Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Dec 13;18(12):e0294296. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294296

The role of playgrounds in the development of children’s fundamental movement skills: A scoping review

Charlotte Skau Pawlowski 1,2,*, Cathrine Damsbo Madsen 1,2, Mette Toftager 1,2, Thea Toft Amholt 3, Jasper Schipperijn 1,2
Editor: Renato S Melo4
PMCID: PMC10718446  PMID: 38091275

Abstract

Fundamental movement skills (FMS) are the basic skills children should develop but are low in children from high-income countries. Literature indicates that playgrounds can play an important role challenging children’s balance, agility, and coordination. However, knowledge on the influence of playgrounds on children’s FMS development is fragmented. The aim of the present scoping review was to create an overview of all research that is relevant when studying the influence of unstructured playground play on children’s FMS. Four electronic databases (Scopus, Web of Science, SportDiscus, and PsycInfo) were searched systematically in May 2022 and October 2023 following the PRISMA guidelines, leading to a final set of 14 publications meeting the inclusion criteria. The results of these publications indicate that it is important to design playgrounds with various features targeting balance, climbing, throwing, and catching to provide opportunities for children to enhance each FMS (i.e., stability, locomotor skills, and object control skills). Also, spreading features over a large area of the playground seems to ensure ample space per child, stimulate children to use locomotor skills by moving to and from features, and to play active games without equipment. Possibly, also natural play settings develop children’s FMS. These findings, however, should be read with caution. More experimental studies using objective and standardized FMS tests are needed in this research field for a more robust conclusion.

Introduction

Fundamental movement skills (FMS) are the basic skills children should be competent at, such as stability skills (e.g., sitting, standing, balancing), locomotor skills (e.g., running, jumping, climbing), and object control skills (e.g., throwing, catching) [1]. Preschool years are crucial in terms of developing various FMS [2]. Developing proficiency in these skills is important, as FMS provide an underlying base for successful participation in physical activities across the life course [1,3,4]. Despite the importance of FMS for physical activity, FMS have previously been shown to be low in preschool and school aged children from high-income countries [5]. A recent systematic review showed that children (3–10 years old) in 25 countries were not achieving the FMS competence required to successfully participate in physical activity [6]. The ability to perform and master different types of FMS has also been associated with school readiness and performance [7,8], social interaction with peers [9], and self-perception [10].

To increase FMS, many interventions have been developed in various contexts [1113], but few FMS interventions have been implemented at scale [14]. Adult-directed, structured FMS programs are considered effective in developing children’s FMS [15]. However, structured programs require educated staff and thus are expensive to conduct. Further, specific training only affects the development of the specific task trained and not necessarily other tasks related to the same FMS competence [16].

Literature indicates that unstructured play at playgrounds is important for children’s physical development, challenging their movement abilities such as balance, agility, coordination, and spatial awareness [17,18]. The World Health Organization claims that active play and opportunities for unstructured physical activity can contribute to the development of motor skills in children under five years [19]. However, playgrounds can be designed very differently, vary in size, features, and be built in conventional or natural materials. Further, studies addressing the influence of playgrounds on children’s FMS development are carried out in different research fields with varying study designs, methods, and outcomes. A systematic review of active play interventions aimed at promoting physical activity and FMS conducted in 2016 only included four studies [20], and could not draw firm conclusion due to the small number of eligible studies and their heterogeneity. Thus, knowledge on the role of playgrounds in children’s FMS development is fragmented and a coherent overview of relevant research in this field is needed to help guide future FMS interventions [21,22].

The aim of the present scoping review was to create a global, interdisciplinary overview of relevant research investigating the influence of unstructured playground play on children’s FMS. More specifically we investigated the impact of playground size, playground setting, and playground features, respectively, on children’s FMS. We did this investigation to be able to identify and outline existing knowledge in this research field and hereby identify knowledge gaps and set the agenda for future research.

Materials and methods

The scoping review was conducted in accordance with the JBI methodology for scoping reviews [23] and the PRISMA guidelines were followed [24]. The review protocol was registered at Open Science Framework (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/UYN2V) in May 2022.

The current scoping review is part of a broader project synthesizing evidence on the relationship between unstructured playground play and physical, mental, and social health among children and adolescents. Findings for other health outcomes than FMS when using playgrounds will be presented in separate articles.

Search strategy

Four electronic databases, Scopus, Web of Science, SportDiscus, and PsycInfo, were searched systematically in May 2022. Search terms were tested and revised by all authors (having expertise in the research field) in collaboration with a research librarian from the University of Southern Denmark. It was decided to create a comprehensive search strategy with two search blocks to obtain all relevant research on the topic. One search block contained all identified synonyms for ‘playground’, the other contained all identified synonyms for ‘children’.

To be fully updated on the literature when writing this article, we did a second search in the same four databases in October 2023. We search literature published in 2022–2023. This time we added one more search block to the two existing search blocks to specify our search around FMS as the health outcome investigated. The third search block contained all identified synonyms for ‘fundamental movement skills’. In Table 1, the search terms for all three search blocks are shown for Scopus. The search terms were slightly adapted to fit each database.

Table 1. Search terms for Scopus.

(TITLE (playground*)) OR (((TITLE-ABS (schoolyard* OR "school ground*") OR AUTHKEY (schoolyard* OR "school ground*")) OR (TITLE-ABS (play W/3 (area* OR space* OR environment* OR field* OR natural OR nature OR outdoor OR place* OR structure* OR equipment OR park*)) OR AUTHKEY (play W/3 (area* OR space* OR environment* OR field* OR natural OR nature OR outdoor OR place* OR structure* OR equipment OR park*))) OR (TITLE-ABS ((school* OR daycare* OR "day care" OR childcare OR "child care" OR kindergarten*) W/6 (play OR playable OR played OR playing OR "physical* activ*" OR "organi?ed activit*" OR "unorgani?ed activit*" OR "structured activit*" OR "unstructured activit*" OR "recreation* activit*" OR "leisure activit*" OR "outdoor activit*" OR "vigorous activit*")) OR AUTHKEY ((school* OR daycare* OR "day care" OR childcare OR "child care" OR kindergarten*) W/6 (play OR playable OR played OR playing OR "physical* activ*" OR "organi?ed activit*" OR "unorgani?ed activit*" OR "structured activit*" OR "unstructured activit*" OR "recreation* activit*" OR "leisure activit*" OR "outdoor activit*" OR "vigorous activit*"))) OR (TITLE-ABS (playfield* OR playplace* OR playscape* OR playspace* OR "public open space") OR AUTHKEY (playfield* OR playplace* OR playscape* OR playspace* OR "public open space")) OR (INDEXTERMS (playground)) OR (ABS (playground*) OR AUTHKEY (playground*))) AND ((TITLE-ABS (adolescen* OR baby OR boy OR schoolboy* OR boyhood OR girlhood OR child* OR schoolchild* OR girl OR schoolgirl* OR infan* OR juvenil* OR kid OR minor OR newborn* OR new-born* OR paediatric* OR pediatric* OR preschool* OR puber* OR pubescen* OR teen* OR tween* OR toddler* OR youth* OR student* OR schoolage*) OR AUTHKEY (adolescen* OR baby OR boy OR schoolboy* OR boyhood OR girlhood OR child* OR schoolchild* OR girl OR schoolgirl* OR infan* OR juvenil* OR kid OR minor OR newborn* OR new-born* OR paediatric* OR pediatric* OR preschool* OR puber* OR pubescen* OR teen* OR tween* OR toddler* OR youth* OR student* OR schoolage*)) OR (INDEXTERMS (child) OR INDEXTERMS (adolescent) OR INDEXTERMS (pediatric)))) AND ((TITLE-ABS ("Motor skill*")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Motor competence")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Motor planning")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Motor fitness")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Motor development")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Locomotor skill*")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Object control skill*")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Gross-motor")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Perceptual-motor competence")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Perceptual-motor behavior")) OR (TITLE-ABS ("Perceptual-motor behaviour"))) OR (AUTHKEY ("Motor skill*" OR "Motor competence" OR "Motor planning" OR "Motor fitness" OR "Motor development" OR "Locomotor skill*" OR "Object control skill*" OR "Gross-motor" OR "Perceptual-motor competence" OR "Perceptual-motor behavior" OR "Perceptual-motor behaviour")))

Selection criteria

For studies to be included in the current scoping review, they needed to take place on a playground. Playgrounds were defined as places designed or designated to facilitate play. We included publicly available outdoor playgrounds e.g., in parks or neighborhoods, as well as playgrounds at early childhood education and care (ECEC) centers, schools, and healthcare centers. We included all studies examining children aged 0–17 years regardless of the children’s health condition and physical abilities. Studies were also included if the participants were parents or professionals around children. We included studies using all forms of study designs. Studies that were peer-reviewed, published from January 2000 to October 2023, and written in English were included. There was no limit on country or origin of studies. Further, in the present scoping review, studies were only included if FMS was one of the outcomes measured.

Studies were excluded if they only examined sports facilities (e.g., soccer fields, parkour parks, basketball courts, beach volley) and portable playground features (e.g., balls or socio-dramatic props such as teacups). Further, studies only focusing on adult-led playground activities such as physical education, organized activities, or supervision on the playground were excluded as well as studies about playground policy.

Selection procedure

All references were imported to Endnote 20.0.1 where duplicates were removed by one researcher (CM) and uploaded to Covidence. Then all authors screened the publications’ title and abstract (Jun-Aug 2022) whereafter each full-text was assessed by two of the authors independently (Aug-Oct 2022). Conflicts were solved by one of two authors (CSP or JS). The reference list of 10 randomly chosen included publications were screened by one author (CM) for additional publications of relevance (Jan 2023). Due to the comprehensive search strategy, this did not result in additional publications being included. Therefore, we did not check all reference lists for included publications.

For the present review, all full-text publications investigating the association between unstructured playground play and FMS were selected. Relevant citations in these publications were screened but no additional publications of relevance were found. Then after the second search, one author (CSP) screened the new publications’ title and abstract whereafter full-text was assessed by two of the authors (CSP and CM) independently (Oct 2023). The results of the search and the study inclusion process are presented in a flowchart, Fig 1.

Fig 1. Flowchart for selected publications.

Fig 1

Data extraction

For the current review, data extraction of the selected publications with FMS as a health outcome was completed by the first author (CSP) and cross-checked by MT. Data extracted included the aim of the study, study design, participants, setting, methods used to measure FMS, and key findings relevant to the review, as well as general information about the study such as author, country, and year of publication.

Results

As seen in the flowchart (Fig 1), the total number of hits for search one was 66,279 related to the broad search on physical, social, and mental health outcomes in relation to children’s playground use. After duplicates were removed 42,110 publications remained, of which 39,721 irrelevant titles and abstracts were excluded leaving 2,389 full-text publications to be screened. 1,941 full-text publications were excluded with reasons detailed in Fig 1. A total of 222 publications met the inclusion criteria, of which, 12 included FMS as at least one of the health outcomes investigated. For search two the total number of hits was 775 of which we removed 224 duplicates. From the 551 remaining publications, 541 irrelevant titles and abstracts were excluded leaving 10 full-text publications to be screened. Eight full-text publications were excluded with reasons detailed in Fig 1. A total of 2 publications from search two met the inclusion criteria. Thus, in total 14 publications were included in the current scoping review. Extracted data from the 14 included publications can be found in Table 2.

Table 2. Data extraction of the included publications.

Author, publication year, and country Study aim and design Study setting and population Measurement and results
Adams et al (2018), Australia
[29]
Aim: Whether playground design facilitated different levels of PA and FMS
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: Neighborhood
(3 playgrounds in 1 city; traditional, adventure, contemporary).
Population: 57 children, 5–10 yrs.
Measurement: SOFIT (modified)
Results: There were no significant associations with FMS between the three playgrounds.
The most frequent performed FMS were locomotor skills (31.3%), specifically walking (18.3%) and running (11.3%). Body management skills (15.2%) and climbing (12.3%) was also observed at all three playgrounds, whereas object control skills such as catching and throwing were rarely observed (0.0–0.2%)
Children performed few FMS but used a wider variety of equipment in the contemporary and adventure playgrounds.
Famelia et al (2018), Indonesia
[35]
Aim: Relationships among FMS, playground PA, and gender
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: 4 ECEC centers (2 urban—2 rural)
Population: 66 children, 3–6 yrs.
Measurement: PMSC + the Test of Gross Motor Development-3
Results: No main effect of location for locomotor skills and perceived movement skill competence.
Fjørtoft (2001), Norway
[30]
Aim: How playing in the natural environment might stimulate FMS
Design: Intervention
(play in forest versus traditional playground 1–2 hours/day for 9 months)
Setting: 3 ECEC centers (1 experimental—2 control)
Population: 75 children, 5–7 yrs. (46 in the experimental group)
Measurement: EUROFIT + Beam walking and Indian skip
Results: At the posttest 9 months after the pretest, significant differences were found in eight out of nine FMS test items in the experimental group (flamingo balance (p<0.001), plate tapping (p<0.001), standing broad jump (p<0.001), bent arm hang (p<0.001), Indian skip (p<0.001), sit-ups (p<0.01), beam walking (p<0.01), and shuttle run (p<0.01)) whereas the control group experienced a significant difference in three test items (standing broad jump (p<0.01), bent arm hang (p<0.001), and Indian skip (p<0.001).
At the pretest the experimental group scored lower than the control group, but scored better in all test items at the posttest
Foweather et al (2021), England
[21]
Aim: The association between play behavior and FMS during recess at preschool
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: 12 ECEC centers
Population: 133 children, 3–5 yrs.
Measurement: Video-assessment using CMSP
Results: Relative to time spent in other types of play behaviors, time spent in play without equipment was positively associated with total FMS and locomotor skills, while time spent in locomotion activities (moving while not engaged in an active play game) was negatively associated with total FMS and locomotor skills
Gil-Madrona et al (2019), Spain
[37]
Aim: The contribution of public playgrounds to obesity reduction, motor, social, and creative development
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: Neighborhood (41 parks in 1 city) Population: 1019 adults Measurement: Survey
Results: 53.7% parents agreed with the positive contribution of public playgrounds to motor skills (38% quite agree and 15.7% totally agree).
Grunseit et al (2020), Australia
[28]
Aim: Relationship between school playground size and PA, fitness, and FMS
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: 43 primary schools
Population: 5238 children, 5–12 yrs.
Measurement: Scoring of 7 FMS skills
Results: No association between playground space and motor skills.
Loftesnes (2021), Norway
[31]
Aim: Evaluating a new-built nature playground for children aged 2–6 years
Design: Cross-sectional post-intervention study
(A nature playground used in one year for min. 2 days a week)
Setting: 9 ECEC centers
Population: 30 adults (18 staff + 12 parents)
Measurement: Survey and interview
Results: Parents found their child being more able to cope with motor skills.
Miller et al (2017), USA
[27]
Aim: Quantify equipment/areas impacted for children with sensory challenges
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: Child rehabilitation center
(1 playground)
Population: 181 children, 3–15 yrs. (41 with special needs)
Measurement: Coding of camera recordings
Results: The behavior most often observed across all pieces of equipment was novel use, ranging from 41.82–97.66% of the time. Least = Mobius, most = sand and water. Motor planning was highest for the Mobius Climber (58.18%) and lowest for sand and water.
Nobre et al (2023), Brazil
[36]
Aim: factors associated with FMS in preschoolers from a Brazilian urban area
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: 9 ECEC centers
Population: 211 children, 3–5 yrs.
Measurement: TGMD-2
Results: Children from ECEC centers with a park or courtyard had significant higher locomotion skills than those who did not have outdoor spaces in the ECEC center.
Szeszulski et al (2022), USA
[25]
Aim: Association between the characteristics of the ECEC center environment and FMS
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: 16 ECEC centers
Population: 172 children, 3–5 yrs.
Measurement: CMSP
Results: Better outdoor play environment quality score and more outdoor equipment were positively associated with higher CMSP scores.
Tortella et al (2016), Italy
[32]
Aim: Effects of structured and unstructured activities played at the playground on FMS
Design: Intervention
(10 weeks– 1 hour/week—half structured and half unstructured play at specific playground)
Setting: Neighborhood (1 playground)
Population: 110 children, 5 yrs. (71 in the experimental group)
Measurement: Scoring of 9 FMS skills (3 for fine and 6 for gross motor skills)
Results: The experimental group improved significantly in 4 out of 6 gross motor tasks (putting a medicine ball (p<0.001), one leg balance on left foot (p<0.05), balance on beam (p<0.001), and balance on platform (p<0.001)) and in none of the fine motor tasks.
Tortella et al (2022), Italy
[33]
Aim: Effects of partly structured activities or free play on FMS
Design: Intervention
(10 weeks– 1 hour/week–partly structured versus free play at specific playground)
Setting: Neighborhood (1 playground)
Population: 141 children, 4–6 yrs. (62 in experimental group 1; partly structured, 43 in experimental group 2; free play, 36 in control group)
Measurement: MABC-2 + TCM + Balance on Elastic Platforms Task and Balance on Beam Task
Results: No significant difference in motor competence measured by the TCM or the MABC-2 between groups. A significant improvement was found in the Platforms Task and Balance on Beam Task in the partly structured activity group compared to the free play and control groups.
True et al (2017), USA
[26]
Aim: The contribution of various preschool environmental characteristics to children’s FMS
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: 22 ECEC centers (4 head start, 7 faith-based, 11 commercial)
Population: 229 children, 3–5 yrs.
Measurement: CMSP
Results: Playground size is a significant predictor of total motor score (effect size 0.33) when adjusting the analyses for other significant predictors, e.g., age, classroom size, teacher education and electronic media use but not locomotor score and object control score, individually.
Time spent in outdoor open spaces, fixed and portable playground equipment were non-significant predictors to total gross motor scores.
Virji-Babul et al (2006), Canada
[34]
Aim: Analyzing the level of motor engagement within the playground
Design: Cross-sectional
Setting: 1 Child rehabilitation center
Population: 6 children with DS, 6–7 yrs.
Measurement: Coding of camera recordings
Results: Children spent a great amount of time in motor-based activities (90%) in a playground setting.
The primary motor activity was swinging. The tasks appeared to become more difficult as the environment became more complex (even surface versus grass and incline surface).

CMSP = the Champs Motor Skill Protocol; DS = Downs syndrome; ECEC = Early childhood education and care; EUROFIT = European Test of Physical Fitness, the Motor Fitness Test; FMS = fundamental movement skills; MABC-2 = Movement Assessment Battery for Children; PA = physical activity; PMSC = Pictorial Scale of Perceived Movement Skill Competence; yrs. = years; SOFIT = System for Observing Fitness Instruction Time; TGMD-2 = Test of Gross Motor Development; TMC = Test of Motor Competence.

Characteristics of the studies

Three studies were from the USA [2527]. Two studies each were from Australia [28,29], Norway [30,31], and Italy [32,33]. The remaining studies were from England [21], Canada [34], Indonesia [35], Brazil [36], and Spain [37] (one study per country).

Half of the studies were conducted in early childhood education and care (ECEC) centers (n = 7) [21,25,26,30,31,35,36]. Four studies were conducted in a public area (i.e., neighborhood, park, sports center) [29,32,33,37]. Two studies were conducted at rehabilitation centers for children [27,34], and one study in primary schools [28].

Twelve studies included children as participants, except one study including parents [37] and one study including parents and ECEC staff members [31]. These two studies focused on adults’ perceptions on children’s FMS. Most of the child studies included a population of traditionally developed children (n = 10) [21,25,26,2830,32,33,35,36]. One study included children with Down syndrome [34] and one study included both typically developed children and children with special needs that have led to atypical development [27]. The age of the children studied varied widely across the studies included. Six of the studies included children from the age of three; 3–5 years (n = 4) [21,25,26,36], 3–6 years (n = 1) [35], and 3–15 years (n = 1) [27]. One study included children from 4–6 years [33]. Four studies included children from the age of five; 5 years (n = 1) [32], 5–7 years (n = 1) [30], 5–10 years (n = 1) [29], and 5–12 years (n = 1) [28]. One study investigated children aged 6–7 [34]. Socioeconomic status (SES) was infrequently reported and none of the studies had a focus on low SES population.

Eleven study designs were cross-sectional without control groups [21,2529,31,3437]. Three studies were intervention studies, all with experimental group and control group [30,32,33]. All studies used quantitative methods. However, one study was a mixed methods study using qualitative adult interviews in combination with surveys [31].

Nine of the studies used test protocols to measure FMS, of which three studies used the Champs Motor Skills Protocol (CMSP) [21,25,26], one study used Pictorial Scale of Perceived Movement Skill Competence (PMCS) [35], one study used the Motor Fitness Test of the European Test of Physical Fitness (EUROFIT) [30], one study used the Test of Gross Motor Development (TGMD-2) [36]. Also, one study used the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (MABC-2), the Test of Motor Competence (TCM), and two playground balance tests [33]. Two studies used selected items of validated FMS tests such as sprint run, vertical jump, side gallop, one leg balance, heel-to-toe walking, catch, and putting a medicine ball [28,32]. Two studies each coded camera recordings [27,34], and used survey [31,37]. Loftesnes [31] combined survey and interview. One study used the systematic observation method SOFIT [29].

Size of playground and fundamental movement skills

Two studies investigated the association between FMS and playground size in ECEC centers and primary schools, respectively. The cross-sectional study by True et al. 2017 including 229 children aged 3–5 from 22 ECEC centers in USA found a small positive relationship (effect size 0.33) between children’s overall FMS competence and ECEC center playground size measured by Champs Motor Skills Protocol. Playground size was significantly associated with total motor score but not locomotor score and object control score individually [26]. In contrast, Grunseit et al. (2020) found no association between playground size and FMS when adjusting for relevant covariates in a cross-sectional study testing 210 children aged 5–12 in 43 Australian primary schools.

Playground setting and fundamental movement skills

Four studies investigated FMS in relation to the setting of the playground. In a cross-sectional study conducted in two urban and two rural ECEC center playgrounds in Indonesia including 66 3-6-year-old children, Famelia et al. [35] found no main effect of rural versus urban ECEC center playgrounds for locomotor skills and perceived movement skill competence even though children at the rural ECEC center playgrounds were found to be more sedentary than children in the urban ECEC center playgrounds. Another cross-sectional study was conducted in 9 ECEC centers in an urban area in Brazil including 211 3-5-year-old children. In this study it was found that the children from ECEC centers with a park or courtyard had significant higher locomotion skills than those who did not have outdoor spaces in the ECEC center [36]. Oppositely, the two Norwegian studies investigated the association between play in rural areas and FMS in a ECEC setting. The one study was a post-intervention study evaluating newly built nature playgrounds in nine ECEC centers. After being used for minimum two days a week in a one-year period, 12 parents being interviewed experienced their 2-6-year-old children being more able to cope with motor skills [31]. Fjørtoft [30] conducted an intervention study investigating 5-7-year-old children’s FMS after playing in a forest for 1–2 hours per day (experimental group of 46 children from one ECEC center) versus playing in a traditional ECEC center playground for 1–2 hours per day (control group of 29 children from two different ECEC centers). At the post-test 9 months after the pretest, significant differences were found in eight out of nine FMS test items in the experimental group (flamingo balance (p<0.001), plate tapping (p<0.001), standing broad jump (p<0.001), bent arm hang (p<0.001), Indian skip (p<0.001), sit-ups (p<0.01), beam walking (p<0.01), and shuttle run (p<0.01)) whereas the control group experienced a significant difference in three test items (standing broad jump (p<0.01), bent arm hang (p<0.001), and Indian skip (p<0.001). Thus, the motor fitness test showed a general tendency that the children using the forest as a playscape performed better in a variety of motor skills than the children on the traditional playground [30].

Playground features and fundamental movement skills

Eight studies investigated FMS in relation to playground features. Six of the studies included traditionally developed children in ECEC centers (n = 3) and public playgrounds (n = 3), whereas two studies included children on a playground in a child rehabilitation center. These two studies focused on children with special needs that have led to atypical development. The one ECEC center study was a cross-sectional study from the USA including 172 3-5-year-old children conducted in 16 ECEC centers. They found that a higher-quality outdoor play environment (e.g., shade, number of play areas, bike path quality), and more outdoor play equipment were associated with higher locomotor skills measured using Champs Motor Skills Protocol [25]. In contrast, another cross-sectional study also using Champs Motor Skills Protocol to measure 133 3-5-year-old children from 12 UK ECEC centers found that time spent in active games without use of play equipment (e.g., chasing games/rough and tumble) was positively associated with higher total FMS and locomotor skills scores [21]. Active games with fixed equipment (e.g., climbing frame) or portable equipment (e.g., balls or socio-dramatic props such as teacups) were not associated with FMS in this study. Likewise, time spent in locomotion activities (i.e., moving while not engaged in an active play game) was negatively associated with total FMS and locomotor skills. In line with this, the third ECEC center study conducted among 229 3-5-year-old children in 22 ECEC centers also found that fixed and portable playground equipment were non-significant predictors to total gross motor scores when using Champs Motor Skills Protocol as an FMS measurement tool [26].

In a cross-sectional study from Spain, Gil-Madrona et al. [37] investigated the contribution of public playgrounds with classic features (such as slides, climbing frames, and swings) on children’s FMS seen from a parent perspective. They found that 53.7% of the 1,019 parents included in the study agreed with the positive contribution of public playgrounds to motor skills. An Italian intervention study investigated a public playground designed with specific features to promote mobility, balance and manuality. They showed a significant improvement in the experimental group of 5-year-old children (n = 71) versus a control group of children (n = 39) in four out of six gross motor tasks (putting a medicine ball (p<0.001), one leg balance on left foot (p<0.05), balance on beam (p<0.001), and balance on platform (p<0.001)) after 30 minutes of structured play and 30 minutes of unstructured play once a week for a 10 weeks period [32]. In an Australian cross-sectional study, Adams et al. [29] investigated three different public playgrounds; a traditional, an adventure, and a contemporary public playground, in relation to FMS by systematically observing play in 57 children aged 5–10 years at the respective playgrounds. They found that children used a wider variety of features in the contemporary and adventure playgrounds compared to the traditional playground. However, no significant association with FMS between the three types of public playgrounds was found, possibly because a low amount of time in motor-based activities was observed. Still, the most frequently performed FMS were locomotor skills (31.3%), whereas object control skills were rarely observed (0.0–0.2%) at the three different public playgrounds [29].

Oppositely, coding of camera recordings in a Canadian cross-sectional study conducted at a rehabilitation center playground showed that six 6-7-year-old children with Down syndrome spent a great amount of time in motor-based activities (90%) in the playground setting. The primary motor activity was swinging. The tasks appeared to become more difficult as the environment became more complex (i.e., from even surface to grass and incline surface) [34]. In a cross-sectional study conducted at a rehabilitation center playground in USA among 181 3-15-year-old children both typically developed and with special needs atypically developed (n = 41), Miller et al. [27] coded camera recordings and found that novel use (i.e., ideation; child uses the equipment in a novel way), and motor planning (i.e., skilled, nonhabitual movements used to accomplish multistep tasks) were observed at all six playground features (sand and water table, jungle gym, Roller Slide, Mobius Climber, Cozy Dome, Omnispin Spinner). Novel use was observed most at a ‘sand and water’ table and least at ‘Mobius Climber’ (a climbing wall). In contrast, motor planning was highest for the Mobius Climber and lowest for the ‘sand and water’ table [27].

Discussion

In the following, we will discuss our results on playground size, setting, and features and highlight how future playgrounds should be designed to support FMS development of children.

Do we need large playgrounds?

Since studies have demonstrated that children are more active in large playgrounds [38,39], it seems obvious to conclude that more space provides more opportunities for FMS acquisition. In the study by Grunseit et al. [28], the authors found an association between the amount of playground space available and self-reported physical activity and objectively measured fitness, but interestingly they did not find an association between playground space and FMS. Given the strong predictive association between levels of physical activity and FMS competence [5] and the positive association between playground space and both physical activity and fitness showed in the study by Grunseit et al. [28], the reason for the lack of an association between playground space and FMS is unclear. However, in the study by True et al. [26], larger playground size was significantly associated with higher total FMS score. The reason could be that the age of the children in the two studies differed. In the study by Grunseit et al. [28], the children were 5–12 years old whereas in the study by True et al. [26], the children were 3–5 years old (preschool years) which is identified as a crucial time in terms of forming and developing FMS [5]. It is therefore possible that the children in the study by Grunseit et al. [28] had past the crucial time for developing FMS lowering the influence of playground size on FMS. In fact, in another study they found that 3–7 years-old children from rural areas with the lowest residential density had better FMS than their peers from urban areas with the highest residential density [40]. Although the focus in this study was not specifically on playgrounds, Niemiströ et al. [40] concluded that because children spend multiple hours in ECEC centers, they believe that the size of the outdoor environment near these centers (such as playgrounds) plays a notable role in children’s motor development.

Do we need nature playgrounds?

Jointly, the two Norwegian studies included in this review [30,31], indicated a positive impact of the natural environment on children’s motor development. Further, the study by Nobre et al. [36] showed a positive effect of parks/courtyard in ECEC centers on children’s locomotor skills. Similarly, a systematic review indicated some association between nature play and FMS even though this review did not focus specifically on playgrounds [41]. However, it is worth examining if the effect shown on green playgrounds is due to these playgrounds being placed in areas that might be larger and having a lower population density, as discussed above. In the study by Fjørtoft [30], the nature space used for playing by the experimental group of children was larger and herewith lower in population density than the traditional playgrounds in the ECEC centers used by the control groups. Also, in the study by Loftesnes [31], the natural space used for building a nature playground was larger and lower in population density than the traditional playground. In the study by Nobre et al. [36] size and population density was not mention but could be the explanation for effect since the ECEC centers with parks/courtyards were compared with ECEC centers without outdoor spaces at all. On the other hand, no effect of location was found in the study by Famelia et al. [35] investigating urban playgrounds in the city against rural playgrounds in farming areas in Indonesian ECEC centers. In this study, size or population density of the playgrounds were not mentioned, but it was described that limited space occurred at some settings, and they found children to be sedentary in the playgrounds around 70% of playground time, indicating that the playgrounds were relatively small. In line with this, a Norwegian study showed no differences in FMS competence of children attending nature preschools and traditional preschools [42]. This could support that playground size and density have a greater impact on FMS than nature itself. However, we know too little about how the natural environment functions as a playground developing children’s FMS to draw any conclusions on this topic.

What features do we need on the playgrounds?

In the study by Szeszulski et al. [25], the authors found both number of features and quality of features in the ECEC centers’ outdoor environment to influence children’s locomotor skills. On the other hand, Foweather et al. [21] found that time spent in active games without equipment was positively associated with higher locomotor skills score and total FMS. This finding suggests that spending more time on active games such as dancing, chasing games, and rough and tumble play without use of playground features may be important for FMS development. Previous research has also demonstrated that preschool children in the highest locomotor skill tertile generally engaged in more dancing than children in the lowest tertile [43]. In the study by Foweather et al. [21], however, children spent a relatively large proportion of time (41%) engaged in active games with equipment, but this type of play was not associated with FMS, possibly because the children were frequently observed being sedentary on the equipment. It is possible that these pieces of equipment supported other FMS capacities, such as climbing or stability skills, not assessed in the study by Foweather et al. [21]. Nevertheless, this finding is similar to Adams et al. [29] reporting that the children used a wider variety of equipment in the contemporary and adventure playgrounds than the traditional playground, but they did not find a statistically significant association between the FMS observed at the three playgrounds varying in features. These authors suggest that it is possible that the general low FMS mastery among children could be influenced by the lack of FMS required to play in playgrounds [29].

From the studies, however, various features seem to encourage varying motor competences making it complex to answer exactly what features are needed in the playground to improve children’s FMS development. In the study by Adams et al. [29], locomotor skills such as walking and running were observed most frequently in the contemporary playground where the features were spread over a large area requiring children to use locomotor skills to move around. Conversely, locomotor skills were observed less frequently at the adventure playground where the features were linked off a large walkway and children needed different FMS to move to and from different features such as balancing. Still, climbing nets were the most used play feature at the adventure playground stimulating locomotor skills [29]. Climbing and hanging features are also important to develop upper-body strength [44]. Importantly, Adams et al. [29] and True et al. [26] found no association between playground play and object control skills. According to True et al. [26], features to improve object control skills seem not to be provided very often in playgrounds for preschool children. In line with this, a study found object control skills to develop at a slow rate before the age of 9–10 [45]. Portable features such as balls might influence object control skills. Portable features, however, was not studied in the current review. As Tortella et al. [32] showed, specifically targeted playground equipment may be necessary to encourage FMS development. The authors conclude, however, that specific training using specific playground features, only affects the development of that task trained and not necessarily other tasks related to the same FMS competence [32]. This statement was supported in the other study by Tortella et al. [33]. In this study structured activity at the FMS specific playground was found to improve children’s balance more than free play at the same playground. In line with this, Revie and Larkin [16] found a significant effect of eight sessions of intensive teaching of FMS in children with poor coordination. Though, this study did not take place on a playground.

A sensory-rich playground provided with varied features, enticing colors, and multitextured materials seem to be valuable for the development of children with disabilities [27]. However, according to Virji-Babul et al. [34] children with special needs atypically developed seem to have more difficulties in extracting and processing relevant information from the physical environment than children typically developed, leading to decreased engagement in free play at the playground. Thus, it seems important that playground features are easy to interpret and can be used at different developmental stages.

Strengths and limitations

We followed the JBI methodology and PRISMA guidelines for scoping reviews for a robust, rigorous, and transparent review protocol, thus the risk of bias in our review methodology is low [23]. Further, a strength is that the search procedure was developed by a research group of experts in the research field of playground usage in collaboration with a librarian with huge expertise in search strategies. To capture as much relevant research, four different databases were searched. However, given the large number of publications retrieved, we questioned if we should have created a third block containing health outcomes to narrow-down our search. That was also the reason why we added a third search block in our second search. Further, no quality assessment of included publications was performed. Since only 14 publications were included in the present scoping review, we wanted to cover all knowledge on the subject regardless of the design and quality of the study. A challenge was that the studies used many different child-monitoring instruments to measure FMS, possibly because there is little agreement on what FMS measurement should be used [46].

Conclusion and future directions

The aim of the current scoping review was to create an overview of all research that is relevant when studying the influence of unstructured playground play on children’s FMS. Fourteen studies investigated unstructured playground play and children’s FMS. From the current scoping review, it seems important to design playgrounds with various features targeting balance, climbing, throwing, and catching to provide opportunities for children to enhance each FMS (i.e., stability, locomotor skills, and object control skills). Further, spreading features over a large area seems to both ensure ample space per child and to stimulate children to use locomotor skills by moving to and from features and by playing active games without equipment. Possibly, also natural play settings develop children’s FMS. Our results, however, should be read with caution. Overall, based on only 14 studies reviewed, we still know too little about the association between unstructured playground play and FMS, and more effort should be dedicated to future studies in this field. In particular, we need more experimental studies using standardized FMS tests since only three of the 14 studies had this high-quality design. Also, it is needed to discuss the quality of the used FMS tests in future research.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) checklist.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We thank academic officer Danielle Nørager Johansen from University of Southern Denmark who helped organizing and coordinating the search and extraction process. Also, thanks to librarian Lasse Østergaard from University of Southern Denmark who helped with the search strategi.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.

Funding Statement

Our study is funded by Kompan A/S who has funded the World Playground Research Institute at University of Southern Denmark. The authors Charlotte Skau Pawlowski, Mette Toftager, and Jasper Schipperijn are employed part time in the World Playground Research Institute and receive 50% of their salary from Kompan A/S and 50% of their salary from University of Southern Denmark. Cathrine Damsbo Madsen is employed fulltime in the World Playground Research Institute and receive 100% of her salary from Kompan A/S. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Hulteen RM, Morgan PJ, Barnett LM, Stodden DF, Lubans DR. Development of Foundational Movement Skills: A Conceptual Model for Physical Activity Across the Lifespan. Sports Medicine. 2018; 48(7): 1533–1540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Xin F, Chen ST, Clark C, Hong JT, Liu Y, Cai YJ. Relationship between Fundamental Movement Skills and Physical Activity in Preschool-aged Children: A Systematic Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(10). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lima RA, Pfeiffer K, Larsen LR, Bugge A, Moller NC, Anderson LB, et al. Physical Activity and Motor Competence Present a Positive Reciprocal Longitudinal Relationship Across Childhood and Early Adolescence. Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2017; 14(6): 440–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Robinson LE, Stodden DF, Barnett LM, Lopes VP, Logan SW, Rodrigues LP, et al. Motor Competence and its Effect on Positive Developmental Trajectories of Health. Sports Medicine. 2015; 45(9): 1273–1284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hardy LL, Reinten-Reynolds T, Espinel P, Zask A, Okely AD. Prevalence and Correlates of Low Fundamental Movement Skill Competency in Children. Pediatrics. 2012; 130(2): E390–E398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bolger LE, Bolger LA, O’Neill C, Coughlan E, O’Brien W, Lacey S, et al. Global levels of fundamental motor skills in children: A systematic review. Journal of Sports Science. 2021; 39(7): 717–753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Ericsson I, Karlsson MK. Motor skills and school performance in children with daily physical education in school-a 9-year intervention study. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports. 2014; 24(2): 273–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.van der Fels IM, Te Wierike SC, Hartman E, Elferink-Gemser MT, Smith J, Visscher C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4–16 year old typically developing children: A systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2015; 18(6): 697–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Herrmann C, Bretz K, Kühnis J, Seelig H, Keller R, Ferrari I. Connection between Social Relationships and Basic Motor Competencies in Early Childhood. Children-Basel. 2021; 8(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Vedul-Kjelsås V, Sigmundsson H, Stensdotter AK, Haga M. The relationship between motor competence, physical fitness and self-perception in children. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2012; 38(3): 394–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lai SK, Costigan SA, Morgan PJ, Lubans DR, Stodden DF, Salmon J, et al. Do School-Based Interventions Focusing on Physical Activity, Fitness, or Fundamental Movement Skill Competency Produce a Sustained Impact in These Outcomes in Children and Adolescents? A Systematic Review of Follow-Up Studies. Sports Medicine. 2014; 44(1): 67–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Morgan PJ, Barnett LM, Cliff DP, Okely AD, Scott HA, Cohen KE, et al. Fundamental movement skill interventions in youth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2013; 132(5): e1361–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jiménez-Díaz J, Chaves-Castro K, Salazar W. Effects of Different Movement Programs on Motor Competence: A Systematic Review With Meta-Analysis. Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2019; 16(8): 657–666. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ma J, Hogan MJ, Eyre EL, Lander N, Barnett LM, Duncan MJ. Enhancing the implementation and sustainability of fundamental movement skill interventions in the UK and Ireland: lessons from collective intelligence engagement with stakeholders. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity. 2021; 18(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Logan SW, Robinson LE, Wilson AE, Lucas WA. Getting the fundamentals of movement: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of motor skill interventions in children. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2012; 38(3): 305–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Revie G, Larkin D. Task specific intervention with children reduces movement problems. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly. 1993, 10: 29–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Berg S. Children’s Activity Levels in Different Playground Environments: An Observational Study in Four Canadian Preschools. Early Childhood Education Journal. 2015; 43(4): 281–287. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bento G, Dias G. The importance of outdoor play for young children’s healthy development. Porto Biomedical Journal. 2017; 2(5): 157–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.World Health Organization. Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour and sleep for children under 5 years of age. 2019; License: CC BY-NC-SA 3.0 IGO: [https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664. [PubMed]
  • 20.Johnstone A, Hughes AR, Martin A, Reilly JJ. Utilising active play interventions to promote physical activity and improve fundamental movement skills in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health. 2018; 18(1): 789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Foweather L, Crotti M, Foulkes JD, O’Dwyer MV, Utesch T, Knowles ZR, et al. Foundational Movement Skills and Play Behaviors during Recess among Preschool Children: A Compositional Analysis. Children-Basel. 2021; 8(7). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Parrish AM, Yeatman H, Iverson D, Russell K. Using interviews and peer pairs to better understand how school environments affect young children’s playground physical activity levels: a qualitative study. Health Education Research. 2012; 27(2): 269–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Peters MDJ, Godfrey C, McInerney P, Munn Z, Tricco AC, Khalil H. Chapter 11: Scoping reviews, in JBI Manual for Evidence Synthesis, E. Aromataris and Z. Munn, Editors. JBI; 2020.
  • 24.Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O’Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al., PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med, 2018; 169(7):467–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Szeszulski J, Lorenzo E, Todd M, O’Connor TM, Hill J, Shaibi GQ, et al. Early Care and Education Center Environmental Factors Associated with Product- and Process-Based Locomotor Outcomes in Preschool-Age Children. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022. 19(4). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.True L, Pfeiffer KA, Dowda M, Williams HG, Brown WH, O’Neill JR, et al. Motor competence and characteristics within the preschool environment. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2017; 20(8): 751–755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Miller LJ, Schoen SA, Camarata SM, McConkey J, Kanics IM, Valdez A, et al. Play in natural environments: A pilot study quantifying the behavior of children on playground equipment. Journal of Occupational Therapy, Schools, & Early Intervention. 2017; 10(3): 213–231. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Grunseit AC, O’Hara BJ, Drayton B, Learnihan V, Hardy LL, Clark E, et al., Ecological study of playground space and physical activity among primary school children. BMJ Open. 2020; 10(6). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Adams J, Veitch J, Barnett L. Physical Activity and Fundamental Motor Skill Performance of 5–10 Year Old Children in Three Different Playgrounds. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018; 15(9). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Fjørtoft I. The Natural Environment as a Playground for Children: The Impact of Outdoor Play Activities in Pre-Primary School Children. Early Childhood Education Journal. 2001; 29: 111–117. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Loftesnes JM, In the nature playground—initiation and evaluation of the project at 9 kindergartens. Pitesti. 2021; Feb: 542–548. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Tortella P, Haga M, Loras H, Sigmundsson H, Fumagalli G. Motor Skill Development in Italian Pre-School Children Induced by Structured Activities in a Specific Playground. PLoS One. 2016; 11(7): e0160244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Tortella P, Haga M, Loras H, Fumagalli GF, Sigmundsson H. Effects of free play and partly structured playground activity on motor competence in preschool children: a pragmatic comparison trial. Int J of Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2022; 19:7652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Virji-Babul N, Hovorka R, Jobling A. Playground dynamics: Perceptual-motor behaviour and peer interactions of young children with Down syndrome. Journal on Developmental Disabilities. 2006; 12(1): 29–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Famelia R, Tsuda E, Bakhtiar S, Goodway J. Relationships among perceived and actual motor skill competence and physical activity in Indonesian preschoolers. Journal of Motor Learning and Development. 2018; 6: 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nobre JNP, Morais RLS, Viegas ÂA, Fernandes AC, Scheidt Figueiredo PH, Costa HS, et al., Factors Associated with Motor Competence in Preschoolers from a Brazilian Urban Area. Child Youth Care For. 2022; 52:721–736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gil-Madrona P, Martínez-López M, Prieto-Ayuso A, Saraiva L, Vecina-Cifuentes J, Vicente-Ballesteros T, et al. Contribution of Public Playgrounds to Motor, Social, and Creative Development and Obesity Reduction in Children. Sustainability. 2019; 11(14). [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Worobey J, Worobey HS, Adler AL. Diet, activity and BMI in preschool-aged children: Differences across settings. Ecology of Food and Nutrition. 2005; 44(6): 455–466. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Escalante Y, Backx K, Saavedra JM, García-Hermoso A, Dominguez AM. Play area and physical activity in recess in primary schools. Kinesiology: international journal of fundamental and applied kinesiology. 2012; 44: 123–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Niemistö D, Finni T, Haapala EA, Cantell M, Korhonen E, Sääkslahti A. Environmental Correlates of Motor Competence in Children-The Skilled Kids Study. International Journal of Environmental Research in Public Health. 2019; 16(11). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Dankiw KA, Tsiros MD, Baldock KL, Kumar S. The impacts of unstructured nature play on health in early childhood development: A systematic review. PLoS One. 2020; 15(2): e0229006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lysklett OB, Berg A, Moe B. Motor competence and physical fitness among children attending nature preschools and traditional preschools. International Journal of Play. 2019; 8(1): 53–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.O’Neill JR, Williams HG, Pfeiffer KA, Dowda M, McIver KL, Brown WH, et al. Young children’s motor skill performance: relationships with activity types and parent perception of athletic competence. Journal of Science in Medicine and Sport. 2014; 17(6): 607–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Suttervy J, Thornton C. It doesn’t just happen! Essential contributions from playgrounds. Young Children. 2005; 60: 26–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Butterfield SA, Angell RM, Mason CA. Age and sex differences in object control skills by children ages 5 to 14. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 2012; 114(1): 261–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Haywood KM, Getchell N. Life span motor development. 4th ed. Human Kinetics; 2005.

Decision Letter 0

Renato S Melo

16 Aug 2023

PONE-D-23-14269The role of playgrounds in the development of children’s fundamental movement skills: A scoping reviewPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Pawlowski

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 30 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Renato S. Melo, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that the grant information you provided in the ‘Funding Information’ and ‘Financial Disclosure’ sections do not match.

When you resubmit, please ensure that you provide the correct grant numbers for the awards you received for your study in the ‘Funding Information’ section.

3. Please address the following queries:

a)        Please clarify the sources of funding (financial or material support) for your study. List the grants or organizations that supported your study, including funding received from your institution.

b)        State what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role in your study, please state: “The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

c)        If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funders.

d)        If you did not receive any funding for this study, please state: “The authors received no specific funding for this work.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. Please complete your Competing Interests on the online submission form to state any Competing Interests. If you have no competing interests, please state "The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.", as detailed online in our guide for authors at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submit-now

 This information should be included in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

5. In your Data Availability statement, you have not specified where the minimal data set underlying the results described in your manuscript can be found. PLOS defines a study's minimal data set as the underlying data used to reach the conclusions drawn in the manuscript and any additional data required to replicate the reported study findings in their entirety. All PLOS journals require that the minimal data set be made fully available. For more information about our data policy, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability.

Upon re-submitting your revised manuscript, please upload your study’s minimal underlying data set as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and include the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers within your revised cover letter. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories. Any potentially identifying patient information must be fully anonymized.

Important: If there are ethical or legal restrictions to sharing your data publicly, please explain these restrictions in detail. Please see our guidelines for more information on what we consider unacceptable restrictions to publicly sharing data: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions. Note that it is not acceptable for the authors to be the sole named individuals responsible for ensuring data access.

We will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide in your cover letter.

6. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

7. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Additional Editor Comments :

Dear Dr. Charlotte Skau Pawlowski

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sept 14 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Renato S. Melo, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This manuscript has been reviewed according to the PRISMA extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-SCR). The main aim of this scoping review was to create an overview of research that is related to the influence of structured playground play on children’s FMS. The authors findings will be a valuable contribution to the paediatric research literature and assists to fill a gap in this literature and provide focus for future research by the authors and other researchers working in this space. I have provided line by line comments below and hope this feedback assists the authors to progress their manuscript through to publication.

Thank you for the opportunity to review this interesting and valuable manuscript.

Line 37 - This is a firm statement with only one reference to support the statement. Could this be rephrased to state something like: FMS have previously been shown to be low in preschool and school aged children from high-income countries.

Line 48 - For clarity, please replace the word 'this' with exactly what you are referring to.

Line 50 - The word 'also' is over used in this manuscript. Please consider editing the manuscript to reduce the number of times it is required.

Line 56 - Please insert 'systematic' in front of the word 'review'.

Line 58 - Please insert full-stop after the word 'heterogeneity'.

Line 61 - Please replace the word 'article' with 'scoping review'.

Line 62 - It appears in the way you have presented your results that you had some specific aims (e.g. impact of playground size, location etc). If this is correct, could you please more accurately state your aims?

Line 68 - Please confirm if you have utilised the PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR).

Tricco, AC, Lillie, E, Zarin, W, O'Brien, KK, Colquhoun, H, Levac, D, Moher, D, Peters, MD, Horsley, T, Weeks, L, Hempel, S et al. PRISMA extension for scoping reviews (PRISMA-ScR): checklist and explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018,169(7):467-473. doi:10.7326/M18-0850.

Line 76 - As this review is now over 12 months old, could you please update your search to ensure it is within a 6 month period of recency?

Line 109 - Currently you jump back and forth between inclusions and exclusions. Could you please reorder this content to have the inclusions and exclusions grouped together for improved clarity?

Line 154 - Please check Table numbers. At the end of the manuscript you list Table 1 (not 2) as the data extraction table.

Line 160 - Can you provide some rationale for your dissecting the publications by decades? Are the decades meaningful by decades (new rules / policy etc). Why is this information important for the reader to know about? How does it relate to your aims? You may need to put some information in your methods about this step of data synthesis.

Line 165 - Could you please rewrite these characteristics summaries to refer to studies. I.e., the publications were not undertaken in early childhood education centers but rather the studies were.

Line 168 - Here you state 2 studies included adults. Did they also include children 0-17? (As in your inclusion criteria you state you were including publications examining children aged 0-17 years). If this study did also included children 0-17 years, then in your methods, you will need to be clear that you will accept studies / publications where 0-17 year olds are included in addition to adults. However, if these two studies are just about adults, then according to your inclusion criteria you should exclude them. It is unclear to me if these two studies with adults in ref 35 and 31 or not?

Line 172 - Were the children actually disabled? Or did they have a condition / impairments that led to atypical development? Please consider the use of the word 'disabled' and if it is the correct use of this term (I am not sure, it may or may not be, but the use of this language should carefully considered in line with ICF language expectations).

Line 192 - I am not sure what is meant by this sentence. Loftesnes is not an English Language word (to my knowledge). Could you please help the English language readers to better understand the meaning of this sentence? ADDITIONAL STATEMENT: I think from reading the discussion this is actually an author (Please add the reference citation for the author).

Line 217 - Please correct to post-test

Line 228 - Please see statement above about the use of the word 'disabled' children. If the children were in fact 'disabled' please use person first language in line with expectations of ICF. E.g. Children with diagnosed conditions resulting in disability.

Not all children with impairments will be 'disabled'.

Line 235 - Are you able to provide more information about the 'play equipment' here...so that the reader can better interpret the findings?

Line 251 - Replace 'at' with 'a'

line 254 - insert years after 5-10.

Line 267 - As above for statement - change to person first language.

Lines 275 - 276: If this was not connected with your original aims, then I would encourage you to write something about these key themes prior to presenting them, in your results section.

Line 283 - Replace 'objective' with 'objectively'

Line 304 - Please replace 'worth to examine' with 'worth examining'

Line 364 - Please replace 'traditional' with 'traditionally'

Line 368 - Please provide some detail about the robust features of your methods.

Line 373 - This is a nice insight to share.

Line 531 - For Table 1 - Please consider reformatting so that it can be included in the manuscript. Currently the final right hand side column has most relevant content. You could merge the FMS headings and in the column put sub headings:

FMS Measurement:

FMS Result:

Also the same for

Study Aim:

Study Design:

and

Population:

Setting:

This way your table can more easily be included with the manuscript without a lot of empty space.

Reviewer #2: This is a scope review, which was very well designed by its authors and minor revisions were requested. After the corrections made by the authors in this second review, we observed that all the suggestions requested by the reviewers were accepted and carried out in the manuscript, which they did a good job. Therefore, the article is now ready to be accepted for publication.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Dec 13;18(12):e0294296. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0294296.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


2 Sep 2023

Thank you for letting us correct and clarify the manuscript. We have responded to the editor comments in a rebuttal letter. We were a little unsure about what to do in relation to some of the comments - so let us know if we have misunderstand some requested corrections.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewer.docx

Decision Letter 1

Renato S Melo

24 Sep 2023

PONE-D-23-14269R1The role of playgrounds in the development of children’s fundamental movement skills: A scoping reviewPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Pawlowski

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 08 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Renato S. Melo, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Additional Editor Comments:

Dear authors, the modifications requested by one of the reviewers were not fully clarified by you, therefore, the reviewer requested that the suggestions be made and those that were not, that is, those that were refuted, that you justify in a response letter to the reviewers.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Dear Authors,

I appreciate the additional work you have completed to address the reviewer comments. I can see that you have addressed the comments I provided in the PRISMA checklist but I cannot see that you have addressed the comments (x 30 comments) in the original manuscript supplied by this reviewer on the first review. My apologies if this has somehow not reached you. I have attached the manuscript with comments again. Could I please ask that you address each comment line by line to demonstrate the response in a 'response to reviewer' letter and also highlight the changes made in the manuscript. I believe this is a valuable contribution to the field and look forward to seeing the author responses on your next submission.

Kind regards,

Reviewer.

Reviewer #2: I congratulate the authors for the changes, and I believe that this way the article is clearer for readers.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PONE-D-23-14269_Reviewer Feedback.pdf

Decision Letter 2

Renato S Melo

30 Oct 2023

The role of playgrounds in the development of children’s fundamental movement skills: A scoping review

PONE-D-23-14269R2

Dear Dr. Pawlowski,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Renato S. Melo, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you to each of the authors for making these additional changes. I feel they have contributed greatly to enhancing the paper for your audience. Congratulations on a great contribution of research to this important space. There is just one minor change that needs to be made (and could be made at proof checking), that is that your Scopus search terms is listed as table 2, when it is referenced as Table 1 in the text.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Renato S Melo

20 Nov 2023

PONE-D-23-14269R2

The role of playgrounds in the development of children’s fundamental movement skills: A scoping review

Dear Dr. Pawlowski:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Renato S. Melo

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Checklist. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) checklist.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewer.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PONE-D-23-14269_Reviewer Feedback.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES