Abstract
Genetic predispositions play an important role in alcohol use. Understanding the psychosocial mechanisms through which genetic risk unfolds to influence alcohol use outcomes is critical for identifying modifiable targets and developing prevention and intervention efforts. In this study, we examined the role of sensation seeking and social support from family and friends in linking genetic risk to alcohol use. We also examined the role of social support in moderating the associations between genetic risk and sensation seeking and alcohol use. Data were drawn from a sample of 2,836 European American adults from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (46% male, Mean age = 35.65, SD = 10.78). Results from path analysis indicated that genome-wide polygenic scores for alcohol consumption (alc-GPS) were associated with higher sensation seeking, which in turn was associated with higher levels of alcohol use. Alc-GPS was also associated with higher alcohol use indirectly via lower levels of family support. In addition, high friend support attenuated the association between alc-GPS and sensation seeking and alcohol use. The pattern of associations was similar for males and females, with some differences in the associations between social support and alcohol use observed across age. Our findings highlight the important role of intermediate phenotypes and gene-environment interplay in the pathways of risk from genetic predispositions to complex alcohol use outcomes.
Keywords: polygenic scores, sensation seeking, social support, alcohol use, gene-environment interplay
General Scientific Summary:
This study suggests that higher sensation seeking is an important pathway linking genetic risk to alcohol use across emerging to middle adulthood. Social support is important in buffering individuals’ risk predispositions in relation to alcohol use, although effects depend on the source of social support and age.
Alcohol use and misuse is prevalent and represents a significant public health concern in the United States and worldwide (Grant et al., 2015; World Health Organization, 2018). Genes play an important role in alcohol use outcomes, accounting for approximately 50% of the variance in alcohol use disorder (Verhulst et al., 2015). Very little is known, however, about how genetic factors influence complex behavioral outcomes such as alcohol use (Li et al., 2017). Understanding the psychosocial mechanisms through which genetic risk unfolds to influence alcohol use outcomes is critical for identifying modifiable targets and developing prevention and intervention efforts. Genetic factors may influence alcohol use outcomes through multiple mechanisms at different levels (e.g., individual, interpersonal). Two plausible mechanisms involve intermediate phenotypes (i.e., personality characteristics such as sensation seeking) and gene-environment interplay (i.e., social support from friends and family), both of which have been shown to be genetically influenced (Kendler, 1997; Koopmans et al., 1995) and associated with alcohol use outcomes (Hittner & Swickert, 2006; Peirce et al., 2000). In this study, we examined the role of sensation seeking and perceived social support from family and friends in linking genetic risk to alcohol use in a sample of adults.
Sensation Seeking as Intermediate Phenotype Linking Genetic Influences to Alcohol Use
Complex behavioral outcomes such as alcohol use are quite distal from the level of genetic function. Thus, studying the role of intermediate phenotypes may help to unpack the pathways through which genetic risk unfolds to influence psychiatric and behavioral outcomes (Gottesman & Gould, 2003; Lenzenweger, 2013). Sensation seeking, or a tendency to seek out novel sensations and experiences (Zuckerman, 1984), is an especially compelling intermediate phenotype in molecular genetic studies of alcohol phenotypes. First, sensation seeking is a personality trait that co-segregates with alcohol use disorder (AUD). For example, individuals with a family history of AUD tend to report higher levels of sensation seeking than those without a family history of AUD (Grucza et al., 2006; Sher et al., 1991). Second, sensation seeking is moderately heritable, with an estimated heritability of 40%-80% (Harden et al., 2012). In addition, twin studies showed substantial overlap in the genetic etiology between sensation seeking and alcohol use outcomes (Mustanski et al., 2003; Slutske et al., 2002). Finally, high sensation seeking is robustly associated with alcohol use among adolescents and adults (Hittner & Swickert, 2006), with some longitudinal studies suggesting that sensation seeking may be a predictor of increase alcohol use. For example, using longitudinal data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth, Quinn and Harden (2013) found that a slower decline in sensation seeking was associated with more rapid increase in alcohol use from ages 15 to 26.
However, very few studies have explicitly investigated the role of sensation seeking in mediating the association between genetic factors and alcohol use. Using data from the Finnish Twin Cohort, Li et al. (2017) showed that sensation seeking partially mediated the association between genetic risk for alcohol dependence, measured as a genome-wide polygenic score (GPS), and alcohol use problems at age 16. In a sample of college students, sensation seeking also partially mediated the association between genetic predispositions toward risky behaviors and alcohol consumption (Ksinan, et al., 2019). Notably, previous studies on the mediating role of sensation seeking have primarily focused on adolescents or emerging adults. Limited research has examined whether the role of sensation seeking in alcohol use vary across a broader range of ages across adulthood. However, there is some evidence of a general decline in levels of sensation seeking beyond adolescence (Evans-Polce et al., 2018) and between emerging adulthood and middle adulthood (Zuckerman et al., 1978). The association between sensation seeking and binge drinking has also been found to decline across ages 18 to 30 (Evans-Polce et al., 2018). It remains unknown whether the associations between sensation seeking and alcohol use outcomes continue to decline beyond emerging adulthood or are similar between emerging adults and middle-aged adults.
Gene-Environment Interplay: The Role of Social Support
Social support, defined as perception of having support from and access to social connections (Lin et al., 1979), is considered a robust environmental factor associated alcohol use outcomes. Higher social support is associated with lower alcohol consumption and problems (Groh et al., 2007; Peirce et al., 2000), particularly during stressful periods (Steptoe et al., 1996). Social support is thought to be protective against alcohol use and problematic drinking by providing greater social control and/or buffering effects of stressful events, and to reduce individuals’ likelihood of drinking to cope (Brick et al., 2018; Humphreys et al., 1999). High social support from family, romantic partners, and friends have been shown to be negatively associated with alcohol use problems (Jarnecke & South, 2014). However, the role of social support may vary depending on the source of support. For example, in a sample of adult twins, social support from romantic partners was associated with fewer alcohol problems, but support from friends was not (Salvatore et al., 2015).
In addition, the nature and functioning of social support changes across the life course (Ertel et al., 2009). Although different age groups do not have unique desires for social support, research documents that the role of social support from family and friends varies over the life span (Segrin, 2003). For example, younger adults have larger support network and greater perception of support from friends (Levitt et al., 1993; Vaux, 1985) while older adults have greater perception of support from emotionally close family relationships (Antonucci et al., 2004; Levitt et al., 1993). Since health benefits of social support, including lower levels of drinking, depend on receivers’ preferences (Cohen & Wills, 1985), evidence indeed suggests that support from friends may benefit younger adults more and support from family may benefit older adults more (Walen & Lachman, 2000).
Theory and research suggest that individuals shape their social environments through gene-environment correlation (rGE) processes (Plomin et al., 1977; Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Prior evidence suggests that social support is genetically influenced: some studies found that between 40% and 80% of the variance in dimensions of social support can be attributed to genetic differences between individuals (Bergeman et al., 1990; Kendler, 1997; Wang et al., 2017). Others found moderate degree of heritability for social support from spouse and parents (ranged from 15% to 30%) but small to none genetic influence on social support from friends (Kutschke et al., 2018). Twin research also showed overlap in genetic influences between social support and AUD (Salvatore et al., 2015). Taken together, it is plausible that rGE processes related to social support may serve as a pathway linking genetic factors to alcohol use outcomes. That is, higher genetic risk may lead to lower social support, which in turn can result in elevated risk for alcohol use and related problems, although the effects may vary across sources of social support.
Gene-environment interplay can also occur in the form of gene-environment interaction (GxE). That is, environmental factors may buffer or exacerbate the association between genetic factors and developmental outcomes (Plomin et al., 1977). There is evidence that social support moderate genetic influences on alcohol use outcome. For example, using a sample of young adults in the Finnish Twin Study, Barr et al. (2017) found that higher levels of social support were associated with increased genetic influence on alcohol misuse for females and reduced genetic influence for males. Support from parents was found to mitigate the genetic risk effect of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) on developmental trajectories of alcohol use from adolescence to young adulthood in a nationally representative sample (Su et al., 2019). Together, these studies indicate that social support buffers genetic risk in relation to alcohol use and also suggest the need to examine potential sex differences in the role of social support. Given that sensation seeking is a genetically influenced trait related to increased risk for alcohol use, it is also possible that social support also buffers the relation between sensation seeking and alcohol use; however, to our knowledge, no study has tested this hypothesis.
The Present Study
The goal of this study was to examine the role of sensation seeking and social support in linking genetic risk to alcohol use. We examined two alternative models, one in which both sensation seeking and social support serve as indirect pathways linking genetic risk to alcohol use (Figure 1, Panel A), and another in which sensation seeking serves as an indirect pathway linking genetic risk to alcohol use, and social support moderates the association between genetic risk and sensation seeking and alcohol use (Figure 1, Panel B). Complex behavioral outcomes, such as alcohol use, are polygenic such that many common genetic variants contribute to risk. In this study, we index individuals’ genetic predispositions by using a genome-wide polygenic score (GPS) approach that aggregates effects of common genetic variants across the genome. The calculation of reliable GPS requires a discovery genome-wide association study (GWAS) with a large sample size to estimate the effects of common genetic variants across the genome (Dudbridge, 2013). We focused on alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic scores (alc-GPS) because the largest GWAS on alcohol-related phenotype to date was on alcohol consumption (drinks per week) conducted by the GWAS & Sequencing Consortium of Alcohol and Nicotine Use with about 1 million individuals (Liu et al., 2019), allowing us to calculate relatively reliable alc-GPS in our sample.
Figure 1.
Alternative Conceptual Models Linking Polygenic Risk Score, Sensation Seeking, Social Support, and Alcohol Consumption.
We hypothesized that higher alc-GPS would be associated with higher levels of alcohol use. Further, we hypothesized that the effect of alc-GPS would, at least in part, manifest indirectly by influencing sensation seeking and social support. We also hypothesized that social support would buffer risks associated with alcohol use, such that the association between alc-GPS and alcohol use, as well as the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use, would be attenuated by high levels of social support. We considered social support from family and friends separately in order to examine whether patterns of associations vary as a function of the source of social support given prior evidence of differential effects (Salvatore et al., 2015). In addition, we tested sex as a moderator of the hypothesized pathways given evidence of sex differences in rates of alcohol use and levels of sensation seeking and social support (Cross et al., 2013; Grant et al., 2015). Since sensation seeking and sources of social support tend to change across the adulthood years, we capitalized on the rich dataset from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA) which include participants of a wide age range, to explore whether the associations between alc-GPS, sensation seeking, social support, and alcohol use vary across developmental stages (i.e., emerging adulthoods vs. young adulthood vs. middle adulthood).
Method
Sample
Data for this study were drawn from COGA, a multi-site, large, multigenerational family study that aims to understand genetic influences on alcoholism (Begleiter et al., 1995). Probands were identified through alcohol treatment programs at six U.S. sites and were invited to participate if they had a sufficiently large family with two or more members in the COGA catchment areas. Population-based comparison families were also recruited. Data collection for COGA started in 1991 (Phase I) when adults in the target extended families were invited to complete the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA), a comprehensive interview that assesses demographic factors, alcohol use behaviors, and a variety of psychiatric phenotypes (Bucholz et al., 1994). COGA Phase II data collection occurred around 1996, where participants in COGA Phase I were invited for a follow-up assessment and eligible family members who did not participate in Phase I were invited to participate as well. Participants also completed questionnaires at each phase. All COGA participants were asked to provide a DNA sample via blood or saliva. Institutional Review Boards at all sites approved this study, and written consents were obtained from participants.
For the purpose of the present study, we focused on COGA participants of European ancestry (as determined by their genetic information) because the sample for the discovery GWAS we used to calculate genome-wide polygenic scores were primarily of European ancestry (Liu et al., 2019), and genome-wide polygenic scores derived from GWAS of European-centric samples may be biased in non-European samples (Martin et al., 2017). We included participants who 1) participated at both COGA Phase I (T1) and Phase II (T2), providing longitudinal data, 2) aged 18 of older at T1 and aged 65 or younger at T2, 3) had genomic data available for calculation of genome-wide polygenic scores, 4) reported on sensation seeking and social support, and 5) indicated that they have ever used alcohol. The analytic sample included 2,836 adults, 46% male, age ranged from 18 to 61 (Mean = 35.65, SD = 10.78) at T1, and 22 to 65 (Mean = 41.44, SD = 14.30) at T2.
Measures
Alcohol use.
Participants reported the number of drinks of different kinds of alcoholic beverages (beer, wine, liquor, other) they consumed on a typical day (Monday, Tuesday, … Sunday) of the week in the past 6 months, as part of the SSAGA at T2 (Bucholz et al., 1994). Scores were summed across days and kinds of drinks to derive a composite score that indicated drinks per week. A total of 769 (27.1%) participants indicated that they consumed no alcohol (zero drinks) during the past 6 months.
Sensation Seeking.
Participants completed the Sensation Seeking Scale, a 40-item self-report questionnaire designed to measure differences in the degree to which individuals seek out stimulation and arousal (Zuckerman, 1984) at T1. The items are presented as 40 pairs of statements such as “I like wild uninhibited parties” and “I prefer quiet parties with good conversation.” For each pair, respondents chose the statement that best describes their likes or the way they feel. Scores were summed across items and ranged between 0 and 40, with higher scores indicating higher sensation seeking (α = .86).
Social Support.
Participants completed the Perceived Social Support from Family Scale and the Perceived Social Support from Friends Scale (Procidano & Heller, 1983) at T2. These are 20-item self-report measures used to assess social support specifically from family members or friends. Sample items include “my family/friends give me the moral support I need”, and “I rely on my family/friends for emotional support”. Participants responded to each statement on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (generally false) to 4 (generally true). Scores were averaged across items and higher scores represent higher family support (α = .95) or friend support (α = .91).
Genotyping and genome-wide polygenic scores.
Participants’ DNA samples were genotyped using the Illumina Human1M array (Illumina, San Diego, CA), the Illumina Human OmniExpress 12V1 array (Illumina), the Illumina 2.5M array (Illumina) or the Smokescreen genotyping array (Biorealm LLC, Walnut, CA; Baurley et al., 2016). Data processing, quality control and imputation have been described elsewhere (Lai et al, 2019). Briefly, data were imputed to 1000 Genome Phase 3, and single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with a genotyping rate < 0.95 or that violated Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p < 10−6) or with minor allele frequency (MAF) < 0.01 were excluded from analysis.
We calculated genome-wide polygenic scores (GPS) using estimates from a genome-wide association study (GWAS) of alcohol consumption (drinks per week) with about 1 million individuals (Liu et al., 2019). We used PRS-CS (Ge et al., 2019), which employs a Bayesian approach to correct for GWAS summary statistics for the non-independence of SNPs in linkage disequilibrium or LD, to calculate alc-GPS. As recommended by the PRS-CS developers, SNPs for alc-GPS creation was limited to HapMap3 SNPs that overlapped between the original GWAS summary statistics, the LD reference panel (1000 Genomes Phase III European ancestry reference panel).
Covariates.
We included participants’ age at T1 and sex as covariates given their demonstrated associations with alcohol use. We also included the first 3 genetic ancestry principal components (PC1-PC3) as covariates to account for potential population stratification. In addition, given the wide range of age in our sample, which offered a great opportunity for us to examine similarities and differences in pathways of risk across developmental periods, we created an age group variable to classify participants into three groups based at their age at T2: emerging adulthood (ages 18–29, n = 452), young adulthood (ages 30–44, n = 1,367), and middle adulthood (ages 45–65, n = 1,017). Our grouping of age groups is consistent with studies in the epidemiology literature (e.g., Grant et al., 2015).
Analytic strategy
We first conducted descriptive statistics and correlations between the study variables. We then conducted path analysis using Mplus version 8.3 to examine whether sensation seeking and social support serve as indirect pathways linking alc-GPS to alcohol use (Figure 1, Panel A). In the path model, alc-GPS was specified as exogeneous variables associated with alcohol use directly and indirectly through sensation seeking, family support, and friend support. Sensation seeking, family and friend support were specified as associated with alcohol use. Family and friend support were also specified to be correlated. Age, sex, and genetic ancestry principal components were specified as covariates for sensation seeking, family and friend support, and alcohol use.
Indirect effects of alc-GPS on alcohol use via sensation seeking, family and friend support were tested using the MODEL INDIRECT command in Mplus, which provides a test of specific indirect effects in addition to the total indirect and direct effects of alc-GPS on alcohol use. Indirect effects were evaluated using bias-corrected bootstrapping (1,000 times) 95% confidence intervals (CI; MacKinnon et al., 2004), with CI not including zero indicating statistically significant indirect effects. Clustering within families was accounted for using the CLUSTER command in Mplus. Maximum likelihood with robust standard errors (MLR) estimation was used in all analysis to account for non-normality and non-independence of the observed data. Missing data was accounted for using full information maximum likelihood.
Next, we examined an alternative model where social support moderates the associations between alc-GPS and alcohol use, as well as the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use (Figure 1, Panel B). In this model, alc-GPS was specified to be associated with alcohol use directly and indirectly via sensation seeking. Sensation seeking, family and friend support were specified to be associated with alcohol use. In addition, product terms between alc-GPS and family/friend support, as well as product terms between sensation seeking and family/friend support were included as additional independent variables associated with alcohol use; variables were mean-centered before creating the product terms. alc-GPS and family/friend support were specified to be correlated using the “WITH” command in Mplus to account for potential rGE. Follow-up analysis were conducted to check the robustness of significant GxE effects by including covariates ~ G and covariates ~ E interaction terms in the model that further account for potential confounding effects (Keller, 2012).
We also conducted multi-group path analyses to examine whether the patterns of associations differed between males and females, and across age groups. Multigroup analyses were conducted by removing sex or age from the path model and then comparing a model with all remaining paths constrained to equality with another model that had all paths freely estimated across the comparison groups (i.e., females vs males; emerging adulthood vs young adulthood vs middle adulthood). A statistically significant Wald chi-square test of parameter equalities would indicate significant differences in path coefficients across groups.
Results
Preliminary Analysis
Descriptive statistics and correlations between variables are presented in Table 1. Alc-GPS was positively correlated with sensation seeking (r = .07) and alcohol use (r = .10), negatively correlated with family support (r = −.05), but was not significantly correlated with friend support (r = −.03). Sensation seeking was negatively correlated with family and friend support (r = −.13 and r = −.05, respectively) and positively correlated with alcohol use (r = .19). Family and friend support were modestly correlated (r = .44) and both were negatively correlated with alcohol use (r = −.14 and r = −.18, respectively). Age and sex both showed significant correlations with sensation seeking, social support, and alcohol use, suggesting the importance of including them as covariates in analyses and exploring potential differences in pathways of risk across age and sex. PC1-PC3 were all correlated with alc-GPS; PC1 was also correlated with alcohol use, suggesting the importance of accounting for potential confounding effects due to population stratification even in our sample with all participants of European ancestry.
Table 1.
Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations Between Variables
Variables | N | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| |||||||||||
1. Age T1 | 2836 | -- | |||||||||
2. Sex | 2836 | .01 | -- | ||||||||
3. PC1 | 2836 | .03 | −.01 | -- | |||||||
4. PC2 | 2836 | .04* | −.03 | .44** | -- | ||||||
5. PC3 | 2836 | .04* | −.01 | .11** | .45** | -- | |||||
6. alc-GPS | 2836 | .03 | .03 | .07** | .07** | .05* | -- | ||||
7. Sensation Seeking T1 | 2606 | −.37** | .38** | .03 | −.01 | −.01 | .07** | -- | |||
8. Family support T2 | 2587 | .04 | −.14** | −.00 | −.03 | .01 | −.05* | −.13** | -- | ||
9. Friend Support T2 | 2589 | −.11** | −.29** | −.02 | −.03 | .01 | −.03 | −.05* | .44** | ||
10. Alcohol Use T2 | 2835 | −.08** | .23** | .05** | .03 | −.01 | .10** | .19** | −.14** | −.18** | -- |
| |||||||||||
M | 35.65 | .44a | −.01 | .00 | .00 | .01 | 16.14 | 3.02 | 3.02 | 8.49 | |
SD | 10.78 | -- | 1.02 | 1.01 | 1.03 | 1.01 | 7.26 | .67 | .59 | 19.38 |
Note. Alc-GPS = alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic scores. Sex was coded 1 = male, 0 = female.
percentage of male participants. PC1-PC3 = the first three genetic ancestry principal components. T1 = Time 1. T2 = Time 2.
p < .05
p < .01.
Examining Sensation Seeking and Social Support as Indirect Pathways Linking alc-GPS and Alcohol Use
The path model demonstrated excellent fit (χ2 = 24.02, df = 2, p < .001; CFI = .99, RMSEA = .06; SRMR = .01). Results from the path model are summarized in Table 2 and illustrated in Figure 2. Consistent with our expectation, alc-GPS was associated with sensation seeking at T1, which in turn was associated with alcohol use at T2. alc-GPS was associated with reduced family support, but was not significantly associated with friend support. Friend support, but not family support (p = .06), was significantly associated with lower alcohol use at T2. Alc-GPS remained significantly associated with alcohol use at T2, above and beyond covariates (age, sex, PC1-PC3), sensation seeking, and social support.
Table 2.
Path Coefficients, Direct and Indirect Effects from the Path Model Examining Sensation Seeking and Social Support as Indirect Pathways Linking alc-GPS and Alcohol Use
Paths | B | SE | β | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Alc-GPS → alcohol use | 1.58 | .37 | .08 | <.001 |
Alc-GPS → sensation seeking | .52 | .13 | .07 | <.001 |
Alc-GPS → family support | −.03 | .02 | −.05 | .038 |
Alc-GPS → friend support | −.01 | .01 | −.02 | .435 |
Sensation seeking → alcohol use | .24 | .07 | .09 | <.001 |
Family support → alcohol use | −1.38 | .75 | −.05 | .061 |
Friend support → alcohol use | −3.82 | .86 | −.12 | <.001 |
| ||||
Indirect Effects | B | 95% CI | β | p |
| ||||
Alc-GPS → sensation seeking → alcohol use | .127 | [.064, .213] | .007 | .004 |
Alc-GPS → family support → alcohol use | .042 | [.005, .118] | .002 | .173 |
Alc-GPS → friend support → alcohol use | .34 | [−.032, .112] | .002 | .444 |
Note. Alc-GPS = alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic scores; Age at T1, sex, and genetic ancestry principal components were included as covariates for alcohol use, sensation seeking, and family/friend support. Path coefficients for covariates are presented in Supplementary Table 1. Analysis accounted for cluster within extended families. N = 2,836.
Figure 2.
Path Model Examining Sensation Seeking and Social Support as Indirect Pathways Linking Alc-GPS and Alcohol Use
Note. Standardized path coefficients are presented. alc-GPS = alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic scores. Age at T1, sex, and the first three genetic ancestry principal components were included as covariates for alcohol use, sensation seeking, and family/friend support. Family support and friend support were specified to be correlated with each other in the path model (correlation not shown in figure). *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. N = 2,836. Solid paths are statistically significant; dashed paths are not statistically significant; bolded paths indicate statistically significant indirect pathways. For the path coefficients that differed significantly across age groups, group-specific coefficients are presented in parentheses (emerging/young/middle adulthood)
Results indicated that there were significant indirect effects of alc-GPS on alcohol use via sensation seeking and social support. Specifically, alc-GPS was associated with higher levels of alcohol use indirectly via higher sensation seeking (B = .127, 95% CI = [.064, .213]). alc-GPS was also associated with more alcohol use indirectly via lower levels of social support from family (B = .042, 95% CI = [.005, .118]). There were no significant indirect effects of alc-GPS on alcohol use via social support from friends (B = .034, 95% CI = [−.032, .112].
Examining the Moderating Role of Social Support
Results from the path model examining the moderating role of social support in the relationships between alc-GPS, sensation seeking, and alcohol use are presented in Table 3. Results indicated a significant interaction between alc-GPS and friend support in relation to alcohol use (B = −2.38, SE = .93, 95% CI = [−4.11, −1.04], β = .07, p = .01). This GxE effect is robust, remaining statistically significant after adjusting for interactions between alc-GPS and covariates (age, sex, PC1-PC3) and interactions between friend support and covariates (p = .010; see supplemental Table 2). As illustrated in Figure 3 (Panel A), simple slope analyses indicated that alc-GPS was associated with higher alcohol use when friend support was low (−1 SD, B = 2.91, SE = .76, β = .15, p < .001) but not when friend support was high (+1 SD, B = .12, SE = .47, β = .01, p = .799). There was also a significant interaction between sensation seeking and friend support in relation to alcohol use (B = −.27, SE = .10, 95% CI = [−.43, −.12], β = −.06, p = .006). As shown in Figure 3 (Panel B), sensation seeking was associated with more alcohol use when friend support was low (−1 SD, B = .39, SE = .10, β = .15, p < .001) than not when friend support was high (+1 SD, B = .09, SE = .09, β = .04, p = .296). The indirect effect of alc-GPS on alcohol use via sensation seeking was significant when friend support was low (−1 SD; B = .197, 95% CI = [.083, .311], β = .010, p = .005) but was not significant when friend support was high (+1 SD; B = .048, 95% CI = [−.030, .125], β = .002, p = .312).
Table 3.
Path Coefficients from the Path Model Examining the Moderating Role of Social Support in the Relation Between alc-GPS, Sensation Seeking, and Alcohol Use
Paths | B | SE | β | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||
Alc-GPS → alcohol use | 1.51 | .36 | .08 | <.001 |
Sensation seeking → alcohol use | .24 | .06 | .09 | <.001 |
Family support → alcohol use | −1.18 | .74 | −.04 | .100 |
Friend support → alcohol use | −3.85 | .90 | −.12 | <.001 |
Alc-GPS × family support → alcohol use | −.15 | .78 | −.01 | .849 |
Alc-GPS × friend support → alcohol use | −2.38 | .93 | −.07 | .010 |
Sensation seeking × family support → alcohol use | −.05 | .09 | −.01 | .554 |
Sensation seeking × friend support → alcohol use | −.27 | .10 | −.06 | .006 |
Alc-GPS → sensation seeking | .52 | .14 | .07 | <.001 |
| ||||
Indirect Effect | B | 95% CI | β | p |
| ||||
Alc-GPS → sensation seeking → alcohol use | .122 | [.061, .211] | .006 | .007 |
Note. Alc-GPS = alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic scores; Age at T1, sex, and genetic ancestry principal components were included as covariates for alcohol use, sensation seeking, and family/friend support. Path coefficients for covariates are presented in Supplementary Table 2. Analysis accounted for cluster within extended families. N = 2,836.
Figure 3.
Friend Support Moderates the Associations between alc-GPS/Sensation Seeking and Alcohol Use.
Examining Differences in Pathways of Risk across Sex and Age Groups
Sex differences.
Descriptive statistics for males and females are presented in supplemental Table 3. Multigroup analysis was conducted for the path model examining sensation seeking and social support as indirect pathways linking alc-GPS to alcohol use. Wald test of constraining all of the paths linking alc-GPS, sensation seeking, social support, and alcohol use to be equal across sex was not statistically significant (χ2 = 11.76, df = 7, p = .11), suggesting that the patterns of associations between alc-GPS, sensation seeking, social support, and alcohol use were similar for males and females (see supplemental Table 4.
Multigroup analysis to test for sex differences was also conducted for the model examining moderating roles of social support (see Supplemental Table 5). Wald test of constraining the four paths linking interaction terms (i.e., alc-GPS ~ family/friend support, and sensation seeking ~ family/friend support) to alcohol use to equal between males and females was not statistically significant (χ2 = 6.63, df = 4, p = .16), suggesting no sex differences in the moderating role of social support for the associations between alc-GPS, sensation seeking, and alcohol use.
Age group differences.
Descriptive statistics across age groups are presented in supplemental Table 6. Multigroup analysis across age groups was conducted for the path model examining sensation seeking and social support as indirect pathways linking alc-GPS to alcohol use. Wald test of constraining all paths linking alc-GPS, sensation seeking, social support, and alcohol use to be equal across age groups (emerging vs. young vs. middle adulthood) was statistically significant (χ2 = 30.89, df = 14, p = .006), indicating age differences in the associations between alc-GPS, sensation seeking, social support, and alcohol use. Follow up analysis testing the paths one by one across age groups indicated that the association between family support and alcohol use significantly varied across age groups (χ2 = 7.81, df = 2, p = .020); higher family support was associated with lower alcohol use among emerging (B = −3.97, SE = 1.49, β = −.19, p = .008) and middle adults (B = −2.78, SE = 1.00, β = −.10, p = .005), but not among young adults (B = .71, SE = 1.18, β = .02, p = .550). The association between friend support and alcohol use also significantly differed across age groups (χ2 = 19.43, df = 2, p = < .001): higher friend support was associated with lower alcohol use among young adults (B = −7.57, SE = 1.64, β = −.21, p < .001), but not among emerging adults (B = 1.23, SE = 1.22, β = .05, p = .314) and middle adults (B = −.96, SE = .81, β = −.03, p = .232). There were no significant age differences in the associations between alc-GPS and sensation seeking or social support. The association between sensation seeking and alcohol use also did not significantly differ across age groups (see supplemental Table 7).
Multigroup analysis across age groups was also conducted for the model examining moderating roles of social support. Wald test of constraining the four paths linking interaction terms (i.e., alc-GPS ~ family/friend support, and sensation seeking ~ family/friend support) to alcohol use to equal across age groups was not statistically significant (χ2 = 15.02, df = 8, p = .06), suggesting no differences in the moderating role of social support for the associations between alc-GPS, sensation seeking, and alcohol use across age groups (supplemental Table 8).
Discussion
The overarching goal of this study was to examine the role of sensation seeking and social support in in linking genetic risk to alcohol use in adulthood. Using data from a large sample of adults from extended families enriched for AUD, our findings indicated that individuals’ alc-GPS was associated with higher sensation seeking, which in turn was associated with higher levels of alcohol use. In addition, alc-GPS was associated with reduced social support from family (but not friends), which in turn was associated with more alcohol use. Furthermore, social support from friends (but not family) moderated the association between individuals’ risk predispositions and alcohol use, such that the association between alc-GPS and alcohol use, as well as the association between sensation seeking and alcohol use, were attenuated by high levels of friend support. Findings revealed that the pattern of associations was similar between males and females; however, there were some differences in associations between social support and alcohol use across age groups.
Consistent with our hypothesis and prior work, we found significant indirect effect of alc-GPS on alcohol use via sensation seeking, suggesting sensation seeking as a promising intermediate phenotype for understanding the mechanisms of genetic influences on alcohol use outcomes. Prior research on the role of sensation seeking in mediating genetic risk has focused on adolescents and emerging adults (Li et al., 2018; Ksinan et al., 2019). Our findings extend the literature to show that this pathway of risk and the role of sensation seeking are similar in adulthood, spanning across emerging adulthood to middle adulthood, despite the decline in levels of sensation seeking across adulthood. This supports the promise of using sensation seeking to identify at-risk individuals across developmental stages for alcohol use intervention programs (Sargent et al., 2010).
Consistent with our hypothesis, we found evidence for two forms of gene-environment interplay involving the role of social support: rGE and GxE. Supporting the notion that rGE as an important pathway linking genetic risk to developmental outcomes, we found that alc-GPS was associated with lower levels of family support, which in turn was associated with greater levels of alcohol use. This finding of gene-environment correlation can reflect evocative (or reactive) rGE whereby individuals’ predispositions and related traits influence how others respond to them (e.g., the degree of support they provide). Further, since our sample included adults spanning multiple developmental stages, it is also possible that this gene-environment correlation reflects active rGE processes as well such that individuals’ alc-GPS and related traits contribute to their choices and selections of environments (e.g., family). Generally, our finding is consistent with prior research demonstrating that rGE in the family context as an important pathway of risk to alcohol use outcomes. For example, Elam et al. (2018) showed that polygenic risk for aggression was associated with lower levels of family cohesion in adolescence, which in turn was associated with alcohol use in young adulthood. We did not find significant associations between alc-GPS and friend support. This is inconsistent with previous research that provided evidence of rGE in the peer/friend context. For example, polygenic risk for behavioral disinhibition was associated with affiliation with substance-using peers, in part via higher impulsivity and lower parental monitoring (Elam et al., 2017). It is possible that genetic predispositions toward alcohol use influence individuals’ choice and selection of friends who share predispositions and behaviors similar to themselves, but do not influence the perception of amount and quality of support they receive from friends. Future research is needed to replicate our findings and also consider the role of other peer contextual factors such as peer substance use in linking genetic predispositions to alcohol use outcomes.
We also found evidence of GxE involving social support. Specifically, we found a significant interaction between alc-GPS and friend support in relation to alcohol use, such that high friend support buffered the association between alc-GPS and alcohol use. Similarly, high friend support also buffered the association between sensation seeking, a genetically influenced trait, and alcohol use. These findings are consistent with prior research suggesting the moderating role of social support on genetic influences (Barr et al., 2017; Su et al., 2019) and the broader literature suggesting the protective role of social support in alcohol use outcomes (Humphreys et al., 1999; Peirce et al., 2000). To our knowledge, this study is among the first to show that high social support buffered risk associated with sensation seeking in relation to alcohol use. Prior research has largely documented the buffering role of social support for environmental risk factors such as stress and negative life events (Mulia et al., 2008). Consistent with the social support buffering hypothesis (Cohen & Wills, 1985), our findings extend the literature to suggest that the relations between individuals’ risk predispositions, indexed by both genetic risk and genetically influenced traits such as sensation seeking and alcohol use are stronger for adults with lower levels of perceived social support.
Although both social support from family and friends were associated with lower levels of alcohol consumption, our findings indicate some age differences in these associations. Higher support from family was associated with lower levels of drinking among emerging adults and middle-aged adults, but not young adults. In contrast, higher support from friends was associated with lower alcohol use only among young adults (ages 30–44). It is interesting that while younger adults may have greater levels of perception of social support from friends, we did not find an association between friend support and alcohol use among emerging adults. This lack of association may in part reflect that alcohol use often occurs in social contexts among younger individuals (LaBrie et al., 2007). Thus, it is possible that support from friends in younger adulthood years may not provide increased social control in limiting risky behaviors, including alcohol use. Because older adults’ social networks tend to be smaller and include mostly emotionally close relationships, such as close family members, it is unsurprising that we found a link between support from family and alcohol use among middle-aged adults (Antonucci et al., 2004; Levitt, 1993). Finally, among young adults in our sample, only friend support, but not family support, was associated with alcohol use. Young adulthood is characterized with changes in responsibilities associated with adult social roles, such as marriages and parenthood (McCormick et al., 2011). It is possible that family support is already reflected in these adult roles. For example, prior research has shown that new adult social roles are inversely associated with drinking because of increased social control that these new roles/responsibilities offer (e.g., Barr et al, 2017; Christie-Mizell & Peralta, 2009). Our analyses of age differences based on broad age categories revealed interesting age differences in pattern of associations between social support and alcohol use. An important next step would be to examine the within-person pattern of change in these relations across the adulthood years.
This study has a number of strengths. First, we used a genetically-informative design to understand the mechanisms through which genetic risk unfolds to influence alcohol use. Notably, our results showed that alcohol consumption polygenic scores remained significantly associated with phenotypic alcohol use, above and beyond the effects of important covariates (e.g., age, sex), sensation seeking, and social support from family and friends. This highlights the robustness of genetic effects and the need to examine other biological and psychosocial mechanisms underlying genetic risk for alcohol use outcomes. Second, we conducted simultaneous tests of sensation seeking and social support as indirect pathways to delineate the pathways by which genetic influences impact alcohol use. Considering multiple mediating pathways simultaneously allowed for examination of the unique effects of each specific pathway. In addition, we considered alternative conceptual models to examine both rGE and GxE, which allows for a more comprehensive examination of the varying roles that social support could play in the relation between alc-GPS and alcohol use. We also considered social support from family and friends, allowing us to understand whether pathways of associations vary across sources of social support. Furthermore, we used the state of the science genome-wide polygenic score approach to characterize individuals’ genetic predispositions using results