Skip to main content
Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) logoLink to Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY)
. 2023 Oct 13;33(23):11384–11399. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhad373

Graded functional organization in the left inferior frontal gyrus: evidence from task-free and task-based functional connectivity

Veronica Diveica 1,2,, Michael C Riedel 3, Taylor Salo 4, Angela R Laird 5, Rebecca L Jackson 6, Richard J Binney 7,
PMCID: PMC10690868  PMID: 37833772

Abstract

The left inferior frontal gyrus has been ascribed key roles in numerous cognitive domains, such as language and executive function. However, its functional organization is unclear. Possibilities include a singular domain-general function, or multiple functions that can be mapped onto distinct subregions. Furthermore, spatial transition in function may be either abrupt or graded. The present study explored the topographical organization of the left inferior frontal gyrus using a bimodal data-driven approach. We extracted functional connectivity gradients from (i) resting-state fMRI time-series and (ii) coactivation patterns derived meta-analytically from heterogenous sets of task data. We then sought to characterize the functional connectivity differences underpinning these gradients with seed-based resting-state functional connectivity, meta-analytic coactivation modeling and functional decoding analyses. Both analytic approaches converged on graded functional connectivity changes along 2 main organizational axes. An anterior–posterior gradient shifted from being preferentially associated with high-level control networks (anterior functional connectivity) to being more tightly coupled with perceptually driven networks (posterior). A second dorsal–ventral axis was characterized by higher connectivity with domain-general control networks on one hand (dorsal functional connectivity), and with the semantic network, on the other (ventral). These results provide novel insights into an overarching graded functional organization of the functional connectivity that explains its role in multiple cognitive domains.

Keywords: ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, connectivity-based parcellation, meta-analysis, cognitive control, cross-domain

Introduction

The left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) is ascribed a key role in numerous cognitive domains, including language (Friederici 2011), semantics (Lambon Ralph et al. 2017), action (Papitto et al. 2020), social cognition (Diveica et al. 2021), and executive function (Fedorenko et al. 2013). The extent of this overlap is remarkable, but what is driving it is unknown. One possibility is that the LIFG subserves a singular function that manifests as common activation across domains. Alternatively, a detailed exploration of its organization could reveal subregions with multiple functional specializations.

Some clues are gleaned from studies of cellular microstructure and white-matter connectivity that date back to Brodmann (1909). Cytoarchitecture and “fibrillo-architecture” are proposed to determine a region’s functional characteristics by constraining local processing capabilities and the incoming/outgoing flow of information, respectively (Passingham et al. 2002; Cloutman and Lambon Ralph 2012). Indeed, these data reveal that the LIFG is far from uniform and, instead, comprises at least 3 subregions with distinct cytoarchitecture (Amunts et al. 1999; Schenker et al. 2008; Wojtasik et al. 2020), neurotransmitter receptor distributions (Amunts et al. 2010), and structural connectivity (Anwander et al. 2007; Klein et al. 2007; Neubert et al. 2014; Wang et al. 2020). However, the topographic relationship between structural divisions and functional specialization has thus far proven difficult to solve.

Various functional dissociations have been identified within the LIFG by means of functional neuroimaging, including, for example, distinctions between domain-specific (e.g. language-related) and task-general cognitive systems (Fedorenko et al. 2012; Hodgson et al. 2021), between semantic and phonological processing (Devlin et al. 2003; Hodgson et al. 2021), and between memory retrieval and postretrieval selection mechanisms (Badre et al. 2005). It is not yet clear, however, how these observations fit together and whether they can be reconciled under a single unifying framework. One possibility is that there are multiple axes along which LIFG function dissociates. For instance, some functional dissociations are characterized as transitioning along an anterior to posterior axis of the LIFG (e.g. Badre and Wagner 2007; Gough et al. 2005; Spunt and Lieberman 2012), while others also comprise a dorsal–ventral distinction (e.g. Fedorenko et al. 2012; Hodgson et al. 2021; Poldrack et al. 1999). Moreover, the manner in which these transitions occur is also not clear; there may be sharp borders that delineate LIFG subregions (Fedorenko et al. 2012; Fedorenko and Blank 2020), but this contrasts with claims that there are gradual changes in function and areas of overlap (e.g. Choi et al. 2018; Devlin et al. 2003). One of the key reasons that it is challenging to draw together various functional dissociations is that they have been studied in isolation from each other and primarily via hypothesis-driven approaches to small N datasets. This can be overcome by investigating LIFG function via a large-scale data-driven approach and applying it to a dataset that spans multiple cognitive domains (Genon et al. 2018). This approach could better capture the full functional repertoire of a brain region and thereby tease out the underlying and generalizable organizational principles.

Functional connectivity (FC) patterns derived from neuroimaging data are particularly well suited for this aim as they are able to capture the extent to which regional activation covaries over time and, therefore, are sensitive to context-dependent inter-regional interactions. Moreover, they can reveal aspects of the connectome that might not manifest within other modalities (e.g. anatomically derived connectivity); FC can arise between anatomically remote brain areas without direct structural connections (Damoiseaux and Greicius 2009; Suárez et al. 2020). The small number of studies that have attempted to use FC to divide the LIFG into subregions reveal a heterogenous functional architecture (Kelly et al. 2010; Clos et al. 2013). However, they are limited by the fact that they implemented “hard” clustering algorithms, which assume that sharp borders separate intrinsically homogeneous neural regions (Eickhoff et al. 2015). This means they may fail to identify graded transitions that (i) could give rise to functionally intermediate areas (Bailey and Von Bonin 1951; Rosa and Tweedale 2005) and (ii) have been observed in the connectivity patterns of other brain regions (e.g. Cerliani et al. 2012; Bajada et al. 2017; Jackson et al. 2018, 2020; Tian and Zalesky 2018), as well as within the cytoarchitecture of the transmodal cortex (Brodmann 1909; Bailey and Von Bonin 1951). Therefore, the possibility of graded functional differences in the LIFG remains unexplored.

In systems neuroscience, there is growing interest in the potential to advance understanding of brain structural and functional organization using an emergent analytical approach that is designed to capture cortical gradients (Huntenburg et al. 2018; Bajada et al. 2020; Bernhardt et al. 2022). Cortical gradients can be defined as continuous spatial transitions in neural features, and they can capture overlapping organizing axes of the cerebral cortex. As such, gradients are well suited for exploring the organization of brain regions that exhibit connection topographies that imply both functional heterogeneity and functional multiplicity (i.e. overlapping functional modes; Haak and Beckmann 2020). Practically speaking, gradient analysis is a dimensionality reduction technique that identifies the spatial ordering of regions/voxels in a lower-dimensional yet continuous space based on similarity of features (for more detailed descriptions, see Bajada et al. 2020; Haak et al. 2018; Vos de Wael et al. 2020). They produce “gradient maps” that describe how much brain voxels/areas resemble each other in terms of cytoarchitecture, or structural/functional connections, for example, and provide a spatial framework for interpreting and linking functions across different task domains.

In seminal work, Margulies et al. (2016) used gradient analysis to examine variation in brain-wide (task-free) FC and revealed the presence of a “principal functional gradient”. At one end of the gradient lie unimodal systems that underpin sensory and motor function and, at the other end, lie hetero- and supra-modal association cortices. This macroscale property of cortical organization is thought to have an important functional role, which is untethering of the heteromodal cortex from sensory constraints (Mesulam 1998; Buckner and Krienen 2013) and driving cross-modal integration, which ultimately gives rise to more abstract cognitive functions (Huntenburg et al. 2018; Smallwood et al. 2021). At the level of this principal macroscale gradient, the LIFG resembles heteromodal brain regions. Moreover, the FC patterning within the LIFG appears largely homogeneous. However, gradient analysis has the potential to reveal more fine-grained functional distinctions when applied to restricted volumes of interest, as opposed to the whole brain (Haak and Beckmann 2020). Yet, to date, this potential remains unexplored with respect to the LIFG.

As mentioned above, the few previous attempts to parcellate the LIFG using FC have utilized “hard” clustering algorithms, and gradient analyses have a number of advantages over these more standard brain parcellation techniques. First, a key feature is that they do not presuppose the nature of variation and, therefore, can be used to demonstrate both graded changes and discrete boundaries (Johansen-Berg et al. 2004; Bajada et al. 2017; Jackson et al. 2018). Moreover, gradient analyses can be used as a data-driven means to probe the degree of gradation in cortical organization (Bajada et al. 2020). Second, gradient analyses can identify and distinguish between superimposed but orthogonal dimensions of functional variation. In the context of other parcellation techniques, these overlapping functional modes could appear as a singular component of organization (Haak et al. 2018). This is important because brain regions can exhibit multiple coexisting trajectories of functional change (for an extensive discussion, see Haak and Beckmann 2020). For example, the primary visual cortex represents distance from the center of the retina along the calcarine sulcus, whereas the angle is represented perpendicular to this (Wandell et al. 2005, 2007). The existence of multiple superimposed modes of functional change in the LIFG might explain why previous functional neuroimaging studies have identified functional differences along different spatial dimensions. Finally, a presence of overlapping and graded connection topographies would have implications for theories of LIFG function in terms of the computational complexity afforded to it (Jbabdi et al. 2013). For example, Haak and Beckmann (2020) suggest that this is an efficient way to wire a system and that it allows the implementation of transformations between and across different representational systems.

To summarize, the objectives of the current study were as follows. We sought to reconcile various functional dissociations that have been identified within the LIFG by taking a data-driven approach to exploring variation in FC that spans a broad set of cognitive states/tasks. We applied a gradient analysis to LIFG FC for the first time so that it would be possible to (i) detect multiple superimposed spatial dimensions of organization and (ii) identify both discrete changes in function and graded changes that could give rise to intermediate areas and functional multiplicity. Thereby, we aimed to elucidate an overarching spatial framework that explains the LIFG’s role in multiple cognitive domains.

Materials and methods

We used a data-driven approach to explore LIFG gradients based on 2 measures of FC: (i) correlations in task-free fMRI time-series and (ii) meta-analytically derived patterns of task-driven coactivation from across multiple cognitive domains. This bimodal approach not only allowed us to validate our results using independent datasets but also made it possible to assess the generalizability of the functional organization of the LIFG across different mental states. Indeed, one data type captures activation patterns associated with spontaneous thought (e.g. a state of mind-wandering; Chou et al. 2017; Doucet et al. 2012), while the other is assumed to reflect mental processes constrained by extrinsic demands (Laird et al. 2013). The summary of our analytical approach is as follows. For each voxel, we (i) extracted BOLD fluctuations over time from resting-state fMRI scans and (ii) meta-analytically identified the brain voxels with which it consistently coactivates across a broad range of task demands. Then, for each FC modality, we compared the fMRI time-series/coactivation patterns of each pair of voxels within the LIFG region of interest (ROI; see sections Task-Free FC Similarity Matrix and Task-Based Coactivation Similarity Matrix). We conducted gradient analyses on the resulting similarity matrices to extract the principal axes of variation and to estimate the degree to which there are discrete or graded changes in function (see section Gradient Analysis). In a second step, we conducted descriptive analyses to understand which FC differences gave rise to these gradients (see section Functional Characterization). To this end, we performed seed-based resting-state FC and meta-analytic coactivation modeling (MACM) analyses on clusters extracted from the extreme ends of the identified gradients (henceforth “gradient extremes” clusters). Finally, we probed the functional/task terms (e.g. “cognitive control,” “language”) associated with these IFG subregions using functional decoding analyses. A schematic overview of the analytic pipeline is illustrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Schematic overview of the analytic pipeline. The output of each analysis step is highlighted in bold. In the first step, we estimated the whole-brain coactivation patterns of individual ROI voxels using meta-analytic coactivation modeling (first column) and extracted their resting-state BOLD time-series (second column). Then, we performed a pairwise comparison of each voxel’s coactivation patterns/time-series using the product-moment correlation coefficient. This resulted in 2 similarity matrices, which were subsequently used as input for gradient analyses (diffusion embedding algorithm) in order to identify the main axes of variation across the ROI. In the final step, we performed MACM, functional decoding and seed-based resting-state FC analyses on “gradient extremes” clusters extracted from the opposite ends of the gradient maps in order to identify the FC patterns between which FC varies in the LIFG. The code used for data analysis can be accessed at: osf.io/u2834/.

Definition of the LIFG region of interest

The LIFG ROI was created by combining the pars opercularis, pars triangularis, and pars orbitalis as delineated in the second release of the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL2) atlas (Rolls et al. 2015). In addition, we included the region termed lateral orbital gyrus in the AAL2 parcellation because it is considered to pertain to pars orbitalis (Keller et al. 2009). These regions correspond roughly to Brodmann areas 44, 45, and (part of) 47. We retained only the voxels with 50% or greater probability of being gray matter according to the ICBM-152 template (Fonov et al. 2011). To ensure that the ROI did not encompass regions within neighboring gyri that were of no interest to the present study, the ROI was manually cleaned by removing voxels that crossed gyral boundaries into the precentral gyrus and middle frontal gyrus in the MNI-152 T1 template included in FSL (version 6.0.1). The final ROI comprised 1,813 (2 × 2 mm) voxels and is depicted in Fig. 1 (Step 1) and available at: osf.io/u2834/.

Data

Resting-state fMRI data

To assess the functional organization of the LIFG based on task-free FC, we used the resting-state fMRI time-series of 150 randomly selected healthy young adult participants (77 females) from the Human Connectome Project S1200 release (Van Essen et al. 2013). For each participant, data were available from up to 4 15-min runs of resting-state fMRI scans collected using the acquisition protocol described by Smith et al. (2013). All 4 scans were available for 139 participants (92.7% of participant sample), only 3 scans for 3 participants (2%), and only 2 scans for 8 participants (5.3%). The data were already preprocessed in the MNI space using the minimal processing pipeline described by Glasser et al. (2013) and de-noised using ICA-FIX (Salimi-Khorshidi et al. 2014). We regressed the global signal to further reduce the effects of motion artifacts (Burgess et al. 2016), and smoothed the images using a 4-mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian kernel. In keeping with other resting state studies, we took an additional step of band-pass filtering the data to retain only frequencies between 0.01 and 0.08 Hz (Satterthwaite et al. 2013).

Meta-analytic functional neuroimaging data

To assess the functional organization of the LIFG based on task coactivation patterns, we adopted a meta-analytic approach and capitalized on the openly available NeuroQuery database (neuroquery.org). NeuroQuery contains over 400,000 activation coordinates that were automatically extracted from 13,459 neuroimaging studies (Dockès et al. 2020). The database also includes estimates of frequency of occurrence of 6,308 terms (e.g. “cognitive control,” “semantic memory”) in each full-text publication from this corpus, which were used to perform functional decoding (see section Functional Characterization).

Data analysis

Task-free FC similarity matrix

To compute the task-free FC similarity matrix, we first extracted the blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal time-series of every voxel within the ROI, resulting in a voxel by timepoint matrix for each participant and each run. Then, we computed a cross-correlation matrix by calculating the product-moment correlation coefficient between the time-series of all pairs of ROI voxels. The resulting voxel by voxel matrix was z-score-normalized to allow the result of each run to be averaged (Dunlap et al. 2013), in order to generate an average similarity matrix across runs per participant. These participant-level matrices were subsequently averaged resulting in a group similarity matrix. This task-free FC-based similarity matrix was transformed back from z-scores to correlation values for gradient decomposition (see section Gradient Analysis).

Task-based coactivation similarity matrix

To compute the task-based coactivation similarity matrix, we first used MACM analyses to identify the brain areas consistently coactivated with each voxel within the ROI. MACM uses meta-analytic data to quantify the co-occurrence of activation between voxels across a broad range of task demands (Laird et al. 2013). This analysis involved extracting all studies in the NeuroQuery database that reported at least 1 activation peak within 6 mm of a given voxel. Next, we quantified the convergence of activation across the identified experiments using the revised activation likelihood estimation (ALE) algorithm (Eickhoff et al. 2012) as implemented in the Python library NiMARE (Salo et al. 2022). This process was repeated for all voxels within the ROI, resulting in 1,813 unthresholded MACM maps that estimate the strength of coactivation between each ROI voxel and all other brain voxels (ROI voxel by brain voxel matrix). In the second step, we generated a cross-correlation matrix by calculating the product-moment correlation coefficient between the MACM map values of each pair of ROI voxels. The resulting task-based coactivation similarity matrix was used as input for the gradient analysis (see section Gradient Analysis).

Gradient analysis

We conducted gradient analyses to separately explore the principal axes of variation in task-free FC and task-based coactivation patterns across the ROI. To this end, we first sparsified the similarity matrices by retaining only the top 10% of values row-wise and computed a symmetric affinity matrix using a cosine kernel. The application of this threshold ensures that the results are only based on strong connections, rather than weak and potentially spurious connections (Vos de Wael et al. 2020).

Then, we generated gradient maps by using the diffusion embedding algorithm as implemented in the BrainSpace Python toolbox (Vos de Wael et al. 2020). Diffusion embedding is a type of nonlinear dimensionality reduction based on graph theory that describes the high-dimensional connectivity data in terms of distances in a low-dimensional Euclidian space, where the distance between nodes (i.e. voxels) reflects the strength of their connections (i.e. similarity in FC patterns; for a detailed description, see Coifman and Lafon 2006). The diffusion embedding algorithm forces voxels with many and/or strong connections closer together and voxels with few and/or weak connections further apart in the embedding space (resulting in gradient maps). We extracted 10 gradients from each modality-specific matrix, but we further interrogate only the first 2 gradients as they explained considerably more variation in the data compared to the remaining gradients (see Fig. S1).

We quantified the degree of gradation in FC changes across the LIFG (including whether there are, instead, discrete changes and hard boundaries) by estimating the normalized algebraic connectivity of the similarity matrices. This value corresponds to the second largest eigenvalue of the Laplacian of the matrix and represents a descriptive index of how well connected a graph is (Fiedler 1973). It ranges from zero, which indicates that the graph comprises at least 2 completely disconnected subgraphs, to a value of 1, which suggests that the graph is characterized solely by graded differences. Thus, the normalized algebraic connectivity of the similarity matrices is indicative of whether the LIFG comprises at least 2 sharply delineated subregions or graded transitions between subregions with differences in connectivity/coactivation patterns (Bajada et al. 2020). We note that, while this value is influenced by the smoothing of neuroimaging data, a value much higher than 0 and close to the maximal value possible of 1 is unlikely to be caused only by artificially induced local gradation (Bajada et al. 2017, 2019, 2020; Jackson et al. 2020). We separately estimated the algebraic connectivity of the task-based coactivation matrix and the group task-free FC matrix. In addition, we assessed the gradation in task-free FC matrices at the participant level. This was done to avoid relying only on a gradation metric derived based on the group matrix, which is generated by subjecting the individual-level matrices to an additional transformation that may bias the gradation metric.

Functional characterization

While the gradient analysis can estimate the main directions of functional changes, this step alone cannot reveal the qualitative differences in the FC patterns that drive the functional organization of the LIFG. Therefore, to identify the FC differences underlying the gradient dimensions, we first defined “gradient extremes” clusters by extracting the voxels with the 20% lowest and highest gradient values (2 clusters per gradient). We selected these voxels because, in a graded map, voxels located at the gradient poles should differ most in terms of their FC patterns. In other words, probing the FC of these “gradient extremes” clusters can reveal the different connectivity patterns between which the gradients vary. Therefore, we used the “gradient extremes” clusters as seeds in whole-brain FC analyses to compare the task-free FC and task-constrained coactivation patterns that have driven the separation between the gradient extremes in the embedding space. The “gradient extremes” clusters are depicted in Fig. 1 (Step 4), and their MNI coordinates are reported in Table S1. To be clear, these clusters should not be interpreted as a hard parcellation of the LIFG but rather as a tool for gleaning a functional interpretation of the observed gradients. Indeed, similar approaches have been used in previous parcellation studies (Bajada et al. 2017; Jackson et al. 2018, 2020).

We performed seed-based resting-state FC analyses using the Python package Nilearn (Abraham et al. 2014). For each participant, we used the average resting-state fMRI time-series (concatenated across runs; Cho et al., 2021) of all voxels within each cluster as a regressor in a general linear model predicting the time-series of all gray matter voxels. The resulting cluster FC maps were z-transformed and tested for consistency across participants using a one-sample t-test. In addition, to identify the FC specific to each cluster, which is driving the identification of the gradient, paired-samples t-tests were used to generate contrast maps showing the brain regions with greater FC to one “gradient extreme” cluster than the cluster extracted from the opposite end of the same gradient (anterior vs. posterior cluster, dorsal vs. ventral cluster). The group-level FC maps were thresholded using a family-wise error (FWE)-corrected voxel-height threshold of p < 0.05 and the probabilistic threshold-free cluster enhancement approach as implemented in the R package pTFCE (Spisák et al. 2019). We wanted to identify the brain regions that (i) displayed greater functional coupling with one LIFG cluster than the cluster at the opposite end of the same gradient and, at the same time, (ii) were significantly coupled with the respective LIFG cluster. Therefore, the contrast maps (determined using the paired-samples t-tests) were masked by the significant connectivity of each cluster (determined using the one-sample t-tests).

To identify the coactivation patterns of each LIFG cluster, we conducted MACM analyses on seeds defined based on each task-based gradient map using the Python package NiMARE (Salo et al. 2022). Specifically, we ran ALE analyses on all studies from the NeuroQuery database that reported at least 1 activation peak within the seed (see Table S2 for the number of studies identified for each cluster) to identify the brain regions consistently involved in the studies that activate the seed. Specifically, the resulting MACM maps quantify the convergence of activation across all studies that reported activation within the seed. These maps were thresholded using an FWE-corrected voxel-level threshold of p< 0.05. Then, we conducted contrast analyses to identify the brain regions that coactivate more consistently with one “gradient extreme” cluster than the cluster extracted from the opposite end of the same gradient (anterior vs. posterior cluster, dorsal vs. ventral cluster). The contrast maps were thresholded using an uncorrected p < 0.05 threshold. To understand which brain regions display (i) greater coactivation with the cluster located at one extreme of the gradient than the other extreme and (ii) significant coactivation with the cluster, we masked the contrast maps by the significant cluster-specific MACM map (determined using independent ALE analysis).

It is important to note that we conducted contrast analyses using the same FC modality (i.e. MACM of clusters extracted from the task-based gradients, seed-based resting-state FC analyses of clusters extracted from the task-free gradients) in order to visualize the differences that have driven the gradients and not to test whether there were significant FC differences between the clusters. The noninferential and descriptive nature of these follow-up analyses circumvents analytic circularity (Eickhoff et al. 2015). Nonetheless, we repeated these sets of analyses using an independent FC modality (i.e. MACM of clusters extracted from the task-free gradients, seed-based resting-state FC analyses of clusters extracted from the task-based gradients) to confirm whether the FC maps are consistent regardless of the approach adopted to define the clusters. These analyses revealed similar FC patterns and are only reported in Figs. S9 and S10.

In line with recent recommendations (Uddin et al. 2022), we determined the network affiliations of our novel findings by comparing them with a commonly used parcellation scheme. We used the 7-network parcellation proposed by Yeo et al. (2011) as the reference atlas. For each task-free FC and task-based coactivation map, we computed the percentage of voxels that overlap with each of the 7 reference networks. The LIFG has been consistently implicated in semantic processing (Jackson 2021), which is thought to be supported by a functional network that is dissociable from other canonical networks such as the core default network (DN; Humphreys et al. 2015; Jackson et al. 2016, 2019; Branzi et al. 2020; Jung and Lambon Ralph 2023). Therefore, we also computed the overlap between our results and a mask of the semantic network (SN) obtained by Jackson et al. (2016). The reference SN map represents the set of regions that were significantly functionally coupled with the left ventrolateral anterior temporal lobe (ATL), which has been attributed a crucial role in semantic cognition (Binney et al. 2010; Lambon Ralph et al. 2017). It is of note that we are not able to dissociate between the DN and SN in these analyses (as has been done by, for example, Humphreys et al. 2015; Jackson et al. 2019) because there is a considerable degree of spatial overlap between the DN mask from Yeo et al. (2011) and the SN obtained by Jackson et al. (2016).

Lastly, to identify functional terms associated with each cluster as an index of its potential function, we conducted functional decoding analyses using the BrainMap chi-square approach as implemented in NiMARE (Salo et al. 2022). For each term in the NeuroQuery database, the consistency analysis (also known as forward inference) computes the likelihood of activation reported within the seed given presence of the term in the article’s text, whereas the specificity analysis (also known as reverse inference) estimates the posterior probability of an article containing the term given activation reported inside the seed. The results of these analyses were thresholded at p< 0.05 using the Benjamini–Hochberg false discovery rate correction. To aid the interpretability of the results, we retained only the terms with at least 80% likelihood of being related to cognitive functions based on raters’ annotations (Peraza et al. 2023).

Results

Gradient maps

The first 2 task-free FC gradients were selected for further analysis because together they accounted for >50% of variance, while the lower-order gradients explained <11% of variance each (Fig. S1). The voxels’ gradient values, which reflect the similarity between their resting-state fMRI time-series, were visually coded and projected on the brain using a color spectrum from red to dark blue to reveal the pattern of change in task-free FC across the LIFG. As can be seen in Fig. 2A, the FC patterns of the LIFG are principally organized along an anterior–posterior axis that accounted for 30% of the variance. This gradient progressed from the anterior portion of the LIFG, bordering the inferior part of the inferior frontal sulcus (IFS), to the posterior region, bordering the precentral gyrus. The second gradient, which explained 25% of the variance, revealed changes in connectivity along the superior–inferior dimension. This gradient progressed from the superior part of the IFS and the precentral sulcus to the inferior portion of the IFG, bordering the lateral orbital sulcus.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

The first 2 gradient maps extracted from the A) task-free FC similarity matrix and B) task-based coactivation similarity matrix. Compared to regions represented with colors further apart on the color spectrum, regions represented using colors that are closer together show greater similarity in their A) correlation with each other over time during resting fMRI scans and B) their patterns of coactivation across tasks spanning a range of cognitive domains. The ± indicate different poles of these gradient dimensions, but the assignment to a specific end of a dimension is arbitrary. The gradient maps can be accessed and visualized at: https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:13053.

The algebraic connectivity of the group similarity matrix was 0.71, suggesting a high level of gradation in task-free FC changes across the LIFG. The group similarity matrix, reordered based on the voxels’ positions along the first and second gradients, is provided in Fig. S2 and illustrates the graded change in FC across voxels in the LIFG. The graded nature of transitions was confirmed by the distribution of the algebraic connectivity of individual-level similarity matrices (Fig. S4) that had a mean of 0.89 (SD = 0.02). Moreover, inspection of distributions of gradient values within single-subject data consistently reveals an absence of large discontinuities that, once again, implies there are graded transitions rather than discrete boundaries (Fig. S6). For further discussion regarding gradients extracted at the individual level, see Section S1. A key observation, however, is that gradation seen at the group level cannot be explained by an “averaging effect” and variability in the location or extent of FC changes at the individual level. Instead, the graded nature of transitions is consistently observed at the group level and the individual subject level.

The first 2 task-based coactivation gradients were selected for further analysis because, together, they accounted for > 60% of the variance, while the lower-order gradients individually explained < 11% of the variance (Fig. S1). The principal gradient accounted for 42% of the variance and progressed along a dorsal–ventral axis from the inferior frontal junction (IFJ) to the antero-ventral region bordering the lateral orbital sulcus and inferior portion of the IFS. The second gradient explained 21% of the variance and revealed changes in connectivity that followed the rostral–caudal axis in a radial pattern progressing from the inferior portion of the pars opercularis toward the IFS. The algebraic connectivity of the coactivation similarity matrix was 0.77, suggesting that the LIFG is characterized by gradual changes in consistent patterns of coactivation across cognitive domains (see Fig. S2 for the reordered matrices). Because the unit of the task-based analysis is the study rather than the participant, the gradation cannot be assessed at the participant level as in the case of the task-free analysis reported above.

The gradients extracted from the 2 independent datasets converge on 2 principal organizational axes of the LIFG: anterior–posterior and dorsal–ventral. Visual inspection of the gradient maps suggests that the first task-free gradient and the second task-based gradient capture a similar anterior–posterior axis of functional variation, which is supported by a strong positive correlation of 0.77 between voxels’ position ranks on the 2 gradients (see Fig. S3 for the scatterplot). Likewise, the second task-free gradient and the first task-state gradient capture a similar dorsal–ventral organizational dimension. This observation is supported by a strong positive correlation of 0.7 between voxels’ position ranks on the 2 gradients (Fig. S3). The orders in which these gradients appear are switched between the task-free and task-constrained FC data, and this is because of a difference in the relative amount of variance explained by each gradient. Because it is subtle relative to the similarities, this difference could be attributable to noise but it may also reflect meaningful differences in the connectivity revealed by task-free and task-constrained mental states (Eickhoff and Grefkes 2011).

Functional characterization

Differential task-free FC patterns

We contrasted the whole-brain resting-state connectivity patterns of the clusters located at the extremes of the anterior–posterior task-free gradient. This revealed differences in their functional coupling with a bilateral and distributed set of brain regions (Fig. 3A; Table S3). The anterior cluster showed stronger FC with frontal regions, including the right IFG (pars orbitalis), bilateral IFS, dorsal and orbital portion of the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), superior frontal gyrus (SFG), medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), with parietal regions in the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), angular gyrus (AG), and inferior parietal lobule (IPL), with temporal regions along the length of the middle and inferior temporal gyri (MTG/ITG), in the fusiform gyrus (FG), and left hippocampus. In contrast, the posterior cluster showed stronger FC with frontal regions in the right IFG (pars opercularis), the middle portion of the MFG, precentral gyrus, pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA), anterior and middle cingulate cortex (ACC), and insula, with posterior cortical regions in the supramarginal gyrus (SMG) and posterior superior temporal sulcus, and with basal ganglia.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Results of contrast analyses between A) the task-free connectivity patterns (derived using seed-based resting-state FC analyses) of the IFG clusters located at the extremes of the anterior–posterior and dorsal–ventral task-free gradients and B) task-constrained coactivation patterns (derived using MACM analyses) of the IFG clusters located at the extremes of the anterior–posterior and dorsal–ventral task-based gradients. These contrast maps were masked using cluster-independent maps. The spider plots in the right column show the percentage of voxels in each contrast map that overlap with each of the 7 canonical networks from Thomas Yeo et al. (2011), as well as the SN from Jackson et al. (2016; available at: github.com/JacksonBecky/templates). Note that percentage values are relative to the size of each contrast map; therefore, only the relative patterns of overlaps within each contrast map are of interest and direct comparisons between the network affiliations of different contrast maps are misleading. The contrast maps can be accessed at: https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:13053.

Comparison between the task-free FC of the dorsal and ventral clusters revealed stronger coupling between the dorsal LIFG and frontal regions in bilateral IFS and IFJ, left MFG, and pre-SMA, parietal cortex in bilateral IPS and left IPL, and temporal cortex in bilateral posterior ITG and left FG (Fig. 3A; Table S4). In contrast, the ventral LIFG showed increased connectivity to the frontal cortex in the right IFG (pars triangularis and pars orbitalis), bilateral SFG, mPFC, and ACC, to the precuneus, a swathe of temporal cortex progressing from the bilateral ventrolateral ATL through the MTG toward the AG, and to the left hippocampus.

Comparison between the cluster-specific task-free FC patterns and canonical networks indicate stronger functional coupling between the anterior LIFG and regions falling within the bounds of the SN/DN and the frontoparietal network (FPN), and between the posterior LIFG and brain regions associated with the ventral attention network (VAN) and somatomotor network (SMN). The dorsal LIFG showed stronger FC with regions of the FPN and dorsal attention network (DAN), whereas the ventral LIFG showed a preference for SN/DN regions. Additional conjunction analyses showed that both the anterior and posterior clusters are coupled with regions of the FPN and SN/DN and that both the dorsal and ventral clusters are functionally connected mainly with SN/DN regions (Fig. S8a; Tables S3 and S4).

Differential task-constrained coactivation patterns

The anterior LIFG showed increased consistent coactivation across a wide variety of tasks with frontal regions in the right IFS and precentral gyrus, and with bilateral IPS and left posterior ITG, whereas the posterior LIFG coactivated more with the right IFG, bilateral anterior insula, and left superior temporal gyrus (Fig. 3B; Table S5). The dorsal cluster coactivated more with frontal cortex in the right IFJ, bilateral precentral gyrus, dorsal anterior insula, and pre-SMA, and with the IPS, and left posterior FG (Fig. 3B; Table S6). In comparison, the ventral cluster showed increased coactivation with the right IFG (pars orbitalis), and left mPFC, MTG, and AG. Given the conservative threshold applied to the independent maps, we also looked at the whole contrast maps without masking by these independent maps. These additionally revealed more consistent coactivation of the posterior cluster with the bilateral STG and of the ventral cluster with the bilateral ATL, precuneus, and left AG (Fig. S7).

Comparison between the cluster-specific task-based coactivation patterns and canonical networks shows that the anterior LIFG cluster coactivates more consistently with brain regions that are part of the FPN and DAN, whereas the posterior LIFG cluster coactivates mainly with regions associated with the VAN and, when additional masking is not applied, the SMN. The dorsal cluster coactivates preferentially with regions of the FPN and DAN, whereas the ventral LIFG cluster shows stronger coactivation with the DN/SN. Additional conjunction analyses showed overlap between the coactivation maps of the anterior and posterior clusters and those of dorsal and ventral clusters primarily in regions of the FPN (Fig. S8b; Tables S5 and S6).

The FC analyses performed on clusters extracted from the gradient maps derived using the independent dataset (i.e. seed-based FC analyses of clusters derived using NeuroQuery studies and MACM analyses of clusters derived using task-free fMRI time-series), which were conducted to assess the robustness of the results across different strategies for defining seeds, revealed a similar pattern of results (Figs. S9 and S10).

Comparison between the task-free and task-based FC patterns

In sum, the task-free and task-based analyses implicate overlapping regions, although the clusters identified in the task-based analyses were less extensive. Specifically, the anterior LIFG was connected with executive control regions (e.g. IFJ, IPS; Fedorenko et al. 2013; Camilleri et al. 2018; Assem et al. 2020b), but, in the task-free maps, it was also connected to regions implicated in semantic cognition (e.g. ATL, AG; Binder and Desai 2011; Lambon Ralph et al. 2017). Further, the posterior LIFG was connected to areas that have been ascribed important roles in sensorimotor processing, as well as in phonological and articulatory linguistic processes (e.g. bilateral STS/STG, but in the task-free maps, it was also connected to motor and premotor cortices, SMA, MFG, and SMG; Vigneau et al. 2006; Hickok and Poeppel 2007; Hickok 2009; Hartwigsen et al. 2010; Ueno et al. 2011; Price 2012). This cluster was also connected to regions considered crucial for salience processing (e.g. anterior insula, but in the task-free results also to dorsal ACC; Menon and Uddin 2010; Uddin 2015). The dorsal LIFG was connected to regions that are implicated in executive function (e.g. IFJ, MFG, IPS; Fedorenko et al. 2013; Camilleri et al. 2018; Assem et al. 2020b), whereas the ventral LIFG was connected with a set of regions ascribed key roles in semantic and episodic memory (e.g. ATL, medial temporal lobe, AG; (Irish and Piguet 2013; Lambon Ralph et al. 2017).

Despite the similarities in the regions implicated, there were some differences in the network affiliations derived from the task-free and task-based analyses. However, comparing the network affiliations of the different contrast maps directly is not possible because (i) the overlap index depends on the size of the maps, which differs considerably between the task-free and task-based analyses, and (ii) there are differences between the seeds upon which the task-free and task-based analyses are based (see Fig. 1; e.g. the task-based anterior seed extends across the length of the IFS and overlaps with the dorsal LIFG seed, whereas the anterior seed used for the task-free analysis does not). Therefore, we will focus the interpretation on the similarities.

The dorsal LIFG connected to FPN and DAN regions, 2 networks that contribute to the task-general multiple demand network (MDN; Majerus et al. 2018; Assem et al. 2020b). In contrast, the ventral LIFG was affiliated mainly with the DN/SN. The DN and SN cannot be distinguished in our assessment given the high degree of spatial overlap between the masks used. However, we note that both the task-free and the unmasked task-based results suggest strong coupling with the ATL, a key hub of semantic knowledge (Binney et al. 2010; Lambon Ralph et al. 2010), as well as with the left hippocampus/parahippocampal gyrus, known to be important for episodic memory (Burgess et al. 2002; Dickerson and Eichenbaum 2010). As such, the dorsal–ventral organizational dimension seems to distinguish between domain-general control networks at the dorsal end and memory-related networks at the ventral end.

The posterior LIFG showed a preference for the VAN, suggestive of a role in perceptually driven cognition (Corbetta and Shulman 2002; Corbetta et al. 2008). The anterior LIFG showed a preference for regions that overlap with the FPN, consistent with a role in cognitive control (Assem et al. 2020a). The task-free data revealed additional strong coupling with regions that are part of the DN/SN. The task-based analyses might have led to less extensive association with the DN because this network is known for its tendency to deactivate in response to various task demands (Shulman et al. 1997; Mazoyer et al. 2001; Buckner et al. 2005), but it tends to activate during mind-wandering states that frequently occur during resting-state scans (Smallwood et al. 2021). Nonetheless, there is evidence that the DN works with the FPN in support of some types of goal-directed cognition (Spreng et al. 2010, 2013, 2014; Wang et al. 2021) and that it contributes to cognitive control (Crittenden et al. 2015). As such, the anterior–posterior organizational dimension seems to distinguish between higher-order transmodal networks at the anterior edge and perceptually driven networks at the posterior edge.

Functional decoding

The functional decoding results suggest possible functional associations of the different LIFG clusters. We identified a set of common terms that suggest functional associations shared by all clusters. All 4 LIFG clusters were significantly associated with terms related to semantic and linguistic processing, including lexical, linguistic, language, language comprehension, sentence comprehension, reading, orthographic, phonological, lexical decision, and retrieval semantic information/memory/knowledge. In addition, we identified associations with distinct terms that hint at the functional dissociations between the gradient edges. Compared to the posterior cluster, the anterior cluster was additionally associated with the terms executive (function) and memory retrieval, in line with a role in higher-order cognitive control processes . In contrast, the posterior cluster was associated with terms related to lower-order perceptual and motor processing, such as movement, recognition, and auditory, as well as speech-related terms, such as phonetic and vocal. Compared to the ventral cluster, the dorsal cluster was associated with terms related to a wide range of cognitive/behavioral domains and input modalities, including visual, auditory, visuospatial, working memory, executive, social, reward, and mood. In contrast, the ventral cluster was associated with terms such as memories, mentalizing, reappraisal, and autobiographical, which are suggestive of the purported internally oriented functions of the DN (Andrews-Hanna et al. 2014; Smallwood et al. 2021). While functional decoding approaches can provide pointers to the potential functional associations of these regions, it is important to note that the specificity of the results is limited by the drawbacks of automated data mining tools like NeuroQuery. These include the aggregation of all contrasts reported in an article, regardless of the cognitive aspects they isolate (Dockès et al. 2020). Therefore, interpretation should focus on the overall patterns that emerge in the decoding analysis, rather than the associations of individual terms. Detailed lists of the functional associations are presented separately for forward and reverse inference analyses and task-free and task-based clusters in Figs. S11 and S12.

Figure 4 summarizes the functional decoding results that were consistent for the clusters extracted from the task-free and task-based gradients (e.g. terms associated with both the anterior edge of the task-free gradient and the anterior extreme of the task-based gradient). It also includes a schematic of the proposed functional organization, which takes into account the results of the FC contrast analyses, the network affiliations, and the functional decoding, as well as previous literature reviewed in detail in the Discussion.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Schematic representation of the proposed functional organization of the LIFG. The word clouds illustrate functional terms associated with parts of the LIFG located at the extremes of the gradients (derived from task-free and task-based FC) on the basis of forward or reverse inference functional decoding analyses. Terms associated with the anterior but not posterior aspect are shown in blue, those associated with the posterior but not anterior aspect in green, the dorsal but not ventral aspect in red, and with the ventral but not dorsal aspect in orange. The size of the word reflects the effect size of the association. The anterior–posterior organizational axis is represented by the horizontal blue-to-green arrow, and we interpret the sets of associations to reflect a shift from lower-order perceptual processing (posterior LIFG) via affiliation with the SMN and VAN, to higher-order cognitive control (anterior LIFG) via affiliation with the FPN and DN. The dorsal–ventral axis is represented by the vertical orange-to-red arrow, and we interpret the sets of associations to reflect a shift from domain-general executive functions (dorsal LIFG) via affiliation with the FPN and DAN to cognitive control of information stored in long-term memory (ventral LIFG) via affiliation with the DN/SN.

Discussion

The present study made the first attempt to use data-driven gradient analyses of FC data to elucidate the functional organization of the LIFG. We specifically aimed to (i) map the principal axes of change in function and (ii) determine whether these shifts are graded or discrete. In the following 2 sections, we shall summarize our novel findings and then discuss their functional significance.

Graded topographical organization of the LIFG along 2 principal axes

Our analyses converged upon 2 key findings. First, the FC across the LIFG is principally organized along 2 orthogonal axes. One of these axes is oriented in an anterior to posterior direction and driven by stronger coupling with the FPN and DN in the rostral aspect and with the VAN and SMN at the caudal end. The second arose along a ventral to dorsal orientation and reflected greater connectivity of ventral LIFG to the DN, whereas dorsal regions abutting the IFS/IFJ were more tightly coupled with the FPN and DAN. These differential patterns of FC are in line with previous investigations (Kelly et al. 2010; Clos et al. 2013; Neubert et al. 2014; Barredo et al. 2016; Davey et al. 2016; Jakobsen et al. 2016, 2018; Muhle-Karbe et al. 2016; Wang et al. 2020) and suggest that the LIFG interfaces between distinct large-scale functional networks, consistent with its proposed role as a cortical hub (Buckner et al. 2009; Sepulcre et al. 2012).

Our second key finding is that FC of the LIFG shifts in a graded manner. Put another way, the algebraic connectivity of the similarity matrices revealed that FC is not consistent with abrupt boundaries and discrete functional parcels in the LIFG. Our findings are the first direct demonstration of this, but they are consistent with both contemporary descriptions of LIFG connectivity based on intraoperative cortico-cortical evoked potentials (Nakae et al. 2020) and structural properties (Thiebaut de Schotten et al. 2017), as well as classical descriptions that include a fan-shaped set of anatomical projections emanating from the IFG into the lateral temporal lobe (Dejerine and Dejerine-Klumpke 1895).

Overall, our findings are compatible with previous parcellations despite key differences in the methodological approaches (Klein et al. 2007; Kelly et al. 2010; Clos et al. 2013; Neubert et al. 2014; Wang et al. 2020; Blazquez Freches et al. 2021; Abdallah et al. 2022). This includes those that have taken a “hard” clustering approach to parcellating the left prefrontal cortex (PFC). For example, Neubert et al. (2014) parcellated the PFC based on structural connectivity and found that the LIFG fractionated into discrete subdivisions positioned along the anterior–posterior dimension. The connectivity of these parcels was distinct from those situated dorsally in the adjacent IFS and IFJ, which implies a further dorsal–ventral dimension of organization. The coexistence of these 2 axes of LIFG organization is also apparent in hard parcellations of the LIFG (Clos et al. 2013; Wang et al. 2020), its right hemisphere homolog (Hartwigsen et al. 2019), and more encompassing parcellations of cortex (Glasser et al. 2016). Of course, the results of graded and hard parcellation are not identical because hard parcellations (i) force voxels that are part of intermediate regions with gradually changing connectivity to be within the borders of discrete clusters (Haueis 2012; Bajada et al. 2017) and (ii) require the a priori specification of the number of clusters that are to be identified, perhaps making them insensitive to finer details. However, the 2 superimposed yet orthogonal modes of organization identified here (cf. Blazquez Freches et al. 2021; Abdallah et al. 2022) could have driven the results of prior hard parcellations of the LIFG.

The putative functional significance of the LIFG’s functional connectivity gradients

Taken together, the cluster-specific FC patterns and functional decoding results paint a coherent picture regarding the functional significance of the graded connectivity patterns that appear across the LIFG. On this basis, and in conjunction with the results of previous functional neuroimaging studies, we propose the following interpretation, which is illustrated schematically in Fig. 4. First, the dorsal–ventral axis might reflect a functional transition from domain-general executive function (dorsal LIFG) to domain-specific control of meaning-related representations (ventral LIFG). Second, the anterior–posterior axis might reflect a shift from perceptually driven processes (posterior LIFG) to higher-level transmodal control (anterior LIFG). We discuss this proposal in further detail below.

The dorsal LIFG was functionally coupled with regions that comprise the FPN and DAN. These 2 networks contribute to a wide variety of task demands that span multiple cognitive domains (Cole et al. 2013; Assem et al. 2020b). In contrast, the ventral LIFG was preferentially affiliated with the DN, including brain regions that have been ascribed key roles in episodic memory, like the medial temporal lobes (Dickerson and Eichenbaum 2010; Eichenbaum et al. 2012; Ekstrom and Ranganath 2018; Sugar and Moser 2019), and semantic cognition, like the ATLs (Binney et al. 2010; Binder and Desai 2011; Lambon Ralph et al. 2017). The ATLs also make important contributions to episodic/autobiographical memory (Svoboda et al. 2006; Irish et al. 2012; Irish and Vatansever 2020), perhaps providing the semantic scaffold necessary for shaping and constraining the encoding and recollection of past experiences (Irish and Piguet 2013; Renoult et al. 2019). Thus, the shift in FC toward ventral IFG subregions might reflect a specialization toward the application of cognitive control to prior knowledge. Indeed, it has been proposed that the LIFG, as a whole, sits in a unique position at the intersection of the MDN and the DN and that this makes it ideally suited for implementing demanding operations on meaning-related representations from memory (Davey et al. 2016; Chiou et al. 2023). Consistent with this, the LIFG responds reliably to an increased need for the controlled access to stored semantic information across a wide range of experimental paradigms (Diveica et al. 2021; Jackson 2021), extending to those requiring the retrieval of episodic memories (Vatansever et al. 2021). However, it is increasingly apparent that there are finer-grained functional distinctions within the LIFG; dorsal LIFG regions near IFS/IFJ overlap with the MDN and are engaged by control demands that are common across many cognitive tasks/domains (Fedorenko et al. 2013; Assem et al. 2020b; Hodgson et al. 2021), which may include phonology (Poldrack et al. 1999; Hodgson et al. 2021), whereas the ventral LIFG contributes selectively to challenging semantic tasks (Whitney et al. 2011, 2012; Gao et al. 2021). One possible explanation is that ventral LIFG is specifically involved in controlled semantic retrieval processes as opposed to domain-general selection mechanisms, which are under the purview of dorsal LIFG regions (Badre and Wagner 2007; Barredo et al. 2015; but see Thompson-Schill et al. 1997; Crescentini et al. 2010; Snyder et al. 2011). Alternatively, the cognitive mechanisms implemented might be equivalent, but connectivity differences mean that they operate on distinct sets of inputs/outputs (Thompson-Schill et al. 2005).

The anterior and posterior LIFG clusters were each affiliated with networks that occupy different positions along a macroscale cortical hierarchy that transitions from the sensorimotor to transmodal cortex (Margulies et al. 2016). Specifically, the posterior LIFG was coupled with the VAN and SMN, which are positioned at the lower edge of the principal cortical gradient and process inputs from the external environment (Corbetta et al. 2008; Menon and Uddin 2010). In contrast, anterior LIFG was preferentially coupled with regions of the FPN and DN, which are positioned toward the top end of the cortical hierarchy (Margulies et al. 2016). The anatomical and functional separation of anterior LIFG regions from sensorimotor systems might be requisite for the implementation of perceptually decoupled, temporally extended, and higher-order cognitive control (Fuster 2001; Kiebel et al. 2008; Taylor et al. 2015; Raut et al. 2020). This interpretation is consistent with the proposal that the PFC is characterized by a posterior–anterior gradient of hierarchical control (for a review, see Badre 2008; Badre and Desrochers 2019), which was motivated by studies showing that the anterior PFC is preferentially engaged by tasks that require generalization over an extended set of rules, integration of a larger number of dimensions, and/or contexts sustained over longer periods of time (for an investigation focused on the LIFG, see Koechlin and Jubault 2006; also Koechlin et al. 2003; Badre and D’Esposito 2007; Bahlmann et al. 2015; Nee and D’Esposito 2016). In the language domain, it has been suggested that the LIFG has a key role in the integration of subordinate lexical elements into superordinate representational structures (e.g. from phonemes to words to sentences to conceptual gestalts) and that this reflects a caudal–rostral functional gradient from phonological to syntactic to conceptual processing (Hagoort 2005; Uddén and Bahlmann 2012; also see Asano et al. 2021). Our results can account for this functional distinction, as preferential coupling with perceptual-motor networks can explain the reliance of phonology on posterior LIFG, whereas increased FC with the SN explains the involvement of antero-ventral LIFG in semantic processing. The stronger FC with the posterior superior temporal cortex might make the more posterior aspect of the LIFG also suited for implementing syntactic binding computations (Zaccarella et al. 2017; Zaccarella and Friederici 2017), particularly in the case of highly automated linguistic tasks that require lower levels of cognitive control (Jeon and Friederici 2013, 2015).

Concluding remarks

Our analyses revealed 2 main axes of organization in LIFG function, in anterior–posterior and dorsal–ventral orientations, which is consistent with broader proposals concerning the whole PFC (Petrides 2005; Abdallah et al. 2022). Moreover, our results suggest that functional differentiation across the LIFG occurs in a graded manner, and we were not able to find any clear evidence for discrete functional modules. Crucially, we replicated the principal gradients using 2 independent measures of FC, which suggests that our results are not dependent on idiosyncrasies of the datasets and instead reflect stable, generalizable properties of LIFG organization. The high degree of cross-modal similarity also suggests that a comparable LIFG functional organization underpins divergent mental states. Therefore, our work provides a spatial framework that can help us understand the contribution of the LIFG to multiple cognitive domains. Future work is needed to directly probe the functional significance of these organizational dimensions and assess the compatibility of our findings at different spatial scales (e.g. cellular) and within other neuroimaging modalities (e.g. tractography) such that it is possible to arrive at an integrated account of the functional organization of the LIFG.

Supplementary Material

Diveicaetal_LIFGgradients_Supplementary_Information_Final_bhad373

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge the support of the Supercomputing Wales project, which is part-funded by the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) via the Welsh Government, and we are grateful to Aaron Owen for his help with using the associated resources. We thank Julio A. Peraza for compiling and providing access to annotations of Neurosynth terms. Data were provided [in part] by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research; and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University.

Contributor Information

Veronica Diveica, Department of Psychology & Cognitive Neuroscience Institute, Bangor University, Bangor, Wales LL57 2AS, United Kingdom; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery & Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, QC H3A 2B4, Canada.

Michael C Riedel, Department of Physics, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199, United States.

Taylor Salo, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, 19104, United States.

Angela R Laird, Department of Physics, Florida International University, Miami, FL 33199, United States.

Rebecca L Jackson, Department of Psychology & York Biomedical Research Institute, University of York, York, YO10 5DD, United Kingdom.

Richard J Binney, Department of Psychology & Cognitive Neuroscience Institute, Bangor University, Bangor, Wales LL57 2AS, United Kingdom.

Author contributions

Veronica Diveica (Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Visualization, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing), Michael Reidel (Formal analysis, Methodology, Software), Taylor Salo (Methodology, Software), Angela Laird (Methodology, Resources, Writing—review & editing), Rebecca L. Jackson (Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing—review & editing), and Richard J Binney (Conceptualization, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing—review & editing)

Funding

This work was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) Wales Doctoral Training Partnership in the form of a PhD studentship [ES/P00069X/1] awarded to VD and RJB (PhD student: VD). Support for ARL, MCR, and RS was provided by the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH09606) and the National Institute of Drug Abuse (R01-DA041353).

Conflict of interest statement: None declared.

Open science practices and data/code availability statement

We used open data available via the Human Connectome Project (https://www.humanconnectome.org/) and the NeuroQuery database (https://neuroquery.org/). The code used for data preprocessing and analysis relies on open source software (e.g. Abraham et al. 2014; Salo et al. 2022) and can be accessed at: https://github.com/DiveicaV/LIFG_Gradients and https://osf.io/u2834/. The gradient and functional connectivity maps can also be accessed and visualized via NeuroVault at: https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:13053. Due to its exploratory nature, the study was not preregistered.

References

  1. Abdallah  M, Zanitti  GE, Iovene  V, Wassermann  D. Functional gradients in the human lateral prefrontal cortex revealed by a comprehensive coordinate-based meta-analysis Badre D, Frank MJ, editors. elife. 2022:11:e76926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Abraham  A, Pedregosa  F, Eickenberg  M, Gervais  P, Mueller  A, Kossaifi  J, Gramfort  A, Thirion  B, Varoquaux  G. Machine learning for neuroimaging with scikit-learn. Front Neuroinform. 2014:8:14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Amunts  K, Schleicher  A, Bürgel  U, Mohlberg  H, Uylings  HBM, Zilles  K, Vogt  O. Broca’s region revisited: Cytoarchitecture and Intersubject variability. J Comp Neurol. 1999:412:319–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Amunts  K, Lenzen  M, Friederici  AD, Schleicher  A, Morosan  P, Palomero-Gallagher  N, Zilles  K. Broca’s region: novel organizational principles and multiple receptor mapping. PLoS Biol. 2010:8(9):e1000489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andrews-Hanna  JR, Smallwood  J, Spreng  RN. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014:1316(1):29–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anwander  A, Tittgemeyer  M, Von Cramon  DY, Friederici  AD, Knösche  TR. Connectivity-based Parcellation of Broca’s area. Cereb Cortex. 2007:17(4):816–825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Asano  R, Boeckx  C, Seifert  U. Hierarchical control as a shared neurocognitive mechanism for language and music. Cognition. 2021:216:104847. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Assem  M, Blank  IA, Mineroff  Z, Ademoğlu  A, Fedorenko  E. Activity in the fronto-parietal multiple-demand network is robustly associated with individual differences in working memory and fluid intelligence. Cortex. 2020a:131:1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Assem  M, Glasser  MF, Van Essen  DC, Duncan  J. A domain-general cognitive Core defined in multimodally Parcellated human cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2020b:30(8):4361–4380. 10.1093/cercor/bhaa023. [accessed 2020 May 7]. https://academic.oup.com/cercor/advance-article/doi/10.1093/cercor/bhaa023/5815289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Badre  D. Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro–caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008:12(5):193–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Badre  D, D’Esposito  M. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical Organization of the Prefrontal Cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007:19(12):2082–2099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Badre  D, Desrochers  TM. Hierarchical cognitive control and the frontal lobes. Handbook of clinical neurology. 2019:163:165–177. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128042816000094 [accessed 2022 Sep 12]. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Badre  D, Wagner  AD. Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia. 2007:45(13):2883–2901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Badre  D, Poldrack  RA, Paré-Blagoev  EJ, Insler  RZ, Wagner  AD. Dissociable controlled retrieval and generalized selection mechanisms in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2005:47(6):907–918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bahlmann  J, Blumenfeld  RS, D’Esposito  M. The rostro-caudal Axis of frontal cortex is sensitive to the domain of stimulus information. Cereb Cortex. 2015:25(7):1815–1826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bailey  P, Von Bonin  G. The isocortex of man. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, 1951. [Google Scholar]
  17. Bajada  CJ, Jackson  RL, Haroon  HA, Azadbakht  H, Parker  GJM, Lambon Ralph  MA, Cloutman  LL. A graded tractographic parcellation of the temporal lobe. NeuroImage. 2017:155:503–512. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bajada  CJ, Trujillo-Barreto  NJ, Parker  GJM, Cloutman  LL, Lambon Ralph  MA. A structural connectivity convergence zone in the ventral and anterior temporal lobes: data-driven evidence from structural imaging. Cortex. 2019:120:298–307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Bajada  CJ, Costa Campos  LQ, Caspers  S, Muscat  R, Parker  GJM, Lambon Ralph  MA, Cloutman  LL, Trujillo-Barreto  NJ. A tutorial and tool for exploring feature similarity gradients with MRI data. NeuroImage. 2020:221:117140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Barredo  J, Öztekin  I, Badre  D. Ventral Fronto-temporal pathway supporting cognitive control of episodic memory retrieval. Cereb Cortex. 2015:25(4):1004–1019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Barredo  J, Verstynen  TD, Badre  D. Organization of cortico-cortical pathways supporting memory retrieval across subregions of the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2016:116(3):920–937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Bernhardt  BC, Smallwood  J, Keilholz  S, Margulies  DS. Gradients in brain organization. NeuroImage. 2022:251:118987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Binder  JR, Desai  RH. The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011:15(11):527–536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Binney  RJ, Embleton  KV, Jefferies  E, Parker  GJM, Lambon Ralph  MA. The ventral and inferolateral aspects of the anterior temporal lobe are crucial in semantic memory: evidence from a novel direct comparison of distortion-corrected fMRI, rTMS, and semantic dementia. Cereb Cortex. 2010:20(11):2728–2738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Blazquez Freches  G, Haak  KV, Beckmann  CF, Mars  RB. Connectivity gradients on tractography data: pipeline and example applications. Hum Brain Mapp. 2021:42(18):5827–5845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Branzi  FM, Humphreys  GF, Hoffman  P, Lambon Ralph  MA. Revealing the neural networks that extract conceptual gestalts from continuously evolving or changing semantic contexts. NeuroImage. 2020:220:116802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Brodmann  K. Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien dargestellt auf Grund des Zellenbaues. Leipzig: Barth; 1909 [Google Scholar]
  28. Buckner  RL, Krienen  FM. The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013:17(12):648–665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Buckner  RL, Snyder  AZ, Shannon  BJ, LaRossa  G, Sachs  R, Fotenos  AF, Sheline  YI, Klunk  WE, Mathis  CA, Morris  JC, et al.  Molecular, structural, and functional characterization of Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for a relationship between default activity, amyloid, and memory. J Neurosci. 2005:25(34):7709–7717. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Buckner  RL, Sepulcre  J, Talukdar  T, Krienen  FM, Liu  H, Hedden  T, Andrews-Hanna  JR, Sperling  RA, Johnson  KA. Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci. 2009:29(6):1860–1873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Burgess  N, Maguire  EA, O’Keefe  J. The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron. 2002:35(4):625–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Burgess  GC, Kandala  S, Nolan  D, Laumann TO, Power  JD, Adeyemo  B, Harms  MP, Petersen  SE, Barch  DM. Evaluation of denoising strategies to address motion-correlated Artifacts in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data from the human connectome project. Brain Connectivity. 2016:6(9):669–68. 10.1089/brain.2016.0435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Camilleri  JA, Müller  VI, Fox  P, Laird  AR, Hoffstaedter  F, Kalenscher  T, Eickhoff  SB. Definition and characterization of an extended multiple-demand network. NeuroImage. 2018:165:138–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Cerliani  L, Thomas  RM, Jbabdi  S, Siero  JCW, Nanetti  L, Crippa  A, Gazzola  V, D’Arceuil  H, Keysers  C. Probabilistic tractography recovers a rostrocaudal trajectory of connectivity variability in the human insular cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012:33(9):2005–2034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Chiou  R, Jefferies  E, Duncan  J, Humphreys  GF, Lambon Ralph  MA. A middle ground where executive control meets semantics: the neural substrates of semantic control are topographically sandwiched between the multiple-demand and default-mode systems. Cereb Cortex. 2023:33(8):4512–4526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Cho  JW, Korchmaros  A, Vogelstein  JT, Milham  MP, Xu  T. Impact of concatenating fMRI data on reliability for functional connectomics. NeuroImage. 2021:226:117549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Choi  EY, Drayna  GK, Badre  D. Evidence for a functional hierarchy of association networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2018:30(5):722–736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Chou  YH, Sundman  M, Whitson  HE, Gaur  P, Chu  ML, Weingarten  CP, Madden  DJ, Wang  L, Kirste  I, Joliot  M, et al.  Maintenance and representation of mind wandering during resting-state fMRI. Sci Rep. 2017:7(1):1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Clos  M, Amunts  K, Laird  AR, Fox  PT, Eickhoff  SB. Tackling the multifunctional nature of Broca’s region meta-analytically: co-activation-based parcellation of area 44. NeuroImage. 2013:83:174–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Cloutman  LL, Lambon Ralph  MA. Connectivity-based structural and functional parcellation of the human cortex using diffusion imaging and tractography. Front Neuroanat. 2012:6(34). 10.3389/fnana.2012.00034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Coifman  RR, Lafon  S. Diffusion maps. Appl Comput Harmon Anal. 2006:21(1):5–30. [Google Scholar]
  42. Cole  MW, Reynolds  JR, Power  JD, Repovs  G, Anticevic  A, Braver  TS. Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for adaptive task control. Nat Neurosci. 2013:16(9):1348–1355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Corbetta  M, Shulman  GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002:3(3):201–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Corbetta  M, Patel  G, Shulman  GL. The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron. 2008:58(3):306–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Crescentini  C, Shallice  T, Macaluso  E. Item retrieval and competition in noun and verb generation: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010:22(6):1140–1157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Crittenden  BM, Mitchell  DJ, Duncan  J. Recruitment of the default mode network during a demanding act of executive control. elife. 2015:4:e06481. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Damoiseaux  JS, Greicius  MD. Greater than the sum of its parts: a review of studies combining structural connectivity and resting-state functional connectivity. Brain Struct Funct. 2009:213(6):525–533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Davey  J, Thompson  HE, Hallam  G, Karapanagiotidis  T, Murphy  C, De Caso  I, Krieger-Redwood  K, Bernhardt  BC, Smallwood  J, Jefferies  E. Exploring the role of the posterior middle temporal gyrus in semantic cognition: integration of anterior temporal lobe with executive processes. NeuroImage. 2016:137:165–177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Dejerine  J, Dejerine-Klumpke  A. Anatomie des centres nerveux. Paris: Rueff; 1895 [Google Scholar]
  50. Devlin  JT, Matthews  PM, Rushworth  MFS. Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003:15(1):71–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Dickerson  BC, Eichenbaum  H. The episodic memory system: Neurocircuitry and disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010:35(1):86–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Diveica  V, Koldewyn  K, Binney  RJ. Establishing a role of the semantic control network in social cognitive processing: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage. 2021:245:118702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Dockès  J, Poldrack  RA, Primet  R, Gözükan  H, Yarkoni  T, Suchanek  F, Thirion  B, Varoquaux  G. Neuroquery, comprehensive meta-analysis of human brain mapping. elife. 2020:9:e53385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Doucet  G, Naveau  M, Petit  L, Zago  L, Crivello  F, Jobard  G, Delcroix  N, Mellet  E, Tzourio-Mazoyer  N, Mazoyer  B, et al.  Patterns of hemodynamic low-frequency oscillations in the brain are modulated by the nature of free thought during rest. NeuroImage. 2012:59(4):3194–3200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Dunlap  WP, Jones  MB, Bittner  AC. Average correlations vs. correlated averages. Bull Psychon Soc. 2013:21(3):213–216. [Google Scholar]
  56. Eichenbaum  H, Sauvage  M, Fortin  N, Komorowski  R, Lipton  P. Towards a functional organization of episodic memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012:36(7):1597–1608. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Eickhoff  SB, Grefkes  C. Approaches for the integrated analysis of structure, function and connectivity of the human brain. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2011:42(2):107–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Eickhoff  SB, Bzdok  D, Laird  AR, Kurth  F, Fox  PT. Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. NeuroImage. 2012:59(3):2349–2361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Eickhoff  SB, Thirion  B, Varoquaux  G, Bzdok  D. Connectivity-based parcellation: critique and implications. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015:36(12):4771–4792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ekstrom  AD, Ranganath  C. Space, time, and episodic memory: the hippocampus is all over the cognitive map. Hippocampus. 2018:28(9):680–687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Fedorenko  E, Blank  IA. Broca’s area is not a natural kind. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020:24(4):270–284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Fedorenko  E, Duncan  J, Kanwisher  N. Language-selective and domain-general regions lie side by side within Broca’s area. Curr Biol. 2012:22(21):2059–2062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Fedorenko  E, Duncan  J, Kanwisher  N. Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013:110(41):16616–16621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Fiedler  M. Algebraic connectivity of graphs. Czechoslov Math J. 1973:23(2):298–305. [Google Scholar]
  65. Fonov  V, Evans  A, McKinstry  R, Almli  C, Collins  D. Unbiased nonlinear average age-appropriate brain templates from birth to adulthood. NeuroImage. 2011:54(1):313–327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Friederici  AD. The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiol Rev. 2011:91(4):1357–1392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Fuster  J. The prefrontal cortex—an update: time is of the essence. Neuron. 2001:30:319–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Gao  Z, Zheng  L, Chiou  R, Gouws  A, Krieger-Redwood  K, Wang  X, Varga  D, Lambon Ralph  MA, Smallwood  J, Jefferies  E. Distinct and common neural coding of semantic and non-semantic control demands. NeuroImage. 2021:236:118230. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Genon  S, Reid  A, Langner  R, Amunts  K, Eickhoff  SB. How to characterize the function of a brain region. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018:22(4):350–364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Glasser  MF, Sotiropoulos  SN, Wilson  JA, Coalson  TS, Fischl  B, Andersson  JL, Xu  J, Jbabdi  S, Webster  M, Polimeni  JR, et al.  The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the human connectome project. NeuroImage. 2013:80:105–124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Glasser  MF, Coalson  TS, Robinson  EC, Hacker  CD, Harwell  J, Yacoub  E, Ugurbil  K, Andersson  J, Beckmann  CF, Jenkinson  M, et al.  A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. 2016:536(7615):171–178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Gough  PM, Nobre  AC, Devlin  JT. Dissociating linguistic processes in the left inferior frontal cortex with transcranial magnetic stimulation. J Neurosci. 2005:25(35):8010–8016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Haak  KV, Beckmann  CF. Understanding brain organisation in the face of functional heterogeneity and functional multiplicity. NeuroImage. 2020:220:117061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Haak  KV, Marquand  AF, Beckmann  CF. Connectopic mapping with resting-state fMRI. NeuroImage. 2018:170:83–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hagoort  P. On Broca, brain, and binding: a new framework. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005:9(9):416–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Hartwigsen  G, Baumgaertner  A, Price  CJ, Koehnke  M, Ulmer  S, Siebner  HR. Phonological decisions require both the left and right supramarginal gyri. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010:107(38):16494–16499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Hartwigsen  G, Neef  NE, Camilleri  JA, Margulies  DS, Eickhoff  SB. Functional segregation of the right inferior frontal gyrus: evidence from Coactivation-based Parcellation. Cereb Cortex. 2019:29(4):1532–1546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Haueis  P. The fuzzy brain. Vagueness and mapping connectivity of the human cerebral cortex. Front Neuroanat. 2012:6:37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Hickok  G. The functional neuroanatomy of language. Phys Life Rev. 2009:6(3):121–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Hickok  G, Poeppel  D. The cortical organization of speech processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007:8(5):393–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Hodgson  VJ, Lambon Ralph  MA, Jackson  RL. Multiple dimensions underlying the functional organization of the language network. NeuroImage. 2021:241:118444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Humphreys  GF, Hoffman  P, Visser  M, Binney  RJ, Lambon Ralph  MA. Establishing task- and modality-dependent dissociations between the semantic and default mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015:112(25):7857–7862. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Huntenburg  JM, Bazin  P-L, Margulies  DS. Large-scale gradients in human cortical organization. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018:22(1):21–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Irish  M, Piguet  O. The pivotal role of semantic memory in remembering the past and imagining the future. Front Behav Neurosci. 2013:7[accessed 2023 Aug 7]:27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Irish  M, Vatansever  D. Rethinking the episodic-semantic distinction from a gradient perspective. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2020:32:43–49. [Google Scholar]
  86. Irish  M, Addis  DR, Hodges  JR, Piguet  O. Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: evidence from semantic dementia. Brain. 2012:135(7):2178–2191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Jackson  RL. The neural correlates of semantic control revisited. NeuroImage. 2021:224:117444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Jackson  RL, Hoffman  P, Pobric  G, Lambon Ralph  MA. The semantic network at work and rest: differential connectivity of anterior temporal lobe subregions. J Neurosci. 2016:36(5):1490–1501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Jackson  RL, Bajada  CJ, Rice  GE, Cloutman  LL, Lambon Ralph  MA. An emergent functional parcellation of the temporal cortex. NeuroImage. 2018:170:385–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Jackson  RL, Cloutman  LL, Lambon Ralph  MA. Exploring distinct default mode and semantic networks using a systematic ICA approach. Cortex. 2019:113:279–297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Jackson  RL, Bajada  CJ, Lambon Ralph  MA, Cloutman  LL. The graded change in connectivity across the ventromedial prefrontal cortex reveals distinct subregions. Cereb Cortex. 2020:30(1):165–180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Jakobsen  E, Böttger  J, Bellec  P, Geyer  S, Rübsamen  R, Petrides  M, Margulies  DS. Subdivision of Broca’s region based on individual-level functional connectivity. Eur J Neurosci. 2016:43(4):561–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Jakobsen  E, Liem  F, Klados  MA, Bayrak  Ş, Petrides  M, Margulies  DS. Automated individual-level parcellation of Broca’s region based on functional connectivity. NeuroImage. 2018:170:41–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Jbabdi  S, Sotiropoulos  SN, Behrens  TE. The topographic connectome. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013:23(2):207–215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Jeon  H-A, Friederici  AD. Two principles of organization in the prefrontal cortex are cognitive hierarchy and degree of automaticity. Nat Commun. 2013:4(1):2041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Jeon  H-A, Friederici  AD. Degree of automaticity and the prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015:19(5):244–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Johansen-Berg  H, Behrens  TEJ, Robson  MD, Drobnjak  I, Rushworth  MFS, Brady  JM, Smith  SM, Higham  DJ, Matthews  PM. Changes in connectivity profiles define functionally distinct regions in human medial frontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004:101(36):13335–13340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Jung  J, Lambon Ralph  MA. Distinct but cooperating brain networks supporting semantic cognition. Cereb Cortex. 2023:33(5):2021–2036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Keller  SS, Crow  T, Foundas  A, Amunts  K, Roberts  N. Broca’s area: nomenclature, anatomy, typology and asymmetry. Brain Lang. 2009:109(1):29–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Kelly  C, Uddin  LQ, Shehzad  Z, Margulies  DS, Castellanos  FX, Milham  MP, Petrides  M. Broca’s region: linking human brain functional connectivity data and non-human primate tracing anatomy studies. Eur J Neurosci. 2010:32(3):383–398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Kiebel  SJ, Daunizeau  J, Friston  KJ. A hierarchy of time-scales and the brain. PLoS Comput Biol. 2008:4(11):e1000209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Klein  JC, Behrens  TEJ, Robson  MD, Mackay  CE, Higham  DJ, Johansen-Berg  H. Connectivity-based parcellation of human cortex using diffusion MRI: establishing reproducibility, validity and observer independence in BA 44/45 and SMA/pre-SMA. NeuroImage. 2007:34(1):204–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Koechlin  E, Jubault  T. Broca’s area and the hierarchical organization of human behavior. Neuron. 2006:50(6):963–974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Koechlin  E, Ody  C, Kouneiher  F. The architecture of cognitive control in the human prefrontal cortex. Science. 2003:302(5648):1181–1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Laird  AR, Eickhoff  SB, Rottschy  C, Bzdok  D, Ray  KL, Fox  PT. Networks of task co-activations. NauroImage. 2013:80:505–514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Lambon Ralph  MA, Sage  K, Jones  RW, Mayberry  EJ. Coherent concepts are computed in the anterior temporal lobes. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2010:107(6):2717–2722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Lambon Ralph  MA, Jefferies  E, Patterson  K, Rogers  TT. The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2017:18(1):42–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Majerus  S, Péters  F, Bouffier  M, Cowan  N, Phillips  C. The dorsal attention network reflects both encoding load and top–down control during working memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2018:30(2):144–159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Margulies  DS, Ghosh  SS, Goulas  A, Falkiewicz  M, Huntenburg  JM, Langs  G, Bezgin  G, Eickhoff  SB, Castellanos  FX, Petrides  M, et al.  Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2016:113(44):12574–12579. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Mazoyer  B, Zago  L, Mellet  E, Bricogne  S, Etard  O, Houdé  O, Crivello  F, Joliot  M, Petit  L, Tzourio-Mazoyer  N. Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res Bull. 2001:54(3):287–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Menon  V, Uddin  LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010:214(5):655–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Mesulam  M. From sensation to cognition. Brain. 1998:121(6):1013–1052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Muhle-Karbe  PS, Derrfuss  J, Lynn  MT, Neubert  FX, Fox  PT, Brass  M, Eickhoff  SB. Co-activation-based Parcellation of the lateral prefrontal cortex delineates the inferior frontal junction area. Cereb Cortex. 2016:26(5):2225–2241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Nakae  T, Matsumoto  R, Kunieda  T, Arakawa  Y, Kobayashi  K, Shimotake  A, Yamao  Y, Kikuchi  T, Aso  T, Matsuhashi  M, et al.  Connectivity gradient in the human left inferior frontal gyrus: intraoperative Cortico-cortical evoked potential study. Cereb Cortex. 2020:30(8):4633–4650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Nee  DE, D’Esposito  M. The hierarchical organization of the lateral prefrontal cortex Davachi L, editor. elife. 2016:5:e12112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Neubert  FX, Mars  RB, Thomas  AG, Sallet  J, Rushworth  MFS. Comparison of human ventral frontal cortex areas for cognitive control and language with areas in monkey frontal cortex. Neuron. 2014:81(3):700–713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Papitto  G, Friederici  AD, Zaccarella  E. The topographical organization of motor processing: an ALE meta-analysis on six action domains and the relevance of Broca’s region. NeuroImage. 2020:206:116321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Passingham  RE, Stephan  KE, Kötter  R. The anatomical basis of functional localization in the cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002:3(8):606–616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Peraza  JA, Salo  T, Riedel  MC, Bottenhorn  KL, Poline  J-B, Dockès  J, Kent  JD, Bartley  JE, Flannery  JS, Hill-Bowen  LD, et al.  Meta-analytic decoding of the cortical gradient of functional connectivity. BioRxiv 2023. 10.1101/2023.08.01.551505. [accessed 2023 Aug 8]. [DOI]
  120. Petrides  M. Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005:360(1456):781–795. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Poldrack  RA, Wagner  AD, Prull  MW, Desmond  JE, Glover  GH, Gabrieli  JDE. Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage. 1999:10(1):15–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Price  CJ. A review and synthesis of the first 20years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. NeuroImage. 2012:62(2):816–847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Raut  RV, Snyder  AZ, Raichle  ME. Hierarchical dynamics as a macroscopic organizing principle of the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020:117(34):20890–20897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Renoult  L, Irish  M, Moscovitch  M, Rugg  MD. From knowing to remembering: the semantic–episodic distinction. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019:23(12):1041–1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Rolls  ET, Joliot  M, Tzourio-Mazoyer  N. Implementation of a new parcellation of the orbitofrontal cortex in the automated anatomical labeling atlas. NeuroImage. 2015:122:1–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Rosa  MGP, Tweedale  R. Brain maps, great and small: lessons from comparative studies of primate visual cortical organization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005:360(1456):665–691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Salimi-Khorshidi  G, Douaud  G, Beckmann  CF, Glasser  MF, Griffanti  L, Smith  SM. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. NeuroImage. 2014:90:449–468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Salo  T, Yarkoni  T, Nichols  TE, Poline  J-B, Bilgel  M, Bottenhorn  KL, Jarecka  D, Kent  JD, Kimbler  A, Nielson  DM, et al.  NiMARE: neuroimaging meta-analysis research environment. NeuroLibre. 2022:1(1):7. [Google Scholar]
  129. Satterthwaite  TD, Elliott  MA, Gerraty  RT, Ruparel  K, Loughead  J, Calkins  ME, Eickhoff  SB, Hakonarson  H, Gur  RC, Gur  RE, et al.  An improved framework for confound regression and filtering for control of motion artifact in the preprocessing of resting-state functional connectivity data. NeuroImage. 2013:64(1):240–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Schenker  NM, Buxhoeveden  DP, Blackmon  WL, Amunts  K, Zilles  K, Semendeferi  K. A comparative quantitative analysis of cytoarchitecture and minicolumnar organization in Broca’s area in humans and great apes. J Comp Neurol. 2008:510(1):117–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Sepulcre  J, Sabuncu  MR, Yeo  TB, Liu  H, Johnson  KA. Stepwise connectivity of the modal cortex reveals the multimodal organization of the human brain. J Neurosci. 2012:32(31):10649–10661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Shulman  GL, Fiez  JA, Corbetta  M, Buckner  RL, Miezin  FM, Raichle  ME, Petersen  SE. Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 1997:9(5):648–663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Smallwood  J, Bernhardt  BC, Leech  R, Bzdok  D, Jefferies  E, Margulies  DS. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021:22(8):503–513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Smith  SM, Beckmann  CF, Andersson  J, Auerbach  EJ, Bijsterbosch  J, Douaud  G, Duff  E, Feinberg  DA, Griffanti  L, Harms  MP, et al.  Resting-state fMRI in the human connectome project. NeuroImage. 2013:80:144–168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Snyder  HR, Banich  MT, Munakata  Y. Choosing our words: retrieval and selection processes recruit shared neural substrates in left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011:23(11):3470–3482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Spisák  T, Spisák  Z, Zunhammer  M, Bingel  U, Smith  S, Nichols  T, Kincses  T. Probabilistic TFCE: a generalized combination of cluster size and voxel intensity to increase statistical power. NeuroImage. 2019:185:12–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Spreng  RN, Stevens  WD, Chamberlain  JP, Gilmore  AW, Schacter  DL. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage. 2010:53(1):303–317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Spreng  RN, Sepulcre  J, Turner  GR, Stevens  WD, Schacter  DL. Intrinsic architecture underlying the relations among the default, dorsal attention, and Frontoparietal control networks of the human brain. J Cogn Neurosci. 2013:25(1):74–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Spreng  RN, DuPre  E, Selarka  D, Garcia  J, Gojkovic  S, Mildner  J, Luh  W-M, Turner  GR. Goal-congruent default network activity facilitates cognitive control. J Neurosci. 2014:34(42):14108–14114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Spunt  RP, Lieberman  MD. Dissociating modality-specific and supramodal neural systems for action understanding. J Neurosci. 2012:32(10):3575–3583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Suárez  LE, Markello  RD, Betzel  RF, Misic  B. Linking structure and function in macroscale brain networks. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020:24(4):302–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Sugar  J, Moser  M-B. Episodic memory: neuronal codes for what, where, and when. Hippocampus. 2019:29(12):1190–1205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Svoboda  E, McKinnon  MC, Levine  B. The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006:44(12):2189–2208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Taylor  P, Hobbs  JN, Burroni  J, Siegelmann  HT. The global landscape of cognition: hierarchical aggregation as an organizational principle of human cortical networks and functions. Sci Rep. 2015:5(1):18112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Thiebaut de Schotten  M, Urbanski  M, Batrancourt  B, Levy  R, Dubois  B, Cerliani  L, Volle  E. Rostro-caudal architecture of the frontal lobes in humans. Cereb Cortex. 2017:27(8):4033–4047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Thomas Yeo  BT, Krienen  FM, Sepulcre  J, Sabuncu  MR, Lashkari  D, Hollinshead  M, Roffman  JL, Smoller  JW, Zöllei  L, Polimeni  JR, et al.  The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011:106(3):1125–1165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Thompson-Schill  SL, D’Esposito  M, Aguirre  GK, Farah  MJ. Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997:94(26):14792–14797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Thompson-Schill  SL, Bedny  M, Goldberg  RF. The frontal lobes and the regulation of mental activity. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005:15(2):219–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Tian  Y, Zalesky  A. Characterizing the functional connectivity diversity of the insula cortex: subregions, diversity curves and behavior. NeuroImage. 2018:183:716–733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Uddén  J, Bahlmann  J. A rostro-caudal gradient of structured sequence processing in the left inferior frontal gyrus. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2012:367(1598):2023–2032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Uddin  LQ. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015:16(1):55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Uddin  LQ, Betzel  RF, Cohen  JR, Damoiseaux  JS, Brigard  FD, Eickhoff  SB, Fornito  A, Gratton  C, Gordon  EM, Laird  A, et al.  Controversies and current progress on large-scale brain network nomenclature from OHBM WHATNET: workgroup for HArmonized taxonomy of NETworks. OSF Preprints, 2022. 10.31219/osf.io/25za6. [accessed 2022 Aug 31]. [DOI]
  153. Ueno  T, Saito  S, Rogers  TT, Lambon Ralph  MA. Lichtheim 2: synthesizing aphasia and the neural basis of language in a Neurocomputational model of the dual dorsal-ventral language pathways. Neuron. 2011:72(2):385–396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Van Essen  DC, Smith  SM, Barch  DM, Behrens  TEJ, Yacoub  E, Ugurbil  K. The WU-Minn human connectome project: an overview. NeuroImage. 2013:80:62–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Vatansever  D, Smallwood  J, Jefferies  E. Varying demands for cognitive control reveals shared neural processes supporting semantic and episodic memory retrieval. Nat Commun. 2021:12(1):2134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Vigneau  M, Beaucousin  V, Hervé  PY, Duffau  H, Crivello  F, Houdé  O, Mazoyer  B, Tzourio-Mazoyer  N. Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing. NeuroImage. 2006:30(4):1414–1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Vos de Wael  R, Benkarim  O, Paquola  C, Lariviere  S, Royer  J, Tavakol  S, Xu  T, Hong  SJ, Langs  G, Valk  S, et al.  BrainSpace: a toolbox for the analysis of macroscale gradients in neuroimaging and connectomics datasets. Commun Biol. 2020:3(1):1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Wandell  BA, Brewer  AA, Dougherty  RF. Visual field map clusters in human cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2005:360(1456):693–707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Wandell  BA, Dumoulin  SO, Brewer  AA. Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron. 2007:56(2):366–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Wang  J, Yang  Y, Zhao  X, Zuo  Z, Tan  LH. Evolutional and developmental anatomical architecture of the left inferior frontal gyrus. NeuroImage. 2020:222:117268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Wang  X, Gao  Z, Smallwood  J, Jefferies  E. Both default and multiple-demand regions represent semantic goal information. J Neurosci. 2021:41(16):3679–3691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Whitney  C, Kirk  M, O’Sullivan  J, Lambon Ralph  MA, Jefferies  E. The neural Organization of Semantic Control: TMS evidence for a distributed network in left inferior frontal and posterior middle temporal gyrus. Cereb Cortex. 2011:21(5):1066–1075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Whitney  C, Kirk  M, O’Sullivan  J, Lambon Ralph  MA, Jefferies  E. Executive semantic processing is underpinned by a large-scale neural network: revealing the contribution of left prefrontal, posterior temporal, and parietal cortex to controlled retrieval and selection using TMS. J Cogn Neurosci. 2012:24(1):133–147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Wojtasik  M, Bludau  S, Eickhoff  SB, Mohlberg  H, Gerboga  F, Caspers  S, Amunts  K. Cytoarchitectonic characterization and functional decoding of four new areas in the human lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Front Neuroanat. 2020:14:2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Zaccarella  E, Friederici  AD. The neurobiological nature of syntactic hierarchies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017:81:205–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Zaccarella  E, Meyer  L, Makuuchi  M, Friederici  AD. Building by syntax: the neural basis of minimal linguistic structures. Cereb Cortex. 2017:27(1):411–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Diveicaetal_LIFGgradients_Supplementary_Information_Final_bhad373

Articles from Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY) are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES