Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2023 Sep 7;54(1):95–104. doi: 10.1007/s40279-023-01906-0

Quantifying the Extent to Which Junior Performance Predicts Senior Performance in Olympic Sports: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Michael Barth 1,, Arne Güllich 2, Brooke N Macnamara 3, David Z Hambrick 4
PMCID: PMC10799111  PMID: 37676619

Abstract

Background

To what extent does junior athletic performance predict senior athletic performance (i.e., in the highest, open-age category)? This question is the subject of a lively debate in the literature. Following traditional theories of giftedness and expertise, some researchers and practitioners have proposed that a high level of junior performance is a prerequisite for the development of a high level of later senior performance. Sceptics of this view hold that junior performance has limited predictive value for later senior performance, pointing to empirical evidence indicating that predictors (e.g., participation patterns) of junior performance and of senior performance differ. The straightforward way to resolve this controversy empirically is to test the correlation between junior and senior performance.

Objective

To provide robust and generalizable evidence on this issue, we performed a systematic review and meta-analysis of relevant studies. The aim was to quantify the overall correlation between junior and senior performance and then test whether correlations vary across junior age categories and subsamples (e.g., types of sports).

Methods

A systematic literature search was conducted in SPORTDiscus, Eric, ProQuest, PsychInfo, PubMed, Scopus, WorldCat, and Google Scholar from 27 January through 30 April 2022. We searched for original studies that recorded athletes’ junior and senior performance longitudinally and included measures of association between junior and senior performance. Quality of evidence was evaluated using the Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool version for nonrandomized studies.

Results

The search yielded k = 129 effect sizes from N = 13,392 athletes from a wide range of Olympic sports, 62% male and 38% female, from 2006 to 2021. Four central findings emerged: (1) Overall, the meta-analytic pooled correlation between junior and senior performance was r¯ = 0.148. That is, junior performance explained only 2.2% of the reliable variance in senior performance. (2) The finding was robust across types of sports, sexes, wider or narrower performance ranges, national or international samples, and binary or continuous performance measures. (3) Effects varied across junior age categories: the younger the junior age category, the lower the correlation between junior and senior performance, with r¯ ranging from r¯ = − 0.052 to r¯ = 0.215. That is, across junior age categories, junior performance explained 0–4.6% of the reliable variance in senior performance. (4) The quality of primary studies was high.

Discussion

The results suggest that junior performance has very little, if any, predictive value for senior performance. The findings run counter to claims from traditional theories of both giftedness and expertise. From an applied perspective, talent selection typically begins around puberty or younger—age ranges where youth performance is uncorrelated or negatively correlated with later senior performance. The evidence presented here raises serious questions about the use of junior performance for talent selection purposes.

A PRISMA-P protocol was registered at https://osf.io/gck4a/.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s40279-023-01906-0.

Key Points

Junior performance has very little predictive value for later senior performance.
The finding is robust across different populations (e.g., types of sports, sexes, geographical regions).
Effects vary across junior age categories: The younger the junior age category, the lower the correlation between junior and senior performance.

Introduction

“Talent” in sports can be understood as a youth athlete’s potential for the long-term development of a high performance level in future elite sports. The belief in a youth athlete’s talent often leads to expanded investments by the athlete, parents, and talent promotion programs.

Talent identification in sports often begins around puberty or younger [13] and includes the forecasting of the youth athlete’s future performance development. Attempts to forecast human performance development are common in multiple fields, such as school entry tests, admission to music schools and conservatoria, admission to college or university, and job application interviews [46], although long-term prognostic validity is often poor [715]. In sports, forecasts of future performance are the basis of admission to talent promotion programs, including federations’ youth squads and selection teams, youth sport academies, and centers of excellence.

Although more complex talent identification systems have been developed in the recent years [2], most talent selection procedures have one fundamental idea in common: predicting future performance by past or current performance. Here, we investigate the extent to which earlier performance predicts later performance in sports. More specifically, we ask: to what extent does junior athletic performance predict senior athletic performance?

Although junior performance is a predictor commonly used in talent selection, a lively debate exists in the sports science and medicine literature. Proponents of using junior performance to predict senior performance assume a high level of performance in youth is a prerequisite for the development of a high level of performance in adulthood. This view follows from theories of giftedness and expertise (e.g., [4, 1618]) and is in line with claims made by numerous applied researchers and practitioners (e.g., [1926]). It also corresponds with sport policies and practices in terms of establishment of continental and world championships, festivals, and circuits for competitors as young as 11–15 years old [27].

Sceptics of this practice point to several empirical observations. First, predictors of junior performance and senior performance are different and in some cases opposite [28, 29]. For example, highly successful junior athletes reach performance milestones rapidly, whereas highly successful senior athletes progressed more gradually in their early years. Furthermore, early biological maturation (puberty, growth spurt) [30] facilitates junior performance, but not senior performance. Likewise, youth athletes who are born early within their age year have a performance advantage in adolescence (relative age effect, RAE, e.g., [31]), which diminishes or is even reversed by adulthood [3234].

Second, multiple factors develop at different timescales and rates within individuals and between individuals (e.g., coaching, practice, skill acquisition, psychosocial development, or social support). Third, predictors of performance do not necessarily predict persistence: many youth athletes, including highly successful juniors, withdraw from competition sports before adulthood [35]. Finally, at junior-level championships, athletes compete within a single, typically 2-year age group, whereas at senior-level championships, athletes compete with peers from a wider age range, often from late teens to late 30s [27].

In short, the popular view that the best predictor of later performance is earlier performance notwithstanding (e.g., [4]), there is ample reason to question the validity of using junior performance to predict senior performance [28, 29]. Fortunately, there is a straightforward empirical test to resolve this controversy: The analysis of the correlation between earlier and later performance—in our case, the correlation between junior and senior performance.

If junior performance is a strong predictor of senior performance, there should be strong positive correlations between junior and senior performance. If junior performance is not a strong predictor of senior performance, there should be weak or null correlations between junior and senior performance. The former finding would support the use of junior performance for talent selection; the latter finding would indicate this practice may be misguided.

To test these two competing hypotheses, we performed a systematic review and meta-analysis examining the relationship between junior performance and senior performance. Compiling all available relevant empirical evidence, we estimated the overall magnitude of the correlation between junior performance and senior performance, and also tested whether findings vary across junior age categories and different subsamples (e.g., type of sports, sex).1

Methods

The study search and selection procedures were guided by the PRISMA 2020 statement (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses [36], a PRISMA-P [37] protocol was registered at https://osf.io/gck4a/). We searched for original studies that recorded athletes’ junior and senior performance longitudinally and reported a measure of association between junior and senior performance or data needed to compute that association. Figure 1 shows the flowchart of the major steps of the literature search and screening, which was conducted from 27 January through 30 April 2022.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Flow diagram of the literature search and study coding

Sample

The search and selection yielded a total of k = 129 effect sizes from N = 13,392 athletes, 61.9% male and 38.1% female, reported in 21 study reports from 2006 to 2021. There were additional effect sizes because individual studies reported associations between junior and senior performance for different subsamples (e.g., male and female, different sports or disciplines) and for multiple junior age categories (see below).

Table 1 presents characteristics of the total sample. The sample included athletes from a wide range of sports of the Olympic Games. About three-quarters of the athletes (72.1%) were from international samples (athletes from multiple countries, e.g., participants at the Olympic Games, world or continental championships, or athletes listed in international rankings) and about one-quarter (27.9%) were from national samples (athletes from one country, respectively), all from Western European countries. The characteristics and corresponding measures of association between junior and senior performance of the relevant studies are provided in the electronic supplementary material (ESM) 1.

Table 1.

Sample characteristics and subsample sizes (number of athletes)

Subsample N
Year of study report
 Until 2009 958
 2010–2014 5943
 2015–2021 6491
Sex
 Male 7590
 Female 4676
 Male and female 1126
Types of sport by the task in competitiona
 Cgs sports (e.g., athletics, rowing, swimming, ski racing) 5803
 Game sports (e.g., basketball, soccer, tennis) 6310
 Combat sports (e.g., fencing, judo, taekwondo) 158
 Artistic composition sports (e.g., artistic gymnastic, figure skating) 35
 Other sports (e.g., modern pentathlon, ski jumping)/multiple sports 1086
Individual and team sportsb
 Individual sports (e.g., athletics, judo, race cycling, swimming, tennis) 9623
 Team sports (e.g., basketball, hockey, rugby, soccer, volleyball) 2593
 Individual sports and team sports 1176
Regionc
 International samples (e.g., participants at world or continental championships) 9650
 National samples (from just one country, respectively) 3742
Junior age categoryd
 Junior A 5576
 Junior B 2761
 Junior C 4312
 Junior D 743

aTypes of sports were categorized on the basis of the task in competition [38]: Cgs (performance is measured in centimeters, grams, or seconds), game, combat, artistic composition, and others/multiple sports (sports meeting none or multiple criteria of the aforementioned sports types or samples involving different types of sports)

bIndividual sports (individual athletes compete with one another), team sports (teams compete with one another)

cInternational samples (athletes from multiple countries), national samples (athletes from just one country, respectively)

dJunior A, Junior B, Junior C, Junior D junior age categories, with Junior A being the oldest and Junior D the youngest age category within each sport

The primary studies either reported athletes’ ages as the sample mean and standard deviation or the minimum and maximum ages. Across studies, the sample-weighted mean age as a senior was 25.7 years, the sample-weighted mean minimum age as a senior was 20.2 years, and the sample-weighted mean maximum age was 36.3 years. Athlete performance data were collected from public records (official championship results or ranking lists) for 11,742 athletes and by athlete questionnaires or interviews for 1650 athletes.

Data Extraction, Performance Levels, and Junior Age Categories

Each study was coded for (1) author(s) and year, (2) publication status, (3) method of data collection, (4) sample characteristics (country, sport, sex, age, junior age categories involved, range of competition levels, number of athletes), and (5) the correlation between junior performance and senior performance.

Performance differences between athletes were determined in the primary studies by either dichotomous classification (k = 39) or continuous performance measures (k = 90). Dichotomous performance measures recorded whether an athlete did or did not achieve a defined competition level, respectively at junior and senior age, such as (1) playing in the highest national league; (2) nomination for a national selection team or talent promotion program; or (3) participating or achieving a top-ten placing at major international championships. For these dichotomous performance variables, the corresponding measure of association between junior and senior performance is the φ coefficient.

Continuous performance measures, both at junior and senior competitions, included (1) athletes’ championship level (world, continental, national, regional level) and placing at that championship; (2) placings in official international rankings (e.g., Association of Tennis Professionals ranking list); (3) an athlete’s number of international junior championships, games or minutes played at an international championship; (4) swimmers’ peak swim time; or (5) basketball players’ composite score from official FIBA game statistics (including minutes played, scored points, assists, rebounds, steals, and blocks). For these continuous performance variables, the corresponding measure of association between junior and senior performance is the correlation coefficient r.

Thus, the two approaches allowed us to investigate (1) the extent to which being within a defined junior performance range (e.g., being among junior international 1st–10th) predicts achieving a defined senior performance range (e.g., being among senior international 1st–10th), and (2) the extent to which individual differences in athlete’s competition level and exact rank at junior age (e.g., national 1st, 2nd, 3rd, …, 10th, international 1st, 2nd, 3rd, …, 10th) predict their individual differences in competition level and rank at senior age (national 1st, 2nd, 3rd, …, 10th, international 1st, 2nd, 3rd, …, 10th). This also enabled us to compare whether the approaches differ in predictive power.

Furthermore, we considered the range of the junior and senior performance levels examined in each study because wider performance ranges (e.g., regional level to international level) may yield larger associations, whereas narrower performance ranges (e.g., only national to international level) may yield smaller associations (restriction of range, see [39]). In our analysis, all primary studies involved senior international and national-level athletes and k = 32 also involved lower, regional-level athletes. We thus distinguished between “narrow range” samples (national to international championship level, k = 97) and “wide range” samples (regional to international championship level, k = 32) (see also [28]).

Junior age categories are defined by the regulations of the international sport federation for each sport. However, the labels for the different junior age groups differ by sport and country. For this report, we uniformly use the following labels: Junior A, Junior B, Junior C, and Junior D, where Junior A is the oldest junior age category (in most sports 17–18 or 18–19 years), Junior B is two years younger than Junior A, Junior C is 2 years younger than Junior B, and Junior D is the youngest age category within each sport (in most sports 11–12 or 12–13 years). The sample included k = 62 effect sizes for the association between Junior A performance and senior performance, k = 45 effect sizes for the association between Junior B performance and senior performance, k = 13 effect sizes for Junior C performance and senior performance, and k = 9 effect sizes for Junior D performance and senior performance.

Meta-analytic Approach

All analyses were performed using the publicly available R environment, version 4.1.3. Effect sizes are expressed as r. Correlations reported as φ were converted to r. To synthesize effect sizes across studies, all r were converted to Fisher’s z (using the function “transf.rtoz,” package metafor 3.4.0). After estimating the meta-analytic models, we reconverted the meta-analytic mean Fisher’s z¯ metrics to pooled correlation coefficients r¯ (function “transf.ztor,” metafor 3.4.0 package).

We searched for outliers, defined as a Fisher’s z whose residuals had z scores > 3 [28]. No more than two outliers were identified within any subsample. Outlying values were excluded from subsequent analyses.

Independence of effect sizes is a crucial assumption of conventional meta-analytic procedures. We assumed that athletes involved in different studies, male and female athletes, and athletes from different sports were from independent samples. However, in several studies, athletes’ performance data were collected from various junior age categories within sexes and sports, where the extent of sample independence or dependence was not always clear from the primary reports. Becker [40] described four central approaches for dealing with partially dependent samples. The four approaches are: (1) treating data as independent, (2) combining across different outcomes, (3) creating independent data sets, and (4) modeling dependence.

Given the nature of the data in our synthesis, we decided to apply the following approaches to estimate the overall pooled correlation and compare the respective models: (1) assuming independence of all samples; (2) combining across different effect sizes; for this approach, Cheung and Chan [41] described a sample-wise adjustment by individual effect size (see Cheung and Chan’s R script “MADependentESFunctions.r” in [41]); (3) modeling dependence by application of three-level modeling using cluster-robust variance estimation (RVE, metaphor 3.4.0 package, functions “rma.mv” and “robust” [42]), and with using a structural equation modeling approach (metaSem 1.2.5.1 package, function “meta3”).

Pooled effects were similar across the different models, 0.145 ≤ r¯ ≤ 0.151 (model differences 0.597 ≤ p ≤ 0.955). Following Becker’s recommendations, the most complete and accurate portrayal of the effects of dependence requires the modeling of this dependency. Therefore, we used three-level RVE modeling for subsequent moderator analyses. We estimated the overall effect of junior performance on senior performance by conducting a random effects meta-analysis (k = 129) and then employed mixed-effect models to analyze whether defined subsample characteristics moderated effects. All subsamples are provided separately in the ESM 1.

Moderator Analyses

We investigated whether subsamples differed in effect sizes using mixed-effects models with Wald’s F [43]. For all moderator analyses, we used the rule of thumb that k ≥ 5 is required for each subgroup [44].

We tested for effects of six moderators:

  1. Type of performance measure: dichotomous versus continuous.

  2. Range of performance levels: narrow versus wide.

  3. Gender: male versus female.

  4. Junior age category: Junior A, Junior B, Junior C, and Junior D.

  5. Type of sports: cgs, game, and combat sports; and individual versus team sports. Unlike in individual sports, in team sports a relatively low-performing individual athlete may be successful by playing on a high-performing team, whereas a high-performing player may be unsuccessful by playing on a low-performing team.

  6. Region: national samples, i.e., athletes from just one nation (e.g., participants at a national championship), versus international samples, i.e. athletes from multiple nations (e.g., participants at Olympic Games, world, and continental championships). The latter may show a higher and more homogeneous performance level than the former.

All moderator analyses (except for junior age category) were performed controlling for junior age category. All hypothesis testing was two-tailed, and p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Following Cohen [45], we considered r ≈ 0.10 a “small” effect, r ≈ 0.30 a “medium” effect, and r ≈ 0.50 a “large” effect.

Quality Assessment and Risk of Bias

We assessed the quality of the primary studies by the Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool (MMAT) version for nonrandomized studies [46]. Given that we analyzed empirical quantitative studies only, the two preliminary screening questions of the MMAT pertaining to qualitative studies were not relevant here. Furthermore, the fifth MMAT item—whether an intervention was administered as intended—was not applicable, as the primary studies did not involve interventions. Thus, we assessed the quality of the primary studies by the following four MMAT criteria (see ESM 2): representativeness, appropriateness of measurements, completeness of outcome data, and consideration of confounders. All studies were independently assessed by the first and the second author; inter-rater reliability was excellent (Cohen’s κ = 95%). In addition, we examined whether studies collecting athletes’ performance by document analyses (of data from official public records) or by athlete inquiry (questionnaires, interviews) differed in effect size.

To investigate potential publication bias, we tested whether publication status (published versus unpublished) was a significant moderator and then inspected the funnel plot and computed Egger’s regression analysis.

Results

Across all effect sizes, the mean correlation between junior performance and senior performance was r¯ = 0.148 (r¯2 = 0.022, Table 2).

Table 2.

Effects of junior performance on senior performance

r¯ 95% CI p k Comparison F p
Overall 0.148 0.114, 0.182  < 0.001 129
Junior age category
 Junior A 0.215 0.174, 0.257  < 0.001 62
 Junior B 0.128 0.100, 0.156  < 0.001 45 Versus Junior A 12.216  < 0.001
 Junior C  − 0.024  − 0.106, 0.057 0.515 13 Versus Junior A 34.156  < 0.001
Versus Junior B 15.940  < 0.001
 Junior D  − 0.052  − 0.101, − 0.002 0.045 9 Versus Junior A 62.586  < 0.001
Versus Junior B 34.895  < 0.001
Versus Junior C 0.042 0.839
Performance measure
 Dichotomous 0.140 0.078, 0.201  < 0.001 39
 Continuous 0.151 0.110, 0.192  < 0.001 90 Versus dichotomous 0.026 0.873
Performance bandwidth
 Narrow 0.178 0.146, 0.211  < 0.001 97
 Wide 0.081 0.014, 0.148 0.022 32 Versus narrow 1.119 0.292
Region
 International samples 0.183 0.150, 0.214  < 0.001 74
 National samples 0.103 0.037, 0.167 0.004 55 Versus international 0.844 0.360
Sex
 Female 0.167 0.108, 0.225  < 0.001 54
 Male 0.150 0.105, 0.194  < 0.001 67 Versus female 0.432 0.512
Types of sports
 Cgs sports 0.172 0.134, 0.210  < 0.001 88
 Game sports 0.110 0.035, 0.185 0.007 35 Versus cgs 0.175 0.676
 Combat sports 0.176  − 0.365, 0.629 0.298 7 Versus cgs 0.047 0.828
Versus game 0.125 0.724
 Individual sports 0.166 0.129, 0.202  < 0.001 101
 Team sports 0.108 0.008, 0.206 0.036 29 Versus individual 0.661 0.418

r¯ pooled correlation coefficient, 95% CI confidence interval lower and upper limit, k number of effect sizes; F and p for moderator analyses controlling for junior age category (except among junior age categories themselves); Junior A, Junior B, Junior C, Junior D junior age categories, with Junior A being the oldest junior age category within each sport and Junior D the youngest age category; international samples: athletes from multiple countries, national samples: athletes from just one country, respectively; cgs performance is measured in centimeters, grams, or seconds (e.g., athletics, rowing, swimming, ski racing), game e.g., basketball, soccer, tennis, combat e.g., fencing, judo; narrow national to international competition level, wide regional to international competition level. The forest plots and I2 statistics are reported in the ESM 2, Figs. S2.1–S2.4

The effects did not significantly differ between (1) dichotomous and continuous measures of performance; (2) narrower and wider performance ranges; (3) sexes (male, female); (4) types of sports (cgs, game, and combat sports; individual and team sports); or (5) national and international samples (Table 2). The effects did differ across junior age categories: the younger the junior age category, the lower the association between junior performance and senior performance. This pattern held except for the youngest categories, Junior C and Junior D, which did not significantly differ from one another. Although effects varied across junior age categories, they were all small: junior performance explained 0–4.6% of the variance in senior performance (Table 2).

All primary studies had a high methodological quality, and risk of bias was generally low. See Table S2.1 in the ESM 2. Effects significantly differed between studies collecting performance data from public records and studies using athlete questionnaires/interviews for data collection (r¯ = 0.177, 95% CI 0.145 to 0.209, versus r¯ = 0.020, 95% CI − 0.037 to 0.077, F = 5.236, p = 0.024).

Of the 21 study reports, 14 were published (k = 106) and 7 unpublished (k = 23). Effect sizes did not significantly differ between published and unpublished studies (F = 2.367, p = 0.126). The funnel plot (see ESM 2, Fig. S2.5) was nearly symmetrical and Egger’s regression was nonsignificant (b = 9.408, 95% CI − 1.336 to 20.152, p = 0.086).

Discussion

The question of whether junior performance is a valid predictor of senior performance is the subject of ongoing debate in the sports science and medicine literature. To resolve this controversy, we quantified the extent to which junior performance predicts senior performance. Specifically, we investigated the overall magnitude of the correlation between junior performance and senior performance, while testing for effects of potential moderators.

Across all effect sizes, the pooled correlation between junior and senior performance was r¯ = 0.148. That is, overall, junior performance explained only 2.2% of the reliable variance in senior performance, whereas 97.8% of the variance of senior performance was explained by factors other than junior performance, along with random measurement error. The finding was robust across sexes, types of sports, dichotomous and continuous performance measures, narrower as well as wider performance ranges, and across national and international samples. Results varied across junior age categories: The younger the junior age category, the lower the association between junior performance and senior performance. However, effects were all weak. Across junior age categories, junior performance explained 0–4.6% of the variance of senior performance, meaning that 95.4–100% of the variance was explained by other factors and random measurement error.

Our finding that junior performance has little, if any, predictive validity regarding senior performance is in line with findings indicating that (1) predictors of early junior performance and of later senior performance differ and are partly opposite and (2) that successful juniors and successful seniors are largely two disparate populations [2729]. This finding—similar to findings from other domains ([715], see Introduction)—has two critical theoretical implications. First, it runs counter to claims made by traditional theories of giftedness and expertise [4, 17, 18], which posit that a high level of junior performance is a prerequisite for a high level of senior performance. Second, given the weak correlation between junior performance and senior performance, the results indicate quite clearly that predictors of performance from studies among junior athletes cannot justifiably be extrapolated to senior performance. To put it another way, drawing conclusions about senior performance from research on junior performance is empirically unfounded; it may also be misleading.

From an applied perspective, talent identification and selection typically begin around puberty or younger [1, 3]—age ranges where youth performance is uncorrelated or even negatively correlated with later senior performance. Our results have major implications for policies and practices of youth sports in general and talent promotion programs in particular, in three regards. First, enhancing a youth athlete’s junior performance does not reliably lead to increased senior performance. Second, talent identification and talent promotion programs that preferentially select the highest-performing youth athletes cannot be confident they are selecting the highest future senior performers. Rather, they are selecting many individuals who will not turn out to be high performers at the senior level, while rejecting many individuals who would have become high performers at the senior level. Third, and relatedly, youth athletes’ current performance is not a sensitive criterion for the evaluation of talent promotion programs or of youth sport programs and youth coaches in general. The weak correlation between junior and senior performance signifies that many of the highest-performing senior athletes had low performance levels relative to many of their peers when they were juniors. That is, many of the highest-performing seniors had greater performance improvement from junior to senior age and, thus, must have developed a greater potential during junior age for long-term development through subsequent years. Therefore, a more adequate evaluation criterion of youth talent promotion is the future performance progress the youth athletes make during subsequent years into adulthood [2729].

This study has several strengths, including a large international sample from a wide range of Olympic sports and both sexes; consideration of different junior age categories; multiple moderator analyses that showed robust findings across many different samples; and a high methodological quality of primary studies. However, several limitations should be acknowledged. First, the weak correlations found are to be interpreted as associations, not causal links. Second, male samples and national samples from Western European countries were over-represented. Third, primary studies involved athletes from regional to international competition levels, but no athletes below these levels. It may be that associations between junior and senior performance vary among lower-performing populations or populations extending over wider performance ranges. Fourth, successful juniors who withdrew from competitive sports before reaching senior age, as well as successful seniors who did not begin competing within junior age categories, may not have been considered in primary studies. Finally, although we used multiple databases, bias related to availability of study reports, location of study (i.e., country), and language of study report is possible, as in any systematic review.

Future research should seek to extend investigations to populations that have previously been under-represented, including athletes from countries other than Western Europe, Paralympic sports, and female athletes. Furthermore, future researchers should investigate whether multivariate linear and nonlinear interactions between youth performance and factors associated with athletes’ childhood/adolescent participation patterns, coaching, practice characteristics, relative age, timing of biological maturation, health, basic physical abilities and perceptual-motor skills, social support, and psychological characteristics provide better predictive value than youth performance alone. On a final note, rather than extrapolating from junior samples, theories concerning predictors of the highest senior performance levels should be based on empirical findings from the highest-performing senior athletes themselves. This research will improve the quality of the available empirical evidence, foster the advancement of even better theories of the acquisition of athletic expertise, and thereby provide the empirical substantiation needed for evidence-based practical recommendations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Declarations

Funding

Open access funding provided by University of Innsbruck and Medical University of Innsbruck. The project was supported by the Tyrolean Science Fund (TWF; Number WFF. 33326/6–2021).

Conflict of interest/competing interests

The authors declare no conflicting or competing interests with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.

Data availability

All original data are freely available in the electronic supplementary material 1.

Code availability

Code for data cleaning and analysis is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Authors’ contributions

Conception of the study design: all authors; literature search and selection: AG; study coding and preparation of the data set: AG; data analysis: MB; writing, reviewing, editing, and final approval of the manuscript: all authors.

Ethics approval

As a systematic review and synthesis of primary findings, we did not directly interact with human subjects for this study.

Consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Footnotes

1

A recent systematic review [27] synthesized studies that investigated at a collective level the number of athletes (i.e., proportions of samples) who did or did not achieve an equivalent competition level within both junior and senior age categories. Based on a separate data set, the present meta-analysis extends this previous investigation by investigating the correlation between junior and senior performance, i.e., quantifying at the individual athlete level the extent to which individual differences in junior performance predict individual differences in senior performance.

References

  • 1.Ford PR, Bordonau JLD, Bonanno D, Tavares J, Groenendijk C, Fink C, et al. A survey of talent identification and development processes in the youth academies of professional soccer clubs from around the world. J Sports Sci. 2020;38:1269–1278. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2020.1752440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Johnston K, Wattie N, Schorer J, Baker J. Talent identification in sport: a systematic review. Sports Med. 2018;48:97–109. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0803-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Breitbach S, Tug S, Simon P. Conventional and genetic talent identification in sports: will recent developments trace talent? Sports Med. 2014;44:1489–1503. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0221-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ericsson KA, Krampe RT, Tesch-Römer C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol Rev. 1993;100:363–406. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.100.3.363. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kuncel NR, Hezlett SA. Assessment. Standardized tests predict graduate students’ success. Science. 2007;315:1080–1081. doi: 10.1126/science.1136618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sackett PR, Lievens F. Personnel selection. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:419–450. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Feldon DF, Litson K, Cahoon B, Feng Z, Walker A, Tofel-Grehl C. The predictive validity of the GRE across graduate outcomes: a meta-analysis of trends over time. J Higher Educ. 2023 doi: 10.1080/00221546.2023.2187177. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wolf A, Kopiez R. Do grades reflect the development of excellence music students? The prognostic validity of entrance exams at universities of music. Music Sci. 2014;18:232–248. doi: 10.1177/1029864914530394. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fischer FT, Schult J, Hell B. Sex-specific differential prediction of college admission tests: a meta-analysis. J Educ Psychol. 2013;105:478–488. doi: 10.1037/a0031956. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ogg T, Zimdars A, Heath A. Schooling effects on degree performance: a comparison of the predictive validity of aptitude testing and secondary school grades at Oxford University. Br Educ Res J. 2009;35:781–807. doi: 10.1080/01411920903165611. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ruthsatz J, Detterman D, Griscom WS, Cirullo BA. Becoming an expert in the musical domain: It takes more than just practice. Intelligence. 2008;36:330–338. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2007.08.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lerdau M, Avery C. The utility of standardized tests. Science. 2007;316:1694–1697. doi: 10.1126/science.316.5832.1694b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Goho J, Blackman A. The effectiveness of academic admission interviews: an exploratory meta-analysis. Med Teach. 2006;28:335–340. doi: 10.1080/01421590600603418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Roth PL, Clarke RL. Meta-analyzing the relation between grades and salary. J Vocat Behav. 1998;53:386–400. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.1997.1621. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sommer W. Probation of teacher judgements. An empirical test on the validity of teachers’ assessment of students on their educational and occupational success [in German: Bewährung des Lehrerurteils. Ein Empirische Untersuchung über den Aussagewert des Lehrerurteils für den Bildungs- und Berufserfolg]. Bad Heilbrunn: Klinkhardt; 1983.
  • 16.Ericsson KA. Training history, deliberate practice and elite sports performance: an analysis in response to Tucker and Collins review—what makes champions? Br J Sports Med. 2013;47:533–535. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gagné F. Differentiating giftedness from talent: the DMGT perspective on talent development. New York: Routledge; 2021. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Heller KA, Perleth C, Lim TK. The Munich model of giftedness designed to identify and promote gifted students. In: Sternberg RJ, Davidson JE, editors. Conceptions of giftedness. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. pp. 147–170. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Fuchslocher J, Romann M, Gulbin J. Strategies to support developing talent. Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Sportmedizin und Sporttraumatologie. 2013;61:10–14. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Gulbin J, Weissensteiner J, Oldenziel K, Gagné F. Patterns of performance development in elite athletes. Eur J Sport Sci. 2013;13:605–614. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2012.756542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hohmann A, Seidel I. Scientific aspects of talent development. Int J Phys Educ. 2003;40:9–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hollings SC. World junior success is a prerequisite for world senior success. Mod Athlete Coach. 2006;44:14–17. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hollings SC, Hume P. Progression of elite New Zealand and Australian junior athletes to senior representation. New Stud Athl. 2011;26:127–135. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Li P, de Bosscher V, Pion J, Weissensteiner JR, Vertonghen J. Is international junior success a reliable predictor for international senior success in elite combat sports? Eur J Sport Sci. 2018;18:550–559. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2018.1439104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Li P, de Bosscher V, Weissensteiner JR. The journey to elite success: a thirty-year longitudinal study of the career trajectories of top professional tennis players. Int J Perform Anal Sport. 2018;18:961–972. doi: 10.1080/24748668.2018.1534197. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Reid M, Crespo M, Santilli L, Miley D, Dimmock J. The importance of the International Tennis Federation’s junior boys’ circuit in the development of professional tennis players. J Sports Sci. 2007;25:667–672. doi: 10.1080/02640410600811932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Güllich A, Barth M, Macnamara BN, Hambrick DZ. Quantifying the extent to which successful juniors and successful seniors are two disparate populations: a systematic review and synthesis of findings. Sports Med. 2023;53:1201–1217. doi: 10.1007/s40279-023-01840-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Barth M, Güllich A, Macnamara BN, Hambrick DZ. Predictors of junior versus senior elite performance are opposite: a systematic review and meta-analysis of participation patterns. Sports Med. 2022 doi: 10.1007/s40279-021-01625-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Güllich A, Macnamara BN, Hambrick DZ. What makes a champion? Early multidisciplinary practice, not early specialization, predicts world-class performance. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2022;17:6–29. doi: 10.1177/1745691620974772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Malina RM, Rogol AD, Cumming SP, Coelho e Silva MJ, Figueiredo AJ. Biological maturation of youth athletes: assessment and implications. Br J Sports Med. 2015;49:852–859. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-094623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wattie N, Schorer J, Baker J. The relative age effect in sport: a developmental systems model. Sports Med. 2015;45:83–94. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0248-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ramos-Filho L, Ferreira MP. The reverse relative age effect in professional soccer: an analysis of the Brazilian National League of 2015. Eur Sport Manag Q. 2021;21:78–93. doi: 10.1080/16184742.2020.1725089. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wrang CM, Rossing NN, Diernæs RM, Hansen CG, Dalgaard-Hansen C, Karbing DS. Relative age effect and the re-selection of Danish male handball players for national teams. J Hum Kinet. 2018;63:33–41. doi: 10.2478/hukin-2018-0004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fumarco L, Gibbs BG, Jarvis JA, Rossi G. The relative age effect reversal among the National Hockey League elite. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0182827. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0182827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Crane J, Temple V. A systematic review of dropout from organized sport among children and youth. Eur Phy Educ Rev. 2015;21:114–131. doi: 10.1177/1356336X14555294. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. PLoS ONE. 2021;18:e1003583. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003583. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Shamseer L, Moher D, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ. 2015;350:g7647. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g7647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Güllich A, Emrich E. Considering long-term sustainability in the development of world class success. Eur J Sport Sci. 2014;14(Suppl 1):S383–S397. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2012.706320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ackerman PL. Nonsense, common sense, and science of expert performance: talent and individual differences. Intelligence. 2014;45:6–17. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.04.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Becker BJ. Multivariate meta-analysis. In: Tinsley HEA, Brown SD, editors. Handbook of applied multivariate statistics and mathematical modeling. San Diego: Academic Press; 2000. pp. 499–525. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Cheung SF, Chan DK-S. Meta-analyzing dependent correlations: an SPSS macro and an R script. Behav Res Methods. 2014;46:331–345. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0386-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Tanner-Smith EE, Tipton E, Polanin JR. Handling complex meta-analytic data structures using robust variance estimates: a tutorial in R. J Dev Life Course Criminol. 2016;2:85–112. doi: 10.1007/s40865-016-0026-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Fox J, Weisberg S. An R companion to applied regression, 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 2019.
  • 44.Williams R. Moderator analyses: categorical models and meta-regression: paper presented at the annual Campbell Collaboration Colloquium. Copenhagen; 29 May 2012.
  • 45.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2. New York: Routledge; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hong QN, Pluye P, Fàbregues S, Bartlett G, Boardman F, Cargo M, et al. Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool (MMAT): version 2018: user Guide; 2018.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

All original data are freely available in the electronic supplementary material 1.


Articles from Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.z.) are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES