Abstract
Each of us made the remarkable journey from mere matter to mind: starting life as a quiescent oocyte (“just chemistry and physics”), and slowly, gradually, becoming an adult human with complex metacognitive processes, hopes, and dreams. In addition, even though we feel ourselves to be a unified, single Self, distinct from the emergent dynamics of termite mounds and other swarms, the reality is that all intelligence is collective intelligence: each of us consists of a huge number of cells working together to generate a coherent cognitive being with goals, preferences, and memories that belong to the whole and not to its parts. Basal cognition is the quest to understand how Mind scales—how large numbers of competent subunits can work together to become intelligences that expand the scale of their possible goals. Crucially, the remarkable trick of turning homeostatic, cell-level physiological competencies into large-scale behavioral intelligences is not limited to the electrical dynamics of the brain. Evolution was using bioelectric signaling long before neurons and muscles appeared, to solve the problem of creating and repairing complex bodies. In this Perspective, I review the deep symmetry between the intelligence of developmental morphogenesis and that of classical behavior. I describe the highly conserved mechanisms that enable the collective intelligence of cells to implement regulative embryogenesis, regeneration, and cancer suppression. I sketch the story of an evolutionary pivot that repurposed the algorithms and cellular machinery that enable navigation of morphospace into the behavioral navigation of the 3D world which we so readily recognize as intelligence. Understanding the bioelectric dynamics that underlie construction of complex bodies and brains provides an essential path to understanding the natural evolution, and bioengineered design, of diverse intelligences within and beyond the phylogenetic history of Earth.
Keywords: Morphogenesis, Voltage, Development, Regeneration, Basal cognition, Evolution, Behavior, Cells
Introduction: from cells to cognition in the collective intelligence of organisms
All animals with advanced conventional cognitive capacities are the products of a very lengthy evolutionary process of accumulating gradual modifications of early microbial life. Even more remarkably, each of us personally took a journey in which matter acquired mind: we start life as a quiescent oocyte, and slowly remodel into an adult modern human with metacognitive, self-aware capacities and the ability to reason about entire counterfactual universes. How did the information processing capabilities of a single cell (Lyon 2006, 2015), with metabolic and physiological competencies, inflate to that of a human, who can pursue goals of planetary scale whose endpoint may be far after their expected lifespan (Fig. 1)? Taking evolution, and especially developmental biology, seriously means coming to grips with the continuous process of transformation that enables configurations of matter to express increasingly greater degrees and kinds of cognitive capabilities: decision-making, generalization, perception, causality detection, innate responses, learning, and advanced problem-solving. The emerging field of basal cognition (Baluška and Levin 2016; Levin et al. 2021; Lyon et al. 2021) strives to understand the evolutionary and embryological origins of our current behavioral capacities, and uncover the physical and computational dynamics by which simple minds emerge from chemistry and scale over time.
It is essential to expand the scope of this inquiry beyond traditional metrics of intelligence and behavior (Fig. 2). As humans, we are very good at recognizing intelligence of medium-sized objects moving at medium speeds in three-dimensional space: our sense organs face outward, and our capacities for detecting agency in this action space are strong (Mar et al. 2007; Repp and Knoblich 2007). However, assigning an IQ estimate to any being or object is, in effect, taking an IQ test ourselves: it is easy to miss intelligent behavior if one cannot recognize it. Imagine if we had a well-developed internal sense of our own body chemistry states—if we had the equivalent of a tongue that faced inwards into the bloodstream, it would then be natural for us to recognize all the remarkably clever things our liver and kidneys were doing with respect to managing us in the high-dimensional space of body physiology. Could recognizing diverse intelligence (in unfamiliar substrates) be essential to understanding the mechanisms and origins of conventional cognition and behavior?
William James anticipated a cybernetic approach to this problem by defining intelligence as a degree of “the ability to reach the same goal by different means” (James 1890). This definition is not limited to brains or specific kinds of behaviors—it is intentionally agnostic about the composition of the agent. It challenges us to define a problem space, for any arbitrary system (including unconventional embodiments), and then formulate and test specific hypotheses about the competencies that the system can deploy in navigating that space. Traditional concepts of reflex responses, drives, memory, different kinds of learning, goal-directed activity, and even higher level capacities such as planning and creativity can all be defined in other problem spaces beyond the familiar 3D space, including metabolic, physiological, transcriptional, and anatomical spaces.
It has previously been proposed that the evolutionary path to conventional cognition involves evolutionary pivots across these problem spaces (Fields and Levin 2022). This suggests a broadening of the traditional systems to which these cognitive terms apply, and asking what is essential about them that is deeper than the specific frozen accidents that the course of terrestrial evolution has provided in its N = 1 history of life on Earth (Clawson and Levin 2022). Here, through the lens of a gradualist, evolutionary perspective I explore the possible origins of cognitive capacities and the extension of these concepts to diverse embodiments.
The goal is not to review the steps of evolution of electrically excitable networks (Arendt et al. 2016; Brunet and Arendt 2016; Jekely 2019; Jekely et al. 2015; Keijzer and Arnellos 2017; Keijzer 2017; Levin et al. 2021; Lyon et al. 2021). Rather, I aim to provide a novel perspective on how to understand animal behavior in the broader context of “control of agential activity” in all its diverse guises. A deep unification is within reach, if we can find and exploit invariants—symmetries between a variety of processes and biological systems that shed light on generic principles for scaling of cognition and collective intelligence. In fact, all intelligence is collective intelligence—not just termite mounds and beehives. We too are emergent beings supervening on a collection of cells which all were once independent unicellular organisms. How do they work together to enable the creation of a novel being with goals and memories that belong to it and not to any of its parts? The key question is not only the scale-up of a unitary cognitive capacity, but the many-into-one transition: the emergence of minds from collectives (Levin 2019, 2021b), working in spaces beyond those of their component cells.
At stake is not only a better understanding of our evolutionary history, but also insight into the relationship between genome and functional forms, and strategies for modifying, improving, and building novel synthetic cognitive agents (with implications ranging from exobiology to regenerative medicine). The over-arching framework (Levin 2022) is fundamentally grounded in the continuity hypothesis, using biophysical and evolutionary approaches to identify invariants and symmetries across natural evolved forms, as well as hybrid and fully synthetic life forms (and perhaps, some day, truly alien beings) (Fields and Levin 2022; Levin 2022). Here, I illustrate one segment of this framework’s roadmap by focusing on one highly instructive example: developmental bioelectricity as a precursor of brain-like processes, which reveals not only evolutionary pivots between two different problem spaces, but also shows a path to solving the problem of collective intelligence across scales of organization. In this discussion, I intentionally avoid issues of first-person experiential consciousness, focusing instead on third-person observable capacities for behavior broadly. I describe how computation via voltage states enabled coordinate navigation of anatomical morphospace before brains and muscles enabled us to navigate 3D space, as an example of how familiar behavioral science concepts can be generalized to gain insight into the origin and mechanisms of cognition. Here, cognition is not restricted to advanced capabilities such as planning, self-aware metacognition, language, etc., but is considered broadly in accordance with its continuous developmental and evolutionary origins. It serves as an umbrella term for all degrees of adaptive information processing and problem-solving (Levin 2022), no matter how advanced or primitive. The focus on bioelectricity below, as a kind of tractable cognitive glue, is one facet of a broader emerging research program designed to help detect, understand, and relate to a wide range of natural, artificial, and hybrid intelligences regardless of size scale or material implementation (Abramson and Levin 2021; Calvo et al. 2020; Fields et al. 2021; Fields and Levin 2022; Ginsburg and Jablonka 2021; Gokhale et al. 2021; Kuchling et al. 2020; Levin 2021b, 2022; Lyon 2019; Ramstead et al. 2022; Smith-Ferguson and Beekman 2020; Timsit and Grégoire 2021; Watson et al. 2022).
Beyond standard model organisms: stories of plasticity and change
To begin to broaden the inquiry into the evolutionary origin and mechanisms of conventional cognition, it is important to consider several biological examples that stretch standard assumptions (Fig. 3). First, consider Physarum: a unicellular slime mold, which has been used as a popular model for basal cognition (Beekman and Latty 2015; Nakagaki and Guy 2007; Reid et al. 2016; Saigusa et al. 2008; Vallverdú et al. 2018). Recent work has shown that this organism can exhibit learning following repeated experience, eventually becoming willing to cross areas of noxious chemicals to receive a reward (Boisseau et al. 2016; Boussard et al. 2019; Vogel and Dussutour 2016). Moreover, when placed in an arena containing distant inert glass objects of different mass distributions, it uses a biomechanical mechanism to process information about its environment and then reliably grow towards the heavier object (Murugan et al. 2021). These examples of learning and decision-making occur in the absence of a brain, neurons, or cellularization. From the perspective of evolutionary change across behavioral domains, one interesting thing is that for Physarum, its behavior is its morphological change. In this system, changing body shape to exploit the environment is how it implements behavior. Distinctions between morphological problems and behavioral problems are blurred by the biology, and the classical (Cartesian) conceptual distinctions between mind and body, which lead to separate communities for developmental biology and behavioral science, are now increasingly seen as artificial, for example, in the field of morphological computation in robotics (Bongard and Levin 2021).
It has been proposed that the mechanisms of memory establishment are the same as those which sculpt brain tissue developmentally (Galván 2010; Kandel and O'Dell 1992). Planaria offer another example, where anatomical and behavioral information are tightly linked (Saló et al. 2009). These free-living flatworms, with a true centralized, bilaterian brain (Pagán 2014; Sarnat and Netsky 2002), have the ability to regenerate their bodies—every piece of a planarian gives rise to a properly patterned new worm (Sheiman and Kreshchenko 2015). However, this process not only implements an anatomical memory of body structure. Tails of worms trained on specific tasks regenerate into animals that show recall of the original information: the behavioral memories are apparently also imprinted onto the new brain as it forms (McConnell et al. 1959; Shomrat and Levin 2013). Thus, the information processing required to restore a specific body shape and that required to propagate the results of past experience despite cellular turnover and maintenance are tightly linked.
Metamorphosis tells a similar story. Caterpillars must become moths or butterflies, requiring turning a controller that operates a soft body in a two-dimensional lifestyle into one that operates a hard body in a three-dimensional world. The brain is largely dismantled and rebuilt in a new configuration, but learned information persists (Alloway 1972; Blackiston et al. 2008; Sheiman and Tiras 1996). Such dynamic plasticity is not just for invertebrates. Tadpoles of the frog Xenopus laevis can be produced with no primary eyes, but one eye on their tails; these animals can see out of an ectopic eye, which can connect to the spinal cord (Blackiston and Levin 2013). In one generation, requiring no evolutionary adaptation, the cellular hardware of a frog embryo can adapt to this radical reconfiguration of its visual sensory system.
Thus, radical changes of behavioral repertoires occur not only on evolutionary time-scales, but also at the level of an individual being—paralleling embryogenesis in raising profound questions about the transformation of agents by rearrangements of their parts—a key aspect of recognizing even traditional animals as fundamentally collective intelligences. A key aspect of understanding both basal and traditional cognition is formulating paradigms for predicting the properties and capabilities of emergent Selves from those of the components that comprise them.
This is fundamentally a story of the multi-scale competency architecture that biology employs. Animals are nested dolls made up of cells and tissues but this arrangement is not merely structural; cells were once unicellular organisms and have many competencies in their own problem spaces [an agential material which evolution molds by behavior-shaping as much as changes in their hardware (Davies and Levin 2023)]. When one “trains a rat” to obtain a treat by pressing a lever, the cells that interact with the lever (skin) are not the same cells that obtain the metabolic reward (intestine). No single cell had both experiences, and the owner of the associative memory linking those two events is an emergent collective intelligence (Fig. 1A). Likewise, even gene regulatory networks can learn relationships between their experiences (such as Pavlovian conditioning) as a collective, by virtue of the activity of very simple components (transcriptional elements) (Biswas et al. 2021; Watson et al. 2010). Conversely, the voluntary act of raising one’s arm reveals the functional connection between the highest levels of executive function to the depolarization of muscle cells—information crossing levels from that of human thoughts to the physiological status of individual cells. What is the coordination mechanism that enables this cross-level integrated information processing? It is developmental bioelectricity.
Bioelectricity: the ancient cognitive glue
The functional properties of brains and nervous systems emerge from a network architecture in which neurons (and other cells like glia) communicate via changes in cellular resting potential (Vmem) and the resulting movement of neurotransmitter signals. Vmem, the bioelectric state of each nervous system component, is determined by an integrated balance of charges via the action of ion channels and pumps that enable the segregation of potassium, sodium, chloride, and protons (Fig. 4). Bioelectric states can propagate between cells via electrochemical synapses known as gap junctions (GJs) (Mathews and Levin 2017; Palacios-Prado and Bukauskas 2009). GJs form direct connections between the cellular internal milieus that enable the transfer of current and small chemical messengers. In addition, bioelectric states can also induce the movement of neurotransmitter molecules, such as serotonin, both through the extracellular space (via transporters and vesicles) and directly through gap junctions (Levin et al. 2006; Romero-Reyes et al. 2021). Crucially, every component in this network is functionally regulated by every other, and by its own action: ion channels and gap junctions are themselves often voltage-sensitive (Brink 2000; Palacios-Prado and Bukauskas 2009), while neurotransmitters move under electromotive force and regulation, and in turn regulate ion channel properties.
This extremely rich set of feedback loops establishes computational capacity; for example, ion channels and GJs, as voltage-gated current conductances, are in effect transistors and possess a fundamental property of historicity (memory in which past events impact current signaling state). These events do eventually impact other kinds of pathways (such as gene expression), but it is critical that the information processing in such networks is essentially physiological—the rapid propagation of signals via action potentials and slow waves across the network does not itself require transcriptional change. As a corollary, the information content of this network cannot be read out at the transcriptional or even proteomic level: channels open and close post-translationally, and the same channels can give rise to different voltage states depending on cells’ history, while diverse channels can give rise to the exact same voltage map.
There is no one-to-one mapping between the molecular state and the bioelectrical state, making it essential to study such systems in the living condition (unlike genetic and protein-level information, which can be studied in fractionated or fixed material, bioelectrical information disappears at cellular death). One implication of this feature is a critical separation of hardware and software (Boone and Piccinini 2016). Of course, the ability to form stable behavioral repertoires based on specific kinds of past experience, and the way in which it coarse-grains and generalizes from a sensory stream, are shaped by the network’s structure and physical properties. However, structurally identical networks can have learned different things after physiological experiences—bioelectric networks’ historicity means that their information content is not hardwired by their genetic specification but is dependent on past experience. One cannot know the informational content of a brain merely from knowing its neural layout and genome: the exact same brain can contain numerous different memories, goals, etc. This decoupling of the material state (protein content) from the information content is the first step to the most amazing aspect of neural networks: they enable mind to arise from matter. Specifically, neural networks are the functional layer in which physiology transitions to meaning: electrophysiological events encode memories, plans, preferences, behavioral capacities, and a first-person perspective—the content and shape of a cognitive Self, at whatever level of sophistication.
Neuroscience [via the research program of neural decoding (Huth et al. 2016; Naselaris et al. 2009; Nishimoto et al. 2011)] is committed to the idea that all of the content of minds, from the most primitive to the most complex, can be read out from (and thus resides in) the electrophysiological state of the network. Thus, just as inorganic electric circuits and transistors enable the jump from the physics of Ohm’s law to the truth tables of logic gates (basic elements of formal thought), biological electric circuits and ion channels/GJs enable the jump from chemistry to embodied meaning. A crucial aspect of this is that it provides a scaling up of agency: the homeostatic competencies of single neurons are integrated into a network that supports an emergent, higher order Self with memories, preferences, and other features that belong to itself but not to any of its components individually. Bioelectric networks, with their inherent plasticity, multiscale historicity, and learning capacity, are an ideal kind of “cognitive glue” that binds the primitive goal-directedness (in the cybernetic sense) of single cells into a higher order system with a larger cognitive light cone (Fig. 1E). However, perhaps, the most fascinating and far-reaching aspect is that the key features that enable the supervenience of active information on a material substrate are not unique to brains and nervous systems at all (Fig. 4).
The use of bioelectric networks to enable coherent computation to occur via a spatially and temporally distributed living medium is ancient, both in its molecular components (which date back to our unicellular ancestors) and in the algorithms by which it provides adaptive function. Even bacterial biofilms use electrical networks to synchronize activity within the proto-body of the colony (Martinez-Corral et al. 2019; Prindle et al. 2015; Yang et al. 2020). Evolution discovered long ago that networks made up of ion channels, gap junctions, and neurotransmitters as transduction machinery for electric circuit function provide a remarkably powerful and flexible way to process information. The current use of these components to implement behaviors in 3D space by controlling muscle activity represents an evolutionary pivot: their original usage was to navigate anatomical morphospace (Fields et al. 2020) (that is, to control all cell behaviors toward specific morphogenetic outcomes). This conservation of both molecular mechanisms and algorithms is the reason that the workhorse tools of neuroscience work everywhere in the body—they do not distinguish between neuronal and non-neuronal uses. Clear morphogenetic analogs (Pezzulo and Levin 2015) exist for optogenetics, ion channel mutants, neurotransmitter drugs—the tools of neuroscience—as well as for neuroscience concepts, such as memory, representation, navigation, perceptual bistability, and many others (Table 1).
Table 1.
Cognition | Morphogenesis |
---|---|
Action potential movement within an axon | Differential patterns of Vmem across single cells’ surface |
Local field potential (EEG) | Vmem distribution of cell group |
Intrinsic plasticity | Change of ion channel expression based on Vmem levels |
Synaptic plasticity | Change of cell:cell connectivity via Vmem’s regulation of gap junctional connectivity |
Activity-dependent transcriptional changes | Bioelectric signals’ regulating gene expression during patterning |
Neuromodulation | Developmental (pre-nervous) signaling via neurotransmitters such as serotonin moving under control of bioelectrical gradients |
Direct transmission | Cell:cell sharing of voltage via nanotubes or gap junctions |
Volume transmission | Cell:cell communication via ion levels outside the membrane or voltage-dependent neurotransmitter release |
Synaptic vesicles | Exosomes |
Sensitization | Cells become sensitized to BMP antagonists to stabilize neurogenesis |
Functional lateralization | Left–right asymmetry of body organs |
Taste and olfactory perception | Morphogenetic signaling by diffusible biochemical ligands |
Activity-dependent modification of CNS | Control of anatomy by bioelectric signaling within those same cells |
Critical plasticity periods | Competency windows for developmental induction events |
Autonomic reflexes | Wound healing |
Voluntary movement | Remodeling, regeneration, metamorphosis |
Memory | Shorter term: regeneration of specific body organs. Longer term: morphological homeostasis over decades as individual cells senesce; altering basic body anatomy in planaria by direct manipulation of bioelectric circuit |
Generalization via multi-layer neural networks | Generalization by bow-tie architectures of signaling pathways |
Pattern completion ability of neural networks (e.g., attractor nets) | Regeneration of missing parts in partial fragments (e.g., planaria) |
Forgetting | Cancer, loss of regenerative ability |
Addiction | Limb becomes unable to regenerate without nerve once exposed to nerve |
Encoding | Representation of patterning goal states by bioelectric properties of tissue |
Perceptual bi-stability | Stochastic flipping between two target morphologies by planarian fragments in the cryptic state |
McGurk effect | Modification of interpretation of biochemical signals by bioelectric state of cells |
Visual system feature detection | Organ-level decision-making during morphogenesis |
Mirror neurons and mirror focus in epilepsy | Contralateral bioelectric signals mirroring sites of amputation damage in frog legs |
Holographic (distributed) storage | Any small piece of a planarian remembers the correct pattern (even if it has been re-written) |
Instinct | Hardwired patterning programs (mosaic development) |
Behavioral plasticity | Regulative developmental programs and regenerative capacity |
Self-modeling | Surveillance of anatomical state by brain |
Goal-seeking | Embryogenesis and regeneration work towards a specific target configuration despite perturbations |
Sub-goaling in problem solving tasks | Developmental modularity |
Adaptivity and intelligence | Morphological rearrangements carrying out novel, not hardwired, movements to reach the same anatomical configuration despite unpredictable initial starting state |
Tabula rasa | Cells could be a (semi) universal constructor, able to build any shape that can be specified via the pattern memory code |
Attention and context salience | Tissues responding to specific bioelectric states only when relevant (e.g., when in damage state) |
Age-dependent cognitive decline | Age-dependent loss of regenerative ability |
Optogenetic insertion of false memories | Optogenetic induction of regeneration or ectopic organs |
Reading of semantic content from brain scans | Detecting differences in target morphology from fluorescent voltage dye data |
Executive control (free action) filtering down to regulate muscle motion | Large-scale circuit decisions that dictate whole bodyplan axial patterning transduced to individual cell gene expression changes as needed to implement large-scale phenotype |
The bioelectric system is so versatile that it was readily exapted for behavior when nerve and muscle evolved (Keijzer et al. 2013), with two major changes: a significant speed-up (milliseconds, instead of hours, as the primary time scale) and a focus on temporal signaling (spiking patterns) for behavior instead of development’s reliance on spatial bioelectric patterns across tissues. Despite these differences, profound symmetries between the problems of morphogenesis and the problems of cognition exist; remarkably, while only recently explored in detail, this idea was already obvious to Alan Turing as early as 1952 (Turing 1952) and to others in the following decades (Grossberg 1978). Much as electrophysiological neural events are the fundamental currency that underlies the emergence of a coherent behavioral Self with some degree of cognitive activity, their slower, ancient ancestor mechanisms enable individual cells to cooperate toward coherent anatomical goals and deploy problem-solving capabilities in morphospace that belong to the “embryo”—an emergent collective.
Basal cognition without neurons: what dynamic bodies think about
The deep conceptual and mechanistic parallels between behavior and morphogenesis (Grossberg 1978) suggest a research program that can catalyze novel progress in the life sciences: accessing multiple levels of anatomical control (Figs. 5,6). To truly understand the origin of cognition, and conversely, to deploy the insights of neuroscience for regenerative medicine and bioengineering, it is important to investigate the proto-cognitive capacities of morphogenesis, specifically, the range of behaviors and capabilities that the collective intelligence of body cells can deploy toward adaptive behaviors in anatomical morphospace (Figs. 7, 8). These range from purely emergent, hardwired morphogenetic cascades (corresponding to fixed, inborn instinctual behaviors) and complex, flexible ability to reach the correct target morphology despite novel circumstances (corresponding to a degree of intelligent problem-solving behavior). It is critical to recognize and study the ways in which morphogenesis is not simply an emergent result of local rules but has complex, multi-scale feedback mechanisms that implement a degree of goal-directed activity and problem-solving in a way that was not directly encoded by genome (a hallmark of cognitive mechanisms).
Memory and target morphology
The most basic component of proto-cognitive capacities is that of memory. Morphogenetic memory is ubiquitous in, for example, regeneration. A salamander whose limbs, eyes, tail, or other organs are amputated will regrow exactly the right structure and stop all of the complex cell proliferation and remodeling activity when, and only when, a correct structure is complete (Harris 2018; Pezzulo and Levin 2016). While no individual cell knows what a finger is or how many a salamander is supposed to have, the tissue collective clearly has a setpoint for anatomical homeostasis that functionally guides morphogenetic behavior (memory) as an error minimization (i.e., objective function) process. This pattern memory can, like any good memory, be modified by experience (it is stable, but also labile to the right kinds of stimuli). For example, repeated amputation of axolotl limbs leads to the tissue habituating to the loss of limb and eventually giving up trying to re-grow (Bryant et al. 2017). Trophic memory in deer (Bubenik and Pavlansky 1965; Lobo et al. 2014) enables an ectopic branch in their otherwise stereotypical antler structure to be formed year after year, at a specific point of earlier damage, long after the damaged antler rack has been shed (a remarkable example of memory of position in 3D space, used to guide the growth of bone and nerve toward a new pattern, by cells at the scalp that have to regenerate the large structure every year).
Re-writing morphogenetic memories: ontogenetic and evolutionary timescales
The ability of morphogenetic machinery to modify its outcome due to experiences can be externally controlled (Fig. 9), analogous to the inception of false memories in neuroscience contexts (Liu et al. 2014; Ramirez et al. 2013; Vetere et al. 2019). For example, the number of heads in a regenerating planarian flatworm fragment is set by the state of a bioelectric circuit in the tissue (Beane et al. 2011). It has a built-in default of 1 (much like default, innate behaviors of brainy creatures), but is re-writable. Transient modification of the bioelectric state is remembered by the circuit, which can be experimentally re-set to read “2”: worms regenerating from such fragments indeed have two functional heads, one at each end (Durant et al. 2019; Oviedo et al. 2010). Remarkably, when cut again in multiple subsequent rounds of regeneration (with no further manipulation), the fragments continue to result in two-headed animals. This shift in the target morphology for regeneration is permanent (unless the animal’s bioelectric pattern memory is reset back to a one-headed state by an experimental manipulation). Thus, much like in nervous systems, the architecture underlying memory in this space consists of electrophysiological hardware with a highly reliable default behavior (guiding to the one-head region of morphospace) but also the capacity to be re-written toward other states, which ultimately feed into changes in gene expression and long-term changes in cell properties (Bischof et al. 2020; Pai et al. 2016; Pietak et al. 2019). The standing pattern of resting potential differences which instructively determines the number of heads that will be built is quite literally the memory of the collective intelligence of the body—a bioelectric state that guides its behavior in anatomical morphospace and can be modified by experience in physiological space. This system matches all the key criteria for memory: it is long-term stable, yet labile (rewritable), enables discrete behaviors induced by stimuli, and can even be latent until needed (see below).
While the genome encodes the hardware (the cellular affordances, such as ion channels), the actual outcome is the result of context- and experience-sensitive electrophysiological software that this hardware supports (see (Bongard and Levin 2021; Bongard and Levin 2023; Nicholson 2014) for an in-depth discussion of the machine analogy in biology). The question, of how many heads a given cellular collective will produce, is, surprisingly, not directly encoded in its genome because exactly the same, wild-type set of cells can produce 1-, 2-, or 0-headed planaria depending on their history. The same is true of a single embryonic blastoderm, which normally produces 1 “embryo”, but can produce multiple conjoined individuals if the cell:cell communication is temporarily disrupted (Lutz 1949). Of course, despite its essentially epigenetic [in the broad, original sense of the word (Ginsburg and Jablonka 2009; Jablonka 2012; Jablonka and Lamb 1995; Jablonka and Raz 2009)] nature, the bioelectrical control system works together with genetic information. Evolutionary tweaks of ion channel properties that eventually lead to genetic assimilation of bioelectrically induced phenotypes is likely an important aspect of the evolution of body plans and other morphological features, much as the Baldwin effect is thought to be a key dynamic for the role of cognition and learning in evolutionary lineages (Baldwin 1896).
Reprogrammability, external manipulation, and representation
Three especially interesting analogs with behavioral cognition have been shown in this system. First, the advantage of such a system is the same as for neuro-cognitive architectures: it allows behavior modes that are not specifically genetically encoded, broadening the range of adaptive maneuvers that can occur in novel circumstances. The disadvantages are similar too: much as the mechanisms of brain function can be exploited by parasites using chemical (da Silva and Langoni 2009) or linguistic hijacking (Kurbel and Kurbel 2019; Panchal and Jack 2022), bioelectric control mechanisms also subject morphogenetic plasticity to potential hijacking (Williams et al. 2020). For example, much as host behavior can be specifically altered when parasites manipulate neurobehavioral systems to their own advantage (da Silva and Langoni 2009; Vyas and Sapolsky 2010; Webster and McConkey 2010), the planarian morphogenetic control system too can be hijacked not only by bioengineers (as described above) but also by commensal bacteria which can over-ride the default system and lead to two-headed regeneration (Williams et al. 2020).
Second, two-headed patterns can exist in a one-headed animal (prior to injury). This means that, analogously to the brain’s capacity for counterfactual thinking, a single physical structure [normal planarian body, with normal gene expression profiles (Durant et al. 2017)] can encode multiple memories of what the correct morphogenetic goal state is. In the case of a two-headed pattern in a one-headed host, this means that the bioelectrical pattern is not a read-out of what the anatomy is currently, but rather a representation of what it should be, weeks later, following regeneration if the animal is injured. It is in effect then, a counterfactual memory that is tied not to current state but to a possible future. This primitive system is one way to begin to think about the origins of mental time travel that brains enable, with all of its obvious adaptive advantages. It should be emphasized that the traditional lens of viewing development as an emergent property of a dynamical system that simply ends up in specific states does not facilitate the kind of research direction that enabled this new biology to be discovered (and in general, the open-loop emergence paradigm makes it very hard to see how novel outcomes can be rationally controlled). In contrast, it is specifically the search for an encoded representation as a pattern memory that guides morphospace navigation that led to the methods to directly observe (Figs. 5A,9A) and edit this information structure leading to permanent, top-down changes of the target morphology.
Finally, planarian regeneration offers an example of bistability (Pezzulo et al. 2021)—a common feature of perceptual systems with top-down control. Exposure to ion channel drugs (a physiological experience akin to electrical signals from a retina or other sense organ, not a genetic change) can place the bioelectric circuit into a state in which it cannot decide between two outcomes and flips back and forth stochastically at each regenerative event (recall of target morphology). Fragments from such “cryptic” worms (Durant et al. 2017) will regenerate as one-headed or two-headed upon each cut, randomly. These fascinating parallels between key features of cognitive and morphogenetic systems shed light on the origins of behavior and offer a simplified context in which to probe the complex aspects of conventional cognition.
Creative problem-solving by adaptive morphogenesis: plasticity, not just emergent complexity
Another interesting aspect of cognition is creative problem-solving: the ability of systems to achieve goals in novel circumstances or in ways that are different from their default [this is William James’ (James 1890) definition of intelligence—the ability to achieve the same goals by different means]. This definition is suitably cybernetic in its framing (emphasizing substrate-independent function and a degree of autonomous agency, not a specific neural architecture of phylogenetic position). At the same time, it reminds the experimenter that the formalization of goals and behavior are not absolute but observer-dependent: claims of goal-directedness (Clawson and Levin 2022; Levin 2023) must include a specification of a proposed problem space, a proposed ensemble of states that are hypothesized to be the goal states, and testable claims of how much and what kind of competencies to reach the goal can be expected when that system is stressed away from its normal course of events (Fields and Levin 2022; Levin 2022).
One example of this plasticity of morphogenetic behavior (expanding on the default genetically encoded repertoire) is the fact that temporary exposure to a blocker of electrical synapses (i.e., an anesthetic) causes planarian fragments to create the heads of other species, with no genetic change required (Emmons-Bell et al. 2015; Sullivan et al. 2016). The bioelectric network incorrectly navigates to additional attractors in morphogenetic space that are normally used by species 100–150 million years of evolutionary distance—all without any genetic change needed (much like nervous systems allow an animal to dynamically remember or envision novel, distant scenarios without the need for genetic change to its brain hardware to enable each new thought). Much as with human patients exiting general anesthesia, who often hallucinate for a time while the brain network is finding its way back to the correct pre-anesthetic state, but then (usually) recover their correct personal identities, planaria that build the wrong species’ head shapes eventually remodel back to normal.
The robust regulative properties of bodies strongly emphasize the system’s ability to solve novel problems. For example, early mammalian embryos cut in half do not form two half-bodies (as any hardwired, purely emergent system would). Instead, each side recognizes the damage, makes up for it exactly, and creates one of a pair of monozygotic twins. Perhaps even more remarkable is the case of newt kidney tubules (Fankhauser 1945). By default, they consist of 8–10 cells in cross section. However, if the cells of the early embryo are artificially made to be larger, fewer cells will be used, resulting in the same (normal) tubule diameter and overall body size. Remarkably, this can be pushed to a fascinating extreme: if the cells are made to be enormous, a single cell will bend around itself, producing the normal size tubule diameter. This example illustrates not only the ability to reach the same anatomical state despite diverse and novel starting conditions with no need for periods of lengthy adaptation, but also the startling ability to call up diverse molecular mechanisms (cell:cell communication in normal conditions, but cytoskeletal bending in the case of huge cells) as needed in the service of a large-scale anatomical goal. This is an example of top-down control, in which lower level mechanisms are activated based on high-level needs—an essential feature of nervous system architectures which enables the same phenomenon in behavioral space (e.g., executive-level decisions and high-order goal states that filter down to control of actin dynamics in muscle cells as implementation machinery).
Another example of cellular plasticity beyond the default behaviors of the genetically specified hardware is that of Xenobots—the proto-organisms that spontaneously form from dissociated frog embryo skin cells (Blackiston et al. 2021; Kriegman et al. 2020, 2021a). These living forms are motile (via cilia normally used to distribute mucus along the frog’s skin) and self-directed, performing a variety of spontaneous behaviors. The most remarkable novel behavior is that of kinematic self-replication: Xenobots build copies of themselves by rearranging loose cells provided to them in the medium. This is Von Neumann-style replication that is not, to our knowledge, used by any other species on Earth. Having been deprived of the normal ability to reproduce, Xenobots arrive at a novel solution within 48 h of being created for the first time (they have no history of evolutionary selection to be a good Xenobot). These examples of real-time morphological and functional adaptation, including classic ones, such as Slijper’s Goat (Slijper 1942), which acquired, in its own brief lifetime and not millennia, the body structures needed for upright bipedal walking, reveal the prodigious capacity for morphogenesis to enact creative solutions to novel problems using the same hardware. This is another essential hallmark of cognitive systems.
Thus, coherent, effective organisms form despite not being able to count on having the right number of cells, cells of the expected size, or even the same number of chromosomes (in the case of planaria and polyploid newts). This ability to handle novelty, not only in its external environment but also in that of its component parts, is the envy of the robotics and AI communities (Aubin et al. 2022; Bongard and Levin 2021; Kriegman et al. 2021b). The on-the-fly competencies of the morphogenetic control system offers evolution the same thing that nervous systems eventually offered: the ability to not over-train on evolutionary priors and instead generate problem-solving machines. Much like with behavior, it is impossible for evolution to foresee all of the novel circumstances that organisms will be required to deal with [well beyond the handful of possible environments usually studied in phenotypic plasticity and epigenetic controls (Fraebel et al. 2020; Santos et al. 2015; West-Eberhard 2005a, b)]. The amazing inter-operability of life [e.g., chimeras, bio-tech hybrids, etc. (Clawson and Levin 2022)] is a testament to the fact that life uses a “play the hand you’re dealt” system. It produces cellular collectives which carry out, not a rote set of steps, but rather a suite of second-order functions [such as active inference (Pezzulo et al. 2015; Pezzulo et al. 2018a, b) and other computational tasks] to achieve coherent function in a wide range of changing and unpredictable scenarios, both with respect to environment and also its own parts.
Navigation: an invariant between morphological, behavioral, and other spaces
One formalism for understanding these capacities, and for learning to rationally manipulate them, is that of navigation (Fields and Levin 2022). Both behavioral and morphogenetic competencies can be modeled as various policies for navigating problem spaces. For example, during metamorphosis, the tadpole of Xenopus laevis can reach the correct frog face region of morphospace not only from its default starting position (a normal tadpole configuration), but also from a wide range of scrambled configurations (Vandenberg et al. 2012)—using new paths through that morphospace that nevertheless end up in the same goal region (Friston et al. 2015; Pezzulo and Levin 2016). Neural wiring does this as well, finding new paths to achieve functional network architecture in mutant mice (Little et al. 2009). As with certain animals that have automated, built-in behavioral repertoires, there are embryos (e.g., the nematode C. elegans) whose development seems largely hardwired. However, the vast majority of model species appear to use a combination of default modes and ability to improvise.
The full range of cognitive capacities for navigation of morphospace has only begun to be investigated—it is unclear how many of the advanced concepts from cognitive science (place cells, path planning, etc.) will become relevant. Moreover, these concepts in neuroscience are themselves in flux (Keijzer and Arnellos 2017; Keijzer 2017; Levin et al. 2021; Lyon et al. 2021; Pinotsis and Miller 2022). Given that cognitive capacities are present very widely across the biosphere, it is likely that advances in developmental bioelectricity may help to scaffold our understanding of neural systems and behavior.
A number of additional competencies (beyond the ability to achieve the target morphology despite modification to starting position or internal structures) have been found. For example, when an ectopic lens (in tail tissue) is induced by bioelectric modulation, it is sufficient to misexpress the channel only in a small subset of the needed cells: once their organ-level goal is specified, these cells will secondarily recruit other (un-modified) host cells as needed to achieve the critical mass needed to produce a normal-sized lens. While the engineer re-specifies the morphospace target region for a set of cells via a simple trigger, the necessary downstream modules (size control, substructure such as lens/retina/optic nerve placement, etc.) are automatically activated and do not need to be micromanaged (Gallistel 1980; Powers 1973). This modular control architecture, implemented by bioelectric circuits that set properties of a developmental compartment, is fundamental to the action of the brain (Bizzi et al. 1995; Callebaut et al. 2005; Levin and Yuste 2022), and in both contexts provides an interesting counterbalance with mechanisms of global integration. More specifically, this kind of ability of the collective to ascertain needs and modify the behavior of the right number of components is seen across swarm intelligences beyond morphogenesis, from that of ants recruiting conspecifics to a task (Burchill et al. 2022; Collignon et al. 2014; Wilson 1980), to neurons being recruited according to cognitive load (Bryer et al. 2013; Rossi et al. 2012).
E pluribus unum
The mechanisms that enable collectives to make high-level decisions and deploy their components as needed (deform the action space of subunits toward the goals of the collective in a new problem space) are only beginning to be understood. However, those dynamics are likely related to two key properties of the gap junctions common to morphogenetic and neurocognitive systems (Peracchia 2004; Trosko 2007). First, as with ion channels, they are themselves voltage-sensitive valves, enabling feedback loops and historicity. Second, by providing direct connections between the intracellular milieus of cells, they provide an “owner wiping property” for stress signals and other molecular traces of experience: cells cannot tell whether a given memory molecule (e.g., calcium flux) belongs to itself or its neighbors. This leads to partial erasure of individual identity for cells in a network, and enables a “collective” to scale up the measurements, stored goal states, and actions toward the emergence of larger scale agents operating in new spaces. Thus, while not necessarily an essential component of cognition in every possible living system (e.g., exo-biological contexts), bioelectric networks have been exploited extensively by life on Earth to provide the integrated computations needed to scale the homeostatic competencies of the most humble self-reproducing units into agents with flexible, highly adaptive behaviors in complex problem spaces.
Interfacing with the collective intelligence: from evolutionary perspectives to biomedicine
Bioelectricity is fundamentally a mechanism to scale computation. While bioelectric states do control cell-level properties, such as plasticity, proliferation, differentiation, etc. (Levin 2021a), the real power in this system is in determining large-scale behaviors at the tissue and organ level (Harris 2021; Levin and Martyniuk 2018). It has been shown to control size (Daane et al. 2018; Perathoner et al. 2014; Yi et al. 2021), organ identity, and whole body axes (Levin 2021a). A critical (and brain-like) aspect of bioelectrical networks is the hierarchical organization of functionality and the association of complex morphogenetic activity with simple stimuli (triggers). Much like the central nervous system (CNS) allows complicated multi-step behaviors to be triggered by a low-information content stimulus, a brief and transient bioelectrical signal can induce whole eyes (Pai et al. 2012) and appendages (Adams et al. 2007; Tseng et al. 2010) in which all the internal details are handled autonomously.
This key feature is currently beginning to be explored for regenerative medicine applications. For example, amphibian tails (including spinal cord) and limbs can be induced to regenerate by specific bioelectric states triggered by ion channel drugs or optogenetic stimulation (Adams et al. 2013; Tseng et al. 2010). Severe brain defects in the frog model, induced by chemical teratogens or mutation of a critical neurogenesis gene (Notch), have been rescued by reinforcing appropriate bioelectrical signaling (Pai et al. 2020, 2015; Pai and Levin 2022). Especially in the case of Notch mutation-induced malformations, the fact that such fundamental hardware defects can be in effect resolved “in software” (by drugs modulating Vmem to force the correct pattern) suggests the potential power of these interventions for addressing defects and traumatic injury for which we have few viable treatments today. This potential is akin to how behavioral accommodations can often make up for even severe structural defects (Slijper 1942). Similarly, tumorigenesis has been shown to be controllable by modulation of bioelectric state—normalizing cancer by reconnecting cells to the electrical network that harnesses them toward adaptive tissue homeostasis (Chernet et al. 2016, 2015; Chernet and Levin 2013b), a promising alternative to current toxic chemotherapy approaches.
Efforts to control the information processed by bioelectric networks toward particular outcomes are now being guided by computational tools at multiple levels of organization, applying Marr’s (Marr 1982; Peebles and Cooper 2015) taxonomy to control of body form just as neuroscience addresses the problems of behavior at levels from that of synaptic proteins to psychiatric therapies (Adams et al. 2016; Friston et al. 2014; Wang and Krystal 2014). However, it is likely that this work has only begun to scratch the surface of what is possible. Biomedicine today remains largely focused on the hardware—micromanaging genes and pathways toward desired functionality— and has not yet internalized the fundamental wisdom of the multi-scale approach in the neuro-behavioral sciences. The research summarized above suggests that behavior-shaping and training paradigms for cells and tissues will likely enable much greater control of morphology, gene expression, and pathway function than can be realistically achieved bottom-up (just as training animals for specific behaviors is more efficient than attempting to run their muscles directly like puppeteers). Transformative improvements in capabilities in bioengineering and regenerative medicine are within reach, if we learn to appropriate as much as possible of the multi-scale wisdom of neuroscience and generalize it beyond neurons.
Conclusion
The impacts of behavioral neuroscience, and of its advances in explaining cognitive and proto-cognitive capacities, lie well beyond classical neural cells. Understood broadly, developmental bioelectricity provides an entry-point into unifying adaptive “behavior” and problem-solving intelligence in diverse spaces in a way that makes it natural to think of plant, microbial, and even synthetic life (Baluška and Mancuso 2012; Baluška et al. 2022; Baluška and Reber 2021a, b; Bassel 2018; Calvo et al. 2020, 2017; Debono and Souza 2019; Martinez-Corral et al. 2019; Prindle et al. 2015; Reber and Baluška 2021; Schofield et al. 2020; Solé et al. 2016; Souza et al. 2017; Urrios et al. 2016; Yang et al. 2020) using the same conceptual tools from behavioral and physiological sciences. The on-going debate around representation and morphological computation is likewise being enriched by data in this field (Keijzer 1998, 2001).
The remarkable fundamental mechanism that enables a true emergent collective intelligence—a mind (at whatever scale of sophistication)—is ancient, and is also responsible for the plasticity and robustness of morphogenesis. Evolution re-used some of the same computational strategies, for binding competent signaling subunits into networks with memory and problem-solving capacity, to navigate a diverse set of spaces (physiological, anatomical, behavioral, and linguistic). Bioelectricity offers a tractable and powerful entry-point into understanding this process, because it serves as the cognitive medium of collective intelligence—whether of neurons in the brain, or of cells in a body trying to achieve anatomical outcomes. Thus, firm conceptual (and disciplinary) distinctions between the science of the brain and those of the body are largely artificial hold-overs from past limitations of technology and evolutionary understanding, and are increasingly being erased (Anderson et al. 2012; Beer 1995; Pezzulo and Levin 2015; Pfeifer et al. 2007; Willems and Francken 2012). Future work will address the mechanisms by which developmental bioelectricity sets up the structure of the CNS (Pai et al. 2015) and the ways in which neural signals control morphogenesis (Belgacem and Borodinsky 2015; Borodinsky et al. 2012; Herrera-Rincon and Levin 2018; Herrera-Rincon et al. 2017); beyond the molecular mechanisms linking these two systems, there is also much opportunity for unification into a single underlying conceptual architecture.
Evolution exploits the generic computational properties of such networks (learning, generalization, counterfactual memories, representation, distributed control, etc.) at many scales, building flexible problem-solving engines instead of fixed solutions to specific environments (Moczek et al. 2011; Oyama 2000; Sultan et al. 2022). While bioelectricity is an especially convenient modality, proto-cognitive capacities (such as learning) are found also in biochemical networks and biomechanical networks (Biswas et al. 2021; Stern et al. 2020a, b; Watson et al. 2010). Emerging frameworks that focus on a ubiquitous multi-scale competency architecture, where each layer has some degree of behavioral and proto-cognitive capabilities that await discovery, are already generating new capabilities and driving novel research programs (Aubin et al. 2022; Bongard et al. 2006; Davies and Levin 2023; Kriegman et al. 2021b; Levin et al. 2017; Merrild and Rasmussen 2018; Pezzulo 2020; Pezzulo et al. 2021; Pezzulo and Levin 2015, 2016, 2018; Pfeifer et al. 2005; Taylor et al. 2016). The implications of this approach (Fig. 10) range far beyond behavioral and developmental sciences, to encompass synthetic bioengineering of novel cognitive life forms (Clawson and Levin 2022; Ebrahimkhani and Levin 2021; Kamm and Bashir 2014) and related fields, such as robotics and AI. All of these disciplines are beginning to intersect in the exciting emerging field of diverse intelligence research, which will not only provide numerous useful applications in engineering and biomedicine, and help to understand our evolutionary history, but most importantly, will shed light on the deepest philosophical problems of the origin and nature of possible cognitive Selves.
Acknowledgements
I thank Chris Fields, Kevin Mitchell, Karl Friston, Mark Solms, and many of the members of the Levin lab and the bioelectricity and basal cognition communities for many useful discussions. I also thank Ken Chang, Pamela Lyon, and 3 anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript. I gratefully acknowledge funding via the John Templeton Foundation grant 62212 and Astonishing Labs.
Author contributions
Not applicable.
Funding
I gratefully acknowledge funding via the John Templeton Foundation Grant 62212 and Astonishing Labs.
Availability of data and materials
Not applicable.
Declarations
Conflict of interest
M.L. declares that he is a co-founder of a company, Morphoceuticals, which works in the space of bioelectric signals for regenerative medicine.
Ethical approval
Not applicable.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Abramson CI, Levin M. Behaviorist approaches to investigating memory and learning: a primer for synthetic biology and bioengineering. Commun Integr Biol. 2021;14(1):230–247. doi: 10.1080/19420889.2021.2005863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adams DS, Robinson KR, Fukumoto T, Yuan S, Albertson RC, Yelick P, Kuo L, McSweeney M, Levin M. Early, H+-V-ATPase-dependent proton flux is necessary for consistent left-right patterning of non-mammalian vertebrates. Development. 2006;133(9):1657–1671. doi: 10.1242/dev.02341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adams DS, Masi A, Levin M. H+ pump-dependent changes in membrane voltage are an early mechanism necessary and sufficient to induce Xenopus tail regeneration. Development. 2007;134(7):1323–1335. doi: 10.1242/dev.02812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adams DS, Tseng AS, Levin M. Light-activation of the Archaerhodopsin H(+)-pump reverses age-dependent loss of vertebrate regeneration: sparking system-level controls in vivo. Biology Open. 2013;2(3):306–313. doi: 10.1242/bio.20133665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adams RA, Huys QJ, Roiser JP. Computational Psychiatry: towards a mathematically informed understanding of mental illness. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2016;87(1):53–63. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2015-310737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloway TM (1972) Retention of Learning through Metamorphosis in Grain Beetle (Tenebrio-Molitor). Am Zool, 12(3):471–477. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3881781
- Anderson ML, Richardson MJ, Chemero A. Eroding the boundaries of cognition: implications of embodiment(1) Top Cogn Sci. 2012;4(4):717–730. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01211.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arendt D, Tosches MA, Marlow H. From nerve net to nerve ring, nerve cord and brain–evolution of the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17(1):61–72. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2015.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aubin CA, Gorissen B, Milana E, Buskohl PR, Lazarus N, Slipher GA, Keplinger C, Bongard J, Iida F, Lewis JA, Shepherd RF. Towards enduring autonomous robots via embodied energy. Nature. 2022;602(7897):393–402. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-04138-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin JM. A new factor in evolution. Am Nat. 1896;30:441–445. doi: 10.1086/276408. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baluška F, Mancuso S (2012) Ion channels in plants: from bioelectricity, via signaling, to behavioral actions. Plant Signal Behav 8(1):e23009. 10.4161/psb.23009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Baluška F, Reber AS (2021a) CBC-Clock Theory of Life—integration of cellular circadian clocks and cellular sentience is essential for cognitive basis of life. BioEssays 43(10):e2100121. 10.1002/bies.2021a00121 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Baluška F, Reber AS (2021b) Cellular and organismal agency—not based on genes: a comment on baverstock. Prog Biophys Mol Biol 167:161–162. 10.1016/j.pbiomolbio.2021.11.001 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Baluška F, Miller WB, Reber AS (2022) Cellular and evolutionary perspectives on organismal cognition: from unicellular to multicellular organisms. Biol J Lin Soc. 10.1093/biolinnean/blac005
- Baluška F, Levin M. On having no head: cognition throughout biological systems. Front Psychol. 2016;7:902. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassel GW. Information processing and distributed computation in plant organs. Trends Plant Sci. 2018;23(11):994–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.tplants.2018.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beane WS, Morokuma J, Adams DS, Levin M. A chemical genetics approach reveals H, K-ATPase-mediated membrane voltage is required for planarian head regeneration. Chem Biol. 2011;18(1):77–89. doi: 10.1016/j.chembiol.2010.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beekman M, Latty T. Brainless but multi-headed: decision making by the acellular slime mould Physarum polycephalum. J Mol Biol. 2015;427(23):3734–3743. doi: 10.1016/j.jmb.2015.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beer RD. A dynamical-systems perspective on agent environment interaction. Artif Intell. 1995;72(1–2):173–215. doi: 10.1016/0004-3702(94)00005-L. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Belgacem YH, Borodinsky LN. Inversion of Sonic hedgehog action on its canonical pathway by electrical activity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112(13):4140–4145. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1419690112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bischof J, Day ME, Miller KA, LaPalme JV, Levin M. Nervous system and tissue polarity dynamically adapt to new morphologies in planaria. Dev Biol. 2020;467(1–2):51–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2020.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswas S, Manicka S, Hoel E, Levin M. Gene regulatory networks exhibit several kinds of memory: quantification of memory in biological and random transcriptional networks. iScience. 2021;24(3):102131. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.102131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bizzi E, Giszter SF, Loeb E, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Saltiel P. Modular organization of motor behavior in the frog's spinal cord. Trends Neurosci. 1995;18(10):442–446. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(95)94494-p. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackiston DJ, Levin M. Ectopic eyes outside the head in Xenopus tadpoles provide sensory data for light-mediated learning. J Exp Biol. 2013;216(6):1031–1040. doi: 10.1242/jeb.074963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackiston DJ, Silva Casey E, Weiss MR. Retention of memory through metamorphosis: can a moth remember what it learned as a caterpillar? PLoS ONE. 2008;3(3):e1736. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackiston D, Shomrat T, Nicolas CL, Granata C, Levin M. A second-generation device for automated training and quantitative behavior analyses of molecularly-tractable model organisms. PLoS ONE. 2010;5(12):e14370. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackiston D, Adams DS, Lemire JM, Lobikin M, Levin M. Transmembrane potential of GlyCl-expressing instructor cells induces a neoplastic-like conversion of melanocytes via a serotonergic pathway. Dis Model Mech. 2011;4(1):67–85. doi: 10.1242/dmm.005561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blackiston D, Lederer E, Kriegman S, Garnier S, Bongard J, Levin M. A cellular platform for the development of synthetic living machines. Sci Robot. 2021;6(52):eabf1571. doi: 10.1126/scirobotics.abf1571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boisseau RP, Vogel D, Dussutour A (2016) Habituation in non-neural organisms: evidence from slime moulds. Proc Biol Sci. 10.1098/rspb.2016.0446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Bongard J, Levin M. Living things are not (20th century) machines: updating mechanism metaphors in light of the modern science of machine behavior. Front Ecol Evolut. 2021 doi: 10.3389/fevo.2021.650726. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bongard J, Levin M. There's plenty of room right here: biological systems as evolved, overloaded. Multi-Scale Mach Biomimetics (basel) 2023;8(1):110. doi: 10.3390/biomimetics8010110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bongard J, Zykov V, Lipson H. Resilient machines through continuous self-modeling. Science. 2006;314(5802):1118–1121. doi: 10.1126/science.1133687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boone W, Piccinini G. The cognitive neuroscience revolution. Synthese. 2016;193(5):1509–1534. doi: 10.1007/s11229-015-0783-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Borodinsky LN, Belgacem YH, Swapna I. Electrical activity as a developmental regulator in the formation of spinal cord circuits. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2012;22(4):624–630. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boussard A, Delescluse J, Pérez-Escudero A, Dussutour A. Memory inception and preservation in slime moulds: the quest for a common mechanism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2019;374(1774):20180368. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2018.0368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brink P. Gap junction voltage dependence. A clear picture emerges. J Gen Physiol. 2000;116(1):11–12. doi: 10.1085/jgp.116.1.11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunet T, Arendt D. From damage response to action potentials: early evolution of neural and contractile modules in stem eukaryotes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1685):20150043. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryant DM, Sousounis K, Farkas JE, Bryant S, Thao N, Guzikowski AR, Monaghan JR, Levin M, Whited JL. Repeated removal of developing limb buds permanently reduces appendage size in the highly-regenerative axolotl. Dev Biol. 2017;424(1):1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2017.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bryer EJ, Medaglia JD, Rostami S, Hillary FG. Neural recruitment after mild traumatic brain injury is task dependent: a meta-analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19(7):751–762. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713000490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bubenik AB, Pavlansky R. Trophic responses to trauma in growing antlers. J Exp Zool. 1965;159(3):289–302. doi: 10.1002/jez.1401590302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burchill AT, Pavlic TP, Pratt SC, Reid CR. Weaver ants regulate the rate of prey delivery during collective vertical transport. bioRxiv. 2022 doi: 10.1101/2022.06.22.497253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callebaut W, Rasskin-Gutman D, Simon HA. Modularity : understanding the development and evolution of natural complex systems. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Calvo P, Sahi VP, Trewavas A. Are plants sentient? Plant Cell Environ. 2017;40(11):2858–2869. doi: 10.1111/pce.13065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calvo P, Gagliano M, Souza GM, Trewavas A. Plants are intelligent, here's how. Ann Bot. 2020;125(1):11–28. doi: 10.1093/aob/mcz155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chernet BT, Levin M (2013a) Endogenous voltage potentials and the microenvironment: Bioelectric signals that reveal, induce and normalize cancer. J Clin Exp Oncol 2013(S1). 10.4172/2324-9110.S1-002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Chernet BT, Levin M (2013b) Transmembrane voltage potential is an essential cellular parameter for the detection and control of tumor development in a Xenopus model. Dis Model Mech 6(3):595–607. 10.1242/dmm.010835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Chernet BT, Fields C, Levin M. Long-range gap junctional signaling controls oncogene-mediated tumorigenesis in Xenopus laevis embryos. Front Physiol. 2015;5:519. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2014.00519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chernet BT, Adams DS, Lobikin M, Levin M. Use of genetically encoded, light-gated ion translocators to control tumorigenesis. Oncotarget. 2016;7(15):19575–19588. doi: 10.18632/oncotarget.8036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clawson WP, Levin M. Endless forms most beautiful 2.0: teleonomy and the bioengineering of chimaeric and synthetic organisms. Biol J Linn Soc Lond. 2022 doi: 10.1093/biolinnean/blac073. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Collignon B, Cervantes Valdivieso LE, Detrain C. Group recruitment in ants: who is willing to lead? Behav Processes. 2014;108:98–104. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- da Silva RC, Langoni H. Toxoplasma gondii: host-parasite interaction and behavior manipulation. Parasitol Res. 2009;105(4):893–898. doi: 10.1007/s00436-009-1526-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daane JM, Lanni J, Rothenberg I, Seebohm G, Higdon CW, Johnson SL, Harris MP. Bioelectric-calcineurin signaling module regulates allometric growth and size of the zebrafish fin. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):10391. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28450-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies J, Levin M. Synthetic morphology with agential materials. Nat Rev Bioeng. 2023;1(1):46–59. doi: 10.1038/s44222-022-00001-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Debono MW, Souza GM. Plants as electromic plastic interfaces: a mesological approach. Prog Biophys Mol Biol. 2019;146:123–133. doi: 10.1016/j.pbiomolbio.2019.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durant F, Morokuma J, Fields C, Williams K, Adams DS, Levin M. Long-term, stochastic editing of regenerative anatomy via targeting endogenous bioelectric gradients. Biophys J. 2017;112(10):2231–2243. doi: 10.1016/j.bpj.2017.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durant F, Bischof J, Fields C, Morokuma J, LaPalme J, Hoi A, Levin M. The role of early bioelectric signals in the regeneration of planarian anterior/posterior polarity. Biophys J. 2019;116(5):948–961. doi: 10.1016/j.bpj.2019.01.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebrahimkhani MR, Levin M. Synthetic living machines: a new window on life. iScience. 2021;24(5):102505. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.102505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emmons-Bell M, Durant F, Hammelman J, Bessonov N, Volpert V, Morokuma J, Pinet K, Adams DS, Pietak A, Lobo D, Levin M. Gap junctional blockade stochastically induces different species-specific head anatomies in genetically wild-type Girardia dorotocephala flatworms. Int J Mol Sci. 2015;16(11):27865–27896. doi: 10.3390/ijms161126065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fankhauser G. Maintenance of normal structure in heteroploid salamander larvae, through compensation of changes in cell size by adjustment of cell number and cell shape. J Exp Zool. 1945;100(3):445–455. doi: 10.1002/jez.1401000310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields C, Glazebrook JF. Levin M (2021) Minimal physicalism as a scale-free substrate for cognition and consciousness. Neurosci Conscious. 2021;2:niab013. doi: 10.1093/nc/niab013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields C, Levin M. Competency in navigating arbitrary spaces as an invariant for analyzing cognition in diverse embodiments. Entropy. 2022;24(6):819. doi: 10.3390/e24060819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fields C, Bischof J, Levin M. Morphological coordination: a common ancestral function unifying neural and non-neural signaling. Physiology. 2020;35(1):16–30. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00027.2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraebel DT, Gowda K, Mani M, Kuehn S. Evolution of generalists by phenotypic plasticity. iScience. 2020;23(11):101678. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2020.101678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston KJ, Stephan KE, Montague R, Dolan RJ. Computational psychiatry: the brain as a phantastic organ. Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1(2):148–158. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70275-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friston K, Levin M, Sengupta B, Pezzulo G. Knowing one's place: a free-energy approach to pattern regulation. J R Soc Interface. 2015 doi: 10.1098/rsif.2014.1383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallistel CR (1980) The organization of action : a new synthesis. L. Erlbaum Associates; distributed by Halsted Press, Hillsdale, N.J. New York
- Galván A. Neural plasticity of development and learning. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(6):879–890. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ginsburg S, Jablonka E. Epigenetic learning in non-neural organisms. J Biosci. 2009;34(4):633–646. doi: 10.1007/s12038-009-0081-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ginsburg S, Jablonka E. Evolutionary transitions in learning and cognition. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021;376(1821):20190766. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gokhale CS, Giaimo S, Remigi P. Memory shapes microbial populations. PLoS Comput Biol. 2021;17(10):e1009431. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S (1978) Communication, memory, and development. In: Rosen R & Snell F (eds) Progress in theoretical biology, pp 183–232. 10.1016/B978-0-12-543105-7.50012-9
- Harris AK. The need for a concept of shape homeostasis. Biosystems. 2018;173:65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2018.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris MP. Bioelectric signaling as a unique regulator of development and regeneration. Development. 2021 doi: 10.1242/dev.180794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrera-Rincon C, Levin M. Booting up the organism during development: pre-behavioral functions of the vertebrate brain in guiding body morphogenesis. Commun Integr Biol. 2018;11(1):e1433440. doi: 10.1080/19420889.2018.1433440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herrera-Rincon C, Pai VP, Moran KM, Lemire JM, Levin M. The brain is required for normal muscle and nerve patterning during early Xenopus development. Nat Commun. 2017;8(1):587. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-00597-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huth AG, Lee T, Nishimoto S, Bilenko NY, Vu AT, Gallant JL. Decoding the semantic content of natural movies from human brain activity. Front Syst Neurosci. 2016;10:81. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jablonka E. Epigenetic variations in heredity and evolution. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2012;92(6):683–688. doi: 10.1038/clpt.2012.158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jablonka E, Lamb MJ. Epigenetic inheritance and evolution: the Lamarckian dimension. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Jablonka E, Raz G. Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance: prevalence, mechanisms, and implications for the study of heredity and evolution. Q Rev Biol. 2009;84(2):131–176. doi: 10.1086/598822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James W. The principles of psychology. New York: H. Holt and company; 1890. [Google Scholar]
- Jekely G. Evolution: how not to become an animal. Curr Biol. 2019;29(23):R1240–R1242. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jekely G, Keijzer F, Godfrey-Smith P. An option space for early neural evolution. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2015 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamm RD, Bashir R. Creating living cellular machines. Ann Biomed Eng. 2014;42(2):445–459. doi: 10.1007/s10439-013-0902-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kandel ER, O'Dell TJ. Are adult learning mechanisms also used for development? Science. 1992;258(5080):243–245. doi: 10.1126/science.1411522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keijzer FA. Doing without representations which specify what to do. Philos Psychol. 1998;11:269–302. doi: 10.1080/09515089808573262. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Keijzer FA. Representation and behavior. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Keijzer FA. Evolutionary convergence and biologically embodied cognition. Interface Focus. 2017;7(3):20160123. doi: 10.1098/rsfs.2016.0123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keijzer F, Arnellos A. The animal sensorimotor organization: a challenge for the environmental complexity thesis. Biol Philos. 2017;32(3):421–441. doi: 10.1007/s10539-017-9565-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keijzer F, van Duijn M, Lyon P. What nervous systems do: early evolution, input-output, and the skin brain thesis. Adapt Behav. 2013;21(2):67–85. doi: 10.1177/1059712312465330. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kriegman S, Blackiston D, Levin M, Bongard J. A scalable pipeline for designing reconfigurable organisms. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2020;117(4):1853–1859. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1910837117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kriegman S, Blackiston D, Levin M, Bongard J. Kinematic self-replication in reconfigurable organisms. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2021 doi: 10.1073/pnas.2112672118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kriegman S, Nasab AM, Blackiston D, Steele H, Levin M, Kramer-Bottiglio R, Bongard J (2021b) Scale invariant robot behavior with fractals. arXiv. 10.48550/arXiv.2103.04876
- Kuchling F, Friston K, Georgiev G, Levin M. Integrating variational approaches to pattern formation into a deeper physics: Reply to comments on "Morphogenesis as Bayesian inference: a variational approach to pattern formation and manipulation in complex biological systems". Phys Life Rev. 2020;33:125–128. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2020.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurbel S, Kurbel B. Memes: food for attitudes and behavior crucial for our survival. BioEssays. 2019;41(9):e1900075. doi: 10.1002/bies.201900075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M. Bioelectric mechanisms in regeneration: Unique aspects and future perspectives. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2009;20(5):543–556. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2009.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M. The computational boundary of a "self": developmental bioelectricity drives multicellularity and scale-free cognition. Front Psychol. 2019;10(2688):2688. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M. Bioelectric signaling: reprogrammable circuits underlying embryogenesis, regeneration, and cancer. Cell. 2021;184(8):1971–1989. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2021.02.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M. Life, death, and self: fundamental questions of primitive cognition viewed through the lens of body plasticity and synthetic organisms. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2021;564:114–133. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2020.10.077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M. Technological approach to mind everywhere: an experimentally-grounded framework for understanding diverse bodies and minds. Front Syst Neurosci. 2022;16:768201. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2022.768201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M, Martyniuk CJ. The bioelectric code: an ancient computational medium for dynamic control of growth and form. Biosystems. 2018;164:76–93. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2017.08.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M, Buznikov GA, Lauder JM. Of minds and embryos: left-right asymmetry and the serotonergic controls of pre-neural morphogenesis. Dev Neurosci. 2006;28(3):171–185. doi: 10.1159/000091915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M, Pezzulo G, Finkelstein JM. Endogenous bioelectric signaling networks: exploiting voltage gradients for control of growth and form. Annu Rev Biomed Eng. 2017;19:353–387. doi: 10.1146/annurev-bioeng-071114-040647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M, Pietak AM, Bischof J (2019) Planarian regeneration as a model of anatomical homeostasis: Recent progress in biophysical and computational approaches. Semin Cell Dev Biol 87:125–144. 10.1016/j.semcdb.2018.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Levin M, Keijzer F, Lyon P, Arendt D. Uncovering cognitive similarities and differences, conservation and innovation. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021;376(1821):20200458. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin M, Yuste R (2022) Modular cognition. Magazine article in Aeon March 8, 2022. https://aeon.co/essays/how-evolution-hacked-its-way-to-intelligence-from-the-bottom-up
- Levin M (2023) Collective intelligence of morphogenesis as a teleonomic process. In: Corning PA (ed) Teleology. MIT Press, Cambridge. https://psyarxiv.com/hqc9b/
- Little GE, López-Bendito G, Rünker AE, García N, Piñon MC, Chédotal A, Molnár Z, Mitchell KJ. Specificity and plasticity of thalamocortical connections in Sema6A mutant mice. PLoS Biol. 2009;7(4):e98. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu X, Ramirez S, Tonegawa S. Inception of a false memory by optogenetic manipulation of a hippocampal memory engram. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;369(1633):20130142. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobo D, Solano M, Bubenik GA, Levin M. A linear-encoding model explains the variability of the target morphology in regeneration. J R Soc Interface. 2014;11(92):20130918. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2013.0918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lutz H. Sur la production expérimentale de la polyembryonie et de la monstruosité double chez les oiseaux. Archs Anat Microsc Morph Exp. 1949;38:79–144. [Google Scholar]
- Lyon P. The biogenic approach to cognition. Cogn Process. 2006;7(1):11–29. doi: 10.1007/s10339-005-0016-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyon P. The cognitive cell: bacterial behavior reconsidered. Front Microbiol. 2015;6:264. doi: 10.3389/fmicb.2015.00264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyon P. Of what is “minimal cognition” the half-baked version? Adapt Behav. 2019;28(6):407–424. doi: 10.1177/1059712319871360. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lyon P, Keijzer F, Arendt D, Levin M. Reframing cognition: getting down to biological basics. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021;376(1820):20190750. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mar RA, Kelley WM, Heatherton TF, Macrae CN. Detecting agency from the biological motion of veridical vs animated agents. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2(3):199–205. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marr D. Vision: a computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. San Francisco: Freeman; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Corral R, Liu J, Prindle A, Süel GM, Garcia-Ojalvo J. Metabolic basis of brain-like electrical signalling in bacterial communities. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2019;374(1774):20180382. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2018.0382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathews J, Levin M. Gap junctional signaling in pattern regulation: physiological network connectivity instructs growth and form. Dev Neurobiol. 2017;77(5):643–673. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McConnell JV, Jacobson AL, Kimble DP. The effects of regeneration upon retention of a conditioned response in the planarian. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1959;52(1):1–5. doi: 10.1037/h0048028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merrild J, Rasmussen MA, Risi S. HyperNTM: evolving scalable neural turing machines through HyperNEAT. Appl Evolut Comput Evoapplicat. 2018;10784:750–766. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-77538-8_50. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moczek AP, Sultan S, Foster S, Ledón-Rettig C, Dworkin I, Nijhout HF, Abouheif E, Pfennig DW (2011) The role of developmental plasticity in evolutionary innovation. Proc Biol Sci 278(1719):2705–2713. 10.1098/rspb.2011.0971 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Murugan NJ, Kaltman DH, Jin PH, Chien M, Martinez R, Nguyen CQ, Kane A, Novak R, Ingber DE, Levin M. Mechanosensation mediates long-range spatial decision-making in an aneural organism. Adv Mater. 2021;33(34):e2008161. doi: 10.1002/adma.202008161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nakagaki T, Guy RD. Intelligent behaviors of amoeboid movement based on complex dynamics of soft matter. Soft Matter. 2007;4(1):57–67. doi: 10.1039/b706317m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naselaris T, Prenger RJ, Kay KN, Oliver M, Gallant JL. Bayesian reconstruction of natural images from human brain activity. Neuron. 2009;63(6):902–915. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicholson DJ. The machine conception of the organism in development and evolution: a critical analysis. Stud Hist Philos Biol Biomed Sci. 2014;48(Pt B):162–174. doi: 10.1016/j.shpsc.2014.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nishimoto S, Vu AT, Naselaris T, Benjamini Y, Yu B, Gallant JL. Reconstructing visual experiences from brain activity evoked by natural movies. Curr Biol. 2011;21(19):1641–1646. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oviedo NJ, Morokuma J, Walentek P, Kema IP, Gu MB, Ahn JM, Hwang JS, Gojobori T, Levin M. Long-range neural and gap junction protein-mediated cues control polarity during planarian regeneration. Dev Biol. 2010;339(1):188–199. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2009.12.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oyama S. The ontogeny of information: developmental systems and evolution. Durham: Duke University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Pagán OR (2014) The first brain: the neuroscience of planarians. Oxford University Press, Oxford, New York
- Pai VP, Levin M. HCN2 channel-induced rescue of brain, eye, heart and gut teratogenesis caused by nicotine, ethanol and aberrant notch signalling. Wound Repair Regen. 2022;30(6):681–706. doi: 10.1111/wrr.13032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pai VP, Aw S, Shomrat T, Lemire JM, Levin M. Transmembrane voltage potential controls embryonic eye patterning in Xenopus laevis. Development. 2012;139(2):313–323. doi: 10.1242/dev.073759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pai VP, Lemire JM, Pare JF, Lin G, Chen Y, Levin M. Endogenous gradients of resting potential instructively pattern embryonic neural tissue via notch signaling and regulation of proliferation. J Neurosci. 2015;35(10):4366–4385. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1877-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pai VP, Martyniuk CJ, Echeverri K, Sundelacruz S, Kaplan DL, Levin M. Genome-wide analysis reveals conserved transcriptional responses downstream of resting potential change in Xenopus embryos, axolotl regeneration, and human mesenchymal cell differentiation. Regeneration (oxf) 2016;3(1):3–25. doi: 10.1002/reg2.48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pai VP, Cervera J, Mafe S, Willocq V, Lederer EK, Levin M. HCN2 channel-induced rescue of brain teratogenesis via local and long-range bioelectric repair. Front Cell Neurosci. 2020;14(136):136. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2020.00136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palacios-Prado N, Bukauskas FF. Heterotypic gap junction channels as voltage-sensitive valves for intercellular signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106(35):14855–14860. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901923106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panchal R, Jack A. The contagiousness of memes: containing the spread of COVID-19 conspiracy theories in a forensic psychiatric hospital. Bjpsych Bull. 2022;46(1):36–42. doi: 10.1192/bjb.2020.120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peebles D, Cooper RP. Thirty years after Marr's vision: levels of analysis in cognitive science. Top Cogn Sci. 2015;7(2):187–190. doi: 10.1111/tops.12137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peracchia C. Chemical gating of gap junction channels; roles of calcium, pH and calmodulin. Biochem Biophys Acta. 2004;1662(1–2):61–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bbamem.2003.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perathoner S, Daane JM, Henrion U, Seebohm G, Higdon CW, Johnson SL, Nusslein-Volhard C, Harris MP. Bioelectric signaling regulates size in zebrafish fins. PLoS Genet. 2014;10(1):e1004080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1004080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G. Disorders of morphogenesis as disorders of inference: Comment on "Morphogenesis as Bayesian inference: a variational approach to pattern formation and control in complex biological systems" by Michael Levin et al. Phys Life Rev. 2020;33:112–114. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2020.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G, Levin M. Re-membering the body: applications of computational neuroscience to the top-down control of regeneration of limbs and other complex organs. Integr Biol (camb) 2015;7(12):1487–1517. doi: 10.1039/c5ib00221d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G, Levin M. Top-down models in biology: explanation and control of complex living systems above the molecular level. J R Soc Interface. 2016 doi: 10.1098/rsif.2016.0555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G, Levin M. Embodying Markov blankets: comment on "Answering Schrodinger's question: a free-energy formulation" by Maxwell James Desormeau Ramstead et al. Phys Life Rev. 2018;24:32–36. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2017.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G, Rigoli F, Friston K. Active Inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Prog Neurobiol. 2015;134:17–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G, Rigoli F, Friston KJ. Hierarchical active inference: a theory of motivated control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2018;22(4):294–306. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pezzulo G, LaPalme J, Durant F, Levin M. Bistability of somatic pattern memories: stochastic outcomes in bioelectric circuits underlying regeneration. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2021;376(1821):20190765. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfeifer R, Iida F, Bongard J. New robotics: design principles for intelligent systems. Artif Life. 2005;11(1–2):99–120. doi: 10.1162/1064546053279017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfeifer R, Bongard J, Grand S. How the body shapes the way we think: a new view of intelligence. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Pietak A, Bischof J, LaPalme J, Morokuma J, Levin M. Neural control of body-plan axis in regenerating planaria. PLoS Comput Biol. 2019;15(4):e1006904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinotsis DA, Miller EK. Beyond dimension reduction: stable electric fields emerge from and allow representational drift. Neuroimage. 2022;253:119058. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powers WT. Behavior: the control of perception. Chicago: Aldine Pub. Co.; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Prindle A, Liu J, Asally M, Ly S, Garcia-Ojalvo J, Suel GM. Ion channels enable electrical communication in bacterial communities. Nature. 2015;527(7576):59–63. doi: 10.1038/nature15709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez S, Liu X, Lin PA, Suh J, Pignatelli M, Redondo RL, Ryan TJ, Tonegawa S. Creating a false memory in the hippocampus. Science. 2013;341(6144):387–391. doi: 10.1126/science.1239073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramstead MJD, Sakthivadivel DAR, Heins C, Koudahl M, Millidge B, Da Costa L, Klein B, Friston KJ (2022) On bayesian mechanics: a physics of and by beliefs. arXiv. 10.48550/arXiv.2205.11543 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Reber AS, Baluska F. Cognition in some surprising places. Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2021;564:150–157. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2020.08.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid CR, MacDonald H, Mann RP, Marshall JA, Latty T, Garnier S. Decision-making without a brain: how an amoeboid organism solves the two-armed bandit. J R Soc Interface. 2016 doi: 10.1098/rsif.2016.0030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Repp BH, Knoblich G. Toward a psychophysics of agency: detecting gain and loss of control over auditory action effects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2007;33(2):469–482. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Romero-Reyes J, Molina-Hernández A, Díaz NF, Camacho-Arroyo I. Role of serotonin in vertebrate embryo development. Reprod Biol. 2021;21(1):100475. doi: 10.1016/j.repbio.2020.100475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi L, Iacopetti P, Salvetti A. Stem cells and neural signalling: the case of neoblast recruitment and plasticity in low dose X-ray treated planarians. Int J Dev Biol. 2012;56(1–3):135–142. doi: 10.1387/ijdb.123505lr. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saigusa T, Tero A, Nakagaki T, Kuramoto Y (2008) Amoebae anticipate periodic events. Phys Rev Lett 100(1):018101. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18232821 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Saló E, Abril JF, Adell T, Cebrià F, Eckelt K, Fernandez-Taboada E, Handberg-Thorsager M, Iglesias M, Molina MD, Rodríguez-Esteban G. Planarian regeneration: achievements and future directions after 20 years of research. Int J Dev Biol. 2009;53(8–10):1317–1327. doi: 10.1387/ijdb.072414es. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santos M, Szathmáry E, Fontanari JF. Phenotypic plasticity, the Baldwin effect, and the speeding up of evolution: the computational roots of an illusion. J Theor Biol. 2015;371:127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2015.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sarnat HB, Netsky MG. When does a ganglion become a brain? Evolutionary origin of the central nervous system. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2002;9(4):240–253. doi: 10.1053/spen.2002.32502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schofield Z, Meloni GN, Tran P, Zerfass C, Sena G, Hayashi Y, Grant M, Contera SA, Minteer SD, Kim M, Prindle A, Rocha P, Djamgoz MBA, Pilizota T, Unwin PR, Asally M, Soyer OS. Bioelectrical understanding and engineering of cell biology. J R Soc Interface. 2020;17(166):20200013. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2020.0013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheiman IM, Tiras KL (1996) Memory and morphogenesis in planaria and beetle. In Abramson CI, Shuranova ZP, & Burmistrov YM (eds) Russian contributions to invertebrate behavior Praeger, Westport, pp 43–76
- Sheiman IM, Kreshchenko ND. Regeneration of planarians: experimental object. Ontogenez. 2015;46(1):3–12. doi: 10.1134/S1062360415010075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shomrat T, Levin M. An automated training paradigm reveals long-term memory in planarians and its persistence through head regeneration. J Exp Biol. 2013;216(Pt 20):3799–3810. doi: 10.1242/jeb.087809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slijper EJ. Biologic anatomical investigations on the bipedal gait and upright posture in mammals—with special reference to a little goat born without forelegs II. Proc K Ned Akad Wet. 1942;45(1/5):407–415. [Google Scholar]
- Smith-Ferguson J, Beekman M. Who needs a brain? Slime moulds, behavioural ecology and minimal cognition. Adapt Behav. 2020;28(6):465–478. doi: 10.1177/10597123198265. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sole R, Amor DR, Duran-Nebreda S, Conde-Pueyo N, Carbonell-Ballestero M, Montanez R. Synthetic collective intelligence. Biosystems. 2016;148:47–61. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2016.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Souza GM, Ferreira AS, Saraiva GF, Toledo GR. Plant "electrome" can be pushed toward a self-organized critical state by external cues: evidences from a study with soybean seedlings subject to different environmental conditions. Plant Signal Behav. 2017;12(3):e1290040. doi: 10.1080/15592324.2017.1290040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern M, Arinze C, Perez L, Palmer SE, Murugan A. Supervised learning through physical changes in a mechanical system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2020;117(26):14843–14850. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2000807117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern M, Pinson MB, Murugan A. Continual learning of multiple memories in mechanical networks. Phys Rev X. 2020;10(3):031044. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevX.10.031044. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan KG, Emmons-Bell M, Levin M. Physiological inputs regulate species-specific anatomy during embryogenesis and regeneration. Commun Integr Biol. 2016;9(4):e1192733. doi: 10.1080/19420889.2016.1192733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sultan SE, Moczek AP, Walsh D. Bridging the explanatory gaps: What can we learn from a biological agency perspective? BioEssays. 2022;44(1):e2100185. doi: 10.1002/bies.202100185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor T, Auerbach JE, Bongard J, Clune J, Hickinbotham S, Ofria C, Oka M, Risi S, Stanley KO, Yosinski J. WebAL comes of age: a review of the first 21 years of artificial life on the web. Artif Life. 2016;22(3):364–407. doi: 10.1162/ARTL_a_00211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Timsit Y, Grégoire SP. Towards the idea of molecular brains. Int J Mol Sci. 2021;22(21):11868. doi: 10.3390/ijms222111868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trosko JE. Gap junctional intercellular communication as a biological "Rosetta stone" in understanding, in a systems biological manner, stem cell behavior, mechanisms of epigenetic toxicology, chemoprevention and chemotherapy. J Membr Biol. 2007;218(1–3):93–100. doi: 10.1007/s00232-007-9072-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng AS, Beane WS, Lemire JM, Masi A, Levin M. Induction of vertebrate regeneration by a transient sodium current. J Neurosci. 2010;30(39):13192–13200. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3315-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turing AM (1952) The Chemical Basis of Morphogenesis. Philos T Roy Soc B 237(641):37–72. http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org.ezp-prod1.hul.harvard.edu/content/237/641/37.full, http://rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/237/641/37.full.pdf (<Go to ISI>://A1952YF90500001)
- Urrios A, Macia J, Manzoni R, Conde N, Bonforti A, de Nadal E, Posas F, Solé R (2016) A synthetic multicellular memory device. Acs Synth Biol 5(8):862–873. 10.1021/acssynbio.5b00252 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Vallverdú J, Castro O, Mayne R, Talanov M, Levin M, Baluška F, Gunji Y, Dussutour A, Zenil H, Adamatzky A (2018) Slime mould: the fundamental mechanisms of biological cognition. Biosystems 165:57–70. 10.1016/j.biosystems.2017.12.011 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Vandenberg LN, Morrie RD, Adams DS. V-ATPase-dependent ectodermal voltage and pH regionalization are required for craniofacial morphogenesis. Dev Dyn. 2011;240(8):1889–1904. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.22685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vandenberg LN, Adams DS, Levin M. Normalized shape and location of perturbed craniofacial structures in the Xenopus tadpole reveal an innate ability to achieve correct morphology. Dev Dyn. 2012;241(5):863–878. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.23770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetere G, Tran LM, Moberg S, Steadman PE, Restivo L, Morrison FG, Ressler KJ, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW. Memory formation in the absence of experience. Nat Neurosci. 2019;22(6):933–940. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0389-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogel D, Dussutour A. Direct transfer of learned behaviour via cell fusion in non-neural organisms. Proc Biol Sci. 2016 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.2382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vyas A, Sapolsky R. Manipulation of host behaviour by Toxoplasma gondii: what is the minimum a proposed proximate mechanism should explain? Folia Parasitol. 2010;57(2):88–94. doi: 10.14411/fp.2010.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang XJ, Krystal JH. Computational psychiatry. Neuron. 2014;84(3):638–654. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson RA, Levin M, Buckley CL. Design for an individual: connectionist approaches to the evolutionary transitions in individuality. Front Ecol Evol. 2022 doi: 10.3389/fevo.2022.823588. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Watson RA, Buckley CL, Mills R, Davies A (2010) Associative memory in gene regulation networks. In: Proceedings of Artificial Life Conference XII, Odense, Denmark, pp 194–201
- Webster JP, McConkey GA. Toxoplasma gondii-altered host behaviour: clues as to mechanism of action. Folia Parasitol. 2010;57(2):95–104. doi: 10.14411/fp.2010.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West-Eberhard MJ. Developmental plasticity and the origin of species differences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102(Suppl 1):6543–6549. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0501844102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West-Eberhard MJ. Phenotypic accommodation: adaptive innovation due to developmental plasticity. J Exp Zool B Mol Dev Evol. 2005;304(6):610–618. doi: 10.1002/jez.b.21071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willems RM, Francken JC. Embodied cognition: taking the next step. Front Psychol. 2012;3:582. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams KB, Bischof J, Lee FJ, Miller KA, LaPalme JV, Wolfe BE, Levin M. Regulation of axial and head patterning during planarian regeneration by a commensal bacterium. Mech Dev. 2020;163:103614. doi: 10.1016/j.mod.2020.103614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson EO. Caste and division of labor in leaf-cutter ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae: Atta) Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1980;7(2):157–165. doi: 10.1007/BF00299521. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yang CY, Bialecka-Fornal M, Weatherwax C, Larkin JW, Prindle A, Liu J, Garcia-Ojalvo J, Süel GM. Encoding membrane-potential-based memory within a microbial community. Cell Syst. 2020;10(5):417–423. doi: 10.1016/j.cels.2020.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yi C, Spitters TW, Al-Far EAA, Wang S, Xiong T, Cai S, Yan X, Guan K, Wagner M, El-Armouche A, Antos CL. A calcineurin-mediated scaling mechanism that controls a K(+)-leak channel to regulate morphogen and growth factor transcription. Elife. 2021 doi: 10.7554/eLife.60691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.