Skip to main content
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience logoLink to Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
. 2024 Feb 8;66:101355. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101355

Multi-level fMRI analysis applied to hemispheric specialization in the language network, functional areas, and their behavioral correlations in the ABCD sample

Trevor KM Day a,b,, Robert Hermosillo b,c, Gregory Conan b, Anita Randolph b,c, Anders Perrone b, Eric Earl d, Nora Byington b, Timothy J Hendrickson b,e, Jed T Elison a,b,c, Damien A Fair a,b,c, Eric Feczko b,c
PMCID: PMC10875197  PMID: 38354531

Abstract

Prior research suggests that the organization of the language network in the brain is left-dominant and becomes more lateralized with age and increasing language skill. The age at which specific components of the language network become adult-like varies depending on the abilities they subserve. So far, a large, developmental study has not included a language task paradigm, so we introduce a method to study resting-state laterality in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. Our approach mixes source timeseries between left and right homotopes of the (1) inferior frontal and (2) middle temporal gyri and (3) a region we term “Wernicke’s area” near the supramarginal gyrus. Our large subset sample size of ABCD (n = 6153) allows improved reliability and validity compared to previous, smaller studies of brain-behavior associations. We show that behavioral metrics from the NIH Youth Toolbox and other resources are differentially related to tasks with a larger linguistic component over ones with less (e.g., executive function-dominant tasks). These baseline characteristics of hemispheric specialization in youth are critical for future work determining the correspondence of lateralization with language onset in earlier stages of development.

Keywords: FMRI, RsfMRI, Hemispheric specialization, Cortical specialization

1. Introduction

Language serves not only its important communicative role, but also supports higher-order reasoning such as theory of mind (e.g. Ebert et al., 2017), and emotion regulation (e.g. Monopoli and Kingston, 2012). In the brain, language is processed by a left hemisphere-dominant network that comprises multiple regions thought to be specialized for distinct linguistic subskills.

This laterality has been found to increase between childhood and young adulthood (Holland et al., 2007, Lidzba et al., 2011, Sepeta et al., 2016, Szaflarski et al., 2006, Szaflarski et al., 2006), and with increasing language skills within the same age range (Bartha-Doering et al., 2018), which may be due to a decrease in right hemisphere (RH) involvement (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2002, Olulade et al., 2020, Petitto et al., 2012). In older children, organization and laterality of the language network is not adultlike by 9 years (Enge et al., 2020, Skeide et al., 2014).

1.1. Challenges in investigating lateralization

Considering the drastic developments in language skills, the first few years of life are an important time period for understanding changes in cortical organization (Hervé et al., 2013, Johnson, 2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), due to its high spatial resolution and non-invasiveness is well-suited to questions of cortical topology. However, using task-based fMRI (tfMRI) with infants and toddlers is difficult, as it relies on both the ability of the participants to effectively perform the task and lie very still. Resting state fMRI (rsfMRI) does not induce task demands, and is uniquely suited to the study of the development of functional brain organization and can even be conducted during natural sleep in infants (Dubois et al., 2021, Howell et al., 2018, Korom et al., 2022).

Resting state (RS) organization is individualized and accounts for a significant portion of task activation patterns suggesting it can be used to study online activation (Tavor et al., 2016). However, changes in the RS signal do occur over development: primary networks are further developed in neonates, compared to higher-order networks (Fransson et al., 2011, Gao et al., 2009, Gao et al., 2017) and become more dissimilar between individuals with age (Moore et al., 2023), which suggests that much of the infrastructure reflected by rsfMRI is in place early in development. Together, this suggests rsfMRI can recapitulate changes in task-response topology.

1.2. Using rsfMRI data to examine the development of the language network

MRI studies of language have been constrained by sample sizes (Bradshaw et al., 2017, Enge et al., 2020) that have been criticized for a lack of power (Button et al., 2013, Poldrack et al., 2017). Developing a new technique could prove problematic when using a small sample size by providing false-positive validation of an incorrect technique, so we leveraged the well-powered Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development study (ABCD; Barch et al., 2018; Casey et al., 2018; Volkow et al., 2018) to develop and validate a novel technique for the study of lateralization using rsfMRI. Central to our assumption is that the cortical regions that support online language processing also vary together in RS and thus can be recapitulated using RS correlations (Biswal et al., 1995, Hermundstad et al., 2013, Mennes et al., 2010).

For these analyses we leveraged the MIDB Probabilistic Atlas that accounts for individual variability in our sample in network topology (Hermosillo et al., 2022). Specifically, we used regions of interest (ROIs) selected from the ventral attention network (VAN). The VAN is a RH-dominant exogenous attention network (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002) that closely mirrors the left hemisphere (LH)-dominant language network topologically (Bernard et al., 2020, Braga et al., 2020).

From this VAN, we used ROIs from inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and middle temporal gyrus (MTG). MTG is thought to subserve morphosyntactic and lexico-semantic categorization, lexical access, and phrase structure building (Skeide and Friederici, 2016), or to be involved non-selectively in a range of lexico-semantic as well as syntactic processing (Fedorenko and Blank, 2020). Our understanding of the role of IFG is in substantially more flux. One major line of work has argued the role of IFG in language is articulation (Broca, 1861, Flinker et al., 2015, Hillis et al., 2004, Long et al., 2016, Papoutsi et al., 2009). However, more recent work suggests IFG comprises subregions individually selective for language processing and another that is modulated by processing difficulty (Fedorenko and Blank, 2020, Weiss-Croft and Baldeweg, 2015).

1.3. Using rsfMRI to study the language network

In comparison to tfMRI literature, the rsfMRI literature is less mature. Researchers have used independent component analysis (ICA) to isolate the independent component most like a template. These studies so far have had small sample sizes (n = 15) and wide variation in overlap between individual-specific and canonical language networks (Branco et al., 2016, Smitha et al., 2019). While promising, such ICA approaches are affected by the number of components selected, cannot give region-level analyses, and bias the estimate toward the template, which was asymmetric in the former paper and symmetric in the latter. Likewise, Braga and colleagues (2020) used an iterative approach to refine a subject-specific ROI based on the qualities of its resulting seedmap and its similarity to the canonical language network. However, to re-apply such ROIs to the same timeseries would introduce data leakage.

This paper proposes and validates a new RS functional connectivity analysis specifically for the purposes of investigating for hemispheric specialization. We generate many seed-based connectivity maps meant to be representative of the language network, varying the relative proportion of LH and RH grayordinates in the seed region. This increases the reliability of our characterization of laterality by estimating the network multiple times. We demonstrate external validity by demonstrating a relationship between this technique and associations with behavioral tasks conducted outside of the scanner. Thus, it is informative with regards to brain-behavior associations and suitable for use across development, for example in infants and toddlers from whom only RS data was collected.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Data were taken from the ABCD baseline (Year 1) acquisition (ages 9–11 years). We used the ABCD Reproducibility Matched Samples (ARMS; Feczko et al., 2021), which are matched across the ABCD study based on site, age, sex, ethnicity, grade, highest level of parental education, handedness, combined family income, exposure to anesthesia, and family structure. Family members were kept in the same ARMS, and the ARMS were matched to have equivalent numbers of siblings, twin pairs, and triplets. Participants were included if they had more than 10 min of low-motion (framewise displacement < 0.2 mm) rsfMRI data, leaving 3098 individuals in ARMS-1 and 3055 in ARMS-2, see Supplementary Figure (SF) 2. The ARMS function as a within-study test/replication sample to increase our confidence in our findings by immediately performing a replication. Relative to the entire sample, the low-motion group is older, more female, more non-Hispanic white, and is more likely to have married parents, parents with graduate degrees, higher household incomes, has fewer mental and physical health problems, higher general cognition, executive functioning, and learning and memory scores (Cosgrove et al., 2022, Feczko et al., 2021), which limits generalizability.

While we modeled handedness as a continuous variable, we present the number of individuals in each handedness category based on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI): left: EHI < −0.5; mixed: − 0.5 ≤ EHI ≤ 0.5, right: EHI > 0.5 (Table 1, Veale, 2014).

