Abstract
Background
Most research on early outcomes in infants with a family history (FH) of autism has focussed on categorically defined autism, although some have language and developmental delays. Less is known about outcomes in infants with a FH of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Methods
Infants with and without a FH of autism and/or ADHD, due to a first‐degree relative with either or both conditions, were recruited at 5 or 10 months. Three year outcomes were characterised using latent profile analysis (LPA) across measures of cognitive ability, adaptive functioning and autism, ADHD and anxiety traits (n = 131). We additionally ran an LPA using only autism and ADHD measures, and the broader LPA in an independent cohort (n = 139) and in both cohorts combined (n = 270).
Results
A Low Developmental Level + High Behavioural Concerns class had elevated autism, ADHD and anxiety scores, low cognitive and adaptive function, and included all but one child with autism. A Low Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour class had average cognitive ability and typical behaviour but low adaptive function. A Typical Developmental Level + Some Behavioural Concerns class had average cognitive and adaptive function but slightly elevated behaviour scores. A High Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour class had above average cognitive ability and typical behaviour. All four LPAs identified classes characterised by combinations of either, or both, Low Development Level and elevated behaviour scores, as well as a typically developing class. No classes had elevated autism or ADHD traits in isolation.
Conclusions
Some infants with a FH of autism or ADHD have atypical developmental and behavioural outcomes, but do not show strong autism or ADHD traits in isolation. The field needs to recalibrate aims and methods to embrace the broader transdiagnostic pattern of outcomes seen in these infants.
Keywords: ADHD, autism, early childhood, infancy, latent profile analysis
We used a broad range of outcome measures to identify the ‘natural categories’ of early atypical outcome in infants with family history of autism or ADHD. Some infants with a family history of autism or ADHD have neurodevelopmental profiles characterised by combinations of either, or both, low developmental level and atypical behaviour but not elevated autism or ADHD traits in isolation. The field needs to adopt a transdiagnostic approach in the study of early development in autism and ADHD in order to develop fit‐for‐purpose developmental models and translational opportunities.
Key points.
We used a broad range of outcome measures to identify the ‘natural categories’ of early atypical outcome in infants with FH of autism and/or ADHD.
Some infants with a FH of autism and/or ADHD have neurodevelopmental profiles characterised by combinations of either, or both, low developmental level and atypical behaviour but not elevated autism or ADHD traits in isolation.
The field needs to adopt a transdiagnostic approach in the study of early development in autism and ADHD in order to develop fit‐for‐purpose developmental models and translational opportunities.
INTRODUCTION
Autism is a strongly heritable condition (Tick et al., 2016). Against a population prevalence of ∼1%–2% (Maenner et al., 2020) sibling recurrence rates in clinically ascertained cohorts are approximately 10% (Hansen et al., 2019). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is also highly heritable (Thapar & Cooper, 2016). Its prevalence is ∼3%–5% (Polanczyk et al., 2015) but clinically‐ascertained sibling recurrence rates for later‐born siblings are also ∼10% (Miller, Musser, et al., 2019). Autism and ADHD commonly co‐occur at both the clinical and the trait level (Hollingdale et al., 2020; Miller, Musser, et al., 2019). Twin and family studies show moderate shared heritability between the two conditions (Ghirardi et al., 2019; Taylor et al., 2013). Consistent with this, siblings of children with autism have elevated rates of ADHD and, conversely, siblings of children with ADHD have elevated rates of autism (Ghirardi et al., 2019; Jokiranta‐Olkoniemi et al., 2016; Miller, Musser, et al., 2019). Understanding the aetiology of this co‐occurrence has important implications for science and clinical practice (Thapar et al., 2017). Prospective infant designs may provide new insights into this co‐occurrence. Infants are not recruited based on an existing diagnosis of either, or both, autism and ADHD but on the basis of family history (FH). Studying development in infancy before clear manifestations of the behaviours that define autism and ADHD have emerged may help us to identify shared and distinct neurodevelopmental pathways to each condition, both in isolation and in combination (Johnson et al., 2015).
In prospective studies of infants with a FH of autism, recurrence rates of autism are ∼20% (Messinger et al., 2015; Ozonoff et al., 2011). However, there is also an increased rate of broader atypical outcomes beyond an autism diagnosis. Subclinical autism traits, and lower cognitive, language and adaptive ability characterise ∼20% of infants who do not have an autism diagnosis (Charman et al., 2017; Marrus et al., 2018; Messinger et al., 2013; Ozonoff et al., 2014). Few studies have reported the rates of intellectual disability and language delay in prospectively‐studied infants who have autism, but group‐level data from large cohorts suggests these rates are significant (e.g., Table 2, Zwaigenbaum et al., 2021; Figure 2, Messinger et al., 2015).
Fewer studies have reported on early developmental outcomes in prospectively‐studied infants with a FH of ADHD, and findings are more preliminary (Johnson et al., 2015). One recent study of infants with a FH of autism or ADHD characterised clinical outcomes at 3 years. Miller et al. (2020) used latent profile analysis (LPA) of observation and parent‐report measures of autism and ADHD traits and identified three outcome classes: an autism class (with both high autism and ADHD traits), an ADHD class (with high ADHD traits but low autism traits), and a typically developing class. In the same cohort, Reetzke et al. (2021) reported clinically derived outcome groups, comprising those with an autism diagnosis and those with ‘ADHD concerns’ based on a combination of researcher clinical best estimate (CBE) and parent and teacher report of ADHD traits. Miller et al. (2020; Table 3) show high correspondence between the LPA data‐derived autism class and a CBE autism judgement but only moderate correspondence between the ADHD classifications, with significant proportions of those identified in the LPA ADHD class having a CBE of autism or no concerns. To date, no study has reported on broader developmental and behavioural outcomes in a cohort of infants with a FH of ADHD.
The current study
The extant literature on infants with an autism FH, and the wider literature on family recurrence of neurodevelopmental conditions, suggests that family liability is not specific to autism or to ADHD. Transdiagnostic approaches to understanding early developmental psychopathology are important due to the shared liability factors and high co‐occurrence of these conditions but also because they reflect presentations seen in clinical settings (Astle et al., 2022; Talbott & Miller, 2020; Thapar et al., 2017). In line with this, in the current study we characterised outcome in infants at elevated familial likelihood of autism and ADHD, due to having a first‐degree relative with either or both conditions, by classes defined using LPA across measures of cognitive ability, adaptive behaviour and early autism, ADHD and anxiety traits. We also conducted an analysis similar to Miller et al. (2020) using only autism and ADHD measures and replicated our broader LPA in an independent cohort and in both our cohorts combined. We expected to identify a class of children with autism and broader atypical development and behaviour. We examined whether additional group(s) with atypical outcomes existed and if these showed predominantly elevated early ADHD traits or broader developmental and behavioural difficulties.
METHODS
Participants
Infants at either 5 or 10 months of age were enroled if they had a first‐degree relative with a community clinical diagnosis of autism, a first‐degree relative with a community clinical diagnosis of ADHD or elevated ADHD traits, or both. Parental report of an existing clinical diagnosis of autism and/or ADHD in an older sibling (proband) was the most common route. Some parents reported that they themselves had a diagnosis of either condition or they or their older child had suspected ADHD following which screening with a short version of one the Conners suite of measures was employed (see Supplementary Materials Appendix S1, Table S1 and Table S2). Comparison infants with no FH of either condition and a typically developing older sibling were also recruited. Inclusion criteria also included full‐term birth (gestational age >36 weeks) and no known medical or developmental condition. Informed written consent was provided by parents/carers. The total sample (n = 161) comprised 80 infants with a first‐degree relative (family history (FH)) with autism only (FH‐Autism), 31 infants with a first‐degree relative with ADHD only (FH‐ADHD), 21 infants with first‐degree relatives with both autism and ADHD (FH‐Autism + ADHD), and 29 infants with no first degree relative with either condition (typical likelihood (TL)) (Table S3). Of these, 131 infants (81.4%) had at least one outcome measure used in the LPA (see below) and are included in the current analysis: 67 FH‐Autism, 26 FH‐ADHD, 16 FH‐Autism + ADHD, and 22 TL (Table 1).
TABLE 1.
Participant demographic characteristics by family history (FH) sampling frame.
FH‐Autism | FH‐Autism + ADHD | FH‐ADHD | TL | |
---|---|---|---|---|
N = 67 | N = 16 | N = 26 | N = 22 | |
N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | |
Sex | ||||
Male | 32 (48%) | 11 (69%) | 15 (58%) | 13 (59%) |
Female | 35 (52%) | 5 (31%) | 11 (42%) | 9 (41%) |
Age in months | ||||
Mean (SD) | 37.21 (1.46) | 37.19 (1.52) | 37.35 (2.69) | 36.79 (1.78) |
Ethnicity (maternal) | ||||
White/European/Irish | 53 (83%) | 14 (93%) | 24 (92%) | 17 (85%) |
Asian/African/African‐caribbean/Mixed heritage | 11 (17%) | 1 (7%) | 2 (8%) | 3 (15%) |
Maternal education | ||||
Up to high school/Further education | 19 (30%) | 8 (53%) | 8 (31%) | 1 (5%) |
University degree or higher | 45 (70%) | 7 (47%) | 18 (69%) | 19 (95%) |
Abbreviations: FH‐Autism, autism family history; FH‐Autism + ADHD, autism + ADHD family history; FH‐ADHD, ADHD family history; TL, Typical likelihood.
Three year outcome measures
The Mullen Early Learning Composite (ELC) (Mullen, 1995) – derived from Expressive language, Receptive language, Visual reception and Fine motor subscales – and the Vineland‐II Adaptive Behaviour Composite (ABC) (Sparrow et al., 2005) – derived from Socialization, Communication, Daily Living Skills and Motor domains – were administered as measures of cognitive ability and adaptive functioning, respectively.
The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule‐2 (ADOS‐2; Lord et al., 2012), Autism Diagnostic Interview‐Revised (ADI‐R; Lord et al., 1994) and the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Rutter et al., 2003) were administered. Autism traits were also measured with the Social Responsiveness Scale 2‐Preschool Form (SRS‐2; Constantino & Gruber, 2012). Best estimate Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM‐5) clinical diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder was informed by, but not dependent on, scores on the ADOS‐2, ADI‐R, SCQ, Vineland‐II, and Mullen, and researcher observations and parent‐reported information, by experienced researchers (TC, GP).
Emerging ADHD and anxiety traits were measured using the parent‐report DSM subscales of the Child Behaviour Checklist‐Preschool (CBCL‐P 1.5–5; Achenbach & Ruffle, Achenbach and Rescorla (2000)). An observational measure of early ADHD behaviours (attentiveness, activity level, and inhibition to objects/the environment; scored on a 7 point Likert scale) was completed using consensus coding by researchers based on observations made across the visit.
Statistical analysis
We conducted LPA using continuous indicator variables to identify homogeneous classes based on the following 3 years outcome variables: Mullen ELC, Vineland ABC, ADOS‐2 CSS, SCQ total score, researcher early ADHD observation total score, and CBCL ADHD and anxiety subscale raw scores. Variables were modelled, conditional on latent class, using Poisson distributions for all variables except SCQ where overdispersion required a negative binomial distribution. Latent profile analysis was performed using the gsem command in Stata 16 (StataCorp., 2019) on the whole sample with at least one of the seven outcome measures available (n = 131; 122 children had ≥4 measures, 4 ≥ 3, 3 ≥ 2, and 2 children 1 measure only). Models were estimated using maximum likelihood to account for data missing at random. To select the “best fitting” solution we examined conventional likelihood‐based (Bayesian information criterion (BIC)) and classification‐based (Integrated Classification Likelihood (ICL); entropy) fit statistics (Henson et al., 2007); the proportion of participants represented in each class; and the extent to which classes captured clinically meaningful subgroups of participants (Nylund et al., 2007). Individuals were assigned to classes based on the maximum aposterior probability of class membership (MAP).
We named the outcome classes in terms of developmental level (cognitive ability and adaptive function) and behaviour (autism, ADHD and anxiety traits) – namely the indicator variables from which they were derived. We compared the scores of the outcome classes using ANOVA with post‐hoc Tukey‐Kramer corrections to account for unequal cell sizes and tested sex differences in class assignment using chi‐square tests followed by post‐hoc residuals adjusted for the sample size of each group (Kirk, 1998). The high values of MAP we report made more complex multi‐step post‐assignment analysis methods unnecessary.
RESULTS
Three‐ and four‐class solutions had similar entropy values (0.82 and 0.80, respectively) and the best fit statistics (BIC = 4486.24, ICL = 4537.94 and BIC = 4484.21, ICL = 4555.60, respectively; Table S4). We chose the four‐class solution as providing the most robust and clinically meaningful distribution of classes, with a minimum class size comprising 15.6% (n = 20) of the sample and average MAP values for each class all ≥0.83. Table S5 shows the correlations between the class indicator variables and R‐squared values from regressing each indicator onto the set of classes.
Scores of the four‐classes on the outcome measures used to derive the classes are shown in Table 2 1 and Figure 1. Based on the pattern across the measures we labelled the classes: Low Developmental Level + High Behavioural Concerns (LDL + HBC; n = 20, 16%), Low Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour (LDL + TB; n = 23, 19%), Typical Developmental Level + Some Behavioural Concerns (TDL + SBC; n = 33, 24%), and High Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour (HDL + TB; n = 55, 42%).
TABLE 2.
3 Year developmental and behavioural characteristics by latent profile analysis (LPA) outcome class – main cohort.
LDL + HBC1 class | LDL + TB2 class | TDL + SBC3 class | HDL + TB4 class | ANOVA | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N = 20 | N = 23 | N = 33 | N = 55 | |||
Measure | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | M (SD) | Post‐hocs | |
Measures used to derive LPA outcome classes | ||||||
Mullen ELC a | 84.38 (11.96) | 96.57 (10.72) | 114.84 (8.70) | 130.19 (8.83) | F(113,3) = 119.17*** | 4 > 1, 2, 3; 3 > 1, 2; 2 > 1 |
Vineland ABC a | 81.52 (10.60) | 87.14 (8.27) | 94.86 (7.42) | 105.17 (7.58) | F(105,3) = 43.47*** | 4 > 1, 2, 3; 3 > 1, 2 |
ADOS‐2 CSS b | 3.11 (2.19) | 1.83 (1.19) | 2.19 (1.38) | 1.78 (1.30) | F(119,3) = 3.97** | 1 > 2, 4 |
SCQ c | 19.63 (7.50) | 3.48 (1.99) | 6.43 (3.76) | 2.25 (1.70) | F(109,3) = 100.62*** | 1 > 2, 3, 4; 3 > 2, 4 |
CBCL ADHD d | 65.24 (7.50) | 51.19 (2.66) | 55.48 (5.56) | 51.02 (2.85) | F(112,3) = 46.39*** | 1 > 2, 3, 4; 3 > 2, 4 |
Researcher ADHD c | 12.67 (2.69) | 12.33 (2.24) | 11.27 (1.71) | 10.96 (2.20) | F(104,3) = 3.50* | 1 > 4 |
CBCL anxiety d | 64.41 (10.08) | 50.95 (1.83) | 57.33 (7.55) | 50.27 (1.02) | F(112,3) = 35.24*** | 1 > 2, 3, 4; 3 > 2, 4 |
Other measures (not used in the LPA analysis) | ||||||
SRS d | 71.47 (13.79) | 45.75 (4.83) | 52.28 (8.73) | 42.17 (3.57) | F(103,3) = 63.66*** | 1 > 2, 3, 4; 3 > 2, 4 |
ADI‐social c | 12.37 (7.33) | 2.57 (3.16) | 2.81 (3.20) | 0.82 (1.01) | F(118,3) = 47.93*** | 1 > 2, 3, 4 |
ADI‐comm c | 8.74 (5.32) | 2.83 (3.65) | 2.48 (2.61) | 0.51 (1.00) | F(118,3) = 34.64*** | 1 > 2, 3, 4; 2, 3 > 4 |
ADI‐RRB c | 3.68 (2.31) | 0.52 (0.73) | 1.61 (1.86) | 0.24 (0.69) | F(118,3) = 29.85*** | 1 > 2, 3, 4; 3 > 2, 4 |
Note: 1, 2, 3, 4 – Class labels for ANOVA Tukey‐Kramer corrected post‐hocs.
Abbreviations: ABC, Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Composite; ADI, Autism Diagnostic Interview; ADOS‐2 CSS, ADOS‐2 Calibrated Severity Score; CBCL, Child Behaviour Checklist; Comm, Communication; ELC, Mullen Early Learning Composite; HDL + TB, High Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour; LDL + HBC, Low Developmental Level + High Behavioural Concerns; LDL + TB, Low Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour; RRB, Repetitive and Restricted Behaviours; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale; TDL + SBC, Typical Developmental Level + Some Behavioural Concerns.
astandard score.
bADOS‐2 Calibrated Severity Score.
craw score.
dT‐score.
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
FIGURE 1.
Profile on indicator variables of latent profile analysis (LPA) outcome classes. Y‐Axis scale is z‐score derived separately for each measure from the current sample so all measures are similarly scaled to provide a profile across the measures.
ANOVAs and Tukey‐Kramer post‐hocs for class differences are in line with class identification. The LDL + HBC class had elevated autism, ADHD and anxiety scores, low cognitive ability and adaptive function. The LDL + TB class had low autism, ADHD and anxiety scores, average cognitive ability but below average adaptive function. The TDL + SBC class had average cognitive ability and adaptive function but slightly elevated scores on the SCQ and the CBCL ADHD and anxiety subscales. The HDL + TB class had low autism, ADHD and anxiety scores, above average cognitive ability and average adaptive function. When considered in terms of the proportion of each class falling above or below clinical thresholds (>1SD for standardised scores and T‐scores; above the autism threshold on the ADOS‐2 and SCQ 2 ) the pattern was similar (Table 3). Thirteen children met diagnostic criteria for autism – 12 were identified in the LDL + HBC class and 1 in the TDL + SBC class. 3
TABLE 3.
Number and percentage of each latent profile analysis (LPA) class in atypical range on 36 months characterisation measures – main cohort.
LDL + HBC class | LDL + TB class | TDL + SBC class | HDL + TB class | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Measure | N = 20 | N = 23 | N = 33 | N = 55 |
Mullen ELC <85 | 7 (44%) | 3 (13%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) |
Vineland ABC <85 | 12 (71%) | 9 (41%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (2%) |
ADOS CSS ≥4 | 7 (39%) | 3 (13%) | 7 (23%) | 9 (18%) |
SCQ score ≥12 | 15 (88%) | 0 (0%) | 2 (8%) | 0 (0%) |
ADHD rating | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A |
CBCL ADHD T‐score ≥60 | 15 (88%) | 1 (5%) | 5 (20%) | 2 (4%) |
CBCL anxiety T‐score ≥60 | 12 (71%) | 0 (0%) | 10 (38%) | 0 (0%) |
Abbreviations: ABC, Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Composite; ADOS‐2 CSS, ADOS‐2 Calibrated Severity Score; CBCL, Child Behaviour Checklist; ELC, Mullen Early Learning Composite; HDL + TB, High Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour; LDL + HBC, Low Developmental Level + High Behavioural Concerns; LDL + TB, Low Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour; N/A, Not applicable; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; TDL + SBC, Typical Developmental Level + Some Behavioural Concerns.
Table 4 shows the association between the derived classes and the autism and ADHD FH sampling frame and sex. All children in the LDL + HBC and LDL + TB classes were from the FH groups. Children from the TDL + SBC and HDL + TB classes were from all the sampling groups. Boys were over‐represented in the LDL + HBC and LDL + TB classes but these differences were non‐significant (overall chi‐square p = 0.18).
TABLE 4.
Latent profile analysis (LPA) outcome class by familial likelihood group and sex – main cohort.
LDL + HBC class | LDL + TB class | TDL + SBC class | HDL + TB class | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N = 20 | N = 23 | N = 33 | N = 55 | |||
Familial likelihood group | ||||||
TL | N (%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) | 4 (18%) | 18 (82%) | 22 |
FH‐Autism | N (%) | 14 (21%) | 15 (22%) | 16 (24%) | 22 (33%) | 67 |
FH‐Autism + ADHD | N (%) | 5 (31%) | 2 (13%) | 6 (38%) | 3 (19%) | 16 |
FH‐ADHD | N (%) | 1 (4%) | 6 (23%) | 7 (27%) | 12 (46%) | 26 |
131 | ||||||
Sex | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | Total N | |
Male | 13 (65%) | 16 (70%) | 17 (52%) | 25 (45%) | 71 | |
Female | 7 (35%) | 7 (30%) | 16 (48%) | 30 (55%) | 60 | |
131 |
Abbreviations: LDL + HBC, Low Developmental Level + High Behavioural Concerns; LDL + TB, Low Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour; TDL + SBC, Typical Developmental Level + Some Behavioural Concerns; HDL + TB, High Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour.
We ran a second LPA using only measures of autism and ADHD (ADOS‐2 CSS, SCQ total score, researcher ADHD observation total score, and CBCL ADHD raw score). The three‐class solution provided the most robust and clinically meaningful distribution of classes, with a minimum class size comprising 17.0% (n = 20) of the sample and average MAP values for each class all ≥0.84 (Table S6). The identified classes were: High Behavioural Concerns (HBC; n = 20, 17%), Slight Behavioural Concerns (SBC; n = 72, 54%), and Typical Behaviour (TB; n = 37, 29%). These 3 classes are similar to those identified by the LPA with a broader set of indicator variables except that the class with LDL + TB was not identified, reflecting the fact that Mullen and Vineland were not indicators. The HBC class had elevated autism and ADHD scores on the measures used to derive the classes and included all 12 children 4 with an autism diagnosis. However, when examining scores on the other measures not used to derive the classes they also had elevated anxiety scores, low cognitive ability and low adaptive function (Table S7).
We repeated the broader LPA analysis in an independent cohort of n = 139 infants with a FH of autism and/or ADHD with outcome data at 3 years using the Mullen ELC, Vineland ABC, ADOS‐2 CSS, SCQ total score, and CBCL ADHD and anxiety subscale raw scores. 5 The four‐class solution provided the most robust and clinically meaningful distribution of classes, with a minimum class size comprising 17.0% (n = 20) of the sample and average MAP values for each class all ≥0.91 (Table S8). Based on the pattern across the measures we labelled the classes: Low Developmental Level + High Behavioural Concerns (LDL + HBC; n = 21, 15%), Typical Developmental Level + High Behavioural Concerns (TDL + HBC; n = 27, 19%), Low Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour (LDL + TB; n = 20, 14%), and High Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour (HDL + TB; n = 71, 51%) (See Appendix S2, Table S9, Table S10, Table S12 and Figure S1). Sixteen children met diagnostic criteria for autism: 10 of these were identified in the LDL + HBC class, 5 in the TDL + HBC class, and 1 in the LDL + TB class. Girls were over‐represented in the HDL + TB class (p < 0.01) and boys in the LDL + HBC (p < 0.01) and LDL + TB (p < 0.05) classes (Table S11).
We then repeated the broader LPA in the combined sample of both cohorts (n = 270). Similar LDL + HBC and HDL + TB classes to those described above emerged. A second atypical Low Developmental Level + Typical Behaviour (LDL + TB) class had slightly low cognitive ability and adaptive function but did not have elevated scores on autism, ADHD or anxiety measures. A Typical Developmental Level + Some Behavioural Concerns (TDL + SBC) class had slightly elevated scores on ADHD and anxiety measures and on the SCQ but not the ADOS, high average cognitive ability and average adaptive function. These patterns were reflected both in terms of group means (Table S13, Figure S2) and the proportion of the class scoring above or below the 1SD and clinical thresholds (Table S14; Appendix S3, correlations shown in Table S16). Twenty eight children met diagnostic criteria for autism: 19 in the LDL + HBC class, 8 in the TDL + SBC class, and 1 in the LDL + TB class. Girls were over‐represented in the HDL + TB class (p < 0.01) and boys in the LDL + HBC (p < 0.05) and LDL + TB (p < 0.05) classes (Table S15).
DISCUSSION
We used a data‐driven approach to identify outcome classes across measures of cognitive ability and adaptive functioning, and autism, ADHD and anxiety traits, at 3 years in infants with a FH of autism or ADHD, or both. We identified a class with elevated autism, ADHD and anxiety scores, low cognitive ability and adaptive function, which included most children with an autism diagnosis. A second atypical class had low autism, ADHD and anxiety scores, average cognitive ability but below average adaptive functioning; and a third atypical class had average cognitive ability and adaptive function but slightly elevated behavioural scores. A typically developing class with above average cognitive ability was also identified.
In an independent cohort, we also identified three atypical classes with overlapping but slightly different profiles: two with high behaviour scores, one with low cognitive ability and one average cognitive ability, and a third class with low cognitive ability but typical behaviour, although this class had elevated ADOS scores most likely reflecting their developmental delay. Boys were over‐represented in the atypical outcome classes compared to girls and whilst this did not reach significance in our primary analysis it did in the independent cohort and both cohorts combined. When an LPA was conducted in the combined sample, one typically developing class with above average cognitive ability was found. Atypical classes comprised one with LDL + HBC that included most children with autism; another with slightly low cognitive ability and adaptive behaviour; and a third atypical classes with typical cognitive ability and slightly elevated ADHD, anxiety and autism scores. Across all analyses no outcome class with elevated autism or ADHD traits in isolation was found.
When only measures of autism and ADHD were used as class indicators we identified a similar (largely overlapping) class that had elevated autism and ADHD behaviours but also elevated anxiety and below average cognitive ability and adaptive function on measures that were not used to derive the LPA classes. Even when considering this class only in terms of the autism and ADHD indicator variables used in the LPA they had both elevated autism and ADHD scores. One other class had very slightly elevated scores on the parent‐report SCQ and CBCL‐ADHD subscale and another class typical behaviour. This pattern differs from Miller et al. (2020) who used a similar data‐driven LPA approach to characterise outcome classes at 3 years in infants with a FH of autism or ADHD. Miller et al. identified an autism class, which also had elevated ADHD scores and low cognitive ability (p.1327 and Table S2, Miller et al., 2020), an ADHD class who had slightly but not clinically elevated autism scores on the SCQ and the ADOS‐2 and average cognitive ability, and a typically developing class. 6 We did not identify a class with elevated ADHD scores only, even in the LPA model using only autism and ADHD measures to derive classes. There are several differences between the studies that may in part explain these discrepancies. In terms of recruitment, Miller et al. (2020) created separate ADHD and autism FH groups and infants with a FH of both autism and ADHD were allocated to the autism FH group, whereas we treated FH of autism and ADHD alone and in combination independently. Whilst both studies used the ADOS and the SCQ as autism indicator variables in their LPA, Miller et al. used different researcher‐ and parent‐rated measures of ADHD to the current study.
Approaches to characterising outcomes in infants with a family history of autism and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
Although we found moderately high recurrence rates of autism in our samples (10% in the primary sample, 12% in the independent sample), autism in isolation is not the primary atypical outcome in early childhood of infants with an autism FH (with or without an ADHD FH). This is neither a new nor a surprising finding. In clinical samples both autism and ADHD commonly co‐occur with other neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric conditions. Studies of older siblings and family members show not only that autism and ADHD commonly co‐occur (Miller, Musser, et al., 2019; Musser et al., 2014) but also that in siblings of children with autism and ADHD other neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric conditions including anxiety, conduct disorder, intellectual disability and language delay are also common (Jokiranta‐Olkoniemi et al., 2016, 2019). This is consistent with the atypical outcome classes we identified in all four analyses conducted where a combination of either, or both, broader atypical behaviour and low cognitive and/or adaptive function was seen across the classes. In studies of autism FH infants many of those who have autism at 3 years have low cognitive ability and adaptive behaviour (Messinger et al., 2015; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2021). Consistent with previous studies, both in infants with autism and those without (Messinger et al., 2013, 2015), we found boys to be over‐represented in the classes with lower developmental level and higher behavioural concerns.
In infants with an autism FH who do not have autism ∼20% are characterised by elevated but sub‐clinical autism traits, low cognitive, language and adaptive ability (in combination sometimes referred to as the ‘broader autism phenotype’ or ‘other developmental concerns’), and also elevated rates of emotional and behavioural problems (Charman et al., 2017; Marrus et al., 2018; Messinger et al., 2013; Miller, Iosif, et al., 2019; Ozonoff et al., 2014). In most previous autism FH infant studies, including ours, the group of children who meet diagnostic criteria for autism at 3 years are labelled the ‘autism outcome’ group. Clearly a group of children who do meet the diagnostic criteria for autism exist, and without exception children with an autism diagnosis were in the atypical LPA classes identified here. However, the use of the singular ‘autism’ term to describe this group may inadvertently overshadow their broader profile of developmental and behavioural atypicality, as well as the fact that they share many characteristics with children who may fall just below the clinical threshold for a diagnosis.
We chose to name the outcome classes identified in terms of developmental level (cognitive ability and adaptive function) and behaviour (autism, ADHD and anxiety traits) – namely the indicator variables from which they were derived. Since our focus was on broader transdiagnostic characterisation of developmental and behavioural outcomes in the cohorts at 3 years and not categorical diagnosis we have described the developmental outcomes as ‘low’, ‘typical’ and ‘high’ developmental level and the behavioural outcomes as ‘typical behaviour’ and ‘some’ or ‘high’ ‘behavioural concerns’. These do not equate to clinical classifications and we did not apply a strict threshold for their application – in part because across the LPAs we ran classes often had slightly different profiles of scores across the individual measures. We have used terminology and concepts from the developmental psychopathology/child psychiatry field to describe the outcome classes since ‘developmental level’ and ‘behavioural concerns’ map directly onto the indicator measures used to form LPA classes (Mullen and Vineland, and ADOS, SCQ and CBCL, respectively) and align with both previous literature and conventional clinical usage. However, we acknowledge the changing conceptual and linguistic landscape in the field and the outcome groups could alternatively be labelled as showing moderate or high ‘neurodivergent’ development (Dwyer, 2022; Sonuga‐Barke & Thapar, 2021).
However, inspection of both the mean scores and the proportions of each identified class falling above or below the clinical cut‐points on screening and diagnostic measures, or −/+ 1SD on standardised measures, indicate that the atypical classes identified have partially distinct but also partially overlapping profiles. This approach is similar to that adopted in previous studies (e.g. Charman et al., 2017; Messinger et al., 2013; Miller et al., 2020). Children with elevated autism, ADHD and anxiety traits at this age may not meet the threshold for a clinical condition nor necessarily require intervention or support. Notwithstanding this, across the current analyses between 50% and 60% of the infants with a FH of autism and/or ADHD were identified in one of the atypical classes. Given the FH nature of the cohorts and the fact that here we characterise outcomes only up to age 3 years it will be important to study and monitor these children as they grow older.
Implications for the study of family history infants
The findings of the present analysis go beyond nomenclature and should influence the motivation, design, analysis and interpretation of future FH studies. For example, the broader transdiagnostic pattern of atypical neurodevelopmental outcomes we have identified has important implications for studies that aim to identify early biomarkers and endophenotypes of autism (Johnson et al., 2015; Jones et al., 2014; Szatmari et al., 2016; Wolff & Piven, 2021). Although many infant signs and biomarkers have been mooted (and some patented) as predicting later autism, if the ‘natural categories’ of early outcome in infants with a FH are broader – both across autism versus other behavioural phenotypes (here, ADHD and anxiety) and across behavioural versus developmental outcomes (here, cognitive ability and adaptive behaviour) – then there needs to be a re‐calibration of aims and methods. These cautions also apply to mechanistic interpretations where infant experimental signals are taken to be precursors of later social communication, repetitive or sensory behaviours, without addressing the extent to which they may be confounded by cognitive ability, adaptive function or later emerging ADHD or anxiety traits. This may motivate research groups, including ours, to re‐analyse previously published findings to test the specificity of prediction to autism versus other atypical neurodevelopmental outcomes.
Other important future goals include examining whether there is greater differentiation of the developmental and behavioural phenotypic pattern we have observed at age 3 years as children are followed to mid‐childhood (e.g. Shephard et al., 2017) and exploring the age and the indicators in the infancy period that might identify children who will later develop this presentation (e.g. Kostyrka‐Allchorne et al., 2020).
Strengths and limitations
The study has several strengths. In a moderate size sample of infants with an autism and/or ADHD FH followed to age 3 years we have used a data‐driven approach to derive outcomes employing a broad range of developmental and behavioural measures rather than only autism symptoms. We replicated our approach in an independent sample and in both samples combined. However, this latter sample was recruited based on an established FH of autism and while information on FH of ADHD was collected this was not an integral part of the recruitment. Limitations include that we only studied outcomes through to 3 years. The characterisation of ADHD, anxiety and other neurodevelopmental outcomes – as well as autism itself (Ozonoff et al., 2018) – is incomplete at this age (Rocco et al., 2021) and further work is needed to develop and validate accurate and reliable measures of different forms of emerging developmental psychopathology in the early years. We have previously followed autism FH infants to mid‐childhood and found elevated levels of ADHD and anxiety traits both in those children with an autism diagnosis and those without (Shephard et al., 2017). We did not obtain sufficient information at 3 years to ascertain ADHD diagnoses. A number of measures employed in the current study are parent‐report and suffer from potential shared methods and reporting bias limitations. Examination of the regressions of LPA indicator variables onto the LPA class solution shows that these measures (and also the direct Mullen assessment) were most strongly correlated with the identified classes, and that whilst the observational measures (ADOS and researcher ADHD ratings) contributed significantly the correlations were notably weaker. Another, related limitation of reliance on parental report questionnaires is the ability both of parents and the questionnaires themselves to distinguish between specific behaviours that may be manifestations of different emerging difficulties (see Hus et al., 2013). Finally, our researcher observational measure of early ADHD behaviours was not a standard measure, observers were not blind to FH, and whilst we conducted researcher consensus ratings no inter‐rater reliability data was collected.
Conclusions
Family history, twin and genetic studies suggest that family liability is not specific to autism or to ADHD. There is overlap in early genetic liabilities related to the later expression of both phenotypes (Constantino et al., 2021), and autism and ADHD (and other early neurodevelopmental phenotypes) are underpinned by partially overlapping and partially separate developmental processes (Johnson et al., 2015, 2021; Miller et al., 2020). Much progress has been made in understanding the early development of autism, including from infant FH studies, and recent progress is apparent in the early ADHD field (Miller et al., 2023). However, if as we demonstrate here, the ‘natural categories’ of atypical outcome of infants with a FH of autism and/or ADHD are not the autism and ADHD phenotypes in isolation, but broader atypical developmental and behavioural outcomes, then the field needs to embrace this challenge. This concept is similar to that identified previously by Gillberg (2010) as ‘ESSENCE’ (Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmental Clinical Examinations). Gillberg argued that early manifestations of neurodevelopmental conditions (seen in clinic) are commonly non‐specific, affecting motor, cognitive, communicative and social development, as well as sleep, feeding and behavioural regulation.
Research on developmental models and approaches to clinical translation should adopt a transdiagnostic approach in developmental studies of early autism and ADHD. Initial suggestions include the need to explicitly test the ‘phenotype‐specificity’ of early infant neurodevelopmental markers of later outcomes; and the development of pre‐emptive interventions that promote broader developmental competencies and outcomes, rather than targeting emergent symptoms of specific neurodevelopmental conditions (Constantino et al., 2021; Manzini et al., 2021). One critical question is whether at this age atypical development is truly transdiagnostic, akin to the ESSENCE concept, and may only differentiate later in development; or whether developmental ability, autism, ADHD and anxiety are separable but simply cluster or co‐occur (in this cohort, with the current measures) at this developmental timepoint. Experimental measures that assay underlying neuroendophenotypic processes, continued study of these cohorts as they age, and item‐level network analysis approaches to measurement, may all in future studies help answer these questions.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
TC, MHJ, AP and EJHJ obtained funding and designed the study. AH, TB, NN, AG, HH, MA, RH, and ME were involved in conducting the study and/or data collection or analysis. TC and AP conducted the data analysis The article was drafted by TC and all authors read, made revisions and approved the final version.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
TC has served as a paid consultant to F. Hoffmann‐La Roche Ltd. and Servier; and has received royalties from Sage Publications and Guilford Publications. AP receives royalties from Western Psychological Services, Imperial College Press, and OUP. MJ receives royalties from Wiley‐Blackwell, OUP and MIT Press. EJ is a Joint Editor on JCPP Advances. The remaining authors have declared that they have no competing or potential conflicts of interest.
ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS
Ethical approval was granted by the National Research Ethics Service (13/LO/0751 and 08/H0718/76) and the Research Ethics Committee, Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London.
Supporting information
Supplementary Material
Figure S1
Figure S2
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors are grateful to all the families who took part in this study. The STAARS team consists of: Leila Dafner, Laurel Fish, Teodora Gliga, Rianne Haartsen, Sarah Kalwarowsky, Anna Kolesnik, Sarah Lloyd‐Fox, Luke Mason, Laura Pirazzoli, Chloë Taylor. We thank the researchers who assisted with recruitment and data collection of the independent cohort: Anna Blasi, Kim Davies, Mayada Elsabbagh, Janice Fernandes, Isobel Gammer, Jeanne Guiraud, Michelle Liew, Helen Maris, Louise O’Hara, Helena Ribeiro, Erica Salomone, Leslie Tucker. For the purposes of open access, the author has applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence to any Accepted Author Manuscript version arising from this submission.
This research was supported by awards from the Medical Research Council (G0701484, MR/K021389/1, MR/T003057/1), the BASIS funding consortium led by Autistica, MQ (MQ14PP_83), EU‐AIMS and AIMS‐2‐TRIALS funded by the Innovative Medicines Initiative (IMI) Joint Undertaking Grant Nos. 115300 and No. 777394. This Joint Undertaking receives support from the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme, with in‐kind contributions from the European Federation of Pharmaceutical Industries and Associations (EFPIA) companies and funding from Autism Speaks, Autistica and SFARI. Capital equipment funding from the Maudsley Charity (980) and Guy's and St Thomas' Charity (STR130505). AP acknowledges support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) through NF‐SI‐0617‐10120 and the NIHR Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London. AH is supported by an NIHR Advanced Fellowship (NIHR300880). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.
Charman, T. , Pasco, G. , Hendry, A. , Bazelmans, T. , Narvekar, N. , Goodwin, A. , Halkola, H. , Agyapong, M. , Holman, R. , Ali, J. B. , Ersoy, M. , Johnson, M. H. , Pickles, A. , & Jones, E. J. H. (2023). Three year outcomes in infants with a family history of autism and/or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. JCPP Advances, 3(4), e12189. 10.1002/jcv2.12189
STAARS Team authors listed in Acknowledgements.
Order of authors Alexandra Hendry, Tessel Bazelmans, Nisha Narvekar, Amy Goodwin, Hanna Halkola, Mary Agyapong, Rebecca Holman, Jannath Begum Ali and Mutluhan Ersoy are randomly generated.
ENDNOTES
Table 2 shows T‐scores for the CBCL measures for descriptive purposes but raw scores were entered into the LPA analysis.
We used the ≥12 threshold on the SCQ for 3 year olds (Corsello et al., 2007).
We note that only 48% of children with elevated ADHD scores and 36% of children with elevated anxiety scores had an autism diagnosis.
One fewer child with autism had at least one of more restrictive list of outcome class indicators.
The researcher ADHD observation measure was not completed in this cohort.
We are very grateful to Dr. Meghan Miller for generously sharing unpublished data from Miller et al. (2020).
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
Data available following a review of requests as indicated here: https://www.basisnetwork.org/collaboration‐and‐project‐affiliation/.
REFERENCES
- Achenbach, T. M. , & Rescorla, L. A. (2000). Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms and profiles. University of Vermont. [Google Scholar]
- Astle, D. E. , Holmes, J. , Kievit, R. , & Gathercole, S. E. (2022). Annual Research Review: The transdiagnostic revolution in neurodevelopmental disorders. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 63(4), 397–417. 10.1111/jcpp.13481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charman, T. , Young, G. S. , Brian, J. , Carter, A. , Carver, L. J. , Chawarska, K. , Curtin, S. , Dobkins, K. , Elsabbagh, M. , Georgiades, S. , Hertz‐Picciotto, I. , Hutman, T. , Iverson, J. M. , Jones, E. J. , Landa, R. , Macari, S. , Messinger, D. S. , Nelson, C. A. , Ozonoff, S. , & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2017). Non‐ASD outcomes at 36 months in siblings at familial risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A baby siblings research consortium (BSRC) study. Autism Research, 10(1), 169–178. 10.1002/aur.1669 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantino, J. N. , Charman, T. , & Jones, E. J. H. (2021). Clinical and translational implications of an emerging developmental substructure for autism. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 17(1), 365–389. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-110503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Constantino, J. N. , & Gruber, C. P. (2012). Social responsiveness scale: SRS‐2. Western psychological services Torrance. [Google Scholar]
- Corsello, C. , Hus, V. , Pickles, A. , Risi, S. , Cook, E. H. , Leventhal, B. L. , & Lord, C. (2007). Between a ROC and a hard place: Decision making and making decisions about using the SCQ. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(9), 932–940. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01762.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dwyer, P. (2022). The neurodiversity approach(es): What are they and what do they mean for researchers? Human Development, 66(2), 73–92. 10.1159/000523723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghirardi, L. , Pettersson, E. , Taylor, M. J. , Freitag, C. M. , Franke, B. , Asherson, P. , Larsson, H. , & Kuja‐Halkola, R. (2019). Genetic and environmental contribution to the overlap between ADHD and ASD trait dimensions in young adults: A twin study. Psychological Medicine, 49(10), 1713–1721. 10.1017/S003329171800243X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillberg, C. (2010). The ESSENCE in child psychiatry: Early symptomatic Syndromes eliciting neurodevelopmental clinical examinations. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(6), 1543–1551. 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hansen, S. N. , Schendel, D. E. , Francis, R. W. , Windham, G. C. , Bresnahan, M. , Levine, S. Z. , Reichenberg, A. , Gissler, M. , Kodesh, A. , Bai, D. , Yip, B. H. K. , Leonard, H. , Sandin, S. , Buxbaum, J. D. , Hultman, C. , Sourander, A. , Glasson, E. J. , Wong, K. , Öberg, R. , & Parner, E. T. (2019). Recurrence risk of autism in siblings and cousins: A multinational, population‐based study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(9), 866–875. 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.11.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henson, J. M. , Reise, S. P. , & Kim, K. H. (2007). Detecting mixtures from structural model differences using latent variable mixture modeling: A comparison of relative model fit statistics. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(2), 202–226. 10.1080/10705510709336744 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hollingdale, J. , Woodhouse, E. , Young, S. , Fridman, A. , & Mandy, W. (2020). Autistic spectrum disorder symptoms in children and adolescents with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta‐analytical review. Psychological Medicine, 50(13), 2240–2253. 10.1017/S0033291719002368 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hus, V. , Bishop, S. , Gotham, K. , Huerta, M. , & Lord, C. (2013). Factors influencing scores on the social responsiveness scale. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54(2), 216–224. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02589.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M. H. , Charman, T. , Pickles, A. , & Jones, E. J. H. (2021). Annual research review: Anterior modifiers in the emergence of neurodevelopmental disorders (AMEND)‐a systems neuroscience approach to common developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(5), 610–630. 10.1111/jcpp.13372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, M. H. , Gliga, T. , Jones, E. , & Charman, T. (2015). Annual Research Review: Infant development, autism, and ADHD – early pathways to emerging disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(3), 228–247. 10.1111/jcpp.12328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jokiranta‐Olkoniemi, E. , Cheslack‐Postava, K. , Joelsson, P. , Suominen, A. , Brown, A. S. , & Sourander, A. (2019). Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and risk for psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders in siblings. Psychological Medicine, 49(1), 84–91. 10.1017/S0033291718000521 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jokiranta‐Olkoniemi, E. , Cheslack‐Postava, K. , Sucksdorff, D. , Suominen, A. , Gyllenberg, D. , Chudal, R. , Leivonen, S. , Gissler, M. , Brown, A. S. , & Sourander, A. (2016). Risk of psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders among siblings of probands with autism spectrum disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(6), 622–629. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones, E. J. H. , Gliga, T. , Bedford, R. , Charman, T. , & Johnson, M. H. (2014). Developmental pathways to autism: A review of prospective studies of infants at risk. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 1–33. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirk, R. E. (1998). Experimental design: Procedures for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Brooks/Cole Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Kostyrka‐Allchorne, K. , Wass, S. V. , & Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. S. (2020). Research review: Do parent ratings of infant negative emotionality and self‐regulation predict psychopathology in childhood and adolescence? A systematic review and meta‐analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 61(4), 401–416. 10.1111/jcpp.13144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord, C. , Rutter, M. , DiLavore, P. , Risi, S. , Gotham, K. , & Bishop, S. (2012). ADOS‐2) autism diagnostic observation schedule (2nd ed.). (ADOS‐2) Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. [Google Scholar]
- Lord, C. , Rutter, M. , & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview‐revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685. 10.1007/bf02172145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maenner, M. J. , Shaw, K. A. , Baio, J. , EdS1Washington, A. , Patrick, M. , DiRienzo, M. , Christensen, D. L. , Wiggins, L. D. , Pettygrove, S. , Andrews, J. G. , Lopez, M. , Hudson, A. , Baroud, T. , Schwenk, Y. , White, T. , Rosenberg, C. R. , Lee, L.‐C. , Harrington, R. A. , Huston, M. , & Dietz, P. M. (2020). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 Years—Autism and developmental Disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2016. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report ‐ Surveillance Summaries, 69(4), 1–12. 10.15585/mmwr.ss6904a1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manzini, A. , Jones, E. J. H. , Charman, T. , Elsabbagh, M. , Johnson, M. H. , & Singh, I. (2021). Ethical dimensions of translational developmental neuroscience research in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(11), 1363–1373. 10.1111/jcpp.13494 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marrus, N. , Hall, L. P. , Paterson, S. J. , Elison, J. T. , Wolff, J. J. , Swanson, M. R. , Parish‐Morris, J. , Eggebrecht, A. T. , Pruett, J. R. , Hazlett, H. C. , Zwaigenbaum, L. , Dager, S. , Estes, A. M. , Schultz, R. T. , Botteron, K. N. , Piven, J. , & Constantino, J. N. , & IBIS Network . (2018). Language delay aggregates in toddler siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 10(1), 29. 10.1186/s11689-018-9247-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Messinger, D. , Young, G. S. , Ozonoff, S. , Dobkins, K. , Carter, A. , Zwaigenbaum, L. , Landa, R. J. , Charman, T. , Stone, W. L. , Constantino, J. N. , Hutman, T. , Carver, L. J. , Bryson, S. , Iverson, J. M. , Strauss, M. S. , Rogers, S. J. , & Sigman, M. (2013). Beyond autism: A baby siblings research consortium study of high‐risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 300–308.e1. 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.12.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Messinger, D. S. , Young, G. S. , Webb, S. J. , Ozonoff, S. , Bryson, S. E. , Carter, A. , Carver, L. , Charman, T. , Chawarska, K. , Curtin, S. , Dobkins, K. , Hertz‐Picciotto, I. , Hutman, T. , Iverson, J. M. , Landa, R. , Nelson, C. A. , Stone, W. L. , Tager‐Flusberg, H. , & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2015). Early sex differences are not autism‐specific: A baby siblings research consortium (BSRC) study. Molecular Autism, 6(1), 32. 10.1186/s13229-015-0027-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, M. , Arnett, A. B. , Shephard, E. , Charman, T. , Gustafsson, H. C. , Joseph, H. M. , Karalunas, S. , Nigg, J. T. , Polanczyk, G. V. , Sullivan, E. L. , & Jones, E. J. H. (2023). Delineating early developmental pathways to ADHD: Setting an international research agenda. JCPP Advances, 3(2). 10.1002/jcv2.12144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, M. , Austin, S. , Iosif, A.‐M. , de la Paz, L. , Chuang, A. , Hatch, B. , & Ozonoff, S. (2020). Shared and distinct developmental pathways to ASD and ADHD phenotypes among infants at familial risk. Development and Psychopathology, 32(4), 1323–1334. 10.1017/S0954579420000735 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, M. , Iosif, A.‐M. , Young, G. S. , Bell, L. J. , Schwichtenberg, A. J. , Hutman, T. , & Ozonoff, S. (2019). The dysregulation profile in preschoolers with and without a family history of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(5), 516–523. 10.1111/jcpp.13003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller, M. , Musser, E. D. , Young, G. S. , Olson, B. , Steiner, R. D. , & Nigg, J. T. (2019). Sibling recurrence risk and cross‐aggregation of attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Pediatrics, 173(2), 147–152. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.4076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. American Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- Musser, E. D. , Hawkey, E. , Kachan‐Liu, S. S. , Lees, P. , Roullet, J.‐B. , Goddard, K. , Steiner, R. D. , & Nigg, J. T. (2014). Shared familial transmission of autism spectrum and attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55(7), 819–827. 10.1111/jcpp.12201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nylund, K. L. , Asparouhov, T. , & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(4), 535–569. 10.1080/10705510701575396 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ozonoff, S. , Young, G. S. , Belding, A. , Hill, M. , Hill, A. , Hutman, T. , Johnson, S. , Miller, M. , Rogers, S. J. , Schwichtenberg, A. J. , Steinfeld, M. , & Iosif, A.‐M. (2014). The broader autism phenotype in infancy: When does it emerge? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(4), 398–407.e2. 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozonoff, S. , Young, G. S. , Brian, J. , Charman, T. , Shephard, E. , Solish, A. , & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2018). Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder after age 5 in children evaluated longitudinally since infancy. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(11), 849–857.e2. 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozonoff, S. , Young, G. S. , Carter, A. , Messinger, D. , Yirmiya, N. , Zwaigenbaum, L. , Bryson, S. , Carver, L. J. , Constantino, J. N. , Dobkins, K. , Hutman, T. , Iverson, J. M. , Landa, R. , Rogers, S. J. , Sigman, M. , & Stone, W. L. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A baby siblings research consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495. 10.1542/peds.2010-2825 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polanczyk, G. V. , Salum, G. A. , Sugaya, L. S. , Caye, A. , & Rohde, L. A. (2015). Annual research review: A meta‐analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(3), 345–365. 10.1111/jcpp.12381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reetzke, R. , Iosif, A.‐M. , Hatch, B. , de la Paz, L. , Chuang, A. , Ozonoff, S. , & Miller, M. (2021). Patterns of objectively measured motor activity among infants developing ASD and concerns for ADHD. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 63(6), 663–673. 10.1111/jcpp.13504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rocco, I. , Corso, B. , Bonati, M. , & Minicuci, N. (2021). Time of onset and/or diagnosis of ADHD in European children: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 21(1), 575. 10.1186/s12888-021-03547-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter, M. , Bailey, A. , & Lord, C. (2003). The social communication questionnaire. Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- Shephard, E. , Milosavljevic, B. , Pasco, G. , Jones, E. , Gliga, T. , Happe, F. , Johnson, M. , & Charman, T. , & BASIS Team . (2017). Mid‐childhood outcomes of infant siblings at familial high‐risk of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 10(3), 546–557. 10.1002/aur.1733 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sonuga‐Barke, E. , & Thapar, A. (2021). The neurodiversity concept: Is it helpful for clinicians and scientists? The Lancet Psychiatry, 8(7), 559–561. 10.1016/S2215-0366(21)00167-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow, S. , Cicchetti, D. , & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales second edition survey forms manual. America Guidance Service. [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp (2019). Stata statistical software: Release 16. StataCorp LLC. [Google Scholar]
- Szatmari, P. , Chawarska, K. , Dawson, G. , Georgiades, S. , Landa, R. , Lord, C. , Messinger, D. S. , Thurm, A. , & Halladay, A. (2016). Prospective longitudinal studies of infant siblings of children with autism: Lessons learned and future directions. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(3), 179–187. 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talbott, M. R. , & Miller, M. R. (2020). Future directions for infant identification and intervention for autism spectrum disorder from a transdiagnostic perspective. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 53(5), 1–15. 10.1080/15374416.2020.1790382 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor, M. J. , Charman, T. , Robinson, E. B. , Plomin, R. , Happé, F. , Asherson, P. , & Ronald, A. (2013). Developmental associations between traits of autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A genetically informative, longitudinal twin study. Psychological Medicine, 43(8), 1735–1746. 10.1017/S003329171200253X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar, A. , & Cooper, M. (2016). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet, 387(10024), 1240–1250. 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00238-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thapar, A. , Cooper, M. , & Rutter, M. (2017). Neurodevelopmental disorders. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(4), 339–346. 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30376-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tick, B. , Bolton, P. , Happé, F. , Rutter, M. , & Rijsdijk, F. (2016). Heritability of autism spectrum disorders: A meta‐analysis of twin studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(5), 585–595. 10.1111/jcpp.12499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolff, J. J. , & Piven, J. (2021). Predicting autism in infancy. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(8), 958–967. 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.07.910 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zwaigenbaum, L. , Brian, J. , Smith, I. M. , Sacrey, L.‐A. R. , Franchini, M. , Bryson, S. E. , Vaillancourt, T. , Armstrong, V. , Duku, E. , Szatmari, P. , Roberts, W. , & Roncadin, C. (2021). Symptom trajectories in the first 18 months and autism risk in a prospective high‐risk cohort. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(12), 1435–1443. 10.1111/jcpp.13417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplementary Material
Figure S1
Figure S2
Data Availability Statement
Data available following a review of requests as indicated here: https://www.basisnetwork.org/collaboration‐and‐project‐affiliation/.