Abstract
Courtship interactions are remarkably diverse in form and complexity among species. How neural circuits evolve to encode new behaviors that are functionally integrated into these dynamic social interactions is unknown. Here we report a recently originated female sexual behavior in the island endemic Drosophila species D. santomea, where females signal receptivity to male courtship songs by spreading their wings, which in turn promotes prolonged songs in courting males. Copulation success depends on this female signal and correlates with males’ ability to adjust his singing in such a social feedback loop. Functional comparison of sexual circuitry across species suggests that a pair of descending neurons, which integrates male song stimuli and female internal state to control a conserved female abdominal behavior, drives wing spreading in D. santomea. This co-option occurred through the refinement of a pre-existing, plastic circuit that can be optogenetically activated in an outgroup species. Combined, our results show that the ancestral potential of a socially-tuned key circuit node to engage the wing motor program facilitates the expression of a new female behavior in appropriate sensory and motivational contexts. More broadly, our work provides insights into the evolution of social behaviors, particularly female behaviors, and the underlying neural mechanisms.
Introduction
Social interactions between the sexes during mating are pivotal for their reproductive success1–5, and animals often employ a suite of behaviors to communicate their quality and interests to potential mates5–8. To maintain reproductive barriers between species while permitting sexual selection within species, courtship interactions are often rapidly diversifying. Courtship behaviors exhibit exceptional diversity in complexity and form, often with quantitative and qualitative differences among even closely-related lineages8–11. The real-time production of social behaviors requires complex neural orchestration that integrates external and internal cues to guide adaptive motor responses in relevant social contexts. During the elaboration and diversification of courtship behaviors, how new behaviors are incorporated into existing complex social contexts and neural circuitry in a temporally coordinated and meaningful manner remains unknown.
Newly originated behaviors offer a favorable time window to infer the ancestral and derived states, and to pinpoint the initial changes at play before extensive secondary evolutionary changes mask their origins. However, a system to investigate recently originated social behaviors in species amenable to functional comparison of neural circuits has been lacking. In this study, we leveraged Drosophila species as an emerging neural comparative model12–16 and established a comparative paradigm to explore the origin of new social behaviors at both behavioral and neural levels. Shifting away from the traditional spotlight on male sexual behaviors17, we report a recently originated female behavior in D. santomea, referred to as wing spreading (WS), in which a female extends her wings in response to a male’s courtship song to signal her receptivity. Combining a phylogenetic survey, behavioral characterization, and functional manipulation of neural circuits between species, we provide insights into the ultimate and proximate mechanisms underlying the origin of WS. We demonstrate that WS evolved as a new receptive female signal that dynamically shapes a male’s courtship efforts and copulation outcome. We further show that the origin of WS is mediated by the co-option of a descending circuit node that drives a conserved abdominal behavior and the refinement of a latent and plastic ancestral circuit.
Results
WS is a newly originated receptive female response to male song
In Drosophila, the two sexes typically engage in an extended period of courtship interaction, where a female assesses a male based on his signals such as song, dance, and sex pheromone to inform her copulation decision2,18. Females communicate sexual interests through two conserved female-specific displays: vaginal plate opening (VPO), indicative of receptivity19, and ovipositor extrusion (OE), indicative of rejection20,21. In D. santomea, a closely-related species of D. melanogaster, we observed that a female may extend her wings laterally when a male vibrates one wing to sing a courtship song (Fig. 1a, Video 1). In response to the female’s wing extension, the male may continue singing in place or approach the female to lick her genitalia, the latter of which may be followed by a copulation attempt. This interaction often occurred repeatedly before copulation (Fig. 1a and Extended Data Fig. 1). The female wing extension behavior has not been reported within the melanogaster subgroup but is reminiscent of the female WS behaviors described before copulation in species of some distantly related lineages such as the virilis group19,22,23. Therefore, we refer to this behavior in D. santomea also as WS based on similarities of their motor pattern and the pre-copulatory context, noting that the precise social conditions and functions of WS may differ among species.
Drosophila santomea males produce two types of courtship songs: trains of louder clack generated by bilateral wing vibration, primarily during chasing, and trains of quieter pulses generated by unilateral wing vibration, often when females slow down to allow males to sing in close proximity13,24–26. We found that WS responded selectively to pulse and not clack trains (Fig. 1a). Consistent with the observation that female WS followed an auditory signal, removing a female’s antennae or aristae to abolish her hearing27 completely eliminated WS (Fig. 1b). In comparison, females invariantly performed WS in light versus dark conditions, showing that the production of WS does not depend on visual signals (Fig. 1b).
We further determined how WS is modulated by a female’s internal state of receptivity. In sexually mature unmated females, 30.4% of pulse trains elicited female WS. However, unreceptive females, either sexually immature or recently mated, rarely exhibited WS (Fig. 1c). Moreover, among the mature unmated females, those who had accepted a male’s copulation attempt responded with WS more frequently than those that did not, suggesting a correlation between WS probability and female receptivity to copulation (Fig. 1d). During the courtship interaction, a female continuously evaluates male quality based on his signals, which might influence her receptivity and inform her copulation decision. Indeed, we observed a major increase in WS probability leading up to copulation (Fig 1e,f), and 75.0% of the last pulse train before copulation elicited WS. Therefore, WS probability reflects not only female receptivity at the level of sexual maturity and mating status, but also temporal changes during the courtship interaction.
Given that WS behavior has not been previously reported in the melanogaster subgroup, we asked if WS represents a recent behavioral innovation in D. santomea. We therefore recorded receptive females from five species in this subgroup spanning ~6 million years (Myr) of divergence28: D. melanogaster, D. simulans, D. yakuba, D. teissieri, and D. erecta. In none of these species did we detect WS (Fig. 1g; Spieth19 documented a 10° WS as an acceptance signal in female D. simulans, but we did not observe such behavior in our strain). We further sampled additional strains of D. santomea and its closest sibling species D. yakuba. Consistently, females from all D. santomea strains exhibited WS, while none from the D. yakuba strains did (Fig. 1g). This indicated that WS might be a fixed species difference instead of an intraspecific variation among D. santomea strains. D. santomea is endemic to the volcanic island of São Tomé, while D. yakuba is widely distributed in Africa29. We conclude that WS recently originated in the island species D. santomea when it diverged from D. yakuba about 0.4–1 Myr ago30–32.
Function of WS as a receptive female signal
Female WS might be a social signal that actively modulates a male’s behavior or simply a facilitating act that exposes her genitalia and thereby assists a male’s licking and attempted copulation. To distinguish between these scenarios, we examined the effect of abolishing WS, by removing a female’s wings, on copulation success. A reduction in copulation success would be suggestive of WS’s signaling role, while an increase would be suggestive of WS’s facilitative role. We found that males paired with wing-cut females sang a similar amount of pulse trains (Extended Data Fig. 2a), but had a much lower copulation rate than those paired with intact females (Fig. 2a), suggesting that female WS is a functional signal. Consistent with WS being a species-specific signal, wing removal in D. yakuba and D. melanogaster, whose females do not perform WS, did not affect their copulation rate (Fig. 2a).
We next sought to understand how WS, by communicating a female’s receptivity, alters a male’s behavior to influence the copulation outcome. We observed that WS coincided with a longer pulse train in pairs where females eventually accepted the males’ copulation attempts (Fig. 2b and Extended Data Fig. 2b), thus prompting two possibilities. Firstly, female WS motivates a male to sing longer pulse trains, with the male’s ability to adjust singing efforts predicting or directly affecting his copulation success. Alternatively, longer pulse trains are more potent at eliciting a female’s WS response, and males who produce these longer pulse trains have higher copulation success. We found that WS typically occurred shortly after the start of a pulse train, indicating that a female’s decision to display WS did not depend on hearing a long pulse train. Concordantly, the long pulse train associated with WS in copulated pairs resulted from continued singing after WS began (Fig. 2c,d and Extended Data Fig. 2c,d). In addition, we directly tested the impact of WS on the length of pulse trains by removing female wings to prevent WS. We found that the duration of pulse trains were comparable to those not associated with WS and significantly shorter than WS-associated pulses (Fig. 2e and Extended Data Fig. 2e). Taken together, WS serves as a functional female signal that promotes sustained pulse singing in males. The link between enhanced male singing efforts and copulation success further points to sexual selection favoring males who adeptly respond to the WS signal.
Relationship between WS and VPO
To understand how the newly originated WS behavior is integrated into the pre-existing courtship ritual, we examined the relationship between WS and other female behaviors. Like WS, VPO (when a female extends her abdomen and pushes open her vaginal plates) was reported to be a response to male courtship song in receptive females in D. melanogaster33. Given the similarity between WS and VPO in both the external sensory stimulus and the associated female receptive state, we tested if the two behaviors are associated.
In this dataset, a pulse train could evoke WS and VPO simultaneously (VPO+WS; 39.5%), just VPO (VPO-only; 17.1%), or neither behavior (Neither; 43.4%). Thus, WS always co-occurred with VPO, and we never observed OE in sexually mature unmated females. Using SLEAP, a deep-learning based animal pose tracker34, we monitored changes in female abdomen length as a quantitative readout for VPO and wing angle for WS before, during, and after hearing a pulse train (Fig. 3a–c). The velocity and relative positions of the interacting sexes are shown in Extended Data Fig. 3. Mostly notably, the VPO+WS events revealed a linearly correlated increase (p<1×10−10, adjusted R2=0.980) in abdomen length and wing angle upon pulse song onset until the maximum abdomen length was reached (Fig. 3c). Nonetheless, many VPO events happened without WS. VPO+WS events showed significantly more intense VPO than VPO-only events, measured by the maximum extension of abdomen (Fig. 3d). The co-occurrence and quantitative scaling of WS with VPO, as well as its preferential association with more intense VPO, together suggest that WS is layered on top of the conserved behavior VPO to communicate non-identical social information, potentially a higher receptivity level, during the courtship interaction.
Co-option of VPO command neurons in WS
We hypothesized that WS emerged through modification of pre-existing female sexual circuits. Many circuit elements that encode female-typical behaviors express the sex determination gene doublesex (dsx), which undergoes splicing into sex-specific isoforms to guide the development of sexually dimorphic neural circuits33,35–42. In the brain of D. melanogaster, dsx neurons are organized in anatomically and functionally discrete neuronal clusters that function in various aspects of female reproductive behaviors33,35,37–40,42–45. For instance, pC1 neurons encode a female’s mating status39,44,46. Additionally, vpoDN (also known as pMN2) is a single pair of descending neurons that integrates the external and internal signals to function as a command control of VPO. They receive direct inputs from pC1 neurons and the male song-tuned auditory neurons in the brain, and project to the ventral nerve cord (VNC), primarily targeting the abdominal circuit33.
To compare the function of dsx brain neurons across species in relation to the origin of WS, we developed genetic tools that specifically labeled and manipulated dsx brain neurons in D. santomea, its sibling species D. yakuba, and the model species D. melanogaster. Specifically, we generated dsx-GAL4 alleles, using CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing, and brain-specific flippase transgenes47, which together restricted GAL4-dependent expression of effector genes to dsx neurons in the brain. The gross anatomy of dsx brain neurons labeled was similar across the three species (Fig. 4a). By expressing CsChrimson48, we optogenetically activated dsx brain neurons in isolated, freely-moving females and tracked their body coordinates using SLEAP34. Neural activation drove robust abdomen extension in all three species (Fig. 4b,c and Video 2). Based on findings in D. melanogaster, this abdomen phenotype can be readily explained by the activity of vpoDN in triggering VPO and/or the activities of DNp13 (also known as pMN1) and pC2l in triggering OE33,40,42,43. In contrast to the conserved abdomen phenotype, the same activation only triggered WS (manifested as an increased wing angle) robustly in D. santomea females (Fig. 4d,e and Video 2). Remarkably, decapitated females with only VNC neurites of vpoDN and DNp13 descending neurons activated (Fig. 4a) recapitulated both abdomen extension and wing angle results: females from all three species showed similar abdomen extension, but only D. santomea females displayed robust WS (Fig. 4f–i, Video 2). This result effectively restricted the neurons responsible for WS in D. santomea to the two candidate pairs. Unlike VPO and WS, OE represents a rejective female state20,21,40,42. Further, in natural behaviors of D. santomea, WS obligately co-occurs with VPO while never with the rejective behavior OE. Taken together, we inferred that activation of vpoDN elicited WS in D. santomea.