Table 1.

Number of individuals in each ARMS by handedness category (right: EHI > 0.5, left: EHI < −0.5, mixed: −0.5 ≤ EHI ≤ 0.5).

Handedness
n (%)
ARMS Right Mixed Left
1 2488 (80) 378 (12) 232 (7)
2 2476 (81) 376 (12) 203 (7)

Additionally, we divided the participants’ linguistic environment (as reported in the ABCD Longitudinal Parent Demographics Survey; Barch et al., 2018) into the following mutually exclusive categories (Table 2), where each child was assigned to the most-bilingual-exposure category in which they fit. In this rating, we consider categories 3 and 4 “bilingual” (14%). There was no difference in distribution between ARMS, χ2(4,6153)=3.988,p=.41.

Table 2.

Number of participants in each category. Dual immersion was the “L2 in school” category. L1: first language; L2: second language.

Language Exposure ARMS 1 N (%) ARMS 2 N (%)
  • 1.

    English at home and in school

2340 (76) 2244 (73)
  • 2.

    English at home, L2 in school

152 (5) 158 (5)
  • 3.

    Non-English home, child L1 is English

205 (7) 211 (7)
  • 4.

    Child L1 is not English

228 (7) 247 (8)
  • 5.

    Unknown

173 (6) 195 (6)

Each brain-behavior model was tested against demographic covariates, including gender, age, handedness, parental education, binned parental income (breaks: $2500, $8500, $14,000, $20,500, $30,000, $42,500, $62,500, $87,500, $150,000, $250,000), child anesthesia status, child race, and ABCD site.

2.2. Preprocessing

Data were processed using the ABCD-BIDS processing pipeline (Sturgeon et al., 2021), which is a modified version of the HCP pipeline that is more general and compatible with the different MR platforms used across the ABCD study. This pipeline was previously described in detail (Feczko et al., 2021), and contains the HCP pipeline steps of PreFreeSurfer (with the modification of allowing a missing T2), FreeSurfer, PostFreeSurfer (conversion to CIFTI), fMRIVolume, fMRISurface (registration), and the additional steps of DCAN BOLD processing, and Executive Summary creation.

2.3. Network level results

2.3.1. Network creation

For a complete description of the generation of the template matching (TM) maps used in this paper, see Hermosillo et al. (2022), which implements methods from Gordon and colleagues (2017). Briefly, for each subject, a network assignment was generated for each grayordinate based on the similarity of spatial pattern of functional connectivity to an independent set of network templates. Then, each cortical and subcortical grayordinate was assigned a likelihood of belonging to each of 14 networks across the group. Those networks were the default mode network (DMN), the visual network (VIS), the frontal parietal network (FPN), the premotor network (PMN), the dorsal attention network (DAN), the ventral attention network (VAN), the salience network (Sal), the cingulo-opercular network (CO), the sensorimotor dorsal network (SMd), the sensorimotor lateral network (SMl), the auditory network (AUD), the temporal network (MTL), parieto-occipital network (PON), and the parietal medial network (PMN).

Ten minutes of data with a framewise displacement (FD) of less than 0.2 mm were sampled from each subject, and the probability of network observation (η2) is calculated for each network. Network templates were initially created in ARMS-3, the small, held-out development set of ABCD participants (Feczko et al., 2021). η2 values are calculated by correlating the template dense time series across all grayordinates within a session. Whole-brain connectivity at each grayordinate was thresholded to Z-scores greater than or equal to one (independently within hemisphere, subcortex, and cortex-to-subcortex). The correlation between this suprathreshold map and each template is theη2. Each grayordinate is assigned to the network with the maximum η2, i.e. a “winner-take-all” approach. Finally, some clean-up, including hole-filling, is performed after binarization to create assignments for each network for each subject.

2.3.2. Characterizing networks

First we calculated a network binary LI (bLI), using the classic LI formula LI=LRL+R, where L and R are the number of in-network vertices in each hemisphere. Secondly, binarization of η2 obscures information about the underlying probability of network observation. In other words there is no distinction between these two scenarios: (1) a clear correct assignment, where the highest η2 is much greater than all others; and (2) a vertex where there is no clear winner, and multiple networks may be detectable, e.g. at the edge of a network, yet one must be selected.

We explored two methods of comparing grayordinate-wise η2values in an attempt to be more sensitive to the underlying distribution of η2 values, a continuous LI (cLI), where the left and right η2 values were subtracted from one another pairwise, and a t-test LI (tLI), where the vectors of left and right η2 were treated as paired estimates.

Initially, we calculated cLI. In order to do this, because the grayordinate CIFTI matrix is not symmetric, we projected the LH values onto the RH, so that values can be paired. Then, we simply subtracted the projected RH η2 values from their corresponding LH value, resulting in 29,706 difference values for each network. The average voxelwise difference across the brain is thus the cLI. As only positive template probabilities were assigned, the theoretically largest possible difference is 1 or − 1, representing perfect left or right dominance, placing it on the same scale as bLI, however cLI ranged mostly between 0.1 and −0.1.

As this model of the cortex in this framework pairs values across the hemispheres, we can also perform a paired t-test to distinguish more intricately between hemispheres (tLI), although due to the large sample size (29,706) the test itself is often trivially significant, and so we report effect sizes (Cohen’s d). Because cLI and this metric tLI are so highly correlated (r=0.92 across all individuals and networks), we just use tLI going forward. It is important to note that, just as a grayordinate does not represent the signal from a meaningful grouping of neurons (other than adjacency), pairing of values across the central sulcus this way matches values only homotopically, not homologously, and so a tLI takes into account both the greater extent in one hemisphere — spatial asymmetry — and underlying differences in the confidence of network assignment.

Secondly, because the ipsilateral connectivity bias theoretically causes extraneously elevated correlation coefficients between language areas and non-language areas, our method controls for this bias by evaluating the laterality of the language network estimate against the laterality of the seed.

2.4. The integrated laterality index

2.4.1. ROI selection

The optimal method to create a session-level ROI would be using an approach similar to that of Braga and colleagues (2020), by identifying what ROI creates a language-network-like seedmap. However, applying such an ROI to the data it was created from would introduce data leakage and circularity issues, and we long enough fMRI scans to split a single session in half across the ABCD sample to both create and apply an ROI. Thus, we decided on a technique that would maximize the overlap of the selected ROI across participants without introducing data leakage. We leveraged previous work on ABCD to identify regions of cortex that are reliably found to host the VAN in ABCD.

The VAN is a consistently identified cortical network (Gordon et al., 2016, Hermosillo et al., 2022, Power et al., 2011, Yeo et al., 2011) that seems to comprise a LH language network and RH attention network (Bernard et al., 2020, Lee et al., 2012). We used the VAN map described above, and began by thresholding it at 0.5 probability (i.e. all grayordinates that were present in at least 50% of participants). We used the large inferior frontal region from this map as our IFG ROI, see Fig. 1. Qualitatively, we noticed that MTG segregated into a large ventral component and a smaller anterior component at a slightly higher threshold of 0.586 in LH. In this analysis, we term the larger component “MTG” and the smaller “Wernicke’s area” (WA) based on its location near the temporoparietal junction, similar to Tremblay and Dick’s (2016) definition, although much smaller in extent (see Fig. 1). We created ROIs for both ARMS, but they were so spatially similar that we used ROIs from ARMS-1 in analyses of both ARMS for consistency. This method only identified the three regions discussed above, and did not identify that other regions important for language were consistent enough within the sample to pass the threshold for inclusion, such as the middle frontal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, temporal pole and ventral temporal lobe.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The surface-based ROIs selected for this analysis. Red: inferior frontal gyrus (IFG); blue: middle temporal gyrus (MTG); yellow: “Wernicke’s area.” Only the LH is shown because left ROIs were projected onto the RH for all analyses.

The left ROIs were uniformly larger than the right (IFG L: 351 grayordinates, R: 177; MTG L: 490, R: 482). Indeed, there was no right-hemisphere Wernicke’s ROI at this threshold (L size: 81). We projected all canonical left ROIs onto the right hemisphere for the next step, creating homotopes, rather than using the functional homolog. This was necessary to facilitate our analysis, as described below, that swaps out left for right vertices as necessary.

Our three ROIs overlap spatially with Fedorenko’s Language Atlas (Lipkin et al., 2022), providing external validity for our ROI selection (see SF 6.1), however, some mismatch in temporoparietal junction is evident, as is the inclusion of non-language IFG, which possibly impacted our results.

For all three conceptual ROIs, we created sets of CIFTI ROIs that incorporated nL grayordinates from the canonical left ROI and nR grayordinates from the mirrored ROI, where nL and nR sum to equal the total size of the left ROI (nmax). For each value of increasing nRbetween 1 and (nmax1), we randomly selected nRgrayordinates to involve from the right ROI, and removed the corresponding grayordinate from the left, see Figure LLL(a). We repeated this process 10 times for each value of nR, so that later steps can randomly select a pregenerated bilateral seed out of 10, rather than creating a new one during processing. These seeds are available on Zenodo (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7289743). We refer to the proportion of right-hemisphere grayordinates relative to the ROI as seed laterality (SL), however remapped such that nR=0 receives an SL value of 1; and nR=nmax a value of − 1. Therefore, a seed with equal proportions left/right has an SL value of 0. This sign convention follows the laterality literature in representing LH with positive values.

2.4.2. Seed maps

For increasing values of nR, we used as a source ROI for the DCAN Lab Seed Map Wrapper (link: https://gitlab.com/Fair_lab/Cifti_conn_matrix_to_corr_dt_pt), which generates a whole-brain correlation map. For each map, we used exactly 10 min of low motion (FD < 0.2 mm) data randomly sampled from available data, removing motion outliers, and performed r-to-Z transformation on the result (Figure LLL(b-c)). For images of the r-value maps, see SF 3b.

We calculated an LI for each seed map, using wb_command -cifti-find-clusters with a Z threshold of 0.4 and a surface area threshold of 10 mm2 to remove only the most spurious areas of correlation and wb_command -cifti-stats to count the number of surviving grayordinates in each hemisphere. Then we calculated the LI as LI=LRL+R (Figure LLL(d)). Although there is technically a different number of grayordinates in each hemisphere in the CIFTI matrix, the difference is small enough to make no difference in the overall LI calculation.

Seed maps with more-rightward SL were on average slightly smaller (IFG β1 = −3.55, β2 = −3.52 grayordinates per percentage point increase in SL, values for both ARMS given in subscript; Wernicke’s β1 = −0.991, β2 = −0.873; MTG β1 = −1.97, β2 = −2.05, all p values <.001).

2.4.3. Integration

Initially, we calculated laterality indices using all 10×nmax possible seed maps (e.g. for IFG, 3510). However, visual inspection of the results demonstrated that the clustering of the points along a polynomial line of best fit (R2 >.99; SF 3a) showed that using multiple replicates at each SL value was unnecessary. In order to reduce computation time, we used 100 seed maps per session to calculate ILI, using random selection of one out of the 10 pre-hoc ROIs for each SL, which performed well relative to the best-estimate using seedmaps at each nR value, see Supplementary Material 3. For WA, which was smaller than 100 grayordinates, all values of nR were included.

Across all participants, the LI trend was clearly not linear. We performed a simple ANOVA model selection approach to select the best functional form to use across sessions. We randomly selected 100 sessions (50 from each ARM) and fit polynomial models between linear and quintic, inclusive. The most frequent best functional form was a cubic (n=29), with the other models being selected as the best-fitting form with the following frequencies: linear: 4, quadratic: 26, quartic: 25, quintic: 16. The median best-fitting model was the cubic. Furthermore, there is no theoretical reason to suggest that the laterality of these mixed estimates of language function laterality would uniformly approach one extreme more rapidly than the other, thus a symmetric function like a cubic is consistent with how we might expect the estimate to behave relative to biology. That said, all models performed very well (mean R2 linear: 0.963; quadratic: 0.989; cubic: 0.992; quartic: 0.993; quintic: 0.993). Visualizations of some example trends can be found in Supplementary Material 3.

From this curve, there are a number of estimates that become immediately apparent: the y-intercept,1 the value at SL = -1 and 1, as well as the x-intercept. The x-intercept in particular is not always interpretable, as it does not always occur between −1 < SL < 1, and cubics can have multiple x-intercepts. The extreme SL values, of course, reflect the laterality of language activity alone as it relates to a single ROI, and thus does not avoid theipsilateral effects we set out to control for.

Thus, we chose to calculate the integral of the cubic fit, creating an integrated laterality index (ILI). The correlation between these values and the integration of the cubic fit are presented in Table 3. Notably, the two most interpretable parameters (y-intercept and ILI) are at unity, however, as we will show, ILI has better predictive value for behavior. In the below analysis, the smallest root of the polynomial was taken as the x-intercept.

Table 3.

The correlation between values extracted from cubic fits. ARMS 1 on the left, ARMS 2 on the right. All r values are trivially significant at large n.

Value
Value 1 2 3 4
  • 1.

    y-intercept

  • 2.

    x-intercept

.73 /.70
  • 3.

    SL = 1 (L)

.22 /.25 .43 /.41
  • 4.

    SL = −1 (R)

.37 /.36 .13 /.12 -.36 / − .36
  • 5.

    ILI

.97 /.97 .76 /.72 .41 /.44 .40 /.38

Fig. 2 shows the processes of calculating an ILI for a non-lateralized participant. First, (a) shows a mixed source ROI; (b) shows the resulting seedmaps at different mixing percentages (75% left, 50%, and 25%); (c) shows the resulting laterality indices based on 100 samples of source ROIs; and finally (d) shows the cubic fit as it changes based on the source ROI, showing this individual had high degrees of ipsilateral connectivity, but that the underlying network trends toward bilaterality.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Graphic representation of ILI creation. (a) Bilateral ROIs including grayordinates from both hemispheres are created, by removing n grayordinates from the left ROI and adding n matching homotopic ROIs from the RH. (b) Seedmaps are created from each bilateral ROI. Example seedmaps from a non-lateralized participant are shown for ROIs containing 75%, 50%, and 25% LH voxels. (c) Mixing proportions are sampled between 100% and 0% LH to create a regression line (blue). The same proportions and individual as (b) are shown. (d) The area under the curve (cubic fit) is calculated; resulting in the “ILI.” The same individual is shown in each subfigure and the areas under the curve are highlighted.

After Benjamini-Hochberg correction, there was a statistically significant relation between mean seed map size and ILI for IFG in both ARMS (β1 = 0.0000249, β2 = 0.0000250, p1 <.001, p2 <.001), but not for MTG (p1 =.527, p2 = 0.410) or WA (p1 =.197, p2 = 0.414).

Across both ARMS, ILIIFG and ILIMTG were significantly (all p < .001, one-sided t-test) left-lateralized, but ILIWer was significantly right-lateralized (both ARMS p < .001), see Fig. 3(a). Example MTG seedmaps for a left-lateralized participant (Z2) and a right-lateralized participant (Z −2) are shown in Fig. 3(b), see Supplementary Materials 3 for more details.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

(a) Distribution of ILI for all three ROIs in both ARMs (1: red, 2: blue). Wer: Wernicke’s area. (b) Example seedmaps for two participants at source mixing percentages of (top to bottom) 100% left, 50/50%, and 100% right, indicated with schematized ROI representations. Participants are (i) strongly left-lateralized and (ii) strongly right-lateralized).

2.4.4. Behavioral and environmental characteristics

We examined the specificity of the relationship between ILI and not only language-related behavior measures, but with all behavioral scores previously analyzed in ABCD by Thompson and colleagues (2019). These include all seven subscores from the NIH Toolbox (Bauer and Zelazo, 2014, Weintraub et al., 2013), as well as the Little Man Task (LMT; Ratcliff, 1979) and the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; Lezak, 1983).

Under the differentiation hypothesis, the underlying structure of cognition changes from childhood into adolescence (Mungas et al., 2013, Shing et al., 2010, Thompson et al., 2019), and to that end, we investigated two sets of composite scores measuring latent components of cognition. The first set are the sum-score composites from the Toolbox: Total Composite (TC; all instruments), Crystallized Composite (CC), and Fluid Composite (FC), see Table 4. We additionally included the three principal component (PC) scores described by Thompson and colleagues: General Ability, Executive Function, and Learning & Memory. These scores will be referred to as PC1-GA, PC2-EF, and PC3-LM. Although PC scores are calculated with all instruments, each instrument is listed with the PC(s) where its weight was greater than 0.4.

Table 4.

List of all behavioral measures included in the analysis, including the short form of their name, their Toolbox composite score (sum score) and their weights onto Thompson PCs that exceed 0.4. All NIH-TB instruments are included in the Total Composite.

Instrument Short Name NIH Composite Thompson PC (weights > 0.4)
NIH-TB
Picture Vocabulary Pic Vocab Crystallized PC1-GA (.754)
Oral Reading Recognition Reading C PC1-GA (.820)
Dimensional Card Sort Card Sort Fluid PC2-EF (.710)
Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Flanker F PC2-EF (.712)
Picture Sequence Memory Picture F PC3-LM (.863)
List Sorting Working Memory Test List F PC1-GA (.471)
Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Pattern F PC2-EF (.813)
PC3-LM (.493)
Others
Little Man Task LMT - PC1-GA (.500)
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Immediate Recall) RAVLT - PC3-LM (.712)

The majority of participants had behavioral assessment and MRI acquisition on the same day (77%), with 22% of participants doing the MRI acquisition within the month following behavioral assessment, and 1% doing the MRI within the month before behavioral assessment. Less than 1% of the total sample had more than a month between assessment and acquisition.

2.5. Brain-behavior correlations

In the models below, we included child age in months, child grade, highest parent education in years, parent income (as factor), child gender, child anesthesia exposure (yes/no), child race,2 and site as covariates. We independently tested for an effect of handedness as it is of substantive interest in laterality effects. Including handedness, these are the variables originally used to balance the ARMS, and tests were conducted independently in each ARMS to achieve test-retest reliability.

We also examined the effect of linguistic exposure on laterality. To simplify, we examined the difference between monolinguals (Group 1) and bilinguals (Groups 3 and 4), leaving out the relatively small dual-immersion-only group (Group 2). There was no difference in laterality between language exposure groups, except in bLI in ARMS2 (p=0.038 following Benjamini-Hochberg [BH] correction), so we do not include linguistic exposure in models going forward.

Rather than testing our brain measures against only relevant behavioral measures, we instead show the effect of laterality on the 15 behaviors listed above (NIH Youth Toolbox, Little Man Task, RAVLT, and Thompson PC scores), providing external validity and specificity. We separate all analyses by ARMS to provide immediate, within-study replication.

3. Results

3.1. Network laterality scores are associated with each other and handedness, but not linguistic exposure

The original networks were calculated from a set of 6066 participants across both ARMS. The bLI ranged from [− 0.65, 0.864] and tLI from [− 0.62, 0.61], but the first is bound [−1, 1] and the latter unbound (an effect size). The correlation between the two metrics is r=0.79, but correlation varied by network, between [.52,.86]. Notably low were the correlations between LIs in the MTL, perhaps reflecting low confidence in the probabilistic assignments in that network, likely due to the susceptibility weighted artifact common in the mesial temporal lobe (e.g. Olman et al., 2009). Accordingly, the TP network was the second-lowest. In our network of interest, the VAN, correlations were approximately r=0.78 (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Correlations between bLI and tLI across networks, by ARM. Average correlation by ARM shown with solid (ARMS-1) and dashed (ARMS-2) lines. Points are colored by network. Correlations are notably low in the MTL, perhaps reflecting low confidence in the probabilistic assignments in that network, likely due to the susceptibility weighted artifact common in the mesial temporal lobe.

There was an association between handedness score and bLI (β=0.021,p<.001) after controlling for the covariates listed above, with the effect that the average entirely right-handed participant had a bLI 0.042 units more lateralized than the average entirely left-handed participant, which approximates a Cohen’s d of 0.4. There was likewise an effect on tLI (β=0.015,p<.001), with the effect that the average entirely right-handed participant had a tLI 0.030 units more lateralized than the average entirely left-handed participant, which approximates a Cohen’s d of 0.3. In both scores, the average left-handed LI was greater than zero, i.e. still left-lateralized.

3.2. tLI, but not bLI is associated with behavior scores

Each estimate is the single beta weight for the laterality metric, controlling for the variables mentioned above (including handedness score). We differentiate point estimates of beta weights by p-values in Fig. 5, those greater than 0.05 (gray) and those that survive correction (red).

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Beta weights for behavior-brain associations for both LIs. Horizontal ranges represent 95% CI. Associations significant after BH correction in red, non-significant in gray. ARMS-1 in circles, ARMS-2 in triangles. On the y-axis, composites prefixed with an asterisk.

In Fig. 5, the behaviors are standardized and ordered by average beta weight across ARM and metric (bLI/tLI). The same order is used on both facets. We note a general increase in weight as the linguistic skills needed for the behavior increase; e.g. Toolbox Reading and RAVLT-Immediate scores are among the highest. bLI showed fewer significant associations; with no behavioral task showing replicable associations after BH correction for 60 multiple comparisons.3 Using tLI, PC1-GA, Toolbox Crystallized Intelligence, Toolbox Reading, and RAVLT were significant in both ARMS. Models evaluating the effect of all network laterality on all behaviors can be found in Supplementary Material 4.

Finally, due to the high correlation between ILI and the y-intercept of the cubic fit, we examined the explanatory power of ILI compared to the y-intercept (i.e., the 50–50 ROI), and the left and right ROIs. Uniformly across behaviors, ILI explained a higher proportion of the behavior than the left ROI, which explained more of the behavior than the 50/50 ROI. Rarely did the laterality associated with the right ROI explain a significant proportion of the variance (only in MTG AMRS-1 and in PC1-GA, Toolbox CC, and Toolbox Reading), see SM 5.

Importantly, this also demonstrates a non-reliance of the summary measure on laterality changes associated with the RH, which is a mechanism that has been proposed in children younger than the 9–11 year old participants in this study (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2002, Enge et al., 2020, Olulade et al., 2020, Perani et al., 2011, Petitto et al., 2012).

3.3. ILI is not associated handedness or linguistic exposure

We tested handedness and linguistic exposure for ILI as well. The effects of handedness on ILIIFG were not significant. Handedness had an effect on ILIMTG in ARMS-2 (β=.281,p=.002, but not ARMS-1 (β=.602,p=.158). The effect was significant in both ARMS for ILIWA, but in opposite directions (ARM1: β=0.272,p<.001; ARM2: β=0.172,p<.001), all p-values BH corrected for multiple comparisons. Because the effects of handedness on ILI do not replicate between ARMS, we consider this to indicate correlations with handedness are spurious. Likewise, controlling for covariates, there was no effect of bilingual status on any ILI, so linguistic exposure is not included in future covariates. The partial correlation between ILIs was IFG-WA: 0.18 - 0.22; IFG-MTG: 0.42 - 0.43; WA-MTG: 0.14 - 0.17 (values for both ARMs shown).

3.4. ILI for MTG is associated with behavior, but not IFG or WA

We present a similar behavioral plot for ILI measures. In IFG and MTG, the association between behaviors and ILI increased as the amount of linguistic skill needed for the task increased (Fig. 6). No patterns emerged for WA, so we will discuss IFG and MTG. Below, we present statistics for individual models predicting neurobehavioral outcome against demographic variables and ILI, following BH correction for 90 multiple comparisons.4

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6

Beta weights for behavior-brain associations in all three ROIs. The vertical line shows no relation between brain/behavior. Horizontal ranges represent 95% CI. Associations significant after BH correction in red, non-significant in gray. ARMS-1 in circles, ARMS-2 in triangles. On the y-axis, composite scores are prefixed with an asterisk. Note the different order from the previous plot.

Between IFG and MTG, associations were significant in both ARMS for the RAVLT, Toolbox Pic Vocab (only in MTG), Toolbox TC, Toolbox Reading, Toolbox CC, PC1-GA. The LMT and Toolbox List Sort tasks were significant in both ARMS for MTG, but not IFG.

3.5. Multilevel Analysis

One of the goals of this project was to assess laterality of language function through the lenses of network-level laterality (bLI, tLI) and the lateralization of functional regions (ILIIFG, ILIWA, ILIMTG). To this effect, we must examine whether these different metrics explain different components of the outcome.

Due to the large correlation between the network metrics (bLI/tLI: r0.8), and because bLI was not consistently associated with any of the behavioral outcomes, we use tLI to examine network laterality. Likewise, while ILIWA was not associated with any metric and ILIIFG was not reliably associated with any metric, we only examine ILIMTG, which was reliability associated with PC1-GA, Toolbox CC, Toolbox Reading, Toolbox TC, and the RAVLT.

We fit nested models on the five behavioral measures reliably predicted by ILIMTG (see above) to understand whether network- and ROI-level laterality explain distinct elements of the outcome. We performed ANOVAs on the null model (demographic predictors only), a second-level model model incorporating network laterality, and a third-level model additionally incorporating MTG laterality. The AIC for these sets of models are shown in Table 5; all improvements are significant for PC1-GA, Toolbox CC and Toolbox Reading. Toolbox TC and RAVLT showed no reliable improvement adding either the network-level or ROI-level laterality, i.e. the only significant improvement was in ARMS-2 demographics-to-network.

Table 5.

Information criteria for six nested models predicting three behaviors from both ARMS. DOF and p-values in columns 1 and 2 show values for the test comparing the current model to previous. P-values Benjamini & Yekutieli corrected for 20 correlated multiple comparisons. The DOF for all tests was 1.



Model
Behavior ARMS 0. Demographics 1. Network DOF= 1 2. Network + MTG DOF= 1
PC1-GA 1 4094 4079
p < 0.001
4068
p = 0.002
2 4469 4450
p < 0.001
4442
p = 0.007
Toolbox CC 1 5890 5879
p = 0.002
5871
p = 0.008
2 6582 6571
p = 0.002
6564
p = 0.008
Toolbox Reading 1 6342 6327
p < 0.001
6310
p < 0.001
2 6940 6931
p = 0.004
6925
p = 0.014
Toolbox TC 1 5759 5755
p = 0.069
5755
p = 0.847
2 6369 6349
p < 0.001
6349
p = 0.565
RAVLT 1 6340 6331
p = 0.006
6332
p = 1.000
2 6866 6852
p = 0.001
6853
p = 1.000

4. Discussion

Laterality of functions could be an important bioindicator of neurodevelopment and ongoing cortical and hemispheric specialization in young children. Many important skills have most frequently been indexed with tasks — either due to the study population or aims — but routine collection of RS data in addition to study aims has become increasingly common. Therefore, we developed a new RS technique to characterize the laterality of the language network using ROIs known to participate in the adult language network, allowing examination of those data without an in-scanner language task. Our method may also extend to other areas demonstrating HS, such as visual word form detection and face processing.

Generally, we found that associations between our network-level and ROI-level brain measures and behavior were larger for tasks requiring language-specific, rather than domain-general skills. This is true even in the analyses where task-laterality relations did not meet statistical significance.

4.1. Linguistic tasks show the largest associations

It is not common to report the lack of association between brain measurements and instruments that should not be associated with the outcome of interest. Previous fMRI studies (Hwang et al., 2019) and studies on the ABCD behavioral data (Dick et al., 2019) studies have examined relevant parts of the Toolbox, but not necessarily discriminant validity. Other fMRI studies have examined external validity with the Oral and Written Language Scales (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1996, Szaflarski et al., 2006) and verbal IQ as measured by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence (Bernal et al., 2018, Everts et al., 2009, Wechsler, 2014), but not contrasting instruments. By far, most fMRI studies rely on performance based on in-scanner tasks for validity, rather than behavioral instruments collected outside of the scanner (e.g. Joliot et al., 2016; Smitha et al., 2017; Szaflarski, Holland et al., 2006; Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2016; Zago et al., 2016).

Lacking in-scanner tasks, we were forced to rely on out-of-scanner tasks. However, we aimed to demonstrate validity by showing that our ILI is associated more highly with behavioral metrics involving language skills, and less to not at all with metrics not involving language skills. Our results support this pattern. We succeeded in demonstrating this with laterality associated with MTG, but did not find laterality associated with IFG to be more highly associated with language skills over all others.

To summarize, we reported on five analyses: two ways of measuring network-level laterality across the entire cortex and laterality associated with three language ROIs. Except for “WA,” the tasks most frequently appearing with the highest beta values were Toolbox Reading, Toolbox Picture Vocabulary, the RAVLT, and the composite score Toolbox CC (the linear combination of Reading and Pic Vocab), as well as two other composite scores: Toolbox TC, and PC1-GA. The laterality associated with the MTG specifically, rather than the VAN at large explained more of the variance for Toolbox Reading, Toolbox CC, and PC1-GA, but not Toolbox TC, and the RAVLT. This may reflect the more domain-general skills invoked in the latter tests, namely all the fluid scores and the increased memory demands of the RAVLT over the other tasks (which require no memory beyond the trial).

The “WA'' ROI showed no patterns of association. As can be seen in SF 6.1, while our ROI is contained within the extent of the language network as defined by Lipkin and colleagues, it also misses a considerable amount of the area identified by those authors as involved in the language network. Its small size also indicates a low amount of inter-subject spatial reliability, and as mentioned, there was no RH homolog, thus we consider that under our method of ROI creation, this area captured more irrelevant cortex within individuals. For example, the mixed seedmaps that include LH language areas and non-language RH areas would not be very lateralized, and thus the variance of the laterality scores would be meaningless when regressed against behavior variance.

Regarding the IFG analysis, the results show a trend consistent with an understanding of the IFG as containing distinct language and domain-general sub-regions. Because our ROI was large, to capture the IFG across more than 6000 individuals, our brain-behavior results do not show high degrees of specificity for language, as it may have captured both language and domain-general sub-regions. Even then, there is large heterogeneity in topology (Gordon et al., 2017, Hermosillo et al., 2022, Keller et al., 2022) that dampens the usefulness of ROIs applied across individuals. Likewise, Moore and colleagues (2023) demonstrated that, although adultlike resting state networks exist in infancy, networks are continuing to differentiate through adolescence, which likewise limits the applicability of a single ROI applied against individuals.

However, the relative simplicity of the tasks that do not reflect higher-order syntactic processing that is also consistent with IFG as a syntactic node: If the asynchronous linguistic tasks do not index syntactic processing, then it would likewise show small-to-no effects in the syntactic node. Future research - such as in the Human Connectome Project - should analyze subject-specific, task-localized, language-selective IFG nodes in order to dissociate domain-general signal.

Language is only one facet of cognition, and even those Toolbox tasks with more language components don’t rely on language alone. Cognitive skills do not exist in isolation, and composite scores decrease method variance that can emerge from using a single task. Therefore, it is not surprising that some composite scores emerge as having higher associations with laterality than single-task measurements, based on the relative amount of noise in each measurement. This is especially true for Toolbox CC, which is a linear composite of the two language tasks.

In both IFG and MTG, we also find that the smallest beta values were found for the Toolbox Flanker, Toolbox Card Sort, and Toolbox Pattern Matching tasks. This pattern suggests that the laterality of the language network in adolescents is differentiably associated with language-specific, rather than domain-general executive function skills, however the effect sizes for IFG are perhaps decreased by the inclusion of domain-general processing subregions.

Together, this pattern shows that our laterality measure indexes the appropriate behaviors, and is not associated with behaviors reliant on other aspects of cognition. If we were, for example, indexing attentional control rather than language specifically, beta values among tasks would be much more similar to one another. The fact that language, but not other domains of cognition such as working memory, executive function, or processing speed emerged as possible explanations for this pattern provide external validity of our resting state measure.

These effects are large for brain-behavior associations. For example, out of the set of reliably significant effects (0.02 < B < 0.08), the largest, MTG/PC1-GA approaches the top-1% mark of brain-wide associations as reported by Marek et al. (2022), i.e. the association approaches the maximally detectable size within Marek et al. As ILI and network laterality are summary measures of functional topology, behavioral associations are naturally larger than those with raw imaging phenotypes.

4.2. Drawbacks to this analysis include task asynchrony and characteristics

One important caveat to note is that there is no straightforward method to quantify the amount of linguistic skills needed for each task, and while the associations between laterality and tasks follow roughly the same pattern between methods, the beta weights do not order in exactly the same way. A second caveat is that, contrary to most imaging analyses of brain organization, we are attempting to draw correlations between observed resting-state organization and task scores completed at another time, which precludes large relationships.

A third major drawback in the present study is that the “language” tasks themselves do not examine more than single words. Toolbox Reading requires proper pronunciations of words presented on a screen and crucially requires no semantic understanding of the stimulus; Toolbox Picture Vocabulary asks the participant to select semantically relevant images following a verbal stimulus, and the RAVLT requires the memorization of loosely semantically organized sets of nouns. In each of these cases, the stimuli are single items, i.e. there is no top-down processing required to understand a sentence or phrase. We hypothesize that the associations would be larger if tasks that examined higher-order processing skills were analyzed. However, increasing difficulty may inflate the associations with IFG due to its domain-general role, rather than its linguistic role.

The third area, Wernicke’s area, showed no correlation with any behavioral scores. However, given the uncertainty over the existence and role of “Wernicke’s area” (Tremblay and Dick, 2016) and the small size of the ROI generated for this analysis may justify this lack of assocation.

As a final point, we approached ROI selection at the population level. We use ROIs constructed from ABCD ARMS 1 that comprise grayordinates that are involved in the VAN in the majority of participants. However, it is possible that IFG or MTG in a given individual is completely or mostly disjoint from this ROI, however, lacking a functional localizer, this approach prioritizes intersubject validity.

5. Conclusions

RS data can be used to calculate coarse measures of hemispheric organization as related to components of the language network. We found that, consistent with earlier work, greater laterality associated with MTG is associated with greater linguistic skills (as measured out of the scanner) in a sample of school-aged children. However, we did not find the same pattern in IFG, which is consistent with more recent work describing IFG as a mosaic of language-selective and task-general regions. We suggest that implementing this technique in other RS data will be fruitful for understanding the development of the language network.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Elison Jed T.: Funding acquisition, Project administration, Resources, Supervision. Fair Damien A.: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources. Hermosillo Robert J.M.: Investigation, Methodology, Software. Conan Gregory: Methodology, Software. Feczko Eric: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources. Day Trevor Kincaid McAllister: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. Earl Eric: Data curation. Byington Nora: Data curation. Randolph Anita: Methodology, Resources, Software. Perrone Anders J.: Data curation. Hendrickson Timothy J.: Data curation.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Appendix A

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found in the online version at doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2024.101355.

1

Here is where the value of remapping nR becomes useful, as the y-intercept now represents equal sampling from both hemispheres.

2

White, Black, Native American/Alaskan Native, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, Asian, more than one, other

3

Fifteen behaviors × two ARMs × two LIs

4

Fifteen behavioral scores × three ROIs × two ARMs

Appendix A. Supplementary material

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (7MB, docx)

.

Data availability

The neuroimaging and demographic data used in this paper are available through wider efforts to distribute data from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development study. The mixed ROIs, “crossotopes” used in this study are available on Zenodo (https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.01.12.475422), and information about the Hermosillo probabilistic atlases can be found at https://midbatlas.io.

References

  1. Barch D.M., Albaugh M.D., Avenevoli S., Chang L., Clark D.B., Glantz M.D., Hudziak J.J., Jernigan T.L., Tapert S.F., Yurgelun-Todd D., Alia-Klein N., Potter A.S., Paulus M.P., Prouty D., Zucker R.A., Sher K.J. Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: rationale and description. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:55–66. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bartha-Doering L., Kollndorfer K., Kasprian G., Novak A., Schuler A.-L., Fischmeister F., Ph S., Alexopoulos J., Gaillard W.D., Prayer D., Seidl R., Berl M.M. Weaker semantic language lateralization associated with better semantic language performance in healthy right-handed children. Brain Behav. 2018;8(11) doi: 10.1002/brb3.1072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bauer P.J., Zelazo P.D. The national institutes of health toolbox for the assessment of neurological and behavioral function: a tool for developmental science. Child Dev. Perspect. 2014;8(3):119–124. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12080. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernal B., Guillen M., Korman B. Nontask-related brain lateralization biomarkers in children: the asymmetry of language areas on functional connectivity functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Connect. 2018;8(6):321–332. doi: 10.1089/brain.2017.0553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bernard F., Lemee J.-M., Mazerand E., Leiber L.-M., Menei P., Ter Minassian A. The ventral attention network: the mirror of the language network in the right brain hemisphere. J. Anat. 2020;237(4):632–642. doi: 10.1111/joa.13223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Biswal B., Yetkin F.Z., Haughton V.M., Hyde J.S. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magn. Reson. Med. 1995;34(4):537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bradshaw A.R., Thompson P.A., Wilson A.C., Bishop D.V.M., Woodhead Z.V.J. Measuring language lateralisation with different language tasks: a systematic review. PeerJ. 2017;5 doi: 10.7717/peerj.3929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Braga R.M., DiNicola L.M., Becker H.C., Buckner R.L. Situating the left-lateralized language network in the broader organization of multiple specialized large-scale distributed networks. J. Neurophysiol. 2020;124(5):1415–1448. doi: 10.1152/jn.00753.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Branco P., Seixas D., Deprez S., Kovacs S., Peeters R., Castro S.L., Sunaert S. Resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging for language preoperative planning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10 doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Broca M.P. Remarques sur le siége de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d′une observation d′aphémie (perte de la parole) Bull. De. la Société Anat. 1861:18. [Google Scholar]
  11. Button K.S., Ioannidis J.P.A., Mokrysz C., Nosek B.A., Flint J., Robinson E.S.J., Munafò M.R. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013;14(5) doi: 10.1038/nrn3475. Article 5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carrow-Woolfolk E. American Guidance Service, Inc; 1996. Oral and Written Language Scales. [Google Scholar]
  13. Casey B.J., Cannonier T., Conley M.I., Cohen A.O., Barch D.M., Heitzeg M.M., Soules M.E., Teslovich T., Dellarco D.V., Garavan H., Orr C.A., Wager T.D., Banich M.T., Speer N.K., Sutherland M.T., Riedel M.C., Dick A.S., Bjork J.M., Thomas K.M., Imaging Acquisition Workgroup A.B.C.D. The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Corbetta M., Shulman G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cosgrove K.T., McDermott T.J., White E.J., Mosconi M.W., Thompson W.K., Paulus M.P., Cardenas-Iniguez C., Aupperle R.L. Limits to the generalizability of resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of youth: an examination of ABCD Study® baseline data. Brain Imaging Behav. 2022;16(4):1919–1925. doi: 10.1007/s11682-022-00665-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dehaene-Lambertz G., Dehaene S., Hertz-Pannier L. Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science. 2002;298(5600):2013–2015. doi: 10.1126/science.1077066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dick A.S., Garcia N.L., Pruden S.M., Thompson W.K., Hawes S.W., Sutherland M.T., Riedel M.C., Laird A.R., Gonzalez R. No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019;3(7) doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0609-3. Article 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dubois J., Alison M., Counsell S.J., Hertz-Pannier L., Hüppi P.S., Benders M.J.N.L. MRI of the neonatal brain: a review of methodological challenges and neuroscientific advances. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 2021;53(5):1318–1343. doi: 10.1002/jmri.27192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ebert S., Peterson C., Slaughter V., Weinert S. Links among parents’ mental state language, family socioeconomic status, and preschoolers’ theory of mind development. Cogn. Dev. 2017;44:32–48. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Enge A., Friederici A.D., Skeide M.A. A meta-analysis of fMRI studies of language comprehension in children. NeuroImage. 2020;215 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Everts R., Lidzba K., Wilke M., Kiefer C., Mordasini M., Schroth G., Perrig W., Steinlin M. Strengthening of laterality of verbal and visuospatial functions during childhood and adolescence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30(2):473–483. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Feczko, E., Conan, G., Marek, S., Tervo-Clemmens, B., Cordova, M., Doyle, O., Earl, E., Perrone, A., Sturgeon, D., Klein, R., Harman, G., Kilamovich, D., Hermosillo, R., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Adebimpe, A., Bertolero, M., Cieslak, M., Covitz, S., Hendrickson, T., … Fair, D.A. (2021). Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Community MRI Collection and Utilities (p. 2021.07.09.451638). bioRxiv. 10.1101/2021.07.09.451638. [DOI]
  23. Fedorenko E., Blank I.A. Broca’s area is not a natural kind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020;24(4):270–284. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Flinker A., Korzeniewska A., Shestyuk A.Y., Franaszczuk P.J., Dronkers N.F., Knight R.T., Crone N.E. Redefining the role of Broca’s area in speech. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112(9):2871–2875. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414491112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fransson P., Åden U., Blennow M., Lagercrantz H. The functional architecture of the infant brain as revealed by resting-state fMRI. Cereb. Cortex. 2011;21(1):145–154. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gao W., Zhu H., Giovanello K.S., Smith J.K., Shen D., Gilmore J.H., Lin W. Evidence on the emergence of the brain’s default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106(16):6790–6795. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811221106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gao W., Lin W., Grewen K., Gilmore J.H. Functional connectivity of the infant human brain: plastic and modifiable. Neuroscientist. 2017;23(2):169–184. doi: 10.1177/1073858416635986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gordon E.M., Laumann T.O., Adeyemo B., Huckins J.F., Kelley W.M., Petersen S.E. Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26(1):288–303. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Gordon E.M., Laumann T.O., Adeyemo B., Gilmore A.W., Nelson S.M., Dosenbach N.U.F., Petersen S.E. Individual-specific features of brain systems identified with resting state functional correlations. NeuroImage. 2017;146:918–939. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hermosillo, R.J.M., Moore, L.A., Fezcko, E., Dworetsky, A., Pines, A., Conan, G., Mooney, M.A., Randolph, A., Adeyemo, B., Earl, E., Perrone, A., Carrasco, C.M., Uriarte-Lopez, J., Snider, K., Doyle, O., Cordova, M., Nagel, B.J., Ewing, S.W.F., Satterthwaite, T., … Fair, D.A., 2022. A Precision Functional Atlas of Network Probabilities and Individual-Specific Network Topography (p. 2022.01.12.475422). bioRxiv. 10.1101/2022.01.12.475422. [DOI]
  31. Hermundstad A.M., Bassett D.S., Brown K.S., Aminoff E.M., Clewett D., Freeman S., Frithsen A., Johnson A., Tipper C.M., Miller M.B., Grafton S.T., Carlson J.M. Structural foundations of resting-state and task-based functional connectivity in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2013;110(15):6169–6174. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1219562110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hervé P.-Y., Zago L., Petit L., Mazoyer B., Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Revisiting human hemispheric specialization with neuroimaging. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2013;17(2):69–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hillis A.E., Work M., Barker P.B., Jacobs M.A., Breese E.L., Maurer K. Re‐examining the brain regions crucial for orchestrating speech articulation. Brain. 2004;127(7):1479–1487. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Holland S.K., Vannest J., Mecoli M., Jacola L.M., Tillema J.-M., Karunanayaka P.R., Schmithorst V.J., Yuan W., Plante E., Byars A.W. Functional MRI of language lateralization during development in children. Int. J. Audiol. 2007;46(9):533–551. doi: 10.1080/14992020701448994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Howell B.R., Styner M.A., Gao W., Yap P.-T., Wang L., Baluyot K., Yacoub E., Chen G., Potts T., Salzwedel A., Li G., Gilmore J.H., Piven J., Smith J.K., Shen D., Ugurbil K., Zhu H., Lin W., Elison J.T. The UNC/UMN baby connectome project (BCP): an overview of the study design and protocol development. NeuroImage. 2018;185:891–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hwang G., Dabbs K., Conant L., Nair V.A., Mathis J., Almane D.N., Nencka A., Birn R., Humphries C., Raghavan M., DeYoe E.A., Struck A.F., Maganti R., Binder J.R., Meyerand E., Prabhakaran V., Hermann B. Cognitive slowing and its underlying neurobiology in temporal lobe epilepsy. Cortex. 2019;117:41–52. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.02.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Johnson M.H. Functional brain development in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;2(7) doi: 10.1038/35081509. Article 7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Joliot M., Tzourio-Mazoyer N., Mazoyer B. Intra-hemispheric intrinsic connectivity asymmetry and its relationships with handedness and language Lateralization. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93:437–447. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Keller, A.S., Pines, A.R., Sydnor, V.J., Cui, Z., Bertolero, M.A., Barzilay, R., Alexander-Bloch, A., Byington, N., Chen, A.A., Conan, G.M.2022. Personalized Functional Brain Network Topography Predicts Individual Differences in Youth Cognition. bioRxiv, 2022–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  40. Korom M., Camacho M.C., Filippi C.A., Licandro R., Moore L.A., Dufford A., Zöllei L., Graham A.M., Spann M., Howell B., Shultz S., Scheinost D. Dear reviewers: responses to common reviewer critiques about infant neuroimaging studies. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2022;53 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lee M.H., Hacker C.D., Snyder A.Z., Corbetta M., Zhang D., Leuthardt E.C., Shimony J.S. Clustering of resting state networks. PLOS ONE. 2012;7(7) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lezak M.D. Neuropsychological assessment. 2nd ed.., Oxford University Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  43. Lidzba K., Schwilling E., Grodd W., Krägeloh-Mann I., Wilke M. Language comprehension vs. language production: age effects on fMRI activation. Brain Lang. 2011;119(1):6–15. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lipkin, B., Tuckute, G., Affourtit, J., Small, H., Mineroff, Z., Kean, H., Jouravlev, O., Rakocevic, L., Pritchett, B., Siegelman, M., Hoeflin, C., Pongos, A., Blank, I.A., Struhl, M.K., Ivanova, A., Shannon, S., Sathe, A., Hoffmann, M., Nieto-Castañón, A., Fedorenko, E., 2022. LanA (Language Atlas): A probabilistic atlas for the language network based on fMRI data from >800 individuals (p. 2022.03.06.483177). bioRxiv. 10.1101/2022.03.06.483177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  45. Long M.A., Katlowitz K.A., Svirsky M.A., Clary R.C., Byun T.M., Majaj N., Oya H., Howard M.A., Greenlee J.D.W. Functional segregation of cortical regions underlying speech timing and articulation. Neuron. 2016;89(6):1187–1193. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Marek S., Tervo-Clemmens B., Calabro F.J., Montez D.F., Kay B.P., Hatoum A.S., Donohue M.R., Foran W., Miller R.L., Hendrickson T.J., Malone S.M., Kandala S., Feczko E., Miranda-Dominguez O., Graham A.M., Earl E.A., Perrone A.J., Cordova M., Doyle O., Dosenbach N.U.F. Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature. 2022;603(7902):654–660. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mennes M., Kelly C., Zuo X.-N., Di Martino A., Biswal B.B., Castellanos F.X., Milham M.P. Inter-individual differences in resting-state functional connectivity predict task-induced BOLD activity. NeuroImage. 2010;50(4):1690–1701. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Monopoli W.J., Kingston S. The relationships among language ability, emotion regulation and social competence in second-grade students. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2012;36(5):398–405. doi: 10.1177/0165025412446394. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Moore, L.A., Hermosillo, R.J., Feczko, E., Moser, J., Koirala, S., Allen, M.C., Buss, C., Conan, G., Juliano, A.C., Marr, M., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Mooney, M., Myers, M., Rasmussen, J., Rogers, C., Smyser, C., Snider, K., Sylvester, C., Thomas, E., … Graham, A.M. , 2023. Towards Personalized Precision Functional Mapping in Infancy (SSRN Scholarly Paper 4428815). 10.2139/ssrn.4428815. [DOI]
  50. Mungas D., Widaman K., Zelazo P.D., Tulsky D., Heaton R.K., Slotkin J., Blitz D.L., Gershon R.C. VII. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): factor structure for 3 to 15 year olds. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2013;78(4):103–118. doi: 10.1111/mono.12037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Olman C.A., Davachi L., Inati S. Distortion and signal loss in medial temporal lobe. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(12) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Olulade O.A., Seydell-Greenwald A., Chambers C.E., Turkeltaub P.E., Dromerick A.W., Berl M.M., Gaillard W.D., Newport E.L. The neural basis of language development: changes in lateralization over age. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2020;117(38):23477–23483. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1905590117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Papoutsi M., de Zwart J.A., Jansma J.M., Pickering M.J., Bednar J.A., Horwitz B. From phonemes to articulatory codes: an fMRI study of the role of Broca’s area in speech production. Cereb. Cortex. 2009;19(9):2156–2165. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Perani D., Saccuman M.C., Scifo P., Anwander A., Spada D., Baldoli C., Poloniato A., Lohmann G., Friederici A.D. Neural language networks at birth. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2011;108(38):16056–16061. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102991108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Petitto L.A., Berens M.S., Kovelman I., Dubins M.H., Jasinska K., Shalinsky M. The “perceptual wedge hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: new insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain Lang. 2012;121(2):130–143. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Poldrack R.A., Baker C.I., Durnez J., Gorgolewski K.J., Matthews P.M., Munafò M.R., Nichols T.E., Poline J.-B., Vul E., Yarkoni T. Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017;18(2) doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.167. Article 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Power J.D., Cohen A.L., Nelson S.M., Wig G.S., Barnes K.A., Church J.A., Vogel A.C., Laumann T.O., Miezin F.M., Schlaggar B.L., Petersen S.E. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron. 2011;72(4):665–678. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ratcliff G. Spatial thought, mental rotation and the right cerebral hemisphere. Neuropsychologia. 1979;17(1):49–54. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(79)90021-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sepeta L.N., Berl M.M., Wilke M., You X., Mehta M., Xu B., Inati S., Dustin I., Khan O., Austermuehle A., Theodore W.H., Gaillard W.D. Age-dependent mesial temporal lobe lateralization in language fMRI. Epilepsia. 2016;57(1):122–130. doi: 10.1111/epi.13258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Shing Y.L., Lindenberger U., Diamond A., Li S.-C., Davidson M.C. Memory maintenance and inhibitory control differentiate from early childhood to adolescence. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2010;35(6):679–697. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.508546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Skeide M.A., Friederici A.D. The ontogeny of the cortical language network. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17(5) doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.23. Article 5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Skeide M.A., Brauer J., Friederici A.D. Syntax gradually segregates from semantics in the developing brain. NeuroImage. 2014;100:106–111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Smitha K.A., Arun K.M., Rajesh P.G., Thomas B., Kesavadas C. Resting-state seed-based analysis: an alternative to task-based language fMRI and its laterality index. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2017;38(6):1187–1192. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A5169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Smitha K.A., Arun K.M., Rajesh P.G., Thomas B., Radhakrishnan A., Sarma P.S., Kesavadas C. Resting fMRI as an alternative for task-based fMRI for language lateralization in temporal lobe epilepsy patients: a study using independent component analysis. Neuroradiology. 2019;61:803–810. doi: 10.1007/s00234-019-02209-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sturgeon D., Earl E., Perrone A., Snider K. DCAN-Labs/abcd-hcp-pipeline: minor update to DCAN BOLD Processing and MRE version [Computer software] Zenodo. 2021 doi: 10.5281/zenodo.4571051. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Szaflarski J.P., Holland S.K., Schmithorst V.J., Byars A.W. An fMRI study of language lateralization in children and adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2006 doi: 10.1002/hbm.20177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Szaflarski J.P., Schmithorst V.J., Altaye M., Byars A.W., Ret J., Plante E., Holland S.K. A longitudinal fMRI study of language development in children age 5–11. Ann. Neurol. 2006 doi: 10.1002/ana.20817. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Tavor I., Jones O.P., Mars R.B., Smith S.M., Behrens T.E., Jbabdi S. Task-free MRI predicts individual differences in brain activity during task performance. Science. 2016;352(6282):216–220. doi: 10.1126/science.aad8127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Thompson W.K., Barch D.M., Bjork J.M., Gonzalez R., Nagel B.J., Nixon S.J., Luciana M. The structure of cognition in 9 and 10 year-old children and associations with problem behaviors: findings from the ABCD study’s baseline neurocognitive battery. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2019;36 doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Tremblay P., Dick A.S. Broca and Wernicke are dead, or moving past the classic model of language neurobiology. Brain Lang. 2016;162:60–71. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Tzourio-Mazoyer N., Joliot M., Marie D., Mazoyer B. Variation in homotopic areas’ activity and inter-hemispheric intrinsic connectivity with type of language lateralization: An FMRI study of covert sentence generation in 297 healthy volunteers. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016;221:2735–2753. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1068-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Veale J.F. Edinburgh handedness inventory - short form: a revised version based on confirmatory factor analysis. Laterality. 2014;19(2):164–177. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Volkow N.D., Koob G.F., Croyle R.T., Bianchi D.W., Gordon J.A., Koroshetz W.J., Pérez-Stable E.J., Riley W.T., Bloch M.H., Conway K., Deeds B.G., Dowling G.J., Grant S., Howlett K.D., Matochik J.A., Morgan G.D., Murray M.M., Noronha A., Spong C.Y., Weiss S.R.B. The conception of the ABCD study: from substance use to a broad NIH collaboration. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018;32:4–7. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Wechsler, D., 2014. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAISIV) (4th ed.).
  75. Weintraub S., Bauer P.J., Zelazo P.D., Wallner-Allen K., Dikmen S.S., Heaton R.K., Tulsky D.S., Slotkin J., Blitz D.L., Carlozzi N.E., Havlik R.J., Beaumont J.L., Mungas D., Manly J.J., Borosh B.G., Nowinski C.J., Gershon R.C. I. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): introduction and pediatric data. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2013;78(4):1–15. doi: 10.1111/mono.12031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Weiss-Croft L.J., Baldeweg T. Maturation of language networks in children: a systematic review of 22years of functional MRI. NeuroImage. 2015;123:269–281. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Yeo B.T.T., Krienen F.M., Sepulcre J., Sabuncu M.R., Lashkari D., Hollinshead M., Roffman J.L., Smoller J.W., Zöllei L., Polimeni J.R., Fischl B., Liu H., Buckner R.L. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Zago L., Petit L., Mellet E., Jobard G., Crivello F., Joliot M., Mazoyer B., Tzourio-Mazoyer N. The association between hemispheric specialization for language production and for spatial attention depends on left-hand preference strength. Neuropsychologia. 2016;93:394–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material

mmc1.docx (7MB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

The neuroimaging and demographic data used in this paper are available through wider efforts to distribute data from the Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development study. The mixed ROIs, “crossotopes” used in this study are available on Zenodo (https://doi.org/10.1101/2022.01.12.475422), and information about the Hermosillo probabilistic atlases can be found at https://midbatlas.io.


Articles from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES