Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2023 Nov 28;21(11):e3002393. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002393

Cardiac activity impacts cortical motor excitability

Esra Al 1,2,3,4,5,*, Tilman Stephani 1,6, Melina Engelhardt 7,8, Saskia Haegens 4,5,9, Arno Villringer 1,2,3, Vadim V Nikulin 1
Editor: Joachim Gross10
PMCID: PMC10684011  PMID: 38015826

Abstract

Human cognition and action can be influenced by internal bodily processes such as heartbeats. For instance, somatosensory perception is impaired both during the systolic phase of the cardiac cycle and when heartbeats evoke stronger cortical responses. Here, we test whether these cardiac effects originate from overall changes in cortical excitability. Cortical and corticospinal excitability were assessed using electroencephalographic and electromyographic responses to transcranial magnetic stimulation while concurrently monitoring cardiac activity with electrocardiography. Cortical and corticospinal excitability were found to be highest during systole and following stronger neural responses to heartbeats. Furthermore, in a motor task, hand–muscle activity and the associated desynchronization of sensorimotor oscillations were stronger during systole. These results suggest that systolic cardiac signals have a facilitatory effect on motor excitability—in contrast to sensory attenuation that was previously reported for somatosensory perception. Thus, it is possible that distinct time windows exist across the cardiac cycle, optimizing either perception or action.


Human cognition and action can be influenced by internal bodily processes such as heartbeats. By studying motor excitability and muscle activity across the cardiac cycle of humans, this study reveals previously unknown heart-brain interactions that suggest distinct time windows optimized for either action or perception.

Introduction

How we perceive and engage with the world is influenced by the dynamic relationship between the brain and the rest of the body including respiratory, digestive, and cardiac systems [16]. For example, cardiac activity has been found to influence visual and auditory perception [79]. In the domain of somatosensation and pain, perception and neural processing of stimuli have been reported to decrease during the systolic compared to the diastolic phase of the cardiac cycle [1013]. An overall systolic dampening of cortical processes was suggested to be due to baroreceptor activation during systole [14]. In support of this view, reaction times to auditory, visual, and tactile stimuli have been shown to be slower for systolic presentation [15,16]. Furthermore, when touch was initiated during systole, the duration of active sensation was longer [17]. However, for some other motor activities, a facilitatory effect of systole has been observed, for instance, self-initiated movements occurred more frequently during systole [17,18] or around the R-peak [19]; but see [20]. Similarly, an effect of the cardiac cycle was also observed in gun shooting, with shooters preferentially triggering a gun in a cardiac window that included a large part of systole and the initial part of diastole [21]; but also see an opposite effect for elite shooters [22]. Furthermore, (micro)saccades occur more often during systole whereas fixations during diastole [23,24]. One possible explanation for these effects can be related to blood movement within the body (peripheral), rather than being driven by top-down neural mechanisms (central) [23]. In addition to cardiac phase effects, stronger neural responses to heartbeats, i.e., heartbeat-evoked potentials (HEPs), are followed by increases in visual perception and decreases in somatosensory detection [11,13,25]. These results, therefore, suggest that cardiac activity could interact with both perception and action. However, it is not known whether a central/cortical mechanism underlies these cardiac effects.

One possibility is that cardiac activity exerts its effects through alterations of neuronal excitability in different parts of the brain. A few previous studies have investigated this hypothesis using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the primary motor cortex, which induces motor-evoked potentials (MEPs), an indicator of corticospinal excitability. However, so far, no main effect of the cardiac phase on excitability levels has been observed [2629]. Otsuru and colleagues [28] showed corticospinal excitability 400 ms after R-peak to be significantly higher in poor compared to good interoceptive perceivers, but the authors did not observe a significant main effect of cardiac phase on motor excitability. There are several possible methodological reasons for these findings. Importantly, the examination of excitability was limited to specific time intervals (only up to 400 ms after R-peak), rather than across the entire cardiac cycle including both systolic and diastolic window. Furthermore, individual brain anatomy was not taken into consideration, possibly resulting in higher variability of the stimulated brain regions. Lastly, these previous studies only included peripheral measures of corticospinal excitability without direct measurement of cortical excitability via concurrent electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings. This approach requires considering multiple concomitant factors to minimize TMS artifacts (see a recent review [30]). To address this limitation, stimulation intensity needs to be minimized by carefully determining the motor threshold, the effects of TMS clicks should be minimized to avoid acoustic stimulation, the experiment should have a sham condition to account for bone-conducted sound from the coil vibration [31] and thorough data cleaning to remove TMS artifacts using spatial (for instance, ICA) and temporal filters.

In the present study, we systemically examine whether cortical and corticospinal excitability change across the entire cardiac cycle and whether they interact with HEPs. Additionally, to comprehensively evaluate cardiac-related activity, we investigated variations in heart rate following TMS stimulation throughout the cardiac cycle. Neuronavigated TMS was used in combination with multichannel EEG, in order to comprehensively investigate both cortical and peripheral TMS-evoked responses. If systolic activity attenuates motor excitability, similar to somatosensory perception, TMS pulses during systole would be expected to produce weaker MEPs in the hand muscle and weaker TMS-evoked potentials (TEPs) in the motor cortex. Similarly, increases in HEPs would be expected to attenuate upcoming motor excitability. Alternatively, if behavioral observations of motor facilitation during systole are correct [18,21,23,24], stronger MEPs and TEPs should be observed. Supporting this latter hypothesis, we found that both peripheral and central TEPs were in fact higher during systole. Moreover, stronger HEPs preceded increases in excitability. In line with these findings, hand–muscle activity and associated desynchronization of sensorimotor oscillations in a motor pinch task were strongest during the systolic heart phase. In addition, in an exploratory analysis, we found a specific influence of TMS on heart rate depending on the cardiac phase, which could be relevant for clinical studies. Taken together, our results reveal that there is a facilitatory effect of systolic activity on motor excitability, possibly connected with an optimal window for action initiation during the cardiac cycle.

Results

Motor-evoked potentials change across the cardiac cycle

We first aimed to determine whether corticospinal excitability changes systematically over the phases of the cardiac cycle. For this purpose, we applied TMS to the right primary motor area at random time points across the cardiac cycle and recorded MEPs of the first dorsal interosseus muscle in the left hand in 36 participants. Following the TMS administration, the corresponding cardiac phase was determined posteriori (see Methods for the details; Fig 1). Consistent with the notion of corticospinal excitability changes across the cardiac cycle, MEP amplitudes were significantly higher during systole to diastole (Wilcoxon signed rank test, V = 463, p = 0.040, Cohen’s d = 0.341; Fig 2). As an additional control of a potential effect of ECG artefact on EMG activity, systolic and diastolic EMG activity during the sham condition was subtracted from real TMS activity. After this control analysis, MEP amplitudes remained significantly higher during systole (V = 463, p = 0.041, Cohen’s d = 0.340). So, these results suggest that corticospinal excitability is higher during systole compared to diastole.

Fig 1. Experimental paradigm.

Fig 1

(a) TMS was applied over the right primary motor cortex of the participants. The motor response to TMS in their left hand, i.e., MEP, was measured by bipolar EMG. Their cortical responses to TMS, the TEP, as well as to heartbeats, the HEP, were measured using multichannel EEG. The heart activity was recorded via ECG. (b) After determining the individual RMT, participants underwent a resting-state EEG measurement. Thereafter, 416 single TMS pulses with an intensity of 120% of the RMT were applied in 4 blocks. There were also 4 blocks of sham conditions, in which a plastic block was placed between the TMS coil and the head of the participant. The pairs of real and sham TMS blocks were randomized across the participants. At the end of the TMS blocks, participants performed a motor pinch task. In this task, they were instructed to squeeze a pinch gauge with their left thumb against the index finger while a red circle was presented in the middle of the monitor. When the circle became green, they relaxed their fingers. In this order, participants performed 30 trials. ECG, electrocardiography; EEG, electroencephalography; EMG, electromyography; HEP, heartbeat-evoked potential; MEP, motor-evoked potential; RMT, resting motor threshold; TEP, TMS-evoked potential; TMS, transcranial magnetic .stimulation

Fig 2. Changes in corticospinal excitability depending on the timing of TMS application across the cardiac cycle.

Fig 2

(a) Schematic of the cardiac cycle. The systolic phase (indicated in red) starts with the R-peak and reflects the ventricular contraction of the heart (leading to blood ejection), whereas the diastolic phase (indicated in blue) represents the relaxation phase during which the heart refills with blood. To equate the probability of a stimulation event occurring in either of the 2 phases, equal lengths of systole and diastole were defined. (b) MEP amplitudes of the first dorsal interosseus muscle in the left hand are higher in response to TMS stimulation during systole (red) compared to diastole (blue). The individual points indicate the mean MEP amplitudes for each participant. *p < 0.05. Access to the data and code for all analytical figures can be obtained at the following link: https://osf.io/eg8dz?view_only=7edb12b6e50e4c709ad60579bf5ebd62.

TMS-evoked potentials vary across the cardiac cycle

In addition to corticospinal excitability, we also aimed to test differences of purely cortical responses to the TMS pulse between the systole and diastole phases of the cardiac cycle. Cortical excitability was probed by early TEPs (15 to 60 ms post-TMS) measured from a cluster of electrodes (C4, CP4, C6, CP6) over the right motor cortex (hotspot). By focusing on this time window, we prevented a possible contamination of cranial muscle artefacts in the very early peaks as well as sensory-evoked potentials in the later time windows [30]. TEP amplitudes between 22 and 60 ms following the TMS stimulation were stronger during systole as compared to diastole (cluster-based permutation t test, pcluster = 0.009; Cohen’s d = 0.41; Fig 3A). To test whether these results were indeed related to neural activity of the cortex, rather than reflecting TMS and cardiac artifacts, we contrasted them with the sham TMS condition. During the sham condition, a plastic block between the coil and the participant’s head was placed to keep air- and bone-conducted auditory and somatosensory sensations similar to the real TMS [31]. Additionally, pulse-related artifacts in the EEG were expected to be similar to the real TMS stimulation. Therefore, if the cardiac cycle effect on TEPs is a genuine modulation of evoked neuronal responses to TMS, it should not be present in the sham condition. A cluster-based permutation test did not reveal any significant difference in TEPs in response to sham TMS during systole and diastole (pcluster = 0.2; Fig 3B). Furthermore, to account for physiological and stimulation artifacts, TEPs during sham were subtracted from those in the real TMS condition. The TEP difference was significantly higher between 24 and 60 ms during systole relative to diastole (pcluster = 0.008, Cohen’s d = 0.41; Fig 3C). The corresponding neural sources of the TEP difference between systole and diastole were observed to be maximal around the right primary motor cortex (Fig 3D). These results show that similar to corticospinal excitability, purely cortical excitability increases during systole, suggesting increased neuronal excitability in the motor system.

Fig 3. Changes in cortical excitability across the cardiac cycle.

Fig 3

(a) TEPs, in response to TMS stimulation, at the electrodes closest to the motor hotspot (C4; CP4; C6; CP6). Early TEPs were significantly larger during systole compared to diastole in motor areas between 22 and 60 ms. The contrast between systole and diastole in this time window is shown in the topography plot. (b) Same as in (a), for the sham TMS condition. No significant differences between systole and diastole were observed here. (c) The difference curve between real TMS and sham, for systole and diastole (sham-corrected TEP contrast). After correcting for the TMS and physiological artifacts, TEPs during systole and diastole were significantly different between 24 and 60 ms. (d) The source reconstruction of the corrected TEP contrast (systole minus diastole) between 24 and 60 ms (left), and same displaying the strongest generators only (thresholded at 85% of the maximum activity and clusters sizes of at least 5 vertices; right).

Muscle-related peripheral and central activity fluctuates across the cardiac cycle

To determine whether changes in motor excitability during systole facilitate muscle activity in the absence of TMS stimulation, we analyzed cardiac effects on muscle activity in a follow-up motor pinch task. During the task, participants were asked to pinch a dynamometer with their index finger and thumb, while we recorded EMG, EEG, and ECG signals (Fig 4A). To estimate peripheral muscle force, we calculated the envelope of EMG activity when participants initiated the pinch during systole and diastole (see EMG envelope in the Methods section). Cluster statistics revealed a significant increase in the EMG envelope from 340 to 454 ms after the onset of the pinch during systole compared to diastole (pcluster = 0.02, Cohen’s d = 0.44; Fig 4A). To test whether this finding might have been related to blood circulation–related changes in the fingers, we sampled systolic and diastolic EMG activity during the resting state condition. This analysis did not reveal any significant difference in the resting EMG envelope between systole and diastole (no significant clusters were found). This indicates that there was no influence of cardiac-related artifacts on the EMG signal across the cardiac cycle.

Fig 4. Fluctuations of muscle-related activity depending on the pinch onset across the cardiac cycle during a motor pinch task.

Fig 4

(a) Muscle force, measured by the linear envelope of EMG activity in the left hand, was significantly higher from 340 to 454 ms following pinch onset (at 0 ms) during systole compared to diastole. (b) Similarly, systolic and diastolic sensorimotor oscillations were analyzed in the range of 8–30 Hz in the sensorimotor electrodes to quantify event-related desynchronization following the muscle activation. Cluster statistics revealed that when participants started the pinch during systole, the desynchronization of sensorimotor oscillations was higher in the frequency range of 8–25 Hz between 0 and 726 ms following pinch onset. The raster plot shows the contrast between systole and diastole. (c) The topography of the significant contrast between systole and diastole.

Following the analysis of the muscle activity in the periphery, we also tested whether sensorimotor oscillations in the motor areas desynchronize differently following the initiation of the pinch during systole and diastole. This analysis demonstrated that the desynchronization of sensorimotor oscillations in the range of 8 to 25 Hz was stronger between 0 and 726 ms following pinch onset during systole as compared to diastole (pcluster = 0.012, Cohen’s d = 0.37; Fig 4B and 4C). To investigate whether this finding was influenced by cardiac-related artifacts, we again sampled systolic and diastolic windows during the resting state and tested the differences in sensorimotor oscillations between systole and diastole. Also, in this control analysis, no significant differences were found (pcluster = 0.12). Thus, these results indicate that both peripheral muscle activity and its central correlates are stronger when the movement starts during systole as compared to diastole.

Heart rate changes depending on the timing of TMS across the cardiac cycle

Previous research suggests that TMS stimulation has significant effects on heart rate [32]. However, it is not known whether this effect depends on the cardiac phase of the TMS stimulation. Therefore, in an exploratory analysis, we investigated the changes of the heart rate in response to TMS stimulation during systole and diastole. As a measure of heart rate, we calculated the length of interbeat intervals in the cardiac cycles before TMS (pre-TMS), during TMS, and post-TMS stimulation. The analysis showed a main effect of time (F2, 70 = 23.11, p = 2⋅10−8, ges = 2⋅10−4) and an interaction of time and cardiac phase (F2, 70 = 10.30, p = 1⋅10−4, ges = 2⋅10−4) on heart rate. Comparison of heartbeat intervals preceding TMS and concurrent with TMS revealed a significant cardiac deceleration when TMS stimulation occurred during systole (t35 = −5.73, p = 2⋅10−6, Cohen’s d = 0.96). This was followed by a cardiac acceleration (from TMS to post-TMS; t35 = 8.58, p = 4⋅10−10, Cohen’s d = 1.43; Fig 5). No significant changes were observed for stimulations during diastole (from pre-TMS to TMS, t35 = 0.75, p = 0.5 and from TMS to post-TMS, t35 = 0.42, p = 0.68; Fig 5). Post hoc t tests showed that there was no significant difference in heart rate before TMS stimulation between systole and diastole (t35 = 1.83, p = 0.075), whereas the heart rate difference was significant during TMS stimulation (t35 = 2.10, p = 0.043, Cohen’s d = 0.35). This difference was no longer statistically significant in the post-TMS window (t35 = 1.80, p = 0.080).

Fig 5. Heart rate changes induced by TMS are influenced by the cardiac cycle.

Fig 5

The heart first slowed down and then accelerated when TMS pulses were delivered during systole. No significant differences were observed for stimulations during diastole. Colored bands indicate 95% within-participant confidence intervals. ***p < 0.0005.

To control whether these heart rate changes were due to genuine effects of TMS rather than artifacts (for instance, auditory, somatosensory) induced by TMS application, heart rate across time was subtracted with the heart rate during the sham TMS condition individually for each time interval and cardiac phase. This analysis again showed a similar main effect of time (F2, 70 = 7.42, p = 1⋅10−3, ges = 4⋅10−3) and an interaction of time and cardiac phase (F1.56, 54.64 = 3.88, p = 0.03, ges = 3⋅10−3) on heart rate. These results suggest that heart rate is influenced by TMS only when administered during systole but not during diastole.

Heartbeat-evoked potentials fluctuate depending on motor excitability levels

The analysis of cardiac phase effects on motor excitability showed a contrasting pattern to the previous findings on somatosensory perception. Here, we asked whether a similar contrast would be observed for another aspect of heart–brain interactions, which is the cortical responses to heartbeats, so-called heartbeat-evoked potentials (HEPs). Therefore, we analyzed the relationship between HEPs and motor excitability levels. To be able to distinguish cardiac and TMS-related neural processing, we first only chose trials in which the TMS stimulation occurred at least 400 ms after the preceding R-peak in line with the previous studies [11,13]. This allowed us to investigate the relationship of HEPs (during systole) with MEPs (during diastole) within the same cardiac cycle. For this purpose, we sorted individual trials based on their MEP amplitudes, ranging from weak to strong, and divided them into 3 equal bins for each participant. The sorting was based on MEP amplitudes since they were present in every single trial included in the analysis. Subsequently, we compared the prestimulus HEP amplitudes preceding weak (first bin) and strong (third bin) MEP levels. This comparison was conducted using a cluster-based permutation t test within the time window of 296 to 400 ms post R-peak, focusing on the centroparietal electrodes as identified in previous studies [11,13]. HEPs were significantly higher preceding strong compared to weak MEP amplitudes, between 304 and 324 ms over the centroparietal electrodes (pcluster = 0.021 corrected for multiple comparisons in space and time; Cohen’s d = 0.48; Fig 6A and 6B). We then asked whether MEP amplitudes during the first 400 ms of the cardiac cycle are influenced by HEP activity in response to the previous cardiac cycle. This analysis showed that HEP amplitudes were more positive between 362 and 394 ms at centroparietal electrodes preceding strong compared to weak MEP amplitudes (pcluster = 0.018 corrected for multiple comparisons in space and time; Cohen’s d = 0.53; S1 Fig).

Fig 6. The cortical responses to heartbeats, HEPs, preceding changes in the strength of motor excitability.

Fig 6

(a) To assess relationship between HEPs and motor excitability, single trials were sorted according to MEP amplitudes and split into 3 equal bins for each participant. HEP amplitudes between 304 and 328 ms following the R-peak (the highlighted gray area) were higher preceding strong compared to weak MEPs across the centroparietal electrodes. The topography contrast represents HEP amplitude difference preceding strong versus weak MEPs between 304 and 328 ms. (b) (left) The neural sources of HEP differences preceding strong and weak MEPs are visualized. (right) Same as the left figure but displaying the strongest generators only (thresholded at 85% of the maximum activity and clusters sizes of at least 5 vertices). *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.0005.

To test whether these effects are induced by overall changes in cardiac activity during TMS stimulation, we further tested the differences in HEP activity during TMS stimulation and resting-state condition (without TMS stimulation). No significant differences in HEP amplitudes during TMS and resting-state were observed (no clusters were found; S2 Fig). Overall, these analyses show that increases in HEP amplitudes lead to stronger motor excitability levels, which again stands in contrast to the previous HEP findings in somatosensory perception.

Control analyses for movement of cortex across the cardiac cycle

Next, we investigated whether our findings of larger motor excitability during systole might have been related to the displacement of the cortex due to blood hemodynamics (as a result of cardiac activity). Previous research has shown that the mechanical displacement of the cortex follows an inverse u-shaped pattern across the cardiac cycle and reaching its maximum at 450 to 500 ms after the R-peak [33]. As the electric field induced by a TMS pulse is stronger when the coil is closer to the brain [34], any heart phase–dependent modulation of MEPs could potentially be influenced by cortical tissue movement towards the skull. If this was the case, we would expect a stronger TMS stimulation at the peak of the displacement. Therefore, we examined whether our findings displayed a similar temporal pattern as observed in the displacement of cortical tissue to shed light on this aspect. To investigate whether MEP amplitudes followed a similar pattern across the cardiac cycle, we first visualized MEP amplitudes across 50 ms bins following the previous R-peak (S3 Fig). This demonstrated that MEPs were maximal during the first 50 ms and gradually decreased across the cardiac cycle. To statistically test the relationship between MEP amplitudes and the distance from the previous heartbeat, linear-mixed-effects model regressions were fit on the single-trial level. The linear regression that included the cardiac distance (MEP ~ distance + (1 | participant)) explained the empirical data better than the null model, i.e., a model with no relationship assumed (MEP ~ (1 | participant); χ2 = 4.67, p = 0.03). Crucially, this linear model also showed a better fit than a second-degree polynomial model (MEP ~ distance + distance^2+ (1 | participant), χ2 = 0.34, p = 0.6). This result indicates that the changes in MEP amplitudes do not follow a u-shaped pattern across the cardiac cycle and are thus not likely to be explained by the displacement of the cortex due to blood influx or efflux.

Discussion

Using simultaneous recordings of brain activity, heart activity, and muscle activity, this study discovered that the timing of heartbeats and their neural processing were linked to changes in the excitability of the motor system. Our findings build upon previous evidence showing that somatosensory perception is affected during the systolic phase of the cardiac cycle and when heartbeats evoke stronger cortical responses. To delve deeper into these cardiac effects, we examined whether they arise from overall changes in cortical excitability. For this, we performed simultaneous recordings of cortical and corticospinal excitability using electroencephalography (EEG) and electromyography (EMG) responses to TMS, along with concurrent monitoring of cardiac activity using electrocardiography (ECG). As a result, we observed multiple complementary effects of cardiac signals on motor activity. Specifically, we discovered that cortical and corticospinal excitability reached their highest levels during systole and were enhanced following stronger neural responses to heartbeats. Additionally, in a motor task, we observed that hand–muscle activity and the associated desynchronization of sensorimotor oscillations were more pronounced during systole. These findings suggest that systolic cardiac signals exert a facilitatory effect on motor excitability, which contrasts with the sensory attenuation previously reported in somatosensory perception. Therefore, our study sheds light on the existence of distinct time windows across the cardiac cycle that potentially optimize perception and action.

Importance of methodology for current findings

Methodological differences are likely to explain the absence of cardiac modulation of motor excitability in previous TMS studies [2628]. In those studies, time resolution across the cardiac cycle was rather limited. Unlike in our study, TMS stimulations were not presented throughout the entire cardiac cycle, they were rather presented up to 400 or 600 ms after the heartbeats and at specific time points (for instance, 100 ms after R-peak). Since our study revealed a linear decrease in motor excitability throughout the cardiac cycle, previous studies might not have been able to sample the decrease towards the end of the cardiac cycle. Furthermore, combining a neuronavigational system with individual brain scans provided us with a higher spatial specificity for the stimulation location, in comparison to previous studies. In addition, our study had the advantage of a larger sample size and trial numbers, which contributed to higher statistical power to detect the cardiac-cycle effects on motor excitability [2628]. Since the observed MEP effect was rather small in our study, a larger set of participants (as in the present study) would indeed facilitate a statistical detection of differences. Finally, previous studies only tested cardiac effects on peripheral MEPs without concurrent cortical recordings. Yet, it is known that peripheral MEPs reflect excitability changes at both cortical and spinal cord levels and special measures should be taken (such as H-reflex) in order to disentangle excitability changes between these 2 levels [35]. On the other hand, TEPs directly reflect changes in cortical excitability, especially at early latencies [36]. This can also explain why we observed a stronger effect size for the cardiac modulation of TEPs as compared to MEPs.

Central and peripheral changes of motor excitability across the cardiac cycle

Cardiac cycle effects on motor excitability are consistent with previous findings of increased frequency of muscle movement during systole as compared to diastole [18,21,23,24]. Here, we showed that TMS during systole is associated with higher corticospinal and cortical excitability in motor areas. Thus, motor-related activity seems to be facilitated during systole. This, in turn, may also explain why eye movements [23,24], for instance, (micro)saccades, and voluntary hand movements [21], for instance, firing a gun, have been found to occur more often during systole. One could also argue that the effects we found were merely due to the fact that the distance of the brain to the skull (and, thus, the TMS coil) changes due to fluctuations in intracranial pressure throughout the cardiac cycle [37]. This, in turn, should affect the induced electric field from TMS. However, our control analyses did not support this argument. More specifically, the movement of the brain follows an inverse u-shaped pattern across the cardiac cycle and reaches a maximal distance at about 450 to 500 ms after the previous heartbeat [33]. If the cortical movement across the cardiac cycle was responsible for the cardiac phase effects, then MEPs would be expected to follow a similar pattern across the cardiac cycle. However, MEP amplitudes decreased rather linearly across the cardiac cycle, differently than effects that would be expected due to cortical movement. This result indicates that the changes in MEP amplitudes are not likely to be explained by the displacement of the cortex across the cardiac cycle. Another possible artifact, which can influence the amplitude of the evoked activity, are muscle-related far-fields from the cardiac activity (typically referred to as “cardiac artifacts” in the EEG). To control for those, we included a sham condition, in which auditory, tactile, and cardiac artifacts were comparable. After the correction of real TMS recordings with the sham condition, cortical excitability was still significantly higher during systole as compared to diastole. Overall, we found that the excitability in the sensorimotor cortex is increased in systole and correspondingly such increase is mirrored in the strength of the motor output. However, the strength of the motor output is not the only parameter important for motor control. The timing of the movement initiation as well as the coordination between different muscle groups could be equally important for the functionally relevant motor output. These aspects should be investigated in future studies.

The cardiac cycle was also observed to affect muscle activity in a motor task, where participants were asked to pinch and release a dynamometer with their left index finger and thumb. When the pinch was initiated during systole, compared to diastole, muscle activity was transiently stronger, suggesting a systolic increase in the applied force [38]. In addition to the peripheral activity, we analyzed cardiac effects on the central neural activity during the motor task. Previous studies have shown that following muscle contractions, sensorimotor oscillations desynchronize in the motor regions, which is reflected as an amplitude decrease in the alpha and beta range [39,40]. Here, we found that this desynchronization transiently increased when the pinch was initiated during systole. Furthermore, these cardiac effects on the muscle-related activity are not likely due to cardiac artifacts, since no significant differences in muscle and neural activity were observed across the cardiac cycle while participants were resting. Overall, these findings suggest that muscle activity is stronger when movement is initiated during systole due to an increase in motor excitability.

Underlying mechanisms and the link to perception

The increased motor excitability during systole seems to be at odds with the previously shown cardiac effects on perception. For example, we recently demonstrated that somatosensory percepts and their neural processing are attenuated during systole [11,13]. To compensate for this reduced perceptual sensitivity, a recent study revealed that touches initiated in the systole phase were held for longer periods of time [17]. These findings could be explained by an interoceptive predictive coding account, which postulates that rhythmic cardiac signals are predicted and suppressed from entering conscious perception. This mechanism was suggested to additionally inhibit the perception of coincident weak external stimuli [11,13]. Furthermore, this suppression of nonsalient sensory stimuli was suggested to lead to a greater uncertainty about threatening factors in the environment [41]. To compensate for it, the organism might increase expectation for a “risk” and use its limited resources for a “flight or fight” motor response, which can be potentially mediated by increased baroreceptor activity during systole. Therefore, it is possible that the increased motor activity during systole might provide a survival advantage. Hence, this would suggest that there are different optimal windows for action and perception throughout the cardiac cycle. This idea also fits well with previous studies on “sensory gating,” in which somatosensory perception and evoked potentials were shown to be attenuated during movement [4244]. This would also explain why eye movements occur more often during systole, whereas eyes fixate on visual stimuli more often during diastole [23]. Given that action has an inhibitory effect on perception, it is plausible that systolic facilitation of action is indeed consistent with inhibition of perception during the systolic phase of the cardiac cycle.

It is also important to note that different cardiac effects occur within the systolic and diastolic cardiac phases. During systole, in response to changes in blood pressure, baroreceptors become maximally active approximately 300 ms after R-peak [45]. This change in baroreceptor activity has been suggested to be the driving force of the cardiac effects on perception. For example, during this time window, the processing of somatosensory and pain stimuli is shown to be minimal [11,13,46]. These findings were in line with the idea that activation of baroreceptors cause an overall decrease in cortical excitability [14,47,48]. However, in the current study, we observed that motor excitability was strongest during the pre-ejection period of the systole, before arterial pressure starts to increase and activate baroreceptors. Therefore, the cardiac-related effects on the motor domain might not be mediated primarily by baroreceptor activity but rather via a direct neural pathway involving cardiac afferent neurons. These neurons receive inputs from sensory receptors within the heart and transmit signals to coordinate heart rate and other cardiac functions. They fire at specific phases of the ECG signal, such as around the R or T-waves, depending on their location and transduction characteristics. Importantly, they have a fast conduction velocity leading to a rapid cortical activation [49]. Future studies could investigate this idea by using animal models.

Possible clinical relevance of heart rate changes following TMS stimulation

Cortical excitability changes have been associated previously with epilepsy, disorders of consciousness, stroke, and depression [50,51]. To counterbalance these abnormalities in cortical excitability, therapeutic applications of TMS have been introduced, for instance, for treating depression [51] or facilitating motor recovery during neurorehabilitation in stroke [50]. Our results on cardiac modulations of cortical excitability raise some important questions for these clinical populations using different TMS protocols. For example, it remains unknown how cortical excitability over the cardiac cycle is modulated in those pathological conditions. Furthermore, our observation that TMS induces changes of the heart rate during systole, i.e., when the cortical processing of heartbeats occurs, but not during diastole, can have important implications for the clinical use of TMS. For instance, in case the changes in heart rate during TMS application are of clinical concern for patients, our results, based on single pulse TMS, suggest that stimulation during diastole may help mitigate these undesired effects. Addressing these questions in future research could provide valuable insights for patients undergoing TMS in clinical settings.

Relationship between heartbeat-evoked potentials and motor excitability levels

Another effect of cardiac activity on motor excitability was found on the cortical level. We observed that HEPs, during systole, showed higher positivity over centroparietal electrodes between 304 and 328 ms preceding strong as compared to weak corticospinal excitability (as measured by TMS-induced MEPs). These results again diverge from our previous results on somatosensory perception, in which we observed higher HEPs preceding attenuated somatosensory processing. We previously explained increases in HEPs as a result of an attentional switch from the external world to internal bodily signals, such as heartbeats [11]. This was further supported by higher HEP amplitudes when participants were resting compared to engaging in an external task [13]. If internal attention levels changed in the current study during the TMS condition compared to rest, we would expect lower HEPs during the TMS condition. However, in the current study, there was no significant change in HEPs during the TMS application in comparison to the resting state of the participants. This was probably related to the absence of an external task during the TMS condition. Another factor, which can positively influence HEP amplitude, is arousal [52]. Increases in arousal are also known to increase motor excitability [53] as well as heart rate [1,54]. Supporting a possible involvement of arousal in our study, we observed that heart rate became higher as motor excitability increased. Therefore, we suggest that increases in arousal might be responsible for increases in HEP amplitudes for stronger motor excitability. It is also possible that since this analysis involves HEPs and MEPs, which were close in time, there was a similar cortical state for both responses due to intrinsic neuronal dynamics. If the magnitude of both HEPs and MEPs reflects increased cortical excitability, then a positive correlation would be expected, since cortical excitability changes on many time scales [55], including a period covering both pre- and immediate post-stimulus intervals.

Limitations

While the present study provides valuable insights into the influence of cardiac oscillations on motor excitability, there are several limitations that should be acknowledged. Firstly, the study sample consisted of young white individuals between the ages of 18 and 40. Although we anticipate that our findings may extend to the broader population, it is crucial for future studies to investigate the effects of cardiac oscillations in more diverse participant populations across different racial groups. Secondly, we only stimulated the right motor cortex. Further research should examine whether similar cardiac effects on motor excitability can be observed for left-hemispheric stimulation. Finally, we did not examine the potential impact of respiratory oscillations on the observed results. As respiration has been shown to organize neural dynamics [56] and modulate cortical excitability [57], it would be important for future studies to investigate how respiratory phase (i.e., inhale and exhale) during TMS stimulation and movement initiation might influence the effects of cardiac oscillations.

Concluding remarks

In conclusion, our study provides novel insights into the regulation of cortical and corticospinal excitability by cardiac function in healthy individuals. Together, these findings strongly suggest that systolic cardiac activity and its cortical processing have facilitatory effects on motor excitability, in contrast to the previous findings on somatosensory perception. Thus, we propose that optimal windows for action and perception are likely to differ across the cardiac cycle. Furthermore, these results may contribute to the development of novel stimulation protocols and promote a better understanding of the interplay between brain dynamics and bodily states in both health and disease.

Methods

Participants

Based on our previous study in which we observed an effect of the cardiac cycle on somatosensory perception [12], we performed a power analysis (using the “pwr” package in R). Given a Cohen’s d of 0.48, we calculated the required sample size as N = 36 (80% power and alpha = 0.05) for observing cardiac effects of this size.

For the experiment, we only invited participants who were between 18 and 40 years old and who did not report any neurological, cognitive, or cardiac health problems. Exclusion criteria further included tinnitus, alcohol or drug abuse, and pregnancy. A total of 37 healthy volunteers participated in the experiments after giving written informed consent. All protocols were approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Leipzig’s Medical Faculty (Ethics no: 179/19) and followed the principles of Declaration of Helsinki. One participant was excluded due to failure in the data acquisition. In the remaining 36 participants (20 female, age: 27.97 ± 4.13, mean ± SD), only one participant was left-handed as assessed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [58].

TMS setup and neuronavigation

The experiment included 4 blocks of sham and 4 blocks of real TMS stimulations. Participants were seated in a comfortable armchair and asked to keep their eyes on a fixation point on a wall in front of them throughout the measurements. TMS pulses were delivered through a Magstim 200 Bistim stimulator (Magstim, Whitland, United Kingdom) connected to a figure-of-eight coil (Magstim “D70 Alpha Coil”). The coil was positioned at an angle of 45° with respect to the sagittal direction. Structural T1 weighted MRIs of the participants were used with the TMS neuronavigation system (Localite GmbH, Bonn, Germany) to identify the hotspot of the left first dorsal interosseous (FDI) muscle. Then, the resting motor threshold was determined as the lowest TMS intensity at which 5 out of 10 trials yielded a motor response greater than 50 μV (peak-to-peak amplitude) [59]. The TMS blocks consisted of 104 trials, i.e., a total of 104 × 8 = 832 stimulations. The neuronavigation system was used to control the coil position over the hotspot during the TMS stimulations. TMS intensity was set to be 20% above the motor threshold at rest (corresponding to 66.58 ± 9.16% of the maximum stimulator output). The interstimulus interval was uniformly randomized between 1.5 and 2.5 s. The blocks were presented as pairs of 2 sham or real TMS, and their order were randomized across participants. For the sham TMS condition, we used a custom-manufactured 3.5 cm plastic block between the coil and the participant’s head to keep air- and bone-conducted auditory sensations similar to the real TMS [31]. This setup also mimicked a tapping somatosensory sensation associated with the vibration of the TMS coil.

EEG, ECG, and EMG recordings

TMS-compatible EEG equipment (NeurOne Tesla, Bittium) was used for recording EEG activity from the scalp. The EEG was acquired with a bandwidth of 0.16 to 1250 Hz from 62 TMS-compatible c-shaped Ag/AgCl electrodes (EasyCap GmbH, Herrsching, Germany) mounted on an elastic cap and positioned according to the 10–10 International System. POz electrode was used as ground. During the measurements, the EEG signal was referenced to an electrode placed on the left mastoid. Additionally, a right-mastoid electrode was recorded so that EEG data could be re-referenced to the average of both mastoid electrodes offline. The signal was digitized at a sampling rate of 5 kHz. Skin/electrode impedance was maintained below 5 kΩ. EEG electrode positions were also coregistered with the structural MRIs using the neuronavigation system. To reduce auditory response artifacts in the EEG induced by coil clicks, participants wore earplugs throughout the experiment. An additional ECG electrode connected to the EEG system was placed under the participant’s left breast to record the heart activity. Furthermore, EMG electrodes were attached to the left FDI muscle in belly–tendon montage via a bipolar channel connected to the EEG system to record the TMS-induced MEPs. At the beginning of the experiment, EEG and ECG data were acquired during a 5-min eyes-open resting-state measurement.

Automated cardiac phase classification

The fluctuations of motor excitability were tested across the systolic and diastolic phases of the cardiac activity. Systole was defined as the time between the R-peak and the end of the t-wave, which was determined by using a trapezoid area algorithm [11,60]. We then used the duration of systole to define an equal length of diastole at the end of each cardiac cycle [11]. By using time windows of equal length for systole and diastole, we equated the probability of a stimulation/event occurring in either of the 2 phases. As a result, the average systole (and diastole) length was 351 ± 21 ms. Before using this automated algorithm, we removed large TMS artifacts on the ECG data by removing −2 to 10 ms window around the TMS stimulation and then applied cubic interpolation. As a result, the number of trials was not significantly different (t35 = −1.05, p = 0.3) between systolic (148 ± 17) and diastolic (150 ± 18) parts of cardiac cycle. Therefore, with this approach, we could ensure comparable trial numbers across conditions.

Motor-evoked potentials

As an index of corticospinal excitability, MEPs were used. After applying a baseline correction using −110 to −10 ms prestimulus window, peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes were calculated in the EMG data in the time window of 20 to 40 ms following the TMS stimulation. A successful TMS stimulation is expected to trigger an MEP higher than 50 μV, whereas a successful sham application does not trigger motor-evoked activity, while having other features of stimulations such as auditory click and bone vibration [59]. We applied these criteria to ensure that only valid TMS and sham trials were included in the analysis. This yielded on average 412 TMS trials and 405 sham trials per participant.

TMS-evoked EEG potentials

EEG data were first segmented between −1,400 and 1,000 ms around TMS stimulations. Then, the baseline correction was performed using −110 to −10 ms prestimulus window. The large-amplitude TMS artifacts between −2 and 8 ms were removed from each trial and then the remaining data segments were concatenated, in line with a previously established procedure [61]. Next, ICA (round 1) was applied using pop_runica as implemented in EEGLAB, used with the FASTICA algorithm implementing the “tanh” contrast function and a symmetrical approach [61,62]. To remove TMS decay artifacts, the 3 components explaining the most variance between −150 and 150 ms were removed and other components were forward projected [61]. After the decay artifact was removed in this way, copies of these datasets were kept. Then, a fourth-order Butterworth bandpass filter (0.5 to 45 Hz) and a 50-Hz notch filter (with a stopband of 45 to 55 Hz) were applied. A second round of ICA was applied to determine remaining TMS, ocular, muscle, and cardiac artifacts [61]. Afterwards, these ICA weights were applied to the copied dataset after the first round of ICA (unfiltered). On average, 11 ± 2 artifactual components were removed and then the data were forward projected. Due to remaining TMS-evoked artifacts, we further removed 15 ms poststimulus window. We then applied a cubic interpolation (from −2 to 15 ms relative to TMS stimulation) before applying the same filtering procedure (as described above) to the data [61]. The interpolated data were only used for data visualization and excluded from statistical analyses. This way we ensured that the TMS artifacts did not smear into the poststimulus window during the filtering process. Then, data were re-referenced offline to the average of the right and left mastoid signals and down-sampled to 500 Hz (Fig 7).

Fig 7. Preprocessing steps of EEG data for cleaning artifacts in the post-TMS window.

Fig 7

These steps were followed before calculating TMS-evoked potentials (TEPs).

To assess cortical excitability in motor areas, we focused on early components of TEPs in the first 60 ms following the stimulation, since this time window specifically involves the activation of local neuronal populations in the motor cortex [36]. Since the first 15 ms were interpolated, we evaluated TEPs between 15 and 60 ms in the post-TMS window in a cluster of electrodes over the right primary motor cortex (C4, CP4, C6, CP6). These electrodes were selected a priori since they were closest to the hotspot of the TMS stimulation.

Cardiac artifact during systole and diastole was estimated during TMS and sham conditions (see [11] for details on the pulse artifact cleaning of the evoked potentials) and subtracted from TEPs during systole and diastole individually.

Heartbeat-evoked potentials

In this analysis, we first analyzed the trials in which TMS stimulation was at least 400 ms after the previous R-peak (i.e., during diastole) to keep the HEP window free of TMS-related activity following previous studies [11,13]. This allowed us to examine the relationship of HEPs (in response to heartbeats during systole) with MEPs (in response to TMS stimulation during diastole) within the same cardiac cycle. In this study, we also tested whether MEP amplitudes during the first 400 ms of the cardiac cycle are influenced by HEP activity from the previous cardiac cycle.

To clean TMS artifacts in the prestimulus window and keep data processing close to our previous work [11,13], some preprocessing steps were altered compared to the steps described above for the poststimulation analyses: After the second round of ICA, we first calculated each distance between the prestimulus R peaks and TMS events. Then, we shuffled these distances and inserted “mock events” by subtracting them from the latency of TMS stimulations in the dataset. Next, we repeated this shuffling process 10 times. Finally, we segmented data between −100 and 400 ms around these mock events. By using an average of these segments, we derived an estimate of the TMS artifact in the time window of the heartbeat-evoked responses per participant.

We then subtracted this estimation from each HEP to remove any potential TMS artifacts. Finally, a baseline removal was performed, using the time window from −100 to 0 ms (relative to the R-peak).

Source analyses

The neural sources of the TEPs and HEPs were reconstructed with the Brainstorm toolbox [63] using individually measured electrode positions with a TMS neuronavigation system (Localite GmbH, Bonn, Germany). For every participant, the individual structural T1-weighted MRI images were segmented using Freesurfer (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). A 3-shell boundary element model (BEM) was constructed to calculate the lead field matrix with OpenMEEG [64,65]. The lead field matrices were inverted using eLORETA individually for each contrast (for instance, TEP difference between systole and diastole) and participant. Individual source data were then projected to the ICBM152 template [66]. Following this for each participant, the absolute values for each contrast were calculated. These absolute values were then used to compute grand averages of the HEP and TEP.

Motor pinch task

After the TMS sessions, participants performed a pinch motor task. At the beginning of the task, their maximal pinch strength, i.e., maximal voluntary contraction (MVC), was calculated using SAEHAN Hydraulic Hand Dynamometer Model SH5005 (SAEHAN Corporation, Korea). Participants were asked to squeeze the dynamometer with their left thumb pad against the lateral aspect of the middle phalanx of the left index finger as hard as possible while keeping their elbow in the 90° position. After calculating MVC, participants first practiced applying 30% of MVC across several trials consistently. During the task, participants were asked to apply the 30% of MVC (corresponding to 3.14 ± 0.88 pounds) when a green circle in the middle of the monitor returned to red. During the presentation of the red circle, which lasted for 3 s, they were asked to keep the contraction. When the circle became green again, they relaxed their hand for 3 s. In this order, participants performed contractions for 30 trials.

Movement onset estimation

The onset of movement was determined by analyzing EMG activity. For this purpose, we used a Matlab package, EMG-onset-detection. This package uses a method based on Teager–Kaiser Energy (TKE) operator and corrections of false onset detection and EMG artifacts [67]. If automatic detection did not work, the onset of movement was determined manually. Then, we determined the corresponding cardiac phase of each movement onset. On average, each cardiac phase included 12 trials.

EMG envelope

To estimate muscle activity during the pinch task, EMG data were analyzed. First, EMG signal was filtered by using a fourth-order Butterworth bandpass filter (10 to 500 Hz) and a 50-Hz notch filter (with a stopband of 45 to 55 Hz). Afterwards, the envelope of EMG was calculated by first taking the absolute value of the signal (“full-wave rectification”) and then applying an 8-Hz low-pass filter [68]. The resulting EMG envelope was epoched between −1 and 4 s around the movement onset. This was followed by a baseline correction using the −110 to −10 ms premovement EMG signal. Finally, the envelope was averaged for trials where movement was initiated during systole and diastole per participant.

Desynchronization of sensorimotor oscillations during the motor task

To investigate the central sensorimotor oscillations following pinch onset during systole and diastole, we also analyzed EEG signals. For this purpose, we first filtered the data with a fourth-order Butterworth bandpass (0.5 to 45 Hz) and a 50-Hz notch filter (with a stopband of 45 to 55 Hz). After cleaning muscular, cardiac, and ocular artifacts through ICA and re-referencing data to the average of both mastoid electrodes, data were segmented between −1,000 and 4,000 ms around the pinch onset. We then performed a Morlet wavelet analysis to investigate sensorimotor alpha and beta activity locked to pinch onset. This analysis was performed on every trial for frequencies from 5 to 40 Hz with the number of cycles increasing linearly from 4 to 10. Thus, a wavelet at 10 Hz was 4.9 cycles long, had a temporal resolution of 0.10 s and a spectral resolution of 4.85 Hz. We then calculated the average time-frequency activity for each cardiac phase per participant.

Statistics

We statistically tested the two-condition comparisons of TEPs, HEPs, EMG linear envelope, and sensorimotor oscillations using cluster-based permutation t tests as implemented in the FieldTrip toolbox [69]. To define clusters, the default threshold value (p < 0.05, two-tailed) was used. To test cluster-level statistics, condition labels were randomly shuffled 1,000 times to estimate the distribution of maximum cluster-level statistics obtained by chance. We report the temporal windows and spatial regions that we tested for each individual analysis below.

Statistical analysis of TEP activity during systole and diastole were conducted at electrodes C4, CP4, C6, and CP6 between 15 and 60 ms. Pre- and post-TMS changes in heart rate for stimulation during systole and diastole were evaluated using within-participant ANOVAs (ezANOVA function in R, v 1.3.1093; [70,71]), in which heart rate was the dependent variable and time (pre-TMS, TMS, post-TMS) as well as cardiac phase (systole, diastole) were independent variables. For statistical testing of HEP activity relating to motor excitability, we first sorted single trials according to their MEP amplitudes and split them into 3 equal bins for each participant. We specifically used 3 bins to create a strong contrast between weak and strong MEPs while providing a sufficiently high number of trials in each category. For the weakest and strongest MEP bins, we then contrasted prestimulus HEP amplitudes between 296 and 400 ms in a cluster of electrodes (FC2, Cz, C4, CP1, CP2, Pz, P4, C1, C2, CPz, CP4, P1, P2), where we previously observed significant modulations of HEP preceding somatosensory processes [11,13]. In the cluster analysis of both TEPs and HEPs, clusters were formed in the spatiotemporal domain using the a priori defined set of electrodes and temporal windows.

During the motor task, the statistical analysis focused on the first second of the muscle contraction following the pinch onset, as cardiac effects are expected to be transient and may last for one cardiac cycle only. Therefore, statistical analysis of the EMG envelope during systole and diastole were conducted in a time window from 0 to 1,000 ms. During the same time window, sensorimotor oscillations were compared in the range from 8 to 30 Hz over a set of electrodes over sensorimotor regions (C4, CP4, C6, CP6) using cluster statistics, in order to account for multiple comparisons in the temporal, spatial, and frequency domain.

Linear-mixed-effects model

To test the relationship between MEP amplitudes and the distance from the previous heartbeat, linear-mixed-effects models were fitted on the single-trial level. First, we hierarchically compared a null model, which assumes no relationship (MEP ~ (1 | participant) to a model that assumes a linear relationship between MEP amplitudes and the distance (MEP ~ distance + (1 | participant)). Then, we compared this linear model to a second-degree polynomial model (MEP) ~ distance + distance^2+ (1 | participant). In these models, we used the natural logarithmic transformation of MEP amplitudes.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. The influence of previous heartbeat-evoked potentials (HEPs) on motor excitability.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Heartbeat-evoked potential (HEP) during rest and TMS stimulation.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. MEP amplitudes across the cardiac cycle.

(TIF)

Abbreviations

BEM

boundary element model

ECG

electrocardiography

EEG

electroencephalography

EMG

electromyography

FDI

first dorsal interosseous

HEP

heartbeat-evoked potential

MEP

motor-evoked potential

MVC

maximal voluntary contraction

TEP

TMS-evoked potential

TKE

Teager–Kaiser Energy

TMS

transcranial magnetic stimulation

Data Availability

The experimental code and analysis scripts are available at https://github.com/Esra-Al/Cardiac_Motor_TMS_EEG. Access to the data and code for all analytical figures can be obtained at the following link: https://osf.io/eg8dz?view_only=7edb12b6e50e4c709ad60579bf5ebd62. The datasets generated during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Due to a lack of explicit consent on the part of the participants to data sharing, structural MRI and EEG data cannot be shared publicly. Such data can only be shared if data privacy can be guaranteed according to the rules of the European General Data Protection Regulation. If the corresponding author is not reachable for data request, info@cbs.mpg.de can be contacted.

Funding Statement

For this project, EA received funding from the Max Planck School of Cognition, Walter Benjamin Program of the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the experiment was funded by the Max Planck Society. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Allen M., Frank D., Samuel Schwarzkopf D., Fardo F., Winston J. S., Hauser T. U., Rees G., Unexpected arousal modulates the influence of sensory noise on confidence. Elife. (2016) 5, doi: 10.7554/eLife.18103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Critchley H. D., Garfinkel S. N., The influence of physiological signals on cognition. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2018) 19, 13–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Azzalini D., Rebollo I., Tallon-Baudry C., Visceral Signals Shape Brain Dynamics and Cognition. Trends Cogn Sci. (2019) 23, 488–509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Perl O., Ravia A., Rubinson M., Eisen A., Soroka T., Mor N., Secundo L., Sobel N., Human non-olfactory cognition phase-locked with inhalation. Nat Hum Behav. (2019) 2019. 35. 3, 501–512. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0556-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Park H.-D., Barnoud C., Trang H., Kannape O. A., Schaller K., Blanke O., Breathing is coupled with voluntary action and the cortical readiness potential. Nat Commun. (2020) 2020. 111. 11, 1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13967-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Grund M., Al E., Pabst M., Dabbagh A., Stephani T., Nierhaus T., Gaebler M., Villringer A., J Neurosci, in press, doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0592-21.2021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Sandman C. A., McCanne T. R., Kaiser D. N., Diamond B., Heart rate and cardiac phase influences on visual perception. J Comp Physiol Psychol. (1977) 91, 189–202. doi: 10.1037/h0077302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Saxon S. A., Detection of near threshold signals during four phases of cardiac cycle. Ala J Med Sci. (1970) 7, 427–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Van Elk M., Lenggenhager B., Heydrich L., Blanke O., Suppression of the auditory N1-component for heartbeat-related sounds reflects interoceptive predictive coding. Biol Psychol. (2014) 99, 172–182. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wilkinson M., McIntyre D., Edwards L., Electrocutaneous pain thresholds are higher during systole than diastole. Biol Psychol. (2013) 94, 71–73. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Al E., Iliopoulos F., Forschack N., Nierhaus T., Grund M., Motyka P., Gaebler M., Nikulin V. V., Villringer A., Heart-brain interactions shape somatosensory perception and evoked potentials. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2020) 117, 10575–10584. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1915629117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Motyka P., Grund M., Forschack N., Al E., Villringer A., Gaebler M., Interactions between cardiac activity and conscious somatosensory perception. Psychophysiology, (2019) e13424. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Al E., Iliopoulos F., Nikulin V. V., Villringer A., Heartbeat and somatosensory perception. Neuroimage. (2021) 238, 118247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Duschek S., Werner N. S., del Paso G. A. R., The behavioral impact of baroreflex function: A review. Psychophysiology. (2013) 50, 1183–1193. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Edwards L., Ring C., McIntyre D., Carroll D., Martin U., Psychomotor speed in hypertension: Effects of reaction time components, stimulus modality, and phase of the cardiac cycle. Psychophysiology. (2007) 44, 459–468. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00521.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Saari M. J., Pappas B. A., Cardiac Cycle Phase and Movement and Reaction Times. Percept Mot Skills. (1976) 42, 767–770. doi: 10.2466/pms.1976.42.3.767 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Galvez-Pol A., Virdee P., Villacampa J., Kilner J. M., Active tactile discrimination is coupled with and modulated by the cardiac cycle. Elife. (2022) 11, doi: 10.7554/eLife.78126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kunzendorf S., Klotzsche F., Akbal M., Villringer A., Ohl S., Gaebler M., Active information sampling varies across the cardiac cycle. Psychophysiology. (2019) 56, 1–16. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13322 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Palser E. R., Glass J., Fotopoulou A., Kilner J. M., Relationship between cardiac cycle and the timing of actions during action execution and observation. Cognition. (2021) 217, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Herman A. M., Tsakiris M., Feeling in Control: The Role of Cardiac Timing in the Sense of Agency. Affect Sci. (2020) 2020. 13. 1, 155–171. doi: 10.1007/s42761-020-00013-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Konttinen N., Mets T., Lyytinen H., Paananen M., Timing of triggering in relation to the cardiac cycle in nonelite rifle shooters. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2003) 74, 395–400. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2003.10609110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Helin P., Sihvonen T., Hänninen O., Timing of the triggering action of shooting in relation to the cardiac cycle. Br J Sports Med. (1987) 21, 33–36. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.21.1.33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Galvez-Pol A., McConnell R., Kilner J. M., Active sampling in visual search is coupled to the cardiac cycle. Cognition. (2020) 196, doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ohl S., Wohltat C., Kliegl R., Pollatos O., Engbert R., Microsaccades are coupled to heartbeat. J Neurosci. (2016) 36, 1237–1241. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2211-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Park H.-D., Correia S., Ducorps A., Tallon-Baudry C., Spontaneous fluctuations in neural responses to heartbeats predict visual detection. Nat Neurosci. (2014) 17, 612–618. doi: 10.1038/nn.3671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bianchini E., Mancuso M., Zampogna A., Guerra A., Suppa A., Cardiac cycle does not affect motor evoked potential variability: A real-time EKG-EMG study. Brain Stimul. (2021) 14, 170–172. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2020.12.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ellaway P. H., Davey N. J., Maskill D. W., Rawlinson S. R., Lewis H. S., Anissimova N. P., Variability in the amplitude of skeletal muscle responses to magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Mot Control. (1998) 109, 104–113. doi: 10.1016/s0924-980x(98)00007-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Otsuru N., Miyaguchi S., Kojima S., Yamashiro K., Sato D., Yokota H., Saito K., Inukai Y., Onishi H., Timing of Modulation of Corticospinal Excitability by Heartbeat Differs with Interoceptive Accuracy. Neuroscience. (2020) 433, 156–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Filippi M. M., Oliveri M., Vernieri F., Pasqualetti P., Rossini P. M., Are autonomic signals influencing cortico-spinal motor excitability?: A study with transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Res. (2000) 881, 159–164. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02837-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hernandez-Pavon J. C., Veniero D., Bergmann T. O., Belardinelli P., Bortoletto M., Casarotto S., Casula E. P., Farzan F., Fecchio M., Julkunen P., Kallioniemi E., Lioumis P., Metsomaa J., Miniussi C., Mutanen T. P., Rocchi L., Rogasch N. C., Shafi M. M., Siebner H. R., Thut G., Zrenner C., Ziemann U., Ilmoniemi R. J., TMS combined with EEG: Recommendations and open issues for data collection and analysis. Brain Stimul. (2023) 16, 567–593. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2023.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Nikouline V., Ruohonen J., Ilmoniemi R. J., The role of the coil click in TMS assessed with simultaneous EEG. Clin Neurophysiol. (1999) 110, 1325–1328. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00070-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Iseger T. A., Padberg F., Kenemans J. L., van Dijk H., Arns M., Neuro-Cardiac-Guided TMS (NCG TMS): A replication and extension study. Biol Psychol. (2021) 162, 108097. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Zhong X., Meyer C. H., Schlesinger D. J., Sheehan J. P., Epstein F. H., Larner J. M., Benedict S. H., Read P. W., Sheng K., Cai J., Tracking brain motion during the cardiac cycle using spiral cine-DENSE MRI. Med Phys. (2009) 36, 3413–3419. doi: 10.1118/1.3157109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hannula H., Ilmoniemi R. J., "Basic Principles of Navigated TMS BT—Navigated Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in Neurosurgery" in, Krieg S. M., Ed. (Springer International Publishing, Cham, 2017; 10.1007/978-3-319-54918-7_1), pp. 3–29. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Morita H., Baumgarten J., Petersen N., Christensen L. O. D., Nielsen J., Recruitment of extensor-carpi-radialis motor units by transcranial magnetic stimulation and radial-nerve stimulation in human subjects. Exp Brain Res. (1999) 128, 557–562. doi: 10.1007/s002210050881 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Harquel S., Bacle T., Beynel L., Marendaz C., Chauvin A., David O., Mapping dynamical properties of cortical microcircuits using robotized TMS and EEG: Towards functional cytoarchitectonics. Neuroimage. (2016) 135, 115–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wagshul M. E., Eide P. K., Madsen J. R., The pulsating brain: A review of experimental and clinical studies of intracranial pulsatility. Fluids Barriers CNS. (2011) 8, pp. 1–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Lawrence J. H., De Luca C. J., Myoelectric signal versus force relationship in different human muscles. J Appl Physiol. (1983) 54, 1653–1659. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1983.54.6.1653 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Pfurtscheller G., Stancak A., Neuper C., Neuper C., Event-related synchronization (ERS) in the alpha band -an electrophysiological correlate of cortical idling: A review. Int J Psychophysiol. (1996) 24, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(96)00066-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Pfurtscheller G., Lopes da Silva F. H., Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin Neurophysiol. (1999) 110, 1842–1857. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00141-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Allen M., Levy A., Parr T., Friston K. J., In the Body’s Eye: The Computational Anatomy of Interoceptive Inference. bioRxiv, (2019) 603928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Milne R. J., Aniss A. M., Kay N. E., Gandevia S. C., Reduction in perceived intensity of cutaneous stimuli during movement: a quantitative study. Exp Brain Res. (1988) 70, 569–576. doi: 10.1007/BF00247604 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Starr A., Cohen L. G., “Gating” of somatosensory evoked potentials begins before the onset of voluntary movement in man. Brain Res. (1985) 348, 183–186. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90377-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Seki K., Fetz E. E., Gating of Sensory Input at Spinal and Cortical Levels during Preparation and Execution of Voluntary Movement. J Neurosci. (2012) 32, 890. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4958-11.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rae C. L., Ahmad A., Larsson D. E. O., Silva M., van Praag C. D. G., Garfinkel S. N., Critchley H. D., Impact of cardiac interoception cues and confidence on voluntary decisions to make or withhold action in an intentional inhibition task. Sci Rep. (2020) 10, doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-60405-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Edwards L., Inui K., Ring C., Wang X., Kakigi R., Pain-related evoked potentials are modulated across the cardiac cycle. Pain. (2008) 137, 488–494. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.10.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Rau H., Elbert T., Psychophysiology of arterial baroreceptors and the etiology of hypertension. Biol Psychol. (2001) 57, 179–201. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(01)00094-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lacey J. I., Lacey B. C., "Some autonomic-central nervous system interrelationships" in (1970; https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/B9780121028503500165?via%3Dihub), pp. 205–227. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Park H.-D., Blanke O., Heartbeat-evoked cortical responses: Underlying mechanisms, functional roles, and methodological considerations. Neuroimage. (2019) 197, 502–511. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.04.081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Johansen-Berg H., Rushworth M. F., Bogdanovic M. D., Kischka U., Wimalaratna S., Matthews P. M., The role of ipsilateral premotor cortex in hand movement after stroke. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2002) 99, 14518–14523. doi: 10.1073/pnas.222536799 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Blumberger D. M., Vila-Rodriguez F., Thorpe K. E., Feffer K., Noda Y., Giacobbe P., Knyahnytska Y., Kennedy S. H., Lam R. W., Daskalakis Z. J., Downar J., Effectiveness of theta burst versus high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with depression (THREE-D): a randomised non-inferiority trial. Lancet. (2018) 391, 1683–1692. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30295-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Luft C. D. B., Bhattacharya J., Aroused with heart: Modulation of heartbeat evoked potential by arousal induction and its oscillatory correlates. Sci Rep. (2015) 5, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/srep15717 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Löfberg O., Julkunen P., Pääkkönen A., Karhu J., The auditory-evoked arousal modulates motor cortex excitability. Neuroscience. (2014) 274, 403–408. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.05.060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Azarbarzin A., Ostrowski M., Hanly P., Younes M., Relationship between arousal intensity and heart rate response to arousal. Sleep. (2014) 37, 645–653. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3560 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Stephani T., Waterstraat G., Haufe S., Curio G., Villringer A., Nikulin V. V., Temporal Signatures of Criticality in Human Cortical Excitability as Probed by Early Somatosensory Responses. J Neurosci. (2020) 40, 6572–6583. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0241-20.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ito J., Roy S., Liu Y., Cao Y., Fletcher M., Lu L., Boughter J. D., Grün S., Heck D. H., Whisker barrel cortex delta oscillations and gamma power in the awake mouse are linked to respiration. Nat Commun. (2014) 2014. 51. 5, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4572 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kluger D. S., Balestrieri E., Busch N. A., Gross J., Respiration aligns perception with neural excitability. Elife. (2021) 10, doi: 10.7554/eLife.70907 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Oldfield R. C., The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. (1971) 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rossini P. M., Barker A. T., Berardelli A., Caramia M. D., Caruso G., Cracco R. Q., Dimitrijević M. R., Hallett M., Katayama Y., Lücking C. H., Maertens de Noordhout A. L., Marsden C. D., Murray N. M. F., Rothwell J. C., Swash M., Tomberg C., Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1994) 91, 79–92. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90029-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Vázquez-Seisdedos C. R., Neto J. E., Marañón Reyes E. J., Klautau A., Limão de Oliveira R. C., New approach for T-wave end detection on electrocardiogram: performance in noisy conditions. Biomed Eng Online. (2011) 10, 77. doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-10-77 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Rogasch N. C., Sullivan C., Thomson R. H., Rose N. S., Bailey N. W., Fitzgerald P. B., Farzan F., Hernandez-Pavon J. C., Analysing concurrent transcranial magnetic stimulation and electroencephalographic data: A review and introduction to the open-source TESA software. Neuroimage. (2017) 147, 934–951. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Hyvärinen A., Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis. IEEE Trans Neural Netw. (1999) 10, 626–634. doi: 10.1109/72.761722 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Tadel F., Baillet S., Mosher J. C., Pantazis D., Leahy R. M., Brainstorm: A user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput Intell Neurosci. (2011) 2011, doi: 10.1155/2011/879716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Kybic J., Clerc M., Abboud T., Faugeras O., Keriven R., Papadopoulo T., A common formalism for the Integral formulations of the forward EEG problem. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. (2005) 24, 12–28. doi: 10.1109/tmi.2004.837363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Gramfort A., Papadopoulo T., Olivi E., Clerc M., OpenMEEG: opensource software for quasistatic bioelectromagnetics. Biomed Eng Online. (2010) 9, 45. doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-9-45 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Fonov V., Evans A., McKinstry R., Almli C., Collins D., Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. Neuroimage. (2009) 47, S102. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Yang D., Zhang H., Gu Y., Liu H., Accurate EMG onset detection in pathological, weak and noisy myoelectric signals. Biomed Signal Process Control. (2017) 33, 306–315. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Winter D. A., Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement: Fourth Edition. (2009), pp. 1–370. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Oostenveld R., Fries P., Maris E., Schoffelen J.-M., FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data. Comput Intell Neurosci. (2011) 2011, 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Lawrence M., ez: Easy Analysis and Visualization of Factorial Experiments. (2016), (Available from https://cran.r-project.org/package=ez). [Google Scholar]
  • 71.R Development Core Team, R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (2021). [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Lucas Smith

2 Mar 2023

Dear Dr Al,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "Cardiac Activity Impacts Cortical Motor Excitability" for consideration as a Research Article by PLOS Biology.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff as well as by an academic editor with relevant expertise and I am writing to let you know that we would like to send your submission out for external peer review.

However, before we can send your manuscript to reviewers, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks in preparation for peer review. After your manuscript has passed the checks it will be sent out for review. To provide the metadata for your submission, please Login to Editorial Manager (https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology) within two working days, i.e. by Mar 06 2023 11:59PM.

If your manuscript has been previously peer-reviewed at another journal, PLOS Biology is willing to work with those reviews in order to avoid re-starting the process. Submission of the previous reviews is entirely optional and our ability to use them effectively will depend on the willingness of the previous journal to confirm the content of the reports and share the reviewer identities. Please note that we reserve the right to invite additional reviewers if we consider that additional/independent reviewers are needed, although we aim to avoid this as far as possible. In our experience, working with previous reviews does save time.

If you would like us to consider previous reviewer reports, please edit your cover letter to let us know and include the name of the journal where the work was previously considered and the manuscript ID it was given. In addition, please upload a response to the reviews as a 'Prior Peer Review' file type, which should include the reports in full and a point-by-point reply detailing how you have or plan to address the reviewers' concerns.

During the process of completing your manuscript submission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Luke

Lucas Smith, Ph.D.

Associate Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

Decision Letter 1

Lucas Smith

27 Apr 2023

Dear Dr Al,

Thank you for your patience while your manuscript "Cardiac Activity Impacts Cortical Motor Excitability" was peer-reviewed at PLOS Biology. It has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by several independent reviewers.

In light of the reviews, which you will find at the end of this email, we would like to invite you to revise the work to thoroughly address the reviewers' reports.

As you will see below, the reviewers comment that manuscript offers potentially interesting new insights and that the study is generally well done. However they have each raised a number of important points that will need to be addressed before we can consider your manuscript for publication. We think that an theme that emerges from the reviews, and which will require particular attention, is that the writing of the manuscript needs to be revised to improve the coherence of the story. In particular, we suggest the results be carefully rewritten to ensure that each set of analyses is preceded by a motivation (what was the aim? why was it done?) and followed by a short conclusion (what exactly did we learn from this analysis?) as we think this will improve the broad accessibility of the manuscript.

In addition to addressing the reviewer comments, we also have a number of editorial reporting and policy related requests which we ask that you attend to. I have appended those below the reviews.

Given the extent of revision needed, we cannot make a decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript may be sent for further evaluation by all or a subset of the reviewers.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 3 months. Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension.

At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may withdraw it.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point-by-point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually, point by point.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Revised Article with Changes Highlighted" file type.

*Re-submission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this re-submission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Lucas

Lucas Smith, Ph.D.

Associate Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

------------------------------------

REVIEWS:

Reviewer #1: Al et al., examined whether cortical and corticospinal excitability changes as a function of the cardiac phases (systole and diastole). They examined such a potential modulation via TMS (to measure excitability), with EEG to measure TMS and heartbeat-evoked potentials, and ECG to obtain the corresponding phase-locked measures. The authors found higher excitability and greater TMS-evoked potential in the systole phase of the cardiac cycle (vs. sham condition). In addition, they also recorded EMG during a simple motor task (pinching) and examined muscle-related activity. The subsequent results indicate that EMG activity was higher circa 220-520ms after the pinch onset. If the pinch was initiated in systole, desynchronization of sensorimotor oscillations was higher in the frequency range of alpha and beta oscillations. The authors conclude that the systole phase of the cardiac cycle may be related to motor facilitation (action) whereas the diastole phase to perception.

I read the preprint a while ago, and overall, the authors have done an excellent job. The study involves an amalgam of techniques that is not usually found elsewhere. The manuscript addresses pertinent issues in the literature and moves forward the various fields (e.g., interoception). Overall, the authors have produced a stimulating piece of work. The following minor comments, suggestions, and clarifications may enhance the manuscript's scope and clarity. Please find below the comments by section:

Introduction and discussion:

In the introduction, the authors refer to the study by Ohl et al., which demonstrated that microsaccades are coupled to the cardiac cycle. However, the way the manuscript is currently written, it seems as though two studies (references 17-18) showed this finding. In contrast, study 18 (Galvez-Pol et al.) revealed that saccades are locked around systole and visual fixations in diastole, without presenting microsaccade data. Relatedly, Galvez-Pol et al. also suggested that the diastole phase of the cardiac cycle may be linked to sensory gain (perception), which should be more accurately referenced, as the idea behind the current manuscript has been previously noted.

Furthermore, a recent paper (https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.78126) showed that human subjects freely moved their index finger to tap a key during systole, a finding that is consistent with active visual tasks without perceptual discrimination requirements (see Kunzendorf et al., 2019). The same study discovered that when participants had to sense gratings, if touch initiation occurred during systole, they extended the duration of the touch, whereas during diastole, touches were shorter. This implies that participants needed more time for perception when it began in systole or sought to reach a more sensitive phase of the cycle. This work also supports the current manuscript; however, the introduction and discussion do not sufficiently address it. By incorporating these points, the current manuscript will be better situated within the existing literature.

Results:

My primary concern is the clarity of the rationale underpinning the analyses and the subsequent results. In its current state, the manuscript features numerous analyses with insufficient or unclear reasoning. Enhancing this aspect should not be challenging: considering the journal's broad readership, I recommend providing a more comprehensive explanation of the rationale behind the analyses that led to the results. This can be achieved by including the thought process and bits of explanations at the beginning of each subsection, particularly in the second half of the results section (e.g., IBIs and HEPs). By doing so, the manuscript's framework and the coherence of the narrative that the authors aim to present can be enhanced.

Relatedly, it is not clear to me the rationale behind the IBIs section. Why the TMS applied to the motor area should affect distinctively the duration of heartbeats as a function of cardiac phase?

Figure 1: State briefly in the text accompanying the figure that the diastole window has been equated to the length of the systole one for analyses purposes.

MEPs and figure 2: I suggest the authors to plot the scatter data of the MEPs with the box plots (i.e., individual dots depicting the means or medians of the subjects). If the plot changes the resolution of the axis and the difference between conditions is not easily discernible, the authors can always add a new plot of the difference by subtracting the systole and diastole MEPs (as visual aid).

Figure 4: If I understand correctly, event-related desynchronization following muscle activation was computed for the frequency range between 8 and 25Hz. Were alpha and beat analysed separately or together? The analysis seems to indicate 'together' while the topoplots 'separate'. If the latter is true, I advise consistency between analysis and figures; show one topoplot instead of two.

Discussion:

Regarding the studies about shooting in systole: also note that elite rifle shooters pull the trigger during diastole, with beginners firing during either phase but with better results during diastole (Helin et al., 1987). It has been suggested that this effect is related to the mechanical movement caused by the heart's contraction (Konttinen et al., 2003).

In the discussion section, I advise the authors to write a section of Constraints on Generality (COG); see Simmons et al., 2017. This involves adding some additional information about the constraints of the current work in other scenarios, tasks, paradigms, populations, etc. (see examples in the Simmons' paper). This should not be too long. This section could be also combined with the second paragraph of the discussion (which is more centred on methodological features of the present and past studies).

The current results can be mentioned within recent discoveries on the relationship between breathing and neural function to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the interactions between physiological processes and cognition. By incorporating (briefly) the findings on the respiratory cycle, the scope of the paper can be broadened, and the potential interactions between cardiac and respiratory cycles in modulating neural activity can be discussed. For instance, the research of Kluger et al., (2021) and Allen's review on the topic (2022) can be mentioned in this context.

Methods:

To enhance clarity, what is the reason for the interpolation between -2 and 15ms in step 9? (Aligning the data, missing data..)

Independent Component Analysis (ICA) is a widely used technique for removing artifacts in EEG data. A good point is that It separates the mixed signals into statistically independent components, which can then be analyzed and classified as either neural or artifactual. I believe the implementation of ICA benefits from the data filtering (step 5) in Figure 7. How were the parameters chosen for this filter? (I thought 1Hz was recommended). Also, can the authors provide additional information about the removed ICA? (e.g., did the authors follow any rule or visual inspection? And/or how many ICs were approximately removed in average?). One issue with this sort of study is the quantity of removed data from the recording. In this context, researchers using ICA and removing TMS or cardiac artifacts may face ambiguity in component classification. In other words, identifying and classifying independent components as artifacts or neural signals can be challenging and may require expert knowledge. This can lead to false positives (removing neural signals) or false negatives (retaining artifacts). Also, ICA assumes statistical independence. ICA relies on the assumption that sources are statistically independent. If this assumption is violated, the separation of artifacts from neural signals may be less accurate.

Reviewer #2: The manuscript describe an interesting and timely study on the modulation of cortical and cortico-spinal excitability by cardiac cycle phases (systole vs. diastole). The authors test two alternative hypotheses underlying the changes in perceptual threshold and motor execution, based on earlier literature that have shown both attenuation of somatosensory perception and facilitation of motor activity during systole. Their findings support facilitatory effect on motor excitability, as they show higher central and peripheral TMS-evoked responses during systole. They also report several related evidence including effects in cardiac evoked activity and heart rate, as well as stronger muscle activity and sensorimotor cortex desynchronization during systole. The paper is generally well written, and the experiments are conducted well, with carefully planned control conditions. The number of subjects is not very big, but of needed size to allow appropriate statistical testing, with appropriate power analysis (however, see comment below related to the statistics). There are some questions, suggestions and concerns I think would need to be addressed to improve the readability and confirm the significance of the study for the field. Currently the paper is not written in a generally accessible way, and therefore it is hard to say to what extent it gives outsanding contribution. The structure and multitude of the findings are presented in rather fragmented manner and do not develop into coherent picture of a) what is the current gap (and novelty the paper brings) in the literature and b) how / why are all the different experimental manipulations and measurements needed to fill this gap.

First, it is not entirely clear to me why are the authors forming two alternative hypotheses (e.g. Intro end of page 3), one based on earlier results on *perceptual* threshold and the other based on earlier results on *motor* execution. Their research examines only motor cortex excitability and peripheral motor evoked potentials, and thus it seems more suitable to make hypothesis based on earlier studies on the motor side. Other cardiac-related effects are of course interesting and worth reporting, but I wonder whether they should be presented as equally relevant with the motor studies. Also in the discussion the impression is given that the initial assumption would be to see similar cardiac-brain coupling for perception and action; in my understanding this is not what the earlier literature would propose - I may be mistaken but clearer and more elaborate reasoning on the background would better justify the novelty of the paper.

The research question is clear, but the way the different recordings, manipulations and analysis pipelines are used to answer to this question is not explained well. This gives a rather fragmented impression, for example in the list of results (different sub-titles) in the Result section, as well as beginning of discussion. Now it appears almost like the analysis has been proceeding on ad-hoc manner from one result to the next. Perhaps formulating better in the introduction the way in which (and why) each data/manipulation/analysis is planned to be used would clarify the significance of the list of results. For example, it took some time to figure out how cardiac phase-related changes in TEP's should be interpreted together with heart-evoked responses, where the phase information is lost (and all the responses are similarly phase-locked to heartbeat) - or with effects in heart rate. In general, the paper appears like not fully 'matured' to a form where its scientific significance would be easily grasped. This is especially problematic from the perspective of 'non-expert' readers (or readers from other fields).

The introduction is very concise, and although it reviews the relevant literature related to cardiac-brain coupling, especially the means to measure cortical excitability is introduced in rather superficial and one-sided manner. There are some recent critiques (or at least needs for caution) presented for the straightforward interpretation of TEP's as an index of cortical excitability (especially for the early time-windows, see eg. Hernandez-Pavon et al., 2023). It would provide a more solid basis for the used methodology if the characteristics and possible reservations in using TEPs as indicators of cortical excitability would be good to at least mention in the introduction. This way also the following description of the control conditions in the result section would make more sense for the reader.

Related to the above, Result section does not give enough info about the SHAM condition (how/why was it done) for integrating it to the general picture.

Figure 1 gives helpful info about the methods for the reader to understand the results, but it could be better organized to help grasping the different parts of the experiment (cf. 3rd paragraph above). Moreover, it could be more informative if the timelines of the EEG, ECG and EMG would be synchronized (to understand their temporal relationships) and timeline should be added. A combined or synchronized timeline would also help to estimate the overlap of artefacts/responses.

What is the reason for presenting the correlational analysis of MEP and TEP amplitude differences across cardiac cycle first as 'initial analysis' and the following analysis of the cardiac changes in MEP against the modulation of earlier TEP's as 'exploratory'. When reading the results, it is not clear what is the difference between these analysis approaches and/or what is the particular research questions they respond to.

As a detail (and a topic outside of my expertise): the blood-circulation -related contribution to the cardiac cycle effect on pinching-related EMG envelope is controlled by the analysis of rest condition (?) - is there a possibility that this does not properly represent the situation during muscle activity, and would this influence to the interpretation? In other words, can it be that the blood-circulation related modulation is different (more prominent) when muscles are activated, and thus the contrasting of diastole vs. systole during rest is not a valid control condition to rule out this impact?

Results related to the heart rate changes; some more details should be given for interpreting the results; from what time-window was the heart rate calculated (and what measure was used? Heart acceleration or interval between beats? Where is the analysis described for the heart rate related measures? Authors controlled for the possible artefacts (auditory, somatosensory) on the effects on heart rate by adjusting the heart rate with the one during sham TMS condition; why was this procedure chosen and not conducting the similar analysis for the TMS condition (i.e. time x diastole/systole)? Or is this what authors mean by adjusting?

Results regarding the HEP & MEP analysis; what was the way of categorizing trials to strong vs. weak MEP (the cut point would be useful to mention in the results).

The results (particularly for heart rate changes depending on the timing of TMS and HEP fluctuation depending on motor excitability levels) describe a rather complicated scenario with several dependencies between the TMS pulse, cortical responses and motor evoked potential - it would be really helpful for the reader to get some illustration to support the following of the analysis/results. Along the same lines, it would also guide the reading (and help to confirm the logic of the analysis) if the introduction would give at least a rough outline of the planned examination (this is mentioned also earlier). Now the result section progresses in somewhat unpredictable (but apparently meaningful) steps; for example categorizing the separate sections based on the specific part of the research question they respond to would help to keep track of the results (together with the illustration).

Last result section, regarding the control analysis, do I understand correctly that this analysis is based on the known motion related to cardiac phases or was the analysis done based on the current data?

I am not fully following the reasoning why MEP would be expected to follow a similar pattern as the movement of the brain, if the TMS-effects were due to the movement?

The beginning of discussion gives a nice overview on the results, but here again it may not be self-evident for the reader why and how the motor pinch task-related cardiac-related changes in muscle activity and cortical descynronization and especially the TMS-triggered heart-rate changes are linked to the original excitability changes. In my view, too much expert knowledge is expected form the reader to understand the value and meaning of the findings for the field. What is the phenomenon on which this paper brings the outstanding contribution? Why is it interesting and to what scientific debate or discussion does it contribute to? Occasionally the paper stays at the level of 'technically neat and comprehensive list of results'.

Although not specifically my own field, I wonder how the cardiac cycle influences to the ability of (peripheral) muscles to contract, and how would this influence to the interpretation of the current results?

In Discussion p. 16 the authors present nice reasoning on how the cardiac modulation of perception vs. action could be linked to each other. Do I understand correctly that the authors suggest that the driving force for both the effects in perception and motor activity arise from the need to dampen the rhythmic cardiac signals not to enter conscious perception? This explanation, building on the idea of 'noise cancellation', would naturally explain why there would be any need for rhythmic variation in the perceptual thresholds and motor activity with such a short time-scale. What if this assumption is not correct? Are there alternative ways to explain the effects and how does this reasoning link with the current literature and other speculations on the heart-brain coupling? For better linking with the current literature, it would be important to present the current findings & suggested hypotheses in the broader literature on the topic - the body-brain coupling is a very active field at the moment and there are many recent papers and reviews on the topic.

Authors also speculate their findings could reflect a direct neural pathway via cardiac afferent neurons. This is an interesting suggestion for further studies, but it contrasts with the very narrow background on anatomical (and animal model) research presented in the introduction. What is currently known about the cardiac-brain connections? Are there studies supporting direct connections?

Methods section gives detailed protocols for each data / analysis type, but it remains unclear which part of these analysis steps are according to golden standard principles/established (and published) protocols. Whenever possible, it would help if the authors cited any available papers describing the pipelines or principles that are followed in the analysis.

As a detail, in p. 22, line 565 onwards, authors say that only trials in which TMS stimulation triggered high-enough motor-evoked potential were included, and for sham only trials in which no evoked motor activity was observed were included. Why was this the case? It would help (especially the non-expert reader) to remind where were these categories/contrasts used in the analysis.

In p. 23, Source Analysis, line 614, authors say that individual source data were projected to the template brain, and their absolute values were used in group averages - what group averages are referred to?

Several different statistical tests are used for various time-windows, contrasts etc. How is the problem of multiple testing (also across different data types/contrasts) dealt with? The power analysis is only reported for limited tests, but given the numerous different data and manipulations, it may not be sufficient to guarantee rigorous statistical approach. In its current form, it is also difficult to see to what extent is the analysis based on clear hypothesis and to what extent is it more of exploratory nature.

Refs.

Hernandez-Pavon JC, Veniero D, Bergmann TO, Belardinelli P, Bortoletto M, Casarotto S, Casula EP, Farzan F, Fecchio M, Julkunen P, Kallioniemi E, Lioumis P, Metsomaa J, Miniussi C, Mutanen TP, Rocchi L, Rogasch NC, Shafi MM, Siebner HR, Thut G, Zrenner C, Ziemann U, Ilmoniemi RJ. TMS combined with EEG: Recommendations and open issues for data collection and analysis. Brain Stimul. 2023 Feb 23;16(2):567-593. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2023.02.009. Epub ahead of print. PMID: 36828303.

Reviewer #3, Florent Lebon (Note, reviewer 3 has signed this review): In the current study, the authors investigated the influence of cardiac cycles (systole and diastole) on corticospinal and cortical excitability and on muscle activity during contraction. They also tested the effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on cardiac measures and the relationship between heartbeat-effect cortical activity and corticospinal excitability. Contrary to the literature, the authors found greater corticospinal and cortical activity, as well as greater muscle activity during a specific time-window of pinch movement, during systole in comparison to diastole. Overall, this study brings further information regarding the relationship between cardiac activity and cortical and corticospinal excitability. However, it does not provide evidence of functional effects.

Please find general remarks and minor comments below.

In the abstract, the authors wrote that "Cortical and corticospinal excitability were found to be highest (…) following stronger cortical responses to heartbeats". This sentence is ambiguous as cortical activity is mentioned twice (as the source and the consequence).

In the introduction section, the authors stipulated that "What remains unknown are the underlying mechanisms of these effects". The authors could be more specific to highlight the goal and the originality of their study, to explain the opposite results.

It is not clear if the authors synchronized the TMS pulses and the cardiac phases or if they stimulated, then categorized a posteriori in which cycle the TMS pulse was triggered.

l.172 "significant increase in the normalized EMG envelope". Normalized to what?

Did the authors use the force produced as a covariable? As it may have influenced the EMG signal.

l.180 "we also tested whether sensorimotor oscillations (in the range of 8 - 30 Hz)" "the desynchronization of sensorimotor oscillations in the range of 8 - 25 Hz was stronger". Why this range? And why is the range different (8-30; 8-25)?

l.234: "we sorted single trials according to their MEP amplitudes and split them into three equal bins for each participant". Why did the authors choose 3 bins (and not 2 or 4)?

From l. 203 and l.255: it is not clear why the authors investigated "the changes in the heart rate in response to TMS stimulation" and "the effect of motor excitability on heart rates". After focusing on the modulations of central and peripheral changes across the cardiac cycles, the authors analyzed the other way around. They should clarify why these analyses were important to answer the general problematic.

L.260 "as motor excitability increased, the heartbeats became faster (cardiac acceleration)". Could we also say that "as heartbeats became faster, motor excitability increased"? How can the authors interpret the causality between heartbeat and MEP amplitude modulation? And I would say corticospinal or corticomotor (not just motor) excitability when interpreting MEP amplitude modulation.

l.366 "Overall, these results suggest that motor excitability is higher during systole, suggesting an optimal window for motor activity across the cardiac cycle". Could the authors be more specific about this argument? Do the small (but significant) differences in cortical and corticospinal excitability across the cardiac cycles prevail for functional efficiency?

l.468: "Given a Cohen's d of 0.48, we calculated the required sample size as N=36 (80% power and alpha=0.05) for observing cardiac effects on perception". Why do the authors refer to 'perception' here?

l.627: "The trials were categorized into systole and diastole according to the movement onset of the subject after the visual cue". Was the beginning of the contraction synchronized with the cardiac cycles? Or was the categorization performed a posteriori? How many trials during systole?

Also, between the presentation of the Go signal, and the beginning of the contraction, there is a reaction time (between 130 and 150ms if the instructions are to move as fast as possible at the imperative signal). How did the authors make sure that the analyzed trial was categorized into the right category? Did they take into account the presentation of the signal or the beginning of EMG activation? EMG activation could begin during systole while the presentation of the signal was during diastole, and inversely.

l.638: "an average of the envelope was calculated when pinch onset coincided with the systolic and diastolic phases of the cardiac cycle per subject". What was the window for averaging EMG envelope?

------------------------------------

EDITORIAL REQUESTS:

1) TITLE: After some discussion, we think the title could be strengthened to more directly reflect the findings of your study. If you agree (and if supported), we might suggest it be changed to something like "Systolic cardiac signals facilitate motor excitability'

2) FINANCIAL DISCLOSURES: I noticed in your financial disclosure that you report having received no specific funding for this work. Even if you did not have a grant specifically written to fund this project, we would require that you acknowledge the sources of money that funded this project, including salaries, etc. You should also acknowledge institutional funding or private grants as well, if relevant.

3) ETHICS STATEMENT: Please update your ethics statement to note whether your study was conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki.

4) BLURB: When re-submitting, please provide a blurb which (if accepted) will be included in our weekly and monthly Electronic Table of Contents, sent out to readers of PLOS Biology, and may be used to promote your article in social media. The blurb should be about 30-40 words long and is subject to editorial changes. It should, without exaggeration, entice people to read your manuscript. It should not be redundant with the title and should not contain acronyms or abbreviations.

5) DATA AVAILABILITY: You may be aware of the PLOS Data Policy, which requires that all data be made available without restriction: http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability. For more information, please also see this editorial: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001797

Note that we do not require all raw data (we understand that your protocol does not permit the public distribution of the raw data collected in your study). Rather, we ask that all individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures and results of your paper be made available in one of the following forms:

a - Supplementary files (e.g., excel). Please ensure that all data files are uploaded as 'Supporting Information' and are invariably referred to (in the manuscript, figure legends, and the Description field when uploading your files) using the following format verbatim: S1 Data, S2 Data, etc. Multiple panels of a single or even several figures can be included as multiple sheets in one excel file that is saved using exactly the following convention: S1_Data.xlsx (using an underscore).

b- Deposition in a publicly available repository. Please also provide the accession code or a reviewer link so that we may view your data before publication.

Regardless of the method selected, please ensure that you provide the individual numerical values that underlie the summary data displayed in the following figure panels as they are essential for readers to assess your analysis and to reproduce it:

Fig 2B; Fig 3; Fig 4; Fig 5; Fig 6; Fig S1; Fig S2; Fig S3;

NOTE: the numerical data provided should include all replicates AND the way in which the plotted mean and errors were derived (it should not present only the mean/average values).

---Please also ensure that figure legends in your manuscript include information on where the underlying data can be found, and ensure your supplemental data file/s has a legend.

---Please ensure that your Data Statement in the submission system accurately describes where your data can be found.

Decision Letter 2

Lucas Smith

28 Sep 2023

Dear Dr Al,

Thank you for your patience while we considered your revised manuscript "Cardiac Activity Impacts Cortical Motor Excitability" for consideration as a Research Article at PLOS Biology. Your revised study has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, the Academic Editor and the original reviewers, who are largely satisfied by the changes made in the revision, but who have a number of additional suggestions and concerns that we think should be addressed.

In light of the reviews, which you will find at the end of this email, we are pleased to invite an additional, short revision to address the remaining points from the reviewers. We will then assess your revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments with our Academic Editor aiming to avoid further rounds of peer-review, although might need to consult with the reviewers, depending on the nature of the revisions.

**EDITORIAL REQUESTS: Thank you also for addressing our previous editorial requests in this revision. For the most part, we are satisfied by your responses, but we noticed a couple of lingering issues with the response to our data policy requests.

1) We see that you have provided code and subject data as a deposition to OSF. Thank you for this. Can you please update the OSF link to include a readme file detailing what is contained there and how it relates to the figures?

2) We understand that you are not able to share the raw EEG and MRI data related to your study due to consent issues. This is OK, under our acceptable data restriction policy (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-acceptable-data-access-restrictions) - but we ask that you please update your data availability statement to provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent (rather than directing researchers to a corresponding author). This provides researchers with a more durable point of contact to ensure data will be accessible even if an author changes email addresses, institutions, or becomes unavailable to answer requests.

3) While we understand that you cannot provide the raw data related to your study, I wonder if for some figures you can provide the anonymized summary statistics that underlie the graphs presented here? For example, can you provide, in an excel or on OSF, the individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures 5, S1, S3?

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 1 month. Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension.

At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we withdraw the manuscript.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point-by-point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Revised Article with Changes Highlighted " file type.

*Resubmission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this resubmission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Lucas

Lucas Smith, Ph.D.

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

----------------------------------------------------------------

REVIEWS:

Reviewer #1: Al et al. investigated the relationship between cardiac phases (systole and diastole) and cortical as well as corticospinal excitability through an amalgam of techniques: TMS, EEG, and ECG. Elevated excitability was observed during the systole phase, alongside increased EMG activity during a simplistic motor task. These findings potentially indicate a correlation between the systole phase and motor facilitation. Upon reviewing both the preprint and subsequent versions, it is apparent that the authors have judiciously employed diverse methodologies to address salient questions, particularly in the realm of interoception. Overall, the manuscript has been substantially improved, aligning well with prior reviewer feedback. The revisions have enhanced the paper's scope, clarity, and primary conclusions. Minor observations, which are readily addressable, are as follows:

General Comments:

1) The results indicating increased excitability are modest, with a p-value approximating 0.040. Consequently, I recommend circumspect language in both the abstract and discussion where these findings are discussed. For example, in the abstract, the phrase "Cortical and corticospinal excitability were found to be highest during systole" could be cautiously reworded. Additionally, consideration should be given to the use of words like "may" in statements like "distinct time windows 'may' exist across the cardiac cycle that either optimize perception or action.". Overall please revise the sentences along the manuscript where absolute/superlative terminology are used to state the results of excitability. Also, noting at least once that such results yielded modest significance would give a transparent and clear image of the present work.

2) The manuscript's discussion section, similar to the introduction and results in the earlier version, suffers from a disordered flow of ideas. Adding subheadings to delineate different content types (e.g., Cortical Excitability and Cardiac Cycle, Brain Activity in Systole and Diastole, Further Research and Limitations, etc.) may enhance readability and engagement. Otherwise, the discussion is sometimes read as a bit of chaotic amalgam of ideas/results.

Specific Comment:

The study by Galvez-Pol et al. (2020) mentioned that pulsations due to blood circulation could contribute to observed effects of the cardiac cycle on cognition but did not delve/research this aspect. When referencing this idea in the text, a more nuanced statement or even deleting such statement would be prudent. Otherwise it seems that they (Galvez-Pol) studied such a research question and gave a result accordingly.

Reviewer #2: The authors have clarified the manuscript making it more accessible and the structure now better supports also the understanding of the different data types. All my questions has been thoroughly answered and when relevant manuscript modified accordingly.

As a minor puzzling point: the rationale/motivation for including the TMS induced effects in heart rate is still not clear to me based on the introduction; the added sentence only mentions it is included leaving the topic rather separate from the rest of the introduction. However, in the discussion, heart rate results are elaborated, making it clearer and more understandable how this analysis/section link with the overall scope of the study. I wonder if it would be possible to add some more reasoning from this perspective to introduction. This issue does not require re-review.

Typo: line 158-159; neuronal responses e by TMS, it should not be present in the sham condition.

Reviewer #3, Florent Lebon (note, this reviewer has signed their review): The authors responded point-by-point to my remarks.

While they made substantial modifications within the manuscript to provide a clear message, I still have few general concerns, which could be easily addressed.

General concerns

I do not know the policy of the journal, but it seems odd to provide the results at the end of the introduction section (l.82-89), without any further information about the protocol. I leave the editors decide whether the authors should keep the structure as it is.

In the current form, the result section looks like an accumulation of analyses with a lack of coherence between the different sub-sections. While the two first paragraphs ("Motor-evoked potentials change across the cardiac cycle", and "TMS-evoked potentials vary across the cardiac cycle") are obvious to respond to the main hypothesis, the other parts (e.g., "Muscle-related peripheral and central activity fluctuates across the cardiac cycle", "Heart rate changes depending on the timing of TMS across the cardiac cycle") are less likely to provide a clear message. I suggest the authors to provide a short sentence at the beginning of each result sub-section, explaining why this is important to perform these analyses. This will help the reader understand the common thread.

Is there any relationship between the paragraphs "Heart rate changes depending on the timing of TMS across the cardiac cycle" line 203 and "Heart rate fluctuates depending on motor excitability levels" line 255? Both deal with "changes in heart rate due to the cardiac timing of TMS". Is there any chance that both paragraphs could be merged? Or at least one after the other?

The paragraph lines 415-430 should be revised. In rehabilitation, repetitive TMS is used to improve the condition of the patients. Single-pulse TMS (as the method used in this study) provides information about a neurophysiological status at the time of the stimulation. Repetitive TMS includes blocks of stimulation, for which the duration is between 3 and 15 minutes. In the article by Johansen-Berg et al. (reference 45), single-pulse TMS is used to estimate the involvement of specific brain in regions in motor control comparing healthy individuals and stroke patients. I would suggest the authors to revise this perspective section, focusing on the evaluation (single or paired-pulse TMS) and not (or less) the treatment (rTMS).

Minor changes

P.2, l.48 : delete « . , » before « though »

Decision Letter 3

Lucas Smith

19 Oct 2023

Dear Dr Al,

Thank you for the submission of your revised Research Article "Cardiac Activity Impacts Cortical Motor Excitability" for publication in PLOS Biology, and thank you also for addressing the last editorial and reviewer requests in this revision. On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Joachim Gross, I am pleased to say that we can in principle accept your manuscript for publication, provided you address any remaining formatting and reporting issues. These will be detailed in an email you should receive within 2-3 business days from our colleagues in the journal operations team; no action is required from you until then. Please note that we will not be able to formally accept your manuscript and schedule it for publication until you have completed any requested changes.

Please take a minute to log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information to ensure an efficient production process.

PRESS

We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have previously opted in to the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may opt out on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for choosing PLOS Biology for publication and supporting Open Access publishing. We look forward to publishing your study. 

Sincerely, 

Lucas Smith, Ph.D.

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. The influence of previous heartbeat-evoked potentials (HEPs) on motor excitability.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. Heartbeat-evoked potential (HEP) during rest and TMS stimulation.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. MEP amplitudes across the cardiac cycle.

    (TIF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Review_plosbiology_round4_VN.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response_plosbiology_rev2_final.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The experimental code and analysis scripts are available at https://github.com/Esra-Al/Cardiac_Motor_TMS_EEG. Access to the data and code for all analytical figures can be obtained at the following link: https://osf.io/eg8dz?view_only=7edb12b6e50e4c709ad60579bf5ebd62. The datasets generated during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Due to a lack of explicit consent on the part of the participants to data sharing, structural MRI and EEG data cannot be shared publicly. Such data can only be shared if data privacy can be guaranteed according to the rules of the European General Data Protection Regulation. If the corresponding author is not reachable for data request, info@cbs.mpg.de can be contacted.


    Articles from PLOS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES