Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2023 Dec 21;18(12):e0295606. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0295606

Impact of school SES on literacy development

Naymé Salas 1,*, Mariona Pascual 1,2
Editor: Jie Wang3
PMCID: PMC10734911  PMID: 38127961

Abstract

Effective literacy skills are essential to actively participate in today’s society. However, little research has been conducted that examined the impact of contextual variables on literacy development. The present paper addressed whether and how the socioeconomic status of the school (S-SES) children attend affects their literacy achievements. Eight-hundred and seventy-eight 2nd and 4th grade children participated in the study. Data were collected in low-SES (vulnerable) and in mid-high-SES (non-vulnerable) schools. Children completed a large battery of language, cognitive, and literacy tasks in Catalan, a language spoken in a region in Spain where virtually all children are at least bilingual (they also speak Spanish) and it is the main language of instruction. Results showed that children in vulnerable schools were outperformed by children in non-vulnerable schools across all literacy competencies, but particularly affected higher order skills; that is, text quality and reading comprehension. Differences with their non-vulnerable peers remained, even after controlling for context-level covariates, including familial SES. However, S-SES ceased to exert significant influence once children’s cognitive and, especially, linguistic skills were considered. The study adds to previous research claiming that school SES has an effect on students’ literacy skills, above and beyond children’s home SES. However, our findings also suggest that literacy performance is ultimately mostly dependent on educationally actionable, subject-level skills. Educational implications are discussed.

Introduction

Academic and professional success are highly dependent on learning to read and write effectively [1, 2]. Moreover, competent reading and writing are not only necessary to understand and to communicate ideas at school and in professional environments, but they are both powerful learning tools [3]. Learning to read and to write are, thus, the cornerstones on which equality of opportunity rests. In recent decades, the role of subject-level factors (e.g., oral language, cognitive skills) on literacy development has been investigated extensively [47]. However, comparatively little research has been conducted that examined the impact of contextual variables on literacy, particularly in non-English-speaking contexts. The present paper addressed whether and how children’s socioeconomic environment affects their achievement in literacy competencies.

Socioeconomic status’ effects on learning to read and write

A child’s socioeconomic status (SES) is a measure that results from a combination of three main aspects: parental education, income or material resources at the household, and parental occupation [8, 9]. Often, a measure of one or more of these aspects is used to determine its effect on development or behavior [10, 11]. Previous meta-analytic studies have consistently found that a child’s SES has a relatively small, but consistent and significant impact on their development, including language and literacy [1214]. The mechanisms through which SES and literacy achievement are related are not entirely clear [9]. The nature of parent-child factors may be partly responsible for children’s literacy outcomes [10, 15]. In this sense, some studies have pointed out that parental education is more important than their occupation or income to predict children’s academic achievement [16], probably because children are typically exposed to reading and writing practices more often and from birth in households with more educated adults; and more exposure has been found to lead to better outcomes [17]. In any case, there is abundant evidence that children’s early literacy experiences, which are highly influenced by familial SES, set the stage for their subsequent literacy development [18]. Importantly, children are not only sensitive to the SES of their familial context. A number of large-scale studies have provided compelling evidence that the SES of the school children attend has a substantial impact on literacy development above and beyond the effect of child-level SES [13, 19, 20]. For example, the 2010 OECD report for literacy achievements indicated that not only was school SES relevant to explaining findings, but it was found to be even more strongly related to reading outcomes than individual SES [8]. There does not seem to be a consensus over what are the drivers of differing academic performance as a function of school SES. School composition has been proposed as a key factor in academic achievement in several studies. In most of these studies, school SES has been operationalized as the average of students’ individual SES [13, 2124]. Moreover, some studies point to the fact that school climate, teacher practices and attitudes, as well as emphasis on academic achievement may be found to differ in low-SES schools when compared to more affluent schools [25]. In short, SES at the individual but, especially, at the school level has been shown to significantly affect literacy outcomes [2426]. Nonetheless, most research has been conducted in English-speaking populations and has focused on overall academic achievement, rather than on specific literacy skills. Given the importance attributed to school SES in past research, the present paper intended to examine its role in reading, as well as in writing achievements in primary school in a non-English-speaking context: Catalonia (Spain).

Key literacy skills

Reading and writing allow us to understand and interact with others in the social context in which we live [2729]. Reading is the ability to (re)construct the meaning of a text, by generating a mental representation of what the text says, and to integrate such representation with previous knowledge [30]. Reading thus involves the execution of word-level processes, chiefly, word reading, as well as text-level processes, such as comprehension monitoring or inference resolution. These processes are supported by domain-general cognitive resources such as working memory or inhibitory control [31], while they require strong orthographic and linguistic representations at the phonological, morphological, syntactic, and discursive-pragmatic levels [32, 33]. Writing is a complex skill that involves transforming ideas into linguistic formulations, which are transposed into scripted material that serves a communicative function [27, 34, 35]. Writers engage in a series of recursive domain-specific processes, including generating and organizing ideas, setting themselves rhetorical goals, searching for relevant content, as well as revising text [3638]. An important milestone in writing development is the automatization of transcription skills, that is, spelling and handwriting; otherwise, these mechanical aspects of writing overload working memory resources, resulting in poor texts [39]. Similarly to reading, research has accrued that writing competence at the sub-lexical (i.e., handwriting), lexical (i.e., spelling), and text levels rests on an individual’s domain-general cognitive resources, e.g., working memory [40], and linguistic representations, e.g., morphosyntactic skills, vocabulary [33]. In order to ascertain the specific impact of school SES, we collected estimates of important predictors of reading and writing, including general non-verbal intelligence, other domain-general cognitive skills (working memory, inhibition), as well Rapid Associative Naming (RAN), a measure of processing speed, because of its known impact on word- and text-level literacy across languages [4, 41, 42]. Additionally, we obtained measures of children’s linguistic skills (grammar and vocabulary). These measures were used as covariates to estimate S-SES impact on literacy outcomes above and beyond known predictors of literacy competence.

This study

The main aim of the present study was to examine the impact of school SES (henceforth, S-SES) on children’s literacy achievements. We were interested in how S-SES affected grade 2 and grade 4 students learning to read and write in Catalan. Consequently, we collected data in vulnerable and more affluent public schools. Crucially, “vulnerable schools” were regarded as such using the criteria and classification of the Catalan Department of Education (more details in Method), rather than computing the average of children’s individual SES in each school. At the participating schools, we administered tasks to estimate children’s reading (e.g., word identification, reading comprehension) and writing (e.g., spelling, writing productivity, and writing quality) skills and conducted MANCOVA tests to determine whether S-SES had an impact on such skills. Across analyses we controlled for demographic factors: children’s gender and age. Moreover, we investigated whether S-SES effects were modified when we also controlled for contextual factors: child-level SES (henceforth, C-SES) and children’s exposure to the language of instruction outside school (Catalan). Finally, we examined whether S-SES effects remained after also controlling for a host of subject-level factors (e.g., IQ, vocabulary, working memory) known to affect literacy development. The study thus improves on previous research by (1) addressing an under-researched, non-English-speaking context; (2) comparing schools on the basis of external, objective assessments of SES composition, rather than simply averaging the individual SES of children in classrooms; (3) controlling for a large number of demographic, contextual, and individual-level factors with a substantial record of impacting literacy performance; and (4) considering the full range of literacy skills, both reading and writing, at the (sub)word- and text-levels. Our research questions (RQs) were, thus,

  • RQ1: Does school SES affect literacy achievement in 2nd- and 4th-grade students above and beyond the effect of individual SES?

  • RQ2: Which literacy skills (e.g., handwriting, reading comprehension) are most sensitive to S-SES?

  • RQ3: Does school SES have an impact on literacy achievements above and beyond the effect of known predictors of reading and writing performance?

With regards to RQ1, we expected that, in line with previous studies, school SES would make a significant impact on literacy scores even after controlling for demographic variables and the child’s individual SES. Given that previous research has found that both reading and writing are sensitive to SES differences [13, 43], we expected an impact of S-SES across all literacy skills. As for RQ3, we were ambivalent as to the effect of S-SES on literacy skills after controlling for known predictors of performance since, to the best of our knowledge, no studies have addressed this issue to the extent that we have; that is, including the most relevant cognitive and linguistic underpinnings of reading and writing, in addition to demographic and contextual variables. Because individual SES and S-SES have been found to have a consistent but only small-to-moderate effect on literacy outcomes [12], it is possible that the influence of S-SES can no longer be observed after controlling for highly stable literacy-related factors. Nevertheless, we believe that, given that we have used a different means to classify schools as vulnerable, where low-SES overlapped with other risk factors (e.g., low to null exposure to the language of instruction), S-SES might still have an effect above-and-beyond our chosen demographic, contextual, and subject-level covariates.

The Catalan context

This study was carried out in Catalonia, a region of Spain where the population is typically bilingual (Catalan-Spanish). Catalan has a semi-opaque orthography [4446], more consistent than English, but less so than Spanish. It is morphosyntactically similar to Spanish, as they are both Latin-rooted Romance languages. Importantly, Catalan is the language of instruction across the public school system in Catalonia. Nonetheless, not all children have the same degree of exposure to Catalan outside school. Some children (e.g., with an immigrant background) are usually not exposed to Catalan outside of the school environment [47, 48]. Moreover, children in disadvantaged areas, who are typically not exposed to the language of instruction for everyday communication, may take longer to develop linguistic skills in Catalan [49]. Because the particular context of the study may affect the impact of both C-SES and S-SES on literacy outcomes, we obtained information about children’s exposure to the language of instruction.

Materials and methods

Participants

The parents or legal guardians of 436 2nd graders and 437 4th graders gave explicit, written consent to their children participating in the study (N = 873). All data were collected in compliance with the guidelines of the Ethics Commission for Animal and Human Experimentation (CEEAH) of the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona. Table 1 shows basic demographic information for all participants. Children were recruited in socioeconomically vulnerable and non-vulnerable schools in Barcelona and neighboring cities. The Catalan Department of Education applies a set of criteria to identify each type of school. Schools with a high proportion of students from low-income households, who typically do not speak the language of instruction outside of the school, and whose parents generally have low educational levels are labeled “high-complexity” educational centers (henceforth, “vulnerable” schools, VSs). On the other hand, regular or “non-vulnerable” schools (NVSs) are those where children usually come from mid- to high-income backgrounds, whose parents have often completed higher education studies, and who are frequently exposed to Catalan outside the school setting. Upon identification, vulnerable schools are eligible to receive more resources than other schools, including a higher ratio of teachers per student and a more diverse body of professionals to attend to students’ needs [50]. In our study, we compared literacy attainment in “vulnerable” schools to “non-vulnerable” schools, using the classification by the Catalan Department of Education to ascertain the role of S-SES.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and distribution of participants by Grade and School Type.

Non-vulnerable schools Vulnerable schools
Grade 2 Grade 4 Grade 2 Grade 4
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
N (boys) 268 (140) 279 (149) 168 (70) 158 (88)
Mean age 8;8 (0;3) 10;10 (0;4) 8;8 (0;4) 10;10 (0;3)
ISEI Score 54.45 (13.86) 53.38 (14.29) 39.29 (12.09) 37.97 (9.97)
Catalan Exposure 1.29 (1.06) 1.33 (1.05) 0.27 (0.54) 0.21 (0-47)

Note. ISEI = International Socio-economic Index Score [51], range 20—90: Catalan Exposure range (0-2.5).

Gender distribution was similar across school types, Chi2(1) = 1.56, p = .212, and between grades, Chi2(1) = 1.07, p = .302. We administered a sociolinguistic questionnaire to the children, with the assistance and verification of the teachers. Such a procedure was preferred to directly addressing students’ parents because of access issues, and because a large proportion of the sample included children with illiterate parents who, often, did not speak Catalan or Spanish. The main aim of the questionnaire was to determine parents’ occupation and educational level, as well as exposure to Catalan outside the school setting.

Child-level SES

We used the International Socioeconomic Index (ISEI) by Ganzeboom et al. [51], which allowed us to assign a score on a continuous scale according to occupation. We inquired about the occupations of the child’s parents or, alternatively, of their legal guardian. Although the research by Ganzeboom et al. [51] only includes occupation-ISEI score matches for men, we obtained ISEI scores also for women parents and used the average of both parents as the final variable of C-SES, a procedure that has proven reliable in previous studies [52]. A two-way ANOVA showed that the average ISEI score (i.e., child-level SES) of children in VSs was significantly lower than that of children attending NVSs, F(1, 759) = 242.94, p <.001, η2 = .242. There were no significant differences between grades, F (1, 759) = 1.50, p = .221, and there was no Grade by School Type interaction, F < 1,p = .900.

Exposure to Catalan

We also used the sociolinguistic questionnaire to inquire about children’s exposure to the language of instruction (Catalan) outside of the school setting. Specifically, we asked each child which language(s) they used to talk to their parents, siblings, and friends. When Catalan was the only language used to speak to either parent (or the legal guardian), we assigned a score of 1 to each parent. In cases of single-parent families, the score was doubled; that is, when Catalan was the chief means of communication within a monoparental family, we assigned a score of 2. If a child typically used either of two languages (e.g., Catalan or Spanish) to talk to one of their parents, a score of 0.5 was assigned to that parent. If a child would use three possible languages with a parent (e.g., Catalan, Spanish, German), that parent was assigned a score of 0.33. When children reported using Catalan to speak to friends or siblings, we added 0.25 points to the final score for each type of interlocutor (friends, siblings). The final score thus ranged between 0 to 2.5. Children in VSs were significantly less exposed to Catalan outside school than children in NVSs, F(1, 807) = 263.83, p <.001, η2 = .247, and this exposure was not different as a function of Grade, F < 1, p = .923, while there were no significant interactions, F < 1, p <.461.

Tasks and measures

We administered a series of standardized and mostly adapted or bespoke instruments for collecting data on children literacy competence and related skills. The main reason behind the use of non-standardized instruments was that only reading comprehension tests are available that have been standardized for Catalan-speaking school-aged children. Whenever possible, we used adaptations of tests and subtests standardized for Spanish (e.g., Raven’s matrices, to measure non-verbal IQ; or the Digits subtest from the WISC-IV test battery [61, 64]). In the section below, we provide details of the adaptations that have been made and their underlying criteria. We are certain that validity was not an issue with any adaptation nor with bespoke tasksm since they have all been used in previous studies for virtually identical purposes (e.g., alphabet task to measure handwriting fluency [53]). However, we report our own reliability calculations to determine the degree to which the adapted or researcher-designed instruments were reliable.

Handwriting fluency

We used this test to assess handwriting skills. Children wrote the alphabet as fast as possible in 60 seconds [53]. The final score was the sum of all correctly written letters that they were able to produce in the first 15 seconds. In order not to penalize children in centers where the alphabet was not taught [54], order of the letters was not taken into account. Inter-rater reliability on a random 20% of the sample was of.984 (ICC).

Spelling accuracy

To assess spelling, children were asked to write 34 words from a bespoke dictation task [55]. Words were presented in one of two fixed, pseudo-randomized orders, which were counterbalanced. Items in the dictation task were also counterbalances for frequency, length in syllables, and syllabic complexity. Each word was delivered out loud in isolation, contextualized using a carrier sentence, and then repeated in isolation again. The final score was the sum of all correctly written words. Inter-rater reliability on a random 20% of tasks was.989 (ICC). The task’s internal consistency had Cronbach’s α of.79.

Text writing

Each child produced a narrative and an argumentative text (opinion essay). There were two prompts for each genre, which were administered randomly to each classroom. The opinion-essay prompts were (1) Do you think that all children your age should go to school? or (2) Do you think that there should be recess time at school?. The narrative prompts were (1) Write a story about a child who has lost his/her pet or (2) Write a story about a child who is angry at his/her friend. A research assistant presented the prompt and gave children up to 10 minutes to plan their text and then up to 20 minutes to write the text. Both text samples were transcribed by two trained research assistants in CHAT-compatible format [56]. Spelling mistakes were corrected and punctuation was minimally revised or corrected to facilitate the readability of the texts. Transcription fidelity between research assistants was.996 (ICC). From these transcriptions, a number of text-based measures were obtained: (1) the total number of words, to estimate text productivity, was automatically counted with CLAN [55]. All words were counted, regardless of spelling mistakes. Also, (2) texts were evaluated for holistic text quality by at least two independent raters. Ratings ranged from 0 (considerably below grade-level expectations) to 5 (considerably above grade-level expectations). Grade-level expectation followed the milestones and requirements established in the Primary Education curriculum of the Catalan Government for each grade level (Decree 119/2015) [57]. Raters were trained for a specific grade level (2nd or 4th) to ensure consistency and age-appropriate assessment. In cases where raters had a discrepancy of more than 1 point, a third independent rater assessed the text. The final score was the average between the two (or three) ratings. Raters used the Text Handler app (for further details on the procedure and training, see [58]). Interrater reliability (ICC) was.857, for grade 2, and.725, for grade 4.

Word reading

Children were administered the word reading subtest of the PROLEC-R battery [59]. The test invites children to read 40 words aloud of varying complexity. The number of words read correctly is tallied and the time is registered. The score is the number of words accurately read in one minute. Reported reliability in the manual is Cronbach’s α = .79.

Reading comprehension

We administered an adapted version of the ACL: Avaluació de la Comprensió Lectora ‘Reading comprehension Evaluation’ test [60]. The test includes a collection of brief texts of various genres, with multiple-choice questions to measure children’s understanding of literal information, inferences, and their global understanding of the text. The manuals provide a different instrument for each grade level (from 1st through to 6th). Test instructions indicate that 2nd grade children are administered the 1st-grade instrument (3rd-grade children are administered the 2nd-grade instruments, and so on), especially if tested during the first trimester of the school year (as was our case), in order to determine that they have acquired grade-level reading comprehension competences. To prevent ceiling-effects, we administered a slightly longer version of the 1st-grade and 3rd-grade instruments (to our 2nd- and 4th-grade participants, respectively), where we included two more items obtained from the 2nd- and 4th-grade instruments, respectively. Children had up to 45 minutes to complete the test. The score was the total proportion of correct responses. Reliability (Chronbach’s α) was.816, for the 2nd-grade instrument, and.898, for the 4th-grade instrument.

Non-verbal IQ

Non-verbal intelligence was assessed with the Progressive Raven Matrices test [61]. All children completed the five sets of 12 tasks in 45 minutes. The manual reports a reliability of.86 (Cronbach’s α).

Rapid Associative Naming (RAN)

To assess speed of processing, children were administered a RAN-letters task [62]. Children were asked to name five letters of the alphabet (a, s, d, p, o), displayed randomly on an A4 sheet 40 times (in five rows of eight letters each), as fast as they could. There was one practice trial, only to ensure children were familiar with the letters and their names, and two test trials. The final score was the mean time to complete each trial. Reliability was the correlation between test trials, r = .834.

Inhibition

To assess the executive function of inhibition, children were administered the Same-word, Opposite world test [63]. In the “same world” trials, children had to say outloud the numbers ‘1’ or ‘2’ printed on a path, as fast as possible. After a same-world, baseline trial, two-opposite world test trials ensued, where children were asked to say ther number opposite to the one printed on the path (e.g., say “one” if they saw number “2”), as fast as they could; that is, they had to inhibit a prepotent response. The final score was the mean time across the two opposite-world trials. Reliability was the correlation between test trials, r = .823.

Working memory

To assess working memory we used the Digits subtest of the Spanish adaptation of the WISC-IV test battery [64]. Children had to recall lists of digits that increased from two to eight in length. Two practice items preceded test items in both the forward (i.e., repeating digits in the same order as they are delivered) and backward (i.e., repeat digits in reverse order) conditions. As per the manual’s instructions, the test was discontinued if two items within the same list-length were recalled incorrectly. Each correctly recalled string was awarded 1 point, and the overall score was the sum of both direct and reverse tasks. Reliability was.74 (Cronbach’s α).

Vocabulary

We used an adaptation of the Vocabulary subtest of the WISC-IV Spanish test battery [64] to assess Catalan vocabulary. Expert researchers and teachers selected items similar to the words in the Spanish version. We took into account, especially, context and frequency of use, age of acquisition, and difficulty. The final test consisted of 26 items of increasing difficulty, where children had to provide oral definitions. Scoring was based on depth and specificity as per the manual’s instructions, and Spanish definitions were accepted. The final score was the sum of all points obtained. A trained RA rescored a random 16% of the sample, showing high consistency of scoring (ICC = .958). Internal consistency was Cronbach’s α = .887.

Morphosyntactic knowledge

We assessed receptive morphosyntactic skills using an adaptation of a sentence-reading test of the PROLEC-R [59]. The administrator read a sentence out loud while the child was shown four alternative pictures. Children were asked to point to the picture that best matched the aurally-delivered sentence. The test had 16 items of increasing difficulty. One point was given for each correct answer and the final score was the sum of all correct responses. Reliability was Cronbach’s α = .83.

Results

Psychometric properties of variables, descriptive statistics, and analytical strategy

Table 2 shows the means and standard deviations of all outcome and predictor variables by grade and school type. Skewness and kurtosis values were assessed to detect abnormalities in the distribution of values. Absolute skewness values ranged between 0.13 and 2.82, while absolute kurtosis values ranged from 0.21 to 14.82, indicating a relatively normal distribution [65]. We computed z-scores for all outcome measures, for each grade separately. For the sake of parsimony, and to avoid highly-correlated dependent variables, we computed composite variables for the number of words variables and the text quality variables, using the mean of the standardized score for each genre (narrative and argumentative). Missing values were also assessed for all intervening variables. We investigated variables in which missing values exceeded 5%. These were C-SES (14.8%), Exposure to Catalan (9.5%), Spelling accuracy (8.3%), Word Reading (5.5%), and Reading Comprehension (8.1%). To determine whether the rate of missing values was the same across School Types (the variable of interest), we ran Chi2 tests for 2x2 contingency tables for each of the variables with missing values above 5%. These tests indicated that for Catalan Exposure, Spelling Accuracy, the Number of Words composite variable, and Reading Comprehension, there were significantly more missing values in the vulnerable than in the non-vulnerable schools. These missing value rates are not uncommon in research on Education [66], while there are authors that claim that missing values under 10% should not bias results [67]. In this sense, our only variable with a larger percentage of missing values.

Table 2. Grade and School Type.

Non-vulnerable Schools Vulnerable Schools
Grade 2 Grade 4 Grade 2 Grade 4
M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Subject-level predictor variables
Non-verbal IQ1 27.18 (10.51) 38.27 (8.36) 24.58 (9.90) 31.95 (10.79)
WM1 11.36 (2.20) 12.73 (2.49) 9.87 (2.25) 11.45 (2.35)
Inhibition2 52.59 (12.36) 40.18 (7.96) 53.24 (11.17) 41.98 (9.07)
RAN2 31.90 (10.38) 22.12 (5.73) 33.21 (8.78) 22.70 (5.71)
Vocabulary1 14.34 (4.98) 19.38 (4.38) 9.67 (4.28) 14.76 (4.84)
MS skills3 66.51 (17.78) 74.11 (16.04) 55.06 (16.02) 67.74 (14.01)
Outcome Literacy Variables
Handwriting 5.20 (1.95) 8.40 (2.77) 4.93 (1.84) 7.88 (2.89)
Spelling1 8.68 (6.19) 13.85 (6.00) 6.51 (5.72) 11.37 (5.99)
Productivity OP4 22.98 (17.08) 46.69 (24.71) 21.53 (15.11) 41.62 (25.84)
Productivity NA4 60.82 (35.88) 113.76 (54.05) 36.01 (31.71) 86.97 (53.87)
Text quality OP6 0.81 (0.60) 0.83 (0.58) 0.56 (0.55) 0.63 (0.55)
Text quality NA6 1.34 (0.69) 1.47 (0.89) 0.88 (0.69) 0.97 (0.81)
Word reading6 23.02 (15.96) 43.55 (23.32) 19.40 (15.96) 39.19 (20.84)
Reading comprehension3 63.43 (20.47) 65.30 (21.17) 48.44 (19.50) 47.96 (22.03)

Note. WM = working memory; MS = morphosyntactic; ISEI = International Socio-economic Index Score [51], range 20—90: Catalan Exposure range (0-2.5); OP = opinion essay; NA = narrative text.

1 Raw score

2 In seconds

3 Percentage correct

4 Number of words

5 z-score average of OP and NA texts

6 range: 0-5

(C-SES) presented a similar missingness rate across school types (p >.05). However, readers should take into account the potential for bias in the findings to be reported next. Sample size was imbalanced across groups, with a larger number of students in the non-vulnerable schools than in the vulnerable ones. Although we attempted to recruit a similar number of students for both groups, classroom size is often slightly smaller in vulnerable schools to comply with teacher-student ratios that are mandatory in “high-complexity” schools (Resolució ENS/906/2014, 2014 [50]). To avoid bias in the analyses, we used the Weighted Least Square (WLS) option in SPSS, to have balanced (50/50) sample sizes. To answer our research questions we adopted a four-phase procedure. First, we conducted a one-way MANCOVA for all outcome measures, with School Type as the fixed factor and with Gender and Age as covariates. Given that we had standardized all values for each grade separately, there was no need to include Grade in any analyses. Second, we conducted a one-way MANCOVA, identical to the first, but we added the contextual variables of Exposure to Catalan and C-SES as covariates. Third, we ran a MANCOVA where we also included a number of variables that measured children’s levels on a series of cognitive skills; specifically, non-verbal IQ, Rapid Associative Naming (RAN), inhibition, and working memory. Fourth, we added linguistic skills as covariates; specifically, receptive morphosyntax and vocabulary. Across MANCOVAs we assessed the impact of School Type on all literacy outcome measures: word reading, handwriting, spelling, reading comprehension, and text composition. We used MANCOVA particularly to avoid Type I errors resulting from conducting several tests on related, dependent variables [68]. Analyses were carried out using SPSS v.27. Box’s test of equality of covariance matrices was significant across MANCOVAs. Therefore, we used Pillai’s trace criterion to assess multivariate significance, since it is the most robust when this assumption is not met [68]. We compared effect sizes (partial eta-squared) for all significant School-Type effects across our phases of analysis. In order to obtain robust estimates, all analyses included a bootstrapping procedure of 2,000 samples.

School SES effects on literacy achievement

Our first MANCOVA tested the main effect of School Type, while controlling for the effects of Gender and Age, which were entered as covariates into the model. Results showed that School Type had a large significant effect on the dependent variables, F(6, 742) = 24.98, p <.001, ηp2 = .168. Age was non-significant, F(6, 742) = 0.47, p = .835, ηp2 = .004, while Gender was a significant covariate, with a medium effect size, F(6, 742) = 8.95, p <.001, ηp2 = .068, whereby girls outperformed boys. Between-subject analyses indicated that School Type had an effect on all outcome variables, which favored children in non-vulnerable schools. The School Type effect was thus statistically significant and of medium size for handwriting fluency, F(1) = 8.31, p = .004, ηp2 = .011, spelling accuracy, F(1) = 21.50, p <.001, ηp2 = .028, text productivity, F(1) = 27.51, p <.001, ηp2 = .036, and word reading, F(1) = 11.21, p <.001, ηp2 = .015. The impact of School Type on text quality, F(1) = 85.27, p <.001, ηp2 = .102, and reading comprehension, F(1) = 117.48, p <.001, ηp2 = .136, was also significant and of a considerably larger size. In short, after controlling for basic demographic variables, children attending vulnerable schools showed a poorer performance across all literacy skills. Our second MANCOVA included, in addition to Gender and Age, C-SES and Exposure to Catalan, as context-level control variables. This analysis also revealed a significant effect of School Type on the dependent variables, albeit of reduced size, F(6, 625) = 4.61, p <.001, ηp2 = .042. Age remained a non-significant factor, F(6, 625) = 0.45, p = .843, ηp2 = .004, while Gender remained a significant covariate, F(6, 625) = 7.93, p <.001, ηp2 = .071. The two additional, context-level covariates had a significant impact on literacy achievement levels: both Exposure to Catalan, F(6, 625) = 3.95, p <.001, ηp2 = .036, and C-SES, F(6, 625) = 7.24, p <.001, ηp2 =.065, were positively associated with higher scores; in other words, more exposure to the language of instruction outside school and a higher SES family background were each associated with higher literacy performance scores, regardless of the type of school children attended. After controlling for demographic and context-level variables, the School Type effect was no longer significant for handwriting fluency, F(1) = 0.46, p =.498, ηp2 =.001, spelling accuracy, F(1) = 1.12, p =.291, ηp2 =.002, text productivity, F(1) = 2.92, p =.088, ηp2 =.005, or word reading, F(1) = 2.83, p =.093, ηp2 =.004. However, children in vulnerable schools were still outscored by children in non-vulnerable schools in text quality, F(1) = 13.64, p <.001, ηp2 =.021, and reading comprehension, F(1) = 12.22, p <.001, ηp2 =.025. All in all, attending a vulnerable school resulted in a disadvantage for children’s literacy skills, particularly for the development of text composition and comprehension, above and beyond the effect of crucial demographic and context-level variables. Our third MANCOVA tested the effect of School Type, controlling for demographic and contextual variables, while adding relevant cognitive skills as covariates. School Type remained a significant factor, although the effect size was small, F(6, 590) = 2.73, p =.013, ηp2 =.027. Age did not have a significant effect on literacy outcomes, F(6, 590) = 0.28, p =.946, ηp2 =.003, while Gender did, F(6, 590) = 7.88, p <.001, ηp2 =.074. Although children’s exposure to Catalan remained a significant factor explaining literacy performance, F(6, 590) = 4.51, p <.001, ηp2 =.044, C-SES was no longer significant, F(6, 590) = 1.34, p =.236, ηp2 =.013. All cognitive-skill variables had a significant effect on literacy outcomes. The largest one was RAN, F(6, 590) = 19.67, p <.001, ηp2 =.167, followed by non-verbal IQ, F(6, 590) = 15.24, p <.001, ηp2 =.134, and working memory, F(6, 590) = 12.53, p <.001, ηp2 =.113. Finally, inhibition also had a small-size effect on literacy outcomes, F(6, 590) = 3.89, p <.001, ηp2 =.038. Between-subject analyses showed that the School Type effect was only significant for text quality, F(1) = 7.99, p =.005, ηp2 =.013, and reading comprehension, F(1) = 6.94, p =.009, ηp2 =.009; however, it was non-significant for handwriting fluency, F(1) = 1.43, p =.233, ηp2 =.002; spelling, F(1) = 0.04, p =.847, ηp2 <.001; text productivity, F(1) = 1.47, p =.226, ηp2 =.002; or word reading, F(1) = 1.38, p =.240, ηp2 =.002. Finally, in our fourth MANCOVA we tested the School Type effect when adding oral-language skills (receptive morphosyntax and vocabulary) to our host of covariates. Analyses showed the School Type effect was no longer significant, F(6, 587) = 0.83, p =.548, ηp2 =.008. As in previous analyses, Gender had a significant effect on literacy outcomes, F(6, 587) = 9.78, p <.001, ηp2 =.091, while Age did not, F(6, 587) = 0.348, p =.917, ηp2 =.003. C-SES did not have an effect on literacy performance over and above the other variables in the model, F(6, 587) = 0.88, p =.507, ηp2 =.003, but children’s exposure to the language of instruction continued to exert significant influence on literacy skills, F(6, 587) = 3.67, p =.001, ηp2 =.036. Cognitive covariates maintained their significant influence on literacy outcomes, although some were reduced in size. This was the case for non-verbal IQ, F(6, 590) = 10.07, p <.001, ηp2 =.043, and working memory, F(6, 590) = 9.45, p <.001, ηp2 =.088. Inhibition had an effect virtually identical to the previous analysis, F(6, 590) = 3.88, p <.001, ηp2 =.038, while RAN continued to show a large effect size on literacy skills, F(6, 590) = 18.56, p <.001, ηp2 =.159. Both oral-language covariates had a medium to large effect on literacy outcomes, F(6, 590) = 5.82, p <.001, ηp2 =.056, for Receptive morphosyntactic skills, and F(6, 590) = 15.01, p <.001, ηp2 =.133, for Vocabulary. To sum up, children in non-vulnerable schools outperformed children in vulnerable schools across literacy outcomes, even after controlling for several demographic and contextual factors, as well as for a series of key cognitive skills. The type of school children attended influenced, in particular, higher-order literacy skills, such as text-composition quality and reading comprehension. It was only when considering oral-language factors that School SES ceased to have a significant impact on literacy performance. Notably, children’s home SES was generally less critical than School SES; moreover, it was no longer a relevant factor for reading and writing performance when children’s cognitive skills and, in particular, their language skills, were included in the analyses.

Discussion

This paper aimed to examine disparities in literacy learning that may exist between children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Specifically, our goal was to understand the impact that attending socioeconomically vulnerable schools may have on children’s learning and development of reading and writing skills. To accomplish our goal, we recruited a large sample of children attending public schools, which differed greatly in their socioeconomic makeup, and contrasted their reading and writing performances while controlling for several covariates. A major finding of the study was that S-SES had an effect across reading and writing skills, even after controlling for children’s individual SES. This finding is well aligned with previous studies, which indicated that the composition of the school, particularly its socioeconomic characteristics, has an impact on academic and, specifically, literacy achievements, above and beyond the effect of familial SES [13]. It should be noted that most previous research on the topic operationalized school SES as the average or aggregation of children’s individual SES [9]. In this sense, we have arrived at the same conclusion, but using a different means of classifying educational centers as vulnerable or non-vulnerable, using an external, objective set of criteria. Therefore, the present study provides additional support to the claim that schools with higher proportions of socioeconomically disadvantaged students obtain poorer literacy scores and are at a higher risk of school failure. When C-SES and children’s exposure to the language of instruction were considered in the models, the effect of S-SES remained, but it was reduced in size. It also limited its scope of influence only to higher-order literacy skills: the quality of written text productions and reading comprehension, whereas more basic, mechanical aspects of literacy were spared: handwriting, spelling, the amount of text generated, and word reading. Put differently, the negative impact on literacy scores resulting from attending a vulnerable school was ameliorated by children’s familial SES and their familiarity with (and, possibly, competence in) the language of instruction. The fact that children coming from households with more highly educated parents, who also tend to have better income levels, cannot elude the negative impact of attending a vulnerable school requires further consideration. Previous literature has indicated that a literacy-rich home environment acts as a facilitator of later literacy development [17]. In part, this may be because such homes are usually those where parents have more intellectual and material resources (including more time) to both be aware of the importance of literacy for their child and to have frequent, high-quality, literacy-related interactions with their children (e.g., joint reading or writing [69]). These households usually present a myriad of opportunities for children to witness literacy practices, enhancing their understanding of the social functions of reading and writing, from a very early age. In consequence, written language, as a domain of knowledge, is not at all foreign to such children when they enter school. In contrast, children from lower-income, low educational-level households will only rarely be exposed to literate practices and may have literacy-related interactions of lesser quality, less often or not at all. As a result, written language may be perceived by them as a skill that is dissociated from their out-of-school experience. All in all, it is noteworthy that literacy-rich environments were insufficient to compensate for the impact that attending a vulnerable school had on higher-order literacy skills. We would like to speculate that there are at least two possible explanations for this phenomenon: on the one hand, both text writing and reading comprehension are extremely complex skills that have been found to improve only after extended, high-quality, explicit teaching of domain-specific strategies [70, 71]. On the other hand, perhaps the nature of parent-child literacy-related interactions is such that they do not have a powerful effect beyond the mechanical aspects of literacy; in other words, children’s higher familiarity with the functions and characteristics of written language may not be enough to facilitate their learning of high-level literacy skills. Finally, when we introduced subject-level covariates that measured cognitive skills, known to underpin both reading and writing, the effect of C-SES was lost; after introducing linguistic-skills covariates, the effect of S-SES was also lost. This finding further substantiates claims for the critical cognitive underpinnings of reading and writing [31, 40]; it also adds to the robust evidence suggesting that oral language skills are essential to literacy development [32, 33]. The finding also means that SES, both at the individual and at the school-level, was not more relevant than a series of domain-general cognitive skills and, especially, than children’s individual linguistic performance, to predict achievement in literacy competences. This is good news, as these are all skills that may be acted on from an educational point of view; that is, most of children’s cognitive abilities and their linguistic skills respond successfully to treatment and, thus, should help all children achieve adequate literacy levels [7277]. This is not to say, however, that it is easy or simple to bring vulnerable children’s relevant literacy-related skills up to a level that allows them to overcome their initial disadvantages. On the contrary, there are many challenges ahead that make such goal seem out of hand at the moment. First, there is an urgent need for further studies to pinpoint exactly which skills should be targeted and how. Second, it would be essential for highly-effective, evidence-based writing practices to be implemented at schools at large, so that children make substantial progress in key skills. In this sense, research has indicated that the use of evidence-based practices across the curriculum is quite low [75]. Another burning question is the following: if subject-level factors are more important in determining literacy outcomes in 2nd and 4th graders than S-SES, why do we detect these important SES effects at all? One possible answer to this question is that S-SES effects are actually capturing differences on how vulnerable schools operate, as has been suggested elsewhere [25]. This would mean that children in vulnerable schools may perform worse than children in more affluent schools because they are exposed to a worse school climate, or because their teachers might make less emphasis on academic performance, among other differences. Indeed, it has been shown that improving school climate and strengthening academic-performance goals contribute to improving children’s academic outcomes [78]. Most importantly, this is clearly an under-researched area and future studies should try to understand the mechanisms through which S-SES impacts literacy outcomes.

Educational implications and concluding remarks

Our study on the impact of S-SES on literacy outcomes provided evidence that children attending vulnerable schools are typically outperformed by children attending non-vulnerable schools. However, our findings have also shown that SES at the school and at the individual level were not more important than children’s literacy-related skills. Moreover, there is a good chance that S-SES effects may reflect worrying discrepancies in how vulnerable and non-vulnerable schools address students’ academic needs, although this claim requires further research. Literacy interventions exist that are effective with low-SES or vulnerable populations [7981]. However, to the best of our knowledge, no literacy intervention has been able to close the gap between vulnerable and non-vulnerable students; that is, interventions are as effective on different SES populations [82]. Considering the importance of literacy skills for academic, professional, and personal realization, more research is needed on effective literacy intervention that is able to boost disadvantaged children’s competences, particularly focusing on early remediation. Finally, our findings suggest that addressing critical skills supporting literacy development (i.e., cognitive, linguistic) might be more effective than programs to compensate poor SES background effects, although perhaps a combination of approaches may prove to be more successful. Future research should compare the relative efficacy of addressing subject-level skills and/or family-based interventions to improve at-risk children’s literacy achievements, particularly with the aim of closing gaps in children’s performance as a function of socioeconomic factors.

Acknowledgments

The authors want to thank the schools and children who participated in the study. We are also thankful to our RAs for their support collecting the data and for evaluating some of the children’s productions included in this study. Lastly, we want to thank Dr. Gabriel Liberman and Dr. Amir Hefetz for their help with the statistical analyses.

Data Availability

Data contains potentially sensitive information of underaged children. We did not obtain informed consent from parents to make these data available, even if it has been confidentially treated. Besides requesting it to both authors, data can be requested via email to the support unit of the Comisión de Ética en la Experimentación Animal y Humana (CEEAH), from the Autonomous University of Barcelona, at uisad.c.educacio.academic@uab.cat

Funding Statement

This research was supported by grants 2015ACUP 00175 (Recercaixa-ACUP program, https://caixaresearch.org/es/convocatoria-caixaresearch-recercaixa-investigacion), PID2019-108791GA-I00 (Ministerio de Ciencia Innovación y Universidades, https://www.ciencia.gob.es), and 2021-SGR-00205 (AGAUR support to research groups) awarded to N.S. There was no additional external funding received for this study. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Bastug M. The structural relationship of reading attitude, reading comprehension and academic achievement. Int. J. Soc. Sci. Educ. 2014;4(4):931–946. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Bangert-Drowns RL, Hurley MM, Wilkinson B. The effects of school-based writing-to-learn interventions on academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research. 2004;74(1):29–58. doi: 10.3102/00346543074001029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Preiss DD, Castillo JC, Grigorenko EL, Manzi J. Argumentative writing and academic achievement: A longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences. 2013; 28:204–11. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.12.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Caravolas M, Lervåg A, Mousikou P, Efrim C, Litavský M, Onochie-Quintanilla E, et al. Common patterns of prediction of literacy development in different alphabetic orthographies. Psychological Science. 2012;23(6):678–86. doi: 10.1177/0956797611434536 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Caravolas M, Lervåg A, Defior S, Seidlová Málková G, Hulme C. Different patterns, but equivalent predictors, of growth in reading in consistent and inconsistent orthographies. Psychological Science. 2013;24(8):1398–407. doi: 10.1177/0956797612473122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Levy CM, Randsell S (ed.). The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications. Routledge, 2013. 10.4324/9780203811122 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Salas, N. Early development of text writing in two contrasting orthographies: English and Spanish. Ph.D. Thesis. Bangor University. 2014
  • 8. Buckingham J, Wheldall K, Beaman-Wheldall R. Why poor children are more likely to become poor readers: The school years. Australian Journal of Education. 2013;57(3):190–213. doi: 10.1177/0004944113495500 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Gustafsson J-E, Nilsen T, Hansen KY. School characteristics moderating the relation between student socio-economic status and mathematics achievement in grade 8. Evidence from 50 countries in TIMSS 2011. Studies in Educational Evaluation. 2018;57:16–30. doi: 10.1016/j.stueduc.2016.09.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Molfese VJ, Modglin A, Molfese DL. The role of environment in the development of reading skills. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2003;36(1):59–67. 10.1177/00222194030360010701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Palardy GJ. High school socioeconomic segregation and student attainment. American Educational Research Journal. 2013;50(4):714–54. doi: 10.3102/0002831213481240 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Letourneau NL, Duffett-Leger L, Levac L, Watson B, Young-Morris C. Socioeconomic status and child development. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders. 2011;21(3):211–24. doi: 10.1177/1063426611421007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Sirin SR. Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A Meta-analytic review of Research. Review of Educational Research. 2005;75(3):417–53. doi: 10.3102/00346543075003417 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. White K. The relation between socioeconomic status and academic achievement. Psychological Bulletin 1982;91:461–481. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.91.3.461 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Mendive S, Mascareño Lara M, Aldoney D, Pérez JC, Pezoa JP. Home language and literacy environments and early literacy trajectories of low‐socioeconomic status Chilean children. Child Development. 2020;91(6):2042–62. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13382 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. O’Connell M. Is the impact of SES on educational performance overestimated? Evidence from the PISA survey. Intelligence. 2019;75:41–7. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2019.04.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Mol SE, Bus AG. To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137(2):267–96. doi: 10.1037/a0021890 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Buisán C, Rios I, Tolchinsky L. The contribution of teaching practices and pupils’ initial knowledge to literacy learning In: Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Bildungswissenschaften. 2011. 33(1); 47–68. 10.25656/01:8607 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Perry LB, Mcconney A. Does the SES of the school matter? an examination of socioeconomic status and student achievement using Pisa 2003. Teachers College Record: The Voice of Scholarship in Education. 2010;112(4):1137–62. doi: 10.1177/016146811011200401 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Perry L, McConney A. School socio-economic composition and student outcomes in Australia: Implications for educational policy. Australian Journal of Education. 2010;54(1):72–85. doi: 10.1177/000494411005400106 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Benson J, Borman GD. Family, neighborhood, and school settings across seasons: When do socioeconomic context and racial composition matter for the Reading Achievement Growth of young children? Teachers College Record: The Voice of Scholarship in Education. 2010;112(5):1338–90. doi: 10.1177/016146811011200505 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Boonen T, Speybroeck S, de Bilde J, Lamote C, Van Damme J, Onghena P. Does it matter who your schoolmates are? an investigation of the association between school composition, school processes and mathematics achievement in the early years of primary education. British Educational Research Journal. 2013;40(3):441–66. doi: 10.1002/berj.3090 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Opdenakker M, Damme JV. Do school context, student composition and school leadership affect school practice and outcomes in secondary education? British Educational Research Journal. 2007;33(2):179–206. 10.1080/01411920701208233 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Perry LB, Saatcioglu A, Mickelson RA. Does school SES matter less for high-performing students than for their lower-performing peers? A quantile regression analysis of Pisa 2018 Australia. Large-scale Assessments in Education. 2022;10(1). doi: 10.1186/s40536-022-00137-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Rumberger RW, Palardy GJ. Does segregation still matter? the impact of student composition on academic achievement in high school. Teachers College Record: The Voice of Scholarship in Education. 2005;107(9):1999–2045. doi: 10.1177/016146810810700905 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Borman GD, Dowling M. Schools and inequality: A multilevel analysis of Coleman’s equality of educational opportunity data. Teachers College Record: The Voice of Scholarship in Education. 2010;112(5):1201–46. doi: 10.1177/016146811011200507 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Alves, RA. The early steps in becoming a writer: Enabling participation in a literate world. In: International handbook of language acquisition. 2019- Routledge. p. 520–538.
  • 28.Mullis IVS, Kennedy AM, Martin MO, Sainsbury, M. PIRLS 2006 assessment framework and specifications (2nd ed.). 2006. Chestnut Hill, MA: Boston College
  • 29. Ravid D, Tolchinsky L. Developing linguistic literacy: A comprehensive model. Journal of Child Language. 2002;29(2):417–47. doi: 10.1017/S0305000902005111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Perfetti CA, Landi N, Oakhill J. The acquisition of reading comprehension skill. In: Snowling M, Hulme C., editors. The Science of Reading: A Handbook. Blackwell Publishing; 2005. pp.227–47. 10.1002/9780470757642.ch13 [DOI]
  • 31. Follmer DJ. Executive function and reading comprehension: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychologist. 2017;53(1):42–60. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2017.1309295 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Dolean DD, Lervåg A, Visu-Petra L, Melby-Lervåg M. Language skills, and not executive functions, predict the development of reading comprehension of early readers: Evidence from an orthographically transparent language. Reading and Writing. 2021;34(6):1491–512. doi: 10.1007/s11145-020-10107-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Lervåg A, Hulme C, Melby-Lervåg M. Unpicking the developmental relationship between oral language skills and reading comprehension: It’s simple, but complex. Child Development. 2017;89(5):1821–38. 10.1111/cdev.12861 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Berninger VW, Abbott RD, Jones J, Wolf BJ, Gould L, Anderson-Youngstrom M, et al. Early development of language by hand: Composing, reading, listening, and speaking connections; three letter-writing modes; and fast mapping in spelling. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2006;29(1):61–92. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2901_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Graham S. A revised writer(s)-within-community model of writing. Educational Psychologist. 2018;53(4):258–79. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2018.1481406 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Bereiter C, Scardamalia M. The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Hayes JR, Flower L. Identifying the organization of writing processes. In: Gregg LW, Steinberg ER, editors. Cognitive processes in writing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1980. pp. 3–29. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Hayes JR. Modeling and remodeling writing. Written Communication. 2012;29(3):369–88. doi: 10.1177/0741088312451260 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Berninger VW. Coordinating transcription and text generation in working memory during composing: Automatic and Constructive Processes. Learning Disability Quarterly. 1999;22(2):99–112. doi: 10.2307/1511269 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Salas N, Silvente S. The role of executive functions and transcription skills in writing: A cross-sectional study across 7 years of schooling. Reading and Writing. 2019;33(4):877–905. doi: 10.1007/s11145-019-09979-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Araújo S, Reis A, Petersson KM, Faísca L. Rapid automatized naming and reading performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2015;107(3):868–83. doi: 10.1037/edu0000006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Babayiğit S, Stainthorp R. Modeling the relationships between cognitive–linguistic skills and literacy skills: New insights from a transparent orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2011;103(1):169–89. doi: 10.1037/a0021671 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Kim Y-S, Puranik C, Otaiba SA. Developmental trajectories of writing skills in first grade. The Elementary School Journal. 2015;115(4):593–613. 10.1086/681971 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Llaurado A, Tolchinsky L. The developmental pattern of spelling in Catalan from first to fifth school grade. Writing Systems Research. 2015;8(1):64–83. doi: 10.1080/17586801.2014.1000812 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Llauradó A, Tolchinsky L. Growth of text-embedded lexicon in Catalan: From childhood to adolescence. First Language. 2013;33(6):628–53. doi: 10.1177/0142723713508861 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Llaurado A, Dockrell JE. The impact of orthography on text production in three languages: Catalan, English, and Spanish. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Eulp enquesta d’usos Lingüístics de la Població [Internet]. [cited 2023 Jul 11]. Available at: https://www.idescat.cat/pub/?id=eulp
  • 48.Generalitat de Catalunya. Enquesta d’usos lingüístics de la població 2013. Generalitat de Catalunya: Institut d’Estadística de Catalunya, 2015. Available at: <http://www.idescat.cat/cat/idescat/publicacions/cataleg/pdfdocs/eulp2013.pdf>
  • 49. Oller J, Vila I. Effects of sociolinguistic environment and the length of residence on the linguistic performance in Catalan and Spanish of sixth grade immigrant pupils in Catalonia. Sociolinguistic Studies. 2010;4(1):63–84. doi: 10.1558/sols.v4i1.63 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Generalitat de Catalunya. Resolució ENS/906/2014, de 23 d’abril, per la qual es determinen els centres educatius i els llocs de treball docents que tenen la consideració de dificultat especial. 2014
  • 51. Ganzeboom HBG, De Graaf PM, Treiman DJ. A standard international socio-economic index of occupational status. Social Science Research. 1992;21(1):1–56. doi: 10.1016/0049-089X(92)90017-B [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Korupp SE, Ganzeboom HBG, Van Der Lippe T. Do mothers matter? A comparison of models of the influence of mothers’ and fathers’ educational and occupational status on children’s educational attainment. 2002. Quality and Quantity, 36, 17–42. 10.1023/A:1014393223522 [DOI]
  • 53. Berninger VW, Cartwright AC, Yates CM, Swanson HL, Abbott RD. Developmental skills related to writing and reading acquisition in the intermediate grades. Reading and Writing. 1994;6(2):161–96. doi: 10.1007/BF01026911 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Pascual M, Salas N. The cognitive profile and text-based traits of struggling writers. Journal for the Study of Education and Development. 2020;44(1):219–53. doi: 10.1080/02103702.2020.1848090 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Salas N. Non-phonological strategies in spelling development. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.McWhinney, B. The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd. ed.). Vol. 2, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000
  • 57.Decree 119/2015, June 23, d’ordenació dels ensenyaments de l’educació primària. (2015). Diari Oficial de la Generalitat de Catalunya, 6900, Disposicions, 2015, June 26, 15176019. https://portaldogc.gencat.cat/utilsEADOP/PDF/6900/1431926.pdf
  • 58. Salas N, Pascual M, Birello M, Cross A. Embedding explicit linguistic instruction in an SRSD writing intervention. Written Communication. 2023;40(3):857–91. doi: 10.1177/07410883231169516 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Cuetos F, Rodríguez B, Ruano E, Arribas D. Prolec-R: Batería de Evaluación de los Procesos lectores, Revisada. 4th ed. Madrid: TEA Ediciones; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Català G, Comes G, Renom J. Avaluació de la comprensió lectora. Barcelona. Graó; 2004.
  • 61.Raven JC. Test dof Progressive Matrices. Buenos Aires; Editorial Paidos; 2004.
  • 62. Lervåg A, Hulme C. Rapid automatized naming (RAN) taps a mechanism that places constraints on the development of early reading fluency. Psychological Science. 2009;20(8):1040–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02405.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Manly T, Anderson V, Nimmo-Smith I, Turner A, Watson P, Robertson IH. The Differential Assessment of Children’s Attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), Normative Sample and ADHD Performance. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. Cambridge University Press; 2001;42(8):1065–81. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00806 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Wechsler D. WISC-IV: Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para Niños IV. Manual de aplicación y corrección. Adaptación española. Madrid: Pearson; 2011.
  • 65. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Dong Y, Peng C-YJ. Principled missing data methods for researchers. SpringerPlus. 2013;2(1). doi: 10.1186/2193-1801-2-222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Bennett DA. How can I deal with missing data in my study? Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health. 2001;25(5):464–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842X.2001.tb00294.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using multivariate statistics. Uttar Pradesh, India: Pearson India Education Services; 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Hood M, Conlon E, Andrews G. Preschool home literacy practices and children’s literacy development: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2008;100(2):252–71. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.2.252 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Gersten R, Haymond K, Newman-Gonchar R, Dimino J, Jayanthi M. Meta-analysis of the impact of reading interventions for students in the primary grades. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness. 2020;13(2):401–27. doi: 10.1080/19345747.2019.1689591 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Graham S, McKeown D, Kiuhara S, Harris KR. A meta-analysis of writing instruction for students in the elementary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2012;104(4):879–96. doi: 10.1037/a0029185 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Dicataldo R, Florit E, Roch M. Fostering broad oral language skills in preschoolers from low SES background. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020;17(12):4495. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124495 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Fricke S, Burgoyne K, Bowyer-Crane C, Kyriacou M, Zosimidou A, Maxwell L, et al. The efficacy of early language intervention in mainstream school settings: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2017;58(10):1141–51. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12737 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Li W, Shang Y, Zhuang W, Mai W, Cheng W, Chen Z. Effectiveness of response inhibition training and its long-term effects in healthy adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2022;16. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.813975 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Spencer-Smith M, Klingberg T. Benefits of a working memory training program for inattention in daily life: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLOS ONE. 2015;10(3). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119522 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Stahl SA, Fairbanks MM. The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research. 1986;56(1):72–110. doi: 10.3102/00346543056001072 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Kretlow A. Helf G., Teacher S. S. Implementation of Evidence-Based Practices in Tier 1: A National Survey. Teacher Education and Special Education, 2013; 36(3), 167–185. doi: 10.1177/0888406413489838 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Berkowitz R. School matters: The contribution of positive school climate to equal educational opportunities among ethnocultural minority students. Youth and Society. 2020;54(3):372–96. doi: 10.1177/0044118X20970235 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Azevedo R, Rosário P, Magalhães P, Núñez JC, Pereira B, Pereira A. A tool-kit to help students from low socioeconomic status background: A school-based self-regulated learning intervention. European Journal of Psychology of Education. 2022;38(2):495–518. doi: 10.1007/s10212-022-00607-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Hagans KS, Good RH. Decreasing reading differences in children from disadvantaged backgrounds: The effects of an early literacy intervention. Contemporary School Psychology. 2013;17(1):103–17. doi: 10.1007/BF03340992 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. McCallum RS, Krohn KR, Skinner CH, Hilton-Prillhart A, Hopkins M, Waller S, et al. Improving reading comprehension of at-risk high-school students: The art of reading program. Psychology in the Schools. 2010;48(1):78–86. doi: 10.1002/pits.20541 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Salas N, Birello M, Ribas T. Effectiveness of an SRSD writing intervention for low- and high-SES children. Reading and Writing. 2020;34(7):1653–80. doi: 10.1007/s11145-020-10103-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Jie Wang

23 Aug 2023

PONE-D-23-22078Impact of school SES on literacy developmentPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Salas,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Oct 07 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Jie Wang, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf.

2. Thank you for stating in your Funding Statement:

“This research was supported by grants 2015ACUP 00175 (Recercaixa-ACUP program)and PID2019-108791GA-I00 (Ministerio de Ciencia Innovación y Universidades), awarded to N.S. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

Please provide an amended statement that declares *all* the funding or sources of support (whether external or internal to your organization) received during this study, as detailed online in our guide for authors at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submit-now.  Please also include the statement “There was no additional external funding received for this study.” in your updated Funding Statement.

Please include your amended Funding Statement within your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well.

5. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

The authors should pay careful attention to each of the comments below and address the issues raised by the two reviewers.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for the opportunity to review your paper. I was so excited to see the topic, and I was even more excited when I began to read it. Every question I might have had about why you conducted your study, how it fits within the existing canon and adds to our collective knowledge, how you established your design and conducted your analysis, etc. you answered and answered well. I sincerely tried to find critical points because I personally find peer review feedback more helpful when it is critical and specific (although an 'accept as is' would never be turned down), but I struggled to find anything. I found some minor typos (e.g., a "nor" where I don't think you mean it, some spelling typos, etc.), but overall, I kept thinking this paper was a master class in how to be both succinct and detailed in such a way that everything was really compelling and clear. The data analysis was especially robust, and the findings are impactful. I did find myself asking in your results section if the reason the C-SES was less critical than the S-SES and no longer relevant when children's cognitive and literacy skills were included was precisely because of how predictive poverty is to cognitive skills and language development that they essentially cancelled each other out because of their correlatedness? It does seem you addressed this more explicitly in your discussion section, which was also really well done, in my opinion. I was really impressed with the design and analysis, and more importantly I was made hopeful by the outcomes because of the implications they hold for future research and practice. I thoroughly enjoyed reading this paper.

Reviewer #2: The manuscript is largely well-written and offers interesting data on an urgent filed of inquiry. Some clarifications are necessary:

- "Grade-level expectations" should be defined. What are the grade-level expectations in Catalan schools for grades 2 and 4?

- The rationale for the included assessment tools should be strengthened.

- The conclusion, stating that it is "good news" that children's individual linguistic and cognitive abilities were as important as SES, since these are possible to improve through educational intervention, is hugely over-simplified. Indeed, many language and cognitive functions can be improved. However, if this was easily done through the delivery of mainstream classroom, how come all students don't develop reading and writing according to plan? The authors must be aware that the promotion of many language skills require expert knowledge and techniques not easily included in mainstream education, require theoretical and therapeutic skills not part of the knowledge-base of general teachers, and not easily tailored to the highly diverse needs of the students in a class.

- Please check your text for writing errors, e.g. "tquality", "missigness", "have also shown than", "considerable more research".

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Karyn A. Allee

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Dec 21;18(12):e0295606. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0295606.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


27 Oct 2023

Please, note we also submitted these comments as a separate file.

Response to Reviewers

Reviewer #1: Thank you for the opportunity to review your paper. I was so excited to see the topic, and I was even more excited when I began to read it. Every question I might have had about why you conducted your study, how it fits within the existing canon and adds to our collective knowledge, how you established your design and conducted your analysis, etc. you answered and answered well. I sincerely tried to find critical points because I personally find peer review feedback more helpful when it is critical and specific (although an 'accept as is' would never be turned down), but I struggled to find anything. I found some minor typos (e.g., a "nor" where I don't think you mean it, some spelling typos, etc.), but overall, I kept thinking this paper was a master class in how to be both succinct and detailed in such a way that everything was really compelling and clear. The data analysis was especially robust, and the findings are impactful. I did find myself asking in your results section if the reason the C-SES was less critical than the S-SES and no longer relevant when children's cognitive and literacy skills were included was precisely because of how predictive poverty is to cognitive skills and language development that they essentially cancelled each other out because of their correlatedness? It does seem you addressed this more explicitly in your discussion section, which was also really well done, in my opinion. I was really impressed with the design and analysis, and more importantly I was made hopeful by the outcomes because of the implications they hold for future research and practice. I thoroughly enjoyed reading this paper.

>>We are sincerely grateful for Reviewer 1’s comments. We have revised the manuscript entirely and corrected typos and small mistakes. Thank you for your thoughtful comments.

Reviewer #2: The manuscript is largely well-written and offers interesting data on an urgent filed of inquiry. Some clarifications are necessary:

- "Grade-level expectations" should be defined. What are the grade-level expectations in Catalan schools for grades 2 and 4?

>>We have added a reference to the educational Catalan curriculum, which served as the criteria guiding grade-level expectations (please, refer to page 13 of the revised manuscript and reference number 57).

- The rationale for the included assessment tools should be strengthened.

>>We have added an introductory paragraph to our Tasks and measures subsection detailing the rationale for the selection of data-collection instruments. Please, see page 11.

- The conclusion, stating that it is "good news" that children's individual linguistic and cognitive abilities were as important as SES, since these are possible to improve through educational intervention, is hugely over-simplified. Indeed, many language and cognitive functions can be improved. However, if this was easily done through the delivery of mainstream classroom, how come all students don't develop reading and writing according to plan? The authors must be aware that the promotion of many language skills require expert knowledge and techniques not easily included in mainstream education, require theoretical and therapeutic skills not part of the knowledge-base of general teachers, and not easily tailored to the highly diverse needs of the students in a class.

>>We completely agree with this observation by Reviewer 2. Accordingly, we have now expanded our original intention which was to draw attention to the fact that the solution may be implemented in the context of educational centers (as opposed to changes to the social makeup of society, which would clearly be out of our hands), but we now make a subsequent comment about the difficulty of achieving this goal. Please, see page 25.

Please check your text for writing errors, e.g. "tquality", "missigness", "have also shown than", "considerable more research".

>>Done.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.pdf

Decision Letter 1

Jie Wang

27 Nov 2023

Impact of school SES on literacy development

PONE-D-23-22078R1

Dear Dr. Salas,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Jie Wang, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: Thank you for revising the paper in accordance with my recommendations. I look forward to eventually seeing the paper online.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Jie Wang

4 Dec 2023

PONE-D-23-22078R1

Impact of school SES on literacy development

Dear Dr. Salas:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Jie Wang

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.pdf

    Data Availability Statement

    Data contains potentially sensitive information of underaged children. We did not obtain informed consent from parents to make these data available, even if it has been confidentially treated. Besides requesting it to both authors, data can be requested via email to the support unit of the Comisión de Ética en la Experimentación Animal y Humana (CEEAH), from the Autonomous University of Barcelona, at uisad.c.educacio.academic@uab.cat


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES