SUMMARY
Response to threatening environmental stimuli requires detection and encoding of important environmental features that dictate threat. Aversive events are highly salient, which promotes associative learning about stimuli that signal this threat. The nucleus accumbens is uniquely positioned to process this salient, aversive information and promote motivated output, through plasticity on the major projection neurons in the brain area. We describe a nucleus accumbens core local circuit whereby excitatory plasticity facilitates learning and recall of discrete aversive cues. We demonstrate that putative nucleus accumbens substance P release and long-term excitatory plasticity on dopamine 2 receptor-expressing projection neurons are required for cue-dependent fear learning. Additionally, we find that fear learning and recall is dependent on distinct projection neuron subtypes. Our work demonstrates a critical role for nucleus accumbens substance P in cue-dependent aversive learning.
In brief
Belilos et al. find that nucleus accumbens core substance P through cell-type-selective plasticity is required for cue-dependent aversive learning. These findings implicate substance P signaling in aversive learning and response, which may underlie the development of anxiety and mood disorders.
Graphical abstract
INTRODUCTION
Aversive stimuli are inherently salient, as animals need to recognize these threats to survive. Consequently, aversive stimuli elicit rapid learning about and response to cues that predict aversive outcomes. Canonically, the major projection neurons of the nucleus accumbens (NAc) are studied in terms of motivational or incentive salience, a cognitive process that facilitates approach or avoidance in response to a stimulus. These responses, however, require recognition of a salient stimulus to promote these motivated outcomes. The NAc responds to salient cues in humans1 and is required for aversive learning,2,3 which is cue dependent in the NAc core,3–5 suggesting that the NAc core may facilitate Pavlovian learning independently of the motivated response. If true, it is likely consolidation and subsequent recall of these salient events requires long-term plasticity to facilitate persistent memories. These NAc-dependent synaptic mechanisms to enable recognition of threatening, aversive stimuli have yet to be determined.
Within the NAc, medium spiny projection neuron (MSN) subtypes are thought to promote opposing outcomes, with dopamine 1 (D1) receptor MSN activity driving rewarding and positive outcomes and dopamine 2 (D2) receptor MSN activity driving aversive outcomes, suppressed reward, and punishment.6–15 This understanding has been updated, and it is now known MSN subtype activity participates in more complex and pivotal roles in guiding learned behavior. In addition, NAc core D2-MSNs have been shown to be required for discrimination,16 reversal learning,17,18 and reward.19–21 These additional known roles of D2-MSNs may hinge on the ability of D2-MSNs to alter their response to salient stimuli. Thereby, stimulus-dependent activity (e.g., through potentiation of excitatory transmission on D2-MSNs) during learning may provide a means of facilitating learning about aversion-associated stimuli.
We recently uncovered a mechanism whereby glutamatergic long-term excitatory potentiation (LTP) is elicited on D2-MSNs exclusively by activity of D1-MSNs22 and required the activation of cholinergic interneurons by release of the neuropeptide substance P. Both acetylcholine (ACh) and substance P are released by salient stimuli, including drugs of abuse and aversive stimuli,23,24 and these neuromodulators contribute to many of the same behaviors as activation of D2-MSNs. Additionally, phasic activity changes of cholinergic neurons in response to cue-dependent associative outcomes have been observed in primates25,26 and rodents.27 Like D2-MSN neuronal activity, striatal ACh is necessary for behavioral flexibility,28 discrimination,29 and reversal learning30 and aids in the expression of cue-motivated behavior.31 These similarities and the fact that this circuit promotes plasticity position this circuit as a likely candidate for signaling aversion-related outcomes. Therefore, we hypothesized that this NAc core substance P is necessary for supporting aversive fear learning through plasticity on D2-MSNs.
In this study, we aimed to determine synaptic and circuit mechanisms that underpin NAc core-dependent aversive learning. Using pharmacology and gene-knockout technology, we found that substance P receptors in cholinergic neurons were required for cue-dependent aversive learning, in a fear conditioning context. These findings were corroborated through enhanced D1-MSN activity and ACh release in response to aversive foot shocks and D2-MSN activity in response to learned cues. Disrupting this circuit at any synaptic and cellular node diminished learning about cues. Our work provides perspective on how MSN subtypes interact to facilitate learning about aversive cues associated with foot shock.
RESULTS
Neurokinin 1 receptor signaling is required for cue-dependent aversive learning
As substance P is released by strongly salient stimuli23,24 and is involved in many of the same aversive outcomes as D2-MSNs activation, we first tested whether putative substance P release and binding to its primary receptor, the neurokinin 1 (NK1) receptor, was necessary for aversive learning. A Pavlovian fear conditioning model was used as a model of aversive learning, in which freezing was the main measure of learning (Figure 1A). To determine if substance P was required for associative aversive learning, mice were injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) with the brain penetrant NK1 receptor antagonist L-733,060 (10 mg/kg) prior to fear conditioning. NK1 receptor antagonism suppressed cue-dependent but not context-dependent freezing (Figure 1B), without affecting locomotion (Figure S1). Intraperitoneal injection of L-733,060 prior to cue recall had no effect on cue recall (Figure 1C). These results suggest that substance P signaling during aversive cue conditioning is required for the full expression of fear learning.
Figure 1. NAc substance P (SP) signaling is required for cue-dependent aversive learning.
(A) Fear conditioning paradigm for aversive learning.
(B) Injection of the NK1 receptor antagonist L-733,060 (10 mg/kg) or saline was given prior to conditioning. NK1 receptor antagonist suppressed freezing in response to the cue during cue recall (p < 0.001), and no difference in freezing was observed in the context (p > 0.05) (n = 20 and 22 mice).
(C) L-733,060 prior to cue recall does not change the percentage of time freezing to conditioned cues (p > 0.05) (n = 7 and 8 mice).
(D) Fluorescent image of EGFP-KASH expression in the NAc core. Scale bar, 100 μm. Inset: expression of EGFP-KASH (green), Chat RNA (red), Tacr1 RNA (gray/white), and DAPI (blue). CRISPR-Cas9-mediated knockout of Tacr1 decreased measured Tacr1 RNA puncta in Chat RNA-positive cells (p < 0.001) but not Chat RNA-negative cells (p > 0.05) (n = 4 mice per group).
(E) Graph and representative traces showing Cas9 and Tacr1-gRNA was required to suppress SP-dependent activation of ChAT cells (interaction p < 0.0001; post hoc baseline vs. SP ChAT-Cre/Cas9Tacr1 p > 0.05, mice/cells [N/n] = 4/10; baseline vs. SP ChAT-CreTacr1, ChAT-Cre/Cas9, or ChAT-Cre p < 0.0001) (N/n = 4/9, 4/8, and 3/7).
(F) Tacr1 knockout decreased freezing to the cue (p < 0.001) but not the conditioning context (p > 0.05) (n = 15 and 13 mice).
(G) Local NAc infusion of L-733,060 during conditioning suppressed freezing to conditioned cues (p < 0.01) but not the conditioned context (p > 0.05) (n = 7 and 8 mice).
Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001. For detailed statistics see Table S1. See also Figure S1.
The NK1 receptor is found throughout the brain, including in regions known to be associated with fear conditioning (e.g., amygdala and periaqueductal gray). In the NAc, the NK1 receptor (encoded by the Tacr1 gene) is found in all choline acetyl-transferase (ChAT)-expressing cholinergic cells. Therefore, we used a CRISPR-Cas9 system, generated an adeno-associated virus (AAV)-expressing guide RNA (gRNA) for the Tacr1 gene (Tacr1-gRNA), and injected it in the NAc core of ChAT-Cre/Cas9 mice, which express Cas9 exclusively ChAT cells. RNAscope in situ hybridization was used to assess the efficacy of the knockout. As we were unable to separate ChAT cells expressing or not expressing the Tacr1 gRNAs (see STAR Methods), all Chat-expressing cells in the NAc core for which GFP expression was observed were quantified for Tacr1 expression. A significant reduction in the Tacr1 transcript in Chat-positive cells was observed, and no difference was observed in Tacr1 expression in Chat-negative cells (Figure 1D). Additionally, a functional and near complete inhibition of substance P-induced increase in ChAT firing was observed in mice expressing Cas9 and the Tacr1-gRNAs (Figure 1E). Next, we tested the functional knockout in fear-conditioning behavior. NK1 receptor knockout suppressed cue-dependent but not context-dependent freezing (Figure 1F), suggesting that the cue-dependent reduction in freezing due to NK1 receptor antagonism was indeed an effect selective for the NAc core. In agreement with this finding, local NAc core infusion of L-733,060 during conditioning was capable of suppressing cue-dependent but not context-dependent recall compared with saline-infused mice (Figure 1G). Overall, these experiments indicate that substance P signaling is required for the full expression of cue-dependent recall of aversion-associated cues.
D1-MSNs are required for cue-dependent fear conditioning
The primary source of substance P in the striatum is D1-MSNs, which exclusively express and release the peptide, and we have demonstrated that activation of D1-MSNs releases substance P.22 As substance P signaling is required for aversive learning and substance P is released in the striatum in response to salient aversive stimuli,23 we predicted that D1-MSN activity would increase in response to salient, aversive stimuli. To selectively assay the activity of D1-MSNs, Dyn-Cre mice were injected with AAV-DIO-GCaMP7f and implanted with fibers in the NAc core for assessment of in vivo calcium activity with fiber photometry as a means of assessing correlated neural activity (Figure 2A). We first acquired signals in response to random interval, unpredictable foot shocks. D1-MSN calcium activity scaled with the magnitude of foot shock (Figures 2B and 2C) but did not significantly vary across shock trials (Figure 2D). This scaling was not affected by injection of L-733,060 (Figure S2A). Next, we assayed activity during conditioning and cue recall. During conditioning, foot shock but not the novel cue increased D1-MSN calcium activity (Figure 2E). D1-MSN calcium activity was not increased by the cue during cue recall (Figure 2F), despite an increase in freezing (Figure S2B). Additionally, individual freeze events were not associated with changes in D1-MSN calcium activity (Figure S2C). We then scaled foot-shock intensity to determine if the magnitude of the foot shock controlled both the D1-MSN calcium response and subsequent learning. As expected, freezing to the associated cue increased with foot-shock intensity (Figure 2G). Interestingly, the area under the curve (AUC) of the shock-induced calcium signal during conditioning correlated significantly with percentage freezing (Figure 2H). Taken together, these results indicate the strength of D1-MSN activity during conditioning may be necessary for scaling cue-dependent aversive learning and recall.
Figure 2. D1-MSN activity is required for cue-dependent conditioning.
(A) Representation of a Dyn-Cre mouse injected with the calcium indicator GCaMP7f and a representative image of its expression in the NAc core with a fiber track. Scale bar, 200 μm.
(B) Heatmaps and time plots representing average Z scores of D1-MSN calcium-driven fluorescence over increasing foot-shock intensity (0.1–1.0 mA).
(C) AUC from GCaMP7f fluorescent signals increased with shock intensity (interaction p < 0.0001, n = 8–10 mice per shock intensity).
(D) No difference was observed across individual shock trials at 0.6 mA (p > 0.05, n = 10 mice).
(E) Averaged Z score D1-MSN calcium activity during fear conditioning.
(F) Average Z score D1-MSN calcium activity in response to the condition cue during cue recall.
(G) Conditioning foot-shock intensity increasingly enhances freezing to the cue during recall (p < 0.0001).
(H) AUC of the D1-MSN GCaMP7f signal significantly correlates with freezing to the cue during recall (p < 0.01) (n = 5–13 mice per group).
(I) Expression of eNpHR3.0-eYFP, fiber track for optogenetic inhibition of the NAc core, and cartoon displaying bilateral implantation and expression of the optogenetic construct in mice.
(J) Inhibition of D1-MSNs during the cue in conditioning suppressed later cue recall (p < 0.01) but did not affect context recall (p > 0.05) (n = 11 and 10 mice).
(K) Halorhodopsin inhibition of D1-MSNs during cue recall had no effect on subsequent context recall.
(L) Inhibition of D1-MSNs during cue recall had no effect on freezing (laser off vs. on, p > 0.05) (n = 9 and 10 mice).
Data are represented as mean ± SEM. **p < 0.01. For detailed statistics see Table S1. See also Figure S2.
To test the necessity of D1-MSN activity during cue-dependent aversive learning, we used a temporally selective optogenetic strategy to inhibit D1-MSNs during cue-dependent behavior. Dyn-Cre mice were injected with AAV-DIO-eNpHR3.0 and implanted with optogenetic fibers in the NAc core (Figure 2I). Optogenetic inhibition of D1-MSNs did not affect locomotion (Figures S2D and S2E). D1-MSNs were inhibited selectively for 10 s during the cue and shock during conditioning. D1-MSN inhibition during the cue and shock in conditioning diminished cue recall but not context recall (Figure 2J). In another group of mice, we selectively inhibited D1-MSNs during every other 10 s cue to determine if D1-MSN inhibition reversibly inhibited cue-dependent freezing. No change in cue recall was observed from D1-MSN inhibition (Figures 2K–2L). Altogether, these results indicate D1-MSN activity contributes to cue-dependent learning but not recall.
Substance P signaling is required for shock-driven ACh release
Substance P is released from D1-MSNs in response to salient stimuli23,32 and has a role in aversion.33,34 Additionally, substance P release promotes increased activity of ChAT neurons via the NK1 receptor.22,35 In addition, previous studies have indicated that ChAT neurons exhibit phasic activity patterns in response to aversive stimuli in vivo.26 Therefore, it was predicted that substance P would cause ChAT activity and ACh release in vivo. An AAV containing the GPCR-activation-based sensor for ACh (GRAB-ACh3.0)36 was injected into the NAc core of mice and expressed in neurons (Figure 3A). Ex vivo slice studies demonstrated bath application of low (1 μM) and high (100 μM) concentrations of ACh significantly increase bulk fluorescence intensity in a concentration-dependent manner (Figure S3A). To evaluate ACh binding in vivo, the virus was injected, a fiber was implanted in the NAc core, and fluorescent transients were observed using fiber photometry. In an open-field chamber, mice were injected with the broad muscarinic antagonist scopolamine, which has been observed to block ACh binding to the GRAB-ACh3.0 sensor.36 Scopolamine significantly diminished the average magnitude of the GRAB-ACh signal and suppressed the amplitude of all transients (Figure S3B). These results verify that the GRAB-ACh3.0 sensor causes fluorescence through ACh release and binding to the GRAB-ACh3.0 receptor sensor.
Figure 3. Acetylcholine release during foot shock is substance P dependent.
(A) Representative image of GRAB-ACh3.0 expression in the NAc core and cartoon of fiber placement.
(B) Heatmaps and time courses of fluorescent GRAB-ACh3.0 signals from 10 random foot shocks at intensities of 0.1–1.0 mA amplitudes. Time courses are taken from average Z scores from shock 1 and shock 10.
(C and D) AUC measurement of acetylcholine release at varying shock intensities for shock 1 and shock 10. (C) No differences were observed during the AUC (inverse to make positive) during pauses in acetylcholine-mediated GRAB-ACh3.0 fluorescence across shock intensities or repeated shocks (p > 0.05). (D) Acetylcholine release varied across both AUC and shock intensity during the post-shock rebound (cue 1 vs. cue 10, p < 0.05) (n = 9 mice).
(E) Heatmap and time course of GRAB-ACh3.0 fluorescent response after L-733,060 administration.
(F) L-733,060 had no effect on the size of the pause across shocks (p > 0.05).
(G) NK1 receptor antagonism suppressed AUC of the acetylcholine signal varied across both drug treatment (AUC saline vs. L-733,060 during cue 1, p < 0.05) and shock presentation (cue 1 vs. cue 10, p < 0.05) (n = 5 and 6 mice).
(H and I) Phasic changes in GRAB-ACh3.0 fluorescence during aversive cue conditioning in saline- and L-733,060-treated mice.
(J) L-733,060 significantly decreases the rebound AUC GRAB-ACh3.0 fluorescence during cue conditioning (p < 0.05, n = 8 and 12).
(K) The AUC during the rebound in conditioning significantly correlated with percentage freezing to the cue during recall (p < 0.01, n = 12 mice).
Data are represented as mean ± SEM. For detailed statistics see Table S1. *p < 0.05. See also Figure S3.
Next, to determine if ACh is released by aversive stimuli in a substance P-dependent manner, foot shocks (10 total) were given at random, unpredictable intervals in behavior naive mice while recording GRAB-ACh3.0 fluorescent activity in the NAc core. Shock alone produced a decrease in fluorescence (“pause”) followed by an increase (“rebound”) in ACh-mediated fluorescence (Figure 3B), suggesting that ACh is released in a multi-phasic pattern. This phasic pause and rebound has been observed in rewarding stimuli in other studies.37 Similar to D1-MSN calcium activity, increasing the intensity of the shock increased the amount of rebound ACh fluorescence following the shock without significantly changing the AUC of the pause, despite habituation to repeated shock (Figures 3C and 3D). Unlike D1-MSNs, this ACh release habituated to the shock over repeated presentations (Figures 3C and 3D). Next to determine if the NK1 receptor was responsible for the rebound increase in ACh fluorescence, L-733,060 was injected (i.p.), and mice were exposed to random foot shocks at 0.6 mA. Antagonism of the NK1 receptor attenuated the increase in ACh-mediated fluorescence AUC during the rebound only (Figures 3E and 3G) and did not diminish habituation to the shock in the rebound condition (Figure 3G). These results suggest that aversive stimuli increase ACh release via substance P after foot shock and that ACh release scales with the intensity of an aversive stimulus.
To determine how aversive-conditioned stimuli affect this signal, mice injected with GRAB-ACh3.0 in the NAc core were run through fear conditioning. Three prominent phasic events were observed in ACh-mediated fluorescence: a response to the novel cue, a pause, and a rebound (Figure 3H). As in the condition of the shock alone, the rebound response was attenuating (Figure S3C). The rebound fluorescence, but not the other features, was significantly diminished in mice treated with L-733,060 compared with saline-injected mice (Figures 3I and 3J). During cue recall, ACh fluorescence was only increased at the termination of the cue in saline treated mice but was increased at the start of the cue and the termination of the cue in L-733,060-treated mice (Figures S3C and S3D). This increase in ACh fluorescence observed in L-733,060-treated mice is attenuated after the first presentation of the cue during cue recall (Figure S3D). This may suggest that the cue itself still appears to be novel to the mice with NK1 receptor antagonism. Interestingly, the rebound AUC significantly correlated with the time spent freezing to the cue during cue recall (Figure 3K). Overall, these results indicate that the increase in fluorescence following the shock is important for learning about cues predicting aversive foot shock.
Shock-induced ACh is important for learning about a conditioned aversive cue
To determine the necessity of NAc core ChAT cells are influencing learning about cues predicting foot shocks, we injected ChAT-Cre mice with an AAV-DIO-eNpHR3.0 or AAV-DIO-eYFP construct and inhibited ChAT neurons at two different time points during fear conditioning to align inhibition to time windows of GRAB-ACh fluorescence: during the conditioning cue or immediately after the foot shock (Figure 4A). Inhibition of ChAT cells had no observed effects on locomotion (Figure S4). Mice that received inhibition during the cue showed no change in context or cue recall compared with eYFP controls (Figure 4B). However, 5 s of inhibition of ChAT cells following the shock, during the observed rebound, significantly diminished freezing to cues predicting shocks (Figure 4C), suggesting ACh from ChAT neurons during the rebound condition is necessary for the full expression of aversive cue recall.
Figure 4. ChAT-mediated cholinergic signaling is required for cue-dependent aversive learning.
(A) Expression of halorhodopsin in ChAT cells in the NAc core. Schematic showing inhibition paradigms: during the cue in conditioning or after the shock and cue during conditioning.
(B) ChAT inhibition during the cue had no effect on context or cue recall (p > 0.05, n = 6 mice per group).
(C) Inhibition of ChAT cells following the cue and shock during conditioning significantly decreased freezing to the cue during recall (p < 0.001) but did not affect context recall (p > 0.05) (n = 5 and 9).
(D) Selective infusion of the muscarinic 1 receptor antagonist pirenzepine (bilateral) prior to conditioning suppressed cue-dependent (p < 0.01) but not conditioning context-dependent (p > 0.05) freezing (n = 10 and 9).
(E) Infusion of pirenzepine prior to cue recall had no effect on freezing (p > 0.05) in mice showing similar context recall (n = 6 and 7).
Data are represented as mean ± SEM. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. For detailed statistics see Table S1. See also Figure S4.
Substance P causes LTP of excitatory transmission on D2-MSNs through enhanced ACh release and activation of the muscarinic 1 receptor signaling pathway.22 This mechanism would provide a cell-type-specific mechanism for enhancing activity of D2-MSNs to either promote aversive outcomes or to facilitate learning about aversive stimuli. To first determine if aversive conditioning and subsequent ACh release acts through muscarinic 1 receptors on D2-MSNs, mice were implanted with cannulas in the NAc core and infused with the muscarinic 1 receptor-like antagonist pirenzepine (4 ng/μL, bilateral) prior to conditioning or prior to cue retrieval. Compared with saline infused controls, cue recall was suppressed when pirenzepine was infused prior to conditioning (Figure 4D) but not prior to cue recall (Figure 4E).
Substance P promotes plasticity on D2-MSNs during cue-dependent aversive learning
D2-MSNs are likely to receive this excitatory muscarinic 1 receptor signaling more strongly, as D1-MSNs also express the inhibitory muscarinic 4 receptor, which could suppress this signaling. To determine if muscarinic 1 receptor plasticity occurred on D2-MSNs in a substance P-dependent manner, we conditioned mice to a cue after injection with saline or L-733,060 and conducted whole-cell electrophysiology recordings 1 day after cue recall. Substance P-dependent LTP on D2-MSNs is induced primarily through insertion of GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors.22 These GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors are sensitive to intracellular peptides at positive membrane potentials and excitatory synaptic currents at positive potentials show inward rectification when these receptors have been inserted in the synaptic membrane. Therefore, we assessed inward rectification of electrically evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents (EPSCs), which would be suggestive of increased membrane insertion of GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors. Mice that received conditioning (cue + shock) displayed significant rectification at +40 mW compared with non-conditioned mice (cue only) (Figure 5A). Additionally, mice injected with L-733,060 displayed no difference in rectification compared with non-conditioned mice, suggesting that suppressing substance P signaling was responsible for LTP observed on D2-MSNs (Figure 5A). As a secondary measure, we compared the sensitivity of electrically evoked currents on D2-MSNs from conditioned and non-conditioned mice to the selective GluR2-lacking AMPA receptor antagonist 1-naphthyl acetyl spermine trihydrochloride (NASPM) (100 μM). NASPM significantly diminished currents in conditioned mice relative to non-conditioned mice, verifying the presence of more GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors (Figure 5B). Furthermore, amplitude, but not frequency of spontaneous EPSCs (sEPSCs) was increased on D2-MSNs (Figure 5C). Shock-only mice or context-only mice displayed no changes in inward rectification on D2-MSNs (Figure S4). D1-MSNs did not display inward rectification from any groups assayed (Figures 5D and S4). Results indicate signatures of substance P-dependent LTP are expressed selectively in D2-MSNs following cued aversive learning.
Figure 5. Excitatory plasticity on D2-MSNs is caused by cue-dependent aversive learning.
(A) Cue/shock conditioning enhanced inward rectification of the I-V curve generated from electrically evoke excitatory currents on D2-MSNs (interaction p < 0.001) from the cue/shock group compared with cue only (post hoc p < 0.05) and cue/shock with L-733,060 (post hoc p < 0.05). Rectification index indicated more rectification for D2-MSNs from cue/shock mice (interaction p < 0.01) compared with cue only (post hoc p < 0.01) and cue/shock with L-733,060 (post hoc, p < 0.05) (N/n [mice/cells] = 5/10, 5/10, and 4/7).
(B) NASPM suppressed currents more from D2-MSNs from cue/shock mice compared with cue only mice (25–30 min average, p < 0.05) (N/n = 4/6 and 4/5).
(C) Spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic current (sEPSC) amplitude (p < 0.05) but not frequency (p > 0.05) is increased in D2-MSNs following fear conditioning (N/n = 6/14, 5/9, and 4/7).
(D) D1-MSNs do not show inward rectification across conditions (p > 0.05) (N/n = 6/8, 4/9, and 4/5).
Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. For detailed statistics see Table S1. See also Figure S5.
D2-MSN activity is necessary for recall of aversive-conditioned cues
If LTP is selectively expressed on D2-MSNs and is dependent on aversive cue associations, then it is likely that D2-MSNs are active in response to aversively conditioned cues. To verify this activity in real time and in vivo, A2a-cre mice were injected with AAV-DIO-GCaMP7f in the NAc core and implanted with fibers for fiber photometry, A2a-Cre mice were injected with AAV-DIO-GCaMP7f and implanted with fibers for photometry (Figure 6A) and subjected to 10 unpredictable foot shocks at various foot-shock intensities. D2-MSNs responded similarly to all foot-shock intensities (Figure 6B), and L-733,060 did not alter D2-MSN calcium response to foot shock (Figure S6A). In contrast to D1-MSNs, D2-MSN calcium activity did not increase with increasing foot-shock intensity (Figure 6C), and responses were variable across shock events (Figure 6D). Like D1-MSNs, D2-MSNs responded to foot shock during conditioning but not the cue (Figure 6E). Unlike D1-MSNs, D2-MSNs were activated during cue recall (Figure 6F) in response to the associated aversive cue, and this signal did not habituate over repeated plays of the cue (Figure S6B). Furthermore, D2-MSN calcium activity did not increase in response to individual freezing events, suggesting that freezing alone did not explain D2-MSN calcium signals (Figure S6C). To determine if NK1 receptors were required during conditioning to cause this increase to the cue, L-733,060 was injected in mice prior to conditioning. NK1 receptor antagonism did not affect D2-MSN calcium response to the shock during conditioning (Figure 6G). Next, mice conditioned in the presence of L-733,060 were exposed to conditioned cues. Photometry mice injected with L-733,060 showed significantly reduced freezing to the conditioned cue compared with saline-injected mice (Figure S6D). D2-MSN calcium activity did not increase in response to conditioned cues in L-733,060-treated mice (Figures 6H and 6I). In addition, AUC during the cue correlated with freezing (Figure 6J). Taken together, this suggests that NK1 receptors are required for D2-MSN activity in response to conditioned aversive cues.
Figure 6. D2-MSNs are required for cue-dependent aversive learning.
(A) A cartoon of an A2a-Cre mouse injected with the calcium indicator GCaMP7f and a representative image of its expression in the NAc core with a fiber track. Scale bar, 200 μm.
(B) Heatmaps and time plots representing average Z scores of D2-MSN calcium-driven fluorescence over increasing foot-shock intensity (0.1–1.0 mA).
(C) AUC of an average of all 10 foot shocks indicated was no change in response across foot-shock intensity (p > 0.05).
(D) AUC fluorescence from D2-MSN GCaMP7f does not change across all foot shock trials at 0.6 mA intensity (p > 0.05) (n = 10 mice).
(E) D2-MSNs responded to the first conditioning shock but not the cue.
(F) Following conditioning, calcium signals from D2-MSNs were responsive to the conditioned cue.
(G) D2-MSN calcium signals responded to the foot shock during conditioning after being injected with L-733,060.
(H) Cue-driven calcium transients from D2-MSNs were not observed in mice injected with L-733,060.
(I) AUC fluorescent transients during cue recall from mice expressing GCaMP7f in D2-MSNs were larger than in mice expressing GCaMP7f in D1-MSNs (p < 0.01) and in mice injected with L-733,060 expressing GCaMP7f in D2-MSNs (p < 0.05) (n = 11, 8, and 8 mice).
(J) AUC during the cue significantly correlated with freezing to the cue (p < 0.05, n = 8 and 8 mice).
(K) eNpHR3.0-eYFP expression in D2-MSNs within the NAc core and cartoon depicting bilateral inhibition of the NAc core.
(L) Inhibition of D2-MSNs during the cue in conditioning decreased freezing during context recall (p < 0.05).
(M) Inhibition of D2-MSNs reversibly suppressed freezing to recalled cues (over cues p < 0.01; laser off vs. on p < 0.01; post hoc laser on eYFP vs. eNpHR3.0 p < 0.01) (n = 8 and 8 mice).
(N and O) Inhibition of D2-MSNs reversibly suppressed freezing to recalled cues (over cues p < 0.01; laser off vs. on p < 0.0001; post hoc laser on eYFP vs. eNpHR3.0 p < 0.05) in mice that showed no difference in context recall (p < 0.05) (n = 9 and 6 mice).
Data are represented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. For detailed statistics see Table S1. See also Figure S6.
Next, to determine if D2-MSN activity was required for recall of associated cues, AAV-DIO-eNpHR3.0 was injected, and fibers were implanted into the NAc of A2a-cre mice for selective optogenetic inhibition of D2-MSNs (Figure 6K). To compare the necessity of D2-MSNs to D1-MSNs in conditioning, mice received optogenetic inhibition of D2-MSNs during both cues during conditioning (Figure 6K). Surprisingly, inhibition of D2-MSNs during the cue in conditioning significantly diminished freezing in the context (Figure 6L). As expected, D2-MSN inhibition during conditioning had no effect on freezing to cues during cue recall when D2-MSNs were not inhibited (Figure 6M). Despite this, inhibition of D2-MSNs during cue recall diminished freezing (Figure 6M), suggesting that D2-MSNs are required for the full expression of freezing to aversively conditioned cues. To test this outcome without the potential confounds of inhibition during cue conditioning, D2-MSNs were inhibited during the 10 s cue on every other cue (Figure 6N). D2-MSN inhibition reversibly inhibited freezing to the cue (Figure 6O) without affecting freezing to the context (Figure 6N). This suppression in freezing was not dependent on locomotor effects (Figures S6E and S6F). Taken together, associative aversive stimuli promote activity of D2-MSNs in response to associative cues and this response is required expression of cue-dependent aversive learning.
DISCUSSION
Our work has demonstrated a di-synaptic, local circuit within the NAc core that is critical for processing aversive cues, with necessary roles for NAc core D1-MSN and D2-MSN activity at different stages of learning and recall (Figure 7). Although the activity does not appear to be required for the full expression of the fear behavior, activity of these subtypes significantly modulates the overall expression of the cue-dependent behavior, suggesting that it does indeed play a critical role in learning. We have shown that D1-MSN activity and substance P are required for learning during cue-dependent aversive conditioning and that D2-MSNs are required for recall of conditioned cues. This D2-MSN activity change is likely due to excitatory LTP22 produced through muscarinic receptor activation, which would enhance the likelihood of D2-MSN activation during excitatory input that encodes salient stimuli. Overall, our results provide evidence for the involvement of the NAc core in Pavlovian learning that might support later motivated output.
Figure 7. Model for the role of the NAc core in cue-dependent fear learning.
NAc circuitry involved in aversive cue learning (left). Shock activates D1-MSNs releasing substance P and activating ChAT neurons. ChAT neurons release acetylcholine selectively during cue/shock pairings and promotes glutamatergic long-term potentiation (LTP) on D2-MSNs rendering them more responsive to the cue. Timing of putative activity and release of neurotransmitters from NAc neuronal subtypes during cue conditioning and recall (right).
This work is consistent with previous studies showing MSNs respond to different aspects of associative reward conditions, in which D1-MSNs respond to outcomes and D2-MSNs respond to associative cues.21 We found similar outcomes in response to an aversive stimulus, in which D2-MSNs were active in response to a learned cue but not to novel cues and D1-MSNs were active in response to the shock alone. Together these findings indicate that MSN subtypes are responding to salient events, including rewarding events, further supporting the hypothesis that the NAc core supports learning about salient cues.1 Furthermore, the duration of MSN subtype responses is temporally different in both rewarding21 and aversive-associated stimuli. D1-MSNs rapidly respond to the non-associative, salient stimuli, and D2-MSNs tend to display extended activity following associative cue presentation. We speculate that D2-MSNs may be actively engaged in either retaining expression of the learned association or suppressing additional associations. More work will be needed to test the temporal nature of D1-MSN and D2-MSN activity in response to associative conditioning and learning.
We provide a role for D1-MSNs and the neuropeptide substance P in mediating aversive learning. D1-MSNs are thought to facilitate reward; however, the data presented here and data from others assessing activity in the NAc core38,39 and NAc shell40,41 suggest that aversive stimuli activate D1-MSNs. Our data indicate that this activity participates in associative learning through the release of peptides, activation of cholinergic neurons, and release of ACh. Interestingly, scaling up D1-MSN activity appears to scale ACh release, which is consistent with increasing substance P levels.22 Although ACh may act as an attentional signal facilitating the salience of associative stimuli,42,43 we have discovered that substance P works upstream to promote ChAT activation, ACh release, and plasticity on D2-MSNs, which may be dependent on the concentration of neuromodulator release in response to scaled stress. This plasticity facilitates information transfer from excitatory brain regions to the NAc core. As the effect of substance P and ACh plasticity is cue dependent but not context dependent, it is likely that extensive excitatory projections arising from limbic regions that are involved in Pavlovian conditioning, including the basolateral amygdala44,45 or other regions involved in aversion, such as the paraventricular thalamus,46 may facilitate this D2-MSN activity during cue recall. In contrast, strong projections to D1-MSNs from the ventral hippocampus40,47 may facilitate D1-MSN activity for substance P release. Future experiments will be needed to assess what inputs undergo substance P, ACh-dependent potentiation.
Our work shows ACh release plays a significant role in aversive learning, and its release is likely driven by substance P/NK1 receptor signaling. This could result from varying the upstream activity of D1-MSNs, subsequent substance P release, and the activation of ChAT neurons. Our previous work has shown that the activity of ChAT cells can be graded by varying concentration of substance P.22 We demonstrated that D1-MSN activity increases with foot-shock intensity, likely leading to increasing putative substance P release, and this might be a method of grading ACh release and learning or response to an aversive stimulus. Other factors are likely contributing to this effect as well. We cannot rule out the role of dopamine input via D2 receptors,37 midbrain GABA input,48 thalamic input,49 intrinsic inhibition,50 or other mechanisms in generation of these cue-dependent outcomes. In fact, substance P and ChAT ACh is known to promote dopamine release.51–54 Dopamine release could accompany the increase in ACh observed in response to novelty55 or initial phasic transients of foot shock. Therefore, selective plasticity could occur because of coordinated ACh and dopamine release during learning.
Despite a non-habituating D1-MSN calcium signal following repeated foot shocks, the observed ACh signal habituated to repeated foot shocks. After binding of substance P to the NK1 receptor and activation, rapid beta-arrestin-mediated endocytosis occurs56,57 and attenuates additional responses to substance P for 5–15 min.22,35,56 At this point, NK1 receptor activation of ChAT cells would be less sensitive to substance P, thereby decreasing the size of the signal. Therefore, as we observed the ACh release to be NK1 receptor dependent, we predict the habituation of the signal from shock 1 to shock 10 is due to NK1 receptor internalization. This, along with the NK1 receptor antagonist attenuation of the ACh signal, indicates the increase in ACh release is most likely due to release of substance P and activation of the NK1 receptor on ChAT neurons. Such a mechanism may suppress the likelihood of additional stimuli promoting plasticity that underlies associative learning. In addition, the D2-MSN foot shock signal is also rapidly attenuated and much smaller after repeated shocks. Mice required only two cue-shock pairings for associating, perhaps indicating that the plasticity on D2-MSNs occurs quickly, within the first few associations. This may further suggest that particularly salient stimuli can produce this plasticity rapidly.
One curious aspect of the findings is that the activity observed in D1-MSNs, D2-MSNs, and ACh is induced by foot shock alone. It is likely the release of substance P and subsequently ACh interacts with other input on D2-MSNs to promote plasticity. This may include activity from glutamatergic inputs and ventral tegmental area dopamine. In fact, some evidence suggests that the pause in cholinergic neurons is partially mediated by dopamine input to the striatum37 and that NAc D2-MSN spine dynamics in response to learned cues are dependent on dopamine.16,50 Furthermore, coordinated pre-synaptic and postsynaptic activity via glutamatergic input and muscarinic 1 receptor signaling on D2-MSNs may provide a window for spike-timing-dependent plasticity. These secondary sources may also explain the difference in the role of D2-MSNs in contextual and cue recall, as inhibition of D2-MSNs during conditioning suppressed context recall as well. These other sources will need to be investigated in future studies to fully understand the mechanisms that drive these outcomes.
Overall, we demonstrate that substance P signaling and plasticity in the NAc core are critical for learning about salient, aversive cues. We describe a role for D1-MSN local communication to facilitate ACh plasticity on D2-MSNs that is relevant to cue-dependent aversive learning. Enhanced processing of salient stimuli, through this substance P and D2-MSN mechanism, could lead to exacerbated response to aversive stimuli and could enhance susceptibility to aversive stimuli that cause stress or anxiety disorders. Therefore, targeting these neurobiological mechanisms may be useful in therapies aiming to treat anxiety and mood disorders.
Limitations of the study
We found that NAc core substance P signaling is partially responsible for cue-dependent fear learning. These outcomes are driven by D1-MSN activity, NK1 receptor signaling, and ACh-dependent plasticity on D2-MSNs. We predict that the reasons for only a partial response are due to several factors, including (1) spatial restriction of knockdown or pharmacological antagonism in the NAc, (2) the differing action of NK1 receptors in other fear-related regions following systemic antagonist administration experiments, and (3) a limited modulatory role of the NAc core in learning and likely a more robust effect on later motivational outcomes, which were not tested. Despite the partial effect, we still found moderate to strong correlations between measures of activity and freezing behavior, suggesting that the NAc core plays an important role in cue-dependent fear learning. In addition, experiments were conducted to directly test the necessity of ChAT-expressing NK1 receptors for this learning but only indirectly tested substance P release. Because of the lack of viable fluorescent sensors for substance P or slow temporal measurements by microdialysis, we are unable to definitively state that substance P is being released during these tasks. Last, although our work has shown MSN subtypes are necessary for fear learning, additional aversion-related tasks are required to expand these findings to more generalized aversive outcomes, particularly in active avoidance or punishment, which are known to be a feature of D2-MSN activity.6–15
STAR★METHODS
RESOURCE AVAILABILITY
Lead contact
Further information and requests or resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, T. Chase Francis (francit@mailbox.sc.edu).
Materials availability
Plasmids generated in this study have been deposited to Addgene (pOTTC1553, Cat #: 131682; pOTTC1642, Cat #: 195018) and are publicly available as of the date of publication.
Data and code availability
All photometry and behavioral data have been deposited at the Open Science Framework platform as of the date of publication. The DOI is listed in the Key resources table. Microscopy data will be shared by the lead contact upon request.
Python scripts for extraction of behavioral event-related calcium and acetylcholine changes are available through GitHub.com. The DOI is listed in the Key resources table. In some cases, ezTrack was used to quantify freezing behavior and locomotion and can be found on GitHub (https://github.com/denisecailab/ezTrack)60.
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
KEY RESOURCES TABLE.
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
| ||
Antibodies | ||
| ||
Anti-Green Fluorescent Protein Chicken | Aves Labs | RRID: AB_10000240; Cat #: GFP-1020 |
Alexa Fluor 488 Anti-Chicken | Jackson Immuno | RRID: AB_2340375; Cat #: 703–545-155 |
| ||
Bacterial and virus strains | ||
| ||
AAV9-hsyn-GRAB_ACh3.0 | Vigene | N/A |
AAV5-Ef1a-DIO-eNpHR3.0-eYFP | Gradinaru et al.58 | Addgene, Cat #: 26966-AAV5 |
AAV9- syn-FLEX-jGCaMP7f | Dana et al.59 | Addgene, Cat #: 104492-AAV9 |
AAV1-mU6-Tac1r-gRNA1:EF1a-NucEnv-EGFP:hU6-Tacr1-gRNA2 | This paper | NIDA GEVVC Core, OTTC-VR-18–014 |
| ||
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
| ||
L-733,060 hydrochloride | Tocris | Cat #: 1145 |
NASPM trihydrochloride | Tocris | Cat #: 2766 |
Pirenzepine dihydrochloride | Tocris | Cat #: 1071 |
Spermine tetrahydrochloride | Tocris | Cat #: S2876 |
Picrotoxin | Abcam | Cat #: ab120315 |
APV (DL-AP5) sodium salt | Tocris | Cat #: 3693 |
| ||
Critical commercial assays | ||
| ||
RNAscope multiplex fluorescent manual assay V1 | ACD Bio | Cat #: 320850 |
| ||
Deposited data | ||
| ||
Raw and analyzed data | This paper | DOI: 10.17605/OSF.IO/4FE3M |
| ||
Experimental models: Cell lines | ||
| ||
Stellar competent cells | Takara Inc | Cat #: 636763 |
NEB Stable competent cells | New England Biolabs | Cat #: C3040H |
| ||
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
| ||
B6; 129S-Pdyntm1.1(cre)Mjkr/LowlJ (Dyn-Cre) | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 027958 |
Tg(Adora2a-cre)KG139Gsat/Mmucd (A2a-Cre) | Mutant Mouse Resource and Research Centers (MMRRC) | Stock #: 031168-UCD |
B6.Cg-Gt(ROSA)26Sortm9(CAG-tdTomato)Hze/J (Ai9) | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 007909 |
C57BL/6J | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 000664 |
B6; 129S6-Chattm2(cre)Lowl/J (ChAT-IRES-Cre) | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 006410 |
B6J.129(B6N)-Gt(ROSA)26Sortm1(CAG-cas9*,-EGFP)Fezh/J | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 026175 |
| ||
Oligonucleotides | ||
| ||
Mm-Tacr1-C3 (nucleotide target region 845–1775) | ACD Bio | GenBank #: NM_009313.5 |
Mm-Chat-C2 (nucleotide target region 1090–1952) | ACD Bio | GenBank #: NM_009891.2 |
| ||
Recombinant DNA | ||
| ||
pAAV-hsyn-GRAB_ACh3.0 | Jing et al.36 | Addgene, Cat #: 121922 |
pmU6-gRNA | This paper | Addgene, Cat #: 53187 |
phU6-gRNA | This paper | Addgene, Cat #: 53188 |
pOTTC1553 | This paper | Addgene, Cat #: 131682 |
pOTTC1642 | This paper | Addgene, Cat #: 195018 |
| ||
Software and algorithms | ||
| ||
pClamp | Molecular Devices | Version 10 |
Graphpad Prism | Graphpad Software | Version 8, 9, and 10 |
Python custom script for extraction of calcium and acetylcholine events | This paper | https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10048600 |
Synapse | Tucker-Davis Technologies | N/A |
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
Animals
Female and male mice 10–15 weeks old were used in all studies. For GRAB-ACh3.0 virus, cannula infusion experiments, and systemic drug injections without imaging or electrophysiology, C57Bl6/J mice (JAX: 000664) were used. For optogenetic inhibition experiments either B6; 129S-Pdyntm1.1(cre)Mjkr/LowlJ (Dyn-Cre; JAX: 027958) or Tg(Adora2a-cre)KG139Gsat/Mmucd (A2a-Cre; MMRRC: 031168-UCD). For CRISPR-Cas9 experiments, B6; 129-Gt(ROSA)26Sortm1(CAG-cas9*,-EGFP)Fezh/J (LSL-Cas9; JAX: 024857) mice were crossed with B6; 129S6-Chattm2(cre)Lowl/J (ChAT-Cre) mice for conditional expression of Cas9 in ChAT-Cre cells. All transgenic animals were bred in house. Mice were fed ad libitum and were housed in a reverse light/dark (12/12 h). Experimental procedures were approved and conducted in accordance with NIDA-IRP ACUC, University of South Carolina IACUC, and NIH guidelines.
METHOD DETAILS
Surgical methods
Mice used in fluorescence imaging, optogenetic, or CRISPR knockdown experiments were anesthetized with ketamine/xylazine for intracranial AAV injection, hair was removed, scalps were cleaned with ethanol and betadine, and lidocaine was used prior to scalp incision. Body temperature was maintained with an electric heating pad. Mice received bilateral stereotactic injections (0.3–0.5 μL) of either AAV9-hsyn-GRAB-ACh3.0 (Addgene; plasmid# 121922; packaged by Vigene Biosciences),36 AAV9-syn-FLEX-jGCaMP7f (Addgene; 104492-AAV9), AAV5-EF1a-DIO-eNpHR3.0-eYFP (Addgene; 26966-AAV5), or AAV1-mU6-Tac1r-gRNA1EF1a-NucEnv-EGFPhU6-Tacr1-gRNA2 in the NAc core (created and packaged by NIDA GEVVC Core) (A/P: 1.6, Lat: 1.6, D/V: −4.4, 10° angle). For photometry or optogenetics, mice were then implanted with either a 400 μm photometry fiber (Doric) or 200 μm core optogenetic fiber fixed with head screws and dental cement. Following surgery, mice were injected with warm saline to replenish fluids and given carprofen (5 mg/kg) for pain relief during recovery.
In vivo fiber photometry, ex vivo imaging, and in vivo optogenetics
In vivo fiber photometry was used to assess cell-type specific calcium activity (GCaMP7f) or acetylcholine release (GRAB-ACh3.0) using a Tucker-Davis Technologies LUX RZ10 fiber photometry system. Signals were tested and mice were habituated for 3–5 days prior behavioral recordings. For all recordings, sensors were excited at 470 nm to acquire the fluorescent emission signal and the isosbestic point for GFP at 405 nm as a normalization control. Excitation signals were generated by two light-emitting diodes and emission signals were collimated through a Doric fluorescent mini cube before being processed and digitized using the Synapse software suite. Analysis was performed offline using a custom Python script which extracted fluorescent signals, smoothed the signal with a linear fit to the 405 nm signal and applying the regression to the 470 nm signal, calculated ΔF/F, aligned signals based on a timestamp, extracted fluorescent signals around events, and normalized signals (Z score) based on the 10 s prior to an event. Area Under the Curve (AUC) was assessed to find mean changes in calcium or acetylcholine activity. The following intervals were used for AUC calculation in the described experiment: for foot shock during fear conditioning or during shock alone, 0–5 s from the initiation of the shock; for cue recall, 0–10 s from the start of the cue; for shock-related decrease in ACh-mediated fluorescence, 0.5–2 s after from the initiation of the shock; for shock-related rebound in ACh-mediated fluorescence, 2.5 to 7 s from the initiation of the shock.
For ex vivo imaging of GRAB-ACh3.0, slices were prepared as discussed in the electrophysiology methods section. Acetylcholine (1 or 100 μM) was bath applied after a 2 min baseline. Videos were acquired with a 40× objective at a 10 Hz frame rate using QCapturePro7 and analyzed offline with ImageJ. A subregion 100 × 100 μm devoid of fiber tracts in the NAc core was chosen in each image and analyzed for fluorescence intensity over time. Fluorescence traces were flattened using a linear regression fit and normalized (Z score) to the 1 min prior to acetylcholine bath application.
For optogenetic inhibition experiments, mice were habituated to patch cables 5 days prior to behavior. Percent transmission was calculated during creation of optogenetic fibers and 594 nm light was delivered at 3–5 mW from a DPSS laser (OptoEngine LLC). Optogenetic inhibition was delivered during the onset of the cue and 1 s after the offset of the cue for all conditioning cues and on every other cue during cue recall.
Behavior
Pavlovian fear conditioning was conducted over a 3 day period (Figure 1A) in Med Associates Fear conditioning chambers contained in sound attenuating cabinets. Prior to each phase of fear conditioning, mice were habituated in a separate room for at least 1 h in the lighting condition of the test. Conditioning was conducted in white light with vanilla scent (10% vanilla extract in ethanol) chamber with a grid floor. Following a 2 min baseline, 2 white-noise (80 dB) cues co-terminating in a 0.6 mA foot shock separated by a 30 s interval. The next day mice were exposed to the conditioning context for 5 min. On the third day, mice were placed in a new context where floors were covered with white, opaque plexiglass, a curved white plexiglass insert, no light (red light in behavior room), and vinegar scent (1% acetic acid in water). After a 2 min baseline, 5–6 cues at 30 s intervals were played back. In all tests, freezing was measured using VideoFreeze (Med Associates), which has been thoroughly validated in relation to hand scoring.61 Freezing thresholds were set at least 30, 0.033 s frames of frozen motion and motion indices below 6 for mice without fiber or optogenetic tethers and below 500 for mice with optical tethers. For mice exposed to foot shock only, after a 2 min baseline each mouse was exposed to an unpredictable foot shock set to random intervals at least 30 s between each shock. Mice received one intensity of foot shock per day, and each subsequent day were subjected to increasing foot shock intensities or decreasing foot shock intensities to control for previous foot shock intensities from interfering with the signal for any subsequent foot shock.
Electrophysiology
Whole-cell electrophysiological recordings were done one day following cue recall. Mice were anesthetized with Euthasol, brains were extracted, and cut (300 μm) in ice-cold N-Methyl D-Glucamine (NMDG) cutting artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) solution (in mM): 92 NMDG, 20 HEPES, 25 glucose, 30 sodium bicarbonate, 1.2 sodium phosphate, 2.5 potassium chloride, 5 sodium ascorbate, 3 sodium pyruvate, 2 thiourea, 10 magnesium, 0.5 calcium chloride, osmolarity 305–310 mOsm, pH 7.34. Immediately after slicing, slices were placed in warm (32°C) NMDG solution for 2–5 min, then transferred to HEPES holding solution at room temperature which was the same as NMDG solution except NMDG was replaced with 92 mM sodium chloride and contained 1 mM magnesium chloride and 2 mM calcium chloride. Recording began 1 h after brain slicing was complete and was performed at 33°C. Slices were perfused at a rate of 2 mL/min with ACSF (in mM): 125 sodium chloride, 26 sodium bicarbonate, and 11 glucose, 2.5 potassium chloride, 1.25 sodium phosphate, 2.4 calcium chloride, 1 magnesium chloride, osmolarity 305–310 mOsm, pH 7.34. To block inhibitory currents, synaptic recordings were done holding cells at −70 mV in the presence of picrotoxin (100 μM) and with a cesium methanesulfonate internal solution (in mM): 117 cesium methanesulfanate, 20 HEPES, 2.8 sodium chloride, 4 magnesium ATP, 0.3 sodium GTP, 0.4 EGTA, pH 7.30, osmolarity 280–287 mOsm. For IV curves, spermine (100 mM) was added to the pipette solution and currents were recorded in the presence of APV to isolate AMPA receptor currents. NASPM (100 μM) was used to isolate currents from inserted GluR2-lacking AMPA receptors. For cell-attached ChAT recordings, ACSF was used as the internal solution.
Whole-cell currents or cell attached currents were obtained with borosilicate glass pipettes (whole cell: 1.7–3.0 MΩ; cell attached: 3.0–4.0 MΩ) and signals were amplified and digitized using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier (4 kHz low-pass filter for evoked synaptic input, 1 kHz for cell-attached or spontaneous EPSCs) and Digidata 1322 digitizer (10 kHz), respectively (Molecular Devices). Synaptic currents were evoked with a concentric bipolar stimulating electrode (FHC) and evoked currents were stabilized for 5–10 min prior to data acquisition. Series resistance drifts of more than 20% led to exclusion of the cell from analysis. D1-and D2-MSNs were identified under the visual guidance of a BX61 Olympus microscope and Tdtomato fluorescence. Cholinergic neurons were identified by cell size (40–60 μm diameters) and high baseline firing rates.
CRISPR-Cas9 for Tacr1 knockout
The SpCas9 guide RNA seed sequences within the first exon of mouse TACR1 were identified and scored using the Guide Design Tool at CRISPR.mit.edu. Two gRNAs (gRNA 1 A55: CCGTATAGGCGGCTGCCCAA, and gRNA 2 B108: TTCCGTGGTGGGCAACGTAG) were chosen based on their likelihood for functioning as a “nickase-compatible” pair. These sequences were inserted into plasmids already containing RNA pol III promoters and SpCas9 gRNA scaffold sequences. (gifts from Charles Gersbach; Addgene plasmid # 53187; http://n2t.net/addgene:53187; RRID:Addgene_53187, Addgene plasmid # 53188; http://n2t.net/addgene:53188; RRID:Addgene_53188). Then each intact gRNA expression cassette was amplified by PCR and both were inserted into an AAV expression vector that contained a EGFP-KASH reporter (pOTTC1553, Addgene #131682), resulting in plasmid pOTTC1642 (Addgene #195018). All cloning reactions used PCR amplification and ligation-independent cloning (In-Fusion, Takara Inc.). Reactions involving non-AAV plasmids were transformed into Stellar competent cells (Takara Inc.) and those involving AAV plasmids were transformed into NEB Stable competent cells (New England Biolabs).
The pOTTC1642 viral vector was packaged by triple transfection along with pHelper and rep/cap plasmids (serotype 1) and purified by affinity chromatography on GE AVB columns.62 Purified vectors were titered by droplet digital PCR with an assay that recognizes the EF1a reporter. Titered vectors were tested for GFP expression by transduction into rat primary cortical neurons prior to in vivo use.
Immunohistochemistry and in situ hybridization
Expression of GRAB-ACh3.0 or eNpHR3.0-eYFP was identified by histological staining for GFP. Briefly, brains slices (40 μm) from perfused (4% paraformaldehyde) brains were washed with 1X PBS, blocked with 1X PBS buffer containing 0.01% Triton X-, 10% Normal Donkey Serum and incubated with a primary anti-body for GFP made in chicken (Aves lab, cat#: GFP-1020). Slices were washed and incubated with a secondary anti-chicken antibody made in donkey conjugated to Alexa Fluor 488 (Jackson immuno, cat#: 703-545-155), stained with DAPI and mounted on slides. All images were acquired on a Fluoview 1000 BX51WI confocal microscope (Olympus).
In situ hybridization was performed on fresh frozen 16 μm slices according to the ACDBio specifications for assay kit V1.22 Slices were mounted on slides, dehydrated, and washed and treated with protease. Probes were hybridized and amplified at 40°C using AMP1–4 with washes in between each amplification step. AMP 4 Alt A was used to link amplified probes to fluorescent molecules. EGFP-KASH maintained its fluorescence through the protocol. The probes used were as follows: Chat (Mm-Chat-C1 (nucleotide target region 1090–1952), GenBank #: NM_009891.2); Tacr1 (Mm-Tacr1-C3 (nucleotide target region 845–1775), GenBank #: NM_009313.5). All images were taken within a week of the protocol and image intensities were maintained across slices and slides. Quantification was done in the NAc core, where EGFP-KASH expression was found. Due to the cre-independent expression of EGFP-KASH and expression of GFP in ChAT cells, we were unable to specifically identify cells with or without expression and, therefore, all Chat positive cells in the expression region were analyzed. RNA was quantified using ImageJ by selecting 10 individual pixel values for every image and averaging to find the intensity values of a single RNA puncta. Intensity values of Tacr1 in ChAT positive cells were assessed by selecting perinuclear RNA, quantifying intensity, and dividing the total intensity by the intensity of a single puncta value to calculate total RNA puncta. A total of 4 slices from each animal was taken and averaged. Slice averages are represented as individual points.
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Detailed statistics can be found in Table S1. Data were analyzed and statistics were calculated using Graphpad Prism. Tests were considered significant if p < 0.05. For two-sample data, two-sided, un-paired or paired t tests were used. For more than 2 variables over repeated time points one-way or two-way repeated measure (RM) ANOVAs were used followed by post-hoc tests using Sidak’s multiple comparison test. In most cases with an interaction, main effects were reported. For electrophysiology experiments, all are represented as N/n (Mice/cells). Animal exclusions for behavioral experiments were made only when viral expression was absent unilaterally or bilaterally following histological validation. All data are represented as Mean ± standard error. Cartoons were made using BioRender during the subscription period.
Supplementary Material
Highlights.
Aversive stimuli activate D1-MSNs and D2-MSNs and promote acetylcholine release
D1-MSN substance P is necessary for cue-dependent aversive learning
Excitatory LTP on D2-MSNs caused by aversive cues requires substance P signaling
D2-MSN activity is necessary for cue-dependent recall of aversive cues
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We would like to thank Dr. Geoffrey Schoenbaum for his assistance in conceptualization of the fear-conditioning experiments. This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grant R00MH123673, the University of South Carolina College of Pharmacy, and the National Institute on Drug Abuse Intramural Research Program. Construction and packaging of the AAV vector expressing mTACR1 gRNAs was performed by the National Institute on Drug Abuse Genetic Engineering and Viral Vector Core (RRID: SCR_022969).
Footnotes
DECLARATION OF INTERESTS
The authors declare no competing interests.
SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION
Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2023.113488.
REFERENCES
- 1.Zaehle T, Bauch EM, Hinrichs H, Schmitt FC, Voges J, Heinze HJ, Bunzeck N, and Bunzeck N (2013). Nucleus Accumbens Activity Dissociates Different Forms of Salience: Evidence from Human Intracranial Recordings. J. Neurosci. 33, 8764–8771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schoenbaum G, and Setlow B (2003). Lesions of Nucleus Accumbens Disrupt Learning about Aversive Outcomes. J. Neurosci. 23, 9833–9841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dutta S, Beaver J, Halcomb CJ, and Jasnow AM (2021). Dissociable roles of the nucleus accumbens core and shell subregions in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear. Neurobiol. Stress 15, 100365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wendler E, Gaspar JCC, Ferreira TL, Barbiero JK, Andreatini R, Vital MABF, Blaha CD, Winn P, and Da Cunha C (2014). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory The roles of the nucleus accumbens core, dorsomedial striatum, and dorsolateral striatum in learning : Performance and extinction of Pavlovian fear-conditioned responses and instrumental avoidance responses. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 109, 27–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ray MH, Russ AN, Walker RA, and McDannald MA (2020). The Nucleus Accumbens Core is Necessary to Scale Fear to Degree of Threat. J. Neurosci. 40, 4750–4760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Francis TC, and Lobo MK (2017). Emerging Role for Nucleus Accumbens Medium Spiny Neuron Subtypes in Depression. Biol. Psychiatry 81, 645–653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lee HJ, Weitz AJ, Bernal-Casas D, Duffy BA, Choy M, Kravitz A.v., Kreitzer AC, and Lee JH (2016). Activation of Direct and Indirect Pathway Medium Spiny Neurons Drives Distinct Brain-wide Responses. Neuron 91, 412–424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kravitz A. v, Tye LD, and Kreitzer AC (2012). Distinct roles for direct and indirect pathway striatal neurons in reinforcement. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 816–818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Francis TC, Chandra R, Friend DM, Finkel E, Dayrit G, Miranda J, Brooks JM, Iñiguez SD, O’Donnell P, Kravitz A, and Lobo MK (2015). Nucleus accumbens medium spiny neuron subtypes mediate depression-related outcomes to social defeat stress. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 212–222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Smith RJ, Lobo MK, Spencer S, and Kalivas PW (2013). Cocaine-induced adaptations in D1 and D2 accumbens projection neurons (a dichotomy not necessarily synonymous with direct and indirect pathways). Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 546–552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Lobo MK, Covington HE 3rd, Chaudhury D, Friedman AK, Sun H, Damez-Werno D, Dietz DM, Zaman S, Koo JW, Kennedy PJ, et al. (2010). Cell type-specific loss of BDNF signaling mimics optogenetic control of cocaine reward. Science 330, 385–390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ferguson SM, Phillips PEM, Roth BL, Wess J, and Neumaier JF (2013). Direct-pathway striatal neurons regulate the retention of decision-making strategies. J. Neurosci. 33, 11668–11676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tai LH, Lee AM, Benavidez N, Bonci A, and Wilbrecht L (2012). Transient stimulation of distinct subpopulations of striatal neurons mimics changes in action value. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1281–1289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bock R, Shin JH, Kaplan AR, Dobi A, Markey E, Kramer PF, Gremel CM, Christensen CH, Adrover MF, and Alvarez VA (2013). Strengthening the accumbal indirect pathway promotes resilience to compulsive cocaine use. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 632–638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Calipari ES, Bagot RC, Purushothaman I, Davidson TJ, Yorgason JT, Peña CJ, Walker DM, Pirpinias ST, Guise KG, Ramakrishnan C, et al. (2016). In vivo imaging identifies temporal signature of D1 and D2 medium spiny neurons in cocaine reward. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 113, 2726–2731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Iino Y, Sawada T, Yamaguchi K, Tajiri M, Ishii S, Kasai H, and Yagishita S (2020). Dopamine D2 receptors in discrimination learning and spine enlargement. Nature 579, 555–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.MacPherson T, Morita M, Wang Y, Sasaoka T, Sawa A, and Hikida T (2016). Nucleus accumbens dopamine D2-receptor expressing neurons control behavioral flexibility in a place discrimination task in the IntelliCage. Learn. Mem. 23, 359–364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Macpherson T, Kim JY, and Hikida T (2022). Nucleus Accumbens Core Dopamine D2 Receptor-Expressing Neurons Control Reversal Learning but Not Set-Shifting in Behavioral Flexibility in Male Mice. Front. Neurosci. 16, 885380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Soares-Cunha C, Coimbra B, Domingues AV, Vasconcelos N, Sousa N, and Rodrigues AJ (2018). Nucleus Accumbens Microcircuit Underlying D2-MSN-Driven Increase in Motivation. eNeuro 5. ENEURO.0386, 18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Soares-Cunha C, Coimbra B, David-Pereira A, Borges S, Pinto L, Costa P, Sousa N, and Rodrigues AJ (2016). Activation of D2 dopamine receptor-expressing neurons in the nucleus accumbens increases motivation. Nat. Commun. 7, 11829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Owesson-White C, Belle AM, Herr NR, Peele JL, Gowrishankar P, Carelli RM, and Wightman RM (2016). Cue-Evoked Dopamine Release Rapidly Modulates D2 Neurons in the Nucleus Accumbens During Motivated Behavior. J. Neurosci. 36, 6011–6021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Francis TC, Yano H, Demarest TG, Shen H, and Bonci A (2019). High-Frequency Activation of Nucleus Accumbens D1-MSNs Drives Excitatory Potentiation on D2-MSNs. Neuron 103, 432–444.e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Commons KG (2010). Neuronal pathways linking substance P to drug addiction and stress. Brain Res. 1314, 175–182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Nakamura Y, Izumi H, Shimizu T, Hisaoka-Nakashima K, Morioka N, and Nakata Y (2013). Volume Transmission of Substance P in Striatum Induced by Intraplantar Formalin Injection Attenuates Nociceptive Responses via Activation of the Neurokinin 1 Receptor. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 121, 257–271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Aosaki T, Tsubokawa H, Ishida A, Watanabe K, Graybiel AM, and Kimura M (1994). Responses of tonically active neurons in the primate’s striatum undergo systematic changes during behavioral sensorimotor conditioning. J. Neurosci. 14, 3969–3984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ravel S, Legallet E, and Apicella P (2003). Responses of Tonically Active Neurons in the Monkey Striatum Discriminate between Motivationally Opposing Stimuli. J. Neurosci. 23, 8489–8497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Schulz JM, Pitcher TL, Savanthrapadian S, Wickens JR, Oswald MJ, and Reynolds JNJ (2011). Enhanced high-frequency membrane potential fluctuations control spike output in striatal fast-spiking interneurones in vivo. J. Physiol. 589, 4365–4381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Aoki S, Liu AW, Zucca A, Zucca S, and Wickens JR (2015). Role of Striatal Cholinergic Interneurons in Set-Shifting in the Rat. J. Neurosci. 35, 9424–9431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Okada K, Nishizawa K, Fukabori R, Kai N, Shiota A, Ueda M, Tsutsui Y, Sakata S, Matsushita N, and Kobayashi K (2014). Enhanced flexibility of place discrimination learning by targeting striatal cholinergic interneurons. Nat. Commun. 5, 3778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ragozzino ME, Mohler EG, Prior M, Palencia CA, and Rozman S (2009). Acetylcholine activity in selective striatal regions supports behavioral flexibility. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 91, 13–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Collins AL, Aitken TJ, Huang I-W, Shieh C, Greenfield VY, Monbouquette HG, Ostlund SB, and Wassum KM (2019). Nucleus Accumbens Cholinergic Interneurons Oppose Cue-Motivated Behavior. Biol. Psychiatry 86, 388–396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sandweiss AJ, and Vanderah TW (2015). The pharmacology of neurokinin receptors in addiction: prospects for therapy. Subst. Abuse Rehabil. 6, 93–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Tomaz C, and Nogueira PJ (1997). Facilitation of memory by peripheral administration of substance P. Behav. Brain Res. 83, 143–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ebner K, and Singewald N (2006). The role of substance P in stress and anxiety responses. Amino Acids 31, 251–272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Aosaki T, and Kawaguchi Y (1996). Actions of substance P on rat neostriatal neurons in vitro. J. Neurosci. 16, 5141–5153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Jing M, Li Y, Zeng J, Huang P, Skirzewski M, Kljakic O, Peng W, Qian T, Tan K, Zou J, et al. (2020). An optimized acetylcholine sensor for monitoring in vivo cholinergic activity. Nat. Methods 17, 1139–1146. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gallo EF, Greenwald J, Yeisley J, Teboul E, Martyniuk KM, Villarin JM, Li Y, Javitch JA, Balsam PD, and Kellendonk C (2022). Dopamine D2 receptors modulate the cholinergic pause and inhibitory learning. Mol. Psychiatry 27, 1502–1514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Soares-Cunha C, de Vasconcelos NAP, Coimbra B, Domingues AV, Silva JM, Loureiro-Campos E, Gaspar R, Sotiropoulos I, Sousa N, and Rodrigues AJ (2020). Nucleus accumbens medium spiny neurons subtypes signal both reward and aversion. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 3241–3255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Francis TC, Chandra R, Gaynor A, Konkalmatt P, Metzbower SR, Evans B, Engeln M, Blanpied TA, and Lobo MK (2017). Molecular basis of dendritic atrophy and activity in stress susceptibility. Mol. Psychiatry 22, 1512–1519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Pignatelli M, Tejeda HA, Barker DJ, Bontempi L, Wu J, Lopez A, Palma Ribeiro S, Lucantonio F, Parise EM, Torres-Berrio A, et al. (2021). Cooperative synaptic and intrinsic plasticity in a disynaptic limbic circuit drive stress-induced anhedonia and passive coping in mice. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 1860–1879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.He Z-X, Xi K, Liu K-J, Yue M-H, Wang Y, Yin Y-Y, Liu L, He X-X, Yu H-L, Xing Z-K, and Zhu XJ (2023). A Nucleus Accumbens Tac1 Neural Circuit Regulates Avoidance Responses to Aversive Stimuli. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 24, 4346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Picciotto MR, Higley MJ, and Mineur YS (2012). Acetylcholine as a neuromodulator: cholinergic signaling shapes nervous system function and behavior. Neuron 76, 116–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Oldenburg IA, and Ding JB (2011). Cholinergic modulation of synaptic integration and dendritic excitability in the striatum. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 425–432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Britt JP, Benaliouad F, McDevitt RA, Stuber GD, Wise RA, and Bonci A (2012). Synaptic and behavioral profile of multiple glutamatergic inputs to the nucleus accumbens. Neuron 76, 790–803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gale GD, Anagnostaras SG, Godsil BP, Mitchell S, Nozawa T, Sage JR, Wiltgen B, and Fanselow MS (2004). Role of the Basolateral Amygdala in the Storage of Fear Memories across the Adult Lifetime of Rats. J. Neurosci. 24, 3810–3815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Zhu Y, Wienecke CFR, Nachtrab G, and Chen X (2016). A thalamic input to the nucleus accumbens mediates opiate dependence. Nature 530, 219–222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.MacAskill AF, Cassel JM, and Carter AG (2014). Cocaine exposure reorganizes cell type- and input-specific connectivity in the nucleus accumbens. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1198–1207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Al-Hasani R, Gowrishankar R, Schmitz GP, Pedersen CE, Marcus DJ, Shirley SE, Hobbs TE, Elerding AJ, Renaud SJ, Jing M, et al. (2021). Ventral tegmental area GABAergic inhibition of cholinergic interneurons in the ventral nucleus accumbens shell promotes reward reinforcement. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 1414–1428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Matsumoto N, Minamimoto T, Graybiel AM, and Kimura M (2001). Neurons in the thalamic CM-Pf complex supply striatal neurons with information about behaviorally significant sensory events. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 960–976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Zhang Y-F, Reynolds JNJ, and Cragg SJ (2018). Pauses in Cholinergic Interneuron Activity Are Driven by Excitatory Input and Delayed Rectification, with Dopamine Modulation. Neuron 98, 918–925.e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Guzman RG, Kendrick KM, and Emson PC (1993). Effect of substance P on acetylcholine and dopamine release in the rat striatum: a microdialysis study. Brain Res. 622, 147–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Brimblecombe KR, and Cragg SJ (2015). Substance P Weights Striatal Dopamine Transmission Differently within the Striosome-Matrix Axis. J. Neurosci. 35, 9017–9023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cachope R, Mateo Y, Mathur BN, Irving J, Wang HL, Morales M, Lovinger DM, and Cheer JF (2012). Selective activation of cholinergic interneurons enhances accumbal phasic dopamine release: setting the tone for reward processing. Cell Rep. 2, 33–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Threlfell S, Lalic T, Platt NJ, Jennings KA, Deisseroth K, and Cragg SJ (2012). Striatal dopamine release is triggered by synchronized activity in cholinergic interneurons. Neuron 75, 58–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kutlu MG, Zachry JE, Melugin PR, Tat J, Cajigas S, Isiktas AU, Patel DD, Siciliano CA, Schoenbaum G, Sharpe MJ, and Calipari ES (2022). Dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens core mediates latent inhibition. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 1071–1081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.McConalogue K, Corvera CU, Gamp PD, Grady EF, and Bunnett NW (1998). Desensitization of the neurokinin-1 receptor (NK1-R) in neurons: effects of substance P on the distribution of NK1-R, Galphaq/11, G-protein receptor kinase-2/3, and beta-arrestin-1/2. Mol. Biol. Cell 9, 2305–2324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bowden JJ, Garland AM, Baluk P, Lefevre P, Grady EF, Vigna SR, Bunnett NW, and McDonald DM (1994). Direct observation of substance P-induced internalization of neurokinin 1 (NK1) receptors at sites of inflammation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 91, 8964–8968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Gradinaru V, Zhang F, Ramakrishnan C, Mattis J, Prakash R, Diester I, Goshen I, Thompson KR, and Deisseroth K (2010). Molecular and cellular approaches for diversifying and extending optogenetics. Cell 141, 154–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Dana H, Sun Y, Mohar B, Hulse BK, Kerlin AM, Hasseman JP, Tsegaye G, Tsang A, Wong A, Patel R, et al. (2019). High-performance calcium sensors for imaging activity in neuronal populations and microcompartments. Nat. Methods 16, 649–657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Pennington ZT, Dong Z, Feng Y, Vetere LM, Page-Harley L, Shuman T, and Cai DJ (2019). ezTrack: An open-source video analysis pipeline for the investigation of animal behavior. Sci. Rep. 9, 19979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Anagnostaras SG, Wood SC, Shuman T, Cai DJ, Leduc AD, Zurn KR, Zurn JB, Sage JR, and Herrera GM (2010). Automated Assessment of Pavlovian Conditioned Freezing and Shock Reactivity in Mice Using the Video Freeze System. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4, 158. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Howard DB, and Harvey BK (2017). Assaying the Stability and Inactivation of AAV Serotype 1 Vectors. Hum. Gene Ther. Methods 28, 39–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All photometry and behavioral data have been deposited at the Open Science Framework platform as of the date of publication. The DOI is listed in the Key resources table. Microscopy data will be shared by the lead contact upon request.
Python scripts for extraction of behavioral event-related calcium and acetylcholine changes are available through GitHub.com. The DOI is listed in the Key resources table. In some cases, ezTrack was used to quantify freezing behavior and locomotion and can be found on GitHub (https://github.com/denisecailab/ezTrack)60.
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
KEY RESOURCES TABLE.
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
| ||
Antibodies | ||
| ||
Anti-Green Fluorescent Protein Chicken | Aves Labs | RRID: AB_10000240; Cat #: GFP-1020 |
Alexa Fluor 488 Anti-Chicken | Jackson Immuno | RRID: AB_2340375; Cat #: 703–545-155 |
| ||
Bacterial and virus strains | ||
| ||
AAV9-hsyn-GRAB_ACh3.0 | Vigene | N/A |
AAV5-Ef1a-DIO-eNpHR3.0-eYFP | Gradinaru et al.58 | Addgene, Cat #: 26966-AAV5 |
AAV9- syn-FLEX-jGCaMP7f | Dana et al.59 | Addgene, Cat #: 104492-AAV9 |
AAV1-mU6-Tac1r-gRNA1:EF1a-NucEnv-EGFP:hU6-Tacr1-gRNA2 | This paper | NIDA GEVVC Core, OTTC-VR-18–014 |
| ||
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
| ||
L-733,060 hydrochloride | Tocris | Cat #: 1145 |
NASPM trihydrochloride | Tocris | Cat #: 2766 |
Pirenzepine dihydrochloride | Tocris | Cat #: 1071 |
Spermine tetrahydrochloride | Tocris | Cat #: S2876 |
Picrotoxin | Abcam | Cat #: ab120315 |
APV (DL-AP5) sodium salt | Tocris | Cat #: 3693 |
| ||
Critical commercial assays | ||
| ||
RNAscope multiplex fluorescent manual assay V1 | ACD Bio | Cat #: 320850 |
| ||
Deposited data | ||
| ||
Raw and analyzed data | This paper | DOI: 10.17605/OSF.IO/4FE3M |
| ||
Experimental models: Cell lines | ||
| ||
Stellar competent cells | Takara Inc | Cat #: 636763 |
NEB Stable competent cells | New England Biolabs | Cat #: C3040H |
| ||
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
| ||
B6; 129S-Pdyntm1.1(cre)Mjkr/LowlJ (Dyn-Cre) | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 027958 |
Tg(Adora2a-cre)KG139Gsat/Mmucd (A2a-Cre) | Mutant Mouse Resource and Research Centers (MMRRC) | Stock #: 031168-UCD |
B6.Cg-Gt(ROSA)26Sortm9(CAG-tdTomato)Hze/J (Ai9) | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 007909 |
C57BL/6J | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 000664 |
B6; 129S6-Chattm2(cre)Lowl/J (ChAT-IRES-Cre) | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 006410 |
B6J.129(B6N)-Gt(ROSA)26Sortm1(CAG-cas9*,-EGFP)Fezh/J | Jackson Laboratories | Stock #: 026175 |
| ||
Oligonucleotides | ||
| ||
Mm-Tacr1-C3 (nucleotide target region 845–1775) | ACD Bio | GenBank #: NM_009313.5 |
Mm-Chat-C2 (nucleotide target region 1090–1952) | ACD Bio | GenBank #: NM_009891.2 |
| ||
Recombinant DNA | ||
| ||
pAAV-hsyn-GRAB_ACh3.0 | Jing et al.36 | Addgene, Cat #: 121922 |
pmU6-gRNA | This paper | Addgene, Cat #: 53187 |
phU6-gRNA | This paper | Addgene, Cat #: 53188 |
pOTTC1553 | This paper | Addgene, Cat #: 131682 |
pOTTC1642 | This paper | Addgene, Cat #: 195018 |
| ||
Software and algorithms | ||
| ||
pClamp | Molecular Devices | Version 10 |
Graphpad Prism | Graphpad Software | Version 8, 9, and 10 |
Python custom script for extraction of calcium and acetylcholine events | This paper | https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.10048600 |
Synapse | Tucker-Davis Technologies | N/A |