Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2025 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Sleep Res. 2023 Jul 12;33(1):e13994. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13994

ADHD, Chronotype, and Circadian Preference in a Multi-Site Sample of College Students

Stephen P Becker 1,2, Aaron M Luebbe 3, Michael J Kofler 4, G Leonard Burns 5, Matthew A Jarrett 6
PMCID: PMC10784417  NIHMSID: NIHMS1940078  PMID: 37437906

Abstract

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and an evening chronotype are both common among college students, and there is growing interest in understanding the possible link between ADHD and circadian function. However, mixed findings have been reported and many of the existing studies have used small samples that were unable to examine chronotype across ADHD presentations. Participants were 4,751 students (73% female; 80% White) ages 18-29 years (M=19.28, SD=1.50) from five universities who completed measures assessing ADHD, depressive, and anxiety symptoms, as well as the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire to assess chronotype (categorical) and circadian preference (dimensional). Participants with either ADHD predominantly inattentive presentation or ADHD combined presentation had higher rates of being an evening type (47.2% and 41.5%, respectively) than participants without elevated ADHD (28.5%), and participants with ADHD predominantly inattentive presentation also had higher rates of being an evening type than participants with ADHD predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentation (30.7%). Dimensional analyses indicated that ADHD inattentive symptoms were more strongly associated than hyperactive-impulsive symptoms with eveningness preference. Finally, greater eveningness preference strengthened the association between ADHD inattention and depressive symptoms but not anxiety symptoms. This is the largest study to document that college students with elevated ADHD symptoms are more likely to be evening types than other college students, and inattentive symptoms in particular are associated with later circadian preference.

Keywords: attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, chronotype, circadian, inattention, MEQ, morningness-eveningness


Mounting evidence indicates that attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is associated with circadian phase delay involving a later biological clock (Becker, 2020; Bondopadhyay et al., 2022; Coogan & McGowan, 2017). It has more recently been suggested that circadian function may be part of the etiology and pathophysiology of ADHD symptoms for at least a subset of individuals with ADHD (Bijlenga et al., 2019).

A systematic review of ADHD and circadian function concluded that ADHD was most consistently associated with a later chronotype/evening circadian preference (Coogan & McGowan, 2017). Case-control studies have shown a greater preference for eveningness in individuals with ADHD compared to individuals without ADHD (Baird et al., 2012; Bijlenga, van der Heijden, et al., 2013; Bijlenga, Van Someren, et al., 2013; Gruber et al., 2012). However, in two of the studies the sample sizes were limited to 26 children (Gruber et al., 2012) or 13 adults (Baird et al., 2012) with ADHD. Another study included 12 adults with ADHD and confirmed delayed sleep phase syndrome, which indicates the ADHD group by definition had circadian dysfunction (Bijlenga, Van Someren, et al., 2013).

Other studies have used dimensional assessment of ADHD symptoms and circadian preference and generally found ADHD symptoms to be associated with greater eveningness preference (Caci et al., 2009; Martinez-Cayuelas et al., 2022; Rybak et al., 2007; Voinescu et al., 2012). However, several other studies have not found ADHD symptom severity or ADHD status to be significantly associated with circadian preference (Becker et al., 2020; Tarakçioğlu et al., 2018; Van der Heijden et al., 2018). The mixed findings may be due at least in part to small sample sizes, as numerous studies had fewer than 100 participants.

Fewer studies have examined whether any link between ADHD and chronotype varies based on ADHD presentation (i.e., predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive, combined) or symptom dimension (i.e., inattention, hyperactivity-impulsivity). Two studies that each included college students and adults recruited from the community found ADHD inattentive but not hyperactive-impulsive symptoms to be associated with eveningness (Caci et al., 2009; Voinescu et al., 2012). In contrast, Bron et al. (2016) found a composite of total ADHD symptoms, but neither inattentive nor hyperactive-impulsive dimensions individually, to be associated with evening chronotype above and beyond internalizing symptoms and other covariates. Further complicating the picture, Sivertsen et al. (2015) found adolescents with delayed sleep phase had more symptoms of both inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity than adolescents without delayed sleep phase. Finally, a recent study found college students with elevated ADHD symptoms were more likely to be evening types (59%) than college students without ADHD symptoms (44%), though the sample with ADHD symptoms was too small to examine chronotype across separate ADHD presentations (Gabay et al., 2022).

In addition to the need for additional studies examining ADHD presentation and symptoms in relation to circadian preference, it is important to evaluate whether circadian preference is associated with functional outcomes commonly experienced by individuals with ADHD. Approximately 60% of adults with ADHD have a co-occurring internalizing disorder (Miller et al., 2007), and internalizing symptoms are themselves strongly linked to circadian function (Coles et al., 2015; Salgado-Delgado et al., 2011). ADHD inattentive symptoms are especially linked to internalizing symptoms (Willcutt et al., 2012), and so it is possible that the joint presence of ADHD inattentive symptoms and eveningness preference are related to higher internalizing symptoms.