Aside from the WS phenotype in D. santomea, we also observed behavioral changes in the other two species upon activating dsx brain neurons. In D. yakuba, females moved wings inward while generating a polycyclic song (Extended Data. Fig. 4a and Video 3), a behavior that has not been observed in wildtype D. yakuba in this study nor reported before. In D. melanogaster, there was a slight increase in the wing angle upon activation (Fig. 4d,h), contributed by a few females (30.0% intact, and 20.0% decapitated) exhibiting WS (Extended Data Fig. 4b and Video 4). Therefore, D. melanogaster has a latent circuit for WS.
Latent potential of vpoDN to drive WS in D. melanogaster
Given the likely role of vpoDN in WS in D. santomea, we hypothesized that the activated WS phenotype in D. melanogaster also stemmed from the activity of vpoDN. Indeed, optogenetic activation of vpoDN neurons using a previously reported genetic reagent33 (Fig. 5a) induced VPO in all females and WS in 21.9% of females (Fig. 5b, Extended Data Fig. 5a,b, Video 5). We note that this vpoDN line has a different genetic background from the reagent labeling all dsx brain neurons. The idiosyncrasy of vpoDN in inducing WS across different genetic backgrounds of D. melanogaster suggested that it might be attributable to stochasticity during development. Environmental factors, such as a high temperature during development, can challenge the robustness of non-canalized developmental mechanisms and introduce stochasticity49–51. Hence, we tested the effect of developmental temperature, an impactful environmental factor on neuronal morphology and synaptic physiology52–54, on the efficacy of vpoDN activation in eliciting WS. Intriguingly, rearing flies at a high temperature of 29°C, relative to 23°C, during the larva and pupa stages drastically boosted vpoDN’s potential to elicit WS (Fig. 5c–g, Extended Data Fig. 5c–j) to 71.3% of females. This temperature effect was robustly manifested across different activation intensities (Fig. 5g and Extended Data Fig. 5c,d,g,h). In contrast, VPO was fully canalized regardless of the developmental temperature, and no major effect was observed for the proportion of responding females (100% versus 100%) or the extent of abdominal extension (Fig. 5d,f and Extended Data Fig. 5e,f). In sum, vpoDN has a latent potential to induce WS in D. melanogaster, and this potential is idiosyncratic and subjected to temperature-dependent developmental plasticity.
Given the latent potential of vpoDN to elicit WS, we examined whether wildtype D. melanogaster females occasionally exhibited WS in a way that was overlooked in previous studies. In total, we identified 9 WS events contributed by 7 females from assaying the courtship interactions of 141 pairs (7 of 105 pairs with females raised at 29°C, and 0 of 36 pairs with females raised at 23°C, Fig. 5h, Video 6). All WS events co-occurred with VPO, and 4 out of 9 immediately preceded copulation. Also mirroring the natural WS behavior in D. santomea, there was a positive temporal correlation between female wing angle and abdominal length upon the onset of male singing (Fig. 5i). Thus, wildtype D. melanogaster females perform WS at very low frequency in certain conditions.
Drosophila santomea WS is a recurrent variant of a receptive female behavior
Beyond the melanogaster subgroup, female WS has been reported in a few other species within the Sophophora subgenus, and more broadly in the Drosophila subgenus as a pre-copulatory acceptance signal that initiates copulation19,22,23. Whether females also perform WS during the courtship interaction as in D. santomea, and how WS is associated with VPO, have not been explicitly investigated in this broader phylogenetic context. Therefore, we surveyed 22 species in the Sophophora and Drosophila subgenera for VPO and WS during courtship and before copulation (Fig. 6, Video 7). As expected, VPO was a conserved female behavior observed in all species. In contrast, WS was sparsely represented in the Sophophora subgenus, with repeated turnovers in multiple lineages. In species with WS, these events were not specifically linked to copulation: in the Sophophora subgenus, WS was more commonly seen during the courtship interaction than right before copulation; whereas in the Drosophila subgenus, WS appeared obligatory prior to copulation but was also observed during courtship. Therefore, WS can be broadly characterized as a receptive signal that communicates females’ sexual interests instead of an acceptance signal that green-lights copulation, while the precise social context and receptivity state that WS represents may vary across species. Additionally, both the association and the decoupling between WS and VPO are common behavioral features across the phylogeny (Fig. 6), suggesting the possibility of shared circuit mechanisms that facilitate the recurrent evolution of WS.
Discussion
Historically, studies on the evolution of mating behaviors have predominantly focused on male signals. An emerging perspective shift repositions females as active participants in the dynamic courtship interaction and not passive receivers of male signals4,7,55,56. Nonetheless, female courtship behaviors remain much under-characterized, and little is known about how they originate or evolve. Here, we show that D. santomea WS, a species-specific female behavior originated within the last 0.4–1 Myr, is layered on top of conserved elements of the dynamic courtship interaction to affect male behaviors and direct mate selection. WS in D. santomea serves both as a female response to a male’s cue (the pulse song) and as a signal of her sexual interests, thereby influencing the male’s subsequent actions. Intriguingly, whether a male is capable of increasing his efforts accordingly is a predictor of his chances of copulation, supporting a pivotal role of female sexual behavior in organizing a social feedback loop upon which sexual selection operates. Drosophila santomea co-localizes with the sibling species D. yakuba in a hybrid zone on the island of São Tomé29,57,58. As such, WS might be a key phenotype in the reproductive isolation in these two naturally hybridizing species.
Expression of a new behavior in the appropriate social context
Capitalizing on this recently originated female behavior, our neural circuit comparisons across species shed light on the neural mechanisms by which a new social behavior may originate. The co-option of the VPO command neurons vpoDN, which integrate both sensory and motivational information33, would allow a receptive female hearing a potent male song to express the new behavior WS, and thereby communicate her interests to the male. Descending neurons like vpoDN act as a critical information bottleneck that compresses high-dimensional brain dynamics to low-dimensional commands that interface with motor circuits59,60. The co-option of vpoDN in WS suggests that existing descending pathways might be restrictive neural substrates favored by evolution to drive new behaviors, because they readily permit the expression of newly originated behavior in a meaningful social context.
As vpoDN evolved from a uni-functional node that only drives VPO to a possibly bi-functional one that drives both VPO and WS, we further consider how WS can be encoded in a way that communicates non-identical social information from the ancestral VPO signal. Social behaviors, such as mating and aggression, may involve a combination of behaviors that are associated with graded states of drive61–63. We showed that WS tended to co-occur with more intense VPO in wildtype D. santomea, and the intensity of VPO increased with the activation intensity of vpoDN in D. melanogaster. Therefore, compared with VPO, the expression of WS might involve a higher level of vpoDN activity. Because vpoDN activity reflects female receptivity by receiving excitatory inputs from pC1 neurons33, it is possible that WS is differentially gated from VPO by vpoDN activity to represent a higher receptivity level. Alternatively, modulatory inputs independent of vpoDN could contribute to the differential expression of WS and VPO in natural behaviors. Future testing of the hypotheses would benefit from genetic tools that specifically label vpoDN in D. santomea.
Latent circuit potential facilitates the evolution of new behaviors
How does a socially informed behavioral decision lead to a new motor action? The co-option of vpoDN to elicit WS in D. santomea suggests that it must be functionally coupled with a wing motor circuit. When the VNC neurites of vpoDN and DNp13 were activated in decapitated flies (who lacked inputs from the central brain), WS was elicited most robustly in D. santomea. Therefore, the functional connection between vpoDN and the downstream WS motor circuit has evolved to be more potent in D. santomea, pinpointing the neural substrates behavioral divergence. Meanwhile, in D. melanogaster, vpoDN activation sometimes induced WS, and wildtype females displayed WS on rare occasions, suggesting that this connection is not a de novo but rather an ancestral feature that remains largely latent yet potent. Previous studies reported that sex- and developmental stage-typical behaviors can be experimentally induced, suggesting that latent potentials may broadly exist in the nervous system64–69, serving as raw substrates that fuel the rapid evolution of new behaviors. If so, we anticipate that species-specific behaviors may commonly exist in closely-related outgroup species in primitive prototypes that are occasionally expressed under certain conditions.
The absence of WS within the melanogaster subgroup (besides D. santomea) and its presence in some species outside of the melanogaster subgroup raises the possibility that WS in D. santomea evolved from the reactivation of a vestigial circuit that has lost the WS function at least ~6 Myr ago28. Indeed, neurons for lost behaviors, such as wing motoneurons in flightless grasshoppers, can survive millions of years’ evolution66,70. However, it is also possible that the ancestral connection between vpoDN and the wing motor circuit is not a vestigial feature on the way of degeneration but rather actively maintained to serve an as-yet undefined function. In all three species we examined, vpoDN projects to the mesothoracic neuromere, where they branch dorsally and medially to innervate the tectum, potentially permitting a contact with the wing circuit. One hypothesis is that this connection is required for movement coordination, such as the engagement of wing muscle to sustain a proper posture during VPO. In this scenario, the circuit configuration, maintained by selective pressures unrelated to WS, potentiates the recurrent evolution of WS within Sophophora.
Species difference, idiosyncrasy, and plasticity in behaviors
In D. melanogaster, vpoDN’s ability to drive VPO constitutively versus WS as a latent potential presents a comparison. The former is fully penetrant and canalized: all individuals displayed VPO in response to vpoDN activation, and the response was unaffected by developmental temperature. In contrast, the latter is idiosyncratic and plastic: only some individuals responded with WS, and the response was strongly influenced by developmental temperature. Notably, here, the species difference, idiosyncrasy, and plasticity in behaviors, despite operating at different levels, all reflect phenotypic variations of the same neural circuit substrates, highlighting the lability of ancestral circuits in encoding new behavioral prototypes. Such a labile circuit can then be refined to encode stably expressed behaviors when a selective pressure is present. With a prototypic circuit in place, minor modifications on the weights of local excitations or inhibitions might be sufficient to allow vpoDN to stably engage the wing motor circuit. Extensive resources in the model species D. melanogaster, such as EM connectomes and neurogenetic tools71–75, will facilitate future characterizations of the organization and evolution of the underlying circuits.
By developing a comparative behavioral and neural paradigm to investigate the origin of new behaviors, our results revealed how the ancestral nervous system potentiates such changes and shapes the trajectories of behavioral evolution. The themes emerging from this study, such as co-option, ancestral potential, and the plasticity of prototypic phenotypes, converge with Evo-Devo concepts that focus on morphological evolution76–79. For example, analogous to the origin of WS, the recurrent evolution of ‘supersoldiers’ in the ant Genus Pheidole occurred via the actualization of an ancestral developmental potential, where large supersoldier-like anomalies are occasionally found in nature and can be artificially induced by hormonal manipulation in species lacking a supersoldier caste80. The dissection of neural mechanisms underlying the origin of new behaviors contributes to the synthesis of principles unique for behavioral evolution as well as a unifying conceptual framework for phenotypic evolution81.
Material and Methods
Fly stocks
Flies were maintained on cornmeal-agar-yeast medium (Fly Food B, Bloomington Recipe, Lab Express) at 23°C and 50% humidity on a 12 hour light/dark cycle, unless otherwise specified. All fly stocks used in this study are listed in Supplementary Table 1.
Generation of transgenic flies
The dsx-GAL4 knock-in alleles in D. santomea, D. yakuba, and D. melanogaster were generated using an identical design of CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing to replace the majority of the first coding exon (second exon) of dsx in-frame with T2A-GAL4 followed by a Mhc-DsRed marker. The donor plasmids were constructed by concatenating a left homology arm, T2A, GAL4 (amplified from pBPGUw, addgene #1757582), a Mhc-DsRed marker83, a right homology arm, and a 1.8 kb backbone using Gibson Assembly (NEB). A pair of guide RNAs (gRNAs) were cloned into the pCFD4 vector. A cocktail containing the donor plasmid (200 ng/ul), pCFD4 plasmid (200 ng/ul), and in vitro transcribed Cas9-nanos 3’UTR mRNA (200 ng/ul) was injected into fly embryos. The Otd-Flp lines, which drive Flp expression exclusively in brain, were generated by inserting the pBpGuW-Otd-nls:FLPo plasmid47 into the 2253 attP landing site on the third chromosome in D. santomea and the 2285 landing site on the third chromosome in D. yakuba84 using attB/P φc31 integrase system. The FRT-stop-FRT-CsChrimson:mVenus lines were similarly generated by inserting the pJFRC300–20XUAS-FRT>-dSTOP-FRT>-CsChrimson-mVenus plasmid85 into the 2253 site in D. santomea and the 2180 landing site on the second chromosome in D. yakuba84. The dsx-expressing brain neurons were labeled and activated using a genetic intersection of dsx-GAL4 and Otd-Flp to drive the expression of CsChrimson:mVenus, where the brain-specific recombinase (Otd-Flp) excises a transcriptional stop cassette (FRT-stop-FRT) to enable the transcriptional control of UAS-CsChrimson:mVenus under dsx-GAL4 only in brain. All injections were performed at Rainbow Transgenic Flies using a standard protocol.
Preparation of flies for behavioral assays
Flies in the Sophophora subgenus used in behavioral assays were collected within a few hours of eclosion and kept in single-sex vials with 10–15 flies in each vial. Males were separated into individual vials at least 3 days before recording. All flies were 3–6 days old at the time of the assay, with the exception of the 1 day old sexually immature females in Fig. 1c. Mated females in Fig. 1c were generated by mating each female with a wildtype male 24 hr prior to recording. Flies in the Drosophila subgenus were collected the same way, but allowed age for 10–12 days before recording, and males were separated into individual vials at least 8 days before recording.
In optogenetic activation experiments, females were collected the same way as wildtype females but kept on medium supplemented with 0.2 mM all trans-retinal (Sigma Aldrich) in the dark for 5 days until recording. In wing-cut, antennae-cut, or aristae-cut experiments, female wings, antennae, or aristae, respectively, were removed bilaterally under CO2 anesthesia using micro scissors 3 days before recording. Control females were also subjected to CO2 anesthesia alongside the experimental females. Each female was exposed to CO2 for less than 3 minutes. In decapitation experiments, females were cut at the neck using micro scissors under CO2 anesthesia 30 minutes before the recording, and were allowed to recover in a vial with food until the recording. In temperature manipulation experiments, D. melanogaster females were either grown according to the presented scheme (Fig. 5d) or at 29°C throughout development (Fig. 5i,j).
Behavioral recording
Two cameras (FLIR BFS-U3–200S6M-C, Edmud optics #11–521) with 50mm lens (Edmud optics #63–248) were used to record videos at 10 Hz. For audio recording (Figs. 1,2,6), we used a 3D printed behavioral chamber with beveled circular arenas fitted with fine mesh below. The arenas measured 10 mm in diameter and 3 mm in height. Drosophila ezoana pairs (Fig. 6) were placed in arenas measuring 15 mm in diameter to accommodate their larger body size. Each arena was placed on top of a microphone in a custom 96-channel recording apparatus, SongTorrent, that enabled simultaneous audio (5 kHz) and video recording. To optimize video recording (Figs. 3–5) for behavioral tracking, we used acrylic behavioral chambers with circular arenas that measured 10 mm in diameter and 3 mm in height and did not perform audio recording. In all recordings of female-male pairs, the flies were separated by a divider until the start of the recording. Wildtype flies were recorded for 20 min. Optogenetic flies were recorded for the duration of the activation scheme.
In optogenetic activation experiments, flies were allowed to see in blue light and recorded under infrared light (850 nm). Red light (635 nm) was used for activation following a programmed cycle. An activation cycle consisted of 10 activation bouts with increasing intensity, and each 1 s bout was interspersed with 9 s intervals. The only exception to this activation scheme was the D. yakuba audio recording (Extended Data Figure 4a), which had 10 s activation bouts and 10 s intervals. Activation intensity gradient (in μW/mm2) was as follows: 0.4, 0.8, 1.2, 1.6, 2.0, 2.4, 2.9, 3.3, 3.7, and 4.1.
Behavioral tracking
SLEAP (v1.2.0a634) was used to track the behavior of interacting pairs (in wildtype experiments) or individual females (in optogenetic activation experiments). For pairs, we tracked the head, thorax, abdomen, and each of the wing tips for each fly. A classifier was trained using the multi-animal top-down mode with the default settings and the following modifications: anchor part=thorax, rotation min and max angles=−180 and 180, scale=TRUE, contrast=TRUE. Inference was run using a simple tracker with default settings and the following modifications: 2 instances/frame, cull to target instance count=TRUE, all nodes are used for tracking, and connect single track breaks=TRUE. The onset of pulse trains were used as key frames, and we focused on the interval between 2 s before to 3 s after the pulse onset. Manual adjustments were made wherever necessary.
For individuals, we tracked the head, thorax, abdomen, tip of the external genitalia, and each of the wing tips. A single classifier was trained using the single animal model with default settings, and intact and decapitated D. melanogaster, D. yakuba and D. santomea were included in the training dataset. Inference was run using a simple tracker with 1 instance/frame. We focused on 2 s before and after each 1 s activation bout. Manual adjustments were made wherever necessary.
Tracking data was exported as HDF5 files and analyzed in Python (v3.8.13) and R (v4.2.2) to calculate parameters such as the female wing angle and abdomen length. In wildtype recordings, female abdomen length was normalized to the baseline of each female, calculated as the mean abdomen length across the 20 frames (2 s) before each pulse onset. In optogenetic recordings, each female’s baseline abdomen length used for normalization was calculated as the mean abdomen length over the 100 frames (10 s) before the first activation bout. Wing angle change was calculated by subtracting the observed wing angle by each female’s baseline wing angle, which was calculated as the mean wing angle over the 100 frames before the first activation bout.
Behavioral analysis
Probability of WS in response to male song
The custom Matlab software Tempo (https://github.com/JaneliaSciComp/tempo) was used to annotate male songs, and when applicable, female WS in response to song. In Figs. 1c–f, 2b-e, and Extended Data Fig. 1 and 2, all pulse trains and WS were annotated manually. In Figs. 1b,g, if a male produced 20 or fewer pulse trains (or song trains in D. melanogaster and D. simulans), all pulse/song trains were annotated; otherwise 20 pulse/song trains were randomly sampled. Each pulse/song train’s co-occurrence with WS was then recorded.
Wildtype behavior
In wildtype recordings of non-D. santomea species (Fig. 5h,i and Fig. 6), full recordings were carefully examined for VPO and WS. To qualify as WS, a putative female wing extension behavior must occur in response a male courtship song and co-occur with VPO. These criteria were imposed to disambiguate WS from female wing flicking, grooming, and balancing after jumping. If VPO was not observed, it was typically associated with limited courtship history, non-ideal positioning of the female, and scoring challenge due to the subtlety of VPO in certain species.
Activation experiments
For vpoDN activation experiments in D. melanogaster, WS behaviors were manually identified by detecting wing angle changes in response to activation bouts. Responders (Fig. 5b and Extended Data Fig. 5c,d) were defined as females with at least one confirmed WS out of 10 activation bouts in an activation cycle. Responses to activation such as grooming, jumping and turning, and responses with low SLEAP tracking quality were considered invalid. Individuals with more than 5 invalid responses in the activation cycle were removed from Fig. 5b and Extended Data Fig. 5c,d. In Fig. 5b, invalid events were excluded when calculating WS and VPO rates of each individual. Notably, both intact and decapitated D. santomea females exhibited leaning and flipping over more frequently than the other species upon activation, possibly suggesting their lower resistance to activation. When females were not standing still, especially when leaning, SLEAP tended to underestimate the wing angle.
Immunostaining
Female brains and VNCs were dissected in 1X Phosphate Buffered Saline (PBS; Thermo Fisher) within 50 minutes of ice anesthesia, fixed with 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) for 35 min at room temperature rinsed 3 times in PBS with 1% Triton X-100 (PBTX), then blocked with 5% normal goat serum (NGS) in PBTX for 1.5 hours. Samples were incubated in primary antibodies (diluted in 5% NGS) at 4°C overnight. Samples were then washed with PBTX 3 times for 30 min each, and incubated with secondary antibodies (diluted in 5% NGS) at 4°C overnight. After 3 washes with PBTX, each 30 min, the samples were mounted with ProLong™ Gold antifade reagents (Fisher Scientific; Cat.#: P36931) on poly-L-lysine coated coverslips, and sealed on all slides with nail polish. Primary antibodies used were: chicken-anti-GFP (1:600, ab13970, Abcam), mouse-anti-nc82 (1:30, DHSB). Secondary antibodies used were: goat-anti-chicken/AF488 (1:500, A-11039, Thermo Fisher), goat-anti-mouse/AF568 (1:500, A-11031, Thermo Fisher). Confocal images were taken on a Leica DMi8 microscope with a TCS SP8 Confocal system at 40x, and processed with VVDViewer (v1.6.4).
Selection of vpoDN split-GAL4 lines
Three vpoDN split-GAL4 lines (SS1, SS2, and SS3)33 were each crossed to UAS-CsChrimson:mVenus flies to assess their ability in eliciting VPO upon optogenetic activation. Behavioral recording, tracking, and analysis were performed as described above. The line vpoDN-SS2 was chosen for further experiments as it had the most robust abdomen extension phenotype (Extended Data Fig.5a).
Statistical Analysis
Data analysis was performed in Python (v3.8.13) with the following packages: h5py (v3.6.0), numpy (v1.23.5), scipy (v1.8.0), and pandas (v1.4.2), and R (v4.2.2) with the following packages: tidyverse (v1.3.2), lme4 (v1.1–31), emmeans (v1.8.3), and lmerTest (v3.1–3). Scripts are available by request. Linear models and linear mixed models (to account for replicate effects and repeated measurements from the same subjects) were used to determine statistical significance. Variables that were proportions were arcsine-square root transformed to stabilize the variance. When there was significant deviation from the assumptions of linear models, non-parametric alternatives were used. Where relevant, p-values were adjusted for multiple testing using the Bonferroni correction.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Marc Schmdit, Troy Shirangi, Jan Clemens, and Ding lab members for discussion and comments on the draft. We thank David Stern lab for sharing genetic reagents, and David Anderson lab and Gerry Rubin lab for sharing plasmids for generating genetic reagents. We thank Steve Sawtelle for help with the behavioral recording system. This project was supported by Searle Scholarship and NIH grant R35GM142678 to Y.D. Drosophila illustrations were created with BioRender.
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1.Sturtevant A. H. Experiments on sex recognition and the problem of sexual selection in Drosoophilia. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 5, 351 (1915). [Google Scholar]
- 2.Greenspan R. J. & Ferveur J. F. Courtship in Drosophila. Annu. Rev. Genet. 34, 205–232 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Egnor S. R. & Seagraves K. M. The contribution of ultrasonic vocalizations to mouse courtship. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 38, 1–5 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Perkes A., White D., Wild J. M. & Schmidt M. Female Songbirds: The unsung drivers of courtship behavior and its neural substrates. Behav. Processes 163, 60–70 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mitoyen C., Quigley C. & Fusani L. Evolution and function of multimodal courtship displays. Ethology 125, 503–515 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Byers J., Hebets E. & Podos J. Female mate choice based upon male motor performance. Anim. Behav. 79, 771–778 (2010). [Google Scholar]
- 7.Aranha M. M. & Vasconcelos M. L. Deciphering Drosophila female innate behaviors. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 52, 139–148 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Baker C. A., Clemens J. & Murthy M. Acoustic Pattern Recognition and Courtship Songs: Insights from Insects. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 42, 129–147 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bastock M. Courtship: An Ethological Study. (Routledge, 2018). [Google Scholar]
- 10.Markow T. A. & O’Grady P. M. Evolutionary genetics of reproductive behavior in Drosophila: connecting the dots. Annu. Rev. Genet. 39, 263–291 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Andersson M. Sexual Selection. (Princeton University Press, 1994). [Google Scholar]
- 12.Seeholzer L. F., Seppo M., Stern D. L. & Ruta V. Evolution of a central neural circuit underlies Drosophila mate preferences. Nature 559, 564–569 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ding Y. et al. Neural Evolution of Context-Dependent Fly Song. Curr. Biol. 29, 1089–1099.e7 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Auer T. O. et al. Olfactory receptor and circuit evolution promote host specialization. Nature 579, 402–408 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sato K., Tanaka R., Ishikawa Y. & Yamamoto D. Behavioral Evolution of : Unraveling the Circuit Basis. Genes 11, (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Roberts R. J. V., Pop S. & Prieto-Godino L. L. Evolution of central neural circuits: state of the art and perspectives. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 725–743 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ah-King M. The history of sexual selection research provides insights as to why females are still understudied. Nat. Commun. 13, 6976 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sokolowski M. B. Drosophila: genetics meets behaviour. Nat. Rev. Genet. 2, 879–890 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Spieth H. T. Mating behavior within the genus Drosophila (Diptera). Bulletin of the AMNH; v. 99, article 7. (1952). [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bastock M. & Manning A. The Courtship of Drosophila Melanogaster. Behaviour 8, 85–110 (1955). [Google Scholar]
- 21.Connolly K. & Cook R. Rejection Responses by Female Drosophila melanogaster: Their Ontogeny, Causality and Effects upon the Behaviour of the Courting Male. Behaviour 44, 142–166 (1973). [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sturtevant A. H. The North American Species of Drosophila. (Carnegie Institution of Washington, 1921). [Google Scholar]
- 23.Vuoristo M., Isoherranen E. & Hoikkala A. Female wing spreading as acceptance signal in theDrosophila virilis group of species. J. Insect Behav. 9, 505–516 (1996). [Google Scholar]
- 24.Demetriades M. C., Thackeray J. R. & Kyriacou C. P. Courtship song rhythms in Drosophila yakuba. Anim. Behav. 57, 379–386 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Watson E. T., Rodewald E. & Coyne J. A. The courtship song of Drosophila santomea and a comparison to its sister species D. yakuba (Diptera: Drosophilidae). Eur. J. Entomol. 104, 145–148 (2007). [Google Scholar]
- 26.Blyth J. E., Lachaise D. & Ritchie M. G. Divergence in Multiple Courtship Song Traits between Drosophila santomea and D. yakuba. Ethology 114, 728–736 (2008). [Google Scholar]
- 27.Göpfert M. C. & Robert D. The mechanical basis of Drosophila audition. J. Exp. Biol. 205, 1199–1208 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Russo C. A., Takezaki N. & Nei M. Molecular phylogeny and divergence times of drosophilid species. Mol. Biol. Evol. 12, 391–404 (1995). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lachaise D. et al. Evolutionary novelties in islands: Drosophila santomea, a new melanogaster sister species from São Tomé. Proc. Biol. Sci. 267, 1487–1495 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Llopart A., Elwyn S., Lachaise D. & Coyne J. A. Genetics of a difference in pigmentation between Drosophila yakuba and Drosophila santomea. Evolution 56, 2262–2277 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Bachtrog D., Thornton K., Clark A. & Andolfatto P. Extensive introgression of mitochondrial DNA relative to nuclear genes in the Drosophila yakuba species group. Evolution 60, 292–302 (2006). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Turissini D. A. & Matute D. R. Fine scale mapping of genomic introgressions within the Drosophila yakuba clade. PLoS Genet. 13, e1006971 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Wang K. et al. Neural circuit mechanisms of sexual receptivity in Drosophila females. Nature 589, 577–581 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pereira T. D. et al. SLEAP: A deep learning system for multi-animal pose tracking. Nat. Methods 19, 486–495 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Lee G., Hall J. C. & Park J. H. Doublesex gene expression in the central nervous system of Drosophila melanogaster. J. Neurogenet. 16, 229–248 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Billeter J.-C., Rideout E. J., Dornan A. J. & Goodwin S. F. Control of male sexual behavior in Drosophila by the sex determination pathway. Curr. Biol. 16, R766–76 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Robinett C. C., Vaughan A. G., Knapp J.-M. & Baker B. S. Sex and the single cell. II. There is a time and place for sex. PLoS Biol. 8, e1000365 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rideout E. J., Dornan A. J., Neville M. C., Eadie S. & Goodwin S. F. Control of sexual differentiation and behavior by the doublesex gene in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 458–466 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Zhou C., Pan Y., Robinett C. C., Meissner G. W. & Baker B. S. Central brain neurons expressing doublesex regulate female receptivity in Drosophila. Neuron 83, 149–163 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Wang F., Wang K., Forknall N., Parekh R. & Dickson B. J. Circuit and Behavioral Mechanisms of Sexual Rejection by Drosophila Females. Curr. Biol. 30, 3749–3760.e3 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Duckhorn J. C. et al. Regulation of Drosophila courtship behavior by the Tlx/tailless-like nuclear receptor, dissatisfaction. Curr. Biol. 32, 1703–1714.e3 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mezzera C. et al. Ovipositor Extrusion Promotes the Transition from Courtship to Copulation and Signals Female Acceptance in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 30, 3736–3748.e5 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kimura K.-I., Sato C., Koganezawa M. & Yamamoto D. Drosophila ovipositor extension in mating behavior and egg deposition involves distinct sets of brain interneurons. PLoS One 10, e0126445 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Deutsch D. et al. The neural basis for a persistent internal state in Drosophila females. Elife 9, (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Schretter C. E. et al. Cell types and neuronal circuitry underlying female aggression in. Elife 9, (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Wang F. et al. Neural circuitry linking mating and egg laying in Drosophila females. Nature 579, 101–105 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Asahina K. et al. Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in Drosophila. Cell 156, 221–235 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Klapoetke N. C. et al. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nat. Methods 11, 338–346 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Rutherford S. L. & Lindquist S. Hsp90 as a capacitor for morphological evolution. Nature 396, 336–342 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hallgrímsson B., Willmore K. & Hall B. K. Canalization, developmental stability, and morphological integration in primate limbs. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Suppl 35, 131–158 (2002). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Irvine S. Q. Embryonic canalization and its limits-A view from temperature. J. Exp. Zool. B Mol. Dev. Evol. 334, 128–144 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Groh C., Tautz J. & Rössler W. Synaptic organization in the adult honey bee brain is influenced by brood-temperature control during pupal development. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 4268–4273 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Peng I.-F. et al. Temperature-dependent developmental plasticity of Drosophila neurons: cell-autonomous roles of membrane excitability, Ca2+ influx, and cAMP signaling. J. Neurosci. 27, 12611–12622 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kiral F. R. et al. Brain connectivity inversely scales with developmental temperature in Drosophila. Cell Rep. 37, 110145 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Neunuebel J. P., Taylor A. L., Arthur B. J. & Egnor S. E. R. Female mice ultrasonically interact with males during courtship displays. Elife 4, (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Staub N. L., Stiller A. B. & Kiemnec-Tyburczy K. M. A New Perspective on Female-to-Male Communication in Salamander Courtship. Integr. Comp. Biol. 60, 722–731 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Coyne J. A., Kim S. Y., Chang A. S., Lachaise D. & Elwyn S. Sexual isolation between two sibling species with overlapping ranges: Drosophila santomea and Drosophila yakuba. Evolution 56, 2424–2434 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Llopart A., Lachaise D. & Coyne J. A. Multilocus analysis of introgression between two sympatric sister species of Drosophila: Drosophila yakuba and D. santomea. Genetics 171, 197–210 (2005). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Namiki S., Dickinson M. H., Wong A. M., Korff W. & Card G. M. The functional organization of descending sensory-motor pathways in. Elife 7, (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Aymanns F., Chen C.-L. & Ramdya P. Descending neuron population dynamics during odor-evoked and spontaneous limb-dependent behaviors. Elife 11, (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Anderson D. J. & Adolphs R. A framework for studying emotions across species. Cell 157, 187–200 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Nair A. et al. An approximate line attractor in the hypothalamus encodes an aggressive state. Cell 186, 178–193.e15 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Perkes A., Pfrommer B., Daniilidis K., White D. & Schmidt M. Variation in female songbird state determines signal strength needed to evoke copulation. bioRxiv 2021.05.19.444794 (2021) doi: 10.1101/2021.05.19.444794. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Kimchi T., Xu J. & Dulac C. A functional circuit underlying male sexual behaviour in the female mouse brain. Nature 448, 1009–1014 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Clyne J. D. & Miesenböck G. Sex-specific control and tuning of the pattern generator for courtship song in Drosophila. Cell 133, 354–363 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Katz P. S. Evolution of central pattern generators and rhythmic behaviours. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 371, 20150057 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Rezával C. et al. Activation of Latent Courtship Circuitry in the Brain of Drosophila Females Induces Male-like Behaviors. Curr. Biol. 26, 2508–2515 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Gray D. A., Hormozi S., Libby F. R. & Cohen R. W. Induced expression of a vestigial sexual signal. Biol. Lett. 14, (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Wei Y.-C. et al. Medial preoptic area in mice is capable of mediating sexually dimorphic behaviors regardless of gender. Nat. Commun. 9, 279 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Arbas E. A. & Tolbert L. P. Presynaptic terminals persist following degeneration of ‘flight’ muscle during development of a flightless grasshopper. J. Neurobiol. 17, 627–636 (1986). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Zheng Z. et al. A Complete Electron Microscopy Volume of the Brain of Adult Drosophila melanogaster. Cell 174, 730–743.e22 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Guo C., Pan Y. & Gong Z. Recent Advances in the Genetic Dissection of Neural Circuits in Drosophila. Neurosci. Bull. 35, 1058–1072 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Scheffer L. K. et al. A connectome and analysis of the adult Drosophila central brain. Elife 9, (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Hulse B. K. et al. A connectome of the central complex reveals network motifs suitable for flexible navigation and context-dependent action selection. Elife 10, (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Phelps J. S. et al. Reconstruction of motor control circuits in adult Drosophila using automated transmission electron microscopy. Cell 184, 759–774.e18 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.True J. R. & Carroll S. B. Gene co-option in physiological and morphological evolution. Annu. Rev. Cell Dev. Biol. 18, 53–80 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.West-Eberhard M. J. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. (Oxford University Press, 2003). [Google Scholar]
- 78.Moczek A. P. et al. The role of developmental plasticity in evolutionary innovation. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 2705–2713 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Abouheif E. et al. Eco-evo-devo: the time has come. Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 781, 107–125 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Rajakumar R. et al. Ancestral developmental potential facilitates parallel evolution in ants. Science 335, 79–82 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Hoke K. L., Adkins-Regan E., Bass A. H., McCune A. R. & Wolfner M. F. Co-opting evo-devo concepts for new insights into mechanisms of behavioural diversity. J. Exp. Biol. 222, (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Pfeiffer B. D. et al. Tools for neuroanatomy and neurogenetics in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105, 9715–9720 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Stern D. L. Transgenic tools for targeted chromosome rearrangements allow construction of balancer chromosomes in non-melanogaster Drosophila species. G3 12, (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Stern D. L. et al. Genetic and Transgenic Reagents for Drosophila simulans, D. mauritiana, D. yakuba, D. santomea, and D. virilis. G3 7, 1339–1347 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Wu M. et al. Visual projection neurons in the lobula link feature detection to distinct behavioral programs. Elife 5, (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Suvorov A. et al. Widespread introgression across a phylogeny of 155 Drosophila genomes. Curr. Biol. 32, 111–123.e5 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Yeh S.-D., Liou S.-R. & True J. R. Genetics of divergence in male wing pigmentation and courtship behavior between Drosophila elegans and D. gunungcola. Heredity 96, 383–395 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Morales-Hojas R., Reis M., Vieira C. P. & Vieira J. Resolving the phylogenetic relationships and evolutionary history of the Drosophila virilis group using multilocus data. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 60, 249–258 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Wang A. et al. The conserved mitochondrial genomes of Drosophila mercatorum (Diptera: Drosophilidae) with different reproductive modes and phylogenetic implications. Int. J. Biol. Macromol. 138, 912–918 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Conner W. R. et al. A phylogeny for the Drosophila montium species group: A model clade for comparative analyses. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 158, 107061 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.