Skip to main content
Healthcare logoLink to Healthcare
. 2023 Dec 19;12(1):3. doi: 10.3390/healthcare12010003

Effectiveness of Self-Affirmation Interventions in Educational Settings: A Meta-Analysis

Carolang Escobar-Soler 1,2,*, Raúl Berrios 3, Gabriel Peñaloza-Díaz 1, Carlos Melis-Rivera 1, Alejandra Caqueo-Urízar 4, Felipe Ponce-Correa 1, Jerome Flores 2
Editor: Jorge Pérez-Gómez
PMCID: PMC10779329  PMID: 38200909

Abstract

School and university can be stressful contexts that can become an important source of identity threats when social prejudices or stereotypes come into play. Self-affirmation interventions are key strategies for mitigating the negative consequences of identity threat. This meta-analysis aims to provide an overview of the effectiveness of self-affirmation interventions in educational settings. A peer-reviewed article search was conducted in January 2023. A total of 144 experimental studies that tested the effect of self-affirmation interventions in educational contexts among high school and university students from different social and cultural backgrounds were considered. The average effect of self-affirmation interventions was of low magnitude (dIG+ = 0.41, z = 16.01, p < 0.00), with a 95% confidence interval whose values tended to lie between 0.36 and 0.45 (SE = 0.0253). In addition, moderators such as identity threat, participants’ age, and intervention procedure were found. Through a meta-analysis of the impact of self-affirmation interventions in educational contexts, this study suggests that interventions are effective, resulting in a small mean effect size. Thus, self-affirmation interventions can be considered useful, brief, and inexpensive strategies to improve general well-being and performance in educational settings.

Keywords: self-affirmation, intervention, effects, education, schoolchildren, college students

1. Introduction

Self-affirmation as an intervention strategy aims to protect students’ self-esteem by promoting positive self-evaluations and creating self-schemas that act as authentic emotional and affective support in the face of negative experiences such as learning difficulties or school failure [1]. Another experience that can threaten students’ sense of self is discrimination or segregation (stereotype threat) for belonging to a minority group, whereby they are forced to adopt defensive mechanisms that lead them to make their own values invisible and begin to identify with those desired by others [2,3].

Stereotype threat is a real obstacle to the development of students’ identities (especially at critical stages of the life cycle, such as adolescence). It causes socioaffective reactions oriented toward the activation of defensive mechanisms that lead to a reconceptualization of the self and one’s own identity in order to adapt to an environment they perceive as threatening. In this scenario, self-affirmation succeeds in promoting genuinely positive self-definitions that immunize students against the threat of stereotyping [4].

The effects of self-affirmation drive not only the evaluation of difficult circumstances in the form of hopeful or resilient coping against identity threats, but also a positive and broad valuation of self-concept and self-image, where the individual recognizes him/herself as good and capable according to the characteristics of his/her own social and cultural context [5,6,7]. The central idea proposed by this theory suggests that the affirmation of the self points to the recognition and valuation of a broader and more versatile self-concept capable of drawing on a more significant number of personal resources in the face of identity threats. A broad perspective and resources of the self allows us to avoid focusing our attention on the threat, overestimating its importance, and consequently exacerbating its negative effect. Adopting an optimistic and resilient disposition allows people to feel capable of resolving perceived threats and safeguarding psychological well-being [8,9,10]. It should be pointed out that self-affirmation is not synonymous with self-praise; instead, it helps develop the conviction of being worthy of praise or admiration by others, according to what is desirable and approved in a given context.

Individuals frequently face different types of identity threats; therefore, they have different self-affirmation needs. In this regard, the effects of self-affirmation are usually more pronounced when there is a high perception of threat and when it is essential to defend or protect oneself from it by exerting self-confidence or self-efficacy to reduce or mitigate it [8].

When an individual feels highly threatened, the effects of self-affirmation promote perceptions of self-efficacy through which they begin to recognize more personal resources and, therefore, more confidence to manage and cope with the threat [10,11].

This theory proposes three fundamental principles to explain the positive and persistent long-term effects of self-affirmation: (1) recursion, (2) interaction, and (3) subjective interpretation. The first is related to the recurrence of self-affirmation’s effects, as the perception of achievement or progress becomes an important incentive to reassert oneself and continue improving. The second principle is associated with the contexts in which individuals develop. By trusting their resources and feeling increasingly capable of growing in different areas of their lives, individuals tend to show changes that others can perceive, evaluate, and value positively, triggering the effects of self-affirmation from an intersubjective source. Finally, the third principle is related to the reframing of identity in individuals who change the way they evaluate and perceive themselves in such a way that they manage to feel satisfied and motivated to continue discovering and progressing [12,13,14].

Particularly in educational settings, stressful experiences extend beyond studying, taking exams, and writing essays; they include relationships with classmates, which can become an essential source of identity threats when prejudices or social stereotypes emerge [5,15,16]. Indeed, a study conducted on adolescents of African descent who perceived hostility in their educational environment showed that stress levels were not related to academic aspects, but relational ones, with lower grades observed in this group of students. The study found that interventions based on self-affirmation reduced the perception of threat and predicted better academic performance in a group of students of African descent [17].

In another study, results showed that when Latin American students perceived less threat from their classmates, they performed better academically, with similar grades as North American or European students [10].

Other studies using self-affirmation interventions in an educational context with social segregation have also reported improvements in the sense of belonging to groups and social competencies [7,9].

Belonging to a stigmatized group can jeopardize self-integration and negatively affect the sense of personal adequacy in the educational context, compromising not only self-esteem but also learning and academic performance [18,19,20,21]. Given this scenario, self-affirmation manages to reduce stereotype threats, facilitating students’ adaptation to the school environment and their functioning, safeguarding positive self-evaluations, and favoring progress in their studies despite the hostility they may perceive [22,23].

Some studies have found that self-affirming students successfully cope with experiences of discrimination. Even when they experience different difficulties (e.g., problems with regulating emotions, feelings of inadequacy and inferiority, nonconformity with self-image, or affective distancing from family), adolescents can thrive and obtain better grades [7,10,24,25,26].

Regarding interventions in educational settings, for more than a decade, experimental studies have attempted to examine the effects of self-affirmation, particularly in minority and stigmatized groups. For example, strategies such as expressive writing have been used to promote identity self-affirmation in a diverse sociocultural classroom population with improved academic achievement and well-being, while observing greater effectiveness in groups where psychological threats are intense and imminent [7,8,10,17,24,25,27,28,29,30,31].

Some studies have found minimal or even significant effects of self-affirmation interventions and suggesting these could be explained by individual differences between participants. It cannot be assumed that individual responses to identity threats will be the same, even if social or cultural origins correspond to those of studied minorities or segregated groups. Similarly, the way in which the threat affects the identity of an individual or a group depends, to a large extent, on the social and cultural characteristics of the context. Therefore, it is not plausible to expect these interventions to have a positive impact if it is not certain that these individuals or groups are potentially threatened in their own contexts [32]. In this sense, researchers suggest an exhaustive search and a more precise definition of moderators related to individual differences and methodological aspects that may explain the variation in these interventions across studies. We suggest so in this meta-analysis and mainly focus on the review of moderators related to sample characteristics and on the methodological designs of the studies.

Despite the diversity of theoretical conjectures regarding the conditions that influence the effectiveness of these interventions, there is insufficient empirical evidence to make practical sense of and explain why there is so much heterogeneity in the effects of these interventions across studies whose outcome variables have remained the same.

The present meta-analysis aimed to provide a broader and more general overview of the effectiveness of self-affirmation interventions on psychosocial, academic, and health variables in high school and university students and to provide estimates that may shed light on the methodological conditions that are sufficient and necessary to ensure favorable results, regardless of the outcome variable on which the effects of these interventions can be analyzed.

2. Methods

2.1. Eligibility Criteria

The included studies shared seminal theoretical principles of self-affirmation interventions, intervention procedures, and methodological delimitation. Similarly, the study populations coincided in terms of participants, forming groups with and without identity threats as the key elements. In line with the aim of this review, the studies generally measured the effectiveness of self-affirmation interventions in educational contexts.

In order to ensure the rigor and relevance of our study selection process, we established the following inclusion criteria: (1) the studies employed an experimental or quasi-experimental design with a nonclinical sample of subjects; (2) the studies utilized a single self-affirmation intervention strategy (studies whose intervention proposal considered more than one strategy were excluded); (3) the study samples consisted of school or university students in diverse educational and sociocultural contexts; (4) from a methodological perspective, the studies reported a random or at least quasi-random assignment of participants to experimental and control groups; (5) the studies manipulated students’ self-affirmation experience through expressive writing on a significant personal value (a technique most commonly used to promote self-affirmation effects in students) and reported the effectiveness of this intervention on academic performance, aspects of psychological well-being, or interpersonal development, offering intra- and intergroup comparisons. Sometimes, the full texts of the identifiable reports were not available; therefore, they were excluded from the analyses.

If studies reported more than one sample, the effect size of each sample was included in the meta-analysis. If studies reported multiple measures of the same construct, the average effect size for that construct within the study was calculated and coded. In cases where the studies reported multiple outcomes, multiple effect sizes were coded. Finally, for the longitudinal studies, the effect sizes of the self-affirmation interventions on the outcome variables were extracted. Specifically, if a study had only one temporal follow-up point (e.g., T2), the effect was extracted at T2. Conversely, if the study included multiple temporal follow-up points, all effect sizes were extracted and the mean was calculated.

2.2. Search Strategy

In January 2023, peer-reviewed articles investigating the impact of self-affirmation interventions in educational contexts were retrieved through systematic searches of the Web of Science, ProQuest, APA PSYCNET, and Google Scholar databases. The search for articles was temporally placed between the years 1990 and 2023, using the following keywords: “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Educational contexts”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Academic contexts”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Children”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Teenagers/Adolescents”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Elementary School”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Classroom”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND High School”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Secondary School”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Primary School”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Students”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Minorities”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Stereotype”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND University”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND College”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Academic”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Undergraduate”, “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Degree”, and “Self-Affirmation Interventions AND Bachelor”. Further, the reference sections of the most relevant articles were examined and emails were sent to relevant researchers in the field to search for articles that could not be identified through the databases. Finally, no geographical restrictions were applied; however, studies were limited to those published in English or Spanish.

2.3. Assessment of the Methodological Quality

The “Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies”, developed by the Effective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP), was applied to comprehensively evaluate methodological quality in the selected studies. This tool, tailored for quantitative research, examines various dimensions, including selection bias, study design, confounders, blinding, data collection methods, and withdrawals and dropouts.

We examined participant representation for selection bias, ensuring alignment with the target population. The analysis of study design was aimed at appropriateness and robustness. Strategies to address confounders were assessed, and blinding procedures were thoroughly reviewed to gauge participant and assessor blinding adequacy. Additionally, the tool systematically explored data collection methods, emphasizing validity and reliability assessment. Lastly, the evaluation of withdrawals and dropouts focused on the studies’ capability to acknowledge and explain reasons for incomplete participant data.

Each component was rated as strong, moderate, or weak, providing an overall score for the methodological quality of each study. Independent assessments were conducted by two raters, and any discrepancies were resolved through consultation with the first author.

2.4. Data Extraction and Data Items

A wide range of study characteristics were systematically coded, considering their potential influence on the variability of effect sizes. Study characteristics included (a) year of publication and (b) country from which data were collected. Sample sociodemographic data encompassed (a) the mean or age range of the participants, (b) cultural origin, (c) economic status, (d) sample size, and (e) dominant racial group. Various methodological characteristics were also recorded. These included (a) type of execution modality (face-to-face or virtual), (b) rigor of randomization, (c) dependent variable type, (d) self-affirmation intervention style, (e) placebo style, (f) timing of the intervention (before, during, or after stressful academic events), (g) presentation format of the intervention, and (h) teacher role. The first and third authors independently reviewed and systematically coded all eligible studies. Throughout the coding phase, the second author continuously checked the quality of the codes to identify divergent coding. Discrepancies were resolved by consensus.

Moderators were coded according to the sociodemographic characteristics of the sample and the methodological design (see Table 1). These moderators are in line with those suggested in the literature, pointing to features of the delivery of activities, individual characteristics of the participating students, and aspects of the social context [32,33]. Methodological procedural aspects were used as moderating variables to address the need to delimit sufficient preconditions for interventions in educational contexts [13].

Table 1.

Potential moderators of the efficacy of self-affirmation interventions.

Moderators Classification or Category
Sample characteristics
Cultural origin African American
Latin American
Ethnic minority
Others
Age range 11 to 14 years old
15 to 17 years old
18 to 23 or older
Economic status Medium
Low
Sample size Large sample (n > 500 students)
Small sample (n < 500 students)
Dominant group in the sample African American, Latin American, or ethnic minority
American or European (Caucasian)
Design characteristics
Execution modality Face-to-face
Virtual
Rigor of randomization Use of software
Does not use software
Dependent variable type Psychological
Physical
Academic performance
Self-affirmation intervention style Classical intervention (based on important values)
No classical intervention
Placebo style Classical activity (based on unimportant values)
No classical activity
Time of the intervention Before stressful academic event
During stressful academic event
After stressful academic event
Study presentation Routine academic activity
Activity for research
Teacher role Active
Passive

2.5. Calculation of Effect Sizes

Effect sizes were calculated according to the statistical indicators of change reported in each study for the variations observed in the experimental and control groups owing to the self-affirmation intervention.

Because all studies used a design with independent groups (experimental and control), it was not necessary to consider a transformation to alternative metrics to guarantee the compatibility of effect size estimates [34], which were calculated according to a standard metric of independent groups (dIG).

2.6. Meta-Analysis Strategy

Calculations were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25.0. A random effects model was selected because of the variability in effect sizes depending on the characteristics of the samples and the methodological design of the studies.

The restricted maximum likelihood method was used to calculate effect sizes. This offers a more conservative estimate of standard errors and is usually more sensitive to small samples [35]. Effect sizes were interpreted according to Cohen’s proposal [36], where d = 0.20 is small effect size, d = 0.50 corresponds to a medium magnitude, and d = 0.80 is considered a large effect.

The Q statistic of homogeneity [37] was used to assess the variability in the effect sizes of the studies. It was used to determine whether there was an unexplained variability in the selected studies [38]. The assumption of homogeneity is rejected when the Q statistic is statistically significant.

One-factor analysis of variance and meta-regression [39] analysis were used to perform meta-regressions to evaluate the 14 possible moderators of the effectiveness of self-affirmation interventions.

3. Results

3.1. Study Selection

The initial search returned 3819 articles. After identifying and removing duplicate studies from the database, the titles and abstracts of 2969 reports were reviewed and 2105 studies were excluded after the initial review. Subsequently, the full texts of 864 articles were reviewed, excluding 751 studies that did not meet the eligibility criteria. Finally, 114 research reports, comprising 144 studies, were included in the analysis. The flow diagram of the study selection process is shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flow diagram of the search strategy and selection of articles and studies.

3.2. Main Characteristics of the Included Studies

A total of 114 articles published between the years 1998 and 2023 were selected. Seventy-seven studies were conducted in the United States, whereas the remainder were conducted in the United Kingdom (n = 14), Canada (n = 6), the Netherlands (n = 5), China (n = 3), Germany, Belgium, Singapore, Spain, France, Australia, India, South Korea, and Turkey. All studies were authored in English. Regarding the samples, 61 studies considered samples with Caucasian participants, 11 with African American participants, and 13 with Latino participants. The predominant age range was 18–23 years (n = 108), followed by the age range of 11–14 years (n = 30) and 15–17 years (n = 5). A total of 36,419 students were included in the 114 studies. The experimental groups comprised 27,563 participants and the control groups (as reported) included 8856 participants. Regarding the dependent variables used to measure the effect of self-affirmation, 98 studies assessed psychological variables, 17 physical variables, and 50 performance-related variables. Table 2 summarizes the study details.

Table 2.

Characteristics and effect sizes of studies included in this meta-analysis.

Study(s) [Ref] Year and Country Sample Characteristics Methodological Characteristics Effect Size
Cultural Origin Age Range NC/NE Variables Results Cohen’s d Variance
Cohen et al. Study 1 [17] 2006, US African Americans and
European Americans
12–13 243 Academic
performance
(+) 0.26 * 0.0166
Cohen et al. Study 2 [17] 2006, US African Americans and
European Americans
12–13 243 Academic
performance
(+) 0.26 * 0.0106
Cohen et al. [8] 2009, US African Americans and
European Americans
12–14 385 Academic
performance
(+) 0.31 * 0.0105
Bowen et al. [7] 2013, US African Americans and ethnic minorities 11–14 74/58 Academic
performance
(+) 0.52 * 0.0318
Thomaes et al. Study 1 [40] 2012, NL Dutch 11–14 85/88 Prosocial feelings (+) 0.47 * 0.0143
Thomaes et al. Study 2 [40] 2012, NL Dutch 11–14 81/82 Prosocial behaviors (+) 0.19 * 0.0156
Thomaes et al. [41] 2009, NL Caucasians 12–15 405 Narcissistic
aggression
(=) 0.10 0.0127
Armitage [42] 2012, GB Caucasians 13–16 105/115 Perceived threat
Self-esteem
Current body shape
Desired body shape
Body satisfaction
Body self-esteem
(−/+) 0.43 * 0.0182
Sherman et al. Study 1 [10] 2013, US Caucasians and Latin Americans 11–14 92/92 Academic
performance
(+) 0.33 * 0.0222
Sherman et al. Study 2 [10] 2013, US Caucasians and Latin Americans 11–14 79/72 Academic
performance
Interpretation level
Daily adversity
(+) 0.52 * 0.0207
Bratter et al. [6] 2016, US African Americans,
Caucasians, and
Hispanics
14–15 456/430 Academic
performance
(=) 0.09 0.0137
Cook et al. Study 1 [9] 2012, US African Americans and Caucasians 12–14 361 Academic affiliation (=) 0.16 0.0110
Cook et al. Study 2 [9] 2012, US African Americans and Caucasians 12–14 121 Academic affiliation and academic
performance
(+) 0.45 * 0.0343
Lokhande and Müller [43] 2019, DE Ethnic minorities 12–13 294/374 Academic
performance
(=) 0.14 0.0366
Binning et al. [44] 2019, US Caucasians and ethnic groups 11–14 145 School trust
Discipline incidents
(−/+) 0.40 * 0.0281
Hoffman and Kurtz-Costes [45] 2019, US American Indians 11–14 212 Motivation for
science
(=) 0.14 0.0189
Liu and Huang [46] 2019, CN Asians 15–16 48/47 Self-integration
Coping with
homework
Academic
performance
Perceived value
(−/+) 0.42 * 0.0423
Harackiewicz et al. [24] 2014, US Caucasians and ethnic
minorities
M = 19.27, SD = 1.15 396/402 Performance gap
Academic
performance
(−/+) 0.22 * 0.0263
Miyake et al. [25] 2010, US 18–22 399 Gender gap
Learning
(=) 0.15 * 0.0216
Taylor and Walton [23] 2011, US African Americans 18–22 29 Learning (+) 0.83 * 0.0138
Baker et al. [47] 2020, US Caucasians and ethnic groups 551/564 Academic
performance
(=) 0.09 0.0036
Bayly and Bumpus [48] 2019, US Ethnic minorities 18–19 107/389 Academic
performance
(=) 0.00 0.0135
Blanton et al. [49] 2013, US M = 18.7, SD = 1.16 116 Discomfort with the threat
Willingness to have unprotected sex
(−/+) 0.43 * 0.0355
Brady et al. [31] 2016, US Latinos 18–20 183 Academic
performance
Adaptive
appropriateness
Academic
belongingness
Spontaneous
affirmation
Fear of school
Optimism
Rumination
Problem analysis
(+) 0.75 * 0.0204
Cameron et al. [50] 2015, GB Caucasians and ethnic groups 16–24 799/696 Consumption of fruits and vegetables
Physical activity
Alcohol
consumption
Tobacco
consumption
Smoking in college
(−/+) 0.12 * 0.0286
Churchill et al. [51] 2018, GB 18–33 32/35 Musical performance (=) 0.00 0.0598
De Clercq et al. [52] 2019, BE 18–19 129/123 Self-affirmation (+) 0.47 * 0.0163
Covarrubias et al. Study 1 [53] 2016, US Latinos 11–14 81 Academic
performance
(+) 0.19 * 0.0106
Covarrubias et al. Study 2 [53] 2016, US Latinos and Americans of European origin 11–14 269 Academic
performance
(+) 0.22 * 0.0113
Ehret and Sherman [54] 2018, US Caucasians and ethnic groups 74/66 Abstinence from
alcohol
(=) 0.00 0.0356
Epton et al. [55] 2014, GB Caucasians and ethnic groups 18–19 709/736 Tobacco use
Drug use
Hospital admissions
Descriptive norms
Perception of control
(−/+) 0.12 * 0.0199
Goyer et al. [56] 2017, US Latinos and Caucasians 11–14 185 Academic
preparation
Attendance to
selective schools
(+) 0.48 * 0.0227
Gregory et al. [57] 2017, US Caucasian 18–22 64 Self-pity
Perception of pain
Resistance to pain
(+) 0.60 * 0.0139
Harackiewicz et al. [58] 2016, US African Americans, Hispanics, and Native Americans 18–19 1040 Academic
performance
Performance gap
(+) 0.28 * 0.0254
Hernandez et al. [59] 2017, US Latinos 11–14 67 Threat to identity (=) 0.00 0.0615
Jones and Huey [60] 2020, US Caucasians, Latinos, and African Americans 18–24 38/44 Academic
performance
Perception of
self-integration
Social adjustment
(=) 0.13 0.0130
Jordt et al. [61] 2017, US Caucasians and ethnic minorities 18–22 970/963 Academic
performance
(+) 0.27 0.0156
Kamboj et al. [62] 2016, GB 18–35 278/250 Pro-social feelings
Alcohol
consumption
Intention to reduce consumption
Message derogation
Perceived threat
Commitment to the threatening message
(−/+) 0.23 * 0.0109
Kim and Niederdeppe [63] 2016, SG Caucasians and Asians 18–34 74/76 Negative cognitive responses
Perceived risk of
alcohol consumption
(=) 0.28 0.0267
Lannin et al. [64] 2013, US European Americans and ethnic groups 19–46 84 Self-stigma
Willingness to seek help
(−/+) 0.24 * 0.0627
Lannin et al. [65] 2020, US European Americans and ethnic groups 18–22 152 Positive mood
Negative mood
Psychological
distress
(−/+) 0.45 * 0.0295
Layous et al. [66] 2017, US Caucasians and ethnic minorities M = 19.12, SD = 1.28 57/48 Academic
performance
(+) 0.39 * 0.0391
Meier et al. [67] 2015, US Caucasians 18–35 52/58 Importance of the problem AR
Risk perception AR
Alcohol use
Protective Strategies AR
(=) 0.16 0.0368
Norman and Wrona-Clarke [68] 2016, GB Caucasians M = 22.58, SD = 6.31 105/104 Reactivity of AR messages
Message
evaluation AR
Perceived Risk AR
Intention to
binge drink
Binge drinking
(=) 0.12 0.0192
Norman et al. [69] 2018, GB Caucasians M = 18.76, SD = 1.94 738 Frequency of
excessive
consumption AR
(+) 0.13 * 0.0054
Peters et al. [70] 2017, US Caucasians and
African Americans
17–59 194 Subjective numerical capacity (=) 0.29 0.0208
Rosas et al. [71] 2017, US Caucasians, Latinos, and ethnic minorities 18–35 143 Self-esteem
Intention to consume sugar-sweetened
beverages
(+) 0.24 * 0.0135
Sereno et al. [72] 2020, US Caucasians, Latinos, and ethnic minorities M = 20.04,
SD = 2.69
157 Self-assessment
Oral participation
(+) 0.45 * 0.0267
Shapiro et al. Study 3 [73] 2013, US African Americans 18–24 37 Academic
performance
(+) 0.19 * 0.0271
Shapiro et al. Study 4 [73] 2013, US African Americans 18–24 75 Academic
performance
(+) 0.57 * 0.0555
Tibbetts et al. Study 1 [74] 2016, US Caucasians and ethnic minorities 18–24 69/72 Academic
performance
(+) 0.23 * 0.0289
Tibbetts et al. Study 2 [74] 2016, US Caucasians and ethnic minorities 18–24 389/399 Performance gap
Choice of
independent topics
Choice of interdependent topics
(+) 0.29 0.0066
Walton et al. [75] 2015, CA Caucasians, Asians, and ethnic minorities 18–24 228 Academic
performance
Importance of
negative events
Confidence in stress management
Self-esteem
Gender
identification
(−/+) 0.60 * 0.0249
Adams et al. Study 1 [76] 2006, US Caucasians, European Americans, and Latinos 18–24 44/51 Perception of racism
Belief that whites understate the extent of racism
Ratings of the
average white person
(+) 0.58 * 0.0442
Adams et al. Study 2 [76] 2006, US Caucasians, European Americans, and Latinos 18–24 27/36 Belief that whites understate the extent of racism (−) 0.67 * 0.0688
Borman et al. [15] 2016, US Caucasians and ethnic minorities 12–13 499/513 Cumulative seventh grade GPA
Fall reading test
Fall math test
Spring reading test
Spring math test
Spring language
usage test
(−/+) 0.05 0.0040
Briñol et al. Study 1 [77] 2007, ES 18–24 111 Manipulation check (index of personal importance) (+) 2.51 * 0.0644
Briñol et al. Study 2 [77] 2007, ES 18–24 73 Manipulation check (index of personal importance) (+) 1.84 * 0.0781
Briñol et al. Study 3 [77] 2007, ES 18–24 87 Attitudes (−) 0.52 * 0.0475
Briñol et al. Study 4 [77] 2007, ES 18–24 91 Confidence (+) 0.63 * 0.0461
Correll et al. [78] 2004, CA Canadians 18–24 21/18 Advocate’s
arguments
Pro-attitudinal
advocate position
Argument strength
(−/+) 1.05 * 0.1182
Creswell et al. [79] 2013, US Caucasians and ethnic minorities 18–34 73 Rating value
Writing activity
RAT score
Positive affect
(+) 1.40 * 0.0582
Critcher et al. Study 1 [11] 2010, US 18–24 184 Defensiveness (−/+) 0.15 0.0218
Critcher et al. Study 2a [11] 2010, US 18–24 76 Defensively negative score (−) 0.57 * 0.0549
Critcher and Dunning Study 1 [29] 2015, US 18–24 75 Positive feelings of self-worth (+) 0.45 * 0.0547
Critcher and Dunning Study 2 [29] 2015, US 18–24 94 Defensiveness
Perspective on the threat
(−/+) 0.44 * 0.0435
Crocker et al. Study 1 [80] 2008, US Caucasians, Asians, and other or mixed
ethnicity
17–21 70/69 Rating of loving
feelings
(+) 0.84 * 0.0319
Crocker et al. Study 2 [80] 2008, US Caucasians, Asians, and other or mixed
ethnicity
17–22 54 Acceptance of the
article
(+) 0.63 * 0.1182
Dillard et al. [81] 2005, US Caucasians 18–24 65/65 Motivated to quit smoking (+) 0.39 * 0.0314
Epton and Harris [82] 2008, GB 18–46 41/46 Portions of fruit and vegetables
Self-efficacy
Response efficacy
(+) 0.46 * 0.0474
Harris and Napper [83] 2005, GB 18–24 42/40 Importance of self-affirmed
passages Self-positivity
Positive attitudes
(+) 5.08 * 0.4354
Harris et al. [84] 2007, GB Caucasians 18–40 43/44 Threat
Intention
Control
Self-efficacy
Negative thoughts and feelings
(+) 0.67 * 0.0486
Klein et al. Study 1 [85] 2011, US 18–24 120 Feelings of
vulnerability
(+) 0.36 * 0.0339
Klein et al. Study 2 [85] 2011, US 18–24 99 Feelings of
vulnerability
Intentions
(−/+) 0.43 * 0.0413
Klein and Harris [86] 2009, US 18–24 118 Attentional bias
toward threat
(+) 0.37 * 0.0345
Koole and van Knippenberg [87] 2007, NL 18–24 88 Stereotypic word
fragments
Stereotypic
descriptions
(−/+) 0.93 * 0.0511
Koole et al. Study 1 [88] 1999, NL 18–24 60 Value of the AVL subscale
Recognition
accuracy
(−/+) 1.49 * 0.0930
Koole et al. Study 2 [88] 1999, NL 18–24 71 Value of the AVL (+) 1.84 * 0.0801
Koole et al. Study 3 [88] 1999, NL 18–24 70 Value of the AVL
Recognition
accuracy
Positive mood
Relative evaluation of name letters
(−/+) 0.99 * 0.0704
Legault et al. [89] 2012, CA 18–24 35 Errors of
commission
Waveform
amplitude
(−/+) 0.82 * 0.1240
Martens et al. Study 2 [22] 2006, US 18–24 52 Items correct on math SATs (+) 0.55 * 0.0799
Reed and Aspinwall [90] 1998, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asian Americans, Biracials, Hispanics, and others 17–54 61 Reduced bias
processing of
threatening health
information
Reading time
risk disconfirming
Number of facts
recalled
Perceived control over reducing caffeine consumption
(−/+) 0.60 * 0.0688
Schimel et al. Study 1 [91] 2004, CA 18–24 49 Self-handicapping attributions
Performance measures
(−/+) 0.57 * 0.085
Schmeichel and Martens Study 1 [92] 2005, US 18–24 65 Percept negative to anti-U.S. essay (−) 0.50 * 0.0634
Schmeichel and Martens Study 2 [92] 2005, US 18–24 54 Death-related thoughts (−) 0.54 * 0.0768
Schmeichel and Vohs Study 1 [93] 2009, US 18–24 63 Positive mood (+) 0.44 * 0.0650
Schmeichel and Vohs Study 2 [93] 2009, US 18–24 72 Puzzle persistence (+) 0.94 * 0.0617
Schmeichel and Vohs Study 3 [93] 2009, US 18–24 29 Behavioral
descriptions
(+) 0.74 * 0.1474
Sherman et al. [94] 2009, US Caucasians, Asians, Americans, other
ethnicities
18–24 49 Concerns about
failure
Worrying during exam
(−) −0.57 * 0.085
Sherman et al. Study 1 [95] 2000, US 18–24 60 Feel better about selves
Point to most
important value
Accepting of threatening
information
Reduced caffeine consumption
(+) 1.14 * 0.0791
Sherman et al. Study 2 [95] 2000, US 18–24 61 Similar risk
Considered risk of HIV
Perceptions of risk
(+) 0.57 * 0.0683
Shrira and Martin Study 1 [96] 2005, US 18–24 101 Use of stereotypes
Left hemisphere
activation
(+) 0.50 * 0.0409
Shrira and Martin Study 2 [96] 2005, US 18–24 180 Left hemisphere
activation
Stereotyping
(−/+) 0.33 * 0.0226
Sivanathan et al. Study 2 [97] 2008, US 18–24 38 Reinvest funds in initially chosen
Commitment to job candidate
(−) 0.94 * 0.1169
Sivanathan et al. Study 3 [97] 2008, US 18–24 55 Commitment to job candidate (−/+) 0.61 * 0.0748
Spencer et al. Study 3 [98] 2001, US 18–24 24 Choice downward comparisons
Choice upward
comparisons
(−/+) 0.89 * 0.1840
Stone et al. Study 1 [99] 2011, US Caucasians, Hispanics, Asians, and African Americans 18–24 179 Desire to meet target race (+) −0.30 * 0.0226
Stone et al. Study 2 [99] 2011, US Caucasians, Hispanics, Asians, and African Americans 18–24 102 Desire to meet target race
Empathy
Guilt
Perceived injustice
Stereotyped views
(+) −0.59 * 0.0413
van Koningsbruggen et al. [100] 2009, NL 18–24 84 Reaction time as a function of
threat-related
perceptions of
message quality
Reduction in caffeine consumption
(+) 0.47 * 0.0489
Vohs et al. Study 1 [101] 2013, US 18–31 52 Disengagement from a life goal (+) 0.47 * 0.0571
Vohs et al. Study 2 [101] 2013, US 18–24 132 Performance
expectations
Dampening effect
Interest in
performing an
additional task
(+) 0.36 * 0.0308
Vohs et al. Study 3 [101] 2013, US 18–24 119 More effort to task
Dampening effect
Effort to
additional task
Effort to attempt RAT problems
(−/+) 0.42 * 0.0344
Vohs et al. Study 4 [101] 2013, US 18–24 56 Negative self-perceptions of intelligence
Self-perceptions of
intelligence
Self-efficacy
perceptions
Performance in the second set of RAT items
(−/+) 0.59 * 0.0746
Wakslak and Trope Study 1 [102] 2009, US 18–24 24 Self-concept clarity (+) 0.92 * 0.1844
Wakslak and Trope Study 2 [102] 2009, US 18–24 45 Preferences for
high-level action
identifications
(+) 0.73 * 0.0948
Zárate and Garza Study 1 [103] 2002, US Mexicans, Anglos,
African Americans, and Asian Americans
18–24 120 Prejudices (−) 0.36 * 0.0339
Zhao et al. [104] 2014, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asians, Hispanics, and other races 18–24 116 Quitting intentions (+) 0.29 * 0.0348
Sillero-Rejon et al. [105] 2018, GB 18–24 64/64 Avoidance
Reactance
Susceptibility
Effectiveness
Motivation to
drink less
Self-efficacy to
drink less
(=) 0.18 0.0313
Pauketat et al. Study 1 [106] 2016, US 18–24 61 Affective forecasts
Appraisal of negative event as less
disturbing
(+) 1.02 * 0.0741
Pauketat et al. Study 2 [106] 2016, US 18–24 47 Affective forecasts
Appraisal of negative event as less
disturbing
(+) .71 * 0.0905
Gu et al. [107] 2019, CN 18–24 48 Feedback-related negativity (+) 0.82 * 0.0903
Taillandier-Schmitt et al. [108] 2012, FR 18–43 40/55 Performance scores
Temporal
performance scores
(+) 0.42 * 0.0441
Hanselman et al. [32] 2017, US African Americans and Hispanics 12–14 166/165 Academic
performance
(+) 0.24 * 0.0122
Borman et al. [109] 2018, US Caucasians and
African Americans
12–14 920 GPA (+) 0.25 * 0.0044
Borman et al. [110] 2021, US Caucasians and
racial/ethnic groups
11–17 473/479 GPA (+) 0.01 0.0042
Serra-Garcia et al. [111] 2020, US 18–24 283 Exam scores (−) −0.24 * 0.0142
Hayes et al. Study 1 [112] 2019, US Latinos, African
Americans, and Asian
Americans
10–13 116 Overall semester grade (=) 0.00 0.0345
Hayes et al. Study 2 [112] 2019, US Caucasians, Latinos, and Africans Americans 18–22 273 GPA (=) 0.00 0.0147
Hadden et al. [113] 2020, GB 11–14 562 Academic
performance
Levels of stress
(−/+) 0.21 * 0.0071
Scott et al. [114] 2013, AU 18–24 67/54 Intentions to reduce alcohol consumption (+) 0.37 * 0.0340
Perry et al. [115] 2021, US Caucasians and
African Americans
20–43 416 Perceived residency competitiveness (+) 0.20 * 0.0097
Dee [116] 2015, US Caucasians, African Americans, and
Hispanics
12–14 885 Grade in treated subject (−/+) 0.30 * 0.0046
Protzko and Aronson [117] 2016, US Caucasians, Hispanics and Africans
Americans
13–15 243 Overall GPA (=) 0.06 0.0165
Knight and Norman [118] 2016, GB Caucasians 18–24 307 Self-affirmation
manipulation
(+) 0.38 * 0.0133
Kim et al. [119] 2022, US Hispanics and African Americans 9–10 29/37 Affect/emotion in
relation to academic environments or tasks
(=) 0.27 0.0617
More et al. [120] 2022, US Caucasians and ethnic minorities 18–28 125/129 Fear and defensive processing
Exercise intentions
(=) 0.14 0.0158
Smith et al. [121] 2021, US Caucasians and ethnic minorities 18–24 361 Task engagement (+) 0.52 * 0.0115
Kim et al. Study 1 [122] 2022, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asians, Hispanics, and others M = 27.9 1277 GPA (+) 0.07 * 0.0031
Pandey et al. [123] 2021, IN Indians 22–27 40/40 Well-being (+) 0.94 * 0.0709
Celeste et al. [124] 2021, GB Afro-descendants and Caucasians 11–13 43/42 Cognitive
performance
(=) 0.05 0.0476
Li et al. [125] 2022, CA, CN Asians and Caucasians M = 18.13, SD = 1.65 159/137 Psychological
well-being
(=) 0.02 0.0136
Borman et al. [126] 2022, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asians,
Latins
12–14 2149 Suspensions (−) −0.28 * 0.0019
Binning et al. [127] 2021, US Caucasians, African American, Latins, and Asian American 11–14 145 GPA (+) 0.45 * 0.0283
Pilot and Stutts [128] 2023, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asians, Hispanics, and others. 18–22 238 Body dissatisfaction
Negative mood state
(=) 0.06 0.0168
Strachan et al. [129] 2020, CA Caucasians and Asians 18–58 120 Exercise task
self-efficacy
(+) 0.17 * 0.0339
Hagerman et al. [130] 2020, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asians,
Latinos
M = 19.35, SD = 1.61 167 Negative modo
Absent-exempt
Perceived skin
damage
Perceived
vulnerability
Intentions to protect skin
(=) 0.10 0.0241
Dutcher et al. [131] 2020, US M = 19.3; SD = 1.35 27 Stressful
Academic
performance
(−/+) 1.24 * 0.1770
Shin et al. Study 2 [132] 2020, KR M = 21.04, SD = 2.07 75 Acceptance of the threatening
information
(−) 0.89 * 0.0586
Huppin and Malamuth [133] 2022, US Asian American,
European American, Hispanic American,
African American, and others
18–24 70 Affirmative consent
Conceptualization of consent
Knowledge and awareness
Rape beliefs
Fairness of the
outcome
(−/+) 0.61 * 0.0598
Çetinkaya et al. Study 1 [134] 2020, TR M = 21.88, SD = 1.34 60 Task performance (+) 0.84 * 0.0725
Poon et al. Study 4 [135] 2020, CN M = 20.78, SD = 1.70 178 Conspiracy beliefs (=) 0.83 0.0225
Turetsky et al. [136] 2020, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asians, Hispanics, and others 18–44 108/118 Closeness centrality
Degree centrality
Maintaining existing friendships
Forming new
friendships
(+) 0.35 * 0.0193
Rapa et al. [137] 2020, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asians, Hispanics, and others M = 14.97 28/25 GPA (+) 0.54 0.0785
Bosch [138] 2020, US Caucasians, African Americans, Asians, Hispanics, and others 18–24 221/246 Academic
performance
(=) 0.03 0.0086

Abbreviations: Number of subjects in the control group (NC); number of subjects in the experimental group (NE); mean associated with age (M); standard deviation associated with age (SD). Variable abbreviations: Related to alcohol intake (AR); Unadjusted grade-point-average (GPA); Remote Associates Test, a well-known measure of problem solving and creativity (RAT); Allport–Vernon–Lindzey (AVL); Force and Motion Concept Evaluation (FMCE). Abbreviations: Indicator of improvement in the dependent variable (+), mixed results with respect to the change in the dependent variable (−/+), absence of statistically significant changes in the dependent variable (=). Country abbreviations from https://www.nationsonline.org/oneworld/country_code_list.htm (accessed on 10 February 2023): Australia (AU), Belgium (BE), Canada (CA), China (CN), France (FR), Germany (DE), India (IN), Singapore (SG), South Korea (KR), Spain (ES), The Netherlands (NL), Turkey (TR), United Kingdom (GB), United States of America (US). * = p < 0.05.

3.3. Assessment of Methodological Quality

Overall, the majority of studies exhibited a high methodological quality across all evaluated dimensions. The representativeness of the individuals selected in the studies was observed to be very strong. Additionally, most studies presented a moderate design quality and were categorized as analytical cohort studies (two pre + post groups). In terms of controlling for confounding factors, the majority of studies addressed at least 80% of the relevant factors (strong). Seventeen studies [23,41,45,46,49,50,51,55,57,59,61,68,109,121,124,128,134] indicated that evaluators and/or participants were aware of the research objectives (weak). Similarly, most studies employed validated and reliable data collection tools (strong). Regarding participant withdrawals and dropouts, the majority of studies described these aspects with moderate follow-up. These findings suggest a notable consistency in the methodological quality of the reviewed studies (See Figure 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Summary of methodological quality of selected articles.

3.4. Magnitude of Effectiveness

Specifically, our analysis of 144 studies with a total sample of 36.419 participants revealed a small and statistically significant effect size (dIG+ = 0.41, z = 16.01, p < 0.00), indicating that self-affirmation interventions can be effective in diverse sociocultural and educational contexts. The 95% confidence interval, which ranged from 0.36 to 0.45 (SE = 0.0253), supports the robustness of our results. Overall, the results suggest that self-affirmation interventions have the potential to produce positive changes in individuals’ attitudes and behaviors.

Homogeneity analyses revealed statistically significant variability or heterogeneity in the effect sizes. This suggests that the efficacy of self-affirmation interventions may be influenced by a variety of factors such as variations in study design, sample characteristics, and other methodological differences (Q (143) = 563.0122, p < 0.05, v = 0.06).

3.5. Moderators of Effectiveness

We explored several moderators associated with the sample characteristics and research design of different studies and found a large number of variables that influenced the variability of self-affirmation interventions. To assess and compare the percentages of variance accounted for by each identified moderator, we employed the coefficient of determination (R2) to ascertain the capacity of each moderator to explain the observed variability in effect sizes among the incorporated studies. These percentages provide the relative magnitudes of the distinct moderators’ influence (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Moderator variables and their impact on self-affirmation interventions.

Moderator R2 β Effect
Sample characteristics
Cultural origin
  “Other” (Caucasian, European, or Asian) 7.07% β = −0.21, z = −3.87, p < 0.001 (−)
Age Ranges (ARs)
  11–14 years old (AR1) 5.66% β = −0.22, z = −3.45, p < 0.001 (−)
  18–23 years old (AR3) 7.42% β = 0.24, z = 3.98, p < 0.001 (+)
Sample Size
  Small sample (n > 500 students) 7.72% β = 0.28, z = 4.04, p < 0.001 (+)
  Large sample (n < 500 students) 7.72% β = −0.28, z = −4.04, p < 0.001 (−)
Dominant Group in the Sample
  American, European, or Asian (Caucasian) 6.22% β = −0.20, z = −3.59, p < 0.001 (−)
  African American, Latino, or ethnic minority 7.07% β = −0.18, z = −2.25, p < 0.05 (−)
Design characteristics
Execution modality
  Face-to-face 5.26% β = 0.26, z = 3.28, p < 0.05 (+)
  Virtual 2.29% β = −0.13, z = −2.14, p < 0.05 (−)
Rigor of Randomization
  Use of software 4.33% β = −0.25, z = −2.97, p < 0.05 (−)
  Does not use software 3.52% β = 0.19, z = 2.66, p < 0.05 (+)
Dependent Variables
  Psychological (DV1) 12.08% β = 0.28, z = 5.19, p < 0.001 (+)
  Academic performance (DV3) 6.29% β = −0.20, z = 3.59, p < 0.001 (−)
Self-affirmation intervention style
  Classical intervention (based on important values) 7.87% β = −0.27, z = −4.06, p < 0.001 (−)
Timing of Intervention
  Before stressful academic event 7.79% β = −0.22, z = −4.04, p < 0.001 (−)
  During stressful academic events 3.70% β = −0.23, z = −2.81, p < 0.05 (−)
Teacher role
  Active 4.63% β = −0.20, z = −3.10, p < 0.05 (−)

Abbreviations of the results: Indicator of improvement in the dependent variable (+), indicator of decrease in the dependent variable (−).

The analysis revealed that, in interventions that target the Caucasian population (other), the impact of self-affirmation is likely to decrease by 7.07%. Further, the effects of studies applying self-affirmation to students aged 11–14 years (AR1) were 5.66% lower, whereas the effects of studies focusing on students aged 18–23 years (AR3) were 7.42% higher. Studies with small sample sizes (<500) showed a positive effect on the effectiveness of self-affirmation, whereas those with large sample sizes (>500) displayed a negative effect. This discrepancy suggests that, as the sample size increased, effect sizes tended to decrease by 7.72%. Concerning intervention group characteristics, in studies with a predominance of Caucasian participants (American, European, or Asian) and studies with predominantly ethnic minority students (African Americans, Latinos, etc.), the effects of self-affirmation decreased by 6.22% and 7.07%, respectively. In both cases, regardless of the participation of specific racial and ethnic groups, there was a decrease in the effectiveness of self-affirmation, indicating that ethnic and demographic characteristics may influence how these interventions affect outcomes. Finally, economic status had no significant influence on the effectiveness of self-affirmation interventions.

Regarding the moderators associated with research design characteristics, our results indicate that face-to-face interventions are more effective than virtual interventions, as the former can explain 5.26% of the data variability, underscoring the relevance of considering intervention designs. In addition, in studies that did not use software for random participant assignment, the effects of self-affirmation decreased by 4.33%, whereas in those that included such software, effects increased by 3.52%.

Concerning dependent variables, the results suggest that studies that focused on inducing changes in variables related to mental health (DV1) showed increased effects of self-affirmation by 12.08%, whereas studies aimed at improving academic performance (DV3) showed lower and negative effects (6.29%). These findings imply that self-affirmation interventions can have different effects depending on the outcome variable. Further, studies that included value writing as an intervention method found a negative impact on the effectiveness of self-affirmation interventions, explaining 7.87% of the variance. Typically, value writing as an intervention method is expected to have a positive impact on effect size; however, our results indicate otherwise. No significant differences in effect sizes were found in the studies that used a placebo activity in the control group.

Regarding the timing of self-affirmation interventions, regardless of whether the intervention was applied before or during potentially stressful events, the effects of the intervention were negative (7.79% and 3.70%, respectively). This suggests that interventions applied after such stressful events demonstrated a stability in effect sizes. Further, in studies in which teachers or educational assistants participated in the intervention, the effectiveness of self-affirmation decreased significantly by 4.63%. In other words, the presence of teachers or educational assistants appeared to be associated with a reduced positive impact of interventions. Finally, the way the studies were presented to students—whether as a routine academic activity or by informing them of the study’s objectives—did not significantly influence the variance in the effects of self-affirmation interventions.

3.6. Publication Biases

The results of this study showed that the findings were resistant to publication bias, as estimated by the Fail-Safe Number test for random effects meta-analyses [139]. The analysis suggested that 23.178 similar studies with null effect sizes would be necessary to nullify the results obtained in this study (see Table 4). However, the precision-effect test and precision-effect estimate with standard errors (PET-PEESE) meta-regression method was used following Staley and Doucouliagos’s [140] guidelines to further examine potential publication bias. The results demonstrated a presence of symmetry indices in the publication bias indicators, as the intercept of the PET estimate was not statistically significant, indicating that the constant in the PEESE regression could not adequately estimate the effect size values. Therefore, under these statistical parameters, the presence of publication bias in the study can be assumed (Table 5).

Table 4.

Fail-Safe Number test for a meta-analysis with random effects [139].

Fail Safe Number
Rosenthal (0.050) 43.646
Rosenberg Normal 32.963
Rosenberg t-N1/t-N+ 22.790/23.178 (three iterations)

Table 5.

PET-PEESE meta-regression indicators based on the Staley and Doucouliagos [140] approach to reduce publication selection bias.

Sample PET PEESE
β0 β1 β0 β1
Full −0.03 (−0.11, 0.05) 2.57 ** 0.14 ** (0.09, 0.18) 7.96 **

For PET-PEESE, β0: Constant or intercept in the meta-regression; β1: Coefficient of standard error in PET meta-regression and variance in PEESE. Values in parentheses correspond to the lower and upper limits of the 95% confidence intervals. ** = p-value < 0.01.

4. Discussion

This meta-analysis examined the efficacy of self-affirmation interventions in various educational contexts, considering demographic and methodological differences between the selected studies. A random effects model was used, and publication bias was assessed following Schmidt’s recommendations [141].

The results indicate that self-affirmation interventions are effective in improving academic performance, interpersonal performance, and aspects of physical and psychological well-being in academic contexts, obtaining an average effect size that is small but sufficient in view of the methodological differences between studies and the existence of moderators that manage to explain part of the variance. This suggests that the efficacy of self-affirmation interventions may vary across studies and in some cases a minimal or meager average effect size is observed, with the analysis of all moderators that may explain it being considerably important [32,33,142].

It is important to note that the literature recognizes predictable patterns that can explain the achievement of heterogeneous effects, such as temporal conditions of implementation, methodological procedures, social and cultural characteristics of the context, and individual differences of the subjects [32]. The findings of studies carried out in the last five years whose results show inconsistent, minimal, or even null effects of self-affirmation interventions present common explanatory assumptions related to individual differences between participants (coping with identity threat), the social and cultural context in which they operate, and differences in methodological procedures the between studies based on the provision of sufficient and necessary conditions for optimal fieldwork, taking as a reference the study by Cohen et al. [17], where the characteristics of the sample in terms of age of participants and size and degree of heterogeneity are usually critical points of disagreement between studies, in line with the results of the present meta-analysis [46,47,54,112,119,120,124,125,128,130].

In their meta-analysis, Wu et al. [143] found a significant moderation in the effect of a self-affirmation intervention by the achievement gap between threatened and nonthreatened students, duration of the study, presentation of the intervention as a classroom activity, and use of materials attached to those originally proposed. The present study complements these findings by adding, among others, the cultural background of the participants, their age ranges, the sample size, the characteristics of the dominant group in the sample, the modality of application of the intervention, and dependent variables such as mental health and academic performance. In addition, this meta-analysis included 144 independent studies, whereas the previous one included 58, which gives significant breadth to the scope of our conclusions. This is enhanced when considering that Wu et al. [143] used academic performance exclusively as the outcome variable, which was expanded in this work by considering additional outcome variables such as health and psychosocial variables. This represents an important contribution to the delimitation of interventions with a view toward enhancing their effects in different areas, which coincides with what has been proposed by other reviews, highlighting the investigation of moderating elements [32,33].

Regarding the moderators associated with sample characteristics, the effect of self-affirmation was weaker when the studies targeted students who were not threatened by stereotypes (Caucasian, European, or Asian). It appears plausible that students who did not encounter stigmatization or additional challenges in academic settings did not derive significant benefits from the interventions. A greater effect of these interventions on academic performance, interpersonal performance, and aspects of physical and psychological well-being is likely to occur when the perception of identity threat is greater [7,9,10,17,42,144,145,146,147,148,149]. These findings corroborate the results of previous studies that observed a reduced or null effect of self-affirmation among nonthreatened students [10,46,53,56,115].

The benefit of self-affirmation was greater when studies targeted individuals between the ages of 18 and 23, while it was lower in participants between the ages of 11 and 14. These differences in the effectiveness of self-affirmation may be associated with the stages of the students’ cognitive and socioemotional development. Young adults tend to have higher self-awareness and a greater ability to reflect on their values and beliefs, which could make the intervention more effective for them than for adolescents [150].

However, the results suggested that, as the sample size increased, the effect sizes of the interventions tended to decrease. In this regard, other studies suggest that the effects of self-affirmation could be more beneficial in social or cultural minorities and in small groups of students whose collective identity is underrepresented and who also experience learning difficulties, compared with the dominant group of students [20,32,151]. Therefore, it is plausible that threatened groups were included in studies with a small sample size (n < 500), which could potentially have influenced the effectiveness of the interventions. Further research is required to fully understand the interactions between these factors.

Similarly, the effect of self-affirmation was weaker in studies in which the sample was dominated by a specific racial group (e.g., Caucasians, African Americans, and Latinos). Recent research suggests that certain characteristics of social and cultural contexts, such as the size, composition, and distribution of groups, can become important sources of variability. For example, Bratter, Rowley, and Chukhray [6] found that identity threat in students of African American and Latin American background varied under certain group distribution conditions in educational settings. Although students of color belong to a socially and culturally marginalized group in the United States, they tend not to feel threatened when they are in the majority, rendering self-affirmation interventions ineffective. These findings suggest that studies with greater cultural diversity among participants may yield more consistent results regarding the effectiveness of interventions a predominant cultural or social group [44].

Regarding moderators related to research design, the results suggest that the effect of self-affirmation was greater when interventions were conducted face-to-face than virtual interventions. This finding aligns with the notion that face-to-face interactions can enhance emotional engagement and personal connections among participants, potentially amplifying the impact of self-affirmation interventions [152]. However, it is important to note that the effectiveness of virtual interventions can be influenced by various factors such as the quality of online platforms and participants’ levels of engagement. Future research should delve deeper into the mechanisms underlying this difference in effect sizes between intervention modalities to provide a more comprehensive understanding of their respective benefits and limitations.

However, the effects of self-affirmation interventions were greater when the studies did not use computer programs for random participant allocation. To some extent, these results contradict natural expectations. In general, one could assume that the use of rigorous randomization techniques would lead to more consistent and predictable intervention outcomes. However, there is also the possibility that the positive effects observed in these studies reflect a bias in sample selection, namely, a conscious or unconscious researcher bias during the process of assigning participants to the study groups [153]. These findings call for deeper and more thorough analysis to understand the potential reasons for this unexpected relationship.

The results indicate that self-affirmation had a more significant impact in studies that investigated its effect on dependent variables linked to mental health but had a less pronounced effect in studies that focused on measuring dependent variables related to academic performance. These results could be related to the very nature of self-affirmation, as it tends to strengthen self-esteem and a sense of personal worth—aspects that could positively influence individuals’ mental health [33,79]. Conversely, self-affirmation may not have a direct or immediate impact on academic skills or student performance. This could be because academic performance is influenced by a range of more complex factors, such as intrinsic motivation, socioeconomic status, the parents’ education level, and the educational environment [154,155].

Similarly, the benefits of self-affirmation diminish when studies include value writing as an intervention method for the experimental group. These results run counter to the expectations set by the literature, given that the application of alternative self-affirmation activities in the experimental groups should have generated these variations in effects, not the opposite [13,80,91,97,142,156,157]. This discrepancy between the expected and actual outcomes underscores the complexity of the factors that can influence intervention efficacy and highlights the importance of meticulously examining how different methods can impact results in specific contexts.

Similarly, the effect of self-affirmation diminished when interventions were administered before or during stressful academic events. These findings contradicted the literature, as some studies had found that the effects of self-affirmation tended to be more favorable when interventions are presented before stressful academic experiences associated with tasks or exams [9,11,151]. Each study had its particularities in terms of design, participants, and context, which could explain discrepancies in the findings. The way individuals perceive and cope with stress can also vary widely, which, in turn, could affect how they respond to self-affirmation during moments of academic stress.

The effect of self-affirmation was reduced when teachers or educational assistants participated in the intervention. These findings also differed from those previously made in the literature, which had shown that teacher support before and during academic activities can enhance the impact of self-affirmation [30,33]. The discrepancy between these findings suggests that the presence of teachers or educational assistants in the intervention may have had an unintended effect on students’ self-affirmation experiences. The dynamics of the interactions between teachers and students during the intervention may have played a crucial role in this effect. For instance, if teachers provided excessive direction or a rigid structure in the implementation of self-affirmation, students may have perceived the activity as less personal and authentic. This could have diminished the impact of self-affirmation interventions.

Some studies indicated that individual differences associated with the psychological impact of identity threats may be influenced by each subject’s personality traits. However, there is limited scientific evidence regarding this issue. It is important to recognize the relationships between individuals’ personalities and the way they perceive and evaluate themselves [158]. This evidence contributes to seriously considering personality features in the study of the effects of self-affirmation interventions, especially when mixed results are observed.

Other findings suggest that self-stigma for belonging to certain social or cultural groups, motivation and openness to improve or change, fear of academic failure, or even the relevance or meaning attributed to self-affirmation exercises may become essential moderators of the effectiveness of these interventions [33,101,159,160].

Taking into account all of the above, some guidelines for future studies whose interest is in investigating the effect of self-affirmation interventions in educational contexts are the choice of, ideally, small groups and culturally and socially diverse educational settings with different structures and systems of group organization.

The above sheds light on how identity threats are posed according to the size, composition, and distribution of the subjects, which few studies have ascertained. In the case of studies with large samples, it is suggested that an intervention proposal be implemented that includes not only self-affirmation as a strategy, but also others based on motivational factors. Further, pre- and post-intervention measures of identity threat should be incorporated in the groups to pre-identify individuals with higher levels of threat.

Future research should also explore the moderating effects of individual differences between subjects in the face of identity threats (e.g., personality traits or coping styles). Likewise, it would be a great contribution for future studies to explore what types of technical and methodological procedures are the most appropriate in terms of (1) the articulation of groups (experimental and control), (2) the characteristics of the self-affirmation or placebo activities, (3) the most suitable moments to execute the intervention, (4) the impact of the participation of members of staff (teachers, counselors, classroom assistants, etc.) before, during, and after the execution of the interventions, and finally, (5) the moderators associated with the contextual aspects already mentioned in previous paragraphs.

Regarding the limitations of this study, it is important to highlight that the results obtained show some evidence of publication bias. It is important to note that publication bias in a meta-analysis reveals a likelihood that the included studies are partially representative of the totality of studies conducted, but in the field of self-affirmation, the main reason for publication bias is the existence of more publications of studies reporting favorable results compared to those reporting unfavorable results. This phenomenon tends to occur because a study reporting evidence of the efficacy of self-affirmation interventions is more likely to be accepted for publication by a journal and therefore more likely to be cited and appear in databases when conducting the literature search required for a meta-analysis.

The publication bias of this meta-analysis highlights a problem facing the field of self-affirmation research, and these results may pave the way for enriching changes to make more room for the publication of studies reporting unfavorable results regarding the effectiveness of these interventions in an educational context.

Another limitation of this study is the generality of these interventions and moderators’ effect size estimates. This is because the calculation of effect size considered different types of outcome variables (psychosocial, academic, and health) to provide a broader view of the effectiveness of these interventions in educational contexts, despite the methodological differences between the studies, and to obtain results consistent with the objective proposed in the present meta-analysis. This also significantly enriches the decision-making of future researchers, whose interest is not limited to studying the effects of these interventions on students’ academic performance but also their impact on psychosocial or health aspects.

5. Conclusions

The results of the present meta-analysis suggest that self-affirmation interventions are effective and yield a small average effect size. Practically, these findings suggest that self-affirmation interventions may be useful tools in educational settings, particularly in terms of academic performance and student well-being. It is noteworthy that, owing to its low cost, this can be of particular benefit to schools in a risk context. This could be implemented through self-managed interventions designed by the educational institutions themselves and carried out by their own staff in accordance with established procedures to maximize their impact and enhance the resources of their students. From a theoretical perspective, according to our findings and the previous literature, it is important to emphasize the role of moderators in intervention effectiveness. The evidence suggests that with good delimitation and control for moderators, such as the age of participants, the intervention procedure, and the presence or absence of identity threats, the effectiveness of self-affirmation interventions could be maximized, which would have important implications at both the individual and educational system levels. Further research is needed to identify the most effective methods and moderators to optimize the benefits of self-affirmation interventions. In addition, specific analyses of the effectiveness of interventions considering individual independent variables would contribute, which is a limitation of the present study.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.E.-S. and A.C.-U.; methodology, C.E.-S., R.B. and G.P.-D.; formal analysis, C.E.-S., R.B. and G.P.-D.; investigation, C.E.-S., G.P.-D. and C.M.-R.; data curation, C.E.-S., G.P.-D. and C.M.-R.; writing—original draft preparation, C.E.-S., G.P.-D., C.M.-R. and F.P.-C.; writing—review and editing, C.E.-S., G.P.-D., C.M.-R. and F.P.-C.; supervision, C.E.-S., J.F. and A.C.-U. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This research study was approved in 23 January 2020 by the Scientific Ethical Committee of the Universidad de Tarapacá, code C98-20, whose resolution accredits that all data collection materials, methodological procedures, and confidentiality criteria comply with current regulations according to Law 20.120 of the Organic Regulations of the Ministry of Health of Chile and under the protocols of the Helsinki Convention of 1964 on scientific research on human beings.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable for studies not involving humans.

Data Availability Statement

All data are contained in the article tables.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Funding Statement

This research was funded by Agencia Nacional de Investigación y Desarrollo de Chile (ANID) through the Programa de Investigación Asociativa PIA-CIE 160007; Agencia Nacional de Investigación y Desarrollo de Chile (ANID) through the Programa Nacional de Becas de Doctorado ANID 2020 N° Folio 21200169 and Programa de Apoyo a la Investigación de Tesis para Programas Académicos de Postgrado de la Universidad de Tarapacá (Código DIPTT 8773-19).

Footnotes

Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

References

  • 1.Garcia T., Pintrich P.R. Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance: Issues and Educational Applications. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.; Hillsdale, NJ, USA: 1994. Regulating motivation and cognition in the classroom: The role of self-schemas and self-regulatory strategies; pp. 127–153. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fordham S., Ogbu J.U. Black students’ school success: Coping with the “burden of acting White”. Urban Rev. 1986;18:176–206. doi: 10.1007/BF01112192. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Steele C.M. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 21: Social Psychological Studies of the Self: Perspectives and Programs. Academic Press; San Diego, CA, USA: 1988. The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self; pp. 261–302. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Major B., Spencer S., Schmader T., Wolfe C., Crocker J. Coping with negative stereotypes about intellectual performance: The role of psychological disengagement. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1998;24:34–50. doi: 10.1177/0146167298241003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Aronson J., Fried C.B., Good C. Reducing the Effects of Stereotype Threat on African American College Students by Shaping Theories of Intelligence. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2002;38:113–125. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2001.1491. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bratter J.L., Rowley K.J., Chukhray I. Does a self-affirmation intervention reduce stereotype threat in black and hispanic high schools? Race Soc. Probl. 2016;8:340–356. doi: 10.1007/s12552-016-9187-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bowen N.K., Wegmann K.M., Webber K.C. Enhancing a brief writing intervention to combat stereotype threat among middle-school students. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013;105:427–435. doi: 10.1037/a0031177. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cohen G.L., Garcia J., Purdie-Vaughns V., Apfel N., Brzustoski P. Recursive Processes in Self-Affirmation: Intervening to Close the Minority Achievement Gap. Science. 2009;324:400–403. doi: 10.1126/science.1170769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cook J.E., Purdie-Vaughns V., Garcia J., Cohen G.L. Chronic threat and contingent belonging: Protective benefits of values affirmation on identity development. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012;102:479–496. doi: 10.1037/a0026312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Sherman D.K., Hartson K.A., Binning K.R., Purdie-Vaughns V., Garcia J., Taborsky-Barba S., Tomassetti S., Nussbaum A.D., Cohen G.L. Deflecting the trajectory and changing the narrative: How self-affirmation affects academic performance and motivation under identity threat. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2013;104:591–618. doi: 10.1037/a0031495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Critcher C.R., Dunning D., Armor D.A. When self-affirmations reduce defensiveness: Timing is key. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2010;36:947–959. doi: 10.1177/0146167210369557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cohen J. Measuring and improving school climate: A pro-social strategy that recognizes, educates, and supports the whole child and the whole school community. In: Brown P.M., Corrigian M.W., Higgins D.A., editors. The Handbook of Prosocial Education. Rowman & Littlefield; Lanham, MD, USA: 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shnabel N., Purdie-Vaughns V., Cook J.E., Garcia J., Cohen G.L. Demystifying values-affirmation interventions: Writing about social belonging is a key to buffering against identity threat. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2013;39:663–676. doi: 10.1177/0146167213480816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Yeager D.S., Walton G.M. Social-Psychological Interventions in Education: They’re Not Magic. Rev. Educ. Res. 2011;81:267–301. doi: 10.3102/0034654311405999. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Borman G.D., Pyne J. What If Coleman Had Known About Stereotype Threat? How Social-Psychological Theory Can Help Mitigate Educational Inequality. RSF Russell Sage Found. J. Soc. Sci. 2016;2:164–185. doi: 10.7758/RSF.2016.2.5.08. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Borman G.D. Advancing values affirmation as a scalable strategy for mitigating identity threats and narrowing national achievement gaps. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:7486–7488. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1708813114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Cohen G.L., Garcia J., Apfel N., Master A. Reducing the racial achievement gap: A social-psychological intervention. Science. 2006;313:1307–1310. doi: 10.1126/science.1128317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Inzlicht M., Schmader T. Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application. Oxford University Press; Oxford, UK: 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Steele C.M., Spencer S.J., Aronson J. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Volume 34. Academic Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2002. Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat; pp. 379–440. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hanselman P., Bruch S.K., Gamoran A., Borman G.D. Threat in context: School moderation of the impact of social identity threat on racial/ethnic achievement gaps. Sociol. Educ. 2014;87:106–124. doi: 10.1177/0038040714525970. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Schmader T., Johns M., Forbes C. An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychol. Rev. 2008;115:336–356. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Martens A., Johns M., Greenberg J., Schimel J. Combating stereotype threat: The effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2006;42:236–243. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.04.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Taylor V.J., Walton G.M. Stereotype threat undermines academic learning. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2011;37:1055–1067. doi: 10.1177/0146167211406506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Harackiewicz J.M., Tibbetts Y., Canning E., Hyde J.S. Harnessing Values to Promote Motivation in Education. Adv. Motiv. Achiev. 2014;18:71–105. doi: 10.1108/s0749-742320140000018002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Miyake A., Kost-Smith L.E., Finkelstein N.D., Pollock S.J., Cohen G.L., Ito T.A. Reducing the Gender Achievement Gap in College Science: A Classroom Study of Values Affirmation. Science. 2010;330:1234–1237. doi: 10.1126/science.1195996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Woolf B.P., Arroyo I., Muldner K., Burleson W., Cooper D.G., Dolan R., Christopherson R.M. Intelligent Tutoring Systems. Springer; Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: 2010. The Effect of Motivational Learning Companions on Low Achieving Students and Students with Disabilities; pp. 327–337. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Woolf K., McManus I.C., Gill D., Dacre J. The effect of a brief social intervention on the examination results of UK medical students: A cluster randomised controlled trial. BMC Med. Educ. 2009;9:35. doi: 10.1186/1472-6920-9-35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Harackiewicz J.M., Priniski S.J. Improving Student Outcomes in Higher Education: The Science of Targeted Intervention. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2018;69:409–435. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Critcher C.R., Dunning D. Self-affirmations provide a broader perspective on self-threat. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2015;41:3–18. doi: 10.1177/0146167214554956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Purdie-Vaughns V., Cohen G.L., García J.A., Sumner R., Cook J., Apfel N.H. Improving Minority Academic Performance: How a Values-Affirmation Intervention Works. Teachers College Record 2010. [(accessed on 16 March 2023)]. Available online: https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:157797471.
  • 31.Brady S.T., Reeves S.L., Garcia J., Purdie-Vaughns V., Cook J.E., Taborsky-Barba S., Tomasetti S., Davis E.M., Cohen G.L. The psychology of the affirmed learner: Spontaneous self-affirmation in the face of stress. J. Educ. Psychol. 2016;108:353–373. doi: 10.1037/edu0000091. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hanselman P., Rozek C.S., Grigg J., Borman G.D. New Evidence on Self-Affirmation Effects and Theorized Sources of Heterogeneity from Large-Scale Replications. J. Educ. Psychol. 2017;109:405–424. doi: 10.1037/edu0000141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cohen G.L., Sherman D.K. The psychology of change: Self-affirmation and social psychological intervention. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2014;65:333–371. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Morris S.B., DeShon R.P. Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychol. Methods. 2002;7:105–125. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Raudenbush S.W. The Handbook of Research Synthesis. Russell Sage Foundation; New York, NY, USA: 1994. Random effects models; pp. 301–321. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 1992;112:155–159. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Cochran W.G. The Combination of Estimates from Different Experiments. Biometrics. 1954;10:101–129. doi: 10.2307/3001666. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Shadish W.R., Haddock C.K. The Handbook of Research Synthesis. Russell Sage Foundation; New York, NY, USA: 1994. Combining estimates of effect size; pp. 261–281. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wilson D.B. Wilson’s Meta-Analysis Page. [(accessed on 16 February 2023)]. Available online: http://mason.gmu.edu/~dwilsonb/
  • 40.Thomaes S., Bushman B.J., de Castro B.O., Reijntjes A. Arousing “gentle passions” in young adolescents: Sustained experimental effects of value affirmations on prosocial feelings and behaviors. Dev. Psychol. 2012;48:103–110. doi: 10.1037/a0025677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Thomaes S., Bushman B.J., Orobio de Castro B., Cohen G.L., Denissen J.J. Reducing narcissistic aggression by buttressing self-esteem: An experimental field study. Psychol. Sci. 2009;20:1536–1542. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02478.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Armitage C.J. Evidence that self-affirmation reduces body dissatisfaction by basing self-esteem on domains other than body weight and shape. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry. 2012;53:81–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02442.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lokhande M., Müller T. Double jeopardy—Double remedy? The effectiveness of self-affirmation for improving doubly disadvantaged students’ mathematical performance. J. Sch. Psychol. 2019;75:58–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2019.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Binning K.R., Cook J.E., Purdie-Greenaway V., Garcia J., Chen S., Apfel N., Sherman D.K., Cohen G.L. Bolstering trust and reducing discipline incidents at a diverse middle school: How self-affirmation affects behavioral conduct during the transition to adolescence. J. Sch. Psychol. 2019;75:74–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2019.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hoffman A.J., Kurtz-Costes B. Promoting science motivation in American Indian middle school students: An intervention. Educ. Psychol. 2019;39:448–469. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2018.1527019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Liu C.-H., Huang P.-S. Beneficial effects of self-affirmation on motivation and performance reduced in students hungry for others’ approval. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2019;56:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.10.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Baker D.J., Skinner B.T., Redding C.H. Affirmative Intervention to Reduce Stereotype Threat Bias: Experimental Evidence from a Community College. J. High. Educ. 2020;91:722–754. doi: 10.1080/00221546.2019.1650582. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bayly B.L., Bumpus M.F. An exploration of engagement and effectiveness of an online values affirmation. Educ. Res. Eval. 2019;25:248–269. doi: 10.1080/13803611.2020.1717542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Blanton H., Gerrard M., McClive-Reed K.P. Threading the needle in health-risk communication: Increasing vulnerability salience while promoting self-worth. J. Health Commun. 2013;18:1279–1292. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2013.778359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Cameron D., Epton T., Norman P., Sheeran P., Harris P.R., Webb T.L., Julious S.A., Brennan A., Thomas C., Petroczi A., et al. A theory-based online health behaviour intervention for new university students (U@Uni:LifeGuide): Results from a repeat randomized controlled trial. Trials. 2015;16:555. doi: 10.1186/s13063-015-1092-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Churchill S., Jessop D.C., Goodwin S., Ritchie L., Harris P.R. Self-affirmation improves music performance among performers high on the impulsivity dimension of sensation seeking. Psychol. Music. 2018;46:292–302. doi: 10.1177/0305735617705007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.De Clercq M., Michel C., Remy S., Galand B. Providing freshmen with a good “starting-block”: Two brief social-psychological interventions to promote early adjustment to the first year at university. Swiss J. Psychol. 2019;78:69–75. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000217. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Covarrubias R., Herrmann S.D., Fryberg S.A. Affirming the interdependent self: Implications for Latino student performance. Basic. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2016;38:47–57. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2015.1129609. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Ehret P.J., Sherman D.K. Integrating Self-Affirmation and Implementation Intentions: Effects on College Student Drinking. Ann. Behav. Med. 2018;52:633–644. doi: 10.1093/abm/kax032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Epton T., Norman P., Dadzie A.S., Harris P.R., Webb T.L., Sheeran P., Julious S.A., Ciravegna F., Brennan A., Meier P.S., et al. A theory-based online health behaviour intervention for new university students (U@Uni): Results from a randomised controlled trial. BMC Public Health. 2014;14:563. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Goyer J.P., Garcia J., Purdie-Vaughns V., Binning K.R., Cook J.E., Reeves S.L., Apfel N., Taborsky-Barba S., Sherman D.K., Cohen G.L. Self-affirmation facilitates minority middle schoolers’ progress along college trajectories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:7594–7599. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1617923114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gregory W.E., Glazer J.V., Berenson K.R. Self-compassion, self-injury, and pain. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2017;41:777–786. doi: 10.1007/s10608-017-9846-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Harackiewicz J.M., Canning E.A., Tibbetts Y., Priniski S.J., Hyde J.S. Closing achievement gaps with a utility-value intervention: Disentangling race and social class. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2016;111:745–765. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000075. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Hernandez D., Rana S., Rao A., Usselman M. Dismantling Stereotypes About Latinos in STEM. Hisp. J. Behav. Sci. 2017;39:436–451. doi: 10.1177/0739986317731100. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Jones E.O., Huey S.J. Affirmation and Majority Students: Does Affirmation Impair Academic Performance in White Males? Basic. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2020;42:176–191. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2020.1732389. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Jordt H., Eddy S.L., Brazil R., Lau I., Mann C., Brownell S.E., King K., Freeman S. Values Affirmation Intervention Reduces Achievement Gap between Underrepresented Minority and White Students in Introductory Biology Classes. CBE Life Sci. Educ. 2017;16:ar41. doi: 10.1187/cbe.16-12-0351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Kamboj S.K., Place H., Barton J.A., Linke S., Curran H.V., Harris P.R. Processing of Alcohol-Related Health Threat in At-Risk Drinkers: An Online Study of Gender-Related Self-Affirmation Effects. Alcohol. Alcohol. 2016;51:756–762. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agw013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kim H.K., Niederdeppe J. Effects of Self-Affirmation, Narratives, and Informational Messages in Reducing Unrealistic Optimism About Alcohol-Related Problems Among College Students. Human. Commun. Res. 2016;42:246–268. doi: 10.1111/hcre.12073. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Lannin D.G., Guyll M., Vogel D.L., Madon S. Reducing the stigma associated with seeking psychotherapy through self-affirmation. J. Couns. Psychol. 2013;60:508–519. doi: 10.1037/a0033789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Lannin D.G., Vogel D.L., Kahn J.H., Brenner R.E., Heath P.J., Guyll M. A multi-wave test of self-affirmation versus emotionally expressive writing. Couns. Psychol. Q. 2020;33:333–351. doi: 10.1080/09515070.2018.1553144. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Layous K., Davis E.M., Garcia J., Purdie-Vaughns V., Cook J.E., Cohen G.L. Feeling left out, but affirmed: Protecting against the negative effects of low belonging in college. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2017;69:227–231. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.09.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Meier E., Miller M.B., Lechner W.V., Lombardi N., Claborn K.R., Leffingwell T.R. The Inability of Self-affirmations to Decrease Defensive Bias Toward an Alcohol-Related Risk Message Among High-Risk College Students. J. Am. Coll. Health. 2015;63:324–329. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2014.1003377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Norman P., Wrona-Clarke A. Combining self-affirmation and implementation intentions to reduce heavy episodic drinking in university students. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2016;30:434–441. doi: 10.1037/adb0000144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Norman P., Cameron D., Epton T., Webb T.L., Harris P.R., Millings A., Sheeran P. A randomized controlled trial of a brief online intervention to reduce alcohol consumption in new university students: Combining self-affirmation, theory of planned behaviour messages, and implementation intentions. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2018;23:108–127. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Peters E., Shoots-Reinhard B., Tompkins M.K., Schley D., Meilleur L., Sinayev A., Tusler M., Wagner L., Crocker J. Improving numeracy through values affirmation enhances decision and STEM outcomes. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0180674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0180674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Rosas C.E., Gregorio-Pascual P., Driver R., Martinez A., Price S.L., Lopez C., Mahler H.I.M. Effects of Social Norms Information and Self-Affirmation on Sugar-Sweetened Beverage Consumption Intentions and Behaviors. Basic. Appl. Soc. Psych. 2017;39:112–126. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2017.1283503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Sereno K., Walter N., Brooks J.J. Rethinking student participation in the college classroom: Can commitment and self-affirmation enhance oral participation? J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2020;50:351–362. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12664. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Shapiro J.R., Williams A.M., Hambarchyan M. Are all interventions created equal? A multi-threat approach to tailoring stereotype threat interventions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2013;104:277–288. doi: 10.1037/a0030461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Tibbetts Y., Harackiewicz J.M., Canning E.A., Boston J.S., Priniski S.J., Hyde J.S. Affirming independence: Exploring mechanisms underlying a values affirmation intervention for first-generation students. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2016;110:635–659. doi: 10.1037/pspa0000049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Walton G.M., Logel C., Peach J.M., Spencer S.J., Zanna M.P. Two brief interventions to mitigate a “chilly climate” transform women’s experience, relationships, and achievement in engineering. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015;107:468–485. doi: 10.1037/a0037461. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Adams G., Tormala T.T., O’Brien L.T. The effect of self-affirmation on perception of racism. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2006;42:616–626. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.11.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Briñol P., Petty R.E., Gallardo I., DeMarree K.G. The effect of self-affirmation in nonthreatening persuasion domains: Timing affects the process. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2007;33:1533–1546. doi: 10.1177/0146167207306282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Correll J., Spencer S.J., Zanna M.P. An affirmed self and an open mind: Self-affirmation and sensitivity to argument strength. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2004;40:350–356. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.07.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Creswell J.D., Dutcher J.M., Klein W.M.P., Harris P.R., Levine J.M. Self-Affirmation Improves Problem-Solving under Stress. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e62593. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Crocker J., Niiya Y., Mischkowski D. Why does writing about important values reduce defensiveness? Self-affirmation and the role of positive other-directed feelings. Psychol. Sci. 2008;19:740–747. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02150.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Dillard A.J., McCaul K.D., Magnan R.E. Why Is Such a Smart Person Like You Smoking? Using Self-Affirmation to Reduce Defensiveness to Cigarette Warning Labels. J. Appl. Biobehav. Res. 2005;10:165–182. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2005.tb00010.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Epton T., Harris P.R. Self-affirmation promotes health behavior change. Health Psychol. 2008;27:746–752. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.6.746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Harris P.R., Napper L. Self-affirmation and the biased processing of threatening health-risk information. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2005;31:1250–1263. doi: 10.1177/0146167205274694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Harris P.R., Mayle K., Mabbott L., Napper L. Self-affirmation reduces smokers’ defensiveness to graphic on-pack cigarette warning labels. Health Psychol. 2007;26:437–446. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.4.437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Klein W.M.P., Harris P.R., Ferrer R.A., Zajac L.E. Feelings of vulnerability in response to threatening messages: Effects of self-affirmation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2011;47:1237–1242. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Klein W.M., Harris P.R. Self-affirmation enhances attentional bias toward threatening components of a persuasive message. Psychol. Sci. 2009;20:1463–1467. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02467.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Koole S.L., van Knippenberg A. Controlling your mind without ironic consequences: Self-affirmation eliminates rebound effects after thought suppression. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2007;43:671–677. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.07.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Koole S.L., Smeets K., van Knippenberg A., Dijksterhuis A. The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999;77:111–125. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Legault L., Al-Khindi T., Inzlicht M. Preserving integrity in the face of performance threat: Self-affirmation enhances neurophysiological responsiveness to errors. Psychol. Sci. 2012;23:1455–1460. doi: 10.1177/0956797612448483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Reed M.B., Aspinwall L.G. Self-affirmation reduces biased processing of health-risk information. Motiv. Emot. 1998;22:99–132. doi: 10.1023/A:1021463221281. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Schimel J., Arndt J., Banko K.M., Cook A. Not all self- affirmations were created equal: The cognitive and social benefit of affirming the intrinsic (vs extrinsic) self. Soc. Cogn. 2004;22:75–99. doi: 10.1521/soco.22.1.75.30984. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Schmeichel B.J., Martens A. Self-affirmation and mortality salience: Affirming values reduces worldview defense and death-thought accessibility. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2005;31:658–667. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Schmeichel B.J., Vohs K. Self-affirmation and self-control: Affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2009;96:770–782. doi: 10.1037/a0014635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Sherman D.K., Bunyan D.P., Creswell J.D., Jaremka L.M. Psychological vulnerability and stress: The effects of self-affirmation on sympathetic nervous system responses to naturalistic stressors. Health Psychol. 2009;28:554–562. doi: 10.1037/a0014663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Sherman D.A.K., Nelson L.D., Steele C.M. Do messages about health risks threaten the self? Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2000;26:1046–1058. doi: 10.1177/01461672002611003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Shrira I., Martin L.L. Stereotyping, Self-Affirmation, and the Cerebral Hemispheres. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2005;31:846–856. doi: 10.1177/0146167204272232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Sivanathan N., Molden D.C., Galinsky A.D., Ku G. The promise and peril of self-affirmation in de-escalation of commitment. Organ. Behav. Human. Decis. Process. 2008;107:1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.12.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Spencer S.J., Fein S., Lomore C.D. Maintaining one’s self-image vis-à-vis others: The role of self-affirmation in the social evaluation of the self. Motiv. Emot. 2001;25:41–65. doi: 10.1023/A:1010659805978. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Stone J., Whitehead J., Schmader T., Focella E. Thanks for asking: Self-affirming questions reduce backlash when stigmatized targets confront prejudice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2011;47:589–598. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.12.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.van Koningsbruggen G.M., Das E., Roskos-Ewoldsen D.R. How self-affirmation reduces defensive processing of threatening health information: Evidence at the implicit level. Health Psychol. 2009;28:563–568. doi: 10.1037/a0015610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Vohs K.D., Park J.K., Schmeichel B.J. Self-affirmation can enable goal disengagement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2013;104:14–27. doi: 10.1037/a0030478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Wakslak C.J., Trope Y. Cognitive consequences of affirming the self: The relationship between self-affirmation and object construal. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2009;45:927–932. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Zárate M.A., Garza A.A. In-Group Distinctiveness and Self-Affirmation as Dual Components of Prejudice Reduction. Self Identity. 2002;1:235–249. doi: 10.1080/152988602760124865. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Zhao X., Peterson E.B., Kim W., Rolfe-Redding J. Effects of Self-Affirmation on Daily Versus Occasional Smokers’ Responses to Graphic Warning Labels. Commun. Res. 2014;41:1137–1158. doi: 10.1177/0093650212465433. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Sillero-Rejon C., Attwood A.S., Blackwell A.K.M., Ibáñez-Zapata J.-A., Munafò M.R., Maynard O.M. Alcohol pictorial health warning labels: The impact of self-affirmation and health warning severity. BMC Public Health. 2018;18:1403. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-6243-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Pauketat J.V.T., Moons W.G., Chen J.M., Mackie D.M., Sherman D.K. Self-affirmation and affective forecasting: Affirmation reduces the anticipated impact of negative events. Motiv. Emot. 2016;40:750–759. doi: 10.1007/s11031-016-9562-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Gu R., Yang J., Yang Z., Huang Z., Wu M., Cai H. Self-affirmation enhances the processing of uncertainty: An event-related potential study. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2019;19:327–337. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-00673-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Taillandier-Schmitt A., Esnard C., Mokounkolo R. Self-affirmation in occupational training: Effects on the math performance of French women nurses under stereotype threat. Sex. Roles A J. Res. 2012;67:43–57. doi: 10.1007/s11199-012-0157-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Borman G.D., Grigg J., Rozek C.S., Hanselman P., Dewey N.A. Self-affirmation effects are produced by school context, student engagement with the intervention, and time: Lessons from a district-wide implementation. Psychol. Sci. 2018;29:1773–1784. doi: 10.1177/0956797618784016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Borman G.D., Choi Y., Hall G.J. The impacts of a brief middle-school self-affirmation intervention help propel African American and Latino students through high school. J. Educ. Psychol. 2021;113:605–620. doi: 10.1037/edu0000570. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Serra-Garcia M., Hansen K.T., Gneezy U. Can Short Psychological Interventions Affect Educational Performance? Revisiting the Effect of Self-Affirmation Interventions. Psychol. Sci. 2020;31:865–872. doi: 10.1177/0956797620923587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Hayes L.J., Zinner L.R., Wise J.C., Carton J.S. Effects of a Self-Affirmation Intervention on Grades in Middle School and First-Year College Students. [(accessed on 16 March 2023)];J. Artic. Support. Null. Hypothesis. 2019 16:58–79. Available online: https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:211142201. [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Hadden I.R., Easterbrook M.J., Nieuwenhuis M., Fox K.J., Dolan P. Self-affirmation reduces the socioeconomic attainment gap in schools in England. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2020;90:517–536. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Scott J.L., Brown A.C., Phair J.K., Westland J.N., Schüz B. Self-affirmation, intentions and alcohol consumption in students: A randomized exploratory trial. Alcohol. Alcohol. 2013;48:458–463. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agt027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Perry S.P., Wages J.E., 3rd, Skinner-Dorkenoo A.L., Burke S.E., Hardeman R.R., Phelan S.M. Testing a Self-Affirmation Intervention for Improving the Psychosocial Health of Black and White Medical Students in the US. J. Soc. Issues. 2021;77:769–800. doi: 10.1111/josi.12448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Dee T.S. Social Identity and Achievement Gaps: Evidence From an Affirmation Intervention. J. Res. Educ. Eff. 2015;8:149–168. doi: 10.1080/19345747.2014.906009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Protzko J., Aronson J. Context Moderates Affirmation Effects on the Ethnic Achievement Gap. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2016;7:500–507. doi: 10.1177/1948550616646426. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Knight R., Norman P. Impact of brief self-affirmation manipulations on university students’ reactions to risk information about binge drinking. Br. J. Health Psychol. 2016;21:570–583. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Kim J.S., Kehle T.J., Bray M.A., Trudel S.M., Fitzmaurice B., Bray A., Del Campo M., DeMaio E. Using self-affirmations to improve achievement in fourth-grade students. Curr. Psychol. 2022;42:15388–15402. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-02697-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.More K.R., Phillips L.A., Green Z., Mentzou A. Examining self-affirmation as a tactic for recruiting inactive women into exercise interventions. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being. 2022;14:294–310. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Smith E.N., Rozek C.S., Manke K.J., Dweck C.S., Walton G.M. Teacher- versus researcher-provided affirmation effects on students’ task engagement and positive perceptions of teachers. J. Soc. Issues. 2021;77:751–768. doi: 10.1111/josi.12417. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Kim J.Y., Brockner J., Block C.J. Tailoring the intervention to the self: Congruence between self-affirmation and self-construal mitigates the gender gap in quantitative performance. Organ. Behav. Human. Decis. Process. 2022;169:104118. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2022.104118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Pandey R., Tiwari G.K., Rai P.K. Restoring and preserving capacity of self-affirmation for well-being in Indian adults with non-clinical depressive tendencies. Curr. Issues Personal. Psychol. 2021;9:135–147. doi: 10.5114/cipp.2021.105260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Celeste L., Baysu G., Phalet K., Brown R. Self-affirmation and test performance in ethnically diverse schools: A new dual-identity affirmation intervention. J. Soc. Issues. 2021;77:824–850. doi: 10.1111/josi.12418. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Li G., Archer Lee Y., Krampitz E., Lin X., Atilla G., Nguyen K.C., Rosen H.R., Tham C.Z.E., Chen F.S. Computer passwords as a timely booster for writing-based psychological interventions. Internet Interv. 2022;30:100572. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2022.100572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Borman G.D., Pyne J., Rozek C.S., Schmidt A. A Replicable Identity-Based Intervention Reduces the Black-White Suspension Gap at Scale. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2022;59:284–314. doi: 10.3102/00028312211042251. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Binning K.R., Cook J.E., Greenaway V.P., Garcia J., Apfel N., Sherman D.K., Cohen G.L. Securing self-integrity over time: Self-affirmation disrupts a negative cycle between psychological threat and academic performance. J. Soc. Issues. 2021;77:801–823. doi: 10.1111/josi.12461. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Pilot I.G., Stutts L.A. The impact of a values affirmation intervention on body dissatisfaction and negative mood in women exposed to fitspiration. Body Image. 2023;44:36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2022.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Strachan S.M., Myre M., Berry T.R., Ceccarelli L.A., Semenchuk B.N., Miller C. Self-affirmation and physical activity messages. Psychol. Sport. Exerc. 2020;47:101613. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2019.101613. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Hagerman C.J., Stock M.L., Molloy B.K., Beekman J.B., Klein W.M.P., Butler N. Combining a UV photo intervention with self-affirmation or self-compassion exercises: Implications for skin protection. J. Behav. Med. 2020;43:743–753. doi: 10.1007/s10865-019-00104-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Dutcher J.M., Eisenberger N.I., Woo H., Klein W.M.P., Harris P.R., Levine J.M., Creswell J.D. Neural mechanisms of self-affirmation’s stress buffering effects. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2020;15:1086–1096. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Shin M., Kim Y., Park S. Effect of Psychological Distance on Intention in Self-Affirmation Theory. Psychol. Rep. 2020;123:2101–2124. doi: 10.1177/0033294119856547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Huppin M., Malamuth N.M. Priming Self-Affirmation Reduces the Negative Impact of High Rape Myth Acceptance: Assessing Women’s Perceptions and Judgments of Sexual Assault. J. Interpers. Violence. 2022;37:NP3728–NP3749. doi: 10.1177/0886260520951312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Çetinkaya E., Herrmann S.D., Kisbu-Sakarya Y. Adapting the values affirmation intervention to a multi-stereotype threat framework for female students in STEM. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 2020;23:1587–1607. doi: 10.1007/s11218-020-09594-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Poon K.T., Chen Z., Wong W.Y. Beliefs in Conspiracy Theories Following Ostracism. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2020;46:1234–1246. doi: 10.1177/0146167219898944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Turetsky K.M., Purdie-Greenaway V., Cook J.E., Curley J.P., Cohen G.L. A psychological intervention strengthens students’ peer social networks and promotes persistence in STEM. Sci. Adv. 2020;6:eaba9221. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aba9221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Rapa L.J., Diemer M.A., Roseth C.J. Can a values-affirmation intervention bolster academic achievement and raise critical consciousness? Results from a small-scale field experiment. Soc. Psychol. Educ. An. Int. J. 2020;23:537–557. doi: 10.1007/s11218-020-09546-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Bosch L. Application of Values Affirmation Intervention in Undergraduate Pre-Calculus Courses. The University of Arizona; Tucson, AZ, USA: 2020. [(accessed on 16 March 2023)]. Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/10150/641359. [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Rosenberg M.S. The file-drawer problem revisited: A general weighted method for calculating fail-safe numbers in meta-analysis. Evolution. 2005;59:464–468. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Stanley T.D., Doucouliagos H. Meta-regression approximations to reduce publication selection bias. Res. Synth. Methods. 2014;5:60–78. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.1095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Schmidt F. Detecting and Correcting the Lies That Data Tell. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2010;5:233–242. doi: 10.1177/1745691610369339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.McQueen A., Klein W.M.P. Experimental manipulations of self-affirmation: A systematic review. Self Identity. 2006;5:289–354. doi: 10.1080/15298860600805325. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Wu Z., Spreckelsen T.F., Cohen G.L. A meta-analysis of the effect of values affirmation on academic achievement. J. Soc. Issues. 2021;77:702–750. doi: 10.1111/josi.12415. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Eagly A.H., Kite M.E. Are stereotypes of nationalities applied to both women and men? J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1987;53:451–462. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.451. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Purdie-Vaughns V., Eibach R.P. Intersectional invisibility: The distinctive advantages and disadvantages of multiple subordinate-group identities. Sex. Roles A J. Res. 2008;59:377–391. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9424-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Sidanius J., Pratto F. Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY, USA: 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Dasgupta N., Scircle M.M., Hunsinger M. Female peers in small work groups enhance women’s motivation, verbal participation, and career aspirations in engineering. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:4988–4993. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422822112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Inzlicht M., Ben-Zeev T. A threatening intellectual environment: Why females are susceptible to experiencing problem-solving deficits in the presence of males. Psychol. Sci. 2000;11:365–371. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Murphy M.C., Steele C.M., Gross J.J. Signaling threat: How situational cues affect women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychol. Sci. 2007;18:879–885. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01995.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Bonnie R.J., Stroud C., Breiner H. Investing in the Health and Well-Being of Young Adults. National Academies Press; Washington, DC, USA: 2014. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Cohen G.L., Garcia J. Educational Theory, Practice, and Policy and the Wisdom of Social Psychology. Policy Insights Behav. Brain Sci. 2014;1:13–20. doi: 10.1177/2372732214551559. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Ransom A., LaGrant B., Spiteri A., Kushnir T., Anderson A.K., De Rosa E. Face-to-face learning enhances the social transmission of information. PLoS ONE. 2022;17:e0264250. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0264250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Pannucci C.J., Wilkins E.G. Identifying and avoiding bias in research. Plast. Reconstr. Surg. 2010;126:619–625. doi: 10.1097/PRS.0b013e3181de24bc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Masud S., Mufarrih S.H., Qureshi N.Q., Khan F., Khan S., Khan M.N. Academic Performance in Adolescent Students: The Role of Parenting Styles and Socio-Demographic Factors—A Cross Sectional Study From Peshawar, Pakistan. Front. Psychol. 2019;10:2497. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Amrai K., Motlagh S.E., Zalani H.A., Parhon H. The relationship between academic motivation and academic achievement students. Procedia—Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011;15:399–402. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.03.111. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Burson A., Crocker J., Mischkowski D. Two types of value-affirmation: Implications for self-control following social exclusion. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2012;3:510–516. doi: 10.1177/1948550611427773. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Blanton H., Cooper J., Slkurnik I., Aronson J. When Bad Things Happen to Good Feedback: Exacerbating the Need for Self-Justification with Self-Affirmations. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1997;23:684–692. doi: 10.1177/0146167297237002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Simkin H., Etchezahar E., Ungaretti J. Personalidad y autoestima desde el modelo y la teoría de los cinco factores. 2012. [(accessed on 16 March 2023)]. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/264348268_Personalidad_y_Autoestima_desde_el_modelo_y_la_teoria_de_los_Cinco_Factores.
  • 159.Aronson J., Lustina M.J., Good C., Keough K., Steele C.M., Brown J. When White men can’t do math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype threat. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1999;35:29–46. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1371. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Shapiro J.R., Neuberg S.L. From stereotype threat to stereotype threats: Implications of a multi-threat framework for causes, moderators, mediators, consequences, and interventions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2007;11:107–130. doi: 10.1177/1088868306294790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All data are contained in the article tables.


Articles from Healthcare are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES