Skip to main content
NeuroImage : Clinical logoLink to NeuroImage : Clinical
. 2023 Dec 19;41:103552. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2023.103552

Mapping neurodevelopment with sleep macro- and micro-architecture across multiple pediatric populations

N Kozhemiako a, AW Buckley b, RD Chervin c, S Redline a,d, SM Purcell a,
PMCID: PMC10788305  PMID: 38150746

Highlights

  • This study demonstrates the potential of sleep-based features to provide practical markers for tracking neurodevelopment in children across multiple clinical populations.

  • Using whole-night polysomnography data, we comprehensively describe robust age-related changes in multiple sleep metrics derived from electroencephalogram and demonstrate their utility in predicting an individual's chronological age with high accuracy.

  • The differences between the predicted age and the chronological age can be indicative of neurodevelopmental alterations.

  • This study suggests that sleep patterns can provide a sensitive way to understand the process of brain maturation and could lead to the creation of objective sleep-based biomarkers that can be used on a larger scale to measure neurodevelopment.

Abstract

Profiles of sleep duration and timing and corresponding electroencephalographic activity reflect brain changes that support cognitive and behavioral maturation and may provide practical markers for tracking typical and atypical neurodevelopment. To build and evaluate a sleep-based, quantitative metric of brain maturation, we used whole-night polysomnography data, initially from two large National Sleep Research Resource samples, spanning childhood and adolescence (total N = 4,013, aged 2.5 to 17.5 years): the Childhood Adenotonsillectomy Trial (CHAT), a research study of children with snoring without neurodevelopmental delay, and Nationwide Children’s Hospital (NCH) Sleep Databank, a pediatric sleep clinic cohort. Among children without neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD), sleep metrics derived from the electroencephalogram (EEG) displayed robust age-related changes consistently across datasets. During non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, spindles and slow oscillations further exhibited characteristic developmental patterns, with respect to their rate of occurrence, temporal coupling and morphology. Based on these metrics in NCH, we constructed a model to predict an individual's chronological age. The model performed with high accuracy (r = 0.93 in the held-out NCH sample and r = 0.85 in a second independent replication sample – the Pediatric Adenotonsillectomy Trial for Snoring (PATS)). EEG-based age predictions reflected clinically meaningful neurodevelopmental differences; for example, children with NDD showed greater variability in predicted age, and children with Down syndrome or intellectual disability had significantly younger brain age predictions (respectively, 2.1 and 0.8 years less than their chronological age) compared to age-matched non-NDD children. Overall, our results indicate that sleep architecture offers a sensitive window for characterizing brain maturation, suggesting the potential for scalable, objective sleep-based biomarkers to measure neurodevelopment.

1. Introduction

Brain structure and function undergo drastic transformations over the first two decades of life (Stiles & Jernigan, 2010). Over the same period, there are also profound changes in multiple facets of sleep, potentially reflecting developmental programs of brain reorganization (Knoop et al., 2021). Given abundant evidence for the critical impact that sleep has on memory consolidation and working memory, deviations from normal sleep patterns during childhood and adolescence – the most learning-intensive periods in life – might have long-lasting consequences (Kopasz et al., 2010). For example, active processes taking place during sleep support synaptic pruning and connectivity restructuring (Buchmann et al., 2011, Tononi and Cirelli, 2014). A more precise characterization of normative childhood trajectories of sleep may therefore aid our understanding of brain developmental processes and shed light on how subsequent cognitive and behavioral dysfunction can emerge.

Children with neurodevelopmental disorders (NDD) and related diagnoses including epilepsy are exceptionally vulnerable to sleep problems (Kamara and Beauchaine, 2020, Robinson-Shelton and Malow, 2015). Sleep dysregulation has been documented in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (Souders et al., 2017), attention deficit / hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Becker, 2020), cerebral palsy (Simard-Tremblay et al., 2011), Down syndrome (Stores & Stores, 2013), and epilepsy (Larson et al., 2012). Comorbid conditions that commonly accompany clinical manifestation of NDDs, including intellectual disability, have also been associated with sleep abnormalities (Surtees et al., 2018).

However, the majority of these reports focused on a single disorder despite evidence of trans-diagnostically shared risk factors and pathogenic mechanisms, especially for psychiatric disorders (Gandal et al., 2016, Thapar et al., 2017). A second limitation is that the most-studied aspects of sleep have been macro-level metrics (e.g. time in bed) approximated by parental report. Such measures are inherently limited with respect to characterizing brain activity. In contrast, electrophysiological characterizations of sleep micro-architecture reflect underlying processes during different sleep stages more directly. One recent review described the presence of sleep oscillation (e.g. spindles, slow oscillations) abnormalities among NDDs but also highlighted the scarcity of relevant studies and limited sample sizes which, combined with varying age ranges and inconsistent methodologies, precluded strong conclusions (Gorgoni et al., 2020).

Across the lifespan, when investigating age-related change numerous studies have adopted the paradigm of predicting chronological age based on neuroimaging or other data sources, on the assumption that its deviation from observed age – the brain age gap – is a putative marker of overall brain health. Compared to healthy controls, altered “brain age gaps” have been observed in various adult-onset disorders and conditions including Alzheimer disease, mild cognitive impairment, schizophrenia and multiple sclerosis (Baecker et al., 2021). Accelerated aging – larger positive brain age gaps – has also been shown to follow traumatic brain injury (Cole et al., 2015). Conversely, opposite patterns in children born very prematurely have been interpreted as reflections of delayed brain development (Franke et al., 2012). Although the majority of studies on brain age used structural MRI data, with predictions usually achieving high correlations (r > 0.9) with chronological age (Franke & Gaser, 2019), recent reports in adults demonstrated that the sleep EEG can also give comparable results (Nygate et al., 2021, Sun et al., 2019). In aggregate, these findings suggest that sleep EEG features track strongly with age and may be valuable for mapping typical and atypical childhood development.

In the present study, we used polysomnography (PSG) data from a large clinical cohort to comprehensively chart the developmental trajectories of metrics derived from sleep electroencephalograms (EEG) across childhood and adolescence. Based on clinical records, we initially removed individuals with any of six major clinical groups including NDDs and used data from remaining individuals (N = 1,828, 2.5 – 17.5 years) to characterize neurodevelopment in non-NDD sample. We further validated findings in an independent sample of generally healthy children with snoring. This latter group of children participated in a clinical trial and were screened to be free of severe neurodevelopmental delay (estimated by the Differential Ability Scales II), although they had a more restricted age range (N = 1,213, 4.5 – 10 years). Based on the profile of associations we detected, we then developed a multivariate, joint model to predict chronological age as a function of sleep macro- and micro-architecture. Specifically, we tested 1) whether such a model was transferable between different studies and populations, and 2) whether deviations between predicted and chronological age distinguished children with NDD from non-NDD children. For the last two goals we employed two additional datasets: the Pediatric Adenotonsillectomy Trial for Snoring, (PATS, N = 627 children with snoring) and the Cleveland Family Study (CFS, N = 730), a family-based study covering a wide age range from 6 to 88 years.

2. Results

The primary discovery dataset comprised 2,800 individuals with whole-night PSGs from the Nationwide Children’s Hospital (NCH) Sleep Databank (accessed via the National Sleep Research Resource, NSRR (Zhang et al., 2018)). Based on ICD codes, we defined six subsets within the NCH sample based on the presence of a diagnosis of the following neurodevelopmental and childhood onset disorders: ASD, ADHD, intellectual disabilities, Down syndrome (DS), cerebral palsy (CP) and epilepsy (for brevity, below we use “NDD group” to refer to these sub-cohorts although we acknowledge that epilepsy is usually not classified as an NDD). We selected these six subgroups based on each having N > 100 subjects in the full dataset and previous reports of alterations in sleep (Becker, 2020, Larson et al., 2012, Simard-Tremblay et al., 2011, Souders et al., 2017, Stores and Stores, 2013, Surtees et al., 2018) (see Table 2 for demographic details, and Sup. Table 1 for diagnostic details). There was a considerable overlap between NDD diagnoses (Sup. Fig. 1a).

Table 2.

Distribution of Gender and Age by NDD group and Availability of Sleep Data in the NCH cohort.

Disorders Macro-architecture Micro-architecture
power & spindles
Micro-architecture
SO & coupling
N (N of females) Age, M(SD)
range
N (N of females) Age, M(SD)
range
N (N of females) Age, M(SD)
range
ASD 196 (45) 9.5 (4.3)* 2.5–17.4 169 (39) 9.3 (4.3)* 2.5–17.4 141 (30) 9.6 (4.3) 2.7–17.1
ADHD 525 (1 4 9) 10.9 (3.5)* 2.6–17.5 483 (1 3 6) 10.9 (3.5)* 2.6–17.5 428 (1 1 9) 11.2 (3.5)* 2.6–17.5
Intellectual disability 167 (60) 10.9 (3.7)* 2.9–17.4 122 (45) 11 (3.6)*
2.9–17.4
95 (33) 11.3 (3.6)* 2.9–17.1
Down Syndrome 140 (56) 8.1 (4.4) 2.5–17.4 122 (49) 8.2 (4.5)
2.5–17.4
70 (23) 8.7 (4.7)
2.5–17.3
Cerebral Palsy 138 (60) 7.5 (3.8)* 2.6–17 93 (48) 7.6 (3.8)* 2.6–17 79 (41) 8 (3.8)*
2.6–17
Epilepsy 242 (1 0 4) 8.7 (4)
2.5–17.4
179 (84) 9 (3.8)
2.5–17.4
151 (72) 9.2 (3.8) 2.6–17.4

* - p < 0.05: mean age is significantly different compared to the rest of the sample.

Table 1.

Demographics characteristics (excluding NDD groups).

Variables Macro-architecture Micro-architecture power & spindles Micro-architecture SO & coupling
NCH CHAT NCH CHAT NCH CHAT
N 1828 1213 1744 1043 1512 996
N of females 877(48 %) 632(52.2 %) 840(48.2 %) 533(51.2 %) 736(48.7 %) 515(51.8 %)
White 1179(64.5 %) 485(40 %) 1138(65.3 %) 432(41.4 %) 983(65 %) 411(41.3 %)
Black 391(21.4 %) 573(47.2 %) 368(21.1 %) 479(45.9 %) 326(21.6 %) 458(46 %)
Other 182(10 %) 146(12 %) 167(9.6 %) 123(11.8 %) 143(9.5 %) 118(11.8 %)
Unknown 76(4.2 %) 9(0.7 %) 71(4.1 %) 9(0.9 %) 60(4 %) 9(0.9 %)
Age, M(SD) 8.6(4.3) 7.1(1.4) 8.5(4.3) 7(1.4) 9(4.3) 7.1(1.4)
Age range 2.5–17.5 4.5–10 2.5–17.5 4.5–10 2.5–17.5 4.5–10

Please see methods for description of the QC procedures that lead to different sample sizes for specific analyses.

As expected in this clinically-referred and ascertained sample, most NCH individuals had a sleep-related clinical diagnosis, precluding a straightforward definition of a “healthy control” comparison group. Although we refer to the “non-NDD sample” of NCH, we note that the individuals therein collectively had more than 9,000 unique diagnostic codes in their medical records, for both acute and chronic disorders (although not necessarily contemporaneous with the PSG). For example, there were diagnoses of cough (N = 1176), obesity (N = 793), skin rash (N = 633), unspecified disturbance of conduct (N = 215) and anxiety disorder (N = 210). Excluding the abovementioned six NDD groups, 93 % of participants had one or more sleep-related diagnostic codes including sleep apnea, insomnia, hypersomnia and others, although sleep disorders were more prevalent still among the NDD groups (Sup. Fig. 1b).

To address possible medication effects on sleep architecture, we further identified individuals across the NCH sample who were prescribed medications likely to affect sleep at a time overlapping the PSG recording. Only 14 % (262 out of 1,829 non-NDD NCH individuals) of the NCH sample had such medication prescribed at the night of PSG, with a majority being antihistamines (9 %) (Sup. Fig. 1c, Sup. Table 2). The proportion of individuals prescribed sleep-impacting medication during PSG was substantially higher among the NDD subgroups (42 %). Medication use was therefore added as a covariate in our primary analyses (see Methods for more details).

For our initial analyses of normative age-related changes in sleep we excluded the NDD groups, resulting in a final sample of N = 1,828 (detailed demographic information in Table 1). To provide an independent replication cohort, we obtained PSGs from N = 1,213 individuals from the Child Adenotonsillectomy Trial (CHAT) study, comprising children with reported snoring who were screened to be without neurodevelopmental delay.

2.1. Sleep macro-architecture in children without NDD

Macro-architecture metrics (i.e. those derived from the hypnogram based on manual staging) showed substantial age-related changes. Congruently in both samples, total sleep time (TST) exhibited a marked linear reduction with age (controlling for race/ethnicity and sex) in both NCH (r = -0.26, p < 10-15) and CHAT (r = -0.13, p = 9 × 10-6), as did sleep maintenance efficiency (SME) (r = -0.17, p = 2 × 10-12 in NCH, r = -0.07, p = 0.021 in CHAT) (Fig. 1A). Age-related effect sizes in CHAT are expected to be attenuated and/or more variable than in NCH, due to the narrower age range. Sleep also grew more fragmented with age in both samples, based on increases in the sleep fragmentation index (SFI) and duration of wake after sleep onset (WASO). The macro-architectural feature showing the strongest age-related change in both datasets was the number of NREM sleep cycles (r = -0.38, p < 10-15 in NCH and r = -0.13, p = 4 × 10-6 in CHAT), which remained significant even after covarying for TST (p < 10-15 in NCH and p = 0.002 in CHAT). At the same time, the average duration of sleep cycles increased with age in both samples, although the number of cycles was still the greatest determinant of TST (e.g. in NCH, r = 0.5 for cycle number compared to r = 0.12 for cycle duration).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Developmental changes in sleep macro-architecture. A – Sleep macro-architecture metrics stratified by age (3-year nonoverlapping windows from 2.5 to 17.5 years in NCH databank and 2-year nonoverlapping windows from 4.5 to 10 years in CHAT). The error bars represent 95 % confidence intervals for the mean. B. The top row represents duration of sleep stages in minutes from 3 to 16 years (N1 – light blue, N2 – blue, N3 – dark blue, R – red); the second row is as for A, for N2, N3, R proportions with respect to total sleep time. Total sleep time (TST), sleep maintenance efficiency (SME), sleep fragmentation index (SFI), wake time duration after sleep onset (WASO), transition index between NREM and R sleep (TI NR-R). (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

As others have reported (Baker et al., 2016, Feinberg et al., 2012), sleep stage composition changed profoundly across this developmental period (Fig. 1B). We observed similar effects in both cohorts, with one exception for N1 (Fig. 1B, top row). N1 duration also showed the largest absolute difference between the datasets, possibly reflecting variations in manual staging protocols and the intrinsically low construct validity of N1 as a distinct and atomic physiological state.

Stage N2 duration increased with age from 177 to 201 min between 4 and 16 years of age in NCH sample (p < 10-13 in NCH, p = 0.003 in CHAT). In contrast, N3 sleep reduced with age from 126 min in 4-year-olds to 81 min in 16-year-olds (p < 10-15 in NCH and p = 10-5 in CHAT) suggesting an age-related reduction in mean NREM depth. In general, the proportion of time spent in all NREM (stages N1, N2 and N3) increased with age (r = 0.29, p < 10-15 in NCH and r = 0.19, p = 3 × 10-10 in CHAT) while stage R displayed age-associated reduction in both duration and proportion (p < 10-15 in NCH and p < 10-13 in CHAT) from 92 to 64 min between 4 and 16 years.

Further, REM latency computed with regard to sleep onset increased with age (r = 0.2, p < 10-15 in NCH, r = 0.13, p = 7 × 10-6 in CHAT). Although there was evident difference in R latency between the two cohort, the estimates were comparable to the previous reports summarized in (Scholle et al., 2011). There were also fewer transitions between NREM and R periods with age in both datasets.

2.2. Sleep EEG spectral characteristics

Within sleep stage, spectral power across classical frequency bands displayed large age-dependent changes (Fig. 2), most notably a reduction in absolute spectral power in slower frequency bands (e.g. the largest effect size is illustrated in Supp. Fig. 2 A – delta band absolute power during R stage, r = -0.82 in NCH and r = -0.3 in CHAT). These effects likely reflect gross changes in the amplitude of the sleep EEG (most evident for slower bands that have higher power due to the 1/f nature of the power spectrum); that they were observed uniformly across sleep stages and channels suggests that these effects may not be specific to sleep neurophysiology (versus gross anatomical changes, for example).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Developmental changes in sleep EEG absolute spectral power. Developmental trajectories with 95 % confidence intervals of the mean, produced by averaging sleep estimates across individuals within 3-year nonoverlapping windows from 2.5 to 17.5 years in NCH databank and 2-year nonoverlapping windows from 4.5 to 10 years in CHAT) for absolute power across sleep stages (N2, N3, R, rows), bands (columns) and channels.

In contrast, absolute sigma power increased with age most strongly during N2 (p < 10-15 in NCH and p < 0.001 in CHAT across all channels with strongest effects at O1, r = 0.39 and r = 0.16 for NCH and CHAT respectively). A smaller but still significant increase was observed during N3, but not REM. Modelling with higher-order age terms suggested a nonlinear trajectory (based on Akaike’s and Bayesian information criteria), reflecting a slight decline in power occurring in adolescence (Fig. 2). Relative sigma power (normalized by total power 0.5 – 35 Hz to account for age-related changes in total power) similarly showed the strongest age-related changes (e.g. during N2 at O1, r = 0.76 in NCH and r = 0.4 in CHAT, Supp. Fig. 2 B, with similar effects in N3, all p < 10-15 in NCH and p < 10-5 in CHAT).

Other frequency bands and stages showed marked developmental changes in relative power with qualitatively distinct stage- and topographically specific developmental trajectories (Fig. 3). Considering NCH only, delta power decreased with age during R across all channels (all p < 10-15, max effect size at F4 r = -0.67), but increased with age in frontal channels, during N2/N3, with a peak around the age of puberty. In occipital channels, delta power decreased with age across all stages (all p < 10-15, with r ranging from r = -0.4 to 0.66). In contrast, theta power increased with age during R (all p < 10-10, max effect size at C4 r = -0.23) but decreased with age during N2/N3 (all p < 10-15, max effect size at F3 r = -0.63 during N2) in frontal and central channels. For the comparable age range, we observed broadly consistent patterns of NREM age-related change in CHAT. Finally, during REM, relative alpha, sigma and beta power increased with age (all p < 10-15, effect sizes ranging from r = 0.6 to 0.76 in NCH and all p < 10-10, effect sizes ranging from r = 0.21 to 0.35 in CHAT).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Developmental changes in sleep EEG relative spectral power. Developmental trajectories with 95 % confidence intervals of the mean, produced by averaging sleep estimates across individuals within 3-year nonoverlapping windows from 2.5 to 17.5 years in NCH databank and 2-year nonoverlapping windows from 4.5 to 10 years in CHAT) for relative across sleep stages (N2, N3, R, rows), bands (columns) and channels.

2.3. Spindles, slow oscillations and their coupling

Across childhood and adolescence, we observed multiple changes in NREM sleep spindles (Fig. 4). Although the density (count per minute) of both slow and fast spindles (SS and FS respectively, targeting 11 Hz and 15 Hz activity, with approximately +/-2 Hz around each central frequency) increased with age in frontal channels (all p < 10-15, from r = 0.21 to 0.45 in NCH and p < 10-14, from r = 0.22 to 0.26 in CHAT), their developmental trajectories were distinct (Fig. 4). Whereas FS density linearly increased across all channels (all p < 10-15 in NCH and p < 10-10 in CHAT) from 0.9 spindles per minute at age of 4 to 1.9 at age of 16 at C3, SS density displayed an inverted-U profile, most pronounced in frontal channels and peaking around 10 years with 2.4 spindles per minute at F3 (compared to 1.5 and 1.9 spindles at 4 and 16 years of age, respectively).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Developmental trajectories of spindles, slow oscillation, and their coupling. Developmental curves with 95 % confidence intervals generated by averaging sleep estimates within 3-year nonoverlapping windows from 2.5 to 17.5 years in NCH databank and 2-year nonoverlapping windows from 4.5 to 10 years in CHAT) for slow (top row) and fast (second row) spindle characteristics, SO parameters (third row) and coupling between SS/FS and SO (bottom row) during N2 across all channels.

Spindle morphology, not rate of occurrence, showed the most marked age-related changes, however. In particular, intra-spindle deceleration (sometimes called “chirp”, a typical characteristic of both fast and slow spindles) grew more pronounced with age, especially for SS (Fig. 4, p across all channels < 10-15 in NCH and p < 10-7 in CHAT, with strongest effects at F3: r = -0.66 and r = -0.38 in NCH and CHAT respectively (Sup. Fig. 2 C) as opposed to largest effect in FS density r = 0.53 and r = 0.29 in NCH and CHAT).

Average spindle frequency varied markedly with increasing age, but differently for slow and fast spindles (Fig. 4): whereas SS frequency became faster with age (across all channels p < 10-15, max effect size at F3 r = 0.5 in NCH and p < 10-10, max effect size at F4 r = 0.3 in CHAT), FS frequency slowed down (all p < 10-15, max effect size at F4 r = -0.5 in NCH and p < 10-8, max effect size at F4 r = -0.29 in CHAT). Opposing directions of change in frequency of SS and FS was also observed in older adults (Djonlagic et al., 2020). Notably, both SS and FS developmental trajectories were highly non-linear with SS maximum and FS minimum frequency observed around 13 years of age.

Note, that the total number of detected spindles (especially FS in occipital channels) was relatively low at younger ages somewhat limiting estimation robustness of their morphology characteristics. Nevertheless, they still displayed very consistent age-related trends and channel-specific patterns in both datasets.

We detected slow oscillations (SOs) during N2 sleep by identifying zero-crossings in 0.5–4 Hz bandpass-filtered signals and applying fixed-duration but relative-amplitude thresholds (see Methods for details). As for spindles, SO density increased with age (Fig. 4) across all channels in both cohorts (all p < 10-15 in NCH and p < 10-15 in CHAT). The steepest increase observed was at F4 with 10 SOs per minute at age of 4 and 15 at 16 years of age (r = 0.7 in NCH and r = 0.38 in CHAT, Sup. Fig. 2D).

With respect to SO morphology, average negative peak amplitude, peak-to-peak amplitude, and the slope between negative and positive peaks decreased linearly across all channels (p < 10-15 in NCH and p < 10-5 in CHAT, with the largest effect for peak-to-peak amplitude at O1, r = -0.74 in NCH and r = -0.23 in CHAT, Sup. Fig. 2F). SO duration was the only parameter to express a marked non-linear trajectory, with a steep increase from 4 to 10 years and a steady decline after age 10–12 years (Fig. 4).

With respect to inter-cohort differences, SO duration estimates (as well as relative power in slow frequency band across all stages) expressed some of the largest absolute differences between the NCH and CHAT cohort. That could potentially be due to distinct technical characteristics of PSG recording devises (e.g. inherent filtering setting). However, it is noteworthy that the age-related trends remained consistent across both cohorts.

SO properties are necessarily dependent upon the criteria used to detect them. In our primary analyses, based on optimizing the strength of observed spindle/SO coupling, we elected to use a relative amplitude threshold (see Methods for details). For example, 63 % of individuals had significant FS/SO overlap at C3 in NCH (66 % in CHAT) versus 58 % and 56 % when an absolute SO amplitude threshold was used. The choice of SO detection criteria can impact patterns of age-related change, however. Indeed, using an absolute threshold, age-related trends for SO rate, slope and amplitude were reversed – the former decreased with age while the latter two increased (Sup. Fig. 3). This apparent contradiction reflects a general decrease in SO activity with age, which is congruent with the observed age-related decrease in slow and delta band power. Reduced SO activity consequently lowers any relative threshold, which can in turn lead to relatively more events being detected. The optimal choice of threshold is an empirical question that will depend on the subsequent analyses, which underscores the importance of always explicitly reporting the type of detection thresholds used.

Returning to the original relative-threshold set of SO, we assessed spindle/SO coupling in three ways: 1) the tendency for spindles to preferentially occur non-uniformly with respect to SO phase (coupling magnitude), 2) the preferred SO phase at spindle peaks (coupling angle), and 3) the extent of any above-chance overlap between spindle and SO events, ignoring SO phase (coupling overlap). In general, SS showed more marked age-related changes, compared to FS (Fig. 4). SS coupling increased in frontal and central channels in both cohorts (all p < 10-15 in NCH and p < 10-1 in CHAT: e.g. at F3 r = 0.56 in NCH and r = 0.44 in CHAT, Supp. Fig. 2E).

With respect to FS coupling, an age-related increase was observed only in the NCH sample (across all channels p < 10-15), primarily driven by a steep increase in adolescence (Fig. 4). The lack of CHAT replication here likely reflects the restricted age range: indeed, among NCH individuals 10 or under associations were greatly attenuated, compared to the same tests in the older NCH subsample (data not shown).

Next, we found that individual's preferential SO phase angle at spindle peak (circular mean) shifted across development. In both cohorts, FS tended to occur before the SO positive peak, whereas SS tended to occur after it. However, with increasing age, both SS and FS shifted closer to the SO positive peak. Again, the most rapid change in preferred FS SO phase was during adolescence.

Finally, rates of above-chance gross overlap between SS and SOs increased with age at frontal channels (F3/F4 p < 10-15, max r = 0.3 in NCH and p < 0.05, max r = 0.1 in CHAT) whereas FS overlap showed a modest (albeit significant) decrease at central channels (C3/C4 p < 0.01, max r = 0.17 in NCH and p < 0.05, max r = 0.13 in CHAT). Unlike other SO metrics, age-related changes in coupling were generally similar despite different approaches for SO detection (Sup. Fig. 3).

Given that the majority of individuals in both cohorts had sleep apnea and/or snoring, we additionally retested all sleep variables for association with age after including apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) as a covariate. Results remained effectively identical, with a Pearson’s correlation r > 0.99 between signed log-transformed p-values in original and AHI-controlled analyses, also with comparable levels of significance. Same results were obtained using arousal index instead of AHI.

2.4. Brain age prediction using sleep macro- and micro-architecture measures

Above, we demonstrated 1) that age was strongly associated with multiple sleep macro- and micro-architecture metrics, and 2) that findings were congruent for two samples from distinct (clinical versus research) sources. We next aimed to condense these multivariate developmental patterns into a single model to estimate chronological age from the sleep EEG, and then to test whether its deviation from observed age - i.e. brain age gap - could identify pathological neurodevelopment. As our starting point, we fit a simple linear regression of age on sleep macro- and micro-architecture metrics adjusting for sex and race, using 70 % of the non-NDD NCH sample (i.e. after first excluding all clinical subgroups). We estimated performance using 10-fold cross-validation (Fig. 4 A). An initial model including 258 sleep variables (see Supp. table 7 for the detailed list) and two covariates (sex and race) achieved R2 = 0.89 (0.03 SD) and mean absolute error (MAE) of 1.08 years (0.11 SD). Whereas models trained on micro-architectural features only (151 spectral power, or 95 sleep spindle, SO and coupling metrics) performed almost as well as the full model, a model including only macro-architecture metrics displayed much lower performance (Fig. 4 B).

To validate and test model transferability, we applied the full NCH-derived model to three held-out validation sets, none of which included any NDD individuals (Fig. 4 A): i.) the remaining 30 % of the non-NDD NCH sample, ii.) the CHAT sample, which was used as the replication dataset in the previous sections and iii.) a new pediatric dataset PATS (Pediatric Adenotonsillectomy Trial for Snoring, PATS: N = 627 [307 females]. The PATS dataset, similarly to CHAT, comprised children with snoring from diverse ethnic backgrounds participating in a clinical trial. Compared to CHAT, PATS had a wider age range that was closer to NCH’s (mean age 6.3 years, range 3 to 13 years).

In all three test samples, the model predicted age with relatively high accuracy, indicating a high degree of transferability (Fig. 5C). The highest performance was observed in the NCH testing set, where predicted and observed age correlated r = 0.93. Although the correlations were lower in CHAT and PATS (r = 0.56 and r = 0.85 respectively), this likely was in part due to the narrower age ranges; the MAEs were comparable to NCH (1.11–––1.31 years). Prediction accuracy was similar for boys and girls in all three testing samples (p > 0.05), whether or not participant sex was not included in the model. Nonetheless there were still significant (albeit relatively subtle) sex differences and sex-by-age interactions in multiple measures of sleep macro and microarchitecture consistent with the previous literature (Baker et al., 2020, Campbell et al., 2012) (Sup. Fig. 4). Finally, the brain age gap (either MAE or ME) was also not significantly associated (p > 0.05) with AHI in either NCH or CHAT.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Brain age prediction based on sleep micro- and macro-architecture in non-NDD and NDD samples. A – a schematic illustration of the samples and steps used to predict individual chronological age based on sleep features. B – model performance based on different domains of sleep features using 10-fold cross validation. Metrics plotted are the explained variance and mean absolute error (the error bars represent SD across folds). C – The scatterplots show predicted vs true chronological age for all held-out datasets with mean absolute error (MAE), mean error (ME) and Pearson’s correlation (r) between chronological and predicted age reported for each sample. D – Bar plots illustrate MAE and ME for each NDD subgroup in comparison to the non-NDD held-out sample. We estimated variability of these estimates by repeating the analysis 100 times where NCH non-NDD training and non-NDD held-out set were resampled each round, but the disorder subgroups remained unchanged. The resulting 100 estimates of MAE and ME were averaged and illustrated in bar plots (error bars are min and max values across 100 rounds). Stars indicate statistical difference between estimates in each clinical subgroup vs testing set (* - p < 0.05, ** - p < 0.01, *** - p < 0.0 01).

Despite significant linear associations across the majority of the sleep features, many of them displayed non-linear developmental trajectories. This prompted us to test whether our age prediction would improve using a non-linear approach. The results, however, were nearly identical when LightGBM – a tree-based learning algorithm – was employed using the same set of sleep EEG features (e.g. linear regression model vs LightGBM in the held-out PATS sample – r = 0.85 vs 0.85, MAE = 1.18 vs 1.1 years, ME = 0.48 vs 0.6 years).

2.5. Brain age prediction and NDD

Brain age gap was significantly negative (i.e. younger than expected) in individuals with DS (ME = -2.1 years, p = 9 × 10-9) or intellectual disability (ME = -0.77 years, p = 0.02) subgroups (Fig. 5 D). Interestingly, for DS the brain age gap effect was exacerbated with increasing age, suggesting greater developmental delays in older patients (Fig. 6 E). Given prior reports of accelerated ageing (i.e. positive age gaps) in DS adults based on structural MRI and epigenetic markers (Cole et al., 2017, Horvath et al., 2015), we sought to contextualize our finding of delayed development in DS, especially given that the non-NDD NCH training sample was obligatorily limited to children and adolescents, combined with the fact that some sleep EEG metrics follow nonlinear, inverted-U trajectories across the lifespan (e.g. fast spindle density increases across childhood, peaks around 20 years of age and slowly declines thereafter, Fig. 6B).

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

Examples of NDD effects on sleep micro-architecture. A – age distribution in the combined PATS and CFS dataset; B, C – fast spindle density and absolute theta power averaged across age bins (lines indicate 95 % CI) in the combined sample, in reference to the mean fast spindle density and absolute theta power in individuals with DS (green horizontal line, dashed vertical line illustrates mean age in DS subgroup); D – normalized mean absolute difference between DS group and each age bin in the combined sample across sleep macro- and microarchitecture estimates (in green; grey points indicate the same but for non-NDD sample). Vertical lines illustrate average age of DS and non-NDD groups. E – scatterplot illustrates changes in brain age gap with age in DS subgroup (in green) and the test sample. F – distinct developmental trajectory of SS coupling overlap at C3 in DS subgroup (in green) compared to the rest of non-NDD sample; G – correlation between SS density at C3 and degree of intellectual disability (grey dashed line illustrates the mean SS density at C3 for the non-NDD sample); H – SS density across subgroups and the rest of the sample at F3 (** - p < 0.01, *** - p < 0.001). (For interpretation of the references to colour in this figure legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

We therefore estimated the developmental trajectories of sleep EEG metrics in a new dataset spanning both childhood and adulthood: PATS augmented by the Cleveland Family Study (N = 730, 401 females, mean age of 41 spanning 6.8 to 89 years) (Fig. 6A). Given the nonlinear, inverted-U lifecourse trajectories of spindle density, one can identify groups of older adults with numerically equivalent mean spindle densities compared to young children, as well as compared to the DS group (e.g. average FS density at 3 and 76 years was 0.85 and 0.78n/min respectively compared to 0.63 in DS group, Fig. 6B), which could be interpreted as (extreme) accelerated ageing in DS.

However, looking across other sleep metrics, the childhood DS group did not resemble individuals of older age, but was instead close to profiles seen in younger children. This held both for individual metrics such as absolute theta power (Fig. 6C) as well as for similarity across a composite of sleep metrics (Fig. 6D). That is, considering the broad profile of all age-dependent sleep metrics, the DS group (mean age of 8 years) was more similar to the average for children aged 6 years (i.e. consistent with a delay) rather than any older adolescent or adult group.

In absolute terms, the brain age gap was significantly larger across all NDD subgroups compared to the non-NDD NCH sample: DS (MAE = 2.48 years, p = 4 × 10-7), intellectual disability (MAE = 2.06 years, p = 3 × 10-5), CP (MAE = 1.6 years, p = 0.011), epilepsy (MAE = 1.57 years, p = 0.003), ASD (MAE = 1.46 years, p = 0.02) and ADHD (MAE = 1.3 years, p = 0.02) groups, consistent with greater levels of heterogeneity among NDD groups.

To gain some insight into which sleep alterations were driving these brain age discrepancies in NDD, we performed a series of exploratory analyses within NCH, comparing each NDD subgroup with the non-NDD sample, statistically controlling for i) the other, potentially overlapping NDD subgroups, ii) diagnoses of major sleep disorders, iii) contemporaneous use of medications likely to impact sleep, iv) age, v) sex, vi) race and vii) AHI (see Methods for details). The results for all NDD groups are provided in Supp. Tables 3–6.

Individuals with DS – the subgroup with the largest mean discrepancy between predicted and chronological age – showed the highest number of significant group differences and age-by-group interactions (Sup. Fig. 5). The largest effect sizes were for spindle characteristics – lower SS spindle density (1.1n/min in DS vs 1.9 in the non-NDD sample at F4, p < 10-15), duration (at F3 bst = -1.1, p < 10-15), less pronounced chirp (at C3 bst = 0.86, p < 10-15) and absolute beta power (at F3 during N2 bst = 0.94, p < 10-15, Supp. Tables 3–5). In terms of age-related trajectories, coupling between SO and spindle metrics were among those showing stronger age-by-group interactions. For example, coupling overlap between SO and SS at F3 significantly decreased with age in DS (Fig. 6F, r = -0.28, p = 0.02) but increased in the non-NDD sample (r = 0.17, p < 10-10).

Despite significant deviations in both MAE and ME, the number of altered sleep estimates were much lower in the intellectual disability group compared to the DS group; these were largely related to spindle frequency characteristics (Supp Fig. 5). SS density was associated with the degree of intellectual disability (the largest effect at C3 r = -0.39, p = 0.0009, Fig. 6G) and was in general decreased compared to individuals without NDD in frontal channels (the largest effect at F3 bst = -0.29, p = 0.003, Supp. Tables 5, Fig. 5H).

Finally, multiple nominally significant group and age-by-group alterations were detected for ADHD, CP and Epilepsy (Supp. Fig. 5, Sup. Table 3–6 contain full reports of uncorrected results). Except for ASD, all NDD-subgroups had common significant (p < 0.05) deficit of stage R sleep expressed as reduced duration (in DS), proportion (in intellectual disabilities) or both (in CP and epilepsy) and stage R latency (in ADHD) even after adjusting for sleep disorders and relevant medication.

3. Discussion

Using both clinical and research datasets, we identified consistent patterns of age-related change during childhood and adolescence, for multiple facets of sleep macro- and micro-architecture. We further showed that, when combined, the different sleep EEG metrics we examined could reliably predict an individual's age in pediatric populations, and that the resulting models were broadly transferable across different cohorts. Finally, we showed that some NDD subgroups (primarily DS) exhibited systematic differences in their predictions of age, reflecting multiple disruptions of sleep architecture.

3.1. Macro-architecture changes with age

Confirming numerous previous reports of decreasing total sleep time during the first two decades of life (Galland et al., 2012, Iglowstein et al., 2003, Ohayon et al., 2004), we additionally showed significant changes in other sleep macro-architecture metrics. Congruently with a large cross-sectional study of children without sleep complaints (Scholle et al., 2011), we found an increase in sleep cycle duration, stage R latency and a decrease in a number of sleep cycles with age. With respect to sleep efficiency, for which both age-related increases (Baker et al., 2012, Scholle et al., 2011) as well as decreases (Baker et al., 2016) have been reported, our findings pointed to a significant decline in both cohorts.

Major developmental changes were also observed with respect to sleep stage composition. Prior reports of developmental trajectories of NREM sleep showed consistent findings of an increase in N2 and a reduction in N3 with age (Baker et al., 2016, Jenni and Carskadon, 2004, Ohayon et al., 2004, Scholle et al., 2011, Tarokh et al., 2011, Tarokh and Carskadon, 2010), as found in our analyses. In contrast, the reports diverge for stage R sleep showing either an increase (Baker et al., 2016, Feinberg et al., 2012, Ohayon et al., 2004), no change (Tarokh et al., 2011, Tarokh and Carskadon, 2010), or a decrease (Scholle et al., 2011) in stage R percentage and/or duration. Our analysis – which represents the largest study to date – supports a decrease in R sleep across ages 2.5 to 17.5 years.

Despite statistically consistent associations with age, the age-adjusted absolute values of many macro-architectural metrics varied greatly between NCH and CHAT, a pattern observed in other contexts (e.g. comparing stage duration statistics between two elderly PSG cohorts, Djonlagic et al., 2021). Although such cohort effects may primarily reflect different recording contexts or other technical factors, rather than physiological differences between these populations, inasmuch as macro-architectural measures are susceptible to cohort-specific measurement biases, these issues may present challenges for the transferability of predictive models based on macro-architectural metrics.

3.2. EEG spectral composition changes with age

Multiple studies have reported age-related changes in absolute delta power during NREM sleep, suggesting an inverted-U trajectory: an increase from childhood to adolescence (Feinberg et al., 2012) and reduction from adolescence to adulthood (Baker et al., 2012, Baker et al., 2016, Feinberg and Campbell, 2013, Jenni and Carskadon, 2004, Tarokh and Carskadon, 2010). During R sleep, a linear decrease in delta power has also been documented (Feinberg & Campbell, 2013). Theta power has also been reported to decline with age across all sleep stages (Feinberg and Campbell, 2013, Gaudreau et al., 2001).

Despite limited number of channels, we confirmed topographical patterns in anterior-posterior direction of relative delta power change across a large and diverse sample. In a sample of 55 individuals (2.4–19.4 years), the scalp location with maximal SWA (1–4.5 Hz) power shifted with age in a posterior-to-anterior direction (Kurth et al., 2010a, Kurth et al., 2010b), mirroring the pattern of cortical thinning during childhood and adolescence(Shaw et al., 2008). Several studies utilizing both MRI and sleep EEG reported a link between slow-wave activity during NREM sleep and cortical thickness and/or gray matter volume (Buchmann et al., 2011, Goldstone et al., 2018). In general, the developmental decline in total power, as well as in delta and theta frequency bands, is not specific to sleep but also widely reported in wake (Barriga-Paulino et al., 2011, Boord et al., 2007, Gasser et al., 1988, Whitford et al., 2006), and has been similarly linked to reduction in cortical gray matter volume, and cerebral metabolic rate (Boord et al., 2007, Whitford et al., 2006). Thus, similar developmental changes in EEG spectral composition during both wake and sleep might reflect global structural changes in the brain, such as synaptic pruning that underlies normal cortical maturation and are not state-specific (Segalowitz et al., 2010).

In contrast to the general decline in total power, absolute sigma power increased with age. A similar increase was previously reported from 10 to 12 years in a longitudinal sample (Tarokh & Carskadon, 2010) but there are also reports of higher absolute sigma in children compared to adults (Gaudreau et al., 2001). Another longitudinal sample reported a complex trajectory of sigma power where it increased linearly from 6 to 12 years of age and then decreased from 12 to 16 (Feinberg and Campbell, 2013, Kurth et al., 2010a, Kurth et al., 2010b), matching our observations of inverted-U trajectories in spindle activity.

When normalized by total power, sigma power displayed one of the strongest age-related effects. During NREM sleep, this increase can be attributed to spindle maturation (discussed below). During R sleep, relative sigma power age-related increases were also accompanied by increases in relative alpha and beta power. While sleep studies reporting age-related changes in relative power are scarce, similar findings of an overall increase in higher frequencies are well reported during wake (Dustman et al., 1999, Gasser et al., 1988, Soroko et al., 2014), once again pointing to brain maturation processes evident across both sleep and wake.

Although, to our knowledge, there are no prior studies explicitly reporting age-related changes in relative power across classical frequency bands in sleep, our results indicate that relative power is a good metric to highlight topographical-, stage- and frequency-dependent aspects of developmental changes in the sleep EEG. For example, we observed distinct and sometimes contrasting trajectories between occipital and anterior channels as well as between NREM and R sleep stages (e.g. in delta and theta frequency bands), that can be practically leveraged when developing multi-channel tools for automatic stage classification in children and adolescents.

3.3. NREM microarchitecture

Rapid increases in spindle density during childhood have been linked to maturation of thalamocortical circuits (Fernandez & Lüthi, 2020). Our results tend to confirm previous reports of slow spindle density increasing during childhood and reaching its peak around puberty (Purcell et al., 2017, Scholle et al., 2007). SS duration also was shown to exhibit a similar developmental pattern, peaking slightly earlier than density, confirming previous reports (Scholle et al., 2007). In contrast, FS density increased through adolescence, congruent with recent studies (Goldstone et al., 2019, Purcell et al., 2017).

One of the most frequently reported findings is that the spindle frequency peak estimated from examination of the power spectrum increases with age (Campbell and Feinberg, 2016, Hahn et al., 2020, Tarokh et al., 2011). Although one early study showed that this is true for both frontal and centro-parietal spindles (Shinomiya et al., 1999), tracking the spindle frequency based on the sigma peak for both slow and fast spindle can be problematic given that the sigma peak in children falls within a range of slow spindles (Hoedlmoser et al., 2014). Alternatively, sigma peak increase could potentially reflect a shift in the ratio of slow versus fast spindle density with fast spindles becoming more prevalent after puberty. Our findings were based on spindle detection as discrete events, an approach that allowed us to estimate change in frequency for both slow and fast spindles in a more direct way. We showed that while SS frequency increases with age congruently with previous reports based on the sigma power peak, the FS frequency decreases. Interestingly, such a pattern appears to be opposite of the aging effect in aging adults (50 s to 80 s) where the SS and FS frequencies tend to diverge with SS becoming slower and FS faster (Djonlagic et al., 2020). A recent study that investigated spindles in three groups of adolescents found that both slow and fast spindles frequency was increased with age (Bocskai et al., 2022). However, in that study, slow and fast spindles were distinguished only using topographical differences (all spindles detected in frontal channels were declared slow and all spindles in centro-parietal channels fast).

We also report a previously undocumented finding for intra-spindle frequency change (or spindle “chirp”). This metric is usually negative, reflecting the characteristic deceleration of both SS and FS (Andrillon et al., 2011). Previous literature has suggested that chirp might be linked to spindle termination mechanisms and cortical modulation (Carvalho et al., 2014). We found that intra-spindle deceleration intensifies over childhood and adolescence, showing some of the most pronounced age-related changes in our study.

SOs also displayed some of the strongest changes within our cohort. SO rate per minute increased dramatically from childhood to adolescence while SO amplitude and slope decreased. Age-related decreases in SO amplitude and slope were reported previously in a small (N = 14) sample (Kurth, Jenni, et al., 2010). We also reported, however, that the interpretation of age-related trends is obligatorily highly dependent on the choice of the SO amplitude detection threshold. Prior work showed that coupling between SOs and spindles also increased from childhood to adolescence in longitudinal sample of 33 individuals (Hahn et al., 2020); this result was confirmed in our sample and extended to show that the strongest changes are for SS coupling magnitude.

3.4. Estimating brain age from the sleep EEG

We showed that individual differences in sleep macro- and micro-architecture can be summarized using simple methods to generate a highly accurate predictor of chronological age that is transferable across multiple independent samples. As has been widely employed by many groups using different brain imaging modalities (as well as epigenetics and other biomarkers), the difference between predicted “biological” age and observed chronological age can be interpreted as a measure of development and health (Franke & Gaser, 2019). In pediatric populations, studies using structural MRI (Franke et al., 2012, Hong et al., 2020) have been able to predict age with high accuracy (r > 0.9). Age has also been predicted using functional MRI (Li et al., 2018, Lund et al., 2022, Qin et al., 2015), albeit with lower accuracy (r = 0.54–––0.73). While a few studies using sleep EEG in adult cohorts achieve good results – r = 0.82–0.93 (Nygate et al., 2021, Sun et al., 2019), to our knowledge this is the first study to demonstrate this in a pediatric cohort.

One recent report used resting state wake EEG spectral power to estimate brain age in a cohort of 5–18 year-olds, with an average MAE of 1.2 years (Vandenbosch et al., 2019). This MAE is broadly similar to our results (MAE 1.1–1.3 years), although we note that are our results are based on performance in two independent samples, not only by means of cross-validation within the same sample, as in (Vandenbosch et al., 2019). Given that many spectral age-related changes were evident in wake as well as sleep, we might expect similar performance for age prediction using either wake or sleep EEG. However, it remains an open and empirical question as to how highly correlated brain age estimates are when based on different modalities (MRI versus EEG) or different physiological states (wake versus sleep). Note that better prediction of chronological age is not, in itself, necessarily the most relevant factor: as a trivial conceptual example, a model that achieved perfect prediction (r = 1, MAE = 0) would be useless. Furthermore, even if two approaches have identical performance with respect to prediction of chronological age per se, they may still yield very different results with respect to how the model residuals (i.e. the so-called brain age gap) relate to brain development and health.

To address the question of the biologically or clinically relevant properties of the brain age gap, we estimated it in NDD subgroups as well as non-NDD children. The largest deviations in both absolute and signed values (MAE and ME) were seen in individuals with DS and intellectual disability, with both groups showing negative gaps, consistent with delayed brain development. While we are not aware of other studies reporting on brain age in children with DS, a similar analysis was conducted for 46 adults (age range 28–––65 years) with DS using structural MRI. In contrast to our results, they reported a positive brain age gap interpreted as accelerated aging (Cole et al., 2017), finding that it was related to increased beta amyloid deposition and cognitive decline. With the use of DNA methylation levels to calculate an ‘epigenetic clock,’ Horvath and colleagues also pointed to accelerating biological aging in brain and blood tissue (Horvath et al., 2015), with evidence that such advanced ageing of blood samples begins prenatally (Xu et al., 2022). Likewise, as well as shorter life expectancies generally, adults with DS display older biological age based on multiple physiological measures (e.g. BMI, blood pressure, etc) (Nakamura & Tanaka, 1998). When cognitive and behavioral levels were assessed, however, individuals with DS tend to have lower developmental age (Gameren-Oosterom et al., 2011), similar to our findings of children with DS being the most similar to younger age children with respect to their sleep macro and microarchitecture. Such results support the notion that accelerated/decelerated aging patterns are not universal and can be tissue and system-specific (Horvath et al., 2015), as well as that brain age based on the sleep EEG may be reflective of cognitive and behavioral development.

Additional analyses controlling for chronological age revealed alterations in multiple sleep macro and micro-architecture metrics in the DS subgroup, many of which were the opposite of typical age-related changes, suggesting altered developmental patterns in DS. For example, we saw a global increase in absolute spectral power in DS versus an age-related decrease in the control groups. Likewise, individuals with DS had reduced SS, FS and SO density across ages 4 to 16, counter to the marked age-related increases in these metrics in this age range. With the exception of one report of increased higher total spectral power (Sibarani et al., 2022), the results of which we confirm here, a fuller assessment of sleep microarchitecture in DS has not been conducted and so our findings provide an important developmental perspective on abnormalities associated with DS.

We also observed a consistently younger functional pattern in the sleeping brain in individuals with intellectual disabilities. In terms of sleep microarchitecture, spindle frequency metrics expressed the most marked alterations. While it is hard to compare our findings to the existing literature due to scarcity of reports available, two reports concluded that children with intellectual disabilities – especially those with more severe impairments – had decreased spindle density based on visual detection (Shibagaki et al., 1980, Shibagaki and Kiyono, 1983). Our findings also pointed to reduction in SS density in ID, that was more profound with the higher degree of intellectual disability.

While other NDD subgroups – ASD, ADHD, CP and epilepsy – did not express consistent shifts towards either younger or older brain (ME), they all expressed larger brain age gaps in absolute terms (MAE). This may indicate considerable heterogeneity within these disorders: indeed, this has been previously reported in other contexts for ASD and ADHD (Dajani et al., 2016, Jeste et al., 2015) as well as CP (Rosello et al., 2021) and epilepsy (Pack, 2019). Alternatively, these results could reflect group-level differences in the sleep EEG leading to increased noise in the age prediction model, as NDD groups were excluded from the primary NCH model fitting. In general, future studies will be needed to fully evaluate the relative merits of different brain age metrics, and to determine whether ones based on the sleep EEG offer additional, unique information or not, as well as how brain age alterations may vary over the course of a disease. One important data-point to guide the development of possible clinical applications would be to determine how state-dependent (versus trait-like) these measures are: for example, considering children before versus after the onset of behavioral and cognitive symptoms, or in response to medication, or as a function of duration of illness.

Except ASD, all subgroups displayed alterations in stage R sleep, either in terms of absolute duration, relative duration or stage R latency. This is consistent with previous reports of stage R deficits in DS (Spanò et al., 2018), CP (Hayashi et al., 1990), epilepsy (Sadak et al., 2022), and intellectual disability (Esposito & Carotenuto, 2014), supporting the notion that R deficits are common characteristics across NDDs. Previous studies reported that lower R duration was associated with worse cognitive performance and mortality in older individuals (Djonlagic et al., 2020, Leary et al., 2020). As such, R sleep metrics may not be good candidates for condition-specific biomarkers, but rather reflect pathophysiological alterations shared between distinct disorders.

3.5. Caveats & conclusions

In summary, the present study provides a comprehensive assessment of age-related changes in sleep macro and microarchitecture, based on a large sample from multiple cohorts spanning the first two decades of life. Nonetheless, certain constraints should be mentioned. One obvious limitation is that the NCH data are from a database of clinical encounters. Based on comparison to CHAT, all primary age-related changes appeared qualitatively (and often quantitatively) conserved across studies, suggesting that these robust effects reflect fundamental developmental processes that may transcend diagnostic status. The subjects in CHAT either qualified for a diagnosis of obstructive sleep apnea, or snored and on that basis had some form of sleep-disordered breathing, meaning that the subjects though not necessarily referred to a sleep disorders center still most likely did not have normal sleep. Therefore, our comparisons may be limited due to lack of truly normative controls. Nonetheless, the presence of sleep-disordered breathing or other sleep disorders in many of the subjects who contributed data for the present analyses does not invalidate the high likelihood that sleep in these individuals still reflected many aspects of normal sleep development across childhood and adolescence. For example, we did not find evidence of significant effects of AHI on the age-related changes of sleep metrics or brain age prediction. Another limitation is the absence of detailed cognitive and behavioral data in the NDD cohort, precluding more direct investigations of how age-related changes in sleep track with development and any subclinical traits. Similarly, puberty status was not available, limiting our ability to interpret developmental differences not captured by age. Due to the absence of resting state data, we also could not account for developmental shifts in the alpha peak that could have effect on power in neighboring frequency bands. We acknowledge that while we report robust age-related changes in NREM micro-architecture across multiple datasets as proof-of-principle, the performance of this algorithm could likely be further improved by optimizing details of the analytic approach.

Our findings from retrospective clinical and clinical research data, meanwhile, appear to suggest very strong age-related changes across numerous sleep metrics in children ages 4 to 17 years, which could be robustly identified across independent samples despite the demographic, clinical and procedural/technical differences. As well as confirming previous reports based on smaller samples, we describe new metrics not previously studied from a developmental perspective, including stage- and channel-specific developmental trajectories of relative spectral power, intra-spindle frequency modulation, and temporal overlap between spindles and SOs. A model of multiple sleep metrics across different domains was able to predict chronological age with high precision in typically developing individuals, whereas this correspondence was lessened in individuals with NDDs, suggesting that these sleep metrics are sensitive to various functional abnormalities present in the (sleeping) brain. Taken together, our results indicated that sleep macro and microarchitecture offer important information about brain maturation which may facilitate a better understanding of the atypical neurodevelopment.

4. Methods

4.1. Participants

We primarily used PSG data from two pediatric samples – the Nationwide Children’s Hospital Sleep DataBank (NCH) and the Child Adenotonsillectomy Trial (CHAT) – both available via the National Sleep Research Resource (http://sleepdata.org). The NCH sample was created to facilitate pediatric sleep research. It was composed of patients (from infants to some adults) who underwent clinical PSG from 2017 to 2019 at the Nationwide Children’s Hospital (Lee et al., 2022), and contained diagnostic (ICD 9/10 codes) and medication data. All the data were de-identified prior to NSRR deposition, and received NCH Institutional Review Board exemption with HIPAA waiver.

The CHAT sample was derived from data collected from six US pediatric clinical centers as part of sleep screening procedures for a clinical trial of children aged 5 to 9 with snoring who were candidates for adenotonsillectomy. All participants were without severe chronic medical conditions or ADHD requiring medications (in total 12 participants had ADHD diagnosis), presented with snoring and were potential candidates for adenotonsillectomy (Marcus et al., 2013, Weinstock et al., 2014). All children (n = 1,244) were screened with a PSG (for detailed description of inclusion criteria for the data acquisition see Marcus et. al., 2013) and here we used the sample of all screened children excluding the follow-up recordings that were available for participants with mild to moderate obstructive apnea. Data collection for CHAT was approved by local Institutional Review Boards and written informed consent was obtained from each individual or their legal guardians.

Primary exclusion criteria for the NCH sample were i) age younger than 2.5 years (due to potential differences in infant and toddler EEGs), ii) age above 17.5 years (due to sparsity of data, and iii) a narcolepsy diagnosis (n = 42). In CHAT, we removed individuals for whom age information was missing (N = 19). In both samples, if the same individual had multiple recordings available, we only used a single (the first) recording.

For the brain age analyses only, to expand the age range of the validation set, we additionally included individuals (N = 1008) from the Pediatric Adenotonsillectomy Trial for Snoring (PATS) dataset comprised of children between 3 and 13 years with snoring but with an AHI < 3 (Wang et al., 2020). This allowed us to test the transferability of the model.

Cleveland Family Study sample (N = 730, including individuals between 6 and 88 years) was used for analysis testing the possibility of accelerated aging in the DS subgroup.

4.2. Clinical information for NCH sample

The DIAGNOSIS.csv file (available via NSRR) was used to delineate clinical sub-groups in the NCH sample. Following recommendations from the original description of the dataset (Lee et al., 2021), we only used final diagnosis codes (DX_ENC_TYPE & DX_SOURCE_TYPE columns equal to “Final Dx”). Since diagnostic codes provided for the sample were either according ICD9 or ICD10, we searched for specific diagnoses using the string search based on the diagnosis description (DX_NAME). For example, searching a string “[Aa]utis” and visually checking all unique matching diagnoses as well as the ICD codes. The information for all matching diagnoses for each condition is provided in the Supplementary table 1.

To control for medication use, we used records available in the MEDICATION.csv file. Specifically, we identified participants whose PSG was performed between the prescribed medication start and end date. We identified four therapeutic classes of medication out of 42 that could potentially affect sleep: 1) antihistamines, 2) psychotherapeutic drugs, 3) CNS drugs, including anticonvulsants, 4) hormones, and 5) sedatives/hypnotics. We summarized them by therapeutic class and subclass (THERA_CLASS and THERA_SUBCLASS), pharmaceutical class (PHARMA_CLASS).

4.3. EEG preprocessing

All steps of sleep EEG data processing were performed using Luna (http://zzz.bwh.harvard.edu/luna/), an open-source package developed by us (S.M.P). All NCH, CHAT and PATS PSGs contained six EEG channels (F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, O2). In CHAT, two temporal channels (T3, T4) were also available. The CFS cohort contained C3 and C4 EEG channels only. We first selected 30-seccond epochs of a particular stage (N2, N3, REM) according to manual, AASM-based staging (in the CFS it was performed using only central channels) in all datasets. Since the original sampling rates varied between and within the datasets (256–512 Hz in NCH and 200–512 Hz in CHAT, 200 Hz in PATS and 128 Hz in CFS), all EEG signals exceeding 200 Hz were datasets were down-sampled to 200 Hz and for CFS sample rate of 128 Hz was kept unchanged. In all datasets, EEG signals were referenced to contralateral mastoids (M1 or M2), converted to uV units and bandpass filtered between 0.5 and 35 Hz (raw signals were exported without any additional filters being set). Due to excessive line noise interference observed in many NCH samples, we applied an approach to remove it based on spectrum interpolation (Leske & Dalal, 2019), as implemented in Luna.

Next, within each stage, we identified all epochs with maximum amplitudes above 200 uV, or with flat or clipped signals for more than 10 % of the epoch; further, epochs were marked as outliers if they were i) more than 3 SDs from the mean (for that individual) of all channels for any of the three Hjorth parameters, activity, mobility and complexity (Hjorth, 1970), ii) 4 SDs from the mean of other epochs of the same channel or iii) 4 SDs from the mean of all epochs across all channels. Hjorth-based epoch outlier removal was performed twice for each individual. Channels and/or epochs were removed if more than 50 % of epochs were outliers. Such thresholds were selected empirically to remove gross artifacts from the signals but also to avoid removing too many epochs. The quality of the signals was inspected visually in several randomly selected EEG recordings and visualizing spectral power across frequency for all signals across all studies. The final averaged number of epochs per participant for the NCH dataset were –M(SD, range) – N2: 370 (95, from 35 to 869), N3: 230 (74, from 24 to 654), R: 151 (58, from 10 to 379); and for CHAT N2: 374 (89, from 96 to 686), N3: 282 (79, from 53 to 652), R: 165 (47, from 10 to 349).

4.4. Spectral power estimation

Spectral power was estimated using Welch’s method separately for N2, N3 and REM, summarized by classical frequency bands – slow (0.5–1 Hz), delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), sigma (12–15 Hz), beta (15–30 Hz), and total power (0.5 to 35 Hz). Specifically, for each 30-seccond epoch, we applied the Fast Fourier Transform with 4 s segments (0.25 Hz spectral resolution) windowing with a Tukey (50 %) taper, with consecutive segments overlapping by 50 % (2 s). We then averaged power across all segments per epoch. Subsequently, epoch-wise power was averaged across all epochs for a particular channel and stage. Relative power was computed with respect to the total absolute power. Absolute power values were then log-transformed prior to analysis.

4.5. Spindle detection

Motivated by recent findings that two classes of spindles – slow frontal and fast central – emerge as early as 18 months after birth (Kwon et al., 2022), we detected both separately. Slow and fast spindles were detected using 7-cycle wavelets with center frequencies of 11 Hz and 15 Hz (with approximately +/-2 Hz around each central frequency) respectively as previously described (Purcell et al., 2017, Warby et al., 2014). Specifically, putative spindles were identified based on temporally smoothed (window duration = 0.1 s) wavelet coefficients (from a complex Morlet wavelet transform) using following criteria. Intervals exceeding 1) 4.5 times the mean for at least 300 ms and also 2) 2 times the mean for at least 500 ms were selected as putative spindles. Intervals over 3 s were rejected; consecutive intervals within 500 ms were merged (unless the resulting spindle was greater than 3 s). Subsequently, additional quality check (QC) procedure was applied. Putative spindles were discarded if the relative increase in non-spindle bands activity (delta, theta, and beta) was greater than the relative increase in spindle frequency activity (i.e. relative to all N2 sleep), thereby ensuring putative spindles preferentially reflect sigma band activity, and not general increases in signal amplitude, which is often due to artifact or other non-spindle activities. Based on the set of spindles that passed QC, we computed spindle density (count per minute), amplitude, duration, observed frequency, and chirp (intra-spindle frequency change computed as a difference in frequency between the first and the last half of a spindle with zero values meaning no change and negative values meaning intra-spindle frequency deceleration).

4.6. SO detection

Zero-crossings were identified based on the EEG signals band-pass filtered between 0.5 and 4 Hz. To define putative SO the following temporal criteria were satisfied: 1) a consecutive zero-crossing leading to negative peak was between 0.3 and 1.5 s; 2) a zero-crossing leading to positive peak were not longer than 1 s. With respect to amplitude criteria, two separate approaches were used, similar to Djonlagic et al (2020). First, an adaptive/relative threshold (our default) such that negative peak and peak-to-peak amplitudes were required to be greater than twice the mean (for that individual/channel). Second, an absolute threshold requiring a negative peak amplitude larger −40 uV, and peak-to-peak amplitude larger then 75 uV. SO density (count per minute) as well as the mean amplitude of the negative peak, peak-to-peak amplitude, duration and the upward slope of negative peak were estimated for each channel.

4.7. Coupling between SO and spindles

For each channel we identified spindles that overlapped with detected SO and characterized their coupling using the following three metrics. First, we computed the proportion of spindles that overlapped with a SO (“gross overlap”). Using the filter-Hilbert method, we also estimated SO phase at the spindle peak, which was averaged (circular mean) across SOs for each channel (coupling angle at spindle peak). In addition, the inter-trial phase clustering assessed the consistency of non-uniform phase coupling between SO and spindles (coupling magnitude). Overlap and magnitude metrics were z-transformed using a null distribution of same metrics generated during 10,000 random permutations where time indices of the time series were shuffled in a manner that preserved the overall number of SOs, spindles and the gross overlap between SO and spindles (the latter is true only for the coupling magnitude).

4.8. Exclusion criteria based on sleep data

For all used datasets (NCH, CHAT, PATS and CFS), an additional exclusion criterion applied for the macro-architecture analysis was TST < 180 mins. For analysis of spectral power and spindles, additional exclusion criteria were applied: i) N of available epochs for each stage (N2, N3, REM) after outlier removal less than 10, ii) persistent line noise interference (SPK measure > 5 SD in least one channel at any stage), iii) outlier spectral power at 1 Hz (<4 SD or > 4 SD in least one channel at any stage) to target movement, ocular artifacts, general low signal to noise ratio, or at 25 Hz (>4 SD in least one channel at any stage) to target muscle activity artifacts.

Signal polarity flips were observed in a portion of recordings in all samples (for details on polarity in several NSRR samples: https://zzz.bwh.harvard.edu/luna/vignettes/nsrr-polarity/) and additional exclusion criteria were necessary for analyses dependent on signal polarity – those involving SO and coupling between SO and spindles. Recordings with ambiguous polarity were removed for these analyses (-1 < T_DIFF < 1 at C3 or C4 during N2 stage from Luna's POL command) and polarity of all recordings with T_DIFF > 1 at C3 or C4 during N2 stage was flipped.

Final sample size and demographic characteristics for the primary samples used (for NCH and CHAT) are in Table 1. Characteristics of the final analytic sample of PATS that was included for brain age prediction were: total N = 627, with 307 females, 384 White, 198 Black or African American individuals and 45 individuals of other ancestry, with a mean age of 6.3 years (range 3 – 13 years); and for CFS: total N = 635, with 361 females, 271 Whites, 343 Blacks and 21 individuals of other ancestry, with a mean age of 39.4 years (range 6.7 – 88.5 years).

4.9. Statistical analysis

We used linear regression models of each sleep metric regressed on age, also controlling for sex and race/ethnicity. Outliers (using a 3 SD criterion) were removed for each sleep metric (repeated twice). In a control analysis we added Apnea-hypopnea index (AHI) computed as an average number of apnea and/or hypopnea events per hour of sleep as an additional covariate to linear regression models, which yielded almost identical results (Pearson correlation between signed -log 10p-value controlling and not controlling for AHI was > 0.99 in both NCH and CHAT samples). We also performed similar analysis using arousal index (AI) for a subset of individuals from the CHAT sample (N = 374, age range 5–10 years) for whom precomputed AI was available on NSRR. To describe effect sizes of age-related changes in sleep metrics, we also calculated Pearson correlations between sleep metrics and age. For metrics with suspected non-linear trajectories, we used Akaike’s and Bayesian information criteria to formally test if the quadratic model was a better fit to describe age-related change.

In analyses of NCH clinical subgroups, we used linear regression models controlling for race, sex, AHI, co-occurring sleep diagnoses, other NDD diagnoses and medication use. For Sup. Fig. 5, prior to running a linear regression analysis, 3-SD outliers were replaced with NAs in two rounds and sleep metrics’ estimates were z-transformed to obtained standardized linear regression coefficients. P-values were adjusted for multiple comparisons (all sleep estimates n = 321) separately for each subgroup and tested effect (subgroup, subgroup by age interaction) using FDR method (Benjamini & Yekutieli, 2001).

To predict individuals' ages, we trained a multiple linear regression model using the sleep metrics studied here. After excluding subjects from NDD subgroups, we randomly split the non-NDD NCH sample into training (70 % of subjects) and held-out (30 %) sets. CHAT, PATS and NDD-NCH subgroups sample were retained as additional, independent testing sets. To reduce the number of features for prediction model, we removed highly correlated variables (abs r > 0.9 in non-NDD NCH training set). Remaining features (list is provided in Supp. Table 7) for all datasets were z-transformed using the mean and standard deviation of the training set. Sex and race were included as covariates in all models. Initially, the model performance in the non-NDD NCH training sample was estimated using 10-fold cross validations using mean explained variance (R2) and mean absolute error and their SD across folds. Further, the model was tested in four held-out validation sets (non-NDD NCH held-out sample, CHAT, PATS and NDD NCH set) and pearson’s correlation, mean absolute error and mean error between predicted and true chronological age were reported.

We additionally applied an alternative to conventional linear regression: specifically, gradient descent boosting machines as implemented in the LightGBM machine learning library as implemented in Luna. Performance was near identical to the linear regression model, in this particular case, and so we retained the simpler model, as performance in terms of age prediction was already high.

In the NDD NCH subgroups analysis of brain age gap we estimated the variability in the prediction of results by repeating the step of fitting the model 100 times with different, non-overlapping individuals from non-NDD NCH randomly assigned to held-out testing and training set each round. The NDD NCH subgroups were kept the same. Estimates of model performance – mean absolute error and mean error between predicted and true age – were then averaged across 100 rounds, as well as their min and max values. We used a two-sample t-test to test if there was significant difference in MAE and ME (mean error) between non-NDD NCH held-out testing set and each clinical subgroup in each round and reported the median p-value in Fig. 5 D.

We applied the following steps to estimate similarity in all sleep variables (macroarchitecture, absolute and relative band power, spindles, SO estimates and coupling metrics) across different age bins of combined sample of PATS and CFS cohorts and DS group (Fig. 6D). First, we z-scored all sleep variables across individuals of the combined sample and NCH sample using mean and standard deviation of the combined sample of each sleep metric Then, we defined age bins in the combined sample (two-year non-overlapping windows centered at 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13 and four-year non-overlapping windows centered at 16, 20, …, 80 years). Larger age windows for older ages were chosen due to expectation of lower rate of change. Another reason was to ensure that there were sufficient number of participants for each bin (at least 10). Then means were computed for each sleep metric across individuals belonging to a particular age bin in the combined sample, as well as the DS subgroup and non-NDD group of the NCH sample. Finally, the average absolute difference between DS (and non-NDD) means and each age bin means were computed and plotted as a function of age.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

N. Kozhemiako: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. A.W. Buckley: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. R.D. Chervin: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing. S. Redline: Conceptualization, Resources, Writing – review & editing. S.M. Purcell: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft, Funding acquisition.

Declaration of competing interest

The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests: Dr. Chervin reports the following financial and non-financial potentially competing interests: consultant (through contract with Michigan Medicine) for Eli Lilly & Company; editor and author, UpToDate; officer and board member for International Pediatric Sleep Association; member, advisory board for a non-profit organization, The Pajama Program. Other authors report no conflict of interests.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH)/National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Grant R03 MH108908 (to S.M.P.), NIH/National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) Grants R01 HL146339 and R21 HL145492 (to S.M.P.), and the NIH/National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities (NIMHD) Grant R21 MD012738 (to S.M.P.).

This research was supported (in part) by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute of Mental Health ZIC MH00296206.

The National Sleep Research Resource was supported by the U.S. National Institutes of Health, National Heart Lung and Blood Institute (R24 HL114473, 75N92019R002). NCH Sleep DataBank was supported by the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01EB025018.

The Childhood Adenotonsillectomy Trial (CHAT) was supported by the National Institutes of Health (HL083075, HL083129, UL1-RR-024134, UL1 RR024989). The Cleveland Family Study (CFS) was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (HL46380, M01 RR00080-39, T32-HL07567, RO1-46380.

Footnotes

Appendix A

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2023.103552.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

The following are the Supplementary data to this article:

Supplementary data 1
mmc1.docx (5MB, docx)

Data availability

NCH-SDB & CHAT are available via NSRR (http://sleepdata.org). PATS will be posted on NSRR at a future date. Luna software is open and available via http://zzz.bwh.harvard.edu/luna

References

  1. Andrillon T., Nir Y., Staba R.J., Ferrarelli F., Cirelli C., Tononi G., Fried I. Sleep Spindles in Humans: Insights from Intracranial EEG and Unit Recordings. J. Neurosci. 2011;31(49):17821–17834. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2604-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baecker L., Garcia-Dias R., Vieira S., Scarpazza C., Mechelli A. Machine Learning for Brain Age Prediction: Introduction to Methods and Clinical Applications. eBioMedicine. 2021;72 doi: 10.1016/j.ebiom.2021.103600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Baker F.C., Turlington S.R., Colrain I. Developmental changes in the sleep electroencephalogram of adolescent boys and girls. J. Sleep Res. 2012;21(1):59–67. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00930.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baker F.C., Willoughby A.R., Massimiliano, de Z., Franzen P.L., Prouty D., Javitz H., Hasler B., Clark D.B., Colrain I.M. Age-Related Differences in Sleep Architecture and Electroencephalogram in Adolescents in the National Consortium on Alcohol and Neurodevelopment in Adolescence Sample. Sleep. 2016;39(7):1429–1439. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baker F.C., Yűksel D., de Zambotti M. In: Sleep Disorders in Women: A Guide to Practical Management. Attarian H., Viola-Saltzman M., editors. Springer International Publishing; 2020. Sex Differences in Sleep; pp. 55–64. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  6. Barriga-Paulino C.I., Flores A.B., Gómez C.M. Developmental changes in the EEG rhythms of children and young adults: Analyzed by means of correlational, brain topography and principal component analysis. J. Psychophysiol. 2011;25(3):143–158. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000052. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Becker S.P. ADHD and sleep: Recent advances and future directions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2020;34:50–56. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Benjamini Y., Yekutieli D. The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. Ann. Stat. 2001;29(4):1165–1188. doi: 10.1214/aos/1013699998. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bocskai G., Pótári A., Gombos F., Kovács I. The adolescent pattern of sleep spindle development revealed by HD-EEG. J. Sleep Res. 2022;n/a(n/a) doi: 10.1111/jsr.13618. e13618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Boord P.R., Rennie C.J., Williams L.M. INTEGRATING “BRAIN” AND “BODY” MEASURES: CORRELATIONS BETWEEN EEG AND METABOLIC CHANGES OVER THE HUMAN LIFESPAN. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2007;06(01):205–218. doi: 10.1142/S0219635207001416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buchmann A., Ringli M., Kurth S., Schaerer M., Geiger A., Jenni O.G., Huber R. EEG Sleep Slow-Wave Activity as a Mirror of Cortical Maturation. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;21(3):607–615. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Campbell I.G., Feinberg I. Maturational Patterns of Sigma Frequency Power Across Childhood and Adolescence: A Longitudinal Study. Sleep. 2016;39(1):193–201. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Campbell I.G., Grimm K.J., de Bie E., Feinberg I. Sex, puberty, and the timing of sleep EEG measured adolescent brain maturation. PNAS. 2012;109(15):5740–5743. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1120860109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carvalho D.Z., Gerhardt G.J.L., Dellagustin G., de Santa-Helena E.L., Lemke N., Segal A.Z., Schönwald S.V. Loss of sleep spindle frequency deceleration in Obstructive Sleep Apnea. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2014;125(2):306–312. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cole J.H., Leech R., Sharp D.J., Initiative, for the A. D. N. Prediction of brain age suggests accelerated atrophy after traumatic brain injury. Ann. Neurol. 2015;77(4):571–581. doi: 10.1002/ana.24367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cole J.H., Annus T., Wilson L.R., Remtulla R., Hong Y.T., Fryer T.D., Acosta-Cabronero J., Cardenas-Blanco A., Smith R., Menon D.K., Zaman S.H., Nestor P.J., Holland A.J. Brain-predicted age in Down syndrome is associated with beta amyloid deposition and cognitive decline. Neurobiol. Aging. 2017;56:41–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.04.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dajani D.R., Llabre M.M., Nebel M.B., Mostofsky S.H., Uddin L.Q. Heterogeneity of executive functions among comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders. Sci. Rep. 2016;6(1):Article 1. doi: 10.1038/srep36566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Djonlagic I., Mariani S., Fitzpatrick A.L., Van Der Klei V.M.G.T.H., Johnson D.A., Wood A.C., Seeman T., Nguyen H.T., Prerau M.J., Luchsinger J.A., Dzierzewski J.M., Rapp S.R., Tranah G.J., Yaffe K., Burdick K.E., Stone K.L., Redline S., Purcell S.M. Macro and micro sleep architecture and cognitive performance in older adults. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020 doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00964-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dustman R.E., Shearer D.E., Emmerson R.Y. Life-span changes in EEG spectral amplitude, amplitude variability and mean frequency. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1999;110(8):1399–1409. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00102-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Esposito M., Carotenuto M. Intellectual disabilities and power spectra analysis during sleep: A new perspective on borderline intellectual functioning. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res. 2014;58(5):421–429. doi: 10.1111/jir.12036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Feinberg I., Campbell I.G. Longitudinal sleep EEG trajectories indicate complex patterns of adolescent brain maturation. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 2013;304(4):R296–R303. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00422.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Feinberg I., Davis N.M., de Bie E., Grimm K.J., Campbell I.G. The maturational trajectories of NREM and REM sleep durations differ across adolescence on both school-night and extended sleep. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 2012;302(5):R533–R540. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00532.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fernandez L.M.J., Lüthi A. Sleep Spindles: Mechanisms and Functions. Physiol. Rev. 2020;100(2):805–868. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00042.2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Franke, K., & Gaser, C. (2019). Ten Years of BrainAGE as a Neuroimaging Biomarker of Brain Aging: What Insights Have We Gained? Frontiers in Neurology, 10. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2019.00789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  25. Franke K., Luders E., May A., Wilke M., Gaser C. Brain maturation: Predicting individual BrainAGE in children and adolescents using structural MRI. Neuroimage. 2012;63(3):1305–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Galland B.C., Taylor B.J., Elder D.E., Herbison P. Normal sleep patterns in infants and children: A systematic review of observational studies. Sleep Med. Rev. 2012;16(3):213–222. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2011.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gandal M.J., Leppa V., Won H., Parikshak N.N., Geschwind D.H. The road to precision psychiatry: Translating genetics into disease mechanisms. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19(11):1397–1407. doi: 10.1038/nn.4409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gasser T., Verleger R., Bächer P., Sroka L. Development of the EEG of school-age children and adolescents. I. Analysis of band power. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1988;69(2):91–99. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90204-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gaudreau H., Carrier J., Montplaisir J. Age-related modifications of NREM sleep EEG: From childhood to middle age. J. Sleep Res. 2001;10(3):165–172. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2001.00252.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Goldstone A., Willoughby A.R., de Zambotti M., Franzen P.L., Kwon D., Pohl K.M., Pfefferbaum A., Sullivan E.V., Müller-Oehring E.M., Prouty D.E., Hasler B.P., Clark D.B., Colrain I.M., Baker F.C. The mediating role of cortical thickness and gray matter volume on sleep slow-wave activity during adolescence. Brain Struct. Funct. 2018;223(2):669–685. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1509-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Goldstone A., Willoughby A.R., de Zambotti M., Clark D.B., Sullivan E.V., Hasler B.P., Franzen P.L., Prouty D.E., Colrain I.M., Baker F.C. Sleep spindle characteristics in adolescents. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2019;130(6):893–902. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gorgoni M., Scarpelli S., Reda F., De Gennaro L. Sleep EEG oscillations in neurodevelopmental disorders without intellectual disabilities. Sleep Med. Rev. 2020;49 doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2019.101224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hahn M.A., Heib D., Schabus M., Hoedlmoser K., Helfrich R.F. Slow oscillation-spindle coupling predicts enhanced memory formation from childhood to adolescence. Elife. 2020;9:e53730. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hayashi M., Inoue Y., Iwakawa Y., Sasaki H. REM sleep abnormalities in severe athetoid cerebral palsy. Brain Dev. 1990;12(5):494–497. doi: 10.1016/S0387-7604(12)80214-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hjorth B. EEG analysis based on time domain properties. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1970;29(3):306–310. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(70)90143-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hoedlmoser K., Heib D.P.J., Roell J., Peigneux P., Sadeh A., Gruber G., Schabus M. Slow Sleep Spindle Activity, Declarative Memory, and General Cognitive Abilities in Children. Sleep. 2014;37(9):1501–1512. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hong J., Feng Z., Wang S.-H., Peet A., Zhang Y.-D., Sun Y., Yang M. Brain Age Prediction of Children Using Routine Brain MR Images via Deep Learning. Front. Neurol. 2020;11 doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.584682. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fneur.2020.584682 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Horvath S., Garagnani P., Bacalini M.G., Pirazzini C., Salvioli S., Gentilini D., Di Blasio A.M., Giuliani C., Tung S., Vinters H.V., Franceschi C. Accelerated epigenetic aging in Down syndrome. Aging Cell. 2015;14(3):491–495. doi: 10.1111/acel.12325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Iglowstein I., Jenni O.G., Molinari L., Largo R.H. Sleep Duration From Infancy to Adolescence: Reference Values and Generational Trends. Pediatrics. 2003;111(2):302–307. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.2.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jenni O.G., Carskadon M.A. Spectral Analysis of the Sleep Electroencephalogram During Adolescence. Sleep. 2004;27(4):774–783. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.4.774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jeste S.S., Frohlich J., Loo S.K. Electrophysiological biomarkers of diagnosis and outcome in neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2015;28(2):110–116. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kamara D., Beauchaine T.P. A Review of Sleep Disturbances among Infants and Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2020;7(3):278–294. doi: 10.1007/s40489-019-00193-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Knoop M.S., de Groot E.R., Dudink J. Current ideas about the roles of rapid eye movement and non–rapid eye movement sleep in brain development. Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(1):36–44. doi: 10.1111/apa.15485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kopasz M., Loessl B., Hornyak M., Riemann D., Nissen C., Piosczyk H., Voderholzer U. Sleep and memory in healthy children and adolescents – A critical review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2010;14(3):167–177. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kurth S., Jenni O.G., Riedner B.A., Tononi G., Carskadon M.A., Huber R. Characteristics of Sleep Slow Waves in Children and Adolescents. Sleep. 2010;33(4):475–480. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.4.475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kurth S., Ringli M., Geiger A., LeBourgeois M., Jenni O.G., Huber R. Mapping of Cortical Activity in the First Two Decades of Life: A High-Density Sleep Electroencephalogram Study. J. Neurosci. 2010;30(40):13211–13219. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kwon, H., Walsh, K. G., Berja, E. D., Manoach, D. S., Eden, U. T., Kramer, M. A., & Chu, C. J. (2022). Normative sleep spindle database and findings from 772 healthy children from birth through 18 years (p. 2022.03.31.486476). bioRxiv. doi: 10.1101/2022.03.31.486476.
  48. Larson A.M., Ryther R.C.C., Jennesson M., Geffrey A.L., Bruno P.L., Anagnos C.J., Shoeb A.H., Thibert R.L., Thiele E.A. Impact of pediatric epilepsy on sleep patterns and behaviors in children and parents. Epilepsia. 2012;53(7):1162–1169. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03515.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Leary E.B., Watson K.T., Ancoli-Israel S., Redline S., Yaffe K., Ravelo L.A., Peppard P.E., Zou J., Goodman S.N., Mignot E., Stone K.L. Association of Rapid Eye Movement Sleep With Mortality in Middle-aged and Older Adults. JAMA Neurol. 2020;77(10):1241–1251. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2020.2108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lee, H., Li, B., DeForte, S., Splaingard, M., Huang, Y., Chi, Y., & Linwood, S. L. (2021). NCH Sleep DataBank: A Large Collection of Real-world Pediatric Sleep Studies. arXiv:2102.13284 [Eess, Stat]. http://arxiv.org/abs/2102.13284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  51. Lee H., Li B., DeForte S., Splaingard M.L., Huang Y., Chi Y., Linwood S.L. A large collection of real-world pediatric sleep studies. Sci. Data. 2022;9(1), Article 1 doi: 10.1038/s41597-022-01545-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Leske S., Dalal S.S. Reducing power line noise in EEG and MEG data via spectrum interpolation. Neuroimage. 2019;189:763–776. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Li, H., Satterthwaite, T. D., & Fan, Y. (2018). Brain age prediction based on resting-state functional connectivity patterns using convolutional neural networks. 2018 IEEE 15th International Symposium on Biomedical Imaging (ISBI 2018), 101–104. doi: 10.1109/ISBI.2018.8363532. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  54. Lund M.J., Alnæs D., de Lange A.-M.-G., Andreassen O.A., Westlye L.T., Kaufmann T. Brain age prediction using fMRI network coupling in youths and associations with psychiatric symptoms. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2022;33 doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Marcus C.L., Moore R.H., Rosen C.L., Giordani B., Garetz S.L., Taylor H.G., Mitchell R.B., Amin R., Katz E.S., Arens R., Paruthi S., Muzumdar H., Gozal D., Thomas N.H., Ware J., Beebe D., Snyder K., Elden L., Sprecher R.C.…Childhood Adenotonsillectomy Trial (CHAT) A randomized trial of adenotonsillectomy for childhood sleep apnea. N. Engl. J. Med. 2013;368(25):2366–2376. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1215881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Nakamura E., Tanaka S. Biological ages of adult men and women with Down’s syndrome and its changes with aging. Mech. Ageing Dev. 1998;105(1):89–103. doi: 10.1016/S0047-6374(98)00081-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nygate Y., Rusk S., Fernandez C., Glattard N., Arguelles J., Shi J., Hwang D., Watson N. 543 EEG-Based Deep Neural Network Model for Brain Age Prediction and Its Association with Patient Health Conditions. Sleep. 2021;44(Supplement_2):A214. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab072.541. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ohayon M.M., Carskadon M.A., Guilleminault C., Vitiello M.V. Meta-Analysis of Quantitative Sleep Parameters From Childhood to Old Age in Healthy Individuals: Developing Normative Sleep Values Across the Human Lifespan. Sleep. 2004;27(7):1255–1273. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.7.1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Pack A.M. Epilepsy Overview and Revised Classification of Seizures and Epilepsies. CONTINUUM: Lifelong Learning in Neurology. 2019;25(2):306. doi: 10.1212/CON.0000000000000707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Purcell S.M., Manoach D.S., Demanuele C., Cade B.E., Mariani S., Cox R., Panagiotaropoulou G., Saxena R., Pan J.Q., Smoller J.W., Redline S., Stickgold R. Characterizing sleep spindles in 11,630 individuals from the National Sleep Research Resource. Nature. Communications. 2017;8(1):Article 1. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Qin J., Chen S.-G., Hu D., Zeng L.-L., Fan Y.-M., Chen X.-P., Shen H. Predicting individual brain maturity using dynamic functional connectivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015;9 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00418. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00418 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Robinson-Shelton A., Malow B.A. Sleep Disturbances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2015;18(1):6. doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0638-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Rosello M., Caro-Llopis A., Orellana C., Oltra S., Alemany-Albert M., Marco-Hernandez A.V., Monfort S., Pedrola L., Martinez F., Tomás M. Hidden etiology of cerebral palsy: Genetic and clinical heterogeneity and efficient diagnosis by next-generation sequencing. Pediatr. Res. 2021;90(2):Article 2. doi: 10.1038/s41390-020-01250-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Sadak U., Honrath P., Ermis U., Heckelmann J., Meyer T., Weber Y., Wolking S. Reduced REM sleep: A potential biomarker for epilepsy – a retrospective case-control study. Seizure. 2022;98:27–33. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2022.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Scholle S., Zwacka G., Scholle H.C. Sleep spindle evolution from infancy to adolescence. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007;118(7):1525–1531. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Scholle S., Beyer U., Bernhard M., Eichholz S., Erler T., Graneß P., Goldmann-Schnalke B., Heisch K., Kirchhoff F., Klementz K., Koch G., Kramer A., Schmidtlein C., Schneider B., Walther B., Wiater A., Scholle H.C. Normative values of polysomnographic parameters in childhood and adolescence: Quantitative sleep parameters. Sleep Med. 2011;12(6):542–549. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Segalowitz S.J., Santesso D.L., Jetha M.K. Electrophysiological changes during adolescence: A review. Brain Cogn. 2010;72(1):86–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Shaw P., Kabani N.J., Lerch J.P., Eckstrand K., Lenroot R., Gogtay N., Greenstein D., Clasen L., Evans A., Rapoport J.L., Giedd J.N., Wise S.P. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 2008;28(14):3586–3594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Shibagaki M., Kiyono S. Duration of spindle bursts during nocturnal sleep in mentally retarded children. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1983;55(6):645–651. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(83)90274-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Shibagaki M., Kiyono S., Watanabe K. Nocturnal sleep in severely mentally retarded children: Abnormal eeg patterns in sleep cycle. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1980;49(3):337–344. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90228-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Shinomiya S., Nagata K., Takahashi K., Masumura T. Development of Sleep Spindles in Young Children and Adolescents. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 1999;30(2):39–43. doi: 10.1177/155005949903000203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Sibarani C.R., Walter L.M., Davey M.J., Nixon G.M., Horne R.S.C. Sleep-disordered breathing and sleep macro- and micro-architecture in children with Down syndrome. Pediatr. Res. 2022;91(5):Article 5. doi: 10.1038/s41390-021-01642-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Simard-Tremblay E., Constantin E., Gruber R., Brouillette R.T., Shevell M. Sleep in Children With Cerebral Palsy: A Review. J. Child Neurol. 2011;26(10):1303–1310. doi: 10.1177/0883073811408902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Soroko S.I., Shemyakina N.V., Nagornova Z.V., Bekshaev S.S. Longitudinal study of EEG frequency maturation and power changes in children on the Russian North. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 2014;38:127–137. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.08.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Souders M.C., Zavodny S., Eriksen W., Sinko R., Connell J., Kerns C., Schaaf R., Pinto-Martin J. Sleep in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2017;19(6):34. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0782-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Spanò G., Gómez R.L., Demara B.I., Alt M., Cowen S.L., Edgin J.O. REM sleep in naps differentially relates to memory consolidation in typical preschoolers and children with Down syndrome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2018;115(46):11844–11849. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811488115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Stiles J., Jernigan T.L. The Basics of Brain Development. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2010;20(4):327–348. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Stores G., Stores R. Sleep disorders and their clinical significance in children with Down syndrome. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2013;55(2):126–130. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04422.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sun H., Paixao L., Oliva J.T., Goparaju B., Carvalho D.Z., van Leeuwen K.G., Akeju O., Thomas R.J., Cash S.S., Bianchi M.T., Westover M.B. Brain age from the electroencephalogram of sleep. Neurobiol. Aging. 2019;74:112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Surtees A.D.R., Oliver C., Jones C.A., Evans D.L., Richards C. Sleep duration and sleep quality in people with and without intellectual disability: A meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2018;40:135–150. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Tarokh L., Carskadon M.A. Developmental Changes in the Human Sleep EEG During Early Adolescence. Sleep. 2010;33(6):801–809. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.6.801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Tarokh L., Van Reen E., LeBourgeois M., Seifer R., Carskadon M.A. Sleep EEG Provides Evidence that Cortical Changes Persist into Late Adolescence. Sleep. 2011;34(10):1385–1393. doi: 10.5665/SLEEP.1284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Thapar A., Cooper M., Rutter M. Neurodevelopmental disorders. The Lancet. Psychiatry. 2017;4(4):339–346. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30376-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Tononi G., Cirelli C. Sleep and the Price of Plasticity: From Synaptic and Cellular Homeostasis to Memory Consolidation and Integration. Neuron. 2014;81(1):12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. van Gameren-Oosterom H.B.M., Fekkes M., Buitendijk S.E., Mohangoo A.D., Bruil J., Wouwe J.P.V. Development, Problem Behavior, and Quality of Life in a Population Based Sample of Eight-Year-Old Children with Down Syndrome. PLoS One. 2011;6(7):e21879. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Vandenbosch, M. M. L. J. Z., van ’t Ent, D., Boomsma, D. I., Anokhin, A. P., & Smit, D. J. A. (2019). EEG-based age-prediction models as stable and heritable indicators of brain maturational level in children and adolescents. Human Brain Mapping, 40(6), 1919–1926. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  87. Wang R., Bakker J.P., Chervin R.D., Garetz S.L., Hassan F., Ishman S.L., Mitchell R.B., Morrical M.G., Naqvi S.K., Radcliffe J., Riggan E.I., Rosen C.L., Ross K., Rueschman M., Tapia I.E., Taylor H.G., Zopf D.A., Redline S. Pediatric Adenotonsillectomy Trial for Snoring (PATS): Protocol for a randomised controlled trial to evaluate the effect of adenotonsillectomy in treating mild obstructive sleep-disordered breathing. BMJ Open. 2020;10(3):e033889. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-033889. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Warby S.C., Wendt S.L., Welinder P., Munk E.G.S., Carrillo O., Sorensen H.B.D., Jennum P., Peppard P.E., Perona P., Mignot E. Sleep-spindle detection: Crowdsourcing and evaluating performance of experts, non-experts and automated methods. Nat. Methods. 2014;11(4):Article 4. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Weinstock T.G., Rosen C.L., Marcus C.L., Garetz S., Mitchell R.B., Amin R., Paruthi S., Katz E., Arens R., Weng J., Ross K., Chervin R.D., Ellenberg S., Wang R., Redline S. Predictors of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Severity in Adenotonsillectomy Candidates. Sleep. 2014;37(2):261–269. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Whitford T.J., Rennie C.J., Grieve S.M., Clark C.R., Gordon E., Williams L.M. Brain maturation in adolescence: Concurrent changes in neuroanatomy and neurophysiology. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2006;28(3):228–237. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Xu K., Li S., Muskens I.S., Elliott N., Myint S.S., Pandey P., Hansen H.M., Morimoto L.M., Kang A.Y., Ma X., Metayer C., Mueller B.A., Roberts I., Walsh K.M., Horvath S., Wiemels J.L., de Smith A.J. Accelerated epigenetic aging in newborns with Down syndrome. Aging Cell. 2022;21(7):e13652. doi: 10.1111/acel.13652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Zhang G.-Q., Cui L., Mueller R., Tao S., Kim M., Rueschman M., Mariani S., Mobley D., Redline S. The National Sleep Research Resource: Towards a sleep data commons. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association: JAMIA. 2018;25(10):1351–1358. doi: 10.1093/jamia/ocy064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary data 1
mmc1.docx (5MB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

NCH-SDB & CHAT are available via NSRR (http://sleepdata.org). PATS will be posted on NSRR at a future date. Luna software is open and available via http://zzz.bwh.harvard.edu/luna


Articles from NeuroImage : Clinical are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES