Abstract
Although most individuals experiencing cognitive impairment (CI) reside with a caregiver, an estimated 800,000 live alone. Such individuals may have an increased risk for injury to self or others through self-neglect as a result of the CI symptoms. While persons living alone with CI have been identified as an important area for needed research, few studies have been able to examine this population due to the challenges of identifying and recruiting study participants. By using the National Health & Aging Trends Study data set, the researchers explored the characteristics to describe this population. The results of this study indicated that the majority of persons living with CI were older, widowed females who were not diagnosed with Alzheimer’s or dementia but tested positive on cognitive screening measures. Further, the majority of persons living alone with CI relied on adult children and paid professionals as the primary care providers.
Keywords: Alzheimer’s disease, dementia, living alone, diagnosis, risk
Introduction
Worldwide, there are an estimated 46.8 million individuals living with some form of cognitive impairment (CI). 1 CI is a general term describing a group of disorders in which memory and thought processes become impaired. CI can be caused by many different dementias and disorders, but the majority of CI cases are caused by Alzheimer’s disease. 2
According to 2014 data from the Administration on Aging, just under 30% of Americans aged 65 or older (approximately 12.5 million people) live alone. The proportion of people who live alone increases with age and is higher among women than men. More than 56% of American women of at least 85 years old live alone. 3 While most individuals diagnosed with CI live with a caregiver, as many as 1 out of 7 persons with CI live alone. 4 Estimates have been as high as 800,000 individuals.
Over the years, researchers have explored the notion of “risk” for persons living alone with CI. Individuals living alone with CI are at an increased risk for self-neglect, 5 -15 delayed diagnosis, 11,12 falling, 15 use of emergency services, 8,14 undertreated psychiatric issues, 16 -19 and wandering. 8 Many individuals who live alone with CI lack support from community services that might help them with their activities of daily living (ADLs). 6,7
We lack data, and especially national data, about the characteristics of persons living alone with CI. In this study, our intent is to identify the characteristics representing this subgroup and add to extent knowledge about the characteristics of persons with Alzheimer’s disease. A broader and more inclusive understanding of individuals with CI will increase service providers’ outreach to those with CI who need the most support. In addition, by enhancing our understanding of individuals with CI who live alone, we can identify those who are most vulnerable to social isolation and take into account their social networks and care providers and create programs to support these families. The following research questions guided this investigation: (1) What are the characteristics of persons living alone with CI? and (2) Who do persons living alone with CI identify as their primary care providers?
Sample
We used participants from the first wave of the National Health & Aging Trends Study (NHATS), which is a cross-sectional survey design supported by the National Institute on Aging. Trained professionals interviewed participants at their homes. In 2012, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health released the NHATS data set comprised of 8,245 participants, all of whom were drawn from Medicare enrollees in the United States and were aged 65 or older. A subsample of these data was used for this analysis based on the following criteria. First, we selected those who reported living alone based on the response to the question how many participants lived in their household. Those who responded telling 1 person resided in their household were selected. Second, we used the dementia classification constructed for this study, which we describe in more detail below, to identify participants who were having “possible” or “probable” dementia. This yielded a sample of 710 individuals who lived alone and had either possible or probable dementia. Forty-six percent (N = 327) of those who lived alone were classified as having probable dementia, while 54% (N = 383) were identified as having possible dementia and lived alone. In 16% of the cases, when the sample person agreed, proxy respondents were used to respond to items although the questions were modified to fit this context. For example, the question was rephrased as, “Now, I’d like to ask some questions about whether you have noticed any changes in the sample person in the last several years/since you have known [him/her] due to thinking of memory problems.” Another example was, “I’ll read some statements about him/her caused by [him/her] thinking or memory problems, please say Yes. Otherwise say No.”
Instrumentation
Although the data from NHATS include information about participants’ disabilities, lifestyle, values, and other biopsychosocial factors, for this investigation we used the demographic and contextual variables and CI measures, which we discuss in more detail below.
Demographic and Contextual Variables
The demographic variables used for this study included participants’ age, gender (1 = “male” and 2 = “female”), marital status (1 = “married,” 2 = “living with a partner,” 3 = “separated,” 4 = “divorced,” 5 = “widowed,” and 6 = “never married”), race/ethnic background (asked as nonexclusive categories so respondents could select multiple races and/or ethnicities; 1 = “yes” and 2 = “no”), and self-reported health status (1 = “excellent,” 2 = “very good,” 3 = “good,” 4 = “fair,” and 5 = “poor”). In addition, questions regarding self-care, which often are referred to as ADLs, focused on whether or not a participant needed assistance with eating, getting cleaned up, using the toilet, and dressing (1 = “yes,” 2 = “no,” and 3 = “can’t do/doesn’t do”). Instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs; 1 = “yes” and 2 = “no”) assessed if participants needed help with laundry, shopping for groceries or other personal items, making hot meals, handling bills and banking, taking prescribed medications, and managing less common money matters. We computed the total scores, respectively, for ADLs and IADLs by adding participants’ responses, indicating that they needed help carrying out a particular activity.
Cognitive Impairment Measures
The CI assessment was designed to provide information about a participant’s memory (self-rated, whether memory interferes with daily activities, and immediate and delayed 10-word recall), orientation (date, month, year, day of week, and naming president and vice president), and executive function (clock drawing test). Scoring guidelines for the clock drawing test were based on those recommended by Schretlen, Testa and Pealson. 20 Clocks were scored on a scale from 0, not recognizable as a clock, to 5, an accurate depiction of a clock. Extensive training was given for clock drawing test coders by neuropsychology specialists. The weighted kappa (κ) to assess interreliability between the 2 neuropsychology coders was 0.81. Two lay coders were also extensively trained, and their weighted κs ranged from 0.78 to 0.80.
In a technical report entitled, Classification of Persons by Dementia Status in the National Health and Aging Trends Study and published by the investigators of NHATS, 2 classifications—possible dementia and probable dementia—were constructed. 21 Previous researchers have identified a cutoff point of <1.5 standard deviations (SDs) below the mean on the cognitive measures to classify an individual as having CI. Following the criteria for diagnosing a dementia of Alzheimer’s disease, in order for a participant to meet the category of probable dementia, they would require impairment in at least 2 of the cognitive measures. The criteria used to classify possible dementia or mild cognitive impairment (MCI) were based on a cut point of <1.5 SDs below the mean in 1 (of any) domain (as opposed to multiple). 22,23
Methods
This is a cross-sectional research design because we used the first wave of data from NHATS. We present descriptive frequencies and percentages for the demographic analyses as well as results from χ 2 analyses used to test significant associations between variables. We used various strategies of imputation, specifically pairwise deletion, maximum likelihood, and full information maximum likelihood, to address missing data. However, most variables had less than 1% missing data. SPSS 20 software was used to analyze our data.
Results
The results from our first research question regarding the characteristics of participants who lived alone and had CI are shown in Table 1. Based on the criteria for CI used in NHATS and the question asking about the number of people in the household, we found that 710 participants (8.6%) lived alone and had CI. As shown in Table 1, these participants were mostly female and widowed, aged 80 or older, and from white backgrounds. About one-quarter of this subgroup reported African American backgrounds, and fewer self-identified as Hispanic. Less than 5% reported backgrounds that were American Indian, Asian American, and other while no one mentioned (0.0%) Alaskan Native, Native Hawaiian, and Pacific Island. Almost three-quarters (71.2%) of this subgroup was over the age of 80. Chi-square analysis revealed a significant difference between age and the likelihood of being classified as having possible or probable dementia.
Table 1.
Demographic Characteristics.a
Characteristics | Percentage of Total Sample | Percentage of Probable Sample | Percentage of Possible Sample |
---|---|---|---|
Gender of sample person | |||
Female | 69.7 | 72.5 | 67.4 |
Male | 30.3 | 27.5 | 32.6 |
Race of sample person | |||
White | 69.9 | 65.4 | 66.5 |
African American | 27.5 | 28.7 | 26.4 |
American Indian | 1.7 | 3.0 | 2.3 |
Alaskan native | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Asian American | 1.9 | 1.2 | 2.6 |
Native Hawaiian | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Pacific Islander | 0.0 | 0.0 | 0.0 |
Other race | 2.8 | 2.8 | 2.9 |
Ethnicity of sample person | |||
Hispanic/Latino | 6.8 | 6.1 | 7.3 |
Age of the sample person | |||
65-69 | 4.4 | 2.8 | 5.7 |
70-74 | 9.9 | 6.4 | 12.8 |
75-79 | 14.5 | 14.1 | 14.9 |
80-84 | 22.1 | 21.4 | 22.7 |
85-89 | 24.9 | 24.5 | 25.3 |
90 or older | 24.2 | 30.9 | 18.5 |
Marital status | |||
Married (living separate) | 3.5 | 4.6 | 2.6 |
Separated | 4.1 | 1.8 | 6.0 |
Divorced | 13.5 | 11.3 | 15.4 |
Widowed | 69.9 | 70.9 | 68.9 |
Never married | 8.5 | 10.4 | 6.8 |
aN = 710.
Table 2 illustrates the various levels of education among the sample. Chi-square analyses between education and the dementia classification were statistically significant. In consideration of the highest level of education, 41.0% (probable dementia = 43.1%, possible dementia = 39.2%) of the sample did “not graduate from high school.” Again, χ 2 analysis revealed a significant relationship between “severity of impairment” and “highest level of education,” suggesting that those with higher education resulted in less CI, χ 2 (10, N = 710) = 30.603, P = .001.
Table 2.
Sample Person’s Highest Level of Education.a
Characteristics | Percentage of Total Sample | Percentage of Probable Sample | Percentage of Possible Sample |
---|---|---|---|
Highest level of education | |||
No school | 2.1 | 3.7 | <1.0 |
1st-8th grade | 21.7 | 22.0 | 21.4 |
9th-12th grade | 17.2 | 17.4 | 16.9 |
High school graduate | 28.6 | 27.8 | 29.2 |
Vocational training | 5.8 | 7.0 | 4.7 |
Some college | 8.5 | 5.8 | 10.7 |
Associate’s degree | 1.5 | 2.1 | 1.0 |
Bachelor’s degree | 8.3 | 7.0 | 9.3 |
Master’s or Professional degree | 3.1 | 1.8 | 4.2 |
aN = 710.
In regard to respondents’ health, the majority of the sample felt they were in good health. Less than 1% of the sample reported as “don’t know” or “refused” when asked about health conditions. Of the total respondents, 26.2% had been diagnosed with diabetes and 20.8% had been diagnosed with heart disease. Interestingly, only 16.3% had been diagnosed with Alzheimer’s or dementia—all of whom met the criteria for probable dementia (36.1% of that sample). The results of the sample’s self-reported health conditions are listed in Table 3.
Table 3.
Summary of Sample Person’s Health Conditions.a
Characteristics | Percentage of Total Sample | Percentage of Probable Sample | Percentage of Possible Sample |
---|---|---|---|
Self-reported health | |||
Excellent | 7.6 | 6.4 | 8.6 |
Very good | 21.1 | 19.3 | 22.7 |
Good | 32.3 | 31.5 | 32.9 |
Fair | 26.3 | 25.4 | 27.2 |
Poor | 12.4 | 17.1 | 8.4 |
Presence of health conditions | |||
Heart attack | 19.9 | 21.4 | 18.5 |
Heart disease | 20.8 | 22.6 | 19.3 |
Blood pressure | 64.8 | 62.1 | 67.1 |
Arthritis | 59.6 | 61.5 | 58.0 |
Osteoporosis | 20.4 | 24.5 | 17.0 |
Diabetes | 26.2 | 23.5 | 28.5 |
Lung disease | 14.9 | 15.3 | 14.6 |
Had stroke | 16.6 | 20.8 | 13.1 |
Dementia or Alzheimer’s disease | 16.3 | 16.3 | 0.0 |
Cancer | 24.8 | 23.5 | 25.8 |
aN = 710.
The interview also included questions regarding ADLs and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs). With regard to the items of ADLs items, 5.1% of the sample were not able to eat independently, 11.5% were not able to get out of bed without help, 15.6% were not able to get out of a chair without help, 17.7% were not able to walk independently, 30.3% were not able to go outside without assistance, 12.5% were not able to get dressed without assistance, 19.9% were not able to bathe independently, and 11% were not able to use the toilet independently. In summary, the individuals who were suspected of possible dementia were much less impaired when it came to ADLs compared to the probable dementia group. Among the probable dementia group, 30% had difficulty dressing without assistance, and just under 40% could not go outside without assistance. Table 4 presents this information.
Table 4.
Summary of Sample Person’s Activities of Daily Living (ADLs).a
Characteristics | Percentage of Total Sample | Percentage of Probable Sample | Percentage of Possible Sample |
---|---|---|---|
Sample person cannot ___ without help of another person or special equipment | |||
Eat | 5.1 | 7.6 | 2.9 |
Get out of bed | 11.5 | 18.3 | 5.7 |
Get out of chair | 15.6 | 23.5 | 8.9 |
Walk around inside | 17.7 | 25.4 | 11.2 |
Go outside | 30.3 | 38.8 | 23.0 |
Dress | 12.5 | 19.3 | 6.8 |
Bathe | 19.9 | 30.0 | 11.2 |
Use toilet | 11.4 | 19.0 | 5.0 |
aN = 710.
With respect to the items of IADLs, 26.8% were not able to prepare meals independently, 31.1% were not able to do laundry without assistance, 23.1% were not able to do light housework without assistance, 45.1% were not able to shop for groceries independently, 35.2% were unable to manage money independently, 27.0% were not able to take medication without assistance, and 14.4% were unable to make telephone calls without assistance. Again, the individuals who were suspected of possible dementia were much less impaired when it came to instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) compared to the probable dementia group. These data are summarized in Table 5.
Table 5.
Summary of Sample Person’s Activities of Daily Living (IADLs).a
Characteristics | Percentage of Total Sample | Percentage of Probable Sample | Percentage of Possible Sample |
---|---|---|---|
Sample person cannot ___ without help of another person or special equipment | |||
Prepare meals | 26.8 | 41.3 | 14.4 |
Do laundry | 31.1 | 46.5 | 18.0 |
Do light housework | 23.1 | 35.8 | 12.3 |
Shop for groceries | 45.1 | 34.3 | 30.5 |
Manage money | 35.2 | 54.4 | 18.8 |
Take medicine | 27.0 | 44.6 | 12.0 |
Make telephone calls | 14.4 | 25.4 | 5.0 |
aN = 710.
Respondents were also asked to rate their memory (16.3% respondents with proxies were not asked this question). Of the self-respondents, 7.7% reported their “memory as excellent,” 18.7% reported their “memory as very good,” 28.7% reported “memory as good,” 20.7% reported “memory as fair,” and 7.7% reported “memory as poor.” When these same respondents were asked to compare their memory to 1 year ago, the majority (60.0%) of respondents felt their memory was about the same from the previous year. Those who did not rate their self-rated memory as excellent or very good were also asked how much of their memory problem interfered with their daily functioning. Of the 66.8% of the sample who were asked this question, 4.8% stated they experienced memory problems “daily,” 4.1% “most days,” 15.8% “some days,” 16.6% “rarely,” and 15.5% “never.”
The majority (75.5%) of persons living alone with CI had visited their friends and family in the last month, and most felt that these informal interactions were important to them. However, they were less involved in clubs and other organized activities and fewer felt this was important. Finally, when asked about going out for entertainment or enjoyment, most participants (54.2%) had gone out in the last month. They differed in how important they felt going out was in their lives. For example, about one-third of the participants felt it was “very important” to go out for enjoyment, whereas almost another third felt it was “somewhat important.” Overall those classified with a probable dementia were less socially engaged than those suspected of a possible dementia. See Table 6 for a more detailed presentation of these results.
Table 6.
Summary of Sample Person’s Social Participation.a
Characteristics | Percentage of Total Sample | Percentage of Probable Sample | Percentage of Possible Sample |
---|---|---|---|
Sample person has visited friends or family in last month | |||
Yes | 75.5 | 73.7 | 77.0 |
Sample person’s health or functioning impacted his or her ability to visit family or friends in the last month | |||
Yes | 18.2 | 24.2 | 13.1 |
Importance of visiting friends/family for the sample person | |||
Very important | 53.0 | 51.7 | 54.0 |
Somewhat important | 25.1 | 23.2 | 26.6 |
Not so important | 20.6 | 22.9 | 18.5 |
Sample person has participated in clubs or activities in last month | |||
Yes | 24.1 | 19.3 | 71.5 |
Sample person’s health or functioning impacted his or her ability to participate in clubs or activities in the last month | |||
Yes | 19.9 | 25.4 | 84.6 |
Importance of participating in clubs or activities for the sample person | |||
Very important | 18.0 | 14.4 | 21.1 |
Somewhat important | 22.5 | 21.7 | 23.2 |
Not so important | 58.7 | 62.7 | 55.4 |
Sample person went out for entertainment in the last month | |||
Yes | 54.2 | 50.2 | 57.7 |
Sample person’s health or functioning impacted his or her ability to go out for entertainment in the last month | |||
Yes | 21.8 | 29.7 | 15.1 |
Importance of going out for entertainment to the sample person | |||
Very important | 31.3 | 28.4 | 33.8 |
Somewhat important | 32.7 | 30.6 | 34.5 |
Not so important | 35.2 | 40.1 | 31.1 |
aN = 710.
Our second research question asked the following question: Who do persons living alone with CI identify as their primary care provider? These results are shown in Table 7. The majority (66.4%) of care providers were adult children: 39.7% were respondents’ daughters, 26.7% were respondents’ sons, and 1.7% were daughters-in-law. Another common care provider for this population was paid assistance (6.9%) and other paid staff (5.2%). Additionally, siblings and their children were a common proxy among the sample. Extended family were also mentioned. For example, 4.3% of care providers were nieces and 2.6% were nephews. Other types of care providers (<4%) who were noted included grandchildren, cousins, friends, and spouses who did not live with them.
Table 7.
Care Provider.a
Characteristics | Percentage |
---|---|
Relationship to the sample person | |
Daughter | 39.7 |
Son | 26.7 |
Paid assistant | 6.9 |
Staff | 5.2 |
Sister | 4.3 |
Niece | 4.3 |
Spouse | 2.6 |
Nephew | 2.6 |
Daughter-in-law | 1.7 |
Brother | <1 |
Grandchild | <1 |
Cousin | <1 |
Friend | <1 |
aN = 116.
Discussion
As stated in the results, 69.7% were female and 71.2% of the subsample was over the age of 80. As expected, among the subsample the older an individual became, the more likely they were to be classified as having CI. Many studies have found that age is the greatest risk factor for the onset of Alzheimer’s disease. 24 Our findings also corroborated previous studies that have shown that more women than men have CI, which in part is due to women’s higher life expectancies compared to men’s. 25 Given that widows outnumber widowers, we expected higher rates of CI among bereaved women than among bereaved men. Future cohorts are likely to have higher numbers of people living alone with CI who were divorced or never married. 26
Education
In contrast to younger cohorts, these older adults included a substantial percentage of persons who had not completed high school. The significant association we found between education and CI is consistent with previous studies, and at least 2 explanations could account for this. First, educational biases (ie, Mini-Mental State Examination) on neuropsychological tests are possible. 24,27 -30 For example, the clock drawing test has been criticized for inaccuracy particularly with education bias. 31 The validity of these tests are especially questionable Among those who are illiterate, which mostly include those with a grade school education. The tests do not capture what they are intended to capture–CI. Rather they capture these individual’s lower level of education. 32,33 Previous studies have, indeed, found that reading disorders can lead to misdiagnosis of MCI. 34 Those with lower levels of school years also may not have the problem-solving skills, executive functioning ability, or appropriate knowledge prior to taking the exam. These factors inevitably will generate “false positives” on such cognitive assessments.
A second and more recent explanation for the significant association between education and CI lies with the cognitive reserve framework. 35 According to Stern, individuals with substantial cognitive reserve less often succumb to CI, or if they do, they succumb later than individuals with limited cognitive reserve. 35 By cognitive reserve, Stern and others 35,36 are referring to the consequences of a lifetime of intellectual enrichment as a result of many years of education, stimulating social interactions, and engagement in complex and challenging cognitive activities. The accumulation of these lifelong intellectual pursuits actually modifies the brain in such a way that it acts as a cognitive reserve, that is, a buffer to cognitive pathology, such as what occurs with Alzheimer’s disease. 35,36 After comparing patients matched for clinical severity in Alzheimer’s disease, Stern found an inverse relationship between resting cerebral blood flow and years of education. 35 Education seemed to modify the association between Alzheimer’s disease pathology and levels of cognitive functioning. Almeida and colleagues found that older adults with at least 16 years of education had less evidence of neurodegeneration in their cerebrospinal fluid compared to people with fewer years of education. 36 More recent studies suggest that cognitive reserved can be strengthened after appropriate cognitive training. 37
Dementia Classifications
Another important factor that could lead to inaccurate results lies with the sensitivity and specificity of the assessment tools. Interestingly, 16.6% (all from the probable dementia sample, which was 36.1%) had been diagnosed with Alzheimer’s or dementia. Possible explanations for this may be due to CI assessment tests not being sensitive enough to pick up MCI, which is a common issue discussed in the neurological literature. 38,39 Another possible explanation is that some clinical assessments may be too sensitive—again resulting in false positive. At this time, it remains unclear as to how accurate the possible and probable dementia classifications are at truly reflecting the incident of CI in this national sample. Longitudinal analyses may help to expand our understanding of the effectiveness of neuropsychological tests as cognitive screenings, particularly among the population of those living alone.
Functional Impairment
As previously mentioned, ADLs and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) differed substantially between the 2 groups of probable and possible dementia—the individuals who were suspected of possible dementia were much less impaired when it came to ADLs compared to the probable dementia sample. This also suggests there may be a relationship between functional impairment and CI, but it would be important to consider age and other characteristics such as frailty as confounding factors.
According to the practice literature, general practitioners (GPs) are using the IADL scales to help them with CI disease diagnoses. In one paper, De Lepeleire and colleagues described the effectiveness of using it as a tool in addition to other cognitive assessments to help GPs more confidently diagnose CI in patients. 40 Additionally, the IADL scale has been used to help determine the level of impairment of individuals by taking into account the hierarchy of “difficulty,” with “shopping” being the most difficult item and “telephone use” being the least difficult item. Shopping was the most discriminatory item differentiating between patients with different levels of ability from both a cognitive and functional perspective. 41 While it was originally assumed functional impairment might predict CI, new research suggests that CI precedes and predicts subsequent functional impairment in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease. 40 This could explain the disparate functional capacities we observed between the possible and probable dementia persons. 42
Social Interactions
The majority (75.5%) of respondents “visited with friends and family in the last month.” However, a fifth (20.6%) of the sample felt such interactions were “not important at all.” Consistently a fourth-to-fifth of the sample reported a lack of social engagement. Some individuals (primarily those in the probable dementia sample) reported wanting social interactions but were hindered by their health or functional limitations. Finally some people might have less of a need for social interaction and enjoy more solitary activities, such as painting or playing a musical instrument. However, when a person changes and becomes less interested in previously enjoyed activities, some experts might view such a withdrawal as a symptom or precursor to CI or dementia. 43
Conclusion
In this study, we examined a subsample of the NHATS data to explore the characteristics of older adults with CI who were living alone in their communities. Although the majority of these older adults had not been formally diagnosed with a CI, most of them tested positive, with 1 or 2 deficits on the cognitive assessment tools used in this study. With only a very small percentage officially diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease or dementia (16.3%) within this subsample, researchers and practitioners need to explore more innovative ways to access and identify persons living alone who might be at risk for CI. While it may not always be appropriate when only a few deficits are present to provide a clinical diagnosis (such as Alzheimer’s disease), more GPs and geriatricians need more knowledge and training on when to assess CI in their patients and how to intervene when they identify people with CI. 44
Researchers must investigate how these older persons meet their daily needs, given that more older persons with CI are expected to be living alone in the future. Many questions remain such as how do the older persons manage their shopping? How do they compensate for reductions in social interactions? How often do these adults end up in hospitals? How are communities supporting these older persons? We also need more studies examining the safety of these older persons to live alone. Finally, we lack data on evidence-based interventions specifically for this subgroup. If we create and implement programs designed to support these older persons’ independence, we will more effectively avoid premature institutionalizations, which, in turn, will lower costs. Most older adults prefer living out their later years in their homes, that is, they prefer to “age in place.” However, until we have more information about older persons with CI who live alone, about their characteristics, and the factors that might influence these older adults’ functioning, we will fail to meet these older persons’ needs and preferences. We will also continue to overspend on residential care that might be unnecessary or at least delayed.
Footnotes
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
References
- 1. Alzheimer’s Disease International. The global impact of dementia. http://www.worldalzreport2015.org/downloads/world-alzheimer-report-2015.pdf. Accessed January 1, 2016.
- 2. Alzheimer’s Association. Types of dementia. http://www.alz.org/dementia/types-of-dementia.asp. Accessed July 3, 2016.
- 3. Administration on Aging. A profile of older Americans: living arrangements. http://www.aoa.acl.gov/aging_statistics/profile/2014/6.aspx. Accessed January 1, 2016.
- 4. Alzheimer’s Association. 2012 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures report. http://www.alz.org/downloads/facts_figures_2012.pdf. Accessed January 1, 2016. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 5. Dong X, Simon MA, Mosqueda L, Evans DA. The prevalence of elder self-neglect in a community-dwelling population: hoarding, hygiene, and environmental hazards. J Aging Health. 2012;24(3):507–524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Nourhashemi F, Mouyal-Barkate K, Gillette-Guyonnet S, Cantet C, Vellas B. Living alone with Alzheimer’s disease: cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis in the REAL FR Study. J Nutr Health Aging. 2005;9(2):117–120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Miranda-Castillo C, Woods B, Orrell M. People with dementia living alone: what are their needs and what kind of support are they receiving? Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(4):607–617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Tierney MC, Charles J, Naglie G, Jaglal S, Kiss A, Fisher RH. Risk factors for harm in cognitively impaired seniors who live alone: a prospective study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2004;52(9):1435–1441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Tierney MC, Snow WG, Charles J, Moineddin R, Kiss A. Neuropsychological predictors of self-neglect in cognitively impaired older people who live alone. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;15(2):140–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Tuokko H, MacCourt P, Heath Y. Home alone with dementia. Aging Ment Health. 1999;3(1):21–27. [Google Scholar]
- 11. Bradford A, Kunik ME, Schulz P, Williams SP, Singh H. Missed and delayed diagnosis of dementia in primary care: prevalence and contributing factors. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23(4):306–314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Sibley A, Macknight C, Rockwood K. The effects of the living situation on the severity of dementia at diagnosis. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2002;13(1):40–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Al-Aama T. Falls in the elderly: spectrum and prevention. Can Fam Physician. 2011;67(1):771–776. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Rubensteinm LZ, Josephson KR. Falls and their prevention in elderly people: what does the evidence show? Med Clin North Am. 2006;90(5):807–824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Shaw FE, Kenny RA. Can falls in patients with dementia be prevented? Age Ageing. 1998;27(1):7–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Apostolova LG, Cummings JF. Neuropsychiatric manifestations in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review of the literature. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;25(2):115–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Lehmann SW, Black BS, Shore A, Kasper J, Rabins PV. Living alone with dementia: lack of awareness adds to functional and cognitive vulnerabilities. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(5):778–784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Okura T, Plassman BL, Steffens DC, Llewellyn DJ, Potter GG, Langa KM. Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms and their association with functional limitations in older adults in the United States: the aging, demographics, and memory study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58(2):330–337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Lyketsos CG, Steinberg M, Tschanz JT, Norton MC, Steffens DC, Breitner JCS. Mental and behavioral disturbances in dementia: findings from the Cache County study on memory and aging. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(5):708–714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Schretlen DJ, Testa SM, Pealson GD. Clock-drawing test scoring approach from the calibrated neuropsychological normative system. Psychological Assessment Resources. 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Kasper JD, Freedman VA, Spillman B. Classification of Persons by Dementia Status in the National Health and Aging Trends Study. Technical paper #5. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University School of Public Health. http://www.NHATS.org. Accessed January 1, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 22. Morris JC. Revised criteria for mild cognitive impairment may compromise the daignosis of alzheimer disease dementia. Arch Neurol. 2012;69(6):700–708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Geda YE, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, et al. Prevalence of neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment and normal cognitive aging: population-based study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;63(10):1193–1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Alzheimer’s Association. 2014 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures report. http://www.alz.org/downloads/facts_figures_2014.pdf. Accessed January 1, 2016. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 25. Hooyman N, Kiyak HA. Social Gerontology: A Multidisciplinary Perspective. 9th ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 26. US Census. Population: marital status and living arrangements. https://www.census.gov/hhes/families/data/cps2014A.html. Accessed January 20, 2016.
- 27. Stern Y, Gurland B, Tatemichi TK, Tang MX, Wilder D, Mayeux R. Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA. 1994;271(13):1004–1010. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Ott A, Breteler MM, van Harskamp F, et al. Prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: association with education. The Rotterdam study. Br Med J. 1995;310(6985):970–973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Letenneur L, Gilleron V, Commenges D, Helmer C, Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF. Are sex and educational level independent predictors of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease? Incidence data from the PAQUID project. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;66(2):177–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Dozzi Brucki SM, Nitrini R. Mini-Mental State Examination among lower educational levels and illiterates: transcultural evaluation. Dement Neuropsychol. 2010;4(2):120–125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Lebowitz BK, Weinstein C, Beiser A, et al. Lifelong reading disorder and mild cognitive impairment: implications for diagnosis. J Alzheimer Dis. 2015;50(1):41–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Jones RN, Gallo JJ. Education bias in the mini-mental state examination. Int Psychogeriatr. 2001;13(3):299–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Powlishta KK, Von Dras DD, Stanford A, et al. The clock drawing test is a poor screen for very mild dementia. J Neurol. 2002;59(6):898–903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. De Lepeleire J, Aertgeerts B, Umbach I, et al. The diagnostic value of IADL evaluation in the detection of dementia in general practice. Aging Ment Health. 2004;8(1):52–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(11):1006–1012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Almeida RP, Schultz SA, Austin BP, et al. Cognitive reserve modifies age-related alterations in CSF biomarkers of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(6):699–706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Mondini S, Madella I, Zangrossi A, et al. Cognitive reserve in dementia: implications for cognitive training. Front Aging Neurosci. 2016;8:84. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4844602/. Accessed July 25, 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Ehreke L, Luppa M, Luck T, et al. Is the clock drawing test appropriate for screening mild cognitive impairment? Results of the Germany study on ageing, cognition and dementia in primary care patients (AgeCoDe). Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;28(4):365–372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Ainslie NK, Murden RA. Clock drawing test for dementia is skewed by education level. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1993;48(8):249–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. McGrory S, Shenkin SD, Austin EJ, Starr JM. Lawton IADL scale in dementia: can item response theory make it more informative? Age Ageing. 2014;43(4):491–495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Liu-Seifert H, Siemers E, Price K, et al. Cognition impairment precedes and predicts functional impairment in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Proceedings from AAIC: Alzheimer’s Association International Conference. Copenhagen, Denmark; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Alzheimer’s Association. Know the 10 signs. http://alz.org/alzheimers_disease_know_the_10_signs.asp. Accessed January 20, 2016.
- 43. Ismail Z, Carrillo MC, Dacks P. Mild behavioral impairment (MBI) checklist. Alzheimer’s Association International Conference (AAIC) 2016. Abstract O1-13-03. Presented July 24, 2016. Toronto, CA. [Google Scholar]
- 44. Brayne C, Fox C, Boustani M. Dementia screening in primary care: is it time? JAMA 2007;298(20):2409–2411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]