Abstract
Background:
Brief Motivational Interventions (BMIs) are one of the most effective individually-focused alcohol intervention strategies for college students. Despite the central theoretical role of motivation for change in BMIs, it is less clear whether BMIs actually increase motivation to change drinking behavior. We conducted a two-step meta-analysis of individual participant data (IPD; 15 trials, N = 5,903; 59% women, 72% White) from Project INTEGRATE to examine whether BMIs increased motivation for change. The BMIs included individually delivered motivational interviewing with personalized feedback (MI+PF), stand-alone personalized feedback (PF), and group-based motivational interviewing (GMI).
Methods:
Different measures and responses used in the original trials were harmonized. Effect size estimates were derived from a model that adjusted for baseline motivation and demographic variables for each trial (step 1) and subsequently combined in a random-effects meta-analysis (step 2).
Results:
The overall intervention effect of BMIs on motivation for change was not statistically significant (standard mean difference [SMD]: 0.026, 95% CI: [−0.001, 0.053], p = .06, k = 19 comparisons). Of the three subtypes of BMIs, GMI, which tended to provide motivation-targeted content, had a statistically significant intervention effect on motivation, compared to controls (SMD: 0.055, 95% CI: [0.007, 0.103], p = .025, k = 5). In contrast, there was no evidence that MI + PF (SMD = 0.04, 95% CI: [−0.02, 0.10], k = 6, p = 0.20) nor PF increased motivation (SMD = 0.005, 95% CI: [−0.028, 0.039], k = 8, p = 0.75), compared to controls. Post hoc meta-regression analysis suggested that motivation sharply decreased each month within the first three months post-intervention (b = −0.050, z = −2.80, p = .005 for k = 14).
Conclusions:
Although BMIs provide motivational content and normative feedback and are assumed to motivate behavior change, the results do not wholly support the hypothesis that BMIs improve motivation for change. Changing motivation is difficult to assess during and following interventions, but it is still a theoretically important clinical endpoint. Further, the evidence cautiously suggests that changing motivation may be achievable, especially if motivation-targeted content components are provided.
Keywords: Brief motivational interventions, motivation for change, individual participant data, meta-analysis, Project INTEGRATE
Introduction
In the United States (US), over half (54.3%) of 18- to 25-year-olds report consuming alcohol in the past month (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration [SAMHSA], 2020). Within this age group, 52.5% of full-time college students ages 18 to 22 report drinking alcohol in the past month, and 33.0% report engaging in heavy episodic drinking (SAMHSA, 2020). Heavy alcohol use—including heavy episodic drinking— is associated with a wide range of negative consequences, including greater risk for academic problems, assault, alcohol use disorder, injuries, hospitalization, and death (Hingson et al., 2017a; Hingson et al., 2017b). To reduce alcohol use, several individually focused intervention strategies have been developed and deployed on college campuses (Larimer et al., 2022). These interventions are aimed at reducing alcohol consumption and alcohol-related negative consequences by changing students’ alcohol-related knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors.
Among these individually focused interventions, brief motivational interventions (BMIs) have shown effectiveness in reducing young adults’ alcohol use (Cronce et al., 2018; Mun et al., 2022b), alcohol-related problems (Huh et al., 2015, 2022; Jiao et al., 2020), and driving after drinking (Mun et al., 2022a). Many BMIs have been modeled after the Brief Alcohol Screening and Intervention for College Students (BASICS; Dimeff et al., 1999), which incorporates personalized feedback with motivational interviewing (Miller & Rollnick, 2002). The body of large-scale evidence from Project INTEGRATE indicates that the effect size of BMIs on typical alcohol consumption per week is an 8% difference (vs. controls) in the overall mean number of drinks through 6 months post intervention (Mun et al., 2022b); a 19% difference (vs. controls) in odds for driving after four+/five+ drinks (an approximately ˗0.12 standardized mean difference; Mun et al., 2022a); and a ˗0.031 reduction per month in latent trait scores for alcohol-related problems through 12 months post intervention for those who received in-person motivational intervention with personalized normative feedback profile (MI + PF vs. controls; Jiao et al., 2020). The reduction of 0.031 per month is 0.372 at 12 months, which can be understood as changing an average person from the 50th percentile at baseline to the 35.49th percentile at 12 months after the intervention. Furthermore, Project INTEGRATE has reported a ˗0.015 difference between stand-alone personalized feedback intervention (PF vs. controls) in the indirect effect on alcohol-related problems via protective behavioral strategies (Huh et al., 2022). Therefore, there is consistent evidence that BMIs change alcohol consumption and alcohol-related consequences.
How behavior change occurs in BMIs, however, has been relatively unclear in the literature. College students often have low motivation to change their drinking behavior; compared to other populations, they are less likely to recognize their own drinking problems while simultaneously overestimating their peers’ pro-drinking norms (Baer, 2002; Cox et al., 2019; Perkins et al., 2005). Through contrasting (a) students’ perceptions with the actual drinking patterns of peers and (b) students’ current drinking behaviors with desired life values and goals, it has been theorized that BMIs develop discrepancies – normative discrepancy and self-ideal discrepancy, respectively – to increase students’ awareness of alcohol-related negative consequences (Borsari & Carey, 2001; Miller & Rollnick, 2013; Neighbors et al., 2004), and motivation for change (Larimer et al., 2021; Miller & Rollnick, 2013). Despite being a critical mechanistic link theorized to explain the efficacy of BMIs, there is limited evidence on the effects of BMIs on young adults’ motivation to change their drinking behavior.
Findings from a few prior studies regarding the effects on motivation for change have been sparse and mixed (Barnett et al., 2007; Borsari et al., 2009; Murphy et al., 2010; Ostafin & Palfai, 2012). For instance, in a study evaluating the efficacy of a counselor-delivered BMI and a computer-delivered intervention with 225 mandated college students, Barnett et al. (2007) reported increased motivation from pre- to post-intervention but did not find significant differences in motivation between the BMI and the computer-based alcohol education program. In another study that analyzed data from three randomized trials implementing BMIs, Borsari et al. (2009) found no evidence of BMI effects on motivation compared to an assessment-only control in one trial (Carey et al., 2006) or to a feedback-only condition in another trial (Murphy et al., 2004). The BMI in the third trial did have significant beneficial effects on student motivation that persisted up to 6 weeks post-intervention compared to an assessment-only control (Borsari & Carey, 2000). In two other studies, Murphy et al. (2010) and Ostafin and Palfai (2012) reported increased motivation for change among college students after receiving a BMI (vs. a computer-based alcohol education program and an assessment-only control, respectively). However, the sample sizes of both studies were relatively small (n = 74 in study 1 and n = 133 in study 2 in Murphy et al. [2010], and n = 87 in Ostafin and Palfai [2012]). As Mun et al. (2015) have noted, individual studies with small sample sizes are more prone to biased findings than those from larger sample sizes studies or meta-analyses because it is common to find large effects in small studies, which often fail to replicate. In sum, it is unclear whether BMIs actually increase motivation for change among young adults, despite this being a purported mechanism of action. The sparse empirical evidence on BMI effects on young adults’ motivation for change may be partially due to “selective reporting” in the literature, where results are selectively withheld from publication due to statistical non-significance (Chan et al., 2004; Dwan et al., 2013). A meta-analysis of individual participant data (IPD) can effectively overcome this barrier and examine whether BMIs affect motivation in a large pool of research trials conducted on college student populations. The current study aimed to address this knowledge gap by utilizing a large pool of IPD from multiple primary BMI trials with a variety of measures of motivation designed to reflect stages of behavior change from a transtheoretical model perspective (Prochaska et al., 1992).
The Current Study
We conducted an IPD meta-analysis using data from Project INTEGRATE (Mun et al., 2015) – the largest IPD meta-analysis project in alcohol intervention research for college students. The IPD included in 24 studies of Project INTEGRATE are reasonably representative of the trials conducted between 1998 and 2009 (see Mun et al., 2015 for a detailed description). Of the studies, multiple independent trials assessed motivation using different measures among samples with diverse participant characteristics. Heterogeneous outcome measures and samples from different trials are considered strengths as well as challenges of IPD meta-analysis. The use of various measures of motivation for change across the included trials allows for a more nuanced examination of whether an overall intervention effect exists and how robust the effect is. Similarly, the diversity of samples and intervention/comparison groups in an IPD meta-analysis permits the examination of the robustness of intervention effects across different participants and settings by directly modeling the sources of heterogeneity in IPD or by examining variation in aggregate estimates in a meta-analysis with many studies. Further, IPD meta-analysis provides an opportunity to assess whether effects are replicated across trials. We hypothesized that BMIs (vs. control) would be associated with greater motivation for change in drinking behavior among college students.
The current study utilized a two-step or two-stage meta-analysis approach as opposed to a one-step or one-stage meta-analysis (Simmonds et al., 2005, 2015). The “one-step” approach typically involves a simultaneous analysis of all IPD in a single multilevel analysis. For example, Huh et al. (2015, 2019, 2022) show examples of a one-step approach to probe intervention effects and mediation effects. This one-step approach, which is closely related to an integrative data analysis (IDA) approach proposed by Curran and Hussong (2009), allows researchers to explicitly address the multilevel data structure, such as clustering within studies and participant-level missing data. However, the one-step IPD meta-analysis approach assumes that all studies have measures and designs that can be made comparable. This can be a prohibitive requirement, especially in alcohol intervention research, given the heterogeneity in the measurement of motivation to change outcomes and other sources of heterogeneity across trials. Even if this requirement is met or circumvented, it can be computationally intensive and challenging to implement (Burke et al., 2017).
A second approach to IPD meta-analysis is a “two-step” approach (Simmonds et al., 2015), in which participant-level data are first analyzed and aggregated at the study level (e.g., study-specific estimates of the treatment effect). These study-level data derived from IPD are then combined in the second step using traditional meta-analytic methods to estimate the overall effect and between-study heterogeneity. This two-step approach is more common (e.g., Jiao et al., 2020; Mun et al., 2022a, 2022b; White et al., 2015) and has certain advantages over the one-step approach, especially for complex models, given increased flexibility in model specification and decreased computational burden. We, therefore, used the two-step IPD meta-analysis approach in the current study, accommodating between-study heterogeneity in a random-effects meta-analysis model. We subsequently examined BMI subtype differences as well as short-term vs. long-term follow-up differences in effect sizes. Finally, we conducted a meta-regression analysis to examine to what extent the overall BMI effect on motivation decreased over time.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Of the 24 studies with available IPD from Project INTEGRATE (Mun et al., 2015; N = 12,630 participants at baseline), 15 trials met the inclusion criteria of the current study: (a) a randomized trial with at least two arms – a BMI arm and a control group arm, and (b) at least one outcome measure representing participants’ motivation for change in drinking behavior, measured at baseline and a follow-up within 12 months post-intervention (see Table 1). At baseline, IPD from 7,433 participants across 15 trials were available. Of those, 1,530 participants did not have outcome data at the first follow-up within 12 months post-intervention, resulting in a final sample of 5,903 participants (41% men, 72% White, and 59% first-year student) across the 15 trials (see Figure 1).
Table 1.
Description of the Individual Participant Data at Baseline from the 15 Studies Included in the Current Study (N = 8,067; 35 Randomized Groups and 15 Studies)
Study | Reference | BMI Type | Randomized Group n | Follow-up (Months) | %White | % Male | % First Year |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 | White et al. (2008) | PF Control |
111 119 |
2 | 67 | 71 | 63 |
7.1 | Fromme et al. (2004) | GMI Control |
100 24 |
1 | 73 | 76 | 57 |
7.2 | Fromme et al. (2004) | GMI Control |
317 135 |
1 | 58 | 59 | 37 |
8a | Larimer et al. (2007) | PF Control |
736 750 |
12 | 86 | 33 | 49 |
8b | Larimer et al. (2007) | PF Control |
1094 1061 |
12 | 61 | 41 | 47 |
8c | Larimer et al. (2007) | PF Control |
303 297 |
12 | 82 | 38 | 36 |
9 | Lee et al. (2009) | GMI MI + PF PF Control |
97 101 100 101 |
3 | 72 | 38 | 100 |
10.1 | Baer et al. (2001) | MI + PF Control |
174 174 |
12 | 84 | 46 | 100 |
11 | Walters et al. (2007) | PF Control |
185 198 |
2 | 63 | 59 | 100 |
12 | Wood et al. (2007) | MI+PF Control |
84 83 |
1 | 93 | 47 | 3 |
14 | Murphy et al. (2001) | MI+PF Control |
30 25 |
3 | 96 | 44 | 38 |
15 | Labrie et al. (2008) | GMI Control |
155 108 |
1 | 51 | 0 | 100 |
16 | LaBrie et al. (2009) | GMI Control |
161 126 |
1 | 56 | 0 | 100 |
18 | Martens et al. (2010) | PF Control |
102 113 |
1 | 89 | 25 | 33 |
20 | Larimer et al. (2001) | MI + PF Control |
318 369 |
12 | 82 | 54 | 74 |
21 | Walters et al. (2009) | MI + PF PF Control |
76 68 72 |
3 | 85 | 36 | 42 |
Notes. Study 7 is a single study with two distinct subsamples. Study 8 is a multi-site study (Studies 8a, 8b, 8c). MI + PF = individually delivered in-person motivational interviewing intervention with personalized feedback, GMI = group motivational interviewing intervention, BMI = brief motivational intervention, PF = stand-alone personalized feedback intervention.
Figure 1.
PRISMA IPD Flow Diagram
Notes. IPD come from Project INTEGRATE (Mun et al., 2015). Data flow at the identification, screening, and eligibility stages is not applicable (unknown).
Intervention and Control Groups
BMIs included in Project INTEGRATE were grouped into three categories: individually delivered motivational interviewing with personalized feedback (MI+PF), stand-alone personalized feedback (PF), and group-based motivational interviewing (GMI). All BMIs were delivered individually in person, in group, via mail, or via computer/online. Stand-alone personalized feedback was printed and handed to participants (Study 2), mailed to participants (Study 8a, 8b, and 8c), or emailed or shown on a computer to participants (Studies 9, 11, 18, and 21). Although the BMIs differed slightly in content topics and personalization levels, they had considerable content overlap across studies (see Ray et al., 2014; Mun & Ray, 2018). The content in each intervention group per study was checked and coded by two independent coders. Of the 20 identified intervention components in BMIs (see Ray et al., 2014), we defined motivation-targeted intervention content as content that included decisional balance information on weighing the pros and cons of alcohol use or change (henceforth referred to as decisional balance) or content related to changing one’s alcohol use, such as readiness to change or personal goal setting (referred to as change discussion). These components were coded based on how they were provided (0 = did not include; 1 = general content; 2 = personalized content; 3 = both general and personalized content; see Figure 2).
Figure 2.
Intervention Exposure to Motivation-related Content by Group per Study
Notes. Decisional balance: Information on weighing the pros and cons of alcohol use or barriers to change. Personal content includes feedback on a participant’s pros and cons or barriers to change, whereas general content includes information on general pros and cons or typical barriers that students face. Change discussion: content that relates to changing one’s alcohol use behaviors, including readiness to change or goal-setting. Personal content addresses a participant’s readiness to change or personal goals about changing; general content includes information such as the importance of goal setting.
Controls in the current study were mostly assessment-only controls (Studies 2, 7–12, 14, 20, 21). Studies 15 and 16 provided a handout to control participants that contained information on alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences. Study 18 provided controls with a handout containing brief educational information about alcohol-related negative consequences and calories (i.e., stating that alcohol has no nutritional value). Study 20 provided a single-page handout with tips to reduce risk from alcohol. These “control” groups were similarly classified with assessment-only controls in an analysis based on their content components (Mun & Ray, 2018).
Measures
Motivation for change.
The primary outcome of interest was motivation for change in drinking behavior. Studies used three different measures: (1) the 12-item Readiness to Change Questionnaire (RCQ; Heather, Rollnick, & Bell, 1993; see Supplemental Table 1 for items, (2) the 13-item University of Rhode Island Change Assessment (URICA; McConnaughy et al., 1983; see Supplemental Table 2 for items, and (3) the Contemplation Ladder (Biener & Abrams, 1991; see Supplemental Table 3 for the item). Table 2 summarizes all measures of motivation for change used in the 15 trials. No trials used more than one measure of motivation for change, so no items were common in all trials.
Table 2.
Variation in the Measurement of Motivation for Change in Drinking Behavior Across Studies
Study | Questionnaire | Number of items | Original response categories (values in original studies) |
Possible range of total scale scores | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
2, 8a, 8b, 8c, 9, 11, 18, 21 | Readiness to Change Questionnaire (RCQ) | 12 | Strongly Disagree Disagree Undecided Agree Strongly Agree |
(−2) (Study 18: 1) (−1) (Study 18: 2) (0) (Study 18: 3) (1) (Study 18: 4) (2) (Study 18: 5) |
−24–24 (Study 18: 12–60) |
7.1, 7.2, 10.1, 14, 20 | University of Rhode Island Change Assessment (URICA) | 13 (Study 14: 5) | Strongly Disagree Disagree Undecided Agree Strongly Agree |
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) |
13–65 (Study 14: 5–25) |
12, 15, 16 | Contemplation Ladder | 1 | I’ve never needed to change my drinking (Study 12: No thought of changing) Sometimes I think about drinking less (Study 12: Think I need to consider someday) I have decided to drink less (Study 12: Think I should change, but not quite ready) I am already trying to cut back on my drinking (Study 12: Start to think about how to change my drinking patterns) My drinking has changed; I now drink less than before (Study 12: Take action to change [e.g., cutting down]) |
(1) (3) (Study 12: 2) (5) (Study 12: 3) (7) (Study 12: 4) (10) (Study 12: 5) |
1–10 (Study 12: 1–5) |
Follow-up schedule.
Most trials had 1 or 2 follow-ups. The first follow-up assessment occurred 1–3 months post intervention in 10 studies (67%). Five trials (33%) had only one assessment at 12 months post intervention.
Demographic information.
Demographic variables included sex at birth (1 = male vs. 0 = female), race (1 = White vs. 0 = non-White), and first-year student status (1 = first-year vs. 0 = non-first-year).
Data Preparation and Analysis
Before data analysis, all IPD were checked for errors, outliers, and statistical assumption violations. First, we checked whether any raw items needed to be reverse-coded to create a scale score for motivation for change. We checked codebooks and also examined item-to-total correlations and descriptive statistics. Second, four different scales and different response options used in the primary trials were harmonized across trials (Table 2, see also Supplemental Tables 1–4 for scale details, and Supplemental Table 6 for Cronbach’s α for the motivation measure for each trial).
Next, we calculated Cohen’s d, standardized mean difference (SMD), a standardized effect size measure that reflects the difference in means on a continuous outcome variable between intervention and control groups at the first follow-up. Due to the various measures of motivation across the trials that ranged in measurement scales (see Figure 3 and Supplemental Table 5), it was necessary to use the SMD so that effect sizes from different trials could be combined meaningfully.
Figure 3.
Motivation for Change in Original Scale by Group per Study at Baseline (N = 7,433) and Follow-up (N = 5,903)
Note. Supplemental Table 5 reports all values shown in this figure. RCQ = The Readiness to Change Questionnaire, URICA = The University of Rhode Island Change Assessment, URICA* = A 5-item URICA, LADDER = The Contemplation Ladder. X-axis = study. Empty round symbols indicate control groups. Filled triangle symbols = PF, filled square symbols = GMI, and filled diamonds = MI + PF. The symbols connected with a grey line indicate baseline and follow-up data.
Because some trials showed baseline differences in motivation for change and demographic characteristics across randomized groups, in the first step of the IPD meta-analysis, we estimated intervention effect sizes within each study while accommodating baseline motivation scores and demographic covariates. This IPD approach is advantageous over aggregate data meta-analysis because derived estimates from the same model have the same interpretation and also because estimates are covariate-adjusted. The full model at the study level can be shown as:
where indexes participant, b0, b1, … b7 are regression coefficients, and ei is a participant-specific residual error term. For the intervention groups MI + PF, PF, or GMI compared to controls, (MI+PF)i, PFi, and GMIi are dummy-coded variables that indicate random allocation to MI + PF, PF, or GMI, respectively (each coded 1), compared to controls (coded 0). The regression coefficients b1, b2, and b3 quantify the covariate-adjusted average difference between participants who received (1) MI + PF, (2) stand-alone PF, or (3) GMI, respectively, compared to control participants within each study. The covariate BL_MOTi refers to baseline motivation scores, and the covariates WHITEi, MALEi, and FIRSTYRi refer to the baseline demographic characteristics: sex (1 = man vs. 0 = woman), race (1 = White vs. 0 = non-White), and first-year student status (1 = first-year vs. 0 = non-first-year). POST_MOTi refers to the post-intervention motivation score for participant i. In the second step of the IPD meta-analysis, we used random-effects meta-analysis models to obtain the pooled overall effect size and meta-regression to examine whether the intervention effects on motivation differed across BMI subtypes and which BMI subtype had stronger intervention effects on motivation in short-term vs. long-term follow-ups.
The analysis in the current study was limited to complete cases. The participants included did not differ statistically at baseline from those excluded due to missing data at follow-ups. We included key covariates, such as baseline covariates, in the analysis so that the resulting inferences account for these variables, assuming that missing responses were related to observed data but not unobserved data (i.e., missing at random [MAR]). In alcohol intervention research, MAR has been assumed reasonable in prior synthesis studies (Huh et al., 2022; Mun et al., 2009). Data preparation was conducted using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC), IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 26), and Microsoft Excel (Version 16); meta-analyses were conducted using the package ‘metafor’ version 3.0–2 (Viechtbauer, 2010) for R version 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2021). Statistical significance was set at p < .05, and analyses were two-tailed. Annotated R code and data can be accessed in the online repository at doi:10.17632/ndzhdv86dg.1(Tan et al., 2023b).
Results
Measure Harmonization
Motivation for change was assessed with different questionnaires and/or items across the included trials. Even when trials used the same questionnaire, they sometimes used different response options. For example, at the item level, items were sometimes worded slightly differently (“No thought of changing” vs. “I’ve never needed to change my drinking” in the Contemplation Ladder). And at the questionnaire level, trials sometimes used different versions of the same questionnaire. For instance, Study 14 used a subset (Items 1, 3, 5, 6, 8) of the items in the full URICA. For item response values, all RCQ responses were assigned scores ranging from −2 to 2, except that in Study 18, the responses ranged from 1 to 5. In Study 10, pre-contemplation items (Items 1, 2, 5, 10) had already been reversely coded in the original primary study (see Supplemental Tables 1–4). Both the RCQ and the URICA used a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “Strongly Disagree” to “Strongly Agree.” The Contemplation Ladder used a 1-to-10 scale from “No thought of changing” to “Take action to change,” except that a 1-to-5 scale was used in Study 12.
We carefully reviewed codebooks for item content and response item scores to harmonize these measures. All raw item variables were checked, and if necessary, some of the items were recoded. Examples included reverse-coding Items 1, 5, 10, and 12 for studies that used the RCQ (except Study 10, which was already reverse-coded). In addition, response options for Study 18 were recoded to make them consistent with other studies that used the RCQ. Responses to the Contemplation Ladder in Study 12 were also appropriately changed (see details in Supplemental Tables 1–4).
Checking Data and Descriptive Analysis
Figure 3 (see also Supplemental Table 5) shows the means and standard deviations of motivation for change scores for intervention and control groups for each included trial at baseline and follow-up. Figure 3 shows the between-study heterogeneity across different measures and baseline imbalance across groups in Study 7.2. We also examined funnel plots to detect any potential publication bias due to the suppression of non-significant findings and found no concerning evidence of possible publication bias (Supplemental Figure 1).
Overall BMI on Motivation for Change
Figure 4 shows a forest plot summarizing the study-level effect of BMIs on motivation for change and the overall effect of BMIs on motivation for change in drinking behavior. The overall effect from the model adjusted for baseline motivation and demographic characteristics was not statistically significant, SMD = 0.026, 95% CI: [−0.001, 0.053], p = 0.06, across k = 19 comparisons (study-specific results of model-based intervention effects are available upon request). The estimated amount of total heterogeneity = 0.001, with a ratio of total heterogeneity to total variability, I2 statistic = 28.1%, indicating a small to moderate between-study heterogeneity. Figure 4 presents the different degrees of efficacy across BMI types and trials, with 13 out of 19 effect sizes in the direction of BMIs increasing motivation for change compared to the control groups.
Figure 4.
Forest Plot of Motivation for Change in Drinking from the Model Adjusted for Baseline Motivation for Change and Demographic Variables
Note. MI + PF = individually delivered in-person motivational interviewing intervention with personalized feedback, GMI = group motivational interviewing intervention, PF = stand-alone personalized feedback intervention, SMD = standardized mean difference.
BMI subtypes and motivation for change.
We subsequently examined whether BMI subtypes differed in their effects on motivation for change. Although there was no statistically significant subgroup difference in SMDs, GMI statistically significantly increased motivation for change compared to controls (SMD = 0.055, 95% CI: [0.007, 0.103], k = 5, = 0.000, I2 = 0.00%, p = 0.02). In contrast, there was no evidence that MI + PF (SMD = 0.04, 95% CI: [−0.02, 0.10], k = 6, = 0.002, I2 = 37.26%, p = 0.20) nor PF increased motivation (SMD = 0.005, 95% CI: [−0.028, 0.039], k = 8, = 0.001, I2 = 20.22%, p = 0.75), compared to controls.
Supplemental Figure 2 shows all effect sizes from the unadjusted models in the first stage of the IPD meta-analysis in a forest plot. All statistical conclusions were the same, except that confidence intervals tended to be wider, which is expected. In addition, the combined effect size of GMI from the unadjusted models was larger (SMD = 0.19, p = 0.01), which was somewhat expected due to the model differences.
Intervention content components targeted for motivation.
Upon examining the intervention content provided in each of the intervention groups, we found that GMIs tended to provide both decisional balance and change discussion (see Figure 2). This motivation-targeted content included both general information (such as typical barriers students might face) and content underscoring the importance of goal setting and personalized information (e.g., barriers to change specifically related to the student). Most interventions (12 out of 19 intervention groups) did not use a decisional balance. Four GMIs (Studies 7.1, 7.2, 15, and 16) included decisional balance activities that were both personalized and general. Change discussion was more common as it was provided in most studies with two exceptions (Study 9: PF and Study 18).
Time since Intervention and Motivation
Finally, we examined whether the effects of BMIs on student motivation varied according to the post-intervention follow-up time. The results showed that studies with short-term (within the first three months) and long-term (12 months) follow-ups did not have statistically significantly different effect sizes, b = 0.002, z = 0.05, k = 19, = 0.001, I2 = 32.08%, p = 0.96. However, meta-regression analysis among the studies with a short-term follow-up indicated that there was a statistically significant decrease in the intervention effects over time within the first three months post-intervention, b = −0.050, z = −2.80, k = 14, = 0.000, I2 = 0.000%, p = 0.01.
Discussion
The current study is the first large-scale meta-analysis evaluating the intervention effect of BMIs on motivation for change in drinking among college students. Facilitated by IPD, we verified raw data, checked baseline balance between randomized groups, and made appropriate adjustments for variation in participant characteristics across 19 comparisons in 15 trials. In this way, we ensured that the necessary assumptions were met for proper estimation and inference and that the same interpretation of effect sizes could be made across heterogeneous trials in a meta-analysis.
The findings from the current study did not support the primary hypothesis that BMIs increase motivation for change in drinking among college students. However, a subsequent subgroup analysis suggests that GMIs led to significantly improved motivation compared to control groups. Although MI + PF interventions yielded effects on motivation that were of similar magnitude to that observed for GMIs, the effects for those types of BMIs were not statistically significant, which could be in part due to the limited statistical power in these subgrouped analyses. Nevertheless, the evidence suggests that if there is an effect on participant motivation, it is likely to be small, equivalent to a 0.06 standard deviation improvement. Using a standard deviation of ~8.5 for the RCQ scale scores in Supplemental Table 5, this improvement of SMD = 0.06 translates to about a half unit of the response scale. For example, it would represent an improvement of 0.5 from Unsure (0) toward Agree (1) in response to all RCQ questions, such as “I enjoy my drinking, but sometimes I drink too much.” This small effect of BMIs on motivation for change among college students is consistent with the findings in a large meta-analysis of treatment trials (Magill et al., 2018). Further, this small effect size may explain the previously reported inconsistencies among study findings (Borsari & Carey, 2000; Carey et al., 2006; Murphy et al., 2004; Murphy et al., 2010; Ostafin & Palfai, 2012).
Given the importance of motivation in behavior change theories (DiClemente and Velasquez, 2002; Miller and Rollnick, 2013) and BMIs (Larimer et al., 2021), an open challenge to the field is how best to strengthen BMIs, so that young adults’ motivation for change is increased while simultaneously reducing drinking motives. Young adults, including college students, are often unmotivated to participate in alcohol-focused interventions (see Murphy et al., 2022, for a scoping review). This is likely because they have established drinking motives that are stable in their 20s and 30s (Windle et al., 2018). Recent studies suggest that drinking motives distinguish high-intensity from non-high-intensity drinking at both the event level (Patrick & Terry-McElrath, 2021) and the individual level (White et al., 2016).
Our post hoc findings suggest that decisional balance and change discussion are potential components to increase motivation. However, these components would be easier to implement during in-person sessions (vs. computerized or online sessions), which require considerable funding, time, and human resources. Where an individually delivered in-person MI session is not practical for motivation-focused content, a GMI that incorporates these exercises may be viable as a more cost-efficient yet effective alternative.
Although BMIs tend to share many of the same intervention components (Ray et al., 2014), BMI subtypes may operate through different mechanisms and thus vary in their effects on different outcomes. For example, stand-alone PF interventions may be efficacious through different therapeutic paths rather than changing motivation. It may be because, with minimal in-person contact, PF focuses more on providing personalized feedback on drinking norms and other helpful feedback, such as protective behavioral strategies, rather than addressing motivation, which may require more interaction and processing. In prior research from Project INTEGRATE, we found that PF was more effective in improving use of protective behavioral strategies, which then helps reduce alcohol-related problems (Huh et al., 2022). PF interventions often require fewer resources to deliver and thus may be easier to scale up in their implementation, especially with newer technological adaptations. Thus, PF interventions are often more practical than in-person interventions (Cronce et al., 2018). How to improve motivation when feedback is delivered through the Web or an app may require additional considerations for better engagement and outcome. Recent studies that provided synchronous teleconferencing (e.g., Zoom) or text-based intervention have demonstrated feasibility and preliminary effectiveness (e.g., Gex et al., 2022). With the COVID-19 pandemic, no-contact in-person meetings have proliferated and have been generally accepted (Mark et al., 2022; Oliveira et al., 2021). Therefore, future trials may explore supplementing PF with an additional “as-needed exposure” to improve motivation for college students in a stepped-care approach (Murphy et al., 2022; Ray et al., 2023).
In previous Project INTEGRATE studies, intervention effects on alcohol consumption (Mun et al., 2022b) and driving after drinking (Mun et al., 2022a) wane over time following intervention completion, which is common among behavioral interventions (Tanner-Smith and Lipsey, 2015). Thus, we expected that motivation might present similar decreasing intervention effects over time within the first 12 months post-intervention. Surprisingly, the findings from the subgroup analysis suggest no evidence of different effects across time. However, this should be cautiously interpreted because the follow-up schedule and study membership were confounded due to the limited sample size at the study level. Nevertheless, there was a significant average decrease of 0.05 in SMD every month in the first three-month post-intervention. Given the small effect size of BMIs (i.e., an SMD = 0.026), this precipitous drop suggests that the motivation level decreased quickly. Hence, there may be a sensitive window of opportunity in which participants may be receptive to intervention and motivated to initiate behavior changes. This “motivated” time window may help intervention researchers determine the timing for additional personalized support for independently sustainable behavior changes. This also suggests that periodic booster sessions, including digitally delivered boosters, may be worth considering at key follow-up periods to promote more sustained effects over time (Barnett et al., 2004; Braitman et al., 2020).
Limitations and Future Directions
The current study had some limitations. First, the IPD sample was not systematically or probabilistically sampled from the entire body of literature. It is, therefore, uncertain how representative the findings are in the broader BMI and brief alcohol intervention literature. Obtaining IPD representative of the entire population is difficult, if not impossible, given numerous structural and procedural barriers to data sharing (Levis et al., 2021; Ventresca et al., 2020). Furthermore, simple random sampling is rare, even in original primary studies. On a related point, even with this large, pooled IPD sample, we could not disentangle all potential sources of confounding due to the limited number of studies across design variations, such as delivery modes of PF and different measures of motivation. However, given the effect sizes across studies, unaccounted systematic influences on effect sizes are expected to be small.
Second, the current analyses retained some of the limitations of the original trials, specifically, the limitation of original measures for motivation for change. The URICA and the RCQ were originally designed to reflect different stages of motivation. Hence, they are inherently multi-unidimensional and may not be ideal for assessing motivation quantitatively. We reverse-coded item responses to approximate a unidimensional, trait-level motivation. More empirical work would be needed to better assess motivation and examine whether measures are invariant across major racial/ethnic groups and sexes and also across time. For example, a recent exploratory factor analysis study to examine invariance suggests that the URICA may not be invariant across three months (Richards et al., 2022). A thorough investigation of the measures of motivation is beyond the scope of the current study. Nonetheless, the development of new measures informed by emerging theories of behavior change and the refinement of the existing measures may help fill the existing knowledge gap about the nature of motivation and change following an intervention.
Third, the frequency and timing of assessing motivation may not have been ideal for capturing motivation that is fluid. Kaysen et al. (2009) showed that motivation does considerably fluctuate week to week among a sample of female college students, accounting for 28% of the total variability. They also showed that although people in the intervention group were more motivated to change than controls, drinking the week before negatively affected peoples’ motivation to change. More broadly, change in motivation may occur relatively quickly within a single session or immediately following the presentation of feedback. Furthermore, once drinking behavior changes have occurred, the individual may no longer feel additional changes are needed. Thus, motivation may appropriately decrease over a follow-up period, or an individual may experience an unsuccessful change attempt and a concomitant reduction in motivation due to discouragement. For example, a recent effectiveness trial among adults experiencing homelessness showed that a motivation-guided, just-in-time intervention delivered via ecological momentary assessment reduced drinking (Walters et al., 2022), suggesting how changing motivation could be utilized to provide a tailored, repeated intervention. Assessing and responding to motivation as a dynamic variable rather than a static indicator of individual differences may help our understanding of how interventions change motivation and, ultimately, drinking behaviors.
Fourth, in the current IPD analysis context, some studies used a single-item measure, while others used the URICA and the RCQ. To overcome the heterogeneity in measures, we used harmonization procedures. Harmonization is an essential preparation step for IPD meta-analysis to make measures comparable across different studies or assessment time points for subsequent synthesis (Huo et al., 2015; Mun et al., 2015, 2016, 2019). In the current study, no items were shared across all trials, which is needed for item equating. Each study used only one of the measures to assess motivation, with none of the measures sharing similar response ranges or anchor points. This makes the heuristic or quantitative harmonization of the measures difficult, especially with single-item measures. This data condition makes it impossible to analyze data in a one-step meta-analysis or IDA. We overcame this challenge by conducting a two-step IPD meta-analysis, which is a prevailing approach to IPD meta-analysis; recent simulation studies indicate that the results from one- vs. two-step IPD meta-analysis generally converge (e.g., Huh et al., 2023).
With sufficient item overlap across primary studies, it may be possible to conduct an item-based, more granular analysis in the future. Compared to traditional scale score-based approaches, novel item-level analysis, such as the cognitive diagnosis modeling (CDM) method used in recent studies (Tan et al., 2023a; Liang et al., 2023), may be feasible to calibrate item parameters better and characterize participants cross-sectionally and longitudinally by taking into account other related behaviors of interest under certain data situations (e.g., large samples). The CDM approach may be a promising future tool for brief alcohol interventions to classify participants at the individual level and examine intervention effects across a broad range of related health behaviors because alcohol misuse commonly co-occurs with other mental health problems. The CDM-informed intervention approach may help develop “precision” alcohol intervention programs.
Finally, we acknowledge that some comparisons were nested within the trial when primary trials had multiple intervention arms. Though it is common in meta-analysis to analyze the multiple arms from the same trial separately, it violates the independence assumption when the analysis shares the same control group for the multiple arms within the trial. However, its impact on the reported results is expected to be limited because only two contributing trials had multiple intervention arms.
Conclusions
Although more work is necessary to fully evaluate the mechanisms of change of BMIs and brief alcohol interventions more broadly, the current study suggests that not all BMIs may motivate change unless specific exercises are included to address motivation. While this finding may be disappointing given that BMIs are associated with reduced drinking (Mun et al., 2022b) and motivation is the stated pathway (Dimeff et al., 1999), motivation is likely a multifaceted and dynamic construct that involves various aspects of salience, efficacy, and timing (e.g., “ready,” “willing,” and “able”). Measures with a better “bandwidth,” which cover these aspects of motivation, may be needed. The current study also highlights the difficulty of assessing motivation over time and disentangling temporal associations. When asking about changing motivation among college students, the timing of measurement may be critical. It is also possible that motivation does not play the same role among young drinkers as it does among older drinkers or those in treatment. Finally, changing motivation among young adults may be possible when carefully developed intervention content components are provided. Although BMIs share commonly assumed mechanisms of behavior change and critical intervention components, there still remains a great deal of heterogeneity in this family of brief alcohol interventions. The current study utilized a fine-grained approach to data synthesis to highlight how BMIs may help improve motivation, particularly for young adults who are motivated to drink.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the following contributors to Project INTEGRATE in alphabetical order: John S. Baer, Department of Psychology, The University of Washington, and Veterans’ Affairs Puget Sound Health Care System; Nancy P. Barnett, Center for Alcohol and Addiction Studies, Brown University; M. Dolores Cimini, University Counseling Center, The University at Albany, State University of New York; William R. Corbin, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University; Kim Fromme, Department of Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin; Joseph W. LaBrie, Department of Psychology, Loyola Marymount University; Mary E. Larimer, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, The University of Washington; Matthew P. Martens, Department of Educational, School, and Counseling Psychology, The University of Missouri; James G. Murphy, Department of Psychology, The University of Memphis; Scott T. Walters, Department of Health Behavior and Health Systems, The University of North Texas Health Science Center; Helene R. White, Center of Alcohol and Substance Use Studies, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey; and the late Mark D. Wood, Department of Psychology, The University of Rhode Island.
We would like to thank Nickeisha Clarke, Yang Jiao, Su-Young Kim, and Anne E. Ray for their earlier work on coding and harmonizing interventions and outcomes, Jimmy de la Torre and Yan Huo for their work on measurement, and Helene R. White for her valuable conceptual and methodological contributions in the early years of Project INTEGRATE. Finally, we acknowledge Feng Geng for his contribution to data visualization.
The project described was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) grants R01 AA019511 and K02 AA028630. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIAAA or the National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
Conflict of Interest Statement
None declared
References
*Studies included in the meta-analysis are marked in the References with an asterisk.
- Baer JS (2002) Student factors: understanding individual variation in college drinking. J Stud Alcohol Suppl 63(2):40–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Blume AW, McKnight P, Marlatt GA (2001) Brief intervention for heavy-drinking college students: 4-year follow-up and natural history. Am J Public Health 91(8):1310–1316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Barnett NP, Murphy JG, Colby SM, Monti PM (2007) Efficacy of counselor vs. computer-delivered intervention with mandated college students. Addict Behav 32(11):2529–2548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnett NP, Tevyaw TOL, Fromme K, Borsari B, Carey KB, Corbin WR, … Monti PM (2004) Brief alcohol interventions with mandated or adjudicated college students. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 28(6):966–975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biener L, Abrams DB (1991) The Contemplation Ladder: Validation of a measure of readiness to consider smoking cessation. Health Psychol 10(5):360–365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Carey KB (2000) Effects of a brief motivational intervention with college student drinkers. J Consult Clin Psychol 68(4):728–733. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Carey KB (2001) Peer influences on college drinking: A review of the research. J Subst Abuse 13(4):391–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsari B, Murphy JG, Carey KB (2009) Readiness to change in brief motivational interventions: A requisite condition for drinking reductions? Addict Behav 34(2): 232–235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Braitman AL, Lau‐Barraco C (2020) Descriptive norms but not harm reduction strategies as a mediator of personalized boosters after a computerized college drinking intervention. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 44(1):284–296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke DL, Ensor J, Riley RD (2017) Meta-analysis using individual participant data: One-stage and two-stage approaches, and why they may differ. Stat Med 36(5): 855–875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey KB, Carey MP, Maisto SA, Henson JM (2006) Brief motivational interventions for heavy college drinkers: A randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 74(5):943–954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan AW, Hróbjartsson A, Haahr MT, Gøtzsche PC, Altman DG (2004). Empirical evidence for selective reporting of outcomes in randomized trials: comparison of protocols to published articles. JAMA, 291(20): 2457–2465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox MJ, DiBello AM, Meisel MK, Ott MQ, Kenney SR, Clark MA, Barnett NP (2019) Do misperceptions of peer drinking influence personal drinking behavior? Results from a complete social network of first-year college students. Psychol Addict Behav 33(3):297–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cronce JM, Toomey TL, Lenk K, Nelson TF, Kilmer JR, Larimer ME (2018) NIAAA’s college alcohol intervention matrix. Alcohol Res 39(1):43–47. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curran PJ, Hussong AM (2009) Integrative data analysis: The simultaneous analysis of multiple data sets. Psychol Methods 14: 81–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiClemente CC, & Velasquez MM (2002) Motivational interviewing and the stages of change. In Miller WR & Rollnick S (Eds.), Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change (pp. 201–216). New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Dimeff LA, Baer JS, Kivlahan DR, Marlatt GA (1999) Brief Alcohol Screening and Intervention for College Students (BASICS): A harm reduction approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Dwan K, Gamble C, Williamson PR, Kirkham JJ, Reporting Bias Group (2013) Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias—an updated review. PloS One, 8(7): e66844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Fromme K, Corbin W (2004) Prevention of heavy drinking and associated negative consequences among mandated and voluntary college students. J Consult Clin Psychol 72(6):1038–1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gex KS, Mun E-Y, Barnett NP, McDevitt-Murphy M, Ruggiero KJ, Thurston IB, Olin CC, Voss AT, Withers AJ, Murphy JG (2022) A randomized pilot trial of a mobile delivered brief motivational interviewing and behavioral economic alcohol intervention for emerging adults. Psychol Addict Behav. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heather N, Rollnick S, Bell A (1993) Predictive validity of the Readiness to Change Questionnaire. Addiction 88(12):1667–1677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hingson RW, Zha W, Smyth D (2017a) Magnitude and trends in heavy episodic drinking, alcohol-impaired driving, and alcohol-related mortality and overdose Hospitalizations among emerging adults of college ages 18–24 in the United States, 1998–2014. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 78(4):540–548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hingson RW, Zha W, White AM (2017b) Drinking beyond the binge threshold: Predictors, consequences, and changes in the U.S. Am J Prev Med 52(6):717–727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huo Y, de la Torre J, Mun E-Y, Kim S-Y, Ray AE, Jiao Y, White HR (2015) A hierarchical multi-unidimensional IRT approach for analyzing sparse, multi-group data for integrative data analysis. Psychometrika 80(3): 834–855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huh D, Baldwin SA, Zhou Z, Park J, Mun E-Y (2023) Which is better for individual participant data meta-analysis of count outcomes with many zeroes, one-step or two-step analysis? A simulation study. Multivariate Behav Res. Online first, 10.1080/00273171.2023.2173135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huh D, Mun E-Y, Larimer ME, White HR, Ray AE, Rhew IC, Kim S-Y, Jiao Y, Atkins DC (2015) Brief motivational interventions for college student drinking may not be as powerful as we think: An individual participant‐level data meta‐analysis. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 39(5):919–931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huh D, Mun E-Y, Walters ST, Zhou Z, Atkins DC (2019) A tutorial on individual participant data meta-analysis using Bayesian multilevel modeling to estimate alcohol intervention effects across heterogeneous studies. Addict Behav 94:162–170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huh D, Li X, Zhou Z, Walters ST, Baldwin SA, Tan Z, Larimer ME, Mun E-Y (2022) A structural equation modeling approach to meta-analytic mediation analysis: Testing protective behavioral strategies as a mediator of brief motivational intervention effects on alcohol-related problems. Prev Sci 23 (3):390–402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiao Y, Mun E-Y, Trikalinos TA, Xie M (2020) A CD-based mapping method for combining multiple related parameters from heterogeneous intervention trials. Stat Interface 13(4): 533–549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaysen DL, Lee CM, LaBrie JW, Tollison SJ (2009). Readiness to change drinking behavior in female college students. J Stud Alcohol Drugs Suppl (16):106–114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.LaBrie JW, Huchting K, Tawalbeh S, Pedersen ER, Thompson AD, Shelesky K, Larimer M, Neighbors C (2008) A randomized motivational enhancement prevention group reduces drinking and alcohol consequences in first-year college women. Psychol Addict Behav 22(1):149–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.LaBrie JW, Huchting KK, Lac A, Tawalbeh S, Thompson AD, Larimer ME (2009) Preventing risky drinking in first-year college women: further validation of a female-specific motivational-enhancement group intervention. J Stud Alcohol Drugs Suppl (16):77–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larimer ME, Kilmer JR, Cronce JM, Hultgren BA, Gilson MS, Lee CM (2022) Thirty years of BASICS: Dissemination and implementation progress and challenges. Psychol Addict Behav 36(6): 664–677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Larimer ME, Lee CM, Kilmer JR, Fabiano PM, Stark CB, Geisner IM, Mallett KA, Lostutter TW, Cronce JM, Feeney M, Neighbors C (2007) Personalized mailed feedback for college drinking prevention: A randomized clinical trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 75(2):285–293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Larimer ME, Turner AP, Anderson BK, Fader JS, Kilmer JR, Palmer RS, Cronce JM (2001) Evaluating a brief alcohol intervention with fraternities. J Stud Alcohol 62(3):370–380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Lee C, Kaysen D, Neighbor C, Kilmer J, Larimer M (2009) Feasibility, acceptability, and efficacy of brief interventions for college drinking: comparison of group, individual, and web-based alcohol prevention formats [Unpublished manuscript]. Seattle, Washington: University of Washington. [Google Scholar]
- Levis B, Hattle M, Riley RD (2021) PRIME-IPD SERIES Part 2. Retrieving, checking, and harmonizing data are underappreciated challenges in individual participant data meta-analyses. J Clin Epidemiol 136:221–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liang Q, de la Torre J, Larimer ME, Mun E-Y (2023) Mental health symptom profiles over time: A three-step latent transition cognitive diagnosis modeling analysis with covariates. In Stemmler M, Wiedermann W, & Huang F (Eds.), Dependent data in social sciences research: Forms, issues, and methods of analysis (2nd ed). Springer. (in press) [Google Scholar]
- Magill M, Apodaca TR, Borsari B, Gaume J, Hoadley A, Gordon REF, Tonigan JS, Moyers T (2018) A meta-analysis of motivational interviewing process: Technical, relational, and conditional process models of change. J Consult Clin Psychol 86(2):140–157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mark TL, Treiman K, Padwa H, Henretty K, Tzeng J, Gilbert M (2022) Addiction treatment and telehealth: review of efficacy and provider insights during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatr Serv 73(5):484–491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Martens MP, Kilmer JR, Beck NC, Zamboanga BL (2010) The efficacy of a targeted personalized drinking feedback intervention among intercollegiate athletes: a randomized controlled trial. Psychol Addict Behav 24(4):660–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McConnaughy EA, Prochaska JO, Velicer WF (1983) Stages of change in psychotherapy: Measurement and sample profiles. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research & Practice 20(3): 368–375. [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR, Rollnick S (2002) What is motivational interviewing? In Motivational interviewing: Preparing people for change (2nd ed, pp. 33–42). New York, NY: Guildford. [Google Scholar]
- Miller WR, Rollnick S (2013) Motivational interviewing: Helping people change (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
- Mun E-Y, de la Torre J, Atkins DC, White HR, Ray AE, Kim SY, Jiao Y, Clarke N, Huo Y, Larimer ME, Huh D, The Project INTEGRATE Team (2015) Project INTEGRATE: An integrative study of brief alcohol interventions for college students. Psychol Addict Behav 29(1):34–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mun E-Y, Huo Y, White HR, Suzuki S, de la Torre J (2019) Multivariate higher-order IRT model and MCMC algorithm for linking individual participant data from multiple studies. Front Psychol 10: 1328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mun E-Y, Jiao Y, Xie M (2016) Integrative data analysis for research in developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti D (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 1042–1087). New York: Wiley. 10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy123 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mun E-Y, Li X, Lineberry S, Tan Z, Huh D, Walters ST, Zhou Z, Larimer ME (2022a) Do brief alcohol interventions reduce driving after drinking among college students? A two-step meta-analysis of individual participant data. Alcohol Alcohol 57(1):125–135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mun E-Y, Ray AE (2018) Integrative data analysis from a unifying research synthesis perspective. In Fitzgerald HE & Puttler LI (Eds.), Alcohol use disorders: A developmental science approach to etiology (pp. 341–353). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oso/9780190676001.003.0020 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mun E-Y, White HR, Morgan TJ (2009) Individual and situational factors that influence the efficacy of personalized feedback substance use interventions for mandated college students. J Consult Clin Psychol 77(1), 88–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mun E-Y, Zhou Z, Huh D, Tan L, Li D, Tanner-Smith EE, Walters ST, & Larimer ME (2022b) Brief alcohol interventions are effective through 6 months: Findings from marginalized zero-inflated Poisson and negative binomial models in a two-step IPD meta-analysis. Online first, Prev Sci. 10.1007/s11121-022-01420-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy JG, Benson TA, Vuchinich RE, Deskins MM, Eakin D, Flood AM, McDevitt-Murphy ME, Torrealday O (2004) A comparison of personalized feedback for college student drinkers delivered with and without a motivational interview. J Stud Alcohol 65(2):200–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy JG, Dennhardt AA, Skidmore JR, Martens MP, Mcdevitt-Murphy ME (2010) Computerized versus motivational interviewing alcohol interventions: Impact on discrepancy, motivation, and drinking. Psychol Addict Behav 24(4):628–639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Murphy JG, Duchnick JJ, Vuchinich RE, Davison JW, Karg RS, Olson AM, Smith AF, Coffey TT (2001) Relative efficacy of a brief motivational intervention for college student drinkers. Psychol Addict Behav 15(4):373–379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy JG, Gex KS, Dennhardt AA, Miller AP, O’Neill SE, Borsari B. (2022). Beyond BASICS: A scoping review of novel intervention content to enhance the efficacy of brief alcohol interventions for emerging adults. Psychol Addict Behav 36(6): 607–618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Lewis MA (2004) Targeting misperceptions of descriptive drinking norms: Efficacy of a computer-delivered personalized normative feedback intervention. J Consult Clin Psychol 72(3):434–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira C, Pereira A, Vagos P, Nóbrega C, Gonçalves J, Afonso B (2021) Effectiveness of mobile app-based psychological interventions for college students: A systematic review of the literature. Front Psychol 12: 647606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostafin BD, Palfai TP (2012) When wanting to change is not enough: Automatic appetitive processes moderate the effects of a brief alcohol intervention in hazardous-drinking college students. Addict Sci Clin Pract 7(1):25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick ME, Lyden GR, Morrell N, Mehus CJ, Gunlicks-Stoessel M, Lee CM, King CA, Bonar EE, Nahum-Shani I, Almirall D, Larimer ME, Vock DM (2021) Main outcomes of M-bridge: A sequential multiple assignment randomized trial (SMART) for developing an adaptive preventive intervention for college drinking. J Consult Clin Psychol 89(7):601–614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick ME, Terry-McElrath YM (2021) Drinking motives and drinking consequences across days: Differences and similarities between moderate, binge, and high-intensity drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 45(5):1078–1090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perkins HW, Haines MP, Rice R (2005) Misperceiving the college drinking norm and related problems: A nationwide study of exposure to prevention information, perceived norms and student alcohol misuse. J Stud Alcohol 66(4):470–478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC, Norcross JC (1992) In search of how people change. Applications to addictive behaviors. Am Psychol 47: 1102–1114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team (2021) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. [Statistical software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Ray AE, Kim S-Y, White HR, Larimer ME, Mun E-Y, Clarke N, Jiao Y, Atkins DC, Huh D , The Project INTEGRATE Team (2014) When less is more and more is less in brief motivational interventions: Characteristics of intervention content and their associations with drinking outcomes. Psychol Addict Behav 28(4):1026–1040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray AE, Mun E-Y, Lewis MA, Litt DM, Stapleton JL, Tan L, Buller D, Zhou Z, Bush H, Himelhoch S (2023) Cross-tailoring integrative alcohol and risky sexual behavior feedback for college students: Protocol for a hybrid type I effectiveness-implementation trial. JMIR Res Protoc 12: e43986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richards DK, Schwebel FJ, Cowie KD, Pearson MR, Witkiewitz K (2022) Measurement invariance of the University of Rhode Island Change Assessment Scale in Project MATCH: An exploratory structural equation modeling approach. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 46: 1258–1267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmonds MC, Higgins JPT, Stewart LA, Tierney JF, Clarke MJ, Thompson SG (2005) Meta-analysis of individual patient data from randomized trials: A review of methods used in practice. Clinical Trials 2(3):209–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simmonds MC, Stewart G, Stewart L (2015) A decade of individual participant data meta-analyses: A review of current practice. Contemporary Clinical Trials 45(Part A): 76–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration [SAMHSA]. (2020) Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2019 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. (HHS Publication No. PEP20–07-01–001, NSDUH Series H-55). Rockville, MD: Center for Behavioral Health Statistics Quality, SAMHSA [Google Scholar]
- Tan Z, de la Torre J, Ma W, Huh D, Larimer ME, Mun E-Y (2023a) A tutorial on cognitive diagnosis modeling for characterizing mental health symptom profiles using existing item responses. Prev Sci 24: 480–492. 10.1007/s11121-022-01346-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan Z, Tanner-Smith EE, Walters ST, Tan L, Huh D, Zhou Z, Luningham JM, Larimer ME, Mun E-Y (2023b) Do brief motivational interventions increase motivation for change in drinking among college students? A 2-step meta-analysis of individual participant data. Mendeley Data, doi: 10.17632/ndzhdv86dg.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanner-Smith EE, Lipsey MW (2015) Brief alcohol interventions for adolescents and young adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Subst Abuse Treat 51: 1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ventresca M, Schünemann HJ, Macbeth F, Clarke M, Thabane L, Griffiths G, … Briel M (2020) Obtaining and managing data sets for individual participant data meta-analysis: Scoping review and practical guide. BMC Med Res Methodol, 20(1):1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viechtbauer W (2010) Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Software 36(3):1– 48. [Google Scholar]
- Walters ST, Mun E-Y, Tan Z, Luningham JM, Hébert E, Oliver JA, Businelle MS (2022) Development and preliminary effectiveness of a smartphone-based, just-in-time adaptive intervention for adults with alcohol misuse who are experiencing homelessness. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 46: 1732–1741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Walters ST, Vader AM, Harris TR (2007) A controlled trial of web-based feedback for heavy drinking college students. Prev Sci 8(1):83–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Walters ST, Vader AM, Harris TR, Field CA, Jouriles EN (2009) Dismantling motivational interviewing and feedback for college drinkers: A randomized clinical trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 77(1):64–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White HR, Anderson KG, Ray AE, Mun E-Y (2016) Do drinking motives distinguish extreme drinking college students from their peers? Addict Behav 60: 213–218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White HR, Jiao Y, Ray AE, Huh D, Atkins DC, Larimer ME, Fromme K, Corbin WR, Baer JS, Labrie JW, Mun E-Y (2015). Are there secondary effects on marijuana use from brief alcohol interventions for college students? J Stud Alcohol Drugs 76(3):367–377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.White HR, Mun E-Y, Morgan TJ (2008) Do brief personalized feedback interventions work for mandated students or is it just getting caught that works? Psychol Addict Behav 22(1):107–116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Windle RC, Windle M (2018) Adolescent precursors of young adult drinking motives. Addict Behav 82:151–157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Wood MD, Capone C, Laforge R, Erickson DJ, Brand NH (2007) Brief motivational intervention and alcohol expectancy challenge with heavy drinking college students: a randomized factorial study. Addict Behav 32(11):2509–2528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.