the exoskeleton increasingly difficult and uncomfortable.

Repeatability analysis

The repeatability of each question was quantified using44:

RC=2.77sw, 5

where sw is the within-condition standard deviation, estimated using the root mean square error from a one-way ANOVA. In the ANOVA, the factor was the subject, the levels were the control parameter sets, and the dependent variable was the VAS answers for one question. The RC was determined for all subjects combined and for each subject individually. For a given set of control parameters, the cycle 2 answer was expected to be within ±RC of the cycle 1 answer in 95% of the cases.

To summarize the results of the repeatability analysis, Bland-Altman plots were constructed44. In these plots, the between-cycle difference Δ=c1-c2 was plotted against the mean VAS score μ=(c1+c2)/2, where c1 is the response to a condition during the first cycle, and c2 is the response to the same set of exoskeleton control parameters during the second cycle.

The percentage of trials with |Δ|<10 mm and |Δ|<20 mm were calculated to quantify the number of cases that had acceptable consistency. The bounds of 10 mm and 20 mm were chosen because the RC for pain is between 12 and 20 mm42,45,46. In addition, the 90% threshold was calculated. The 90% threshold was defined as the difference magnitude for which 90% of responses had absolute differences less than the threshold.

Relative comfort consistency

To assess how consistently subjects compared consecutive pairs of conditions, the VAS responses were converted into relative rankings. Specifically, the VAS responses for each trial were relabeled as “higher”, “lower”, or “no change” from the previous trial. The tolerance for “no change” was set to 2 mm, meaning that responses with a difference below the tolerance were considered the same. For example, if a subject responded 60 for trial 1, 70 for trial 2, and 69 for trial 3, the relative response of trial 2 was higher than trial 1 but the same as trial 3. The 2 mm threshold was chosen because there were multiple cases in which one question had a large difference and the other question had a tiny, but non-zero, difference from the previous trial’s answer. Thus, it appeared that the subject only wanted to change their answer for one question but did not give exactly the same answer for the other question. The 2 mm threshold accommodated this.

To quantify consistency, the relative rankings between cycles 1 and 2 were compared (Table 4). For example, the relative ranking between trials 1 and 2 was compared to the relative ranking between trials 11 and 12. If the relative rankings for a given condition were the same, then that condition was consistent. The percentage of consistent conditions over all subjects was found for each question.

Table 4.

Consistency was evaluated by comparing the relative response from cycles 1 and 2.

Cycle 2 Cycle 1
Higher No change Lower
Higher × ×
No change × ×
Lower × ×

A indicates a consistent relative response, and an × represents an inconsistent relative response.

Supplementary Information

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 1930430.

Author contributions

M.M.E.H. and A.E.M. conceived the experiment, M.M.E.H. and A.M. conducted the experiment, M.M.E.H. analyzed the results, M.M.E.H. and A.E.M. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Data availibility

The data and analysis code are provided as a supplemental file.

Competing interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-023-47430-z.

References

  • 1.Mahlknecht P, et al. Prevalence and burden of gait disorders in elderly men and women aged 60–97 years: A population-based study. Public Libr. Sci. One. 2013;8:e69627. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tremblay MS, Colley RC, Saunders TJ, Healy GN, Owen N. Physiological and health implications of a sedentary lifestyle. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2010;35:725–740. doi: 10.1139/H10-079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Rodríguez-Fernández A, Lobo-Prat J, Font-Llagunes JM. Systematic review on wearable lower-limb exoskeletons for gait training in neuromuscular impairments. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2021;18:22. doi: 10.1186/s12984-021-00815-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.de Miguel-Fernández J, Lobo-Prat J, Prinsen E, Font-Llagunes JM, Marchal-Crespo L. Control strategies used in lower limb exoskeletons for gait rehabilitation after brain injury: A systematic review and analysis of clinical effectiveness. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2023;20:23. doi: 10.1186/s12984-023-01144-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.de Looze MP, Bosch T, Krause F, Stadler KS, O’Sullivan LW. Exoskeletons for industrial application and their potential effects on physical work load. Ergonomics. 2016;59:671–681. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2015.1081988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Crea S, et al. Occupational exoskeletons: A roadmap toward large-scale adoption. Methodology and challenges of bringing exoskeletons to workplaces. Wearable Technol. 2021;2:e11. doi: 10.1017/wtc.2021.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Umer W, Antwi-Afari MF, Li H, Szeto GPY, Wong AYL. The prevalence of musculoskeletal symptoms in the construction industry: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health. 2018;91:125–144. doi: 10.1007/s00420-017-1273-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Jacquier-Bret J, Gorce P. Prevalence of body area work-related musculoskeletal disorders among healthcare professionals: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2023;20:841. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20010841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.da Costa BR, Vieira ER. Risk factors for work-related musculoskeletal disorders: A systematic review of recent longitudinal studies. Am. J. Ind. Med. 2010;53:285–323. doi: 10.1002/ajim.20750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.De Bock S, et al. Benchmarking occupational exoskeletons: An evidence mapping systematic review. Appl. Ergon. 2022;98:103582. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2021.103582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Sanchez-Villamañan MDC, Gonzalez-Vargas J, Torricelli D, Moreno JC, Pons JL. Compliant lower limb exoskeletons: A comprehensive review on mechanical design principles. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2019;16:55. doi: 10.1186/s12984-019-0517-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wolff J, Parker C, Borisoff J, Mortenson WB, Mattie J. A survey of stakeholder perspectives on exoskeleton technology. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2014;11:169. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Elprama SA, Vanderborght B, Jacobs A. An industrial exoskeleton user acceptance framework based on a literature review of empirical studies. Appl. Ergon. 2022;100:103615. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2021.103615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Andrade, C. & Nathan-Roberts, D. Occupational exoskeleton adoption and acceptance in construction and industrial work: A scoping review, in Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, Vol. 66, 1325–1329. 10.1177/1071181322661415 (2022).
  • 15.Kuber PM, Abdollahi M, Alemi MM, Rashedi E. A systematic review on evaluation strategies for field assessment of upper-body industrial exoskeletons: Current practices and future trends. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 2022;50:1203–1231. doi: 10.1007/s10439-022-03003-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kuber PM, Rashedi E. Product ergonomics in industrial exoskeletons: Potential enhancements for workforce efficiency and safety. Theor. Issues Ergon. Sci. 2021;22:729–752. doi: 10.1080/1463922X.2020.1850905. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Tucker, M. et al. Human preference-based learning for high-dimensional optimization of exoskeleton walking gaits, in IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS), 3423–3430, 10.1109/IROS45743.2020.9341416 (2020).
  • 18.Tucker, M. et al. Preference-based learning for exoskeleton gait optimization, in IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation (ICRA)10.1109/ICRA40945.2020.9196661 (2020).
  • 19.Ingraham KA, Remy CD, Rouse EJ. The role of user preference in the customized control of robotic exoskeletons. Sci. Robot. 2022;7:eabj3487. doi: 10.1126/scirobotics.abj3487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Li, K. et al. ROIAL: Region of interest active learning for characterizing exoskeleton gait preference landscapes, in IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation (ICRA), 3212–3218, 10.1109/ICRA48506.2021.9560840 (2021).
  • 21.Young AJ, Foss J, Gannon H, Ferris DP. Influence of power delivery timing on the energetics and biomechanics of humans wearing a hip exoskeleton. Front. Bioeng. Biotechnol. 2017;5:4. doi: 10.3389/fbioe.2017.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Shepherd MK, Azocar AF, Major MJ, Rouse EJ. Amputee perception of prosthetic ankle stiffness during locomotion. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2018;15:99. doi: 10.1186/s12984-018-0432-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Shepherd MK, Rouse EJ. Comparing preference of ankle–foot stiffness in below-knee amputees and prosthetists. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:16067. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-72131-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Shepherd MK, Simon AM, Zisk J, Hargrove L. Patient-preferred prosthetic ankle alignment for level ground and ramp walking. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2020;29:52–59. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2020.3033711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Clites TR, Shepherd MK, Ingraham KA, Wontorcik L, Rouse EJ. Understanding patient preference in prosthetic ankle stiffness. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2021;18:128. doi: 10.1186/s12984-021-00916-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Alili A, et al. A novel framework to facilitate user preferred tuning for a robotic knee prosthesis. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2023;31:895–903. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2023.3236217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Thatte N, Duan H, Geyer H. A sample-efficient black-box optimizer to train policies for human-in-the-loop systems with user preferences. IEEE Robot. Autom. Lett. 2017;2:993–1000. doi: 10.1109/LRA.2017.2656948. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Shetty VS, Lee UH, Ingraham K, Rouse E. A data driven approach for predicting preferred ankle stiffness of a quasi-passive prosthesis. IEEE Robot. Autom. Lett. 2022;7:3467–3474. doi: 10.1109/LRA.2022.3144790. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Díaz MA, et al. Human-in-the-loop optimization of wearable robotic devices to improve human–robot interaction: A systematic review. IEEE Trans. Cybern. 2022 doi: 10.1109/TCYB.2022.3224895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Maxwell C. Sensitivity and accuracy of the visual analogue scale: A psycho- physical classroom experiment. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 1978;6:15–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.1978.tb01676.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Mündermann A, Nigg BM, Stefanyshyn DJ, Humble RN. Development of a reliable method to assess footwear comfort during running. Gait Posture. 2002;16:38–45. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00197-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Downie WW, et al. Studies with pain rating scales. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 1978;37:378–381. doi: 10.1136/ard.37.4.378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Hasson D, Bengt BA. Validation and findings comparing VAS vs. Likert scales for psychosocial measurements. Int. Electron. J. Health Educ. 2005;8:178–192. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Sung Y-T, Wu J-S. The visual analogue scale for rating, ranking and paired-comparison (VAS-RRP): A new technique for psychological measurement. Behav. Res. Methods. 2018;50:1694–1715. doi: 10.3758/s13428-018-1041-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wewers ME, Lowe NK. A critical review of visual analogue scales in the measurement of clinical phenomena. Res. Nurs. Health. 1990;13:227–236. doi: 10.1002/nur.4770130405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Scott J, Huskisson EC. Accuracy of subjective measurements made with or without previous scores: An important source of error in serial measurement of subjective states. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 1979;38:558–559. doi: 10.1136/ard.38.6.558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Goffin RD, Olson JM. Is it all relative?: Comparative judgments and the possible improvement of self-ratings and ratings of others. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2011;6:48–60. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Campbell WI, Lewis S. Visual analogue measurement of pain. Ulst. Med. J. 1990;59:149–154. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Scott J, Huskisson EC. Graphic representation of pain. Pain. 1976;2:175–184. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(76)90113-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hjermstad MJ, et al. Studies comparing numerical rating scales, verbal rating scales, and visual analogue scales for assessment of pain intensity in adults: A systematic literature review. J. Pain Symptom Manag. 2011;41:1073–1093. doi: 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2010.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Joyce CRB, Zutshi DW, Hrubes V, Mason RM. Comparison of fixed interval and visual analogue scales for rating chronic pain. Eur. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 1975;8:415–420. doi: 10.1007/BF00562315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bailey B, Gravel J, Daoust R. Reliability of the visual analog scale in children with acute pain in the emergency department. Pain. 2012;153:839–842. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bartlett JW, Frost C. Reliability, repeatability and reproducibility: Analysis of measurement errors in continuous variables. Ultrasound Obstet. Gynecol. 2008;31:466–475. doi: 10.1002/uog.5256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bland JM, Altman DG. Measuring agreement in method comparison studies. Stat. Methods Med. Res. 1999;8:135–160. doi: 10.1177/096228029900800204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bijur PE, Silver W, Gallagher EJ. Reliability of the visual analog scale for measurement of acute pain. Acad. Emerg. Med. 2001;8:1153–1157. doi: 10.1111/j.1553-2712.2001.tb01132.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.DeLoach LJ, Higgins MS, Caplan AB, Stiff JL. The visual analog scale in the immediate postoperative period: Intrasubject variability and correlation with a numeric scale. Anesth. Analg. 1998;86:102–106. doi: 10.1097/00000539-199801000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Gordon KE, Ferris DP. Learning to walk with a robotic ankle exoskeleton. J. Biomech. 2007;40:2636–2644. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2006.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Poggensee KL, Collins SH. How adaptation, training, and customization contribute to benefits from exoskeleton assistance. Sci. Robot. 2021;6:eabf1078. doi: 10.1126/scirobotics.abf1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Peng X, Acosta-Sojo Y, Wu MI, Stirling L. Actuation timing perception of a powered ankle exoskeleton and its associated ankle angle changes during walking. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 2022;30:869–877. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2022.3162213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Medrano RL, Thomas GC, Rouse EJ. Can humans perceive the metabolic benefit provided by augmentative exoskeletons? J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 2022;19:26. doi: 10.1186/s12984-022-01002-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Bohannon RW. Comfortable and maximum walking speed of adults aged 20–79 years: Reference values and determinants. Age Ageing. 1997;26:15–19. doi: 10.1093/ageing/26.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Schmitthenner D, Martin AE. Performance of position, force, and impedance controllers for a pneumatic cylinder ankle exoskeleton. J. Dyn. Syst. Meas. Control. 2023 doi: 10.1115/1.4063172. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric Theory. 3. McGraw-Hill; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Perreault WD. Controlling order-effect bias. Public Opin. Q. 1975;39(544–551):2748507. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Landon EL. Order bias, the ideal rating, and the semantic differential. J. Mark. Res. 1971;8:375–378. doi: 10.1177/002224377100800317. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The data and analysis code are provided as a supplemental file.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES