Abstract
Background:
Finding the baseline resting-state EEG markers for early identification of cognitive decline can contribute to the identification of individuals at risk of further change. Potential applications include identifying participants for clinical trials, early treatment, and evaluation of treatment, accessible even from a community setting.
Methods:
Analyses were completed on a sample of 99 (ages 60–90) consensus-diagnosed, community-dwelling African Americans (58 cognitively typical/HC, and 41 mildly cognitively impaired/MCI), who were recruited from the Michigan Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (MADRC) and the Wayne State University Institute of Gerontology. In addition to neuropsychological testing with CogState and Toolbox computerized batteries, resting-state EEGs (rsEEG, eyes closed) were acquired before and after participants were engaged in a visual motion direction discrimination task. rsEEG frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA) and frontal beta asymmetry (FBA) were calculated.
Results:
FAA showed no difference across groups for the pre-task resting state. FBA was significantly different between groups, with more asymmetric frontal beta in MCI. Both physiological indices, however, along with computerized neuropsychological tests were significant predictors in logistic regression classification of MCI vs. control participants.
Conclusion:
rsEEG asymmetries can contribute significantly to successful discrimination of older persons with MCI from those without, over and above cognitive testing, alone.
Keywords: Resting-state electroencephalography (rsEEG), Frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA), Frontal beta asymmetry (FBA), Mild cognitive impairment (MCI)
1. Introduction
Understanding the psychophysiological correlates of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is important for several reasons. People diagnosed with MCI, particularly the amnesic subtype (aMCI), transition into Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) at a much higher rate than the general older population (Bachman et al., 1993; Petersen et al., 2001), suggesting that in some cases MCI may represent an early stage of decline into dementia (Galluzzi et al., 2001). At the same time, it is important that underrepresented minority populations be included in such research, so that findings can be generalized beyond majority populations (Clark et al., 2019), but this goal remains challenging to meet (Fisher and Kalbaugh, 2011; Erves et al., 2017). Measurement approaches that are less invasive as compared to PET or other related methodologies and can be administered in a community setting may be important factors in identifying people who may benefit from more invasive, expensive procedures.
Recently, Kavcic et al. (2021) reported greater changes in baseline resting-state EEG spectral power after engaging in a motion direction discrimination task in a group of community-dwelling African Americans diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) relative to neurologically intact African American controls. In this paper, we report findings from a further analysis of this same data set (R21 AG046637, Kavcic PI) investigating resting-state EEG (rsEEG) changes associated with cognitive decline.
1.1. Known resting-state electrophysiological correlates of MCI
Research going back decades has found dozens of electrophysiological correlates of Alzheimer’s disease and MCI in resting state (for review, see Babiloni et al., 2021) and evoked EEG and ERP. A general slowing of oscillatory brain activity, indexed by an increase in delta and theta bands relative to alpha and beta, is seen in normal aging (Hughes and Cayaffa, 1977), is exaggerated in AD (Fernández et al., 2003), and correlates negatively with frontal white matter volume in AD and MCI (Babiloni et al., 2006).
The theta frequency band has a long history of association with both working memory and long-term memory (Klimesch, 1996). Several authors have reported lower resting-state theta in MCI and AD participants compared to neurologically intact comparison groups (Cummins et al., 2008; Finnigan and Robertson, 2011), as well as less event-related synchronization of theta in progressive MCI compared to stable MCI participants (Missonnier et al., 2006).
Babiloni et al. (2010) observed higher delta, lower alpha, and less alpha reactivity (the difference in alpha power between eyes open and eyes closed conditions) in AD and MCI compared to neurologically intact controls. Alpha power deteriorates in amnesic MCI patients relative to controls (Babiloni et al., 2011; Babiloni et al., 2014), and this appears to be related to degeneration of gray matter (Babiloni et al., 2013).
Heuristically, this complex picture can be imperfectly summed up as an exaggerated slowing of oscillatory activity in MCI and AD, which is likely a result of neurodegeneration. However, as discussed below, other popular measures of oscillatory activity, specifically frontal asymmetries, have not been as thoroughly researched in MCI and AD.
1.2. Frontal oscillatory asymmetries
Frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA) is based on the theory that the amplitude of alpha oscillations is inversely related to the degree of information processing activity of underlying cortical tissue (Davidson, 1988; Davidson, 1992). FAA is typically calculated as the difference ln (RIGHT) – ln(LEFT) for a pair of electrodes (Coan and Allen, 2003), with a natural log transform to better conform to a normal distribution (Allen et al., 2004). Since a greater amplitude of alpha corresponds to less activity, a positive FAA corresponds to more activity in the left hemisphere (less alpha) while a negative FAA corresponds to more activity on the right. Although several homologous pairs of electrodes can be used to estimate FAA, the most commonly chosen pair is F3 and F4 (e.g., Davidson et al., 1990; Harmon-Jones and Allen, 1997; Sutton and Davidson, 1997).
When combined with a motivational-emotional dichotomy that is hypothesized to be lateralized in frontal lobes, correlations can be predicted between FAA and behavioral measures of affective-motivational traits and states. The motivational dichotomy corresponding to FAA is not entirely clear, as several have been suggested, including positive vs. negative emotional valence (Davidson et al., 1979), approach vs avoidance motivation (Harmon-Jones and Allen, 1997; Harmon-Jones et al., 2010; Hewig et al., 2006; Sutton and Davidson, 1997) or approach vs. regulatory inhibition (Gable et al., 2018; Lacey et al., 2020), and behavioral activation vs. inhibition (Harmon-Jones and Allen, 1997; Sutton and Davidson, 1997) based on the theory of behavioral activation and inhibition of Gray (1970). In each case, the negative condition is associated with greater right hemisphere activity (i.e., lower alpha on the right and a negative FAA), while the positive condition is associated with greater left hemisphere activity (lower alpha on the left and a positive FAA).
Although it has proven difficult to disentangle these conceptual frameworks, evidence seems to favor a motivational meaning for FAA (Harmon-Jones and Gable, 2018; Rodrigues et al., 2018). In particular a leftward FAA also obtains for anger, a negative emotion but with an approach orientation (Harmon-Jones and Allen, 1998; Harmon-Jones and Gable, 2018), and both right-leaning and left-leaning FAA can be associated with the activation of behavior, whether approach or avoidance (escape), but does not appear to be associated with withholding a behavior (Rodrigues et al., 2018).
There are several reasons to suspect that FAA might be associated with MCI. Neurodegeneration in AD has been found to be asymmetric, and while the side of greatest neurodegeration is unpredictable, it corresponds in meaningful ways to observed deficits (Derflinger et al., 2011; Haxby et al., 1985). For example, greater deterioration of left hemisphere gray matter predicts greater language deficits (Haxby et al., 1985). FAA is predictive of risk for depression (Schaffer et al., 1983; Thibodeau et al., 2006), and depression is sometimes comorbid with MCI and AD (Panza et al., 2009). Further, there is some emerging evidence that mild emotional dysregulation resulting from early degeneration of limbic structures in progressive dementia is characteristic of MCI (Mah et al., 2017; Sturm et al., 2013). Given these associations between MCI and negative affective states, FAA could be predicted to be more negative in MCI compared to intact controls. However, to our knowledge, this is the first attempt to correlate FAA and MCI.
Given the relationship between activity in the beta band and cognitive-emotional processing outlined above, one might wonder whether Frontal beta asymmetry (FBA) might also be related to cognitive decline. FBA is less often investigated, or at least less often reported, than FAA (Ocklenburg et al., 2019), but has an interpretation similar to FAA. Whereas alpha is assumed to be either idling or inhibition of activity (Klimesch et al., 2007; Mathewson et al., 2011) and therefore a relative lack of it to indicate active processing, activity in the beta range is presumed to reflect that activity (Ray and Cole, 1985) and therefore a relatively larger beta power to indicate relatively more processing. Despite the logic, however, FBA, when it has been reported, has had a poor record of success. Lopez-Duran et al. (2012) measured asymmetry in theta, alpha, and beta bands at regions all over the scalp, while participants were watching emotional videos, but failed to find any effect of emotional state or risk of depression on beta asymmetries. Pérez-Edgar et al. (2013) found a relationship between FAA and performance on the dot-probe task, a task designed to measure attentional bias to threatening stimuli, but no such relationship between the dot probe performance and FBA.
1.3. The current data
In this exploratory study, we investigated whether two frontal asymmetries, FAA and FBA, were related to MCI, and if they could add to the predictive ability of two computerized neuropsychological test batteries: CogState (Darby et al., 2002; Fredrickson et al., 2010) and the NIH Toolbox-Cognition (Hammers et al., 2012; Weintraub et al., 2013). The degree to which these instruments distinguish MCI from neurologically intact age-matched controls has already been established (Giordani, 2011; Kairys et al., 2021; Hammers et al., 2012), so we investigated whether the physiological measures used here could increase that discriminability.
Hypotheses:
The current state of the literature, along with the available data, suggests several hypotheses. First, if FAA is inversely proportional to neuronal activity, and MCI is characterized by more negative emotional and motivational states, then FAA might differ between people with MCI and controls. Similarly, if the beta is positively associated with underlying neuronal activity, FBA might also be related to MCI, but in the opposite direction. The following hypotheses are possible to test:
Hypothesis 1.
MCI will be associated with FAA, such that MCI participants will have lower FAA scores, corresponding to relatively greater right-hemisphere (negative valence, avoidance motivation) activity.
Hypothesis 2.
MCI will be associated with FBA, such that MCI participants will have higher FBA scores, corresponding to relatively greater right-hemisphere activity.
Hypothesis 3.
Frontal asymmetries (FAA or FBA) will account for unique variability in group membership (MCI vs. control) in the presence of computerized assessments of cognitive functioning.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
All procedures were approved by the institutional review board of Wayne State University. Participants were community-dwelling African Americans recruited from the volunteer pool of Healthier Black Elders Center (HBEC), a collaboration of the Institute of Gerontology at Wayne State University and the Institute of Social Research at the University of Michigan, and the Michigan Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (MADRC). We attempted to recruit the largest possible sample meeting inclusion criteria from this pool. Participants were recruited based on a screening questionnaire report of having experienced a change in memory in the past year to potentially enrich the sample with participants closer to developing MCI. Originally 129 people from these organization volunteered to participant, but 30 either failed to meet the inclusion criteria or insufficient EEG signal was obtained, leaving a total sample of 99 participants. Of the 41 participants diagnosed as having MCI, there were six males and 35 females, with a mean age of 73.7 years and a standard deviation of 7.2 years. The remaining 58 participants were categorized as neurologically intact controls, with five males and 53 females having a mean age of 71.1 years, with a standard deviation of 6.2 years. The difference in age was not significant, t(97) = 1.95, p = 0.054. The average number of years of education for the MCI and control groups was MMCI = 14.46 and Mcon = 15.26, respectively, and this difference was not significant, t(97) = 1.64, p = 0.103. Additional demographics, as well as descriptive statistics covering the traditional neuropsychological evaluations used in diagnosis, are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Controls |
MCI |
t-test |
||
---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | M (SD) | t | p | |
Demographics | ||||
Age | 71.14 (6.19) | 73.68 (7.19) | 1.88 | 0.06 |
Gender (M, F) | 5, 53 | 6, 35 | ||
Education | 15.24 (2,35) | 14.49 (2.41) | 1.55 | 0.12 |
Handedness (L, R) | 8, 50 | 2, 39 | ||
GDS | 0.95 (1.39) | 0.80 (1.08 | 0.55 | 0.14 |
UDS neurospsych tests | ||||
MMSE | 28.75 (1.11) | 27.65 (1.53) | 4.06 | <0.001 |
Logimem | 13.14 (3.15) | 9.19 (2.97 | 6.07 | <0.001 |
digif | 7.91 ((2.05) | 7.05 (2.17) | 1.94 | 0.06 |
digiflen | 6.40 (1.19) | 5.95 (1.25) | 1.79 | 0.07 |
digib | 6.72 (1.75) | 5.16 (1.82) | 4.15 | <0.001 |
digiblen | 5.02 (1.04) | 3.97 (1.07) | 4.70 | <0.001 |
Animals | 19.11 (4.49) | 15.92 (3.74) | 3.78 | <0.001 |
veg | 15.32 (3.81) | 13.08 (3.37) | 2.90 | 0.004 |
traila | 36.86 (11.79) | 41.22 (13.29) | 1.66 | 0.10 |
trailarr | 0.21 (0.46) | 0.19 (0.46) | 0.26 | 0.80 |
trailb | 92.53 (34.76) | 157.83 (38.16) | 5.24 | <0.001 |
trailbrr | 0.43 ((0.76) | 1.39 (1.63) | 3.31 | 0.002 |
trailbli | 24.00 (0.00) | 21.08 (5.97) | 3.67 | <0.001 |
wais | 46.52 (9.62) | 37.06 (8.71) | 4.70 | <0.001 |
memnits | 11.68 (3.15) | 7.16 (2.80) | 7.09 | <0.001 |
memtime | 18.82 (1.96) | 21.36 (2.52) | 1.40 | 0.17 |
boston | 26.23 (2.52) | 23.59 (4.55) | 3.59 | <0.001 |
Note. Significant p values are bolded.
2.2. Neuropsychological evaluation
All participants were given computerized cognitive testing on the same day that they sat for an EEG. The Cogstate Brief Battery includes a Detection task (DET) that represents simple attention and reaction time, an Identification task (IDN) that assesses attention and concentration, a 1-Back working memory task (ONB), and One Card learning task (OCL). For Cogstate Brief Battery, acceptable test-retest reliability (Hammers et al., 2011) and validity (Hammers et al., 2012) for older adults have been established. The NIH Toolbox battery (Gershon et al., 2013) of tasks presented on a laptop and includes: Picture Vocabulary, Oral Reading Recognitioin, List Sorting Working Memory, Dimensional Change Card Sort, Pattern Comparison Processing Speed, Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention, and Picture Sequence Memory tests. In all cases, cognitive testing was done prior to EEG.
Participants also were given the National Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperating Centers (NACC) standardized evaluation battery, the Unified Dataset (UDS) (Morris et al., 2006), with these results reviewed in a consensus conference for each participant (see Table 1). These conferences included neurologists, neuropsychologists, nurses, and technicians. Diagnoses were given after applying the following exclusion criteria: psychiatric and sleep disorders, head injury with loss of consciousness longer than 30 min, seizure disorder, learning disability, AIDS, radiation therapy of the brain in the past year, diagnosis of dementia, history of alcoholism or drug abuse, and major medical illness in the past five years. All participants scored at or above 25 on the MMSE. From the consensus conferences, 28 participants were diagnosed as having amnesic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), 13 as non-amnesic (naMCI), and 58 as neurologically and cognitively intact. Because the naMCI sample was so small, we judged that there would be insufficient statistical power to distinguish among subtypes of MCI and, therefore, collapsed across these subtypes to compare MCI to neurologically intact controls. Computer testing results and functional brain data (fMRI, PET, EEG) were not reviewed as part of the Consensus Conference.
2.3. EEG recording
The electroencephalogram was recorded with a Brain Vision Inc. 64- channel amplifier and Acti Cap (active electrode) high-density system using a modified international 10–20 System of electrode placements. Online reference was FCz, with the ground at AFz, re-referenced offline to average reference. Data were sampled at 500 Hz (32 bit resolution) and bandpass filtered between 0.1 and 70 Hz, 3 dB down, 12 dB roll-off per octave on each side. Impedances were maintained at <10 kΩ, and balanced across all channels within a 5 kΩ range.
All procedures were conducted while participants were seated in a chair adjusted for height, in front of a computer in a dimly lit room. Resting-state EEG measures were taken twice, before and after a motion direction discrimination task (see below). The resting-state EEG was recorded for a minimum of 3 min with eyes open and 3 min with eyes closed. If noisy epochs were observed during recording, an additional time up to approximately 30 s was recorded in order to ensure a minimum of 60 artifact-free 2 s intervals for spectral analysis.
In between recordings of the resting EEG, participants performed a motion direction discrimination task. Stimuli were controlled by a laptop PC using Windows and custom software written by the last author in C, using the Allegro 4 (https://liballeg.org/) programming library and compiled with MinGW (www.mingw.org). Stimuli were moving dots subtending approximately 0.125° of visual angle, presented with a 10° circular aperture with a density of 0.6 dots/deg2. Dots were white on a dark background. Motion duration was 500 ms, with either rightward or leftward direction on randomly ordered trials and button press responses collected on a laptop.
2.4. EEG processing and frequency analysis
The EEG was inspected offline, and segments containing excessive noise, saturation, or other artifacts were removed. Data were then segmented into 2 s epochs (1024 data points) with a Hanning window, yielding 0.488 Hz resolution. Epochs were automatically rejected if they exceeded 100 mV on any channel. Approximately 90 epochs (range 64–115) were obtained for each participant. Prior to FFT, EEG epochs were transformed into the reference-free current source density (CSD) distribution, which reflects the underlying cortical activity and removes nearly all volume conduction effects (Kayser and Tenke, 2015a, 2015b). In particular this transform has been suggested as better than other reference options for investigating frontal asymmetry (Hagemann et al., 2001). We then estimated power using fast Fourier transform in the traditional bands: delta (2–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), and beta (12–30 Hz). We did not include in power analyses frequencies >30 Hz due to pericranial musculature electrical activity that can contaminate scalp recorded neuroelectric activity (Whitham et al., 2008).
Frontal alpha asymmetry was computed as the log-transformed difference between electrodes F4 and F3 according to the formula FAA = ln (F4) – ln(F3). These locations were chosen to be consistent with the majority of previous literature (e.g., Davidson et al., 1990; Harmon- Jones and Allen, 1997; Sutton and Davidson, 1997). Frontal beta asymmetry was likewise calculated in exactly the same way using spectral power in the beta range for F4 and F3.
2.5. Data analysis
Data were analyzed with SPSS 26 (IBM, Inc.). FAA and FBA were each analyzed with a 2 (timepoint, pre vs. post) X 2 (group, MCI vs. control) mixed ANOVA. Finally, logistic regression was performed to evaluate whether the physiological measures could add to the ability of the computerized testing variables to classify MCI vs. control participants. In this analysis, age and years of education were entered in a first step, followed by behavioral measures from the computerized testing in a second step, with FAA and FBA entered in a third step. For FAA and FBA, the pre-task measures were used as predictors. Each step was evaluated for goodness of fit using sensitivity, specificity, an area under the ROC curve (AUC), the Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic, and percent correct classification.
3. Results
3.1. Behavioral results
Table 2 presents the means, standard deviations, and group (MCI vs. control) t-tests for the computerized neuropsychological assessments. With the exception of the CogState Identity task and Toolbox flanker task, each measure was significantly worse in MCI than controls.
Table 2.
Computerized test | Controls |
MCI |
t-test |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | M (SD) | t | p | |
CogState | ||||
Detection (lmn) | 2.59 (0.08) | 2.63 (0.13) | 2.05 | 0.04 |
Identity (lmn) | 2.77 (0.09) | 2.80 (0.09) | 1.87 | 0.07 |
One card back (acc) | 1.37 (0.17) | 1.26 (0.19) | 2.87 | 0.005 |
One card learning (acc) | 0.98 (0.09) | 0.90 (0.09) | 3.80 | <0.001 |
Toolbox | ||||
Crystallized composite | ||||
Oral Reading | 56.0.84 6.23) | 50.56 (8.18) | 4.24 | <0.001 |
Recognition | ||||
Picture Vocabulary | 57.56 (8.45) | 49.47 (10.56) |
3.46 | |
Fluid composite | ||||
Picture Sequence | 47.93 (13.41) | 39.50 (9.48) | 3.35 | 0.001 |
Memory | ||||
Dimensional Change | 58.04 (14.56) | 48.45 (9.87) | 3.59 | <0.001 |
Card | ||||
Flanker Inhibitory | 52.11 (7.94) | 48.90 (8.96) | 1.84 | 0.07 |
Control | ||||
List Sorting | 51.96 (9.07) | 45.56 (7.05) | 3.67 | <0.001 |
Pattern Recognition | 51.95 (12.56) |
45.03 (10.83) |
2.80 | 0.006 |
Note. Significant p values are bolded. The computerized neuropsychological tests were not used in the diagnosis of MCI. Nevertheless, independent samples t-tests indicate that most individual tests in each battery are significantly different between groups.
3.2. Baseline EEG asymmetry results
Conditional means and measures of dispersion for each dependent variable are presented in Table 3. Results of each ANOVA are presented in Table 4. The analysis of FAA revealed no significant main effects or interaction. For FBA, there were main effects of both timepoint, F(1,94) = 6.63, p = 0.012, and group, F(1,94) = 8.2, p = 0.005, but no interaction. FBA was shifted leftward after the motion direction discrimination task for both groups, and the MCI participants were more asymmetric than the controls.
Table 3.
Measure | Group | Timepoint | Mean | SE | L 95% CI | U 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
FAA | Control | Pre | 0.077 | 0.068 | −0.059 | 0.212 |
Post | 0.033 | 0.079 | −0.124 | 0.191 | ||
MCI | Pre | 0.063 | 0.081 | −0.098 | 0.223 | |
Post | −0.102 | 0.094 | −0.289 | 0.084 | ||
FBA | Control | Pre | 0.100 | 0.072 | −0.042 | 0.242 |
Post | 0.007 | 0.091 | −0.175 | 0.188 | ||
MCI | Pre | −0.159 | 0.086 | −0.329 | 0.011 | |
Post | −0.379 | 0.109 | −0.596 | −0.162 |
Note. FAA = frontal alpha asymmetry. FBA = frontal beta asymmetry.
Table 4.
IV | Statistic | FAA | FBA |
---|---|---|---|
Timepoint | F(1,94) | 3.652 | 6.627 |
p | 0.059 | 0.012 | |
ηp | 0.037 | 0.065 | |
Group | F(1,94) | 0.55 | 8.204 |
p | 0.46 | 0.005 | |
ηp | 0.006 | 0.079 | |
Timepoint X Group | F(1,94) | 1.238 | 1.088 |
p | 0.269 | 0.3 | |
ηp | 0.013 | 0.011 |
Note. Mixed ANOVA analysis. Timepoint was within-subjects, group was between-subjects. Significant p values are bolded. FAA = frontal alpha asymmetry, FBA = frontal beta asymmetry, ηp = partial eta squared. Significant effects are bolded.
Goodness of fit measures for the logistic regression are presented in Table 5, and parameter estimates are presented in Table 6. In the initial step, demographic control factors of age and education level were entered. At the second step the computerized neuropsychological evaluations were entered. Oral Reading from the NIH Toolbox and Det, IDN, and OCL from Cogstate were significant predictors. At the final step, adding in the physiological measures, both FAA and FBA were significant, OCL was no longer a significant predictor, while Card Sorting from the Toolbox was now significant. Fit statistics indicated that overall the model was better with physiological predictors added, with a relatively large increase in sensitivity, an area under the ROC curve (AUC), and the KS statistic, modest gains in the Cox and Snell pseudo-R2 and percent correct classification, and a slight decrease in specificity.
Table 5.
Step 1 | Step 2 | Step 3 | |
---|---|---|---|
Sensitivity | 0.39 | 0.884 | 0.833 |
Specificity | 0.862 | 0.836 | 0.87 |
AUC | 0.668 | 0.884 | 0.933 |
KS | 37.55 | 62.11 | 69.85 |
% Correct | 66.7 | 79.3 | 83.3 |
Cox & Snell R2 | 0.065 | 0.435 | 0.513 |
Note. AUC = area under ROC curve. KS = Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic.
Table 6.
Step 1 | Step 2 | Step 3 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||
Predictor | β | Wald | Exp(B) | β | Wald | Exp(B) | β | Wald | Exp(B) |
Age | 0.063 | 3.816 | 1.065 | 0.06 | 1.00 | 1.06 | 0.037 | 0.287 | 1.037 |
Education | −0.163 | 2.866 | 0.858 | 0.197 | 1.476 | 1.217 | 0.151 | 0.721 | 1.163 |
TB Card Sort | −0.055 | 2.883 | 0.947 | −0.112 * | 6.091 | 0.894 | |||
TB Flanker | 0.009 | 0.035 | 1.009 | 0.045 | 0.814 | 1.046 | |||
TB List Sort | −0.095 * | 4.282 | 0.910 | − 0.096 | 3.732 | 0.908 | |||
TB Oral | −0.196 * | 6.811 | 0.822 | −0.235 ** | 7.958 | 0.790 | |||
TB Pattern | − 0.026 | 0.642 | 0.974 | − 0.047 | 1.346 | 0.954 | |||
TB Picture Sequence | − 0.051 | 2.429 | 0.950 | − 0.010 | 0.068 | 0.990 | |||
TB Pic Voc | − 0.049 | 1.324 | 0.952 | − 0.062 | 1.612 | 0.940 | |||
CS Detect | 0.008 * | 4.902 | 1.008 | 0.009 * | 3.866 | 1.009 | |||
CD Identity | −0.008 * | 4.839 | 0.992 | −0.009 * | 4.255 | 0.991 | |||
CS One-Card learning | −0.003 * | 5.115 | 0.997 | −0.003 * | 5.072 | 0.997 | |||
CS One-Back | 0.003 | 1.876 | 1.003 | −0.001 | 0.178 | 1.001 | |||
FAA | 3.493 * | 4.358 | 32.879 | ||||||
FBA | −4.438 ** | 6.879 | 0.012 | ||||||
Constant | − 2.625 | 1.026 | 0.072 | 23.560 | 7.519 | 1.24E+08 | 26.581 | 9.626 | 3.50E+11 |
Note. Significant p values are bolded. TB = Toolbox, CS = CogState.
p < 0.05.
p < 0.005.
The relatively high correlation between FAA and FBA (r = 0.85, p < 0.001) presents a concern for multicollinearity in the logistic regression model. To assess whether multicollinearity was too extreme, we calculated variance inflation factors for FAA and FBA by regressing each one, one at a time, on the rest of the predicters, and calculating VIF as 1/(1- R2), or in other words, 1/Tolerance. A cutoff of VIF > 10 is typically considered indicative of unacceptable collinearity, although some authors have argued that this is arbitrary (Belsley, 1984), and some have argued that a range of 5–10 could be indicative of problems in logistic regression (Senaviratna and Cooray, 2019). In the case of these two predictors, VIFFAA = 4.96, and VIFTBA = 5.32.
Handedness can complicate the interpretation of frontal asymmetries. Because we did not use handedness as an exclusion criterion, ten participants were left-handed (approximately matching the population rate of left-handedness). To evaluate whether handedness accounted for our results, we did several additional analyses. First, we determined that handedness was not differentially distributed between MCI and control participants, χ2 (1) = 2.102, p = 0.147. We then tested whether or not FAA and FBA were significantly different for right and left handers using independent samples t-tests. Left and right handers were not significantly different with respect to either FAA, t(95) = − 1.43, p = 0.157, or FBA, t(95) = − 1.24, p = 0.218. Finally, we regressed group (MCI vs control) on FAA, FBA, and handedness. Both FAA (p = 0.002, Wald = 9.861, Exp(B) = 30.6) and FBA (p < 0.001, Wald = 13.21, Exp(B) = 0.026) were significant predictors of group, but handedness was not, p = 0.144, Exp(B) = 0.275.
4. Discussion
In this exploratory analysis, we tested three hypotheses. There was no support for Hypothesis 1, that FAA would be associated with MCI. Although a null result always leaves open the possibility of a Type II error owing to measurement error or inadequate sample size, the estimated effect size indicates that if individuals with MCI have a different FAA than neurologically intact controls, it is a very small difference. This does not completely shut the door to FAA in MCI or Alzheimer’s disease. FAA reflects both state and trait variables which have made replications even in the core area of motivation difficult to achieve (see Harmon- Jones and Gable, 2018, for a review). However, this study provides no support to the idea that FAA is associated with MCI.
Hypothesis 2 that participants with MCI would have higher FBA consistent with greater withdrawal motivation was contradicted. MCI were more asymmetric than controls, but with negative FBA indicating relatively greater left frontal activity. In the context of the literature on asymmetric neurodegeneration in AD, this result may be consistent with studies showing greater deficits in the left hemisphere (Loewenstein et al., 1989; Zahn et al., 2004) if we assume an early deficit causes compensatory recruitment or effort, but the majority of studies do not find early or predominant left hemispheric abnormalities (Derflinger et al., 2011). This is consistent with the broader literature on asymmetries and clinical conditions, where the pattern of abnormalities in asymmetry relative to healthy brains appears to depend in complex and interactive ways with specific symptomology (Mundorf et al., 2021). It may be too early to try to interpret this higher relative beta in MCI vs. controls, but this will be an important direction for future research.
To our knowledge, this is the first report of an association between MCI and FBA. Several studies have reported hemispheric asymmetries in AD based on imaging with MRI, fMRI, and PET studies (for review, see Yang et al., 2017), but only a few studies addressed hemispheric asymmetries in MCI. Furthermore, to the best of our effort, we found only one study evaluating hemispheric asymmetries in MCI with EEG. John et al. (2019) reported asymmetry loss in MCI-AD patients with greater right hemispheric asymmetry, i.e., a larger decline of complexity and/or lower activity in the left hemisphere of the brain than the right, primarily over the parietal lobe. Our result of greater relative beta activity in the left hemisphere complicates this picture, but does not contradict John et al. (2019) because they used much different measures (wavelets, non-linear dynamical systems statistics), and their sample included both MCI and AD participants. Our results, for example, may reflect a compensatory mechanism engaged in early MCI that reverses with decline into AD.
FBA shifted leftward after the motion direction discrimination task for both groups, suggesting that FBA may index a more temporary state rather than a stable trait. This effect may very well depend on the specific task used between measures of resting EEG. It may be fruitful for future research to address changes in FBA with different tasks.
There was support for Hypothesis 3, that the psychophysiological measures used here would contribute significantly to the classification of group membership over and above the neuropsychological predictors alone. A higher level of FAA, corresponding to greater alpha power over the right frontal lobe, increased the likelihood that a participant would be classified as MCI. In contrast, the opposite was the case for FBA, where greater levels of beta over the right hemisphere decreased the likelihood of classification as MCI.
Three observations about the model bear comment, although we cannot draw any firm conclusions without further research and hypothesis testing. First, there was no main effect of group on FAA, yet in the context of the logistic regression it was a powerful predictor accounting for unique variance in group membership. Second, FBA was the single most powerful predictor in the context of the model. Diagnostics indicated that estimation errors from collinearity cannot explain the pattern, so it appears to be an unmasking of the relationship between FAA and MCI. Third, FAA and FBA had opposite signs, indicating that MCI was characterized by greater relative left hemispheric beta and greater relative right hemispheric alpha. This is an empirical pattern uncovered in an exploratory analysis, so the mechanism is unknown. We can speculate that it might reflect compensatory processes in the behavioral activating system (BAS) or approach motivation system. Alternatively, it may reflect disruption of the behavioral inhibition system (BIS), but it is unclear why that would disinhibit only or predominantly the left hemisphere. This brings up many questions about whether this phenomenon would be exaggerated in AD or would reverse as neurodegeneration progresses past the point of compensation.
Also important was the observation that computerized neuropsychological tests were predictive, providing further evidence that this mode of testing may be sufficiently reliable and valid to support future diagnosis and research on MCI and AD. Past research with this population has indicated that Oral Reading test scores were predictive of MCI (Kairys et al., 2021). Our observations replicate that finding and add that along with other neuropsychological tests, FAA and FBA both accounted for significant variability in group membership, over and above cognitive test measures, alone. Interestingly, FAA was not significantly associated with MCI in the ANOVAs (see above), nor by itself, but when added to a model along with FBA, both were predictive. Although the variance inflation factors were within commonly accepted limits, the potential for multicollinearity means that it would be valuable to see this result replicated in independently collected data before too much weight is given to this potentially important finding.
The finding that resting FBA was altered after doing a motion direction discrimination task was incidental and not predicted but fits in with a growing set of findings of resting-state oscillatory dynamics that are altered by sensory-perceptual tasks (Kavcic et al., 2021). FAA, although not statistically significant (p = 0.06), was also reduced after the task. Further study is necessary to understand how meaningful such changes might be. Are they purely coincidental and unreplicable, or might the susceptibility to such effects be an index of such characteristics as stability and plasticity?
Frontal theta and delta asymmetries were not investigated in this report. Although both event-related and resting-state theta and delta have been extensively investigated with respect to cognitive functions (Klimesch, 1999; Klimesch et al., 2005; Knyazev, 2007; Tesche and Karhu, 2000), to our knowledge frontal asymmetries in these frequency bands have not been extensively reported. Whether this reflects file drawers full of null results or an important gap in the literature is unclear, but in either case we did not feel that we would have a ready interpretation of any exploratory effects or correlations that might be found.
4.1. Conclusions
We conducted an analysis of common psychophysiological measures, including FAA and FBA, that to our knowledge have not been investigated in relation to MCI. The strongest effect that we found was a group difference in FBA, with MCI being more lateralized than controls. FBA was also shifted rightward after a motion direction discrimination task, and, although not significant, FAA showed a similar rightward shift after the task. Finally, although there was no main effect of group on FAA, FAA was a significant predictor when in a model with FBA, and both FAA and FBA added discriminability after accounting for tasks from two cognitive test batteries. Future research should test the robustness of these findings and potential applications in the diagnosis and evaluation of treatment for neurodegenerative diseases.
Acknowledgments
This research was in part supported by grants from NIA/NIH, 1R21AG046637-01A1, 1R01AG054484-01A1, Alzheimer’s Association Award HAT-07-60437 grant, and a grant from the Slovenian Research Agency, P3-0366/2451 to VK and by partial support from NIH/NIA grant P30AG053760 to the Michigan Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center (MADC).
Footnotes
Declaration of competing interest
The authors declared no conflict of interest with respect to this research, authorship, and/or the publication of this article.
References
- Allen JJ, Coan JA, Nazarian M, 2004. Issues and assumptions on the road from raw signals to metrics of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion. Biol. Psychol 67 (1–2), 183–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babiloni C, Binetti G, Cassetta E, Dal Forno G, Del Percio C, Ferreri F, Ferri R, Frisoni G, Hirata K, Lanuzza B, Miniussi C, Moretti DV, Nobili F, Rodriguez G, Romani GL, Salinari S, Rossini PM, 2006. Sources of cortical rhythms change as a function of cognitive impairment in pathological aging: a multicenter study. Clin. Neurophysiol 117 (2), 252–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babiloni C, Lizio R, Vecchio F, Frisoni GB, Pievani M, Geroldi C, Rossini PM, 2010. Reactivity of cortical alpha rhythms to eye opening in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: an EEG study. J. Alzheimers Dis 22 (4), 1047–1064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babiloni C, Frisoni GB, Vecchio F, Lizio R, Pievani M, Cristina G, Rossini PM, 2011. Stability of clinical condition in mild cognitive impairment is related to cortical sources of alpha rhythms: an electroencephalographic study. Hum. Brain Mapp 32 (11), 1916–1931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babiloni C, Carducci F, Lizio R, Vecchio F, Baglieri A, Bernardini S, Cavedo E, Bozzao A, Buttinelli C, Esposito F, Giubilei F, Guizzaro A, Marino S, Montella P, Quattrocchi CC, Redolfi A, Soricelli A, Tedeschi G, Ferri R, Rossi-Fedele G, Ursini F, Scrascia F, Vernieri F, Pedersen TJ, Hardemark H-G, Rossini PM, Frisoni GB, 2013. Resting state cortical electroencephalographic rhythms are related to gray matter volume in subjects with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp 34 (6), 1427–1446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babiloni C, Del Percio C, Lizio R, Marzano N, Infarinato F, Soricelli A, Salvatore E, Ferri R, Bonforte C, Tedeschi G, Montella P, Baglieri A, Rodriguez G, Fama F, Nobili F, Vernieri F, Ursini F, Mundi C, Frisoni G, Rossini PM, 2014. Cortical sources of resting state electroencephalographic alpha rhythms deteriorate across time in subjects with amnesic mild cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Aging 35 (1), 130–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Babiloni C, Arakaki X, Azami H, Bennys K, Blinowska K, Bonanni L, Guntekin B, 2021. Measures of resting state EEG rhythms for clinical trials in Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations of an expert panel. Alzheimers Dement. 17 (9), 1528–1553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bachman DL, Wolf PA, Linn RT, Knoefel JE, Cobb JL, Belanger AJ, d’Agostino RB, 1993. Incidence of dementia and probable Alzheimer’s disease in a general population: the Framingham study. Neurology 43 (3 Part 1), 515–515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsley DA, 1984. Reply. Am. Stat 38, 90–93. [Google Scholar]
- Clark LT, Watkins L, Piña IL, Elmer M, Akinboboye O, Gorham M, Jamerson B, McCullough C, Pierre C, Polis AB, Puckrein G, Regnante JM, 2019. Increasing diversity in clinical trials: overcoming critical barriers. Curr. Probl. Cardiol 44 (5), 148–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coan JA, Allen JJ, 2003. Frontal EEG asymmetry and the behavioral activation and inhibition systems. Psychophysiology 40 (1), 106–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cummins TD, Broughton M, Finnigan S, 2008. Theta oscillations are affected by amnestic mild cognitive impairment and cognitive load. Int. J. Psychophysiol 70 (1), 75–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darby D, Maruff P, Collie A, McStephen M, 2002. Mild cognitive impairment can be detected by multiple assessments in a single day. Neurology 59 (7), 1042–1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson RJ, 1988. EEG measures of cerebral asymmetry: conceptual and methodological issues. Int. J. Neurosci 39 (1–2), 71–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson RJ, et al. , 1990. Asymmetrical brain electrical activity discriminates between psychometrically-matched verbal and spatial cognitive tasks. Psychophysiology 27 (5), 528–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson RJ, 1992. Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain Cogn. 20 (1), 125–151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson RJ, Schwartz GE, Saron C, Bennett J, Goleman DJ, 1979. Frontal versus parietal EEG asymmetry during positive and negative affect. Psychophysiology 16, 202–203. [Google Scholar]
- Derflinger S, Sorg C, Gaser C, Myers N, Arsic M, Kurz A, Zimmer K, Wohlschläger A, Mühlau M, 2011. Grey-matter atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease is asymmetric but not lateralized. J. Alzheimers Dis 25 (2), 347–357. 10.3233/JAD-2011-110041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erves JC, Mayo-Gamble TL, Malin-Fair A, Boyer A, Joosten Y, Vaughn YC, Sherden L, Luther P, Miller S, Wilkins CH, 2017. Needs, priorities, and recommendations for engaging underrepresented populations in clinical research: a community perspective. J. Community Health 42 (3), 472–480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernández A, Arrazola J, Maestú F, Amo C, Gil-Gregorio P, Wienbruch C, Ortiz T, 2003. Correlations of hippocampal atrophy and focal low-frequency magnetic activity in alzheimer disease: volumetric MR imaging- magnetoencephalographic study. Am. J. Neuroradiol 24 (3), 481–487. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finnigan S, Robertson IH, 2011. Resting EEG theta power correlates with cognitive performance in healthy older adults. Psychophysiology 48 (8), 1083–1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher JA, Kalbaugh CA, 2011. Challenging assumptions about minority participation in US clinical research. Am. J. Public Health 101 (12), 2217–2222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson J, Maruff P, Woodward M, Moore L, Fredrickson A, Sach J, Darby D, 2010. Evaluation of the usability of a brief computerized cognitive screening test in older people for epidemiological studies. Neuroepidemiology 34 (2), 65–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gable PA, Neal LB, Threadgill AH, 2018. Regulatory behavior and frontal activity: considering the role of revised-BIS in relative right frontal asymmetry. Psychophysiology 55 (1), e12910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galluzzi S, Cimaschi L, Ferrucci L, Frisoni GB, 2001. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical features and review of screening instruments. Aging Clin. Exp. Res 13 (3), 183–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gershon RC, Wagster MV, Hendrie HC, Fox NA, Cook KF, Nowinski CJ, 2013. NIH toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function. Neurology 80 (11), S2–S6. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e5f. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giordani B, 2011. Reliability of repeated cognitive assessment of dementia using a brief computer battery. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement 26 (4), 326–333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray JA, 1970. The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion. Behav. Res. Ther 8 (3), 249–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagemann D, Naumann E, Thayer JF, 2001. The quest for the EEG reference revisited: a glance from brain asymmetry research. Psychophysiology 38 (5), 847–857. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammers D, Spurgeon E, Ryan K, Persad C, Heidebrink J, Barbas N, Giordani B, 2011. Reliability of repeated cognitive assessment of dementia using a brief computerized battery. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Dement 26 (4), 326–333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammers D, Spurgeon E, Ryan K, Persad C, Barbas N, Heidebrink J, Giordani B, 2012. Validity of a brief computerized cognitive screening test in dementia. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol 25 (2), 89–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmon-Jones E, Allen JJ, 1997. Behavioral activation sensitivity and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: covariation of putative indicators related to risk for mood disorders. J. Abnorm. Psychol 106 (1), 159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmon-Jones E, Allen JJ, 1998. Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 74 (5), 1310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmon-Jones E, Gable PA, 2018. On the role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in approach and withdrawal motivation: an updated review of the evidence. Psychophysiology 55 (1), e12879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harmon-Jones E, Gable PA, Peterson CK, 2010. The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: a review and update. Biol. Psychol 84 (3), 451–462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haxby JV, Duara R, Grady CL, Cutler NR, Rapoport SI, 1985. Relations between neuropsychological and cerebral metabolic asymmetries in early Alzheimer’s disease. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab 5 (2), 193–200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hewig J, Hagemann D, Seifert J, Naumann E, Bartussek D, 2006. The relation of cortical activity and BIS/BAS on the trait level. Biol. Psychol 71 (1), 42–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes JR, Cayaffa JJ, 1977. The EEG in patients at different ages without organic cerebral disease. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol 42 (6), 776–784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- John TN, Dharmapalan PS, Menon NR, 2019. Exploration of time–frequency reassignment and homologous inter-hemispheric asymmetry analysis of MCI–AD brain activity. BMC Neurosci. 20 (1), 1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kairys A, Daugherty A, Kavcic V, Shair S, Persad C, Heidebrink J, Giordani B, 2021. Laptop-administered NIH toolbox and cogstate brief battery in community-dwelling black adults: unexpected pattern of cognitive performance between MCI and healthy controls. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Kavcic V, Daugherty AM, Giordani B, 2021. Post-task modulation of resting state EEG differentiates MCI patients from controls. Alzheimers Dement. 13 (1), e12153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, Tenke CE, 2015a. On the benefits of using surface laplacian (current source density) methodology in electrophysiology. Int. J. Psychophysiol 97 (3), 171–173. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kayser J, Tenke CE, 2015b. Issues and considerations for using the scalp surface laplacian in EEG/ERP research: a tutorial review. Int. J. Psychophysiol 97 (3), 189–209. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, 1996. Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. Int. J. Psychophysiol 24, 61–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, 1999. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev 29 (2–3), 169–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, Schack B, Sauseng P, 2005. The functional significance of theta and upper alpha oscillations. Exp. Psychol 52 (2), 99–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S, 2007. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev 53 (1), 63–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knyazev GG, 2007. Motivation, emotion, and their inhibitory control mirrored in brain oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 31 (3), 377–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lacey MF, Neal LB, Gable PA, 2020. Effortful control of motivation, not withdrawal motivation, relates to greater right frontal asymmetry. Int. J. Psychophysiol 147, 18–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loewenstein DA, et al. , 1989. Predominant left hemisphere metabolic dysfunction in dementia. Arch. Neurol 46 (2), 146–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopez-Duran NL, Nusslock R, George C, Kovacs M, 2012. Frontal EEG asymmetry moderates the effects of stressful life events on internalizing symptoms in children at familial risk for depression. Psychophysiology 49 (4), 510–521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mah L, Anderson ND, Verhoeff NPL, Pollock BG, 2017. Negative emotional verbal memory biases in mild cognitive impairment and late-onset depression. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25 (10), 1160–1170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathewson KE, Lleras A, Beck DM, Fabiani M, Ro T, Gratton G, 2011. Pulsed out of awareness: EEG alpha oscillations represent a pulsed-inhibition of ongoing cortical processing. Front. Psychol 2, 99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Missonnier P, Gold G, Herrmann FR, Fazio-Costa L, Michel JP, Deiber MP, Giannakopoulos P, 2006. Decreased theta event-related synchronization during working memory activation is associated with progressive mild cognitive impairment. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord 22 (3), 250–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris JC, Weintraub S, Chui HC, Cummings J, DeCarli C, Ferris S, Foster NL, Galasko D, Graff-Radford N, Peskind ER, Beekly D, Ramos EM, Kukull WA, 2006. The uniform data set (UDS): clinical and cognitive variables and descriptive data from alzheimer disease centers. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord 20 (4), 210–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mundorf A, Peterburs J, Ocklenburg S, 2021. Asymmetry in the central nervous system: a clinical neuroscience perspective. Front. Syst. Neurosci 15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ocklenburg S, Friedrich P, Schmitz J, Schlüter C, Genc E, Güntürkün O, Peterburs J, Grimshaw G, 2019. Beyond frontal alpha: investigating hemispheric asymmetries over the EEG frequency spectrum as a function of sex and handedness. Laterality 24 (5), 505–524. 10.1080/1357650X.2018.1543314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panza F, D’Introno A, Colacicco AM, Capurso C, Del Parigi A, Caselli RJ, Gandin C, 2009. Temporal relationship between depressive symptoms and cognitive impairment: the italian longitudinal study on aging. J. Alzheimers Dis 17 (4), 899–911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pérez-Edgar K, Kujawa A, Nelson SK, Cole C, Zapp DJ, 2013. The relation between electroencephalogram asymmetry and attention biases to threat at baseline and under stress. Brain Cogn. 82 (3), 337–343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, Ritchie K, Rossor M, Thal T, Winblad B, 2001. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol 58 (12), 1985–1992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ray WJ, Cole HW, 1985. EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. Science 228 (4700), 750–752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues J, Müller M, Mühlberger A, Hewig J, 2018. Mind the movement: frontal asymmetry stands for behavioral motivation, bilateral frontal activation for behavior. Psychophysiology 55 (1), e12908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaffer CE, Davidson RJ, Saron C, 1983. Frontal and parietal electroencephalogram asymmetry in depressed and nondepressed subjects. Biol. Psychiatry 18 (7), 753–762. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Senaviratna NAMR, Cooray TMJA, 2019. Diagnosing multicollinearity of logistic regression model. Asian J. Probab. Stat 5, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
- Sturm VE, Yokoyama JS, Seeley WW, Kramer JH, Miller BL, Rankin KP, 2013. Heightened emotional contagion in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease is associated with temporal lobe degeneration. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 110 (24), 9944–9949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton SK, Davidson RJ, 1997. Prefrontal brain asymmetry: a biological substrate of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychol. Sci 8 (3), 204–210. [Google Scholar]
- Tesche CD, Karhu J, 2000. Theta oscillations index human hippocampal activation during a working memory task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 97 (2), 919–924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thibodeau R, Jorgensen RS, Kim S, 2006. Depression, anxiety, and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: a meta-analytic review. J. Abnorm. Psychol 115 (4), 715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub S, Dikmen SS, Heaton RK, Tulsky DS, Zelazo PD, Bauer PJ, Carlozzi NE, Slotkin J, Blitz D, Wallner-Allen K, Fox NA, Beaumont JL, Mungas D, Nowinski CJ, Richler J, Deocampo JA, Anderson JE, Manly JJ, Borosh B, Havlik R, Conway K, Edwards E, Freund L, King JW, Moy C, Witt E, Gershon RC, 2013. Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology 80 (11 Supplement 3). S54–S64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitham EM, Lewis T, Pope KJ, Fitzgibbon SP, Clark CR, Loveless S, Willoughby JO, 2008. Thinking activates EMG in scalp electrical recordings. Clin. Neurophysiol 119 (5), 1166–1175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang C, Zhong S, Zhou X, Wei L, Wang L, Nie S, 2017. The abnormality of topological asymmetry between hemispheric brain white matter networks in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Front. Aging Neurosci 9, 261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zahn R, Juengling F, Bubrowski P, Jost E, Dykierek P, Talazko J, Huell M, 2004. Hemispheric asymmetries of hypometabolism associated with semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: a study using positron emission tomography with fluorodeoxyglucose-F18. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 132 (2), 159–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]