Abstract
Sleep is a poorly understood behavior that predominates during infancy but is studied almost exclusively in adults. One perceived impediment to investigations of sleep early in ontogeny is the absence of state-dependent neocortical activity. Nonetheless, in infant rats, sleep is reliably characterized by the presence of tonic (i.e., muscle atonia) and phasic (i.e., myoclonic twitching) components; the neural circuitry underlying these components, however, is unknown. Recently, we described a medullary inhibitory area (MIA) in week-old rats that is necessary but not sufficient for the normal expression of atonia. Here we report that the infant MIA receives projections from areas containing neurons that exhibit state-dependent activity. Specifically, neurons within these areas, including the subcoeruleus (SubLC), pontis oralis (PO), and dorsolateral pontine tegmentum (DLPT), exhibit discharge profiles that suggest causal roles in the modulation of muscle tone and the production of myoclonic twitches. Indeed, lesions in the SubLC and PO decreased the expression of muscle atonia without affecting twitching (resulting in “REM sleep without atonia”), whereas lesions of the DLPT increased the expression of atonia while decreasing the amount of twitching. Thus, the neural substrates of infant sleep are strikingly similar to those of adults, a surprising finding in light of theories that discount the contribution of supraspinal neural elements to sleep before the onset of state-dependent neocortical activity.
Unexpectedly, the anatomy and neurophysiology of brainstem areas associated with sleep in the neonatal rat are strikingly similar to the adult
Introduction
When compared against an adult standard, infant sleep is composed of a reduced set of components. Perhaps the most notable difference between the sleep of infants and adults is the absence of clearly differentiated and sleep-related neocortical electroencephalographic (EEG) activity [1,2,3]. Nonetheless, sleep and wakefulness in the infants of altricial species, including rats, are reliably characterized, respectively, by periods of myoclonic twitching expressed against a background of muscle atonia and high-amplitude behaviors (e.g., locomotion or stretching) expressed against a background of high muscle tone [4]. Although myoclonic twitching during active sleep (AS) in infants is more prevalent and more intense than that seen during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep in adults, its similarities to the adult behavior and its linkage to periods of atonia suggest developmental continuity between the infant and adult sleep states [4,5,6]. Opinions differ, however, as to whether these similarities provide sufficient evidence that infant sleep is developmentally continuous with that of adults. Specifically, it has recently been argued that sleep during the “pre-EEG” period—that is, before postnatal day 12 (P12) in rats [2]—is best considered a primitive state, comprising an amalgam of sleep components, that only gradually differentiates into the distinct sleep states characteristic of adults [1,7].
Proponents of the “amalgam” theory of sleep development have made strong claims concerning the neural substrates of infant sleep. For example, it has been argued that infant sleep is (i) undifferentiated and (ii) controlled by distinct neurophysiological mechanisms from those that generate sleep in adults. In particular, Adrien [8] writes, “In the immediate postnatal period, and furthermore in utero, AS would seem to be very different—even in terms of underlying mechanisms—from what we know of the sleep phenomenon in the adult” (p. 40). Extending this idea, Adrien and Lanfumey [9] state that neural activity during infant sleep indicates “a very primitive system of diffuse activation within the whole central nervous system” (p. 7). More recently, Frank and Heller [1] write, “Brainstem-midbrain nuclei important in mediating REM sleep expression do not mediate the expression of AS, or AS myoclonia” (p. 64). Although several papers have reported state-dependent neural activity within the brainstem of infant rats [10,11], they have not proved convincing [7].
Despite the absence of well-differentiated EEG activity in rats before P12, it is now apparent that the state characterized by nuchal atonia—with or without the simultaneous occurrence of myoclonic twitching—satisfies many established criteria of sleep [12,13,14]. For example, during infant sleep, sensory thresholds increase, both under normal conditions [15] and during sleep deprivation [16]; infant sleep is also homeostatically regulated [16]. In addition, it was recently shown that there exists an inhibitory area within the ventromedial medulla, the medullary inhibitory area (MIA), that appears functionally identical to the corresponding area that mediates REM-sleep atonia in adults [17]. The MIA (i) causes atonia when stimulated; (ii) contains neurons that exhibit atonia-related discharge profiles; and (iii) when lesioned, results in the partial loss of atonia and the decoupling of the components of infant sleep (i.e., “REM sleep without atonia”). The MIA is not sufficient, however, for generating infant sleep, because decerebrations rostral to the MIA, but caudal to the mesopontine region, abolish sleep-wake cyclicity and reduce myoclonic twitching [18] (Karlsson and Blumberg, unpublished data). Thus, expression of the components of infant sleep—atonia and twitching—depends on a network that spans the mesopontine-medullary region.
The aims of the current study were to identify the structures that project to the MIA (and therefore may contribute to the generation of atonia) and to identify supraspinal neural correlates of myoclonic twitching. First, we performed retrograde tracing from the MIA as well as the ventrolateral medulla (an area that typically induces motor activity when stimulated [17]). Next, using the tracing data as a guide, we conducted neural recording studies to determine the relationship, if any, between the discharge profiles of individual neurons and the specific components of infant sleep. Finally, we lesioned structures containing neurons that exhibit opposing state-dependent discharge profiles and examined the effects on behavioral states. These experiments provide the most comprehensive survey to date of the neural control of sleep in infancy and reveal surprising parallels between the neural substrates of sleep in infant and adult rats. Moreover, these results are not consistent with the perspective that views infant sleep as “diffuse,” “undifferentiated,” or arising from “distinct neurophysiologic mechanisms” [1,7,8,9]. Finally, the current findings provide a guide to future theorizing about the ontogeny and function of sleep.
Results
Mesopontine and Medullary Structures Project to the Medullary Inhibitory Area
In order to reveal the afferent structures to the MIA that may be important for the expression of muscle atonia, the retrograde tracer DiI was infused into the medial medulla in 5 P8 rats (Figure 1). Medial DiI infusions resulted in light but widespread labeling throughout the pontomedullary axis. Within the medulla, the giant cells of nucleus gigantocellularis (Gi) were consistently labeled (Figure 2A), as were neurons in nucleus magnocellularis and nucleus paramedianis. The vestibular nuclei and cholinergic C3 were lightly labeled and the nucleus of the solitary tract was moderately labeled. Light to moderate labeling was also found along the longitudinal axis from Gi through the nucleus pontis caudalis (PC) to the nucleus pontis oralis (PO). Within the pons, clusters of labeled neurons were consistently found ventral to the locus coeruleus (LC) in the subcoeruleus (alpha, dorsal, and ventral parts, hereafter designated SubLC; Figure 2B). Neurons associated with the midline raphé system (i.e., raphé obscurus and medial and dorsal raphé, DR) were heavily labeled (Figure 2C). Finally, neurons of the dorsal pontine tegmentum, including the laterodorsal tegmental nucleus (LDT; Figure 2D), the pendunculopontine tegmental nucleus (PPT), and the periaqueductal gray were lightly, but consistently, labeled.
Because our previous study [17] had indicated that motor facilitatory neurons were typically found lateral to the MIA, DiI was also infused laterally to the MIA in five additional P8 rats. Overall, few differences in labeling were observed between the medially and laterally infused groups, with the exception that neurons associated with the raphé system were labeled only after medial infusions.
After visual inspection of the tracing data, seven medullary and pontine structures known to modulate behavioral state regulation in adults (the Gi, PC, SubLC, LC, PO, LDT, and DR) were selected for analysis. The cell counts for these seven structures are summarized in Table 1.
Table 1. Mean Number (± Standard Error) of DiI-Labeled Neurons within Seven Medullary and Mesopontine Structures in P8 Rats.
DiI was infused into the medial or lateral medulla. See Figure 1 for location and extent of infusion sites
Gi, nucleus gigantocellularis; DR, dorsal raphé; LC, locus coeruleus; LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nucleus; PC, nucleus pontis caudalis; PO, nucleus pontis oralis; SubLC, subcoeruleus
Extracellular Recordings Reveal State-Dependent Neuronal Activity in Medullary and Mesopontine Structures
A total of 142 neurons were recorded from 25 P6–P10 rats (1–6 neurons per rat; 1–4 neurons per recording site) with precollicular decerebrations. (Although precollicular decerebrations disconnect the brainstem from rostral structures known to regulate sleep and wakefulness, infants nonetheless exhibit coherent organization of sleep-wake states; the most noticeable change induced by these decerebrations is more rapid cycling between sleep and wakefulness [19].) Fifty-two neurons did not exhibit any state dependency and were not analyzed further. Five neurons were excluded due to unclear histology. The remaining 85 neurons were assigned to one of five classes on the basis of the specific relationship of the unit activity to the components of sleep and wakefulness (Figure 3A). The interrelations among the classes, and the number of neurons found within each class, are depicted in Figure 3B.
The Lateral Medulla Contains Both EMG-On and Atonia-On Neurons
A total of 13 state-dependent neurons were recorded in the medulla, all within the Gi (Figure 4A). Eight neurons exhibited significantly lower discharge rates during periods of atonia (indicative of sleep) than during periods of high muscle tone (indicative of wakefulness; discharge rates, 1.74 ± 0.38 Hz during atonia; 3.79 ± 0.60 Hz during high tone), and five neurons exhibited significantly higher discharge rates during atonia than high tone (discharge rates, 1.21 ± 0.40 Hz during atonia; 0.76 ± 0.29 Hz during high tone). Representative single unit activity and concurrently recorded electromyograms (EMGs) are depicted in Figures 4C and 4D. Average spike durations of atonia-on and EMG-on neurons were 1.87 ± 0.01 ms and 1.74 ± 0.07 ms, respectively (a single-factor ANOVA revealed no significant group differences in spike durations between any of the classes of neurons reported in the current study).
The Subcoeruleus Contains a High Concentration of Atonia-On Neurons
Of a total of 38 neurons classified as atonia-on, 17 were located within the SubLC (discharge rates, 2.64 ± 0.48 Hz during atonia; 0.75 ± 0.23 Hz during high tone); EMG-on neurons were not found within the SubLC. Within the brainstem, atonia-on neurons were also recorded in the dorsomedial tegmental nucleus, ventral tegmental nucleus, and the medial and lateral parabrachial nuclei (MPB and LPB, respectively; see Figure 5A). Spike durations ranged from 1.46 ± 0.75 ms for atonia-on neurons in the LPB to 1.87 ± 0.03 ms for atonia-on neurons in the SubLC. The averaged spike waveform of a representative atonia-on neuron recorded within the SubLC is depicted in Figure 5B. Typically, neurons that were classified as atonia-on discharged tonically throughout the atonia period (Figure 5C).
A Subset of Atonia-On Neurons Discharge at Higher Rates during Active Sleep
In contrast to the neuron depicted in Figure 5C, nine of the atonia-on neurons exhibited significantly higher discharge rates during AS, defined as periods of atonia accompanied by bouts of twitches of the tail and nuchal muscle. These neurons were classified as AS-on (see Materials and Methods). A representative recording of multiunit activity that includes an AS-on neuron is depicted in Figure 5D. In Figure 5E the average discharge rate of a representative AS-on neuron during AS and QS (defined as periods of atonia without myoclonic twitching) is depicted.
Four neurons recorded within the SubLC were AS-on (discharge rates, 1.84 ± 0.67 Hz during AS; 0.73 ± 0.28 Hz during QS); three medullary neurons were AS-on (discharge rates, 1.99 ± 0.60 Hz during AS; 0.91 ± 0.30 during QS); and two neurons within the MPB were AS-on (discharge rates, 0.43 ± 0.02 Hz during AS; 0.09 ± 0.02 Hz during QS). Spike widths of AS-on neurons ranged from 1.95 ± 0.10 ms in the SubLC to 2.03 ± 0.16 ms in Gi.
The Infant Locus Coeruleus Contains EMG-On Neurons
Neurons exhibiting EMG-on profiles were recorded in the LPB, PC, and the LC. Within the LC (Figure 6A), two such units were found. The averaged spike waveform for one of these units is depicted in Figure 6B (both neurons exhibited short spike durations, 1.76 ± 0.12 ms). As shown in Figure 6C, both LC units increased their discharge rates primarily during periods of high muscle tone and were virtually silent during periods of atonia (discharge rates, 0.38 ± 0.17 during atonia; 7.81 ± 0.06 Hz during high tone), apart from rare bursts of activity during atonia (e.g., as indicated by the arrow in Figure 6C).
The Dorsolateral Pontine Tegmentum Contains EMG-On Neurons
In Figure 7A, the recording sites within the pontine tegmentum are depicted. Seventeen of 37 EMG-on neurons were located within a small region of the dorsolateral pontine tegmentum that includes LDT, the cuneiform nucleus, and periaqueductal gray; the remainder were distributed within the LC (see Figure 6), PC (see Figure 5), and Gi (see Figure 4). An averaged spike waveform of a representative EMG-on neuron, recorded in LDT, is depicted in Figure 7B (mean spike durations of EMG-on neurons ranged from 1.62 ± 0.11 ms in the LDT to 2.36 ± 0.14 ms in the PC) and a representative recording of multiunit activity that includes an EMG-on neuron is depicted in Figure 7C. The discharge rates of non-LDT EMG-on neurons ranged from 0.66 ± 0.40 Hz during atonia in the PC to 4.75 ± 1.09 Hz during high tone in the cuneiform nucleus. The relatively high discharge rates (5.67 ± 1.49 Hz during atonia and 11.10 ± 2.22 Hz during high tone) and short spike durations of LDT EMG-on neurons suggest that they may be non-cholinergic [20].
The Laterodorsal Tegmental Nucleus Contains Twitch-On Neurons
The LDT contains neurons that exhibit a burst of action potentials in anticipation of myoclonic twitches (designated as twitch-on neurons). Ten of the 11 recorded neurons that exhibited this activity pattern were found within the LDT (Figure 7A); one neuron was located in the parabrachial nucleus (see Figure 5A). Six of these neurons were both EMG- and twitch-on (designated as EMG-on/twitch-on); that is, they exhibited higher discharge rates during periods of high muscle tone than during periods of atonia (mean discharge rates, 15.92 ± 5.93 during high tone; 6.80 ± 5.61 during atonia) while also firing in anticipation of myoclonic twitches (see below). In addition, the EMG-on/twitch-on neurons had slightly broader average spike durations than did twitch-on neurons (1.92 ± 0.13 ms and 1.63 ± 0.16 ms, respectively).
Over the 60-ms period before a myoclonic twitch, the mean discharge rate of twitch-on neurons increased dramatically, from 2.81 ± 0.87 Hz to 24.22 ± 6.67 Hz. In Figure 7D, a representative recording of multiunit activity that includes a twitch-on neuron is depicted, and the tight coupling of the bursts of multiunit activity and nuchal twitches is apparent. Figure 7E depicts an expanded view of the boxed area from Figure 7D and identifies (with asterisks) an isolated twitch-on unit. Figure 7F depicts a peristimulus histogram and raster plot of an identified unit (the same unit depicted in Figure 7E) triggered on nuchal EMG spikes. Immediately preceding the onset of a twitch, the discharge rate increased more than 30-fold, suggesting that this neuron participates in the production of twitching. It is important to note, however, that the relationship between neuronal discharges and twitches was not perfect; that is, there were occasions when the neuron discharged and a nuchal twitch was not detected, and there were occasions when a nuchal twitch was detected but the neuron did not discharge.
Lesions of Subcoeruleus and Nucleus Pontis Oralis Reduce Atonia Durations and Produce “REM Sleep without Atonia”
As described above, the SubLC contains a high concentration of atonia-on neurons (see Figure 5); in addition, both the present study (see Figure 7) and that of Corner and Bour [10] suggest that the PO is important for regulating infant sleep. To examine the causal roles played by these areas in sleep and wakefulness, both areas were lesioned here using high concentrations (50 mM) of quisqualic acid, a glutamate receptor agonist (Figure 8).
The extent of the lesions of the SubLC (n = 4) and PO (n = 5) is depicted in Figure 8A. As shown in the bar graphs of Figure 8B, these lesions produced significant changes in the mean bout durations of atonia and high muscle tone (atonia, F[2,13] = 10.02, p < 0.01; high tone, F[2,13] = 4.07, p < 0.01). Moreover, as indicated by the pie charts in Figure 8B, the percentage of time in atonia was significantly reduced in the lesion groups (F[2,13] = 14.2, p < 0.0005). Representative recordings of nuchal EMG in pups with SubLC and PO lesions are shown in Figure 8D.
Lesions of SubLC and PO also disrupted the expression of myoclonic twitching of the limbs and tail. Although the absolute number of myoclonic twitches during the 15-min scoring period did not differ between the lesion and sham groups (F[2,13] = 0.79, not significant), lesions of SubLC and PO resulted in significant increases in the proportion of twitches that occurred against a background of high muscle tone (F[2,13] = 7.91, p < 0.01). Specifically, in the SubLC and PO lesion groups, respectively, 27.1% and 16.1% of all twitches were expressed against a background of high muscle tone, in contrast with only 0.01% in the sham group.
Lesions of the Dorsolateral Pontine Tegmentum Increase Atonia Durations and Suppress Myoclonic Twitching
Because the DLPT contained a high concentration of EMG-on and twitch-on neurons (see Figure 7), and because of difficulties using chemical lesion techniques in this area (see Materials and Methods), electrolytic lesions of the DLPT were performed; the extent of the lesions is shown in Figure 8A. As shown in the bar graphs of Figure 8C and in contrast to lesions of SubLC and PO, lesions of the DLPT produced a sizable increase in the mean bout duration of atonia (t = 6.7, df = 11, p < 0.01) and a decrease in the mean bout duration of high muscle tone (t = 3.3, df = 11, p < 0.01). Consequently, as indicated by the pie charts in Figure 8C, the percentage of time in atonia was significantly increased in the lesion group (t = 6.4, df = 11, p < 0.0001). A representative recording of nuchal EMG in a pup with a DLPT lesion is shown in Figure 8D.
In contrast with lesions of SubLC and PO, DLPT lesions significantly reduced the number of twitches. In the sham group, the mean number of limb twitches over the 15-min period was 518.3 ± 34.3, compared to 302.9 ± 51.7 in the lesion group (t = 3.3, df = 11, p < 0.01). Also, in contrast to the SubLC and PO lesion groups and similar to shams, less than 0.1% of twitches in pups with DLPT lesions were observed during periods of high muscle tone.
Discussion
In the present study, we outline, to our knowledge for the first time, the neural substrates of two primary sleep components in week-old rats. First, we confirm that the inputs to the MIA from medullary and mesopontine structures are similar to those reported in adults [21,22,23]. Second, neurons exhibiting atonia-on discharge profiles (indicative of sleep) were found predominantly within the SubLC, and neurons exhibiting EMG-on discharge profiles (indicative of wakefulness) were found predominantly within the LDT. Third, we report the presence of neurons that exhibit a distinct bursting pattern that anticipates myoclonic twitches. Fourth, consistent with the recording data, we demonstrate that atonia durations are decreased after lesions of SubLC and PO, and myoclonic twitching is reduced after lesions of the DLPT despite a dramatic increase in atonia duration. Finally, lesions of SubLC and PO decoupled myoclonic twitching from nuchal atonia, producing a condition that resembles REM sleep without atonia as described in juvenile and adult rats [24,25].
The present study adds critical new evidence in support of the notion that sleep in infant rats satisfies most conventional criteria for defining sleep, including state-dependent neural activity [13,14]. This study also supports the contention that the development of sleep in mammals entails the continuous elaboration of components that are identifiable in early infancy [5,6,26,27,28]. These experiments are particularly significant in light of the fact that they were performed using an altricial species at ages before the onset of sleep-related neocortical activity [1,2,3]. Consequently, these results may prove to be broadly applicable to our understanding of sleep mechanisms in other mammalian species at an analogous time in development, including the period before 120 d postconception in sheep [29,30], 50 d postconception in guinea pigs [31], approximately 32 wk postconception in preterm human infants [32], and P13–15 in kittens [28].
We did not find state-dependent activity in the DR despite its heavy labeling after infusions of DiI into the MIA. Nonetheless, it is possible that the DR does contain state-dependent neurons at the ages studied here. For example, it is possible that cessation of DR activity during sleep depends on histaminergic input from the tuberomammillary nucleus [33], a region that was disconnected by the precollicular decerebrations used here (see Materials and Methods). Conversely, within the sparsely labeled LDT, we found neurons that exhibited state-dependent discharge profiles.
The medulla of adults contains neurons that participate in the generation of both sleep and wake phenomena [34]. In adult cats, classes of medullary neurons have been identified that discharge during wakefulness, during REM sleep [35,36,37], and during REM sleep as well as waking movements [35,38,39]. Medullary neurons with similar discharge profiles have also been identified in adults rats [40,41]. In the present study, we found medullary EMG-on neurons that compare well with the wake-on neurons reported in adult rats [40,41] and cats [38,39], atonia-on neurons that compare well with the REM-sleep specific neurons reported by Kanamori et al. [36], and AS-on neurons that compare well with the phasic REM sleep neurons reported by Steriade et al. [35]. It should be noted that we did not find AS-on neurons in the brainstem that exhibited high, tonic rates of discharge typical of some REM-on neurons in adults [33]. More extensive sampling will be necessary to describe fully the activity patterns exhibited by AS-on neurons in infants.
To our knowledge, LC recordings have not been reported previously in unanesthetized infant rats. Moreover, the EMG-on profile of the LC units described here is consistent with their wake-active profile in adult rats [42]. Interestingly, this LC activity profile is consistent with the notion that increased olfactory sensory thresholds during infant sleep result from a decrease in excitatory LC input to mitral cells of the olfactory bulbs [15,43].
In adult cats, REM sleep depends on the integrity of the peri-LC-α, which corresponds to the sublaterodorsal nucleus (SLD) in rats [33,44,45] and is contained within the region designated as the SubLC here. In a recent study aimed at elucidating the neural substrates of sleep in adult rats, Boissard et al. [44] demonstrated that REM sleep can be induced by increasing SLD activity, either by decreasing GABAergic inhibition or by increasing glutamatergic excitation. Moreover, it was demonstrated that the SLD projects to neurons in the medial medulla that also stain positively for glycine [44].
The current study supports and amplifies the findings of Boissard et al. [44]. First, we did not find any neurons within the SubLC that exhibited an EMG-on profile, and the SubLC contained 17 of the 29 atonia-on neurons reported here. Second, using DiI, we demonstrated that the SubLC connects directly with the MIA. Third, chemical lesions of the SubLC significantly reduced atonia durations. Finally, we note that excitation of the infant MIA with a glutamate agonist is sufficient to produce atonia [17]. All together, these findings are consistent with the notion that atonia in infant and adult rats is produced in part by glutamatergic excitatory input from the SubLC to glycinergic neurons of the ventromedial medulla that, in turn, hyperpolarize spinal motoneurons [46,47].
Although lesions of both the SubLC and PO dramatically reduced the duration of atonia periods, they did not reduce rates of myoclonic twitching. It is not surprising, then, that there were many instances when twitches were observed during periods of high muscle tone, a decoupling of tonic and phasic sleep components that resembles REM sleep without atonia produced in adults after pontine [24] or SubLC [48] lesions.
The increase in atonia and the reduction in twitching after DLPT lesions may have resulted, respectively, from the disinhibition of atonia facilitatory neurons in the reticular formation (perhaps within the PO) and the loss of twitch-on neurons within the LDT. Of course, it is also possible that these electrolytic lesions exerted their effects by destroying nearby structures or fibers of passage. We do know, however, that although the DLPT lesions were large and frequently included the LC and SubLC, the effect of the DLPT lesions on atonia and twitching cannot be attributed to damage to either of those two areas, because electrolytic lesions of the LC [19] and chemical lesions of the SubLC (as shown in the present study) reduced mean atonia durations without affecting twitching. Thus, after large DLPT lesions, the PO may contribute to the elongation of atonia periods via direct and mutually facilitatory interactions with the MIA [49].
Previous work in adult cats has explored the importance of the cholinergic mesopontine tegmentum in REM sleep generation [50,51]. The present findings are consistent with this earlier work (although it should be stressed that we have not demonstrated here that these infant mesopontine mechanisms utilize acetylcholine). Webster et al. [50] reported significant decreases in REM sleep after large lesions that destroyed the majority of cholinergic neurons within the LDT, PPT, the parabrachial nucleus, and the LC. Shouse and Siegel [51] demonstrated that lesions primarily involving the PPT resulted in the loss of phasic sleep components, whereas lesions of the SubLC resulted in REM sleep without atonia. In contrast, lesions primarily localized to the LDT and LC did not alter the expression of REM sleep components. Recently, it was reported that discrete PPT lesions in adult rats did not affect sleep time but did interfere with sleep propensity after sleep deprivation [52]; unfortunately, phasic sleep events were not recorded in that study.
In adult rats, REM sleep induced by selective activation of the SLD lacks phasic components (e.g., rapid eye movements) [44]. The authors of that study hypothesized that activation of the phasic components of REM sleep requires activation of neurons within the LDT, presumably in the form of burst-pause discharges such as those identified previously in reticular neurons in adult cats [38,39]. The presence of such discharges in adult rats, however, has been questioned [53]. Here we demonstrate that the LDT (and the parabrachial nucleus) contains twitch-on neurons that appear to participate in the generation of myoclonic twitching of the limbs and nuchal muscle. The burst-pause discharge profile of these twitch-on neurons compares well with neurons recorded in the pontine tegmentum of adult cats (located among fibers of the brachium conjunctivum) that exhibit sharp bursts that anticipate pontine-geniculo-occipital waves [54]. In this regard, it may be significant that LDT neurons burst after activation of the low-threshold calcium current [55,56,57], thus allowing them to behave as single-cell oscillators [58]. We have here, then, a mechanism by which LDT neurons can produce phasic events such as myoclonic twitches. The reduced rates of twitching after DLPT lesions, as shown here, is consistent with this view.
The bursting of a twitch-on neuron was not always associated with a nuchal muscle twitch. One possible explanation for this is that the downstream effects of the activity of twitch-on neurons are gated by the moment-to-moment levels of glycinergic inhibition provided by descending axons from the MIA [17,46,47]. It is also possible that a burst of activity by twitch-on neurons serves to momentarily override the hyperpolarization of spinal motoneurons and thus allows spinal networks to exhibit behaviorally observable spontaneous activity [59].
It has recently been suggested that myoclonic twitches in infants contribute to the self-organization of neural circuits within the spinal cord that govern withdrawal responses to aversive stimuli [60]. Specifically, it was argued that a limb movement during a twitch provides sensory feedback that helps to calibrate interactions among neurons within a reflex module; according to this hypothesis, the temporal correlations among motor and sensory events modify synapses through a Hebbian-type learning process. Indeed, we note that the information generated by the feedback from a single limb twitch against a background of muscle atonia represents a situation that is well suited for Hebbian learning to occur [61]. But this idea may also apply to any myoclonic twitch, whether produced by phasic activation of limb muscles, nuchal muscle, or eye muscles; in fact, phasic activity during sleep in infants is a global phenomenon occurring as temporally coherent bouts of activity in many muscle groups throughout the body [62]. Therefore, we believe that myoclonic twitching will prove to play a broad role in early motor and somatosensory development [26,63].
Whereas the ontogeny of mammalian sleep remains a topic of dispute [7,14], some aspects of that debate can now be put to rest. First, the extracellular recordings reported here, and previously [17], demonstrate that the discharge profiles of multiple medullary and mesopontine nuclei are tightly coupled to behavioral state. Second, it appears that infant sleep is not mediated by distinct neurophysiological mechanisms, but rather by the very medullary and mesopontine nuclei that participate in sleep expression in adults. Such findings are not easily reconciled with numerous previous statements concerning the ontogeny of mammalian sleep and render moot attempts to explain sleep ontogeny as the gradual emergence of a coherent state from a set of undifferentiated, distinct, diffuse, or primitive sleep mechanisms [1,7,8,9]. In contrast, we have argued that the development of sleep entails the elaboration of an elementary sleep circuit that is largely in place soon after birth in rats [14]; accordingly, sleep components that emerge during ontogeny do so by integrating with those components that already exist. Of course, significant differences exist between infant and adult sleep and should not be discounted; for example, perhaps the most striking difference between infant and adult sleep is the rapidity with which infants cycle between sleep and wakefulness [19,64].
Establishing the developmental relations between sleep in infants and adults has important implications for theories concerning the functions of sleep. Many such theories can be applied to infants only with difficulty, because they address behaviors or contexts that are specific to adults. In addition, some theories place particular emphasis on the cerebral cortex [65,66], a part of the brain that, as mentioned above, does not exhibit state-dependent activity at those ages when infants are thought to sleep the most (e.g., before P12 in rats). If infant and adult sleep were as mechanistically distinct as some have argued, then one might imagine that sleep serves qualitatively different functions at different ages. But, as shown here, the basic mechanisms of infant and adult sleep are not distinct—even at ages before state-dependent neocortical activity is expressed—suggesting that there may be fundamental similarities between the functions of infant and adult sleep that have largely been overlooked. Thus, we believe that obtaining a clearer understanding of the mechanisms of sleep in infants is central to the goal of developing a theory of sleep that has applicability across the lifespan and can account for the inordinate amount of sleep during infancy [67].
Materials and Methods
All experiments were performed in accordance with National Institutes of Health guidelines for the care of animals in research and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Iowa. All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering.
Subjects
A total of 64 P6–P10 Sprague-Dawley Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus) of both sexes from 46 litters were used. Litters were culled to 8 pups within 3 d of birth (day of birth = day 0). Mothers and their litters were housed in standard laboratory cages (48 × 20 × 26 cm) in the animal colony at the University of Iowa where food and water were available ad libitum. All animals were maintained on a 12:12 h light-dark schedule with lights on at 07:00 h. All experiments were conducted during the lights-on phase.
Retrograde tracing
Ten P8 rats from four litters were used (body weights, 16.1–22.9 g). While the pup was anesthetized with isoflurane, the head was secured in a stereotaxic instrument (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, California, United States) and leveled between bregma and lambda. A small burr hole was drilled over the target area and the retrograde tracer DiI (Molecular Probes, Eugene, Oregon, United States; 20 nl of 5% DiI in DMSO) was infused at the rate of 1nl/s using a microsyringe (model 7000.50C, Hamilton, Reno, Nevada, United States). Infusions were localized to the medial medulla (n = 5) and immediately lateral to it (n = 5), as described previously [17]. Coordinates relative to lambda for the medial site were AP, −2.5 mm; ML, 0.0 mm; DV, −12.3 mm; for the lateral site, the coordinates were AP, −2.5 mm; ML, 1.0 mm; DV, −12.3 mm. After the infusion was complete, the syringe remained in place for 5 min, was then retracted slowly, and the wound closed using cyanoacrylate adhesive.
After surgery, pups recovered in a warm, humidified incubator for 1–2 h before being returned to the home cage. After 26–28 h, the pups were overdosed with sodium pentobarbital (approximately 100 mg/kg intraperitoneally) and perfused transcardially with physiological saline followed by 3% formalin. Brains were postfixed in the skull for 1–2 d in a formalin-sucrose solution and then removed from the skull and fixed for at least two more days in a fresh solution. After fixation, the brains were sliced in 50-μm coronal sections with a sliding microtome (model SM 2000 R, Leica, Bensheim, Germany). The slices were mounted and coverslipped with Vectashield (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, California, United States) and stored at 2–4 °C until inspected and photographed under fluorescent light using a digital microscope (model BX-51, Olympus, Japan).
Sections were visually inspected throughout the pontomedullary axis, and seven areas were selected for cell counting (Table 1). Next, digital photographs of one representative section of all seven structures were obtained from the same coronal level from each subject. Every effort was made to ensure that the cell counts were performed at the same level in each brain (roughly corresponding to plates 54, 55, 56, 58, and 66 in the adult rat brain atlas of Paxinos and Watson [68]). Finally, labeled cells were counted manually from each photo using ImageJ software (National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD).
Electrophysiology
Twenty-five P6–P10 rats from 20 litters were used (body weights, 13.4–25.7 g). Under isoflurane anesthesia, a precollicular decerebration was performed and bipolar nuchal EMG electrodes were implanted bilaterally. The infant's fragile skull was bleached, dried, and then coated with Vetbond (3M, St. Paul, Minnesota, United States) to add strength. Next, a custom-built, T-shaped, stainless steel head-plant, designed to attach to the earbar and nosebar holders of a stereotaxic instrument, was attached to the skull over the pretreated area using cyanoacrylate adhesive gel. After securing the head-plant and leveling the skull, a small hole was drilled over the recording site. Finally, to inhibit movement and calm the pup, it was wrapped in gauze [69] without obscuring movements of the tail.
Recordings were performed in a stereotaxic instrument with brain and body temperatures maintained at approximately 37 °C. Within 2–3 h of surgery, pups began exhibiting sleep-wake cycles, as judged by oscillations in muscle tone as well as myoclonic twitches against a background of nuchal atonia [19]. When sleep-wake cyclicity was observed, a stainless steel 8-trode (ALA Science, Westbury, New York United States), connected to a unity gain headstage and digital amplifier (Tucker-Davis Technologies, Alachua, Florida, United States), was lowered into the recording site while neurophysiological activity was monitored using an oscilloscope and audio analyzer (FHC, Bowdoinham, Maine, United States). The 8-trode consists of eight individual recording sites distributed about the tip (500–800 kΩ per electrode), with the entire electrode having a diameter of approximately 150 μm. The signals were amplified (10k) and filtered (500–5000 Hz band-pass) before being sampled at 12.5 kHz. When stable units were observed and at least one of these units appeared to exhibit state-dependent activity, the electrode was left in place for 5–10 min before data collection began. Neurophysiological and EMG data were recorded synchronously for 10 min (encompassing 4–12 sleep-wake cycles) to hard disk for off-line analysis. At the end of the experiment, the recording site was marked by passing a 50 μA anodal current through the eight closely spaced electrodes for 3 s (thereby producing a single small lesion).
In the current study, nuchal EMG was used as an indicator of behavioral state. As described previously [19], the EMG signal was dichotomized into bouts of high muscle tone and atonia (indicative of wakefulness and sleep, respectively). The sleep state was further categorized based on the presence or absence of phasic events (i.e., based on visual observation of myoclonic twitching of the tail, or based on twitching detected in the nuchal EMG). The reliable relationship between myoclonic twitching as assessed by behavioral observation or nuchal EMG has now been documented [62].
Four of the eight continuously recorded channels containing heterogeneous unit activity were selected for off-line analysis. Using Spike2 software (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, United Kingdom), a threshold was set for each of the four selected channels to extract spike data with a signal-to-noise ratio exceeding 2:1. Using the multichannel data, principal components analysis was then used for spike sorting [70,71]. The sorted units were assigned to groups using graphical cluster cutting [72,73]. To remove artificial clusters, spike waveforms were inspected and autocorrelations were constructed for each cluster. A cluster was deemed to contain a single unit only if the autocorrelation analysis indicated a refractory period of at least 2 ms.
Three types of analyses were performed. The first analysis was aimed at classifying neurons as either EMG-on or atonia-on. Periods of nuchal atonia and high muscle tone were identified from the EMG record, as described above. Next, the firing rates of individual sorted units were compared with the behavioral state of the animal across 4–12 pairs of high tone and atonia periods. State-related differences in mean discharge rates were tested using the Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test [74]; alpha was set at 0.05. Neurons that significantly increased their firing rates during periods of high muscle tone were designated as EMG-on, and neurons that significantly increased their firing rates during atonia periods were designates as atonia-on.
The second analysis was aimed at determining whether discharge rates differed between sleep bouts that did or did not contain myoclonic twitching. Discharge rates during twelve 4-s segments of atonia with less than or equal to one nuchal EMG spike (indicative of sleep without phasic processes; i.e., QS), and discharge rates during twelve 4-s segments of atonia with more than four nuchal EMG spikes (indicative of sleep with phasic processes; i.e., active sleep, AS) were compared using the Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test; alpha was set at 0.05.
The third analysis was aimed at revealing the relationship between the nuchal EMG twitches and neuronal burst activity. To this end, an event marker was placed on the center of all nuchal EMG twitches that occurred during a recording session. Next, a peristimulus histogram of unit activity triggered on the event marker was generated. Neurons were classified as twitch-on if the average discharge rate increased at least 4-fold before a twitch (measured 0–20 ms and 40–60 ms before a twitch).
For all experiments presented here, means are presented with their standard errors.
Brains were prepared for histological analysis as described above. After fixation, the brains were sliced in 25–50-μm sections with a sliding microtome and stained with cresyl violet. The locations of the marking lesions were determined by examining serial sections.
Lesions
Chemical lesions of the SubLC and PO were performed in 16 P8 nondecerebrated rats from ten litters (body weights, 17.7–23.9 g). A pup was anesthetized with isoflurane and its head was secured in a stereotaxic instrument, and the skull was leveled between bregma and lambda. A small burr hole was drilled over the target area and a microsyringe (model 7000.50C, Hamilton) was lowered stereotaxically either into the SubLC (n = 4; coordinates relative to lambda: AP, −1.8 mm; ML, ± 1.0 mm; DV, −11.8 mm) or into the PO (n = 5; coordinates relative to lambda: AP, −1.2 mm; ML, ± 1.0 mm; DV, −12.3 mm). Lesions were produced by infusing a high concentration of quisqualic acid (50 mM; 150 nl over 15 s) as described previously [17]. Control animals were treated identically but received infusions of Ringers solution only into the PO (n = 5) and SubLC (n = 2).
Lesions of the DLPT were performed in 13 P8 nondecerebrated rats from 12 litters (body weights, 17.4–21.8 g). Because previous attempts to use ibotenic acid in the brainstem of pups resulted in very high rates of mortality, and attempts to use quisqualic acid to lesion the DLPT were unsuccessful, electrolytic lesions were used here. After the pup was prepared for stereotaxic surgery, as described above, a concentric, bipolar, tungsten electrode (1 MΩ, 3–4 μm at the tip, model TM33CCINS, World Precision Instruments, Sarasota, Florida, United States) connected to a stimulus isolator (model A395, World Precision Instruments) and a stimulus generator (model 48, Grass, Quincy, Massachusetts, United States) was lowered into the PPT/LDT area (coordinates relative to lambda: AP, −0.8 mm; ML, ± 0.8 mm; DV, −4.4 mm). Lesions were produced by applying a 150-μA current for 15–30 s (n = 7). Control animals received identical treatment but without the current (n = 6).
After a chemical or electrolytic lesion was produced, bipolar nuchal EMG electrodes were implanted bilaterally, the pup was placed on a felt pad in a supine position (to enable observation of twitching in the individual limbs), and lightly restrained as described previously [4]. Next, the pups recovered in a humidified incubator (35 °C) for 3 h. After recovery, pups were intubated and infused with 2–4% of their body weight with warm cream and transferred to a humidified recording chamber maintained at 35 °C. A microcamera placed above the lid of the recording chamber allowed recording of sleep-wake behaviors, as described elsewhere [4]. Video data were recorded using a digital video system (model DV8, Vetron, Rebersburg, Pennsylvania, United States). After 1 h of acclimation to the chamber, synchronized behavioral and EMG data were collected for 1 h and recorded to digital tape for off-line scoring and analysis.
EMG signals were digitized at 5 kHz using a data acquisition system (BioPac Systems, Santa Barbara, California, United States). Next, the EMG signals were summed, integrated, and full-wave rectified, and mean bout durations of high-tone and atonia periods were measured over 60 min, as described previously [19]. Next, a trained observer viewed a 15-min taped segment of the pup's behavior and, using an event recorder, pressed a key when myoclonic twitches were detected [4]. Myoclonic twitches are phasic, rapid, and independent movements of the limbs and tail; high-amplitude movements comprise such wake-related behaviors as stretching, kicking, and yawning [75,76]. We and others have used similar scoring procedures in the past and have found them to be highly reliable, with inter- and intrarater reliability coefficients typically exceeding 0.85 [77,78]. The total number of twitches during the 15-min scoring session was determined, as well as the number of twitches that occurred against a background of high muscle tone.
For the DLPT lesion experiment, group differences in the expression of sleep components were assessed using an unpaired t-test. For the SubLC and PO lesion experiment, group differences were assessed using a single-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA); the post hoc test was Fisher's PLSD. For all tests, alpha was set at 0.05.
Histological methods were identical to those described above.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH50701 and MH66424). We thank Cynthia Shaw and Jessica Middlemis-Brown for technical assistance. We also thank the staff at the Central Microscopy Research Facilities at the University of Iowa.
Competing interests. The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
Abbreviations
- AS
active sleep
- DLPT
dorsolateral pontine tegmentum
- DR
dorsal raphé
- EEG
electroencephalogram
- EMG
electromyogram
- Gi
nucleus gigantocellularis
- LC
locus coeruleus
- LDT
laterodorsal tegmental nucleus
- LPB
lateral parabrachial nucleus
- MIA
medullary inhibitory area
- MPB
medial parabrachial nucleus
- P[number]
postnatal day [number]
- PC
nucleus pontis caudalis
- PO
nucleus pontis oralis
- PPT
pendunculopontine tegmental nucleus
- QS
quiet sleep
- REM
rapid eye movement
- SLD
sublaterodorsal nucleus
- SubLC
subcoeruleus
Author contributions. KÆK and MSB conceived and designed the experiments. KÆK, AJG, EJM, and AMHS performed the experiments. KÆK and AJG analyzed the data. KÆK and MSB wrote the paper.
Citation: Karlsson KÆ, Gall AJ, Mohns EJ, Seelke AMH, Blumberg MS (2005) The neural substrates of infant sleep in rats. PLoS Biol 3(5): e143.
References
- Frank MG, Heller HC. Development of REM and slow wave sleep in the rat. Am J Physiol. 1997;272:R1792–R1799. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1997.272.6.R1792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gramsbergen A. The development of the EEG in the rat. Dev Psychobiol. 1976;9:501–515. doi: 10.1002/dev.420090604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirmiran M, Corner M. Neuronal discharge patterns in the occipital cortex of developing rats during active and quiet sleep. Brain Res. 1982;255:37–48. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(82)90074-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson KÆ, Blumberg MS. The union of the state: Myoclonic twitching is coupled with nuchal muscle atonia in infant rats. Behav Neurosci. 2002;116:912–917. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.5.912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corner MA. Sleep and the beginnings of behavior in the animal kingdom—Studies of ultradian motility cycles in early life. Prog Neurobiol. 1977;8:279–295. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(77)90008-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corner MA. Ontogeny of brain sleep mechanisms. In: McGinty DJ, editor. Brain mechanisms of sleep. New York: Raven Press; 1985. pp. 175–197. [Google Scholar]
- Frank MG, Heller HC. The ontogeny of mammalian sleep: A reappraisal of alternative hypotheses. J Sleep Res. 2003;12:25–34. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2003.00339.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adrien J. Ontogenesis of some sleep regulations: Early postnatal impairment of the monoaminergic systems. Prog Brain Res. 1977;48:393–403. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)61037-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adrien J, Lanfumey L. Neuronal activity of the developing raphe dorsalis: Its relation with the states of vigilance. Exp Brain Res Supp. 1984;8:67–78. [Google Scholar]
- Corner MA, Bour HL. Postnatal development of spontaneous neuronal discharges in the pontine reticular formation of free-moving rats during sleep and wakefulness. Exp Brain Res. 1984;54:66–72. doi: 10.1007/BF00235819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamásy V, Korányi L, Lissák K. Early postnatal development of wakefulness-sleep cycle and neuronal responsiveness: A multiunit activity study on freely moving newborn rat. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1980;49:102–111. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90356-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell SS, Tobler I. Animal sleep: A review of sleep duration across phylogeny. Neurosci Biobehav Res. 1984;8:269–300. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(84)90054-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendricks JC, Sehgal A, Pack A. The need for a simple animal model to understand sleep. Prog Neurobiol. 2000;61:339–351. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(99)00048-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg MS, Karlsson KÆ, Seelke AMH, Mohns EJ. The ontogeny of mammalian sleep: A response to Frank and Heller (2003) J Sleep Res. 2005;14:91–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00430_1.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seelke AMH, Blumberg MS. Sniffing in infant rats during sleep and wakefulness. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:267–273. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.2.267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg MS, Middlemis-Brown JE, Johnson ED. Sleep homeostasis in infant rats. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:1253–1261. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.6.1253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson KÆ, Blumberg MS. Active medullary control of atonia in week-old rats. Neuroscience. 2005;130:275–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreider JC, Blumberg MS. Mesopontine contribution to the expression of active “twitch” sleep in decerebrate week-old rats. Brain Res. 2000;872:149–159. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(00)02518-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson KÆ, Kreider JC, Blumberg MS. Hypothalamic contribution to sleep-wake cycle development. Neuroscience. 2004;123:575–582. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.09.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koyama Y, Honda T, Kusakabe M, Kayama Y, Sugiura Y. In vivo electrophysiological distinction of histochemically-identified cholinergic neurons using extracellular recording and labelling in rat laterodorsal tegmental nucleus. Neuroscience. 1998;83:1105–1112. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00439-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Malinowska M, Kubin L. Neurons of the dorsomedial pontine tegmentum make synaptic contacts with caudal pontomedullary neurons sending projections to the dorsomedial pons. Sleep. 2004;27:A35. [Google Scholar]
- Cobos A, Lima D, Almeida A, Tavares I. Brain afferents to the lateral caudal ventrolateral medulla: A retrograde and anterograde tracing study in the rat. Neuroscience. 2003;120:485–498. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00209-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP, Martin GF, Waltzer R. An autoradiographic analysis of ascending projections from the medullary reticular formation in the rat. Neuroscience. 1986;19:873–898. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(86)90305-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison AR. Paradoxical sleep without atonia. Arch Ital Biol. 1988;126:275–289. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirmiran M. “Oneiric” behavior during active sleep induced by bilateral lesions of the pontine tegmentum in juvenile rats. In: Koella WP, editor. Sleep: Sixth European Congress of Sleep Research. Basel: Karger; 1982. pp. 236–239. [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg MS, Lucas DE. A developmental and component analysis of active sleep. Dev Psychobiol. 1996;29:1–22. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199601)29:1<1::AID-DEV1>3.0.CO;2-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGinty DJ, Stevenson M, Hoppenbrouwers T, Harper RM, Sterman MB, et al. Polygraphic studies of kitten development: Sleep state patterns. Dev Psychobiol. 1977;10:455–469. doi: 10.1002/dev.420100506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimizu A, Himwich H. The ontogeny of sleep in kittens and young rabbits. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1968;24:307–318. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clewlow F, Dawes GS, Johnston BM, Walker DW. Changes in breathing, electrocortical and muscle activity in unanaesthetized fetal lambs with age. J Physiol. 1983;341:463–476. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szeto HH, Hinman DJ. Prenatal development of sleep-wake patterns in sheep. Sleep. 1985;8:347–355. doi: 10.1093/sleep/8.4.347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Umans JG, Cox MJ, Hinman DJ, Dogramajian ME, Senger G, et al. The development of electrocortical activity in the fetal and neonatal guinea pig. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1985;153:467–471. doi: 10.1016/0002-9378(85)90089-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dreyfus-Brisac C. Neurophysiological studies in human premature and full-term newborns. Biol Psychiatry. 1975;10:485–496. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakai K, Crochet S. A neural mechanism of sleep and wakefulness. Sleep Biol Rhythms. 2003;1:29–42. [Google Scholar]
- Gottesmann C. The neurophysiology of sleep and waking: Intracerebral connections, functioning and ascending influences of the medulla oblongata. Prog Neurobiol. 1999;59:1–54. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00094-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steriade M, Sakai K, Jouvet M. Bulbo-thalamic neurons related to thalamocortical activation processes during paradoxical sleep. Exp Brain Res. 1984;54:463–475. doi: 10.1007/BF00235472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanamori N, Sakai K, Jouvet M. Neuronal activity specific to paradoxical sleep in the ventromedial medullary reticular formation of unrestrained cats. Brain Res. 1980;189:251–255. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90024-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Netick A, Orem J, Dement W. Neuronal activity specific to REM sleep and its relationship to breathing. Brain Res. 1977;120:197–207. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90900-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel JM, Wheeler RL, McGinty DJ. Activity of medullary reticular formation neurons in the unrestrained cat during waking and sleep. Brain Res. 1979;179:49–60. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90488-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siegel JM, McGinty DJ, Breedlove SM. Sleep and waking activity of pontine gigantocellular field neurons. Exp Neurol. 1977;56:553–573. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(77)90321-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP. Selective firing of rat pontine gigantocellular neurons during movement and REM sleep. Brain Res. 1977;128:146–152. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90242-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP. Brain stem gigantocellular neurons: Patterns of activity during behavior and sleep in the freely moving rat. J Neurophysiol. 1979;42:214–228. doi: 10.1152/jn.1979.42.1.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aston-Jones G, Bloom FE. Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle. J Neurosci. 1981;1:876–886. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.01-08-00876.1981. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aston-Jones G. The locus coeruleus. In: Paxinos G, editor. The rat nervous system. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2004. pp. 259–294. [Google Scholar]
- Boissard R, Gervasoni D, Schmidt MH, Barbagli B, Fort P, et al. The rat ponto-medullary network responsible for paradoxical sleep onset and maintenance: A combined microinjection and functional neuroanatomical study. Eur J Neurosci. 2002;16:1959–1973. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02257.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakai K, Crochet S, Onoe H. Pontine structures and mechanisms involved in the generation of paradoxical (REM) sleep. Arch Ital Biol. 2001;139:93–107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chase MH, Morales FR. The atonia and myoclonia of active (REM) sleep. Annu Rev Psychol. 1990;41:557–584. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.003013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chase MH, Morales FR. Control of motoneurons during sleep. In: Kryger MK, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and practice of sleep medicine. New York: Saunders; 2000. pp. 155–168. [Google Scholar]
- Lu J, Devor M, Saper CB. A pontine tegmental flip-flop switch for the regulation of REM sleep [abstract]. Program No. 895.6. 34th Annual Meeting of the Society of Neuroscience; 23–27 October 2004; San Diego, California. 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Kohyama J, Lai YY, Siegel JM. Inactivation of the pons blocks medullary-induced muscle tone suppression in the decerebrate cat. Sleep. 1998;21:695–699. doi: 10.1093/sleep/21.7.695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webster HH, Jones BE. Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsolateral pontomesencephalic tegmentum-cholinergic cell area in the cat. II. Effect upon sleep-waking states. Brain Res. 1988;458:285–302. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)90471-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shouse MN, Siegel JM. Pontine regulation of REM sleep components in cats: integrity of the pedunculopontine tegmentum (PPT) is important for phasic events but unnecessary for atonia during REM sleep. Brain Res. 1992;571:50–63. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90508-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deurveiler S, Hennevin E. Lesions of the pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus reduce paradoxical sleep (PS) propensity: Evidence from a short-term PS deprivation in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2001;13:1963–1976. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01562.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Datta S, Siwek DF. Single cell activity patterns of pedunculopontine tegmentum neurons across the sleep-wake cycle in the freely moving rats. J Neurosci Res. 2002;70:611–621. doi: 10.1002/jnr.10405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson JP, McCarley RW, Hobson JA. REM sleep burst neurons, PGO waves, and eye movement information. J Neurophysiol. 1983;50:784–797. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.50.4.784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rye DB. Contributions of the pedunculopontine region to normal and altered REM sleep. Sleep. 1997;20:757–788. doi: 10.1093/sleep/20.9.757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luebke JI, McCarley RW, Greene RW. Inhibitory action of muscarinic agonists on neurons in the rat laterodorsal tegmental nucleus in vitro. J Neurophysiol. 1993;70:2128–2135. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.5.2128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilcox KS, Grant SJ, Burkhart BA, Christoph GR. In vitro electrophysiology of neurons in the lateral dorsal tegmental nucleus. Brain Res Bull. 1989;22:557–560. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(89)90111-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llinas RR. The intrinsic electrophysiological properties of mammalian neurons: Insights into central nervous system function. Science. 1988;242:1654–1664. doi: 10.1126/science.3059497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Donovan MJ. The origin of spontaneous activity in developing networks of the vertebrate nervous system. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999;9:94–104. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)80012-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersson P, Waldenström A, Fåhraeus C, Schouenborg J. Spontaneous muscle twitches during sleep guide spinal self-organization. Nature. 2003;424:72–75. doi: 10.1038/nature01719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hebb DO. The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Science Editions; 1961. 335 pp. [Google Scholar]
- Seelke AMH, Blumberg MS. Thermal and nutritional modulation of sleep in infant rats. Behav Neurosci. 2005 doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.2.603. In press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khazipov R, Sirota A, Leinekugel X, Holmes GL, Ben-Ari Y, et al. Early motor activity drives spindle-bursts in the developing somatosensory cortex. Nature. 2004;432:758–761. doi: 10.1038/nature03132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleitman N, Engelmann TG. Sleep characteristics of infants. J Appl Physiol. 1953;6:269–282. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1953.6.5.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rechtschaffen A. Current perspectives on the function of sleep. Persp Biol Med. 1998;41:359–390. doi: 10.1353/pbm.1998.0051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vertes RP. Memory consolidation in sleep; dream or reality. Neuron. 2004;44:135–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roffwarg HP, Muzio JN, Dement WC. Ontogenetic development of the human sleep-dream cycle. Science. 1966;152:604–619. doi: 10.1126/science.152.3722.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1998. 1 v. [Google Scholar]
- Corner MA, Kwee P. Cyclic EEG and motility patterns during sleep in restrained infant rats. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1976;41:64–72. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(76)90215-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abeles M, Goldstein MH. Multispike train analysis. Proc IEEE. 1977;65:762–773. [Google Scholar]
- Lewicki MS. A review of methods for spike sorting: The detection and classification of neural action potentials. Network Comp Neur Sys. 1998;9:R53–R78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson MA, McNaughton BL. Dynamics of the hippocampal ensemble code for space. Science. 1993;261:1055–1058. doi: 10.1126/science.8351520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Wilson MA, Barnes CA. Theta phase procession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences. Hippocampus. 1996;6:149–172. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:2<149::AID-HIPO6>3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mileykovskiy BY, Kiyashchenko LI, Siegel JM. Cessation of activity in red nucleus neurons during stimulation of the medial medulla in decerebrate rats. J Physiol. 2002;545:997–1006. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.028985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg MS, Stolba MA. Thermogenesis, myoclonic twitching, and ultrasonic vocalization in neonatal rats during moderate and extreme cold exposure. Behav Neurosci. 1996;110:305–314. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.110.2.305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gramsbergen A, Schwartze P, Prechtl HFR. The postnatal development of behavioral states in the rat. Dev Psychobiol. 1970;3:267–280. doi: 10.1002/dev.420030407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumberg MS, Lucas DE. Dual mechanisms of twitching during sleep in neonatal rats. Behav Neurosci. 1994;108:1196–1202. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.6.1196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smotherman WP, Robinson SR. Accessibility of the rat fetus for psychobiological investigation. In: Shair HN, Hofer MA, Barr G, editors. Developmental psychobiology: New methods and changing concepts. New York: Oxford University Press; 1991. pp. 148–164. [Google Scholar]