Abstract
Background
The literature on the association between subjective effects (SEs; i.e., how an individual perceives their physiological and psychological reactions to a drug) and substance use disorders (SUDs) is largely limited to community samples. The present study addressed the following aims in a clinical sample: whether SEs predict general versus substance-specific SUD in adolescence and adulthood after controlling for conduct disorder symptoms (CDsymp); whether SEs predict SUDs across drug classes; whether SEs predict change in SUD from adolescence to adulthood; and whether there are racial/ethnic differences in associations.
Methods
Longitudinal analyses were conducted using data from a sample of 744 clinical probands recruited from residential and outpatient SUD treatment facilities in CO during adolescence (Mage = 16.26) and re-assessed twice in adulthood (Mages = 22.56 and 28.96), approximately seven and twelve years after first assessment. SEs and CDsymp were assessed in adolescence. SUD severity was assessed at adolescence and twice during adulthood.
Results
SEs assessed in adolescence robustly predicted general SUD for legal and illegal substances in adolescence and adulthood, whereas CDsymp predicted SUD primarily in adolescence. Higher positive and negative SEs in adolescence were associated with greater SUD severity after controlling for CDsymp, with similar magnitudes. Results indicated cross-substance effects of SEs on SUD. We found no evidence for racial/ethnic differences in associations.
Conclusions
We investigated the progression of SUD in a high-risk sample with greater odds of sustained SUD. In contrast to CDsymp, both positive and negative SEs consistently predicted general SUD across substances in adolescence and adulthood.
Keywords: subjective effects, substance use disorder, longitudinal, clinical sample, polysubstance use, adults, adolescents, conduct disorder
1. INTRODUCTION
Substance use disorders (SUDs) are a pressing public health issue. In 2019, 20.4 million individuals aged 12 and older had a SUD diagnosis (SAMHSA, 2020). Typically, in community samples, substance use and polysubstance experimentation peak in emerging adulthood (Moss et al., 2014; Stone et al., 2012) and decline in adulthood (Johnston et al., 2009; Merrin & Leadbeater, 2018). However, trajectories are heterogenous, especially in clinical samples: SUDs may resolve, decrease in severity, or persist into adulthood (Brown et al., 2011; Trim et al., 2015). Adolescents with SUDs are highly likely to relapse shortly after receiving treatment (Brown et al., 2011). Also, adolescent SUD severity is associated with greater psychological distress and impairment (Ciraulo et al., 2003; Mirza & Mirza, 2008), later crime involvement (Crowley et al., 1998), higher later SUD severity (Ciraulo et al., 2003; Crowley et al., 1998), and early mortality (Clark et al., 2008; Cornelius et al., 2008). Understanding predictors of adolescent SUD and atypical trajectories strengthens our ability to predict and prevent SUDs and their associated consequences.
There are well-established predictors of general SUDs across development, including conduct disorder (CD; Palmer et al., 2013) and novelty seeking (Pardini et al., 2007). For example, CD explains co-occurring SUDs (Button et al., 2007; Iacono et al., 2008) and shares etiological influences with general SUDs (Grant et al., 2015; Krueger et al., 2009). Conversely, we know less about predictors of substance-specific SUDs, although general and substance-specific genetic and environmental influences contribute to SUDs (Palmer et al., 2013). The present study examines subjective effects (SEs; individual differences in physiological and psychological drug experiences) in adolescence as predictors of SUDs. We examined data from a clinical sample in treatment for SUDs as adolescents, then followed in two waves during adulthood. We addressed whether SEs explain general versus specific SUD from adolescence to adulthood after controlling for CD symptoms (CDsymp), a well-established risk factor for general SUDs (Button et al., 2007; Erskine et al., 2016; Palmer et al., 2013).1
1.1. Associations Between SEs and SUDs
Studies have found evidence for two moderately correlated SE factors (Ríos-Bedoya et al., 2009; Zeiger et al., 2010, 2012): positive (e.g., euphoria, buzz, or relaxation) and negative (e.g., paranoia, sadness, or nausea; 9,14–18). Positive SEs at early use are consistently, positively associated with cannabis (Fergusson et al., 2003; Le Strat et al., 2009; Zeiger et al., 2010), alcohol (King et al., 2014; Morean & Corbin, 2008), tobacco (de Wit & Phillips, 2012; Hu et al., 2008; Pomerleau et al., 1998; Ríos-Bedoya et al., 2009), and opioid (Bieber et al., 2008; Haertzen et al., 1983) dependence. Research on other illegal substances is more limited and mixed (J. D. Grant et al., 2005; Karsinti et al., 2018; Lambert et al., 2006), potentially because research focuses on community samples with low rates of SUDs.
In contrast to results on positive SEs, associations between negative SEs and SUDs are less consistent and remain understudied. Researchers have found mixed results regarding negative SEs predicting cannabis (Fergusson et al., 2003; Le Strat et al., 2009; Zeiger et al., 2012), alcohol (King et al., 2014; Ríos-Bedoya et al., 2009; Zeiger et al., 2012), and tobacco SUDs (DiFranza et al., 2004; Pomerleau et al., 1998; Ríos-Bedoya et al., 2009) in community samples. One study found positive associations between cannabis SEs and cannabis SUD in a combined community and clinical sample (Zeiger et al., 2010). Literature on negative SEs and illegal substances is very limited (Karsinti et al., 2018).
1.2. Limitations of the Present Literature
To examine predictors for less commonly used substances (e.g., stimulants and opioids), additional studies including clinical samples are needed. Also, existing studies have mostly focused on associations between SEs and SUDs for one substance (typically alcohol, cannabis, or tobacco; e.g., Karsinti et al., 2018; King et al., 2014; Zeiger et al., 2010). Although existing results are inconsistent (Pomerleau et al., 2004; Zeiger et al., 2012), past research has identified significant cross-substance effects for alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis (Zeiger et al., 2012).
Finally, the literature on associations between SEs and SUDs is largely retrospective, with adults reporting on initial SEs many years after first use (de Wit & Phillips, 2012). Longitudinal studies beginning in adolescence are limited to nicotine (Audrain-McGovern et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2003; DiFranza et al., 2007; Kandel et al., 2007) and cannabis (Fergusson et al., 2003; Le Strat et al., 2009) and showed that positive SEs predict corresponding SUD into early adulthood. Prospective studies on alcohol are typically limited to participants over age 21, and suggest that positive SEs predict alcohol SUD in later adulthood (King et al., 2014). Additional longitudinal studies continuing past early adulthood are needed.
1.3. Present Study
We evaluated positive and negative SEs as predictors of SUD severity (referred to as “SUD”) in a racially and ethnically diverse clinical sample assessed in three waves from adolescence through adulthood by the Center for Antisocial Drug Dependence. We addressed whether SEs predict general or substance-specific SUD for alcohol, cannabis, tobacco, hallucinogens, cocaine, amphetamines, and opioids. All analyses were preregistered, unless otherwise noted: https://osf.io/mc7w8/?view_only=6daabac7e797496db01743426e5683d0.
1.4. Research Questions
1.4.1. Do SEs and CD symptoms predict general or specific SUD?
We hypothesized that positive SEs would predict specific rather than general SUD after controlling for CDsymp and that CDsymp would predict general SUD. Analyses on negative SEs were exploratory, given limited literature on this topic.
1.4.2. Do SEs predict SUD across substances?
Limited literature indicates that SEs may influence SUDs across substances (Pomerleau et al., 2004; Zeiger et al., 2012). Exploratory analyses addressed cross-substance effects of SEs on SUDs.
1.4.3. Do SEs predict change in SUD over time?
Analyses examining whether SEs predict change in SUD from adolescence through adulthood were exploratory, as research on this area is limited.
1.4.4. Associations between predictors and SUD different across race/ethnic groups or sex?
Minoritized racial and ethnic groups experience differential substance use trajectories (Chen & Jacobson, 2012) and experience unique stressors (e.g., bicultural stress via discrimination) predicting SUDs (Romero et al., 2007). There are ethnic differences in predictors of SUD across time (e.g., social conformity as a more robust predictor for Hispanic individuals; Galaif & Newcomb, 1999). No prior studies have examined racial/ethnic differences in associations between SEs and SUD, and our analyses were exploratory. We could not examine whether sex moderates associations between predictors and SUD as intended, given our sample was only 14% women. Sex was a covariate in all models.
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants
The present study included 744 clinical probands participating in a longitudinal study of genetic and developmental influences on adolescent substance use (Hartman et al., 2006; Hopfer et al., 2003). Beginning in 2003, adolescent participants (Wave 1: N = 744, Mage = 16.26, range = 12.95–19.71, SD = 1.15) were recruited from residential and outpatient SUD treatment facilities in Denver, CO. Each participant was contacted at least twice annually with opportunities for updating contact information. Individuals were assessed twice in adulthood (Wave 2: N = 418, Mage = 22.56, SD = 1.83; Wave 3: N = 352, Mage = 28.96, SD = 1.96), approximately seven (M = 7.57, SD = 2.24) and twelve (M = 12.22, SD = 1.89) years after initial assessment. Nearly 53% of individuals were lost to follow-up by Wave 3, but neither greater Wave 1 CDsymp nor SUD severity predicted attrition (Table S1). By Wave 3, approximately two percent of attrition was due to direct refusal and approximately five percent was due to participant death. As this is a high-risk sample, housing instability also likely contributed to attrition. For study inclusion, interview staff had to judge probands as not currently suicidal, homicidal, psychotic, or severely developmentally delayed, with no current intoxication or physical illness preventing participation in evaluation and treatment. The university’s institutional review board approved all procedures. Parents or guardians provided written consent and participants provided assent or consent. The sample was 86% male and 50% non-Hispanic White (Table S2).2
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. SEs
SEs were measured using the modified Lyons Battery for Subjective Effects (Lyons et al., 1997) at Wave 1. At initial assessment, participants reported SEs retrospectively for alcohol, cannabis, hallucinogens, cocaine, amphetamines, and opioids3 if participants reported using the substance more than five times (a standard CIDI-SAM skip threshold). Tobacco SEs were assessed in participants endorsing use of pipes, cigars, and snuff five or more times or tobacco almost every day for a month. Participants were asked, “In the period shortly after you used [substance], did it make you feel [subjective effect]?” (yes/no); questions did not specifically address SEs at initial or first use. Prior confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) and Mokken Scale analyses (Haberstick et al., 2010; Zeiger et al., 2010) indicated evidence for two scales with six items each: positive effects (relaxed, creative, energetic, on top of the world, increased sex drive, sociable) and negative effects (dizzy, nauseous, lazy, out of control, drowsy, unable to concentrate). Responses were averaged to create composite scores for each scale and transformed or collapsed into ordinal categories to address skewness and kurtosis (Derks et al., 2004; see Tables S3 and S4 for frequencies and descriptive statistics).4 CFA results were similar for positive and negative SEs; they indicated that SEs for alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis loaded onto one factor and SEs for hallucinogens, amphetamines, cocaine, and opioids loaded onto a second factor (Table S6, Figure 1), labeled the “legal” and “illegal” SE factors, respectively.5
Figure 1.
Results of Confirmatory Factor Analyses for Positive and Negative Subjective Effects
(See Figure 1 File).
Note: Standardized factor loadings are presented from models examining positive SEs/negative SEs. All factor loadings and correlations significant at p ≤ .001. SEs = Subjective Effects
2.2.2. CD Symptoms
CDsymp at Wave 1 (M = 5.70, SD = 2.95) were assessed using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC; Robins et al., 1996). The DISC is a structured interview assessing DSM-IV CD criteria and is reliable and valid (e.g., Piacentini et al., 1993). 84.86% of participants met diagnostic criteria for CD. A CDsymp composite score was created by summing endorsed symptoms (see Table S7 for descriptive statistics).
2.2.3. SUDs
At all waves, the Composite International Diagnostic Interview – Substance Abuse Module (CIDI-SAM; Cottler et al., 1989), a reliable measure of SUD severity for adolescents and adults in clinical and community settings (Crowley et al., 2001; Üstün et al., 1997), assessed diagnostic criteria for DSM-IV substance abuse and dependence criteria met during lifetime at Wave 1. At Waves 2 and 3, past year SUD criteria met was evaluated (see Tables S8 and S9 for frequencies). Following the present DSM-5, which does not distinguish between substance abuse and dependence, we indexed SUD severity for each substance: 0 = “no criteria endorsed”, 1 = “one criterion endorsed”, 2 = “mild”, 3 = “moderate”, 4 = “severe”, which was the outcome for all analyses. At Wave 1, 91.81% of participants met diagnostic criteria for two or more SUDs and the mean number of SUDs was 3.02 (SD = 1.91). Probands had used a mean of 4.12 substances five or more times by Wave 1 assessment (SD = 1.59). CFA provided evidence for a two-factor SUD model at each wave, with legal SUD loading on one factor and illegal SUD loading onto the second factor (Table S10, Figure 2).
Figure 2.
Results of Confirmatory Factor Analyses for Substance Use Disorder Factors at Waves 1, 2, and 3 (See Figure 2 File)
Note: Standardized factor loadings from models examining SUD criteria assessed in Wave1/Wave 2/Wave 3 are presented.
All factor loadings and correlations significant at p ≤ .001. SUD = substance use disorder.
2.3. Statistical Analyses
Structural equation models examining associations between SEs and SUD were estimated using MPlus Version 8.3 (Muthén & Muthén, 2017), including best-fitting models from CFAs. We used the weighted least squares mean and variance with pairwise deletion. Bentler’s Comparative Fit Indices (CFI; Bentler, 1990) > .95, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Browne & Cudeck, 1992) < .06, and non-significant χ2 statistics (interpreted with caution due to sensitivity to sample size) indicated good model fit. The False Discovery Rate (FDR), which controls for expected proportion of falsely rejected null hypotheses, addressed multiple testing (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995; Radua & Albajes-Eizagirre, 2022).
First, we examined correlations between all measures. Further analyses controlled for CDsymp and sex. At each wave, we regressed SUD factors and residual variance of specific SUDs on SEs for each substance (question 1). Each SUD factor was regressed on each positive and negative SE factor (question 2). For question 3, latent growth curve modeling addressed whether SEs predict the slope of SUD, or change in SUD from wave 1, accounting for different ages at assessment (Figure 3; Muthén & Muthén, 2017). For statistically significant associations between predictors and SUD, we tested for racial/ethnic group differences (non-Hispanic White vs. all other categories) with a χ2 difference test (question 4). Significant χ2 difference tests between models with parameters freed vs. models equating parameters across groups would indicate group differences in associations.
Figure 3.
Example Growth Model Examining Associations between SEs, CD Symptoms, Initial SUD, and Change in SUD Over Time, After Controlling for Sex
(See Figure 3 File).
Note: For each model, the TYPE = RANDOM and TSCORES (time scores) options were used to allow time scores to vary across individuals (i.e., different ages of assessment at Waves 1 through 3), incorporated into the model using the AT command. Latent basis growth models with freed slope loadings are more efficient than separate models for linear, quadratic, and high polynomials while allowing estimation of nonlinear change without collinearity and are simpler to interpret (Ram & Grimm, 2007). The latent SUD intercept had unstandardized loadings of 1.0 for each timepoint (capturing individual stability of SUD across time). The latent slope factor reflected individual change in SUDs over time (Bollen & Curran, 2006). Results after controlling for sex are reported. SEs = subjective effects. SUD = substance use disorder.
3. RESULTS
3.1. Do SEs and CDsymp predict general or specific SUD?
All significant correlations between predictors (SEs and CDsymp) and SUD were positive (Tables S11 – S13). Positive and negative SEs for each substance were significantly correlated with corresponding Wave 1 SUDs. Fewer SEs were significantly correlated with corresponding Wave 2 and 3 SUDs (most significant correlations were between SEs for legal substances and corresponding SUDs). CDsymp were significantly correlated with all SUDs at Wave 1, only with Wave 2 hallucinogen SUD, and only with Wave 3 cannabis SUD.
Below, we address whether SEs and CDsymp independently influence SUDs, controlling for sex and after addressing multiple testing. All significant associations were positive.
3.1.1. Legal SUDs.
3.1.1.1. Legal SUD Factor
General legal SUD factors were predicted by positive and negative SEs for alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis at Wave 1 (β = .226 to .462), positive SEs for cannabis and negative SEs for alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis at Wave 2 (β = .210 to .292), and alcohol negative SEs at Wave 3 (β = .304; Table 1). CDsymp predicted general legal SUD only at Wave 1 (β = .382 to .514).
Table 1.
Legal SUD Factors and Residual Variances of Specific SUDs at Waves 1, 2, and 3 Regressed on Positive and Negative SEs for Each Substance and CD Symptoms, Controlling for Sex
Alcohol | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Legal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Alcohol SUD | Legal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Alcohol SUD | ||||||
β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
| |||||||||
Wave 1 (N = 642) | |||||||||
Alcohol Positive SEs | .227*** [.128, .326] | < .001 | .214*** [.127, .300] | <.001 | Alcohol Negative SEs | .462*** [.365, .558] | < .001 | −.009 [−.132, .114] | .886 |
CD Symptoms | .382*** [.285, .480] | < .001 | .150*** [.062, .238] | .001 | CD Symptoms | .319*** [.224, .415] | < .001 | .160*** [.066, .253] | .001 |
Wave 2 (N = 367) | |||||||||
Alcohol Positive SEs | .172* [.019, .325] | .027 | .037 [−.093, .167] | .578 | Alcohol Negative SEs | .237** [.090, .384] | .002 | −.124 [−.255, .008] | .065 |
CD Symptoms | .060 [−.093, .214] | .443 | .027 [−.093, .146] | .662 | CD Symptoms | .052 [−.100, .204] | .501 | .051 [−.071, .172] | .414 |
Wave 3 (N = 314) | |||||||||
Alcohol Positive SEs | .105 [−.070, .280] | .238 | −.013 [−.159, .134] | .865 | Alcohol Negative SEs | .304*** [.119, .489] | .001 | −.093 [−.267, .082] | .298 |
CD Symptoms | .155 [−.031, .340] | .103 | −.114 [−.266, .038] | .142 | CD Symptoms | .147 [−.034, .328] | .111 | −.113 [−.270, .044] | .159 |
| |||||||||
Tobacco | |||||||||
Legal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Tobacco SUD | Legal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Tobacco SUD | ||||||
β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
| |||||||||
Wave 1 (N = 608–610) | |||||||||
Tobacco Positive SEs | .275*** [.171, .378] | < .001 | .018 [−.089, .124] | .747 | Tobacco Negative SEs | .217*** [.109, .324] | < .001 | −.049 [−.161, .063] | .393 |
CD Symptoms | .488*** [.388, .588] | < .001 | −.109 [−.240, .022] | .103 | CD Symptoms | .499*** [.397, .600] | < .001 | −.125 [−.261, .011] | .072 |
Wave 2 (N = 353–355) | |||||||||
Tobacco Positive SEs | .108 [−.050, .267] | .180 | .102 [−.003, .207] | .056 | Tobacco Negative SEs | .210** [.057, .363] | .007 | −.083 [−.216, .050] | .220 |
CD Symptoms | .086 [−.071, .243] | .283 | −.045 [−.168, .077] | .469 | CD Symptoms | .079 [−.076, .234] | .318 | −.040 [−.164, .085] | .531 |
Wave 3 (N = 306–308) | |||||||||
Tobacco Positive SEs | .170 [−.007, .347] | .060 | .038 [−.097, .174] | .581 | Tobacco Negative SEs | .179* [.008, .349] | .040 | −.030 [−.165, .104] | .661 |
CD Symptoms | .099 [−.087, .285] | .297 | .018 [−.124, .159] | .805 | CD Symptoms | .133 [−.051, .317] | .157 | .003 [−.142, .148] | .967 |
| |||||||||
Cannabis | |||||||||
Legal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Cannabis SUD | Legal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Cannabis SUD | ||||||
β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
| |||||||||
Wave 1 (N = 665) | |||||||||
Cannabis Positive SEs | .226*** [.135, .317] | < .001 | .110* [.023, .198] | .014 | Cannabis Negative SEs | .368*** [.276, .459] | < .001 | .163** [.056, .269] | .003 |
CD Symptoms | .514*** [.427, .600] | < .001 | −.067 [−.181, .046] | .245 | CD Symptoms | .452*** [.364, .540] | < .001 | −.071 [−.176, .033] | .182 |
Wave 2 (N = 378) | |||||||||
Cannabis Positive SEs | .292*** [.154, .431] | < .001 | −.106 [−.234, .022] | .106 | Cannabis Negative SEs | .273*** [.120, .426] | < .001 | .018 [−.097, .134] | .755 |
CD Symptoms | .093 [−.051, .237] | .208 | .003 [−.116, .122] | .963 | CD Symptoms | .059 [−.090, .209] | .436 | −.002 [−.124, .119] | .969 |
Wave 3 (N = 326) | |||||||||
Cannabis Positive SEs | .003 [−.165, .170] | .977 | .188** [.069, .308] | .002 | Cannabis Negative SEs | .207* [.037, .378] | .017 | −.046 [−.179, .087] | .500 |
CD Symptoms | .082 [−.095, .259] | .363 | .067 [−.063, .197] | .315 | CD Symptoms | .057 [−.120, .235] | .527 | .074 [−.059, .207] | .276 |
p < .05
p < .01
p ≤ .001
Bolded estimate: FDR p < .05
Note: Reported Ns include individuals with data for both SEs and SUDs for that specific substance.
3.1.1.2. Residual Variance of Specific SUDs.
Substance-specific residual variances were predicted by positive SEs for alcohol and cannabis at Wave 1 (β = .110 to .214), no SEs at Wave 2, and positive SEs for cannabis at Wave 3 (β = .188; Table 1). CDsymp significantly predicted only Wave 1 alcohol SUD residual variance (β = .150 to .160).
3.1.2. Illegal SUDs
3.1.2.1. Illegal SUD Factor.
General illegal SUD factors were predicted by positive SEs for hallucinogens and cocaine and negative SEs for hallucinogens, amphetamines, and opioids at Wave 1 (β = .197 to .382), no SEs at Wave 2, and negative SEs for hallucinogens at Wave 3 (β = .273; Table 2). CDsymp predicted Wave 1 general SUD (β = .241 to .296), but not Wave 2 or 3.
Table 2.
Illegal SUD Factors and Residual Variances of Specific SUDs at Waves 1, 2, and 3 Regressed on Positive and Negative SEs for Each Substance and CD Symptoms, Controlling for Sex
Hallucinogens | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Illegal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Hallucinogen SUD | Illegal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Hallucinogen SUD | ||||||
β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
| |||||||||
Wave 1 (N =275) | |||||||||
Hallucinogen Positive SEs | .197** [.052, .342] | .008 | .135* [.010, .261] | .035 | Hallucinogen Negative SEs | .382*** [.257, .507] | < .001 | .086 [−.050, .222] | .214 |
CD Symptoms | .292*** [.144, .440] | < .001 | −.023 [−.159, .113] | .742 | CD Symptoms | .275*** [.133, .416] | < .001 | −.018 [−.151, .116] | .795 |
Wave 2 (N = 178) | |||||||||
Hallucinogen Positive SEs | .020 [−.205, .245] | .860 | .075 [−.101, .251] | .402 | Hallucinogen Negative SEs | .221* [.003, .439] | .047 | .064 [−.129, .257] | .517 |
CD Symptoms | .071 [−.163, .304] | .552 | .002 [−.178, .182] | .980 | CD Symptoms | .075 [−.171, .320] | .552 | .005 [−.172, .183] | .954 |
Wave 3 (N = 160) | |||||||||
Hallucinogen Positive SEs | −.133 [−.381, .115] | .292 | .169 [−.045, .384] | .122 | Hallucinogen Negative SEs | .273* [.056, .489] | .014 | .100 [−.094, .294] | .313 |
CD Symptoms | −.068 [−.324, .188] | .603 | −.110 [−.318, .097] | .297 | CD Symptoms | −.142 [−.395, .111] | .271 | −.086 [−.299, .127] | .428 |
| |||||||||
Amphetamines | |||||||||
Illegal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Amphetamine SUD | Illegal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Amphetamine SUD | ||||||
β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
| |||||||||
Wave 1 (N = 220) | |||||||||
Amphetamine Positive SEs | −.001 [−.179, .176] | .988 | .378*** [.249, .507] | < .001 | Amphetamine Negative SEs | .220** [.062, .378] | .006 | .169* [.040, .299] | .011 |
CD Symptoms | .249** [.073, .425] | .006 | .019 [−.127, .164] | .802 | CD Symptoms | .241** [.066, .415] | 007 | .043 [−.111, .197] | .586 |
Wave 2 (N = 130) | |||||||||
Amphetamine Positive SEs | −.008 [−.251, .236] | .950 | .031 [−.168, .230] | .762 | Amphetamine Negative SEs | .084 [−.138, .307] | .458 | −.064 [−.262, .135] | .530 |
CD Symptoms | −.001 [−.254, .252] | .994 | −.183 [−.408, .042] | .110 | CD Symptoms | −.007 [−.250, .235] | .952 | −.184 [−.408, .040] | .107 |
Wave 3 (N = 113) | |||||||||
Amphetamine Positive SEs | .113 [−.308, .533] | .599 | −.132 [−.435, .172] | .394 | Amphetamine Negative SEs | .161 [−.284, .606] | .478 | −.188 [−.544, .167] | .299 |
CD Symptoms | −.044 [−.447, .358] | .829 | −.181 [−.465, .103] | .212 | CD Symptoms | −.036 [−.466, .393] | .868 | −.180 [−.491, .131] | .258 |
| |||||||||
Cocaine | |||||||||
Illegal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Cocaine SUD | Illegal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Cocaine SUD | ||||||
β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
| |||||||||
Wave 1 (N = 231) | |||||||||
Cocaine Positive SEs | .241** [.082, .401] | .003 | .102 [−.044, .247] | .170 | Cocaine Negative SEs | .184* [.023, .344] | .025 | .111 [−.021, .243] | .098 |
CD Symptoms | .278*** [.119, .437] | .001 | −.019 [−.164, .126] | .797 | CD Symptoms | .268*** [.109, .427] | .001 | −.032 [−.178, .114] | .665 |
Wave 2 (N = 133) | |||||||||
Cocaine Positive SEs | N/A | N/A | .048 [−.168, .264] | .666 | Cocaine Negative SEs | N/A | N/A | .089 [−.115, .293] | .395 |
CD Symptoms | N/A+ | N/A | −.081 [−.302, .139] | .471 | CD Symptoms | N/A | N/A | −.092 [−.313, .129] | .415 |
Wave 3 (N = 127) | |||||||||
Cocaine Positive SEs | −.055 [−.333, .223] | .697 | −.030 [−.224, .165] | .765 | Cocaine Negative SEs | .097 [−.159, .353] | .458 | −.109 [−.326, .108] | .325 |
CD Symptoms | −.213 [−.490, .064] | .131 | .122 [−.097, .341] | .275 | CD Symptoms | −.217 [−.491, .057] | .121 | .128 [−.091, .348] | .252 |
| |||||||||
Opioids | |||||||||
Illegal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Opioid SUD | Illegal SUD Factor | Residual Variance of Opioid SUD | ||||||
β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
| |||||||||
Wave 1 (N = 178) | |||||||||
Opioid Positive SEs | .021 [−.184, .227] | .838 | .248** [.080, .416] | .004 | Opioid Negative SEs | .258** [.076, .441] | .006 | .268** [.103, .434] | .002 |
CD Symptoms | .268** [.098, .439] | .002 | −.102 [−.287, .083] | .280 | CD Symptoms | .296*** [.129, .464] | .001 | −.073 [−.255, .109] | .433 |
Wave 2 (N = 110) | |||||||||
Opioid Positive SEs | .054 [−.180, .287] | .652 | .224 [−.092, .540] | .164 | Opioid Negative SEs | −.159 [−.475, .156] | .322 | .425** [.115, .736] | .007 |
CD Symptoms | −.124 [−.410, .162] | .395 | .044 [−.312, .400] | .809 | CD Symptoms | −.229 [−.584, .126] | .206 | .217 [−.248, .682] | .361 |
Wave 3 (N = 106) | |||||||||
Opioid Positive SEs | −.027 [−.325, .271] | .179 | .295* [.036, .554]. | .026 | Opioid Negative SEs | .095 [−.215, .406] | .547 | .148 [−.104, .400] | .249 |
CD Symptoms | −.318 [−.688, .051] | .091 | .026 [−.422, .474] | .910 | CD Symptoms | −.312 [−.699, .075] | .114 | .035 [−.440, .510] | .885 |
p < .05
p < .01
p ≤ .001
Bolded estimate: FDR p < .05
At Wave 2, we were unable to regress cocaine positive SEs on the general SUD factor, due to small cell sizes
Note: Reported Ns include individuals with data for both SEs and SUDs for that specific substance.
3.1.2.2. Residual Variance of Specific SUDs.
Substance-specific residual variances were predicted by positive and negative SEs for amphetamines and opioids at Wave 1 (β = .169 to .268), negative SEs for opioids at Wave 2 (β = .425), and no SEs at Wave 3 (Table 2). CDsymp did not predict specific illegal SUD residual variance.
3.1.3. Exploratory Analyses.
Exploratory analyses addressed whether individual negative SEs negatively predicted SUD, since positive correlations between negative SEs and SUDs appeared counterintuitive. We examined correlations between each individual negative SE item for legal substances and legal SUD factors at all waves (given robust associations between negative SE factors and legal SUD factors). There were no significant negative correlations (Table S14). We also tested whether substantial correlations between positive and negative SEs drove associations between negative SE factors and SUDs. After controlling for positive SE factors, negative SE factors predicted legal and illegal SUD independently at Wave 1 and illegal SUD at Wave 3 (Table S15).
3.1.4. Summary
SEs were more robust predictors of general SUD than substance-specific SUD. Legal SEs predicted general legal SUD into adulthood, whereas illegal SEs primarily predicted general illegal SUD in adolescence. Effect sizes were similar across legal and illegal SUD, although confidence intervals were broader for illegal SUD. Results were robust for both positive and negative SEs. CDsymp consistently predicted general, rather than specific, SUD, and only in adolescence.
3.2. Do SEs predict SUD across substances?
Positive and negative SEs were significantly correlated across substances (Table S16). There was little evidence that correlations between SEs and SUDs for corresponding substances were larger in magnitude than those across substances (Tables S11, S12).
We also examined correlations between SEs and SUD latent factors (Table 3). Positive and negative legal SEs factors and legal SUD factors were significantly correlated at all waves. Illegal SE factors and illegal SUD factors were significantly correlated at Wave 1 (positive and negative SEs) and Wave 3 (negative SEs). Legal SEs factors and illegal SUD factors were significantly correlated at Wave 1 (positive and negative) and Waves 2 and 3 (negative). Illegal SEs factors (positive and negative) and legal SUD factors were significantly correlated at Waves 1 and 2.
Table 3.
Correlations Between SUD Latent Factors, Positive and Negative SEs Latent Factors, and CD Symptom Count
Legal SUD W1 | Legal SUD W2 | Legal SUD W3 | CDsymp | Pos Legal SE | Neg Legal SE | Illegal SUD W1 | Illegal SUD W2 | Illegal SUD W3 | Pos Illegal SE | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||||
Legal SUD W1 | 1 | |||||||||
Legal SUD W2 | .494*** [.349, .639] | 1 | ||||||||
Legal SUD W3 | .530*** [.384, .676] | .945*** [.780, 1.111] | 1 | |||||||
CDsymp | .503*** [.424, .581] | .109 [−.019, .238] | .139 [−.012, .289] | 1 | ||||||
Pos Legal SE | .593*** [.492, .693] | .354*** [.192, .515] | .255* [.058, .451] | .189*** [.095, .284] | 1 | |||||
Neg Legal SE | .773*** [.670, .875] | .383*** [.216, .550] | .338*** [.158, .518] | .284*** [.190, .379] | .640*** [.542, .739] | 1 | ||||
Illegal SUD W1 | .796*** [.711, .882] | .209* [−.059, .359] | .358*** [.201, .515] | .452*** [.367, .537] | .163* [.037, .289] | .556*** [.448, .665] | 1 | |||
Illegal SUD W2 | .327*** [.144, .509] | .549*** [.382, .717] | .265* [.010, .521] | .232** [.078, .386] | .010 [−.177, .197] | .211* [.026, .407] | .771*** [.608, .933] | 1 | ||
Illegal SUD W3 | .197* [.002, .391] | .324** [.123, .524] | .556*** [.371, .741] | .098 [−.069, .265] | .086 [−.129, .302] | .353*** [.141, .632] | .554*** [.369, .738] | .889*** [.671, 1.108] | 1 | |
Pos Illegal SE | .403*** [.265, .540] | .368*** [.167, .569] | .188 [−.020, .395] | .040 [−.073, .152] | .896*** [.786, 1.005] | .473*** [.341, .605] | .232*** [.141, .324] | .114 [−.066, .294] | .012 [−.223, .247] | 1 |
Neg Illegal SE | .575*** [.421, .728] | .241* [.013, .470] | .212 [−.037, .462] | .044 [−.084, .173] | .592*** [.442, .742] | .877*** [.759, .994] | .343*** [.239, .448] | .187 [−.038, .412] | .272* [.040, .503] | .504*** [.365, .644] |
Note: Legal SUD W1, W2, W3: Legal SUD Factors for Waves 1, 2, and 3. Illegal SUD W1, W2, W3: Illegal SUD Factors for Waves 1, 2, and 3. Pos Legal SE: Positive SEs Factor for Legal Substance. Neg Legal SE: Negative SEs Factor for Legal Substances. Pos Illegal SE: Positive SEs Factor for Illegal Substance. Neg Illegal SE: Negative SEs Factor for Illegal Substances. CDsymp: CD Symptom Count.
p ≤ .001
p = .003
p < .05
Below are results from models examining whether each latent SEs factor and CDsymp independently influenced latent SUD factors, after controlling for sex and addressing multiple testing. All significant associations were positive.
3.2.1. Legal SUDs
The legal positive SEs factor predicted general legal SUD at Waves 1 and 2 (β = .328 to .497), and the legal negative SEs factor predicted general legal SUD at all waves (β = .327 to .631; Table 4). Positive and negative illegal SEs factors predicted general legal SUD at Wave 1 (β = .371 to .550; Table 4). CDsymp only predicted general legal SUD at Wave 1 (β = .499 to .506).
Table 4.
Legal and Illegal SUD Factors at Waves 1, 2, and 3 Regressed on Legal and Illegal Positive and Negative SE Factors and CD Symptoms, Controlling for Sex
β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||
Legal SUD Factor | |||||
Wave 1 (N = 723) | |||||
General Legal Positive SEs | .497*** [.404, .591] | < .001 | General Legal Negative SEs | .631*** [.532, .730] | <.001 |
CD Symptoms | .506*** [.428, .583] | <.001 | CD Symptoms | .499*** [.421, .578] | <.001 |
Wave 2 (N = 696) | |||||
General Legal Positive SEs | .328*** [.173,.484] | <.001 | General Legal Negative SEs | .362*** [.195, .529] | <.001 |
CD Symptoms | .090 [ −.043, .223] | .185 | CD Symptoms | .092 [−.041, .224] | .176 |
Wave 3 (N = 693) | |||||
General Legal Positive SEs | .216* [.025, .408] | .027 | General Legal Negative SEs | .327*** [.148, .507] | <.001 |
CD Symptoms | .125 [−.033, .283] | .122 | CD Symptoms | .121 [−.038, .279] | .136 |
Legal SUD Factor | |||||
Wave 1 (N = 723) | |||||
General Illegal Positive SEs | .371*** [.246, .495] | < .001 | General Illegal Negative SEs | .550*** [.408, .692] | < .001 |
CD Symptoms | .504*** [.426, .581] | < .001 | CD Symptoms | .503*** [.426, .581] | < .001 |
Wave 2 (N = 572) | |||||
General Illegal Positive SEs | .360*** [.159, 562] | < .001 | General Illegal Negative SEs | .249* [.016, .482] | .036 |
CD Symptoms | .089 [−.042, .220] | .183 | CD Symptoms | .090 [−.041, .221] | .180 |
Wave 3 (N = 547) | |||||
General Illegal Positive SEs | .181 [−.030, .393] | .093 | General Illegal Negative SEs | .238 [−.018, .493] | .068 |
CD Symptoms | .115 [−.041, .271] | .149 | CD Symptoms | .116 [−.039, .272] | .143 |
Illegal SUD Factor | |||||
Wave 1 (N = 723) | |||||
General Illegal Positive SEs | .221*** [.136, .307] | < .001 | General Illegal Negative SEs | .340*** [.239, .441] | < .001 |
CD Symptoms | .457*** [.371, .542] | < .001 | CD Symptoms | .457*** [.371, .542] | < .001 |
Wave 2 (N = 572) | |||||
General Illegal Positive SEs | .100 [−.072, .272] | .253 | General Illegal Negative SEs | .185 [−.044, .415] | .114 |
CD Symptoms | .202* [.046, .358] | .011 | CD Symptoms | .201* [.046, .357] | .011 |
Wave 3 (N = 547) | |||||
General Illegal Positive SEs | −.010 [−.238, .217] | .930 | General Illegal Negative SEs | .259* [.027, .492] | .029 |
CD Symptoms | .077 [−.089, .243] | .365 | CD Symptoms | .074 [−.098, .247] | .399 |
Illegal SUD Factor | |||||
Wave 1 (N = 723) | |||||
General Legal Positive SEs | .078 [−.034, .189] | .172 | General Legal Negative SEs | .451*** [.349, .552] | < .001 |
CD Symptoms | .456*** [.371, .542] | < .001 | CD Symptoms | .456*** [.371, .542] | < .001 |
Wave 2 (N = 696) | |||||
General Legal Positive SEs | −.030 [−.201, .140] | .728 | General Legal Negative SEs | .157 [ −.028, .341] | .095 |
CD Symptoms | .202* [.045, .358] | .011 | CD Symptoms | .201* [.045, .357] | .011 |
Wave 3 (N = 693) | |||||
General Legal Positive SEs | −.013 [−.209, .182] | .892 | General Legal Negative SEs | .370*** [.169, .570] | < .001 |
CD Symptoms | .078 [−.091, .247] | .365 | CD Symptoms | .070 [−.109, .248] | .443 |
p < .05
p ≤ .001
Bolded estimate: FDR p < .05
3.2.2. Illegal SUDs
Illegal positive and negative SEs factors predicted general illegal SUD at Wave 1 (β = .221 to .340; Table 4). The legal negative SE factor predicted general illegal SUD (β = .370 to .451; Waves 1 and 3; Table 4). CDsymp predicted general illegal SUD at Waves 1 and 2 (β = .201 to .457).
3.2.3. Exploratory Analyses
Exploratory analyses, not preregistered, indicated some significant residual correlations between SEs for each substance and corresponding SUDs at all waves. These results suggest that SEs also explain substance-specific SUD (Tables S17 – S20).
3.2.4. Summary
Overall, results provided strong evidence for cross-substance effects. Correlations between SEs and SUD across substances were significant, particularly at Wave 1. Legal and illegal SE factors predicted legal and illegal SUD in adolescence and adulthood.
3.3. Do SEs predict change in SUD over time?
Overall, mean SUD (lifetime at Wave 1; past year at Waves 2 and 3) decreased significantly from Wave 1 to 3 for most substances, except for cocaine and amphetamines (Table S20). Alcohol, cannabis, and cocaine slope variances were not significant, suggesting lack of variability to examine differential predictors of change in SUD over time. Associations between SEs and change in SUD from adolescence to adulthood for models with significant slope variance (tobacco, hallucinogens, amphetamines, and opioids; Table S21) indicated only positive amphetamine SEs significantly predicted greater decrease in SUD (β = −.280; Table 5). Although this negative association appears contradictory to positive associations between SEs and SUD at each wave, amphetamine SEs predicted amphetamine SUD only at Wave 1. The negative association indicates a floor effect; individuals with lower initial SEs and CDsymp decreased less in SUD over time.
Table 5.
Change in SUD Criteria Over Time for Individual Substances Regressed on SEs and CD Symptoms, Controlling for Sex
SUD Slope of Individual Substance |
SUD Slope of Individual Substance |
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | p-value | β | p-value | ||
| |||||
Tobacco SUD (N = 608–610) | |||||
Tobacco Positive SEs | −.016 [−.039, .007] | .161 | Tobacco Negative SEs | −.008 [−.031, .015] | .489 |
CD Symptoms | −.009 [−.018, .001] | .068 | CD Symptoms | −.010* [−.019, −.001] | .032 |
Hallucinogen SUD (N = 275) | |||||
Hallucinogen Positive SEs | .021 [−.158, .201] | .815 | Hallucinogen Negative SEs | .004 [−.187, .195] | .969 |
CD Symptoms | −.021* [−.039, −.003] | .023 | CD Symptoms | −.020* [−.038, −.002] | .025 |
Amphetamine SUD (N = 220) | |||||
Amphetamine Positive SEs | −.280** [−.487, −.072] | .008 | Amphetamine Negative SEs | −.216* [−.407, −.026] | .026 |
CD Symptoms | −.021* [−.039, −.003] | .023 | CD Symptoms | −.019* [−.036, −.003] | .022 |
Opioid SUD (N = 178) | |||||
Opioid Positive SEs | .239 [−.053, .531] | .109 | Opioid Negative SEs | .087 [−.265, .439] | .630 |
CD Symptoms | −.017 [−.048, .015] | .304 | CD Symptoms | −.018 [−.052, .015] | .284 |
p < .05
p ≤ .01
Bolded estimate: FDR p < .05
Note: Reported Ns include individuals with data for both SEs and SUDs for that specific substance.
3.4. Are there sex or racial/ethnic differences in associations between predictors and SUD?
We found no evidence for racial/ethnic group differences in associations (Tables S22, S23). Limited number of girls/women in our sample prevented direct testing of sex differences in associations between SEs and SUDs. We instead regressed sex on SUD of each substance (Tables S24, S25).
4. DISCUSSION
The present study aimed to increase understanding of the predictors of SUDs across development in a high-risk sample with greater odds of sustained SUD. Extant literature primarily focuses on retrospectively reported predictors for commonly used substances in community samples. Our study extends prior research by addressing whether CD symptoms and SEs for legal and illegal substances assessed during adolescence predict general versus specific SUD from adolescence to adulthood in a longitudinal clinical sample.
Contrary to our hypothesis that SEs would predict specific SUD, results indicated that positive and negative SEs were robust predictors of general SUD. SEs also predicted substance-specific SUD, although less reliably. SEs for legal substances predicted general legal SUD from adolescence to adulthood, whereas SEs for illegal substances predicted general illegal SUD mostly in adolescence. Notably, effect sizes for associations between SEs and legal and illegal SUD were similar, but sample sizes were smaller and confidence intervals were wider in analyses addressing illegal SUDs. Overall, these results indicate that SEs may be relevant predictors for individuals engaging in both legal and illegal polysubstance use.
Our results suggest that intensity and quantity of sensation experienced may be more important predictors than valence, as associations between SEs and SUD were similar in magnitude across positive and negative SEs. Although the result that unpleasant experiences did not deter use seems counterintuitive, they align with prior research on substance use or SUD in a combined community/clinical sample (Zeiger et al., 2010) and community youth samples (Ríos-Bedoya et al., 2009; Zeiger et al., 2012). Prior research suggests that some individuals need higher doses of substances (specifically alcohol) to feel both sedative and stimulant effects, indicating individuals using more of a substance might report more SEs in general (Chung & Martin, 2009). Some individuals at high SUD risk may be “high responders” who are sensitive to both positive and negative substance effects (Chung & Martin, 2009; Zeiger et al., 2012). Lastly, significant associations between negative SEs and SUDs have been found in adolescent or very young adult populations (Ríos-Bedoya et al., 2009; Zeiger et al., 2010, 2012). Adolescents are less deterred by negative consequences and more motivated by rewards, specifically in the context of substance use (Bjork et al., 2007; Colder et al., 2013; Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010). Adolescents experiencing positive and negative SEs may be less deterred by negative physiological drug experiences and more motivated by positive experiences, in congruence with prior research (Colder et al., 2013).
Cross- and within-substance associations between SEs and SUD were equally robust, for both negative and positive SEs. Our results align with prior research finding associations between positive and negative SEs across cannabis, tobacco, and alcohol at one timepoint in a mixed clinical and community sample (Zeiger et al., 2012). Cross-substance effects in our results were general: legal and illegal SE factors predicted general legal and illegal SUD factors in adolescence and adulthood, providing clear evidence for cross-substance effects of SEs on polysubstance SUD for commonly and less commonly used substances.
CDsymp primarily predicted general SUD in adolescence, in contrast to evidence suggesting that CD predicts general SUD over time (Erskine et al., 2016; Palmer et al., 2013). We examined individuals ascertained for SUDs with high CD symptom rates (M = 5.70, SD = 2.95) and CD symptom range restriction may have limited the associations between CDsymp and adult SUDs. However, CDsymp—but not SUD—predicted later antisocial behavior (Border et al., 2018a) and mortality (Border et al., 2018b) in an overlapping sample.
Our clinical sample had high levels of SUDs, allowing us to investigate predictors of significant change in SUD over time. Overall, SUD severity decreased for most substances. However, neither SEs nor CDsymp consistently predicted change in SUD. Very few studies have examined change in SUD severity over time in clinical subjects, and additional research is needed to replicate these results. We also found no evidence for racial/ethnic differences in associations between SEs and SUD.
Substance-specific SEs, both positive and negative, were significantly correlated across substances. Many individuals in our sample met diagnostic criteria for SUD of multiple substances at Wave 1 and may have been using different substances simultaneously, and polysubstance use may have led to difficulties in discerning substance-specific SEs. Lastly, substances may act on common biological mechanisms, although different substances may act on the same neurotransmitters to varying degrees (Wise & Robble, 2020).
4.1. Limitations
First, we could not evaluate whether sex moderated associations; due to small numbers of women, multigroup analyses would not converge. Second, we replicated prior evidence that two-factor models (positive and negative SEs) fit significantly better than one-factor models for most substances but model nonconvergence prevented model fit comparisons for opioids. Third, results were most robust at Wave 1 than Waves 2 and 3. Analyses for the later waves likely had less power due to subject attrition—nearly 53% of individuals were lost to follow-up by Wave 3. Notably, greater Wave 1 CDsymp severity or SUD did not predict attrition (Table S1). Fourth, we collapsed all individuals who were not non-Hispanic White into one group due to small sample size of other racial/ethnic groups. Lastly, participants reported on SEs experienced “shortly after using” and may have referred to general experiences with a substance, recent use, or initial use (Zeiger et al., 2012). Regular or heavy users may have developed tolerance and reported SEs may have differed than those initially experienced.
4.2. Strengths
The present study’s longitudinal design is less vulnerable to recall bias than retrospective studies (Hardt & Rutter, 2004) because our assessment of SEs was more proximal to first use compared to existing retrospective research, as substance experimentation typically begins in adolescence. Mean age of first substance use in this sample ranged from 11.57–14.35 years (Table S26). We expanded the existing literature by examining a clinical, rather than community, sample ascertained for SUDs and CDsymp. CFA results replicated previous findings of a two-factor model of positive and negative SEs and provided evidence of a two-factor model of legal and illegal SUDs in a clinical sample. Finally, our sample’s racial and ethnic diversity facilitated examination of racial/ethnic differences in associations.
4.3. Conclusion
In a clinical sample, SEs predicted polysubstance SUD from adolescence through adulthood, whereas CDsymp predicted SUD primarily in adolescence only. SEs predicted general and substance-specific SUD, but prediction of general SUD was more robust. Associations between negative SEs and SUD were similar or stronger in magnitude compared to those between positive SEs and SUD. We demonstrated significant cross-substance associations between SEs and SUD of multiple substances. Finally, associations between SEs and SUD were consistent across race/ethnic groups. Overall, our results suggest that individuals at elevated risk for SUDs who experience high intensity and quantity of SEs likely have greater SUD severity, across multiple substances and into adulthood. In contrast, CDsymp were not strong longitudinal predictors of SUD in this clinical sample. Assessing SEs early and prospectively may help clinicians identify adolescents who are at highest risk for sustained SUDs.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse grants DA035804, DA011015, DA053693, and DA017637.
Footnotes
In our preregistration, we intended to examine substance use as an outcome. However, substance use variance was very limited, due to high substance use prevalence at all waves. Consequently, we focused analyses on SUDs.
We collapsed ethnic groups into non-Hispanic White vs. all other categories, given limited individuals in each category other than non-Hispanic White.
The present study excluded additional substances with low use prevalence.
For each substance, CFA results indicated a two-factor model (with positive and negative SE factors and significant factor loadings for all items) fit significantly better than a one-factor model (Table S5). We could not conduct a χ2 difference test comparing one versus two-factor SE opioid models, due model nonconvergence.
We refer to alcohol, cannabis, and tobacco as “legal” substances and hallucinogens, amphetamines, cocaine, and opiates as “illegal” substances. All substances were illegal for participants who were adolescents at initial assessment. Colorado fully legalized cannabis in 2014, after most participants were assessed for Wave 3.
Pre-registered hypothesis, design, and analysis plan available through Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/mc7w8/?view_only=6daabac7e797496db01743426e5683d0
5. REFERENCES
- Audrain-McGovern J, Koudsi NA, Rodriguez D, Wileyto EP, Shields PG, & Tyndale RF (2007). The Role of CYP2A6 in the Emergence of Nicotine Dependence in Adolescents. Pediatrics, 119(1), e264–e274. 10.1542/peds.2006-1583 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, & Hochberg Y (1995). Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. JSTOR. [Google Scholar]
- Bieber CM, Fernandez K, Borsook D, Brennan MJ, Butler SF, Jamison RN, Osgood E, Sharpe-Potter J, Thomson HN, Weiss RD, & Katz NP (2008). Retrospective accounts of initial subjective effects of opioids in patients treated for pain who do or do not develop opioid addiction: A pilot case-control study. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(5), 429–434. 10.1037/1064-1297.16.5.429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjork JM, Smith AR, Danube CL, & Hommer DW (2007). Developmental Differences in Posterior Mesofrontal Cortex Recruitment by Risky Rewards. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(18), 4839–4849. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5469-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Border R, Corley RP, Brown SA, Hewitt JK, Hopfer CJ, McWilliams SK, Rhea SA, Shriver CL, Stallings MC, Wall TL, Woodward KE, & Rhee SH (2018). Independent predictors of mortality in adolescents ascertained for conduct disorder and substance use problems, their siblings and community controls. Addiction (Abingdon, England), 113(11), Article 11. 10.1111/add.14366 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Border R, Corley RP, Brown SA, Hewitt JK, Hopfer CJ, Stallings MC, Wall TL, Young SE, & Rhee SH (2018). Predictors of adult outcomes in clinically- and legally-ascertained youth with externalizing problems. PLoS ONE, 13(11), Article 11. 10.1371/journal.pone.0206442 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown SA, Ramo DE, & Anderson KG (2011). Long-Term Trajectories of Adolescent Recovery. In Kelly JF & White WL (Eds.), Addiction Recovery Management: Theory, Research and Practice (pp. 127–142). Humana Press. 10.1007/978-1-60327-960-4_8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Button TMM, Rhee SH, Hewitt JK, Young SE, Corley RP, & Stallings MC (2007). The role of conduct disorder in explaining the comorbidity between alcohol and illicit drug dependence in adolescence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 87(1), 46–53. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen P, & Jacobson KC (2012). Developmental Trajectories of Substance Use From Early Adolescence to Young Adulthood: Gender and Racial/Ethnic Differences. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50(2), 154–163. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.05.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen X, Stacy A, Zheng H, Shan J, Spruijt-Metz D, Unger JB, Gong J, Gallaher P, Liu C, Azen S, Shakib S, & Johnson CA (2003). Sensations from initial exposure to nicotine predicting adolescent smoking in China: A potential measure of vulnerability to nicotine. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 5(4), 455–463. 10.1080/1462220031000118603 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung T, & Martin CS (2009, September). Subjective stimulant and sedative effects of alcohol during early drinking experiences predict alcohol involvement in treated adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 70(5), Article 5. Gale General OneFile. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciraulo DA, Piechniczek-Buczek J, & Iscan EN (2003). Outcome predictors in substance use disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 26(2), 381–409. 10.1016/S0193-953X(02)00106-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark DB, Martin CS, & Cornelius JR (2008). Adolescent-Onset Substance Use Disorders Predict Young Adult Mortality. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42(6), 637–639. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.11.147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colder CR, Hawk LW Jr., Lengua LJ, Wiezcorek W, Eiden RD, & Read JP (2013). Trajectories of Reinforcement Sensitivity During Adolescence and Risk for Substance Use. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 23(2), 345–356. 10.1111/jora.12001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornelius JR, Reynolds M, Martz BM, Clark DB, Kirisci L, & Tarter R (2008). Premature mortality among males with substance use disorders. Addictive Behaviors, 33(1), 156–160. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cottler LB, Robins LN, & Helzer JE (1989). The Reliability of the CIDI-SAM: A comprehensive substance abuse interview. British Journal of Addiction, 84(7), 801–814. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1989.tb03060.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowley TJ, Mikulich SK, Ehlers KM, Whitmore EA, & MacDonald MJ (2001). Validity of structured clinical evaluations in adolescents with conduct and substance problems. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(3), 265–273. 10.1097/00004583-200103000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowley TJ, Mikulich SK, MacDonald M, Young SE, & Zerbe GO (1998). Substance-dependent, conduct-disordered adolescent males: Severity of diagnosis predicts 2-year outcome1Presented in part to the Research Society on Alcoholism, Steamboat Springs, CO, 19 June 1995.1. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 49(3), 225–237. 10.1016/S0376-8716(98)00016-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davidson ES, & Schenk S (1994). Variability in subjective responses to marijuana: Initial experiences of college students. Addictive Behaviors, 19(5), 531–538. 10.1016/0306-4603(94)90008-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, & Phillips TJ (2012). Do initial responses to drugs predict future use or abuse? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(6), 1565–1576. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.04.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derks EM, Dolan CV, & Boomsma DI (2004). Effects of censoring on parameter estimates and power in genetic modeling. Twin Research: The Official Journal of the International Society for Twin Studies, 7(6), Article 6. 10.1375/1369052042663832 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiFranza JR, Savageau JA, Fletcher K, Ockene JK, Rigotti NA, McNeill AD, Coleman M, & Wood C (2004). Recollections and repercussions of the first inhaled cigarette. Addictive Behaviors, 29(2), 261–272. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiFranza JR, Savageau JA, Fletcher K, Pbert L, O’Loughlin J, McNeill AD, Ockene JK, Friedman K, Hazelton J, Wood C, Dussault G, & Wellman RJ (2007). Susceptibility to Nicotine Dependence: The Development and Assessment of Nicotine Dependence in Youth 2 Study. Pediatrics, 120(4), e974–e983. 10.1542/peds.2007-0027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erskine HE, Norman RE, Ferrari AJ, Chan GCK, Copeland WE, Whiteford HA, & Scott JG (2016). Long-Term Outcomes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Conduct Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(10), 841–850. 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.06.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fergusson DM, Hoorwood LJ, Lynskey MT, & Madden PA (2003). Early reactions to cannabis predict later dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 60(10), 1033–1039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galaif ER, & Newcomb MD (1999). Predictors of polydrug use among four ethnic groups: A 12-year longitudinal study. Addictive Behaviors, 24(5), 607–631. 10.1016/S0306-4603(99)00027-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Saha TD, Chou SP, Jung J, Zhang H, Pickering RP, Ruan WJ, Smith SM, Huang B, & Hasin DS (2015). Epidemiology of DSM-5 Alcohol Use Disorder: Results From the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(8), Article 8. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant JD, Scherrer JF, Lyons MJ, Tsuang M, True WR, & Bucholz KK (2005). Subjective reactions to cocaine and marijuana are associated with abuse and dependence. Addictive Behaviors, 30(8), 1574–1586. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green B, Kavanagh D, & Young R (2003). Being stoned: A review of self-reported cannabis effects. Drug and Alcohol Review, 22(4), 453–460. 10.1080/09595230310001613976 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haberstick BC, Zeiger JS, Corley RP, Hopfer CJ, Stallings MC, Rhee SH, & Hewitt JK (2010). Common and drug-specific genetic influences on subjective effects to alcohol, tobacco and marijuana use. Addiction, 106(1), 215–224. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03129.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haertzen CA, Kocher TR, & Miyasato K (1983). Reinforcements from the first drug experience can predict later drug habits and/or addiction: Results with coffee, cigarettes, alcohol, barbiturates, minor and major tranquilizers, stimulants, marijuana, hallucinogens, heroin, opiates and cocaine. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 11(2), 147–165. 10.1016/0376-8716(83)90076-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardt J, & Rutter M (2004). Validity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: Review of the evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 260–273. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00218.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartman CA, Lessem JM, Hopfer CJ, Crowley TJ, & Stallings MC (2006). The family transmission of adolescent alcohol abuse and dependence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 67(5), 657–664. 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.657 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopfer CJ, Stallings MC, Hewitt JK, & Crowley TJ (2003). Family transmission of marijuana use, abuse, and dependence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(7), 834–841. 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046874.56865.85 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu M-C, Muthén B, Schaffran C, Griesler PC, & Kandel DB (2008). Developmental trajectories of criteria of nicotine dependence in adolescence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 98(1), 94–104. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.04.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iacono WG, Malone SM, & McGue M (2008). Behavioral Disinhibition and the Development of Early-Onset Addiction: Common and Specific Influences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(1), 325–348. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, & Schulenberg JE (2009). Monitoring the Future National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2008. Volume II: College students & adults ages 19–50. (NIH Publication No. 09–7403). National Institute on Drug Abuse. [Google Scholar]
- Kandel DB, Hu M-C, Griesler PC, & Schaffran C (2007). On the development of nicotine dependence in adolescence. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 91(1), 26–39. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.04.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karsinti E, Piani K, Zerdazi EH, Maskos U, Faure P, Romo L, Bellivier F, Brousse G, Lépine JP, Bloch V, & Vorspan F (2018). Relevance of treated cocaine users’ retrospective memory of first cocaine use. Psychiatry Research, 264, 210–216. 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King AC, McNamara PJ, Hasin DS, & Cao D (2014). Alcohol Challenge Responses Predict Future Alcohol Use Disorder Symptoms: A 6-Year Prospective Study. Biological Psychiatry, 75(10), 798–806. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, Hicks BM, Patrick CJ, Carlson SR, Iacono WG, & McGue M (2009). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: Modeling the externalizing spectrum (p. 88). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/11855-003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lambert NM, McLeod M, & Schenk S (2006). Subjective responses to initial experience with cocaine: An exploration of the incentive–sensitization theory of drug abuse. Addiction, 101(5), 713–725. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01408.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Le Strat Y, Ramoz N, Horwood J, Falissard B, Hassler C, Romo L, Choquet M, Fergusson D, & Gorwood P (2009). First positive reactions to cannabis constitute a priority risk factor for cannabis dependence. Addiction, 104(10), 1710–1717. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02680.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyons MJ, Toomey R, Meyer JM, Green AI, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, True WR, & Tsuang MT (1997). How do genes influence marijuana use? The role of subjective effects. Addiction, 92(4), 409–417. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1997.tb03372.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merrin GJ, & Leadbeater B (2018). Do Classes of Polysubstance Use in Adolescence Differentiate Growth in Substances Used in the Transition to Young Adulthood? Substance Use & Misuse, 53(13), 2112–2124. 10.1080/10826084.2018.1455702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirza KAH, & Mirza S (2008). Adolescent substance misuse. Psychiatry, 7(8), 357–362. 10.1016/j.mppsy.2008.05.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Morean ME, & Corbin WR (2008). Subjective alcohol effects and drinking behavior: The relative influence of early response and acquired tolerance. Addictive Behaviors, 33(10), 1306–1313. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moss HB, Chen CM, & Yi H (2014). Early adolescent patterns of alcohol, cigarettes, and marijuana polysubstance use and young adult substance use outcomes in a nationally representative sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 136, 51–62. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2013.12.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (2017). Mplus: Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables: User’s Guide (Version 8). Authors. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer RHC, Knopik VS, Rhee SH, Hopfer CJ, Corley RC, Young SE, Stallings MC, & Hewitt JK (2013). Prospective effects of adolescent indicators of behavioral disinhibition on DSM-IV alcohol, tobacco, and illicit drug dependence in young adulthood. Addictive Behaviors, 38(9), 2415–2421. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.03.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pardini D, White HR, & Stouthamer-Loeber M (2007). Early adolescent psychopathology as a predictor of alcohol use disorders by young adulthood. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88, S38–S49. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piacentini J, Shaffer D, Fisher P, Schwab-Stone M, Davies M, & Gioia P (1993). The Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children-Revised Version (DISC-R): III. Concurrent criterion validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(3), 658–665. 10.1097/00004583-199305000-00025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau CS, Marks JL, Pomerleau OF, & Snedecor SM (2004). Relationship between early experiences with tobacco and early experiences with alcohol. Addictive Behaviors, 29(6), 1245–1251. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau OF, Pomerleau CS, & Namenek RJ (1998). Early experiences with tobacco among women smokers, ex-smokers, and never-smokers. Addiction, 93(4), 595–599. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.93459515.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radua J, & Albajes-Eizagirre A (n.d.). FDR online calculator. Seed-Based d Mapping (Formerly Signed Differential Mapping; ). Retrieved November 17, 2022, from https://www.sdmproject.com/utilities/?show=FDR [Google Scholar]
- Ram N, & Grimm K (2007). Using simple and complex growth models to articulate developmental change: Matching theory to method. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(4), Article 4. 10.1177/0165025407077751 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ríos-Bedoya CF, Pomerleau CS, Neuman RJ, & Pomerleau OF (2009). Using MIMIC models to examine the relationship between current smoking and early smoking experiences. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 11(9), 1035–1041. 10.1093/ntr/ntp093 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robins L, Marcus L, Reich W, Cunningham R, & Gallagher T (1996). The Diagnostic Interview Schedule Version IV. Washington University. [Google Scholar]
- Romero AJ, Martinez D, & Carvajal SC (2007). Bicultural Stress and Adolescent Risk Behaviors in a Community Sample of Latinos and Non-Latino European Americans. Ethnicity and Health. 10.1080/13557850701616854 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SAMHSA. (2020). Key Substance Use and Mental Health Indicators in the United States: Results from the 2019 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. [Google Scholar]
- Scherrer JF, Grant JD, Duncan AE, Sartor CE, Haber JR, Jacob T, & Bucholz KK (2009). Subjective effects to cannabis are associated with use, abuse and dependence after adjusting for genetic and environmental influences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 105(1), 76–82. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.06.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stone AL, Becker LG, Huber AM, & Catalano RF (2012). Review of risk and protective factors of substance use and problem use in emerging adulthood. Addictive Behaviors, 37(7), 747–775. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.02.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trim RS, Worley MJ, Wall TL, Hopfer CJ, Crowley TJ, Hewitt JK, & Brown SA (2015). Bivariate Trajectories of Substance Use and Antisocial Behavior: Associations with Emerging Adult Outcomes in a High-Risk Sample. Emerging Adulthood (Print), 3(4), 265–276. 10.1177/2167696815573791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Üstün B, Compton W, Mager D, Babor T, Baiyewu O, Chatterji S, Cottler L, Göğüş A, Mavreas V, Peters L, Pull C, Saunders J, Smeets R, Stipec M-R, Vrasti R, Hasin D, Room R, Van den Brink W, Regier D, … Sartorius N (1997). WHO Study on the reliability and validity of the alcohol and drug use disorder instruments: Overview of methods and results. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 47(3), 161–169. 10.1016/S0376-8716(97)00087-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Leijenhorst L, Zanolie K, Van Meel CS, Westenberg PM, Rombouts SARB, & Crone EA (2010). What Motivates the Adolescent? Brain Regions Mediating Reward Sensitivity across Adolescence. Cerebral Cortex, 20(1), 61–69. 10.1093/cercor/bhp078 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA, & Robble MA (2020). Dopamine and Addiction. Annual Review of Psychology, 71(1), 79–106. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-103337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeiger JS, Haberstick BC, Corley RP, Ehringer MA, Crowley TJ, Hewitt JK, Hopfer CJ, Stallings MC, Young SE, & Rhee SH (2010). Subjective effects to marijuana associated with marijuana use in community and clinical subjects. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 109(1), 161–166. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.12.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeiger JS, Haberstick BC, Corley RP, Ehringer MA, Crowley TJ, Hewitt JK, Hopfer CJ, Stallings MC, Young SE, & Rhee SH (2012). Subjective effects for alcohol, tobacco, and marijuana association with cross-drug outcomes. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 123, S52–S58. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.02.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.