This study had three objectives. First, we used a large, multi-site study of college students to examine the frequency of elevated ADHD symptoms and chronotype and their overlap. Our sample size was sufficiently large to also examine ADHD presentations separately. Second, we examined whether ADHD inattentive and/or hyperactive-impulsive symptoms were uniquely associated with chronotype and circadian preference above and beyond demographic characteristics and internalizing symptoms. Third, as both ADHD inattentive symptoms and eveningness preference are associated with internalizing symptoms, we explored whether circadian preference interacted with ADHD inattentive symptoms in predicting internalizing symptoms. By using a large, multi-site sample of college students, we were able to examine these questions in a population that experiences particularly high rates of ADHD symptoms, evening chronotype, and internalizing symptoms.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 4,751 undergraduate students enrolled at five universities in several regions of the United States. Participant ages ranged from 18 to 29 (M = 19.28, SD = 1.50). As summarized in Table 1, the majority of participants self-identified as female (73%), White (80%), and non-Hispanic (88%). Approximately half (51%) were in their first year of college. Most participants (87%) indicated that they do not typically take any medications for attentional, learning, or behavioral difficulties.

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics of Study Participants (N = 4,751)

Variable % (n)
Sexa
   Male 27.1% (1,289)
   Female 72.7% (3,455)
   Other 0.1% (7)
Year of Study
   Freshman 51.3% (2,435)
   Sophomore 23.7% (1,124)
   Junior 15.1% (717)
   Senior 9.7% (462)
   Other 0.3% (13)
Racea
   White 79.9% (3,782)
   Black 7.0% (330)
   Asian 6.7% (318)
   American Indian/Alaska Native 0.5% (24)
   Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander 0.7% (34)
   Biracial/Multiracial 5.2% (248)
Ethnicitya
   Non-Hispanic 88.2% (4,184)
   Hispanic 11.8% (559)
Psychiatric Medication Use
   No 86.5% (4,110)
   Yes 13.5% (639)

Note.

a

Fifteen participants did not complete the race question, 8 did not complete the ethnicity question, and two did not complete the medication use question.

Procedure

Data were collected during the 2018-2019 academic year. The study was approved by each university’s local Institutional Review Board (IRB). Students enrolled in introductory psychology (general education) courses were able to participate in a research study (or studies) to fulfill a course requirement (or, alternatively, to complete an alternative assignment such as a brief paper). Students were able to choose from a number of studies and could choose to participate in this study if they were ≥18 years old (there were no other inclusion/exclusion criteria). For this study, four of the five universities administered an online Qualtrics survey with measures of the study included, while students at the fifth university selected available time slots to provide in-person consent and then complete the same survey online on their own time. Following completion of the survey, participants were granted course credit. Debriefing information was generated at the end of the survey. All participants received the contact information of the local investigator, IRB, and student counseling center.

Measures

Circadian preference.

The Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) (Horne & Östberg, 1976) is a self-report instrument consisting of 19 items that assesses individual differences in morningness and eveningness (e.g., “what time would you get up if you were entirely free to plan your day?”). Item scores are used to calculate the degree to which a person identifies as a morning or evening type person, where higher scores indicate a greater preference for morningness. In addition to a continuous score of circadian preference, and consistent with previous research (Horne & Östberg, 1976), participants were categorized into evening types (scores 16-41), intermediate types (scores 42-58), and morning types (scores 59-86). In the present study, α = .77.

ADHD symptoms.

The Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV) (Barkley, 2011) was used to assess self-reported ADHD symptoms. The BAARS-IV includes ADHD scales assessing ADHD inattention (9 items; e.g., “I have difficulty organizing tasks and activities”) and ADHD hyperactivity-impulsivity (9 items; e.g., “I fidget with hands or feet or squirm in seat”). Participants indicate on a four-point scale (0 = not at all, 3 = very often) how often each statement describes their behavior over the past six months. These subscales have well supported internal consistency and test-retest reliability in adult samples (Barkley, 2012), including among college students (Becker et al., 2018). In addition to continuous scores of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity, participants were categorized as having elevated ADHD symptoms if they endorsed five or more symptoms as occurring “often” or “very often” in the inattention and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity domain. This symptom count cutoff was selected based on DSM-5 criteria for individuals ≥17 years old (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). This resulted in participants being classified as having no elevated ADHD symptoms or elevations in the predominantly inattentive presentation, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentation, or combined presentation. In the present study, αs = .89 and .82 for inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity, respectively.

Depressive and anxiety symptoms.

