Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Jan 7.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2020;45:71–87. doi: 10.1007/7854_2020_133

Cholinergic Signaling Dynamics and Cognitive Control of Attention

Vinay Parikh 1, Debra A Bangasser 1
PMCID: PMC10771779  NIHMSID: NIHMS1953353  PMID: 32447715

Abstract

The central cholinergic system is one of the most important modulator neurotransmitter system implicated in diverse behavioral processes. Activation of the basal forebrain cortical cholinergic input system represents a critical step in cortical information processing. This chapter explores recent developments illustrating cortical cholinergic transmission mediate defined cognitive operations, which is contrary to the traditional view that acetylcholine acts as a slowly acting neuromodulator that influences arousal cortex-wide. Specifically, we review the evidence that phasic cholinergic signaling in the prefrontal cortex is a causal mediator of signal detection. In addition, studies that support the neuromodulatory role of cholinergic inputs in top-down attentional control are summarized. Finally, we review new findings that reveal sex differences and hormonal regulation of the cholinergic-attention system.

Keywords: Acetylcholine, Attention, Muscarinic receptors, Nicotinic receptors, Sex differences

1. Introduction

Cholinergic inputs to the entire cortical mantle originate in the nucleus basalis of Meynert (nBM), substantia innominata (SI), the horizontal nucleus of the diagonal band (HDB), and the preoptic nucleus (collectively termed basal forebrain, BF). The BF corticopetal cholinergic system constitutes the most rostral component of neuromodulatory input systems, and its anatomical organization reflects its ability to orchestrate cortical information processing. Of the many different behavioral and cognitive processes that relate to the central cholinergic system, fundamental aspects of attention are closely linked to the activity of cortical cholinergic inputs (Ballinger et al. 2016; Sarter et al. 2005, 2016). Therefore, there is considerable interest in the dynamics of cortical cholinergic signaling and cholinergic regulation of attentional processes and capacities and developing procholinergic therapies to treat cognitive deficits in psychiatric and neurological disorders.

Historically, the organization of the BF cortical projection system was described as a diffuse and undifferentiated projection system with widespread cortical innervation, which corresponds to the notion that acetylcholine (ACh) influences the excitability of neurons cortex-wide to modulate global states of arousal and wakefulness. Earlier studies that focused on slow and regionally nonspecific changes in ACh efflux (volume transmission) supported this view (Bartolini and Pepeu 1967; Descarries 1998; Phillis 1968). However, advancement in electrochemical approaches to monitor neurochemical events at high temporal and spatial resolution in the past decade led to the characterization of phasic ACh release that was linked to specific cognitive events (Howe et al. 2013; Parikh et al. 2007). These developments, along with the refinement of neuroanatomical tools that revealed a highly organized topographic arrangement of cortical target-specific groups of BF cholinergic neurons, challenged previous conceptualization and support modality-/region-specific function of ACh (Lean et al. 2019; Zaborszky et al. 2015, 2018).

Cholinergic signaling is elicited by presynaptic release of ACh that activates two classes of ACh receptors, nicotinic (nAChR) and muscarinic (mAChR), in a spatially and temporally selected fashion due to the constraints imposed by the potent ACh-metabolizing enzyme acetylcholinesterase (AChE). nAChRs are a family of ligand-gated ionotropic receptors that mediate fast synaptic transmission by altering cation channel currents. Neuronal nAChRs are pentameric structures that are formed from a combination of five membrane-spanning units consisting of nine isoforms of α subunits (α2–α10) and three isoforms of β subunits (β2–β4) and arranged either as a heteromeric or homomeric assemblies (Gotti et al. 2009). Within the mammalian cortex, homomeric α7 and heteromeric α4β2 are the most predominant and widely distributed nAChRs. mAChRs are metabotropic receptors that, following activation by ACh, transduce their signaling via heterotrimeric G proteins. The M1 family of mAChRs include M1, M3, and M5 and signals via Gq proteins, while M2 and M4 mAChRs belong to the M2 family that are coupled to Gi proteins (Thiele 2013).

Efforts to develop cholinomimetic drugs as cognition enhancers have largely focused on boosting cholinergic transmission. Although psychopharmacological research to augment cholinergic signaling have generally focused on AChE inhibitors, the procognitive therapeutic efficacy of these drugs in human subjects have remained limited (Pepeu and Giovannini 2009). It is suggested that higher baseline ACh levels, as a result of AChE blockade, would result in generalized activation of cholinergic auto- and hetero-receptors that may uncouple presynaptic and postsynaptic mechanisms and, consequently, produce complex changes in the local cortical networks (Hasselmo and Sarter 2011; Pepeu and Giovannini 2009). Likewise, pharmacological studies that focused on nonselectively modulating mAChRs and nAChRs largely reported complex effects on cognition (Hasselmo and Sarter 2011; Sarter et al. 2009a). This chapter explores recent developments in our understanding of the cholinergic mechanisms of attention. Specifically, the evidence that phasic cholinergic signaling in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is a causal mediator of signal detection will be discussed. Moreover, studies that support the tonic neuromodulatory role of cholinergic inputs in top-down attentional control, and those that provide insights into the potential cellular substrates that integrate the phasic and neuromodulatory cholinergic signaling modes, will be reviewed. Because sex differences exist in the central cholinergic system, we will also highlight new findings that reveal sex differences in cholinergic-attention system. In conclusion, the framework to develop procholinergic therapies by targeting specific components of cortical cholinergic signaling will be briefly presented.

2. Cortical ACh and Attentional Performance

Substantial evidence from lesion and microdialysis studies supported the hypothesis that cortical cholinergic projections are necessary for performance in tasks that assess a range of attentional functions. A plethora of studies conducted in rodents demonstrated that selective lesions of BF cholinergic neurons and their cortical inputs produced by the immunotoxin 192-IgG saporin impair performance in various tasks of attention. For instance, cortical cholinergic deafferentation of rats trained in an operant sustained attention task (SAT) disrupted animals’ ability to detect the signal (correctly respond on signal trials), while sparing response accuracies on non-signal trials (McGaughy et al. 1996, 2000). In a cross-modal divided attention task that requires the processing of a visual and auditory conditioned stimulus, selective cholinergic lesions resulted in a speed-accuracy tradeoff under conditions of modality uncertainty, with longer correct response latencies in bimodal than in unimodal blocks of trials (Botly and De Rosa 2009; Newman and McGaughy 2008; Turchi and Sarter 1997). Additionally, removal of cholinergic inputs from the medial PFC reduced choice accuracies under conditions of increased attentional load and increased perseverative responding in animals performing the five-choice serial reaction time task (Dalley et al. 2004; Maddux et al. 2007). It is noteworthy that lesions of BF cholinergic neurons did not robustly impact performance of subjects trained in tasks that did not explicitly tax attentional processes, illustrating the specificity of cognitive impairments produced by cortical cholinergic deafferentation (Baxter et al. 1995; Frick et al. 2004; Vuckovich et al. 2004).

Studies employing in vivo microdialysis procedures in rats reproducibly demonstrated performance-associated increases in ACh release in the frontal and parietal cortex in operant tasks of attention (Arnold et al. 2002; Dalley et al. 2001; Himmelheber et al. 1997; Kozak et al. 2006; Passetti et al. 2000). More importantly, the levels of ACh release in attentional task-performing animals varied as a function of the demands on attention but did not correlate with levels of attentional performance. Such increases in cortical ACh efflux were not observed in animals performing various operant control procedures that do not explicitly tax attentional processes.

Although this research substantiated the claim made by lesions studies concerning the necessary role of BF cholinergic system in attentional performance, it remained limited in explaining the precise cognitive operations that are supported by cortical cholinergic activity. Microdialysis studies typically required 5–10 min of sample collection to detect ACh concentration in the dialysate using HPLC, which took over tens of trials to generate a single data point. Thus, the low temporal resolution of ACh release data limited the demonstration of specific attention task events or the behavioral/cognitive operations that are responsible for the increases in cortical cholinergic signaling. Consequently, conclusions based on microdialysis data were consistent with the conventional characteristics of ACh as a slowly acting cortex-wide neuromodulator optimizing input processing by regulating arousal states. As noted above, the presence of ubiquitous and highly potent ACh-metabolizing enzyme AChE, and the presence of nAChRs that mediate faster cholinergic signaling, suggests that the functions of forebrain cholinergic system are not sufficiently described by such notions. With the advent of electrochemical approaches and refinement in the design of enzyme-based microelectrodes, the measurement of cholinergic transmission on a faster time-scale became possible. The evidence generated from research based on these technical breakthroughs, which show rapid (phasic) changes in cholinergic transmission transients in specific behavioral contexts, led to the revision of previous conceptualizations of BF cholinergic system as discussed next.

3. Prefrontal Cholinergic Mechanisms of Signal Detection and Attentional Control

3.1. Phasic ACh Release Mediates Cue Detection

Considerable progress has been made in the development of enzyme-based biosensors to measure electrochemically rapid (on the time-scale of milliseconds to seconds) changes in extracellular choline levels, as a marker for cortical ACh release (Giuliano et al. 2008; Parikh et al. 2004). These approaches allow real-time monitoring of cholinergic signaling on a trial-by-trial basis in tasks of attention and have advanced our understanding of the specific role of phasic cholinergic signaling in signal (cue) detection. Detection here implies a cognitive-attentional process that relates to the entry of information concerning the presence of a stimulus (signal or cue) into a processing stream that allows the subject to report the existence of a signal by an arbitrary response established by the experimenter (Posner et al. 1980). This process is distinct from orientating that mostly reflects a process of aligning sensory response to the salient cue.

One of the initial studies that employed choline-sensitive biosensors to record cholinergic activity from the medial PFC was conducted in awake rats, performing a cued-appetitive response task (Parikh et al. 2007). This study demonstrated phasic cholinergic signals (cholinergic transients) evoked by “detected” cues (visual stimulus) that generated a distinct shift from ongoing behavior (e.g., grooming) toward the monitoring of the reward ports, followed by port approach and reward retrieval in response to reward delivery. The onset of the cholinergic transient was highly correlated with the onset of the behavioral shift. Moreover, prefrontal cholinergic transients were specifically associated with detected cues and did not occur with other task events such as reward delivery and reward retrieval. In trials involving missed cues, where the animal oriented to the cue but failed to initiate any response, cholinergic signals were not observed. Removal of cholinergic inputs to the recording region by locally infusing cholino-immunotoxin 192-IgG saporin, completely abolished cue-evoked phasic cholinergic signals in detected trials confirming that signals originated from cholinergic terminals. Collectively, these findings suggested that transient or phasic increases in prefrontal cholinergic activity mediate cue-evoked cognitive operations in attention-demanding contexts.

Additional experiments indicated that variation in the time interval between cue and reward delivery caused variation of the timing of the peak amplitude of cue-evoked cholinergic signals (Parikh et al. 2007; Parikh and Sarter 2008). This was an important observation as it indicated that cholinergic transients do not merely reflect sensory encoding of the cue. If that was the case, variations of cue-reward intervals should not affect the timing of the cholinergic transients. The variation of the timing of cue-evoked cholinergic transients indicates that phasic ACh release in the PFC is associated with a cognitive operation (cue detection), the timing of which is a function of cue-reward intervals.

A subsequent study that recorded PFC cholinergic activity in rats performing an operant SAT reported cholinergic transients during “hits,” i.e., correct responses on signal trials (Howe et al. 2013). Surprisingly, phasic cholinergic signals were observed only in 40% of hits. The cholinergic transients that were generated during hits were preceded either by correct rejections (correct responses on non-signal trials) or misses (incorrect responses on signal trials). Hits that were not associated with cholinergic transients were those preceded by hits. These findings indicated that phasic cholinergic signals mediate signal detection specifically in situations that involves a shift from monitoring to cue-directed behavior (shift hits). Additional evidence from fMRI studies conducted in humans performing the SAT task illustrated increase BOLD activation in the right rostrolateral/orbital PFC and right BF during shift hits and that this activation was associated with faster reaction times (Howe et al. 2013; Sarter et al. 2016).

Another study that combined optogenetics with electrochemistry tested the hypothesis that cholinergic transients have the capacity to cause signal detection even in the absence of signals (Gritton et al. 2016). Photostimulation of channel rhodopsin-expressed BF cholinergic neurons and prefrontal cholinergic terminals generated optogenetically evoked cholinergic transients and increased hit rates in SAT-performing mice. Moreover, suppression of phasic cholinergic activity by photostimulating halorhodopsin-expressed BF cholinergic neurons resulted in reduced hits without affecting correct rejections. Collectively, these findings indicate that phasic cholinergic signaling, specifically in the PFC, is not only associated exclusively with cue detection but are actually the causal mediators of shift hits (i.e., shifts from monitoring to signal detection). This view aligns with the lesion studies (discussed earlier) that show the detrimental effects of cortical cholinergic deafferentation were linked to detection performance (i.e., hit rates on signal trials and not correct rejections).

3.2. Top-Down Control of Attention and Cholinergic Neuromodulation

The ability to maintain stable task performance in the face of challenges or distractors requires attentional effort (Sarter et al. 2006). Cholinergic neuromodulation of the prefrontal efferent projections is conceptualized to enhance stimulus processing and to suppress the processing of irrelevant stimuli, distractors, or noise in a top-down fashion (Sarter et al. 2005). This hypothesis was supported by previous microdialysis studies that reported sustained increases in cholinergic activity during attentional challenges. For instance, steady increases in ACh efflux in medial PFC of SAT-performing rats were observed when animals moved from non-performing (baseline) stage to the performing (task) stage; however, ACh levels increased further with the presentation of visual distractors despite a reduction in hits (Kozak et al. 2006; St Peters et al. 2011). Human fMRI studies conducted in subjects performing SAT reported comparable increases in right PFC activity from baseline to SAT and then to the distracting condition (Berry et al. 2017; Demeter et al. 2008, 2011). Furthermore, SAT-associated ACh release in the medial PFC was attenuated in sign-tracking rats that show poor attentional control (Paolone et al. 2013).

Extracellular ACh efflux measured using microdialysis reflects a slower (tonic) component of cholinergic signaling that ranges from hundreds of seconds to tens of minutes. Tonic cholinergic activity is proposed to reflect a top-down neuromodulatory role of BF-cholinergic neurons to regulate cortical detection circuitry in an attempt to maintain task performance under conditions of distraction (Sarter and Lustig 2019). Although the dissociation between phasic and neuromodulatory (tonic) components of cholinergic signaling appears to be distinct in terms of cognitive operations; the two modes may interact to support overall attentional performance. This notion is supported by a previous in vivo amperometry study that reported a positive correlation between the magnitude of slower (timescale of minutes) session-related increases in tonic cholinergic activity and the amplitudes of phasic cholinergic signals in animals performing the cued-appetitive response task (Parikh et al. 2007). Given the constraints imposed by AChE on cholinergic signaling, the view that neuromodulatory/tonic cholinergic activity is driven by “volume transmission” is debated (Sarter et al. 2009b). It remains to be seen whether cholinergic neuromodulation is a consequence of sustained activity of BF cholinergic neurons, local presynaptic regulation in the cortical microcircuits, or another population of BF cholinergic neurons that produce tonic discharges (Sarter and Kim 2015; Sarter et al. 2014; Unal et al. 2012).

4. Cellular Regulation of Cholinergic Signaling Modes

4.1. High-Affinity Choline Transporters (CHTs)

Cholinergic terminals recover choline from the synaptic cleft following ACh degradation by AChE, through a hemicholinium-3 (HC-3)-sensitive high-affinity choline transporter (CHT). Because cholinergic synapses rely heavily on choline for ACh production, the capacity to import choline into presynaptic cholinergic compartments via CHTs dictates the rate of ACh synthesis and release (Ferguson and Blakely 2004; Sarter and Parikh 2005). CHT-mediated choline uptake was enhanced in the synaptosomes isolated from the medial PFC of SAT-performing rats; such increases in choline uptake were not observed in animals that completed a behavioral control session (Apparsundaram et al. 2005). The same study also reported attention performance-associated increases in the densities of CHTs on the surface membrane of prefrontal synaptosomes relative to the intracellular pools (outward CHT trafficking). Another study found a decline in the capacity to generate prefrontal cholinergic transients following sustained BF stimulation in CHT heterozygous mice (Parikh et al. 2013). Moreover, these mutants displayed high vulnerability to the effects of visual distractors in SAT and disrupted trafficking of subcellular CHTs. Likewise, a recent fMRI study that involved human subjects expressing a I89V variant of CHT (low CHT capacity) did not find increases in right prefrontal activity in these subjects during increases in attentional demands that is typically seen in normal subjects (Berry et al. 2015). Taken together, these interesting findings point toward an important role of CHT function in regulating presynaptic cholinergic neuromodulation and in sustaining phasic cholinergic signaling under situations that impose increased demands on BF cholinergic neurons, such as top-down attentional control.

4.2. nAChRs

Substantial evidence indicates that the administration of nicotine and nAChR agonists, specifically those that activate α4β2 nAChRs, exert beneficial effects on attention and related cognitive abilities (Allison and Shoaib 2013; Howe et al. 2010; Newhouse et al. 2004; Sarter et al. 2009a; Stolerman et al. 2000; Wilens and Decker 2007). α4β2 nAChRs situated on thalamic glutamatergic projections in the medial PFC are an important component of attention circuitry and that stimulation of these receptors increase glutamatergic activity (Lambe et al. 2003; LucasMeunier et al. 2009). Moreover, neuropharmacological studies employing in vivo amperometry demonstrated that the stimulation of α4β2 nAChRs produces transient increases in glutamate and ACh release in the medial PFC and that thalamocortical glutamatergic terminals are necessary for the generation of cholinergic transients (Parikh et al. 2008, 2010). Moreover, systemic administration of a full α4β2 nAChR agonist S38232 improved attentional performance following the presentation of distractor in rats (Howe et al. 2010). As noted above, attention control requires cholinergic neuromodulation, and it is possible that α4β2 nAChR activation facilitates phasic cholinergic signaling by tonically modulating glutamatergic-cholinergic interactions (Hasselmo and Sarter 2011). Although α7 nAChR agonists have also been reported to augment prefrontal glutamatergic transmission, they did not produce faster cholinergic transients as observed with the stimulation of α4β2 nAChRs (Bortz et al. 2013; Parikh et al. 2010). It is possible that α7 nAChRs recruit other ascending modulators such as monoamines which impact the dynamics of BF cholinergic signaling in a different way resulting in more complex effects on attention.

4.3. mAChRs

Systemic administration of mAChR antagonist scopolamine has consistently been shown to produce attentional impairments indicating that mAChRs may be important for cholinergic mediation of attention (Callahan et al. 1993; Chudasama et al. 2004; Young et al. 2013). However, the beneficial effects of mAChR agonists on cognitive processes have remained complex and could not be reliably demonstrated in clinics presumably due to lack of the availability of specific ligands targeting specific mAChR subtypes. It has been suggested that postsynaptic M1 receptors localized on cortical pyramidal neurons enhance voltage-dependent Ca2+ influx and action potential output in response to phasic release of ACh (Dasari et al. 2017). Moreover, a recent study reported that cue-evoked cholinergic transients in the medial PFC of animals performing the Pavlovian cued-approach task triggered theta-gamma coupling, and this synchronization and cue detection was disrupted following M1 receptor blockade (Howe et al. 2017). Thus, M1 receptor activation may regulate phasic ACh-induced prefrontal network synchrony required for cue detection.

5. Sex Differences and the Cholinergic Mediation of Attention

5.1. Neurochemical Sex Differences

The synthesis, release, and postsynaptic effects of many neurotransmitters systems are influenced by biological sex, and the BF corticopetal cholinergic system is no exception. As noted, a critical ACh-producing region within this circuit is the nBM. In rats, although a sex difference is not always observed (Gibbs 1996), there are reports that the nBM of females has more neurons containing the cholinergic synthetic enzyme, choline acetyltransferase (ChAT), than the nBM of males (Takase et al. 2007, 2009). The sex difference in ChAT neurons may be specific to the nBM, as it does not occur in the HDB (Takase et al. 2009), suggesting that the regions within the BF corticopetal cholinergic system are differentially influenced by sex. An increase in ChAT in the female nBM could facilitate their production of ACh relative to males. Consistent with this idea, the females have higher tonic ACh release than males in the mPFC (Takase et al. 2007, 2009). This sex differences in cortical ACh is observed across the circadian release profile for ACh (Takase et al. 2009). The enhanced tonic cortical ACh release in females may facilitate their top-down attentional control relative to males. However, sex differences in phasic ACh release have not been assessed. Given the greater capacity of females to synthesis of ACh, it is possible that a similar sex difference in phasic cholinergic signals would be detected, but further studies are needed.

Sex differences in ACh production and release likely result from circulating ovarian hormones. Cholinergic neurons in the BF contain estradiol receptors (ER), including ERα and the G-protein coupled ER (Gibbs 1996; Miettinen et al. 2002). Although ER levels in the BF cholinergic neurons are comparable in male and female rats (Gibbs 1996), the higher circulating levels of estradiol in females could preferentially influence their BF. One mechanism by which estradiol could influence cholinergic neurons is via the regulation of ChAT. Estradiol administration to ovariectomized female rats increases ChAT levels in nBM, but not the HDB (Gibbs 1997; Gibbs et al. 1994). In contrast to the role of estradiol, manipulation of testosterone in males does not affect ChAT in the nBM (Nakamura et al. 2002). Collectively, these studies suggest an increased nBM production and release of ACh in females that is driven by estradiol regulation of ChAT.

As noted, ACh exerts its effects through nAChRs and mAChRs. In humans, women have more β2-containing nAChRs and mAChRs in the frontal cortex than men (Cosgrove et al. 2012; Yoshida et al. 2000). These receptors also appear to be regulated by estrogens. Estradiol potentiates the human alpha4beta2 subtype of the nicotinic receptor (Curtis et al. 2002). In rats, mAChR binding is highest in females in the proestrous stage of the estrous cycle, which is the stage when estradiol levels are highest (van Huizen et al. 1994). Additional evidence for estrogenic regulation of cholinergic receptors comes from studies associating the loss of estrogens in menopause with a reduction in nAChRs and mAChRs (Norbury et al. 2007; Tinkler and Voytko 2005). Interestingly, postmenopausal women receiving estrogen replacement therapy have higher mAChR density in the lateral frontal cortex than untreated postmenopausal women (Norbury et al. 2007), indicating that restoring estrogen levels can mitigate against the negative effect of hormone loss on cholinergic receptor levels. When these findings are considered with the aforementioned studies on ACh production and release, it appears that, compared to males, the basal forebrain corticopetal system of females has a greater capacity for producing and responding to ACh, which could improve attention in females.

5.2. Behavioral Sex Differences

Psychiatric disorders with attention dysregulation often occur at different rates in men and women. For example, men are more likely to be diagnosed with ADHD and schizophrenia (Mendrek and Mancini-Marïe 2016; Ramtekkar et al. 2010). These disorders can also present differently in men and women, such that men with schizophrenia, for example, have greater deficits in cognitive processes, including attention (Goldstein et al. 1998; Mendrek and Mancini-Marïe 2016; Zhang et al. 2012). In aging populations, there is evidence that women have higher rates of Alzheimer’s disease than men (Gao et al. 1998; Mazure and Swendsen 2016). This sex difference has been attributed to a loss of estradiol in women, and there is some evidence that hormone replacement therapy reduces Alzheimer’s disease risk, especially when hormone replacement therapy is initiated within a short period of oophorectomy or natural menopause (Mielke et al. 2014; Rocca et al. 2011; Whitmer et al. 2011).

In healthy populations, there is also evidence for sex differences in certain aspects of attention. For example, women outperform men on a divided attention paradigm and their enhanced capacity to rapidly switch attention is thought to explain their better ability to multitask than men (Seçer and Yılmazoğulları 2016; Stoet et al. 2013). In rodents, females also do better at certain tasks of attention than males. For example, auditory distractors are less disruptive in female than male mice in an interval timing task (Buhusi et al. 2017). However, sex differences in attention may be specific to certain attentional processes because they are not observed in every attention task. For example, male and female rats perform similarly under baseline parameters in task of spatial divided attention (Bayless et al. 2012; Jentsch and Taylor 2003). When the task is made more difficult (e.g., by increasing the intertrial interval, decreasing the visual stimulus), females make more vigilance errors, while males make more errors of inhibitory control (Bayless et al. 2012, Jentsch and Taylor 2003). Similarly, performance on the SAT is comparable between male and female rats, even on the signal trials that require the release of ACh in the mPFC (Bangasser et al. 2017; Cole et al. 2016). These studies indicate that sex differences in attention differ based on the attentional process examined and often do not emerge until tested under challenging conditions.

There are some reports of estradiol regulating attentional processes. On a task of divided attention, a loss of estrogens impaired performance when conditions were challenging and this decrement was rescued by the administration of estradiol (Barnes et al. 2006). In contrast, performance in the sustained attention did not change across the estrous cycle (Cole et al. 2016), and ovariectomy did not impair performance on the task and, surprisingly, prevented a decrease in performance across the session (McGaughy and Sarter 1999). However, if BF cholinergic neurons were damaged with a selective neurotoxin, high levels of estradiol improved aspects of performance on the sustained attention task (McGaughy and Sarter 1999). These data suggest that when task parameters are easy the effects of estradiol on attention are difficult to detect; however, when the system is challenged, estradiol improves attention. In support of this, we challenged male and female rats with the stress neuropeptide, corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF), and assessed their performance on the sustained attention task (Cole et al. 2016). We found a significant dose-dependent impairment in all aspects of attention that was similar between the sexes. However, when the estrous cycle stage was assessed in females, we found that CRF impaired attention during estrous cycle phases with low levels of ovarian hormones but had little effect during phases with high levels of ovarian hormones (Cole et al. 2016). Functional connectivity analysis on brain networks activated (as measured with cFOS) by CRF revealed that female in the proestrous phase of their cycle that is characterized by high ovarian hormone levels had higher connectivity between the nBM and mPFC than females in the phase of their cycle with low ovarian hormones and males (Wiersielis et al. 2016). This finding indicates that estradiol may promote stress resilience by increasing the coupling of brain regions within the of the BF corticopetal cholinergic system. The mechanism by which this occurs, however, remains to be determined.

In sum, females appear to produce and release more ACh in the BF corticopetal system, and this effect is linked to estradiol. When it comes to behavior, which is more complex and often involves many regions, neurotransmitter, and hormones systems, there tends to be a bias toward females being better than males in certain aspects of attention, but this does not occur for all endpoints tested. When the system is challenged, however, estradiol can help promote resilience to attention deficits. This finding suggests that treatment with estrogens may be a method to improve attention in people diagnosed with psychiatric disorders. In support of this idea, the selective estrogen receptor modulator, raloxifene, improved attention/processing speed for both men and women with schizophrenia (Weickert et al. 2015). More work is needed, but understanding sex differences and hormonal regulation of the BF corticopetal cholinergic system will likely lead to novel therapies to improve cognition in psychiatric patients.

6. Conclusions

The presented evidence in support of the view that cortical cholinergic signaling mediates discrete components of attentional processing challenges the traditional conceptualizations that view ACh as a slow neuromodulator of cortical arousal. The findings that phasic ACh release mediates the detection of signals in attention-demanding contexts have major implications in understanding the role of cholinergic dysfunction in the manifestation of cognitive symptoms of neuropsychiatric disorders and age-related dementias. Dysregulated phasic cholinergic transients could disrupt attentional abilities of patients suffering from schizophrenia and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Sarter and Paolone 2011; Sarter et al. 2012). Abnormalities in the orchestration of phasic cholinergic signaling may precede global and structural decline in cholinergic function and consequently the loss of cholinergic neurons in Alzheimer’s disease (Mesulam 2004).

The development of procholinergic drugs to improve cognitive symptoms of psychiatric and neurological conditions may benefit tremendously by moving away from previous views concerning volume transmission of ACh and not focusing on drugs that produce generalized increase in cholinergic transmission (such as AChE inhibitors). As discussed above, the new evidence from neuropharmacology and behavioral studies indicate that drugs that specifically amplify cholinergic transients via tonic neuromodulation of cholinergic synapses (e.g., α4β2 nAChR agonists) may improve attentional control. Likewise, M1-selective mAChR agonists may exert beneficial effects on cue detection by enhancing the efficiency of phasic ACh for synchronizing the activity of prefrontal networks. At the presynaptic level, drugs that influence molecular mechanisms to enhance the capacity of cholinergic synapses to sustain phasic cholinergic signaling (e.g., choline transporter-mediated choline uptake mechanisms) may enhance attentional performance. Finally, research on the hormonal regulation of cholinergic transmission is just beginning to answer specific questions concerning sex differences in the cholinergic-attention system. This research will greatly benefit the development of procholinergic drugs for sex-specific treatment of the cognitive symptoms of psychiatric disorders.

Acknowledgments

The authors’ research is supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging (AG046580) to VP and National Science Foundation (NSF CAREER IOS-15524) to DAB.

References

  1. Allison C, Shoaib M (2013) Nicotine improves performance in an attentional set shifting task in rats. Neuropharmacology 64:314–320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Apparsundaram S, Martinez V, Parikh V, Kozak R, Sarter M (2005) Increased capacity and density of choline transporters situated in synaptic membranes of the right medial prefrontal cortex of attentional task-performing rats. J Neurosci 25:3851–3856 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnold HM, Burk JA, Hodgson EM, Sarter M, Bruno JP (2002) Differential cortical acetylcholine release in rats performing a sustained attention task versus behavioral control tasks that do not explicitly tax attention. Neuroscience 114:451–460 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ballinger EC, Ananth M, Talmage DA, Role LW (2016) Basal forebrain cholinergic circuits and signaling in cognition and cognitive decline. Neuron 91:1199–1218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bangasser DA, Wicks B, Waxler DE, Eck SR (2017) Touchscreen sustained attention task (SAT) for rats. J Vis Exp 127:e56219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barnes P, Staal V, Muir J, Good MA (2006) 17-β estradiol administration attenuates deficits in sustained and divided attention in young ovariectomized rats and aged acyclic female rats. Behav Neurosci 120:1225–1234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bartolini A, Pepeu G (1967) Investigations into the acetylcholine output from the cerebral cortex of the cat in the presence of hyoscine. Br J Pharmacol Chemother 31:66–73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Baxter MG, Bucci DJ, Gorman LK, Wiley RG, Gallagher M (1995) Selective immunotoxic lesions of basal forebrain cholinergic cells: effects on learning and memory in rats. Behav Neurosci 109:714–722 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bayless DW, Darling JS, Stout WJ, Daniel JM (2012) Sex differences in attentional processes in adult rats as measured by performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Behav Brain Res 235:48–54 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Berry AS, Blakely RD, Sarter M, Lustig C (2015) Cholinergic capacity mediates prefrontal engagement during challenges to attention: evidence from imaging genetics. NeuroImage 108:386–395 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Berry AS, Sarter M, Lustig C (2017) Distinct frontoparietal networks underlying attentional effort and cognitive control. J Cogn Neurosci 29:1212–1225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bortz DM, Mikkelsen JD, Bruno JP (2013) Localized infusions of the partial alpha 7 nicotinic receptor agonist SSR180711 evoke rapid and transient increases in prefrontal glutamate release. Neuroscience 255:55–67 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Botly LC, De Rosa E (2009) Cholinergic deafferentation of the neocortex using 192 IgG-saporin impairs feature binding in rats. J Neurosci 29:4120–4130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Buhusi M, Bartlett MJ, Buhusi CV (2017) Sex differences in interval timing and attention to time in C57Bl/6J mice. Behav Brain Res 324:96–99 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Callahan MJ, Kinsora JJ, Harbaugh RE, Reeder TM, Davis RE (1993) Continuous ICV infusion of scopolamine impairs sustained attention of rhesus monkeys. Neurobiol Aging 14:147–151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chudasama Y, Dalley JW, Nathwani F, Bouger P, Robbins TW (2004) Cholinergic modulation of visual attention and working memory: dissociable effects of basal forebrain 192-IgG-saporin lesions and intraprefrontal infusions of scopolamine. Learn Mem 11:78–86 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cole RD, Kawasumi Y, Parikh V, Bangasser DA (2016) Corticotropin releasing factor impairs sustained attention in male and female rats. Behav Brain Res 296:30–34 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Cosgrove KP, Esterlis I, McKee SA et al. (2012) Sex differences in availability of β2-nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in recently abstinent tobacco smokers. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69:418–427 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Curtis L, Buisson B, Bertrand S, Bertrand D (2002) Potentiation of human α4β2 neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptor by estradiol. Mol Pharmacol 61:127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dalley JW, McGaughy J, O’Connell MT, Cardinal RN, Levita L, Robbins TW (2001) Distinct changes in cortical acetylcholine and noradrenaline efflux during contingent and noncontingent performance of a visual attentional task. J Neurosci 21:4908–4914 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dalley JW, Theobald DE, Bouger P, Chudasama Y, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW (2004) Cortical cholinergic function and deficits in visual attentional performance in rats following 192 IgG-saporin-induced lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 14:922–932 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dasari S, Hill C, Gulledge AT (2017) A unifying hypothesis for M1 muscarinic receptor signalling in pyramidal neurons. J Physiol 595:1711–1723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Demeter E, Sarter M, Lustig C (2008) Rats and humans paying attention: cross-species task development for translational research. Neuropsychology 22:787–799 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Demeter E, Hernandez-Garcia L, Sarter M, Lustig C (2011) Challenges to attention: a continuous arterial spin labeling (ASL) study of the effects of distraction on sustained attention. NeuroImage 54:1518–1529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Descarries L (1998) The hypothesis of an ambient level of acetylcholine in the central nervous system. J Physiol Paris 92:215–220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ferguson SM, Blakely RD (2004) The choline transporter resurfaces: new roles for synaptic vesicles? Mol Interv 4:22–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Frick KM, Kim JJ, Baxter MG (2004) Effects of complete immunotoxin lesions of the cholinergic basal forebrain on fear conditioning and spatial learning. Hippocampus 14:244–254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gao S, Hendrie HC, Hall KS, Hui S (1998) The relationships between age, sex, and the incidence of dementia and Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:809–815 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gibbs RB (1996) Expression of estrogen receptor-like immunoreactivity by different subgroups of basal forebrain cholinergic neurons in gonadectomized male and female rats. Brain Res 720:61–68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gibbs RB (1997) Effects of estrogen on basal forebrain cholinergic neurons vary as a function of dose and duration of treatment. Brain Res 757:10–16 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gibbs RB, Wu D, Hersh LB, Pfaff DW (1994) Effects of estrogen replacement on the relative levels of choline Acetyltransferase, trkA, and nerve growth factor messenger RNAs in the basal forebrain and hippocampal formation of adult rats. Exp Neurol 129:70–80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Giuliano C, Parikh V, Ward JR, Chiamulera C, Sarter M (2008) Increases in cholinergic neurotransmission measured by using choline-sensitive microelectrodes: enhanced detection by hydrolysis of acetylcholine on recording sites? Neurochem Int 52:1343–1350 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Goldstein JM, Seidman LJ, Goodman JM, Koren D, Lee H et al. (1998) Are there sex differences in neuropsychological functions among patients with schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatr 155:1358–1364 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gotti C, Clementi F, Fornari A, Gaimarri A, Guiducci S et al. (2009) Structural and functional diversity of native brain neuronal nicotinic receptors. Biochem Pharmacol 78:703–711 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gritton HJ, Howe WM, Mallory CS, Hetrick VL, Berke JD, Sarter M (2016) Cortical cholinergic signaling controls the detection of cues. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:E1089–E1097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hasselmo ME, Sarter M (2011) Modes and models of forebrain cholinergic neuromodulation of cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology 36:52–73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Himmelheber AM, Sarter M, Bruno JP (1997) Operant performance and cortical acetylcholine release: role of response rate, reward density, and non-contingent stimuli. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 6:23–36 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Howe WM, Ji J, Parikh V, Williams S, Mocaer E et al. (2010) Enhancement of attentional performance by selective stimulation of alpha4beta2() nAChRs: underlying cholinergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:1391–1401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Howe WM, Berry AS, Francois J, Gilmour G, Carp JM et al. (2013) Prefrontal cholinergic mechanisms instigating shifts from monitoring for cues to cue-guided performance: converging electrochemical and fMRI evidence from rats and humans. J Neurosci 33:8742–8752 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Howe WM, Gritton HJ, Lusk NA, Roberts EA, Hetrick VL et al. (2017) Acetylcholine release in prefrontal cortex promotes gamma oscillations and theta-gamma coupling during cue detection. J Neurosci 37:3215–3230 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jentsch JD, Taylor JR (2003) Sex-related differences in spatial divided attention and motor impulsivity in rats. Behav Neurosci 117:76–83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kozak R, Bruno JP, Sarter M (2006) Augmented prefrontal acetylcholine release during challenged attentional performance. Cereb Cortex 16:9–17 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lambe EK, Picciotto MR, Aghajanian GK (2003) Nicotine induces glutamate release from thalamocortical terminals in prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:216–225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lean GA, Liu YJ, Lyon DC (2019) Cell type specific tracing of the subcortical input to primary visual cortex from the basal forebrain. J Comp Neurol 527:589–599 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lucas-Meunier E, Monier C, Amar M, Baux G, Fregnac Y, Fossier P (2009) Involvement of nicotinic and muscarinic receptors in the endogenous cholinergic modulation of the balance between excitation and inhibition in the young rat visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 19:2411–2427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Maddux JM, Kerfoot EC, Chatterjee S, Holland PC (2007) Dissociation of attention in learning and action: effects of lesions of the amygdala central nucleus, medial prefrontal cortex, and posterior parietal cortex. Behav Neurosci 121:63–79 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mazure CM, Swendsen J (2016) Sex differences in Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias. Lancet Neurol 15:451–452 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McGaughy J, Sarter M (1999) Effects of ovariectomy, 192 IgG-saporin-induced cortical cholinergic deafferentation, and administration of estradiol on sustained attention performance in rats. Behav Neurosci 113:1216–1232 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McGaughy J, Kaiser T, Sarter M (1996) Behavioral vigilance following infusions of 192 IgG-saporin into the basal forebrain: selectivity of the behavioral impairment and relation to cortical AChE-positive fiber density. Behav Neurosci 110:247–265 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. McGaughy J, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW, Sarter M (2000) The role of cortical cholinergic afferent projections in cognition: impact of new selective immunotoxins. Behav Brain Res 115:251–263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mendrek A, Mancini-Marïe A (2016) Sex/gender differences in the brain and cognition in schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 67:57–78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mesulam M (2004) The cholinergic lesion of Alzheimer’s disease: pivotal factor or side show? Learn Mem 11:43–49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mielke MM, Vemuri P, Rocca WA (2014) Clinical epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease: assessing sex and gender differences. Clin Epidemiol 6:37–48 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Miettinen RA, Kalesnykas G, Koivisto EH (2002) Estimation of the total number of cholinergic neurons containing estrogen receptor-alpha in the rat basal forebrain. J Histochem Cytochem 50:891–902 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nakamura N, Fujita H, Kawata M (2002) Effects of gonadectomy on immunoreactivity for choline acetyltransferase in the cortex, hippocampus, and basal forebrain of adult male rats. Neuroscience 109:473–485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Newhouse PA, Potter A, Singh A (2004) Effects of nicotinic stimulation on cognitive performance. Curr Opin Pharmacol 4:36–46 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Newman LA, McGaughy J (2008) Cholinergic deafferentation of prefrontal cortex increases sensitivity to cross-modal distractors during a sustained attention task. J Neurosci 28:2642–2650 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Norbury R, Travis MJ, Erlandsson K, Waddington W, Ell PJ, Murphy DGM (2007) Estrogen therapy and brain muscarinic receptor density in healthy females: a SPET study. Horm Behav 51:249–257 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Paolone G, Angelakos CC, Meyer PJ, Robinson TE, Sarter M (2013) Cholinergic control over attention in rats prone to attribute incentive salience to reward cues. J Neurosci 33:8321–8335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Parikh V, Sarter M (2008) Cholinergic mediation of attention: contributions of phasic and tonic increases in prefrontal cholinergic activity. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1129:225–235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Parikh V, Pomerleau F, Huettl P, Gerhardt GA, Sarter M, Bruno JP (2004) Rapid assessment of in vivo cholinergic transmission by amperometric detection of changes in extracellular choline levels. Eur J Neurosci 20:1545–1554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Parikh V, Kozak R, Martinez V, Sarter M (2007) Prefrontal acetylcholine release controls cue detection on multiple timescales. Neuron 56:141–154 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Parikh V, Man K, Decker MW, Sarter M (2008) Glutamatergic contributions to nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonist-evoked cholinergic transients in the prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 28:3769–3780 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Parikh V, Ji J, Decker MW, Sarter M (2010) Prefrontal beta2 subunit-containing and alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors differentially control glutamatergic and cholinergic signaling. J Neurosci 30:3518–3530 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Parikh V, St Peters M, Blakely RD, Sarter M (2013) The presynaptic choline transporter imposes limits on sustained cortical acetylcholine release and attention. J Neurosci 33:2326–2337 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Passetti F, Dalley JW, O’Connell MT, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW (2000) Increased acetylcholine release in the rat medial prefrontal cortex during performance of a visual attentional task. Eur J Neurosci 12:3051–3058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Pepeu G, Giovannini MG (2009) Cholinesterase inhibitors and beyond. Curr Alzheimer Res 6:86–96 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Phillis JW (1968) Acetylcholine release from the cerebral cortex: its role in cortical arousal. Brain Res 7:378–389 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Posner MI, Snyder CR, Davidson BJ (1980) Attention and the detection of signals. J Exp Psychol 109:160–174 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ramtekkar UP, Reiersen AM, Todorov AA, Todd RD (2010) Sex and age differences in attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and diagnoses: implications for DSM-V and ICD-11. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49:217–28e1–3 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rocca WA, Grossardt BR, Shuster LT (2011) Oophorectomy, menopause, estrogen treatment, and cognitive aging: clinical evidence for a window of opportunity. Brain Res 1379:188–198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sarter M, Kim Y (2015) Interpreting chemical neurotransmission in vivo: techniques, time scales, and theories. ACS Chem Neurosci 6:8–10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Sarter M, Lustig C (2019) Cholinergic double duty: cue detection and attentional control. Curr Opin Psychol 29:102–107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sarter M, Parikh V (2005) Choline transporters, cholinergic transmission and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:48–56 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Sarter M, Hasselmo ME, Bruno JP, Givens B (2005) Unraveling the attentional functions of cortical cholinergic inputs: interactions between signal-driven and cognitive modulation of signal detection. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 48:98–111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Sarter M, Gehring WJ, Kozak R (2006) More attention must be paid: the neurobiology of attentional effort. Brain Res Rev 51:145–160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sarter M, Paolone G (2011) Deficits in attentional control: cholinergic mechanisms and circuitrybased approaches. Behav Neurosci 125:825–835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Sarter M, Parikh V, Howe WM (2009a) nAChR agonist-induced cognition enhancement: integration of cognitive and neuronal mechanisms. Biochem Pharmacol 78:658–667 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sarter M, Parikh V, Howe WM (2009b) Phasic acetylcholine release and the volume transmission hypothesis: time to move on. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:383–390 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sarter M, Lustig C, Taylor SF (2012) Cholinergic contributions to the cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia and the viability of cholinergic treatments. Neuropharmacology 62:1544–1553 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Sarter M, Lustig C, Howe WM, Gritton H, Berry AS (2014) Deterministic functions of cortical acetylcholine. Eur J Neurosci 39:1912–1920 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sarter M, Lustig C, Berry AS, Gritton H, Howe WM, Parikh V (2016) What do phasic cholinergic signals do? Neurobiol Learn Mem 130:135–141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Seçer I, Yılmazoğulları Y (2016) Are attentional resources a mediator for sex differences in memory? Int J Psychol 51:117–122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. St Peters M, Demeter E, Lustig C, Bruno JP, Sarter M (2011) Enhanced control of attention by stimulating mesolimbic-corticopetal cholinergic circuitry. J Neurosci 31:9760–9771 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Stoet G, O’Connor DB, Conner M, Laws KR (2013) Are women better than men at multi-tasking? BMC Psychol 1:18 [Google Scholar]
  86. Stolerman IP, Mirza NR, Hahn B, Shoaib M (2000) Nicotine in an animal model of attention. Eur J Pharmacol 393:147–154 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Takase K, Mitsushima D, Funabashi T, Kimura F (2007) Sex difference in the 24-h acetylcholine release profile in the premotor/supplementary motor area of behaving rats. Brain Res 1154:105–115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Takase K, Kimura F, Yagami T, Mitsushima D (2009) Sex-specific 24-h acetylcholine release profile in the medial prefrontal cortex: simultaneous measurement of spontaneous locomotor activity in behaving rats. Neuroscience 159:7–15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Thiele A (2013) Muscarinic signaling in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 36:271–294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tinkler GP, Voytko ML (2005) Estrogen modulates cognitive and cholinergic processes in surgically menopausal monkeys. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 29:423–431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Turchi J, Sarter M (1997) Cortical acetylcholine and processing capacity: effects of cortical cholinergic deafferentation on crossmodal divided attention in rats. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 6:147–158 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Unal CT, Golowasch JP, Zaborszky L (2012) Adult mouse basal forebrain harbors two distinct cholinergic populations defined by their electrophysiology. Front Behav Neurosci 6:21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. van Huizen F, March D, Cynader MS, Shaw C (1994) Muscarinic receptor characteristics and regulation in rat cerebral cortex: changes during development, aging and the oestrous cycle. Eur J Neurosci 6:237–243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Vuckovich JA, Semel ME, Baxter MG (2004) Extensive lesions of cholinergic basal forebrain neurons do not impair spatial working memory. Learn Mem 11:87–94 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Weickert TW, Weinberg D, Lenroot R, Catts SV, Wells R et al. (2015) Adjunctive raloxifene treatment improves attention and memory in men and women with schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry 20:685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Whitmer RA, Quesenberry CP, Zhou J, Yaffe K (2011) Timing of hormone therapy and dementia: the critical window theory revisited. Ann Neurol 69:163–169 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Wiersielis KR, Wicks B, Simko H, Cohen SR, Khantsis S et al. (2016) Sex differences in corticotropin releasing factor-evoked behavior and activated networks. Psychoneuroendocrinology 73:204–216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Wilens TE, Decker MW (2007) Neuronal nicotinic receptor agonists for the treatment of attentiondeficit/hyperactivity disorder: focus on cognition. Biochem Pharmacol 74:1212–1223 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Yoshida T, Kuwabara Y, Sasaki M, Fukumura T, Ichimiya A et al. (2000) Sex-related differences in the muscarinic acetylcholinergic receptor in the healthy human brain--a positron emission tomography study. Ann Nucl Med 14:97–101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Young JW, Geyer MA, Rissling AJ, Sharp RF, Eyler LT et al. (2013) Reverse translation of the rodent 5C-CPT reveals that the impaired attention of people with schizophrenia is similar to scopolamine-induced deficits in mice. Transl Psychiatry 3:e324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Zaborszky L, Csordas A, Mosca K, Kim J, Gielow MR et al. (2015) Neurons in the basal forebrain project to the cortex in a complex topographic organization that reflects corticocortical connectivity patterns: an experimental study based on retrograde tracing and 3D reconstruction. Cereb Cortex 25:118–137 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Zaborszky L, Gombkoto P, Varsanyi P, Gielow MR, Poe G et al. (2018) Specific basal forebrain-cortical cholinergic circuits coordinate cognitive operations. J Neurosci 38:9446–9458 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Zhang XY, Chen DC, Xiu MH, Yang FD, Haile CN et al. (2012) Gender differences in never-medicated first-episode schizophrenia and medicated chronic schizophrenia patients. J Clin Psychiatry 73:1025–1033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES