Skip to main content

This is a preprint.

It has not yet been peer reviewed by a journal.

The National Library of Medicine is running a pilot to include preprints that result from research funded by NIH in PMC and PubMed.

bioRxiv logoLink to bioRxiv
[Preprint]. 2023 Nov 13:2023.11.08.565517. [Version 1] doi: 10.1101/2023.11.08.565517

Socioeconomic resources in youth are linked to divergent patterns of network integration and segregation across the brain’s transmodal axis

Cleanthis Michael 1, Aman Taxali 2, Mike Angstadt 2, Omid Kardan 2, Alexander Weigard 2, M Fiona Molloy 2, Katherine L McCurry 2, Luke W Hyde 1,3, Mary M Heitzeg 2, Chandra Sripada 2
PMCID: PMC10680554  PMID: 38014302

Abstract

Socioeconomic resources (SER) calibrate the developing brain to the current context, which can confer or attenuate risk for psychopathology across the lifespan. Recent multivariate work indicates that SER levels powerfully influence intrinsic functional connectivity patterns across the entire brain. Nevertheless, the neurobiological meaning of these widespread alterations remains poorly understood, despite its translational promise for early risk identification, targeted intervention, and policy reform. In the present study, we leverage the resources of graph theory to precisely characterize multivariate and univariate associations between household SER and the functional integration and segregation (i.e., participation coefficient, within-module degree) of brain regions across major cognitive, affective, and sensorimotor systems during the resting state in 5,821 youth (ages 9–10 years) from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. First, we establish that decomposing the brain into profiles of integration and segregation captures more than half of the multivariate association between SER and functional connectivity with greater parsimony (100-fold reduction in number of features) and interpretability. Second, we show that the topological effects of SER are not uniform across the brain; rather, higher SER levels are related to greater integration of somatomotor and subcortical systems, but greater segregation of default mode, orbitofrontal, and cerebellar systems. Finally, we demonstrate that the effects of SER are spatially patterned along the unimodal-transmodal gradient of brain organization. These findings provide critical interpretive context for the established and widespread effects of SER on brain organization, indicating that SER levels differentially configure the intrinsic functional architecture of developing unimodal and transmodal systems. This study highlights both sensorimotor and higher-order networks that may serve as neural markers of environmental stress and opportunity, and which may guide efforts to scaffold healthy neurobehavioral development among disadvantaged communities of youth.

Introduction

Socioeconomic resources (SER) powerfully influence concurrent and lifelong outcomes, especially during childhood and adolescence when environmental experiences have strong and cascading effects on health and functioning (14). For example, household SER levels in youth, typically measured through family income, parental education, and neighborhood resources, have been associated with disparities in educational and occupational attainment, cognitive and socioemotional functioning, and physical (e.g., cardiovascular disease, cancer) and mental health (e.g., anxiety, depression, suicide, criminality, substance use) (59). Elucidating the biological mechanisms through which SER levels instigate pathways of vulnerability and resilience can inform early risk identification, facilitate targeted intervention, and encourage reform of public policies implicated in socioeconomic and mental health inequities.

Technological and computational advancements in non-invasive neuroimaging methods have allowed researchers to demonstrate that SER levels may influence behavior through their impact on brain function and development (10,11). Concurrently, there is increased recognition that the brain constitutes a complex network of interconnected regions (12,13). Task-free “resting-state” functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) uses coherence in spontaneous activity across brain regions to yield maps of functional connectivity patterns that reflect neural communication within and between large-scale brain networks critical for cognition and mental health (14,15).

Previous studies probing how SER levels influence resting-state functional connectivity have predominantly relied on individual, region-specific connections (e.g., amygdala-ventromedial prefrontal connectivity) (16). There is, however, convergent evidence demonstrating that socioemotional, cognitive, and psychiatric characteristics emerge from widespread profiles of tens of thousands of connections across the entire brain, rather than focal profiles involving connections between individual pairs of regions (17).

Our group has therefore recently conducted the first multivariate predictive modeling study interrogating brain-wide connectivity changes associated with household SER (18) in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study, the largest neuroimaging study of youth to date (19,20). We identified robust and generalizable associations between SER and resting-state functional connectivity, with connectivity changes explaining 9% of the variance in SER out-of-sample – a relatively large effect size in the social sciences (21). These connectivity changes were widespread across most pairs of brain networks (72 out of 110 network pairs). A key limitation of this work, however, is in terms of interpretation. While we observed complex and widespread connectivity alterations associated with SER, the neurobiological meaning of these alterations remains elusive.

In the present study, we address this knowledge gap by leveraging the resources of graph theory (22). The human brain is organized into multiple intrinsic connectivity networks (ICNs) (2325). ICNs exhibit developmental refinements in profiles of segregation (i.e., the degree of neural communication within distinct, functionally specialized networks) and integration (i.e., the degree of neural communication across different networks) during sensitive developmental windows (2629). Integration and segregation are reflected in a pair of graph theoretic metrics that describe between-network connectivity (participation coefficient) and within-network connectivity (within-module degree) (30). Profiles of higher participation coefficient and lower within-module degree reflect integration, while the reverse reflects segregation (31).

ICNs are organized along a unimodal-transmodal gradient, which represents the degree to which networks are specialized for encoding specific sensory features versus integrating representations across modalities (3234). Motor and sensory processing networks anchor the unimodal end, heteromodal networks occupy the middle range, and association networks anchor the transmodal end (3234). Across development, unimodal networks become more integrated and transmodal networks become more segregated (27,29). SER levels have been previously associated with functional network integration/segregation in youth (3537). As different ICNs exhibit unique developmental refinements based on their position on the sensorimotor-association gradient, the topological effects of SER may differ along the transmodal axis, though this possibility currently remains unclear.

Multiple ecological mechanisms associated with SER (e.g., parental stimulation, school quality, nutrition, neighborhood adversity) may influence coordinated patterns of ICN organization, especially in terms of integration and segregation (11,38). Thus, in the present study, we quantify multivariate and univariate associations between household SER and the within-module degree and participation coefficient of 418 nodes across 15 major ICNs throughout the brain. Moreover, we assess potential ICN-specific effects of SER (e.g., greater segregation and lower integration in certain networks; the reverse in others). Finally, we interrogate whether the effects of SER on network integration/segregation are spatially patterned along the sensorimotor-association axis.

We performed our analyses in the ABCD Study, a population-based consortium study of 11,875 9- and 10-year-olds with substantial sociodemographic diversity (39). As in our prior report (18), we constructed a latent factor of SER across household and neighborhood contexts. We establish that SER has robust relationships with network integration/segregation, which account for most of the association between SER and the entire functional connectome. Furthermore, we delineate network-specific effects, with higher SER related to greater integration of sensorimotor networks but greater segregation of association networks. Lastly, we demonstrate that the effects of SER strongly relate to the transmodal axis. These findings add valuable interpretive information by suggesting that the associations between SER and functional connectivity spatially conform to the sensorimotor-association axis during development. Such insights may elucidate neural markers of environmental stress and opportunity. Moreover, they may guide interventions that support patterns of brain organization linked to enhanced executive functioning and emotional wellbeing during early adolescence, a critical window when many psychosocial challenges emerge (26,28,40).

Materials and Methods

1. Sample and Data

The ABCD Study is a multisite longitudinal study with 11,875 children between 9–10 years of age from 22 sites across the United States. The study conforms to the rules and procedures of each site’s Institutional Review Board, and all participants provide informed consent (parents) or assent (children). Data for this study are from ABCD Release 3.0.

2. Data Acquisition, fMRI Preprocessing, and Connectome Generation

High spatial (2.4mm isotropic) and temporal resolution (TR = 800ms) resting-state fMRI was acquired in four separate runs (5min per run, 20min total). Preprocessing was performed using fMRIPrep v1.5.0 (41). Briefly, T1-weighted (T1w) and T2-weighted images were run through recon-all using FreeSurfer v6.0.1, spatially normalized, rigidly coregistered to the T1, motion corrected, normalized to standard space, and transformed to CIFTI space.

Connectomes were generated for each functional run using the Gordon 333 parcel atlas (42), augmented with parcels from high-resolution subcortical (43) and cerebellar (44) atlases. Volumes exceeding a framewise displacement (FD) threshold of 0.5mm were marked to be censored. Covariates were regressed out of the time series in a single step, including: linear trend, 24 motion parameters (original translations/rotations + derivatives + quadratics), aCompCorr 5 CSF and 5 WM components and ICA-AROMA aggressive components, high-pass filtering at 0.008Hz, and censored volumes. Next, correlation matrices were calculated. Full details of preprocessing and connectome generation are reported in the Supplement and the automatically-generated fMRIPrep Supplement.

3. Inclusion/Exclusion

There are 11,875 subjects in the ABCD Release 3.0 dataset. Subjects were excluded for: failing ABCD QC, insufficient number of runs each 4 minutes or greater, failing visual QC of registrations and normalizations, and missing data required for regression modeling. This left us with N = 5,821 subjects across 19 sites for the main analysis. Details of exclusions are provided in the Supplement.

4. Graph Theoretic Analysis

Since most graph theory measures require unsigned edge weights, each subject’s connectome resulted in two separate sets of graphs – one for the collection of positive edges and another for the negatively weighted edges (45,46). We focused on positive graphs consistent with previous graph theoretical investigations (45,46), though supplementary analyses revealed that negative graphs did not add meaningful predictive information (see Supplement).

Within-Module Degree is a node-wise measure which captures each node’s degree (i.e., the magnitude of summed connectivity weights) specifically within the node’s own network. This measure is a modification of the “module degree z-score” metric (30), but without within-network z-scoring of node degree to better capture differences across participants, rather than differences across nodes within each network. Formally, the within-module degree of a node i is given by:

j=1Nieij

where eij is the edge weight between nodes i and j, and Ni is the set of nodes incident to node i that are in the same network as i.

Participation Coefficient is a node-wise measure that captures the diversity of a node’s connections with other nodes outside of its own network (30). Intuitively, if a node distributes its connectivity evenly across all networks, its participation coefficient will be 1, while departures from equality yield commensurately lower scores. Formally, the participation coefficient of a node i is given by:

1mM(ei(m)ei)2

where M is the set of networks, ei(m) is the sum of edge weights between node i and all nodes in network m and ei is the sum of edge weights between node i and all other nodes.

For both metrics, we used the community structure defined by the applied parcellation schemes to determine network boundaries. Within-module degree (MDP) and participation coefficient (PCP) for positive edges were calculated for 418 nodes, yielding 836 node-wise graph theoretic features per participant.

To quantify the multivariate relationship between these 836 graph theoretic metrics and SER, we used principal component regression (PCR) predictive modeling (47,48) (Figure S2). Briefly, this method performs dimensionality reduction on the set of predictive features (i.e., graph theoretic metrics), fits a regression model on the resulting components (where the number of components is determined in nested cross-validation), and applies this model out-of-sample in a leave-one-site-out cross-validation framework. We control for multiple nuisance covariates, including sex assigned at birth, parent-reported race/ethnicity, age, age-squared, mean FD, and mean FD-squared. We controlled for race/ethnicity, a social construct, to account for differences in exposure to personal/systemic racism, disadvantage, and opportunity among people of color (49,50). We assessed statistical significance with non-parametric permutation tests, using the procedure of Freedman and Lane (51) to account for covariates. Exchangeability blocks were used to account for twin, family, and site structure and were entered into Permutation Analysis of Linear Models (PALM) (52) to produce permutation orderings. Details on these analyses are provided in the Supplement.

5. Latent Variable Modeling

We constructed a latent variable for SER by applying exploratory factor analysis to household income-to-needs, parental education, and neighborhood disadvantage (18). Household income-to-needs represents the ratio of a household’s income relative to its need based on family size (details provided in the Supplement). Parental education was the average educational achievement of parents or caregivers. Neighborhood disadvantage scores reflect an ABCD consortium-supplied variable (reshist_addr1_adi_wsum). In brief, participants’ primary home address was used to generate Area Deprivation Index (ADI) values (53), which were weighted based on results from Kind et al. (54) to create an aggregate measure. Additional details on construction of this latent variable are provided in the Supplement.

6. Code Availability

The ABCD data used in this report came from NDA Study 901, 10.15154/1520591, which can be found at https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=901. The subsample used for this study can be found at NDA DOI: 10.15154/ebhq-f780. Code for running analyses can be found at https://github.com/SripadaLab/ABCD_Resting_SER_GraphTheory.

Results

1. Within-module degree and participation coefficient are strongly related to household socioeconomic resources

As reported in our previous study (18), using leave-one-site-out cross-validation (LOSO-CV), the out-of-sample multivariate relationship between SER and the whole connectome (reflecting 87,153 connections) was rcv = 0.274, pPERM < 0.0001. Against this benchmark result, we found that the LOSO-CV out-of-sample multivariate relationship between SER and these 836 node-wise graph theoretic measures (i.e., MDP, PCP) was rcv = 0.162, pPERM < 0.0001. Thus, the linear MDP/PCP-SER relationship is 59.1% as strong as the whole connectome-SER relationship.

We next examined whether the 836 MDP/PCP features reflect distinct or overlapping variance in predicting SER relative to the 87,153 connections of the entire functional connectome. To assess this, we built a stacked model by taking the SER predictions from the full connectome predictive model, and the MDP/PCP predictive model, and entering them as predictors of SER in a new regression. This stacked model’s LOSO-CV out-of-sample performance was rcv= 0.268; that is, the stacked model with the addition of graph theory features performed no better than the full connectome model by itself.

These results suggest two conclusions. First, the graph theoretic features represent a subset of the variance explained by the whole connectome. Second, there is strong concentration of SER predictivity in the graph theoretic features, wherein these 836 graph theoretic features account for the majority of the multivariate relationship between the functional connectome and SER.

2. Associations between household socioeconomic resources and patterns of integration/segregation differ across intrinsic connectivity networks

In Figure 1, we display standardized regression weights for 418 node-wise MDP metrics and 418 PCP metrics, each regression weight arising from separate regression models predicting SER from the respective metric (with controls for nuisance covariates). The plot highlights strongly divergent relationships with SER across different ICNs, with four notable zones. Zone 0 contains the majority of nodes that lack statistically significant relations with SER. In Zone 1, we observe large, positive SER-predictive betas for MDP in default mode network, an unlabeled network (dubbed “None”) primarily anchored in orbitofrontal cortex, and cerebellum, indicating greater segregation of nodes within these networks with higher SER. In Zone 2, we observe large, positive SER-predictive betas for PCP primarily in subcortical networks, indicating greater integration of nodes within this network with higher SER. In Zone 3, we observe large SER-predictive betas for both MDP (negative betas) and PCP (positive betas) primarily with nodes in the somatomotor network, indicating greater integration of nodes within this network with higher SER.

Figure 1: Profile Plot Showing Relation Between Within-Module Degree and Participation Coefficient Beta Weights When Predicting Socioeconomic Resources by Network Affiliation.

Figure 1:

We computed beta weights from 836 regression models in which socioeconomic resource (SER) scores were the outcome variable predicted by 418 metrics of within-module degree for positive edges (MDP) and 418 metrics of participation coefficient for positive edges (PCP). Each node’s pair of SER-predictive betas (for MDP and PCP) is shown in the above “profile plot”, with nodes shaded by network affiliation. Orange lines represent the thresholds for statistically significant univariate relationships between SER and MDP/PCP metrics. Four zones are noteworthy. Zone 0 contains the majority of nodes that lack statistically significant relations with SER. Zone 1 nodes exhibit positive SER-predictive betas for MDP, consistent with greater segregation of these nodes with higher SER. Zones 2 and 3 exhibit higher SER-predictive betas for PCP (Zone 2 and 3) and lower SER-predictive betas for MDP (Zone 3), consistent with greater integration of these nodes with higher SER. Somatomotor-hand, in the upper left, stands out as exhibiting particularly extensive integration with higher SER. CinguloOperc = Cingulo-Opercular Network. DorsalAttn = Dorsal Attention Network. SMhand = Somatomotor Hand Network. SMmouth = Somatomotor Mouth Network. VentralAttn = Ventral Attention Network.

3. Household socioeconomic resource levels exhibit divergent relationships with network integration/segregation across the brain’s unimodal-transmodal gradient

Given differences in the segregation/integration profiles of different ICNs in relation to SER, we next examined whether these differences are associated with the transmodality axis. We used transmodality scores from a previous report by Margulies and colleagues (34), which locates nodes along a gradient with sensory processing networks at one end (lowest transmodality scores) and higher-order association networks at the other end (highest transmodality scores). We found that transmodality scores exhibited a strong positive relationship with SER-predictive betas for MDP (r = 0.42, pPERM < 0.007), and a moderate negative relationship with SER-predictive betas for PCP (r = 0.21) that only trended toward significance (pPERM < 0.09). These results provide quantitative support for divergent SER effects across the transmodality gradient, with SER yielding greater integration (lower MDP regression weights, higher PCP regression weights) at the sensorimotor processing pole and greater segregation (higher MDP regression weights, lower PCP regression weights) at the higher-order processing pole.

Discussion

Household SER levels across childhood and adolescence calibrate structural and functional neurodevelopment, with potent implications for physical health, occupational attainment, and emotional wellbeing across the lifespan (10,11,55). In the present report, we leverage graph theory and the largest neuroimaging cohort of youth to date to delineate how variation in household SER becomes biologically expressed along the developing functional architecture of cognitive, affective, and sensorimotor brain systems. We found that SER was robustly associated with two graph theoretic metrics that decompose brain organization in terms of integration and segregation. Importantly, the topological effects of SER were not uniform across the brain; rather, higher SER levels were related to greater integration of somatomotor and subcortical systems, but greater segregation of default mode, orbitofrontal, and cerebellar systems. Finally, we demonstrate that SER-related network reconfiguration was spatially patterned along the brain’s transmodal axis. These findings provide critical interpretive context for the established and widespread effects of SER on the intrinsic functional architecture of the developing brain.

Previous studies characterizing the neurobiological embedding of SER have primarily examined connections between individual pairs of regions (e.g., frontolimbic connectivity) (16). Given the brain-wide effects of SER (36,37,56), and the thousands of connections that undergird complex and clinically relevant phenotypes (17), our group recently conducted the first multivariate predictive modeling study of household SER in the ABCD Study (18). We revealed that the correlation between actual SER and SER predicted from 87,153 functional connections at rest was 0.27, yet the neuroscientific meaning of these findings remained unclear. In this study, we applied graph theory to distill these 87,153 connections into only 836 features that describe the effects of SER with greater neurobiological interpretability in terms of intra- and inter-network relationships. Specifically, we assessed node-level integration and segregation using participation coefficient (between-network connectivity) and within-module degree (within-network connectivity), and we demonstrate that these two metrics capture more than half of the original association with SER (r = 0.16). These findings indicate that these two nodal graph properties largely capture the backbone of functional brain architecture, particularly in relation to SER.

Segregation gives rise to differentiated networks that execute specialized cognitive functions, whereas integration efficiently coordinates these processing streams across the brain (57,58). A combination of high segregation and high integration represents an “optimized” small-world architecture that rapidly integrates specialized, multimodal information at low wiring and energy costs (59,60). Our multivariate findings therefore suggest that the developmental construction of an “optimal” small-world-like configuration may be impacted by SER.

To spatially localize the topological effects of SER, we next conducted univariate analyses probing the within-module degree and participation coefficient of brain regions within 15 major ICNs. First, we found that higher SER levels were associated with greater segregation (higher within-module degree) of the default mode network, an unlabeled network (dubbed “None”) primarily anchored in orbitofrontal cortex, and the cerebellum. These systems are respectively purported to support self-referential and introspective cognition, reward processing and decision-making, and cognitive and motor control (25,6163) and have been previously linked with SER, despite some inconsistencies in directionality (36,37,56,64,65). As segregation of these systems is associated with attention, cognitive control, and impulsivity (62,66,67), these alterations may represent a mechanistic pathway from socioeconomic gradients to goal-directed, regulatory behavior in youth.

Second, we found that higher SER scores were associated with greater functional integration (higher participation coefficient) of the subcortical network implicated in motor planning, threat and safety learning, and emotion processing (6871). These findings converge with extensive evidence linking SER to structural, functional, and connectivity profiles of subcortical regions, such as the amygdala and hippocampus (7274). Given their dense expression of glucocorticoid receptors (75,76), these structures may be particularly sensitive to both nurturing and stressful experiences often associated with SER (11,77). Integration of subcortical regions with cortical systems subserves adaptive emotional learning and regulation (71,78), indicating a plausible network-level neural basis for documented links between poverty and psychopathology (3,6,8).

Lastly, higher SER levels were strongly associated with greater functional integration (lower within-module degree, higher participation coefficient) of the somatomotor hand network. This network is not commonly considered in theoretical accounts linking SER to brain development (10,11,79,80), despite being consistently implicated in SER and transdiagnostic psychopathology in individual studies (36,56,81,82). The somatomotor network supports motor planning and execution (25), and recent data point to its potential involvement in a “somato-cognitive action” network that integrates motoric function with goal-directed planning (83). One possibility is that SER levels not only calibrate association systems that generate and evaluate abstract cognitive representations, but also somatomotor systems that translate these abstract representations into goal-relevant behavior. These findings highlight the need for theoretical accounts and empirical studies to further delineate how adversity constrains or reconfigures somatomotor development to confer vulnerability and resilience.

Since SER displayed divergent associations with the integration/segregation of different ICNs, we investigated whether this heterogeneity could be explained by considering how ICNs are organized along the brain’s unimodal-transmodal axis. This evolutionarily rooted, hierarchical axis of brain organization is anchored by sensory and motor networks on one end and association networks on the other (3234). This sensorimotor-association gradient captures developmental sequences of multiple neurobiological properties, from structure and myelination to plasticity and gene expression (26,84). In the present investigation, we hypothesized that this axis may also provide a unifying framework for characterizing the network-specific effects of household SER. Consistent with this hypothesis, we found that associations between SER and functional network integration/segregation were indeed spatially patterned along the transmodal axis, with higher SER levels associated with greater integration at the unimodal/somatosensory pole and greater segregation at the transmodal/association pole.

Over the course of neurodevelopment from childhood to young adulthood, lower-order unimodal networks (e.g., somatomotor network) become more integrated, whereas higher-order association networks (e.g., default mode network) become more segregated (27,29). Thus, the construction of integrated somatomotor systems and segregated association systems may represent a universal milestone of functional neurodevelopment. Against this backdrop, our findings suggest that higher SER may facilitate the emergence of this sensorimotor-association hierarchy. Conversely, lower SER may predict developmental lags in the emergence of this configuration, consistent with cross-sectional and longitudinal findings suggesting disadvantage-related delays in the pace of neurodevelopment (37,74,8588). Candidate mechanisms for protracted brain development following disadvantage include material hardship (e.g., resource access, lower-quality nutrition), less complex social and cognitive stimulation (e.g., under-resourced schools, complex reading materials), and toxicant exposure (e.g., lead, particulate matter) (11,86). These exposures may alter synaptic proliferation and pruning, and ultimately maturational refinements in functional network communication (integration) and specialization (segregation) (8991),

Nevertheless, an alternative interpretation of our findings is that developmental trajectories and milestones of brain organization may differ as a function of household SER. In other words, the trajectory and outcome of neurodevelopment may be qualitatively different depending on SER. While higher-SER youth may establish an integrated unimodal and segregated transmodal pole with development, lower-SER youth may develop distinct profiles of integration/segregation. These distinct neural profiles may allow youth to successfully navigate the unique demands of disadvantaged environments but may also manifest in cognitive and socioemotional challenges across the lifespan. The former hypothesis is consistent with data indicating that functional connectivity patterns that optimize cognition differ in high- versus low-SER contexts (92), as well as a recent review of longitudinal studies concluding that disadvantage may engender unique, rather than temporally shifted, trajectories of structural brain development (91).

In a separate report (in preparation), we repeated our analyses evaluating associations between sleep duration, rather than SER, with the functional integration/segregation of the same 15 ICNs in the ABCD Study. Strikingly, we found that sleep duration displayed similar but even stronger associations with functional network architecture. Consistent with the reported effects of SER, these associations were strongest for the organization of the somatomotor network, such that youth who sleep for a longer duration exhibit a more integrated somatomotor network. These findings accord with recent studies linking sleep quality to somatomotor connectivity (9395) and suggest that somatomotor architecture may represent a robust neural marker associated with multiple forms of environmental stress, adversity, and opportunity during development.

Our study has several limitations that will be important to address in future research. First, our analyses are cross-sectional and thus do not support inferences about the direction of causality of associations or about patterns of neurodevelopment. As neuroimaging data from future ABCD waves are released, future studies should disentangle causal effects and assess how the spatially divergent effects of SER unfold longitudinally across development. Second, SER scores in the ABCD Study are overall higher compared to the national population, an issue that is further exacerbated by our exclusion criteria (e.g., cutoffs for excessive head motion) (96,97); thus, caution should be exercised when attempting to generalize our findings to the broader population in the United States and worldwide. Lastly, in our previous multivariate study of SER (18), granular analyses demarcated that parental education was the primary factor related to functional connectivity (compared to family income-to-needs and neighborhood disadvantage). Here, our focus is on interpreting and spatially localizing these multivariate effects. This focus introduces challenges in dissecting the unique role of each SER component, which constitutes an important future direction to inform priorities for policy, prevention, and intervention.

In sum, the present study provides essential neuroscientific meaning to the established and widespread effects of household SER on intrinsic functional connectivity. By integrating methodological advancements in network neuroscience with theoretical frameworks of brain organization, we demonstrate that associations between SER and profiles of network integration/segregation in youth unfold differentially along the brain’s transmodal axis, with stronger effects on default mode, cerebellar, subcortical, and somatomotor networks. Our findings illustrate that SER levels may calibrate the intrinsic graphical architecture of the developing brain, highlighting the importance of prevention and intervention efforts that facilitate the development of cognitive, affective, and sensorimotor processes underlying risk and resilience within disadvantaged communities of youth.

Supplementary Material

Supplement 1
media-1.pdf (319KB, pdf)

Figure 2: Scatter Plots Showing Relationships between Within-Module Degree and Participation Coefficient Beta Weights When Predicting Socioeconomic Resources with Transmodality Scores.

Figure 2:

Top Figure: We obtained transmodality scores from 418 nodes from a previous report by Margulies and colleagues (34), which locates nodes along a gradient with sensory processing networks at one end (lowest transmodality scores) and higher-order association networks at the other end (highest transmodality scores). In addition, we calculated associations between within-module degree for positive edges (MDP) scores for each of these nodes and socioeconomic resources (SER) (“SER-predictive betas for MDP”). We found a strong positive association between transmodality scores and SER-predictive betas for MDP. Bottom Figure: We performed this same analysis, but this time with participation coefficient for positive edges (PCP) scores. We found a moderate negative association between transmodality scores and SER-predictive betas for PCP. Nodes shaded by network affiliation. CinguloOperc = Cingulo-Opercular Network. DorsalAttn = Dorsal Attention Network. SMhand = Somatomotor Hand Network. SMmouth = Somatomotor Mouth Network. VentralAttn = Ventral Attention Network.

Acknowledgements

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children aged 9–10 years and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/nih-collaborators. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/principal-investigators.html. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from NDA Study 901, 10.15154/1520591, which can be found at https://nda.nih.gov/study.html?id=901.

Funding

AW was supported by K23 DA051561 and R21 MH130939. KLM, OK, and MFM were supported by T32 AA007477. CS was supported by R01 MH123458 and U01DA041106.

Funding Statement

AW was supported by K23 DA051561 and R21 MH130939. KLM, OK, and MFM were supported by T32 AA007477. CS was supported by R01 MH123458 and U01DA041106.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

References

  • 1.Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Chen E, Matthews KA. Childhood socioeconomic status and adult health. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2010;1186(1):37–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bundy JD, Mills KT, He H, LaVeist TA, Ferdinand KC, Chen J, et al. Social determinants of health and premature death among adults in the USA from 1999 to 2018: A national cohort study. Lancet Public Health. 2023;8(6):e422–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Leventhal T, Dupéré V. Neighborhood effects on children’s development in experimental and nonexperimental research. Ann Rev Dev Psychol. 2019;1(1):149–76. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Masten AS, Cicchetti D. Developmental cascades. Dev Psychopathol. 2010;22(3):491–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gallo LC, Matthews KA. Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emotions play a role? Psychol Bull. 2003;129(1):10–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund PA, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Childhood adversities and adult psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(2):113–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kaplan GA, Turrell G, Lynch JW, Everson SA, Helkala EL, Salonen JT. Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood. Int J Epidemiol. 2001;30(2):256–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.McLoyd VC. Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. Am Psychol. 1998;53(2):185–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Iemmi V, Bantjes J, Coast E, Channer K, Leone T, McDaid D, et al. Suicide and poverty in low-income and middle-income countries: A systematic review. Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(8):774–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the brain: Prospects for neuroscience-informed policy. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(7):428–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hyde LW, Gard AM, Tomlinson RC, Burt SA, Mitchell C, Monk CS. An ecological approach to understanding the developing brain: Examples linking poverty, parenting, neighborhoods, and the brain. Am Psychol. 2020;75(9):1245–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sporns O. The human connectome: A complex network. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2011;1224(1):109–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sporns O. Contributions and challenges for network models in cognitive neuroscience. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(5):652–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Smith SM, Vidaurre D, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Jenkinson M, Miller KL, et al. Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(12):666–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Castellanos FX, Di Martino A, Craddock RC, Mehta AD, Milham MP. Clinical applications of the functional connectome. NeuroImage. 2013;80:527–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Rakesh D, Whittle S. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain – A systematic review of neuroimaging findings in youth. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;130:379–407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Woo CW, Chang LJ, Lindquist MA, Wager TD. Building better biomarkers: brain models in translational neuroimaging. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20(3):365–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sripada C, Gard AM, Angstadt M, Taxali A, Greathouse T, McCurry K, et al. Socioeconomic resources are associated with distributed alterations of the brain’s intrinsic functional architecture in youth. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2022;58:101164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Karcher NR, Barch DM. The ABCD study: Understanding the development of risk for mental and physical health outcomes. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2021;46(1):131–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Volkow ND, Koob GF, Croyle RT, Bianchi DW, Gordon JA, Koroshetz WJ, et al. The conception of the ABCD study: From substance use to a broad NIH collaboration. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:4–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Funder DC, Ozer DJ. Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2(2):156–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(3):186–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, et al. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron. 2011;72(4):665–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Thomas Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Uddin LQ, Yeo BTT, Spreng RN. Towards a universal taxonomy of macro-scale functional human brain networks. Brain Topogr. 2019;32(6):926–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sydnor VJ, Larsen B, Bassett DS, Alexander-Bloch A, Fair DA, Liston C, et al. Neurodevelopment of the association cortices: Patterns, mechanisms, and implications for psychopathology. Neuron. 2021;109(18):2820–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gu S, Satterthwaite TD, Medaglia JD, Yang M, Gur RE, Gur RC, et al. Emergence of system roles in normative neurodevelopment. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2015;112(44):13681–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Keller AS, Sydnor VJ, Pines A, Fair DA, Bassett DS, Satterthwaite TD. Hierarchical functional system development supports executive function. Trends Cogn Sci. 2023;27(2):160–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pines AR, Larsen B, Cui Z, Sydnor VJ, Bertolero MA, Adebimpe A, et al. Dissociable multi-scale patterns of development in personalized brain networks. Nat Commun. 2022;13(1):2647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Guimera R, Nunes Amaral LA. Functional cartography of complex metabolic networks. Nat. 2005;433(7028):895–900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Shine JM, Aburn MJ, Breakspear M, Poldrack RA. The modulation of neural gain facilitates a transition between functional segregation and integration in the brain. eLife. 2018;7:e31130. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hilgetag CC, Goulas A, Changeux JP. A natural cortical axis connecting the outside and inside of the human brain. Netw Neurosci. 2022;6(4):950–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Huntenburg JM, Bazin PL, Margulies DS. Large-scale gradients in human cortical organization. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018;22(1):21–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A, Falkiewicz M, Huntenburg JM, Langs G, et al. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016;113(44):12574–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Gellci K, Marusak HA, Peters C, Elrahal F, Iadipaolo AS, Rabinak CA. Community and household-level socioeconomic disadvantage and functional organization of the salience and emotion network in children and adolescents. NeuroImage. 2019;184:729–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Tooley UA, Mackey AP, Ciric R, Ruparel K, Moore TM, Gur RC, et al. Associations between neighborhood SES and functional brain network development. Cereb Cortex. 2020;30(1):1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Michael C, Tillem S, Sripada CS, Burt SA, Klump KL, Hyde LW. Neighborhood poverty during childhood prospectively predicts adolescent functional brain network architecture. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2023;64:101316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Tooley UA, Bassett DS, Mackey AP. Environmental influences on the pace of brain development. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021;22(6):372–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Garavan H, Bartsch H, Conway K, Decastro A, Goldstein RZ, Heeringa S, et al. Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:16–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(12):947–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Esteban O, Markiewicz CJ, Blair RW, Moodie CA, Isik AI, Erramuzpe A, et al. fMRIPrep: A robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nat Methods. 2019;16(1):111–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Adeyemo B, Huckins JF, Kelley WM, Petersen SE. Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(1):288–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tian Y, Margulies DS, Breakspear M, Zalesky A. Topographic organization of the human subcortex unveiled with functional connectivity gradients. Nat Neurosci. 2020;23(11):1421–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Diedrichsen J, Maderwald S, Küper M, Thürling M, Rabe K, Gizewski ER, et al. Imaging the deep cerebellar nuclei: A probabilistic atlas and normalization procedure. NeuroImage. 2011;54(3):1786–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hallquist MN, Hillary FG. Graph theory approaches to functional network organization in brain disorders: A critique for a brave new small-world. Netw Neurosci. 2018;3(1):1–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage. 2010;52(3):1059–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Sripada C, Angstadt M, Rutherford S, Kessler D, Kim Y, Yee M, et al. Basic units of inter-individual variation in resting state connectomes. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):1900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Sripada C, Rutherford S, Angstadt M, Thompson WK, Luciana M, Weigard A, et al. Prediction of neurocognition in youth from resting state fMRI. Mol Psychiatry. 2020;25(12):3413–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Pager D, Shepherd H. The sociology of discrimination: Racial discrimination in employment, housing, credit, and consumer markets. Ann Rev Sociol. 2008;34(1):181–209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Roberts SO, Rizzo MT. The psychology of American racism. Am Psychol. 2021;76(3):475–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Freedman D, Lane D. A nonstochastic interpretation of reported significance levels. J Bus Econ Stat. 1983;1(4):292–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE. Permutation inference for the general linear model. NeuroImage. 2014;92:381–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Fan CC, Marshall A, Smolker H, Gonzalez MR, Tapert SF, Barch DM, et al. Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study Linked External Data (LED): Protocol and practices for geocoding and assignment of environmental data. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;52:101030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Kind AJH, Jencks S, Brock J, Yu M, Bartels C, Ehlenbach W, et al. Neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage and 30-day rehospitalization. Ann Intern Med. 2014;161(11):765–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Hyde LW, Gard AM, Tomlinson RC, Suarez GL, Westerman HB. Parents, neighborhoods, and the developing brain. Child Dev Perspect. 2022;16(3):148–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Rakesh D, Seguin C, Zalesky A, Cropley V, Whittle S. Associations between neighborhood disadvantage, resting-state functional connectivity, and behavior in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study: The moderating role of positive family and school environments. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021;6(9):877–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Cohen JR. The behavioral and cognitive relevance of time-varying, dynamic changes in functional connectivity. NeuroImage. 2018;180:515–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Sporns O. Network attributes for segregation and integration in the human brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(2):162–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Bassett DS, Bullmore E. Small-world brain networks. Neuroscientist. 2006;12(6):512–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Bassett DS, Bullmore ET. Small-world brain networks revisited. Neuroscientist. 2017;23(5):499–516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10(3):295–307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Raichle ME. The brain’s default mode network. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2015;38(1):433–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage. 2009;44(2):489–501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Machlin L, McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA. Brain structure mediates the association between socioeconomic status and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev Sci. 2020;23(1):e12844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Marshall NA, Marusak HA, Sala-Hamrick KJ, Crespo LM, Rabinak CA, Thomason ME. Socioeconomic disadvantage and altered corticostriatal circuitry in urban youth. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(5):1982–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Rolls ET, Wan Z, Cheng W, Feng J. Risk-taking in humans and the medial orbitofrontal cortex reward system. NeuroImage. 2022;249:118893. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Schmahmann JD, Guell X, Stoodley CJ, Halko MA. The theory and neuroscience of cerebellar cognition. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2019;42(1):337–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Graybiel AM, Aosaki T, Flaherty AW, Kimura M. The basal ganglia and adaptive motor control. Science. 1994;265(5180):1826–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Herman JP, Cullinan WE. Neurocircuitry of stress: Central control of the hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20(2):78–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.LeDoux J. The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 2003;23(4):727–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Meyer HC, Odriozola P, Cohodes EM, Mandell JD, Li A, Yang R, et al. Ventral hippocampus interacts with prelimbic cortex during inhibition of threat response via learned safety in both mice and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116(52):26970–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Gard AM, Maxwell AM, Shaw DS, Mitchell C, Brooks-Gunn J, McLanahan SS, et al. Beyond family-level adversities: Exploring the developmental timing of neighborhood disadvantage effects on the brain. Dev Sci. 2021;24(1):e12985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Suarez GL, Burt SA, Gard AM, Burton J, Clark DA, Klump KL, et al. The impact of neighborhood disadvantage on amygdala reactivity: Pathways through neighborhood social processes. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2022;54:101061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Hair NL, Hanson JL, Wolfe BL, Pollak SD. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(9):822–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Plotsky PM, Thrivikraman KV, Nemeroff CB, Caldji C, Sharma S, Meaney MJ. Long-term consequences of neonatal rearing on central corticotropin-releasing factor systems in adult male rat offspring. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;30(12):2192–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Lupien SJ, McEwen BS, Gunnar MR, Heim C. Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(6):434–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Ceballo R, McLoyd VC. Social support and parenting in poor, dangerous neighborhoods. Child Dev. 2002;73(4):1310–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Caballero C, Nook EC, Gee DG. Managing fear and anxiety in development: A framework for understanding the neurodevelopment of emotion regulation capacity and tendency. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2023;145:105002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(2):65–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Hackman DA, Farah MJ, Meaney MJ. Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010;11(9):651–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Kebets V, Holmes AJ, Orban C, Tang S, Li J, Sun N, et al. Somatosensory-motor dysconnectivity spans multiple transdiagnostic dimensions of psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86(10):779–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Parkes L, Moore TM, Calkins ME, Cook PA, Cieslak M, Roalf DR, et al. Transdiagnostic dimensions of psychopathology explain individuals’ unique deviations from normative neurodevelopment in brain structure. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11(1):1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Gordon EM, Chauvin RJ, Van AN, Rajesh A, Nielsen A, Newbold DJ, et al. A somato-cognitive action network alternates with effector regions in motor cortex. Nat. 2023;617(7960):351–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Sydnor VJ, Larsen B, Seidlitz J, Adebimpe A, Alexander-Bloch AF, Bassett DS, et al. Intrinsic activity development unfolds along a sensorimotor–association cortical axis in youth. Nat Neurosci. 2023;26(4):638–49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Hanson JL, Hair N, Shen DG, Shi F, Gilmore JH, Wolfe BL, et al. Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e80954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Johnson SB, Riis JL, Noble KG. State of the Art Review: Poverty and the developing brain. Pediatr. 2016;137(4):e20153075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Rakesh D, Cropley V, Zalesky A, Vijayakumar N, Allen NB, Whittle S. Neighborhood disadvantage and longitudinal brain-predicted-age trajectory during adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;51:101002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Whittle S, Vijayakumar N, Simmons JG, Dennison M, Schwartz O, Pantelis C, et al. Role of positive parenting in the association between neighborhood social disadvantage and brain development across adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(8):824–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Magariños AM, Verdugo JMG, McEwen BS. Chronic stress alters synaptic terminal structure in hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1997;94(25):14002–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Woo E, Sansing LH, Arnsten AFT, Datta D. Chronic stress weakens connectivity in the prefrontal cortex: Architectural and molecular changes. Chronic Stress. 2021;5:24705470211029254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Rakesh D, Whittle S, Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Childhood socioeconomic status and the pace of structural neurodevelopment: Accelerated, delayed, or simply different? Trends Cogn Sci. 2023;27(9):833–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Ellwood-Lowe ME, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Bunge SA. Brain network coupling associated with cognitive performance varies as a function of a child’s environment in the ABCD study. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):7183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Bai Y, Tan J, Liu X, Cui X, Li D, Yin H. Resting-state functional connectivity of the sensory/somatomotor network associated with sleep quality: evidence from 202 young male samples. Brain Imaging Behav. 2022;16(4):1832–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Zhang L, Li D, Yin H. How is psychological stress linked to sleep quality? The mediating role of functional connectivity between the sensory/somatomotor network and the cingulo-opercular control network. Brain Cogn. 2020;146:105641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Cheng W, Rolls ET, Ruan H, Feng J. Functional connectivities in the brain that mediate the association between depressive problems and sleep quality. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(10):1052–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Cosgrove KT, McDermott TJ, White EJ, Mosconi MW, Thompson WK, Paulus MP, et al. Limits to the generalizability of resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of youth: An examination of ABCD Study® baseline data. Brain Imaging Behav. 2022;16(4):1919–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Gard AM, Hyde LW, Heeringa SG, West BT, Mitchell C. Why weight? Analytic approaches for large-scale population neuroscience data. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2023;59:101196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement 1
media-1.pdf (319KB, pdf)

Articles from bioRxiv are provided here courtesy of Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Preprints

RESOURCES