The depression and anxiety subscales of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-21 (DASS) (Antony et al., 1998; Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) were used to assess adult self-reported depressive and anxiety symptoms. The DASS-21 has demonstrated strong reliability estimates in clinical and nonclinical populations (Antony et al., 1998). Participants responded to each item on the depression (7 items; e.g., “I was unable to become enthusiastic about anything”) and anxiety subscale (7 items; e.g., “I felt scared without any good reason”) in reference to the past week using a four-point scale (0 = did not apply to me at all, 3 = applied to me very much or most of the time). In the present study, αs = .90 and .79 for depression and anxiety, respectively.

Data Quality Check

An instructional manipulation check (IMC), trap questions, and questions measuring participants’ effort were used throughout the survey to improve quality of responses. The IMC question instructs the participant to select a designated answer to a question prior to proceeding the remainder of the survey. In addition to the IMC, trap questions were periodically included in the study which specified the answer for the participant to click. Finally, one question at the end of the survey asked participants to answer how much effort they put into the entire study. A threshold of 50% accuracy or higher for the trap questions and a self-reported effort rating of 5 or higher on a 0 to 10 scale (0 = not much effort, 10 = my best effort) was designated. The same threshold has been used in previous college student samples (Becker et al., 2018; Holdaway et al., 2018), and is intended to identify participants who put forth sufficient effort while not excluding participants with occasional attentional lapses. Of the 5,053 participants who completed the survey, 297 (6%) did not meet the threshold criteria for the trap questions and were removed from data analyses. The remaining 4,756 (94%) participants met criteria and were eligible for inclusion in the current study. Five participants did not complete sufficient items to calculate a MEQ score, resulting in a final sample size of 4,751.

Data Analyses

First, rates of chronotype (evening, intermediate, morning) and ADHD presentation (none, ADHD-I, ADHD-HI, ADHD-C) were computed. Second, a chi-square test was conducted to examine whether ADHD presentations differed in their likelihood of being classified with evening, intermediate, or morning chronotype. Given the large sample size, a p < .001 from the chi-square test was required to perform the six subsequent tests across the four ADHD/comparison groups (per-comparison p < .008, Bonferroni correction).

Third, bivariate correlations were conducted to examine correlations among demographic variables, psychopathology dimensions, and circadian preference. Because seven participants identified as “other” when reporting on sex, these participants were not included in the correlation analyses. However, to avoid excluding participants from primary analyses, two dummy code vectors were created with female as the reference group. We also conducted Steiger’s z-tests for dependent correlations to examine whether inattentive symptoms were more strongly associated than hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms with having an evening chronotype (dichotomous) and greater eveningness circadian preference (dimensional). Fourth, regression analyses were conducted to examine ADHD symptom dimensions in relation to circadian preference (dimensional) and evening type (categorical). For the logistic analysis examining evening type, the morning type and intermediate type categories were combined. In both regression models, age, sex, race, ethnicity, psychiatric medication use1, anxiety symptoms, and depressive symptoms were included as covariates. Fifth, and finally, to evaluate whether circadian preference moderated the association between ADHD inattentive symptoms and internalizing symptoms, the PROCESS macro using bootstrapped sampling (5,000 iterations) (Hayes, 2017) was used to test two moderation models. Specifically, we tested whether inattentive symptoms would interact with circadian preference in relation to either depressive or anxiety symptoms. Significant interactions were probed via simple slopes analyses by testing the relation between inattentive symptoms and internalizing symptoms at specific levels of circadian preference (i.e., −1SD, the mean, and +1SD). Given the large sample size, alpha level for significance tests was set at p < .001 for these analyses.

Results

Frequencies and Co-occurrence of Chronotype and ADHD Symptom Presentations

A total of 656 participants (13.8%) reported elevated ADHD symptoms. Of these, 299 (45.6%) were categorized as ADHD predominantly inattentive presentation, 215 (32.8%) with ADHD predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentation, and 142 (21.6%) with ADHD combined presentation. For chronotype, 2,999 (63.1%) were categorized as intermediate type, 1,432 (30.1%) as evening type, and 320 (6.7%) as morning type.

Co-occurrence of chronotype and ADHD symptom presentations are summarized in Table 2. The chi-square test indicated a significant difference in rates of chronotype across ADHD presentations, Χ2(6) = 67.23, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .12. Rates of each chronotype based on ADHD presentation were compared, using a Bonferroni-adjusted p-value of .008 to account for multiple comparisons. As summarized in Table 2, participants with either ADHD predominantly inattentive presentation or ADHD combined presentation had higher rates of being an evening type (47.2% and 41.5%, respectively) than participants without elevated ADHD (28.5%), and participants with ADHD predominantly inattentive presentation also had higher rates of being an evening type than participants with ADHD predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentation (30.7%). Conversely, participants without elevated ADHD were more likely to be an intermediate type (64.6%) than participants with either ADHD predominantly inattentive or combined presentations (50.8% and 53.5%, respectively). Lastly, participants without elevated ADHD or with ADHD predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentation were more likely to be a morning type (6.9% and 11.2%, respectively) than participants with ADHD predominantly inattentive presentation (2.0%).

Table 2.

Co-occurrence of Chronotype with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Symptom Presentations in College Students

ADHD Presentation
Chronotype 1.
No Elevated ADHD
(n = 4,095)
2.
Predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive
(n = 215)
3.
Predominantly Inattentive
(n = 299)
4.
ADHD Combined
(n = 142)
Contrasts
Evening preference 1,166 (28.5%) a 66 (30.7%) a,c 141 (47.2%) b 59 (41.5%) b,c 1 < 3, 4; 2 < 3
Intermediate 2,646 (64.6%) a 125 (58.1%) a,b 152 (50.8%) b 76 (53.5%) b 1 > 3, 4
Morning preference 283 (6.9%) a 24 (11.2%) a 6 (2.0%) b 7 (4.9%) a,b 1, 2 > 3

Note. N = 4,761. Cell ns and percentages indicate percentage within each ADHD presentation group. Row frequencies with different superscripts differ significantly at p < .008 (Bonferroni correction).

Bivariate Correlations

Bivariate correlations among study variables are presented in Table 3. A significant association was found between ADHD status and both having an evening chronotype (categorical) and eveningness circadian preference (dimensional), though both effects were small (rs = .09 and −.10, respectively). Significant but small effects were also found between ADHD hyperactive-impulsive symptoms and both evening chronotype and eveningness circadian preference (rs = .06 and −.08, respectively). Significant and small-to-medium effects were found between ADHD inattentive symptoms and both evening chronotype and eveningness circadian preference (rs = .16 and −.21, respectively). Steiger’s z-tests indicated that the ADHD inattentive symptoms were more strongly associated than ADHD hyperactive-impulsive symptoms with having an evening chronotype (z = 7.41, p < .001) and, likewise, with greater eveningness preference (z = 9.69, p < .001).

Table 3.

Means, Standard Deviations, and Bivariate Correlations of Study Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
1. Age --
2. Sex −.13* --
3. Race −.09* .03 --
4. Ethnicity .05 .03 −.08* --
5. Medication use .04 .03 .11* −.05* --
6. ADHD Status .01 .01 .003 .03 .18* --
7. ADHD Inattention .02 −.002 −.01 .02 .23* .63* --
8. ADHD Hyp-Imp −.003 .02 .07* .03 .20* .58* .56* --
9. Chronotype −.04 −.03 .02 .02 .05* .09* .16* .06* --
10. Circadian Preference .07* .03 −.02 −.04 −.06* −.10* −.21* −.08* −.75* --
11. Depressive Symptoms .04 .01 −.03 .004 .16* .35* .54* .35* .14* −.17* --
12. Anxiety Symptoms .01 .07* −.001 .001 .21* .39* .52* .48* .07* −.10* .65* --

Mean 19.28 -- -- -- -- -- 1.74 1.71 -- 45.98 0.58 0.53
Standard Deviation 1.50 -- -- -- -- -- 0.56 0.51 -- 8.27 0.64 0.54

Note. For sex, 0 = male, 1 = female. For race, 0 = non-White, 1 = White. For ethnicity, 0 = non-Hispanic, 1 = Hispanic. For medication use, 0 = not taking any prescribed psychiatric medication, 1 = taking at least one prescribed psychiatric medication. For ADHD status, 0 = no elevated ADHD symptoms, 1 = elevated ADHD symptoms. For chronotype, 0 = morning/intermediate type, 1 = evening type. For circadian preference, higher scores indicate a greater preference for morningness. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Hyp-Imp = hyperactivity-impulsivity.

*

p < .001.

ADHD symptoms, as well as evening chronotype and greater eveningness preference, were all associated with both depressive and anxiety symptoms, with effects ranging from small to large (|r|s = .07 – .54; see Table 3). In general, demographic variables were unassociated or had small effect associations with psychopathology and circadian function variables (Table 3).

ADHD Dimensions in Relation to Circadian Preference and Chronotype

Results of the regression analyses of ADHD symptom dimensions in relation to circadian preference (continuous) and chronotype (dichotomous) are summarized in Table 4. Above and beyond participant characteristics (i.e., age, sex, race, ethnicity, psychiatric medication use) and internalizing symptoms, ADHD-IN symptoms were significantly associated with both eveningness preference and evening chronotype (both ps < .001). ADHD-HI symptoms were not significantly associated with circadian preference or chronotype above and beyond the other variables in the model.

Table 4.

Regression Analyses Examining Demographic Characteristics, Internalizing Symptoms, and ADHD Dimensions in Relation to Circadian Preference and Chronotype in College Students

Evening Circadian Preference
(continuous)
Evening Type
(dichotomous)
B SE β t B SE Wald OR (95% CI)
Age   0.43 0.08   0.08  5.47* −0.08 0.02 12.08* 0.92 (0.88, 0.97)
Sex (Male) −0.62 0.27 −0.03   −2.33   0.15 0.07   4.24 1.16 (1.01, 1.34)
Sex (Other)   2.25 3.29   0.01  0.68 −1.00 1.11   0.81 0.37 (0.04, 3.27)
Race −0.47 0.30 −0.02   −1.59   0.15 0.08   3.24 1.16 (0.99, 1.37)
Ethnicity −1.11 0.37 −0.04   −3.02   0.18 0.10   3.20 1.20 (0.98, 1.46)
Medication Use −0.54 0.36 −0.02   −1.52   0.13 0.10   1.72 1.13 (0.94, 1.37)
Depression −1.58 0.25 −0.12   −6.28*   0.38 0.07 32.46* 1.47 (1.29, 1.67)
Anxiety   0.93 0.31   0.06  3.03 −0.28 0.08 10.98* 0.76 (0.64, 0.89)
ADHD-IN −2.91 0.29 −0.20 −10.22*   0.56 0.08 53.57* 1.76 (1.51, 2.04)
ADHD-HI   0.84 0.29   0.05  2.91 −0.16 0.08   3.89 0.86 (0.73, 1.00)

Note. For sex (male), 0 = female/other, 1 = male. For sex (other), 0 = male/female, 1 = other. For race, 0 = non-White, 1 = White. For ethnicity, 0 = non-Hispanic, 1 = Hispanic. ADHD = attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. HI = hyperactivity-impulsivity. IN = inattention. OR = odds ratio.

*

p < .001.

Circadian Preference as a Moderator of the Link between ADHD and Internalizing Symptoms

The moderation analyses included the same variables as the previous regression analyses, with the exception that separate models were conducted with depressive and anxiety symptoms as the outcome variable. Above and beyond participant characteristics, ADHD hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, and anxiety symptoms, there was a significant ADHD inattention × circadian preference interaction (b = −0.01, SE = .001, t = −4.05, p < .001). As shown in Figure 1, there was a significant association between ADHD inattentive symptoms and depressive symptoms at all levels of circadian preference. However, the association between ADHD inattention and depression was stronger as circadian preference for eveningness increased. Main effects were also found for ADHD hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (b = −0.08, SE = .02, t = −4.87, p < .001) and anxiety symptoms (b = 0.63, SE = .02, t = 41.39, p < .001) in relation to depressive symptoms.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Preference for Evening Strengthens the Association between ADHD-IN and Depressive Symptoms

There was not a significant ADHD inattention × circadian preference interaction in relation to anxiety symptoms. Main effects were found for ADHD hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (b = 0.24, SE = .01, t = 18.12, p < .001), depressive symptoms (b = 0.43, SE = .01, t = 41.39, p < .001), psychiatric medication use (b = 0.11, SE = .02, t = 6.27, p < .001), and female sex (b = 0.07, SE = .01, t = 5.93, p < .001) in relation to anxiety symptoms.2

Discussion

This study advances the existing literature on ADHD and chronotype/circadian preference in several ways. By using a large, multi-site sample of college students, we confirmed that students with elevated ADHD symptoms clearly differ from their peers without ADHD in their chronotype, with ADHD being associated with an evening chronotype. We also found that this association is primarily attributable to inattentive, rather than hyperactive-impulsive, symptoms. This study also showed, for the first time, that the association between ADHD inattentive symptoms and depressive symptoms is stronger for individuals with greater preference for eveningness. Taken together, this study shows that ADHD, and inattentive symptoms considered dimensionally, are associated with later chronotype, and that inattentive symptoms and eveningness preference are jointly associated with increased depressive symptoms in college students.

Previous studies have found that 24-59% of adults with ADHD or elevated ADHD symptoms report an evening chronotype, though sample sizes were often small (Gabay et al., 2022; Rybak et al., 2007; Voinescu et al., 2012). The only study comprised solely of college students was conducted by Gabay et al. (2022), who found that college students with elevated ADHD symptoms were more likely to be evening types (59%) than college students without ADHD symptoms (44%). In the current study, we too found that college students with elevated ADHD symptoms were more likely to be classified as evening types (40.5%) than students without ADHD symptoms (28.5%), though our rates were lower than those reported in Gabay et al. (2022). Possible reasons for the discrepant rates include Gabay et al. using a shorter, five-item measure of morningness-eveningness, as well as their smaller sample size of 363 students (98 with elevated ADHD symptoms). Our sample size of 4,761, including 656 with elevated ADHD symptoms, allowed us to examine chronotype in specific ADHD presentations, finding that the higher rates of evening type in the participants with ADHD was specific to students with inattentive symptoms (either predominantly inattentive presentation or combined presentation).

This finding was further supported in our analyses using dimensional measures of ADHD symptoms, wherein higher inattentive symptoms were uniquely associated with both evening preference and evening chronotype above and beyond participant characteristics and internalizing symptoms, whereas hyperactive-impulsive symptoms were not. Although some mixed findings have been reported in previous studies examining ADHD dimensions and circadian preference (Bron et al., 2016; Sivertsen et al., 2015), our findings are consistent with previous studies that included college students specifically (Caci et al., 2009; Voinescu et al., 2012). Circadian rhythms are known to impact neurocognition (Schmidt et al., 2007), and the present study is the largest to date to examine the association between chronotype and clinical ADHD inattentive symptoms specifically.

In moderation analyses, we found greater eveningness preference to strengthen the association between ADHD inattention and depressive symptoms. This effect was specific to depressive symptoms, as similar moderation was not found in relation to anxiety symptoms. Inattentive symptoms are more strongly associated than hyperactive-impulsive symptoms with mood disorder symptoms, whereas the ADHD dimensions do not differ in their associations with anxiety disorder symptoms (Willcutt et al., 2012). In this study we found depressive but not anxiety symptoms to be associated with our dimensional measure of circadian preference. It thus appears that inattention, circadian preference, and depressive symptoms are themselves closely intertwined. It is possible that the combined presence of significant inattentive symptoms and a preference for eveningness compound in contributing to worsened mood. College students with ADHD who are also evening types may also encounter challenges with time management, academic demands, and evening activities including social media and alcohol use, which in turn contribute to poorer overall functioning and greater depressive symptoms (DuPaul et al., 2021; Mochrie et al., 2020).

Findings from the present study point to several areas for further inquiry. The cross-sectional nature of our study does not allow for inferring causality or directionality, and experimental and longitudinal studies are needed to disentangle the complex relations between circadian phase and attention. We focused on ADHD symptoms in relation to circadian function so we could simultaneously examine the unique effects of inattention and hyperactive-impulsive dimensions, though it is also possible that inattention contributes to circadian phase (e.g., via disorganization and time-management difficulties; Barkley, 2012; Becker et al., 2018; Jarrett et al., 2017). In addition, there is some indication that college students with ADHD have poorer cognitive performance when completing tasks at times that are desynchronous with their circadian phase (Gabay et al., 2022), and additional studies are needed to further evaluate time of day effects, including domains of cognitive and academic functioning most impacted. It is also possible that college students with ADHD may do better in classes that are better matched to their circadian phase (e.g., afternoon rather than early morning classes). In terms of treatment, several studies have examined melatonin in children with ADHD, though studies with adults are lacking and the existing studies conducted in children primarily used melatonin as a somnolent to shorten sleep onset latency rather than as a chronobiotic to advance circadian phase (Coogan & McGowan, 2017). Morning light therapy reduced ADHD symptoms, improved mood, and decreased eveningness preference in an open trial of 29 adults with ADHD, with a need for confirmation in a larger, randomized trial (Rybak et al., 2006). Finally, a transdiagnostic cognitive-behavioral intervention was shown to impact circadian phase in adolescents with an evening type (Harvey et al., 2018), and it would be informative to test this intervention in college students specifically, including students with ADHD.

Several limitations are important to note. First, as noted above, the cross-sectional design precludes drawing causal or directional inferences. Second, we relied solely on rating scale measures, and it would be beneficial for future studies to include more robust measurement of ADHD (e.g., diagnostic interviews, collateral report) and circadian function (e.g., dim light melatonin onset, actigraphy-assessed mid-sleep). In particular, our sample had a high percentage of students with predominantly hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, which on one hand ensured we had sufficient numbers for group comparisons and variability in ADHD-HI symptoms, though this presentation is less common after early childhood (Willcutt et al., 2012). It is important to recall that our study grouped participants based solely on ADHD symptoms and was therefore not a comprehensive assessment or diagnosis that considered all diagnostic criteria (e.g., symptoms across settings, impairment, age of onset, symptoms not better explained by another mental disorder). In addition, it would be beneficial for future studies to more thoroughly assess medical history/illness and specific medication formulations/doses which may impact both ADHD symptoms and circadian function. Third, we focused specifically on the association between ADHD and chronotype/circadian function, and there is a need for research that simultaneously considers the extent to which sleep and circadian factors contribute to, maintain, or exacerbate ADHD symptoms and other psychopathologies (Becker, 2023). Fourth, we do not know what percentage of potentially eligible participants chose to participate or whether certain students were more or less likely to participate in this study. Fifth, although our sample size was large and drawn from multiple universities in an effort to increase representativeness and generalizability, only five universities were included, and most participants were female and non-Hispanic White.

Despite these limitations, the current study leveraged a sample of over 4,700 college students from five universities in different regions of the United States to examine ADHD presentations and symptom dimensions in relation to chronotype and circadian preference. Using both categorical and dimensional approaches, ours is the largest study to document that college students with elevated ADHD symptoms are more likely to be evening types than other college students, and inattentive symptoms in particular are associated with later circadian preference above and beyond participant characteristics and internalizing symptoms. Finally, ours is the first study to show that the association between ADHD inattentive and depressive symptoms is stronger for college students with greater preference for eveningness.

Acknowledgement:

When data reported in this study were collected, Stephen Becker was supported by award number K23MH108603 from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Footnotes

Declarations of Interest: None.

1

Results were unchanged when ADHD medication use (dichotomous) was used instead of any psychiatric medication use, and ADHD medication use was not a significant predictor variable in any model. We did not collect information on specific ADHD medications (e.g., stimulant vs. non-stimulant) or dose.

2

Our moderation analyses focused on ADHD inattention and circadian preference given that ADHD inattentive symptoms are more strongly associated than ADHD hyperactive-impulsive symptoms with internalizing symptoms (Willcutt et al., 2012). We conducted analyses to explore whether there was significant interaction between ADHD hyperactive-impulsive symptoms and circadian preference and internalizing symptoms. A significant interaction was not found for either depressive or anxiety symptoms.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Fifth Edition (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  2. Antony MM, Bieling PJ, Cox BJ, Enns MW, & Swinson RP (1998). Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 176–181. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baird AL, Coogan AN, Siddiqui A, Donev RM, & Thome J (2012). Adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder is associated with alterations in circadian rhythms at the behavioural, endocrine and molecular levels. Mol Psychiatry, 17(10), 988–995. 10.1038/mp.2011.149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barkley RA (2011). Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV (BAARS-IV). Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Barkley RA (2012). Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. J Abnorm Psychol, 121(4), 978–990. 10.1037/a0023961 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Becker SP (2020). ADHD and sleep: Recent advances and future directions. Curr Opin Psychol, 34, 50–56. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Becker SP (2023). Commentary: Advancing our understanding of the sleep-circadian pas de deux for adolescent psychopathology–a reflection on Cooper et al. (2023). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(3), 461–463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Becker SP, Burns GL, Garner AA, Jarrett MA, Luebbe AM, Epstein JN, & Willcutt EG (2018, Mar). Sluggish cognitive tempo in adults: Psychometric validation of the Adult Concentration Inventory. Psychol Assess, 30(3), 296–310. 10.1037/pas0000476 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Becker SP, Kapadia DK, Fershtman CEM, & Sciberras E (2020). Evening circadian preference is associated with sleep problems and daytime sleepiness in adolescents with ADHD. J Sleep Res, 29(1), e12936. 10.1111/jsr.12936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bijlenga D, van der Heijden KB, Breuk M, van Someren EJ, Lie ME, Boonstra AM, Swaab HJ, & Kooij JJ (2013). Associations between sleep characteristics, seasonal depressive symptoms, lifestyle, and ADHD symptoms in adults. J Atten Disord, 17(3), 261–275. 10.1177/1087054711428965 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bijlenga D, Van Someren EJW, Gruber R, Bron TI, Kruithof IF, Spanbroek ECA, & Kooij JJS (2013). Body temperature, activity and melatonin profiles in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and delayed sleep: a case-control study. J Sleep Res, 22(6), 607–616. 10.1111/jsr.12075 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bijlenga D, Vollebregt MA, Kooij JJS, & Arns M (2019). The role of the circadian system in the etiology and pathophysiology of ADHD: time to redefine ADHD? Atten Defic Hyperact Disord, 11(1), 5–19. 10.1007/s12402-018-0271-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bondopadhyay U, Diaz-Orueta U, & Coogan AN (2022). A systematic review of sleep and circadian rhythms in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord, 26(2), 149–224. 10.1177/1087054720978556 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bron TI, Bijlenga D, Kooij JJS, Vogel SWN, Wynchank D, Beekman ATF, & Penninx B (2016). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms add risk to circadian rhythm sleep problems in depression and anxiety. J Affect Disord, 200, 74–81. 10.1016/j.jad.2016.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Caci H, Bouchez J, & Bayle FJ (2009). Inattentive Symptoms of ADHD Are Related to Evening Orientation. J Atten Disord, 13(1), 36–41. 10.1177/1087054708320439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Coles ME, Schubert JR, & Nota JA (2015). Sleep, circadian rhythms, and anxious traits. Curr Psychiatry Rep, 17, 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Coogan AN, & McGowan NM (2017). A systematic review of circadian function, chronotype and chronotherapy in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [Review]. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 07, 129–147. 10.1007/s12402-016-0214-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. DuPaul GJ, Gormley MJ, Anastopoulos AD, Weyandt LL, Labban J, Sass AJ, Busch CZ, Franklin MK, & Postler KB (2021). Academic trajectories of college students with and without ADHD: Predictors of four-year outcomes. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 50(6), 828–843. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gabay L, Miller P, Alia-Klein N, & Lewin MP (2022). Circadian effects on attention and working memory in college students with attention deficit and hyperactivity symptoms. Front Psychol, 13, 851502. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.851502 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gruber R, Fontil L, Bergmame L, Wiebe ST, Amsel R, Frenette S, & Carrier J (2012). Contributions of circadian tendencies and behavioral problems to sleep onset problems of children with ADHD. BMC Psychiatry, 12, 212. 10.1186/1471-244X-12-212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Harvey AG, Hein K, Dolsen MR, Dong L, Rabe-Hesketh S, Gumport NB, Kanady J, Wyatt JK, Hinshaw SP, Silk JS, Smith RL, Thompson MA, Zannone N, & Blum DJ (2018). Modifying the impact of eveningness chronotype (“night-owls”) in youth: A randomized controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 57(10), 742–754. 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.04.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Hayes AF (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. Guilford publications. [Google Scholar]
  23. Holdaway AS, Luebbe AM, & Becker SP (2018). Rumination in relation to suicide risk, ideation, and attempts: Exacerbation by poor sleep quality? J Affect Disord, 236, 6–13. 10.1016/j.jad.2018.04.087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Horne JA, & Östberg O (1976). A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol, 4(2), 97–110. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1027738 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jarrett MA, Rapport HF, Rondon AT, & Becker SP (2017). ADHD dimensions and sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms in relation to self-report and laboratory measures of neuropsychological functioning in college students. J Atten Disord, 21, 673–683. 10.1177/1087054714560821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Lovibond PF, & Lovibond SH (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Martinez-Cayuelas E, Moreno-Vinues B, Losada-Del Pozo R, Rodrigo-Moreno M, Soto-Insuga V, & Pérez-Villena A (2022). Sleep, chronotype, and behavior in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives De Pediatrie, 29(4), 277–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Miller TW, Nigg JT, & Faraone SV (2007). Axis I and II comorbidity in adults with ADHD. J Abnorm Psychol, 116(3), 519–528. 10.1037/0021-843x.116.3.519 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mochrie KD, Whited MC, Cellucci T, Freeman T, & Corson AT (2020). ADHD, depression, and substance abuse risk among beginning college students. Journal of American College Health, 68(1), 6–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Rybak YE, McNeely HE, Mackenzie BE, Jain UR, & Levitan RD (2006). An open trial of light therapy in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67(10), 1527–1535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Rybak YE, McNeely HE, Mackenzie BE, Jain UR, & Levitan RD (2007). Seasonality and circadian preference in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: clinical and neuropsychological correlates. Compr Psychiatry, 48(6), 562–571. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2007.05.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Salgado-Delgado R, Tapia Osorio A, Saderi N, & Escobar C (2011). Disruption of circadian rhythms: a crucial factor in the etiology of depression. Depression research and treatment, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Schmidt C, Collette F, Cajochen C, & Peigneux P (2007). A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cogn Neuropsychol, 24(7), 755–789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sivertsen B, Harvey AG, Pallesen S, & Hysing M (2015). Mental health problems in adolescents with delayed sleep phase: Results from a large population-based study in Norway. J Sleep Res, 24(1), 11–18. 10.1111/jsr.12254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Tarakçioğlu MC, Kadak MT, Gürbüz GA, Poyraz BÇ, Erdoğan F, & Aksoy UM (2018). Evaluation of the relationship between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms and chronotype. Archives of Neuropsychiatry, 55(1), 54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Van der Heijden KB, Stoffelsen R, Popma A, & Swaab H (2018). Sleep, chronotype, and sleep hygiene in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum disorder, and controls. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 27, 99–111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Voinescu BI, Szentagotai A, & David D (2012). Sleep disturbance, circadian preference and symptoms of adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J Neural Transm, 119(10), 1195–1204. 10.1007/s00702-012-0862-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Willcutt EG, Nigg JT, Pennington BF, Solanto MV, Rohde LA, Tannock R, Loo SK, Carlson CL, McBurnett K, & Lahey BB (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. J Abnorm Psychol, 121(4), 991–1010. 10.1037/a0027347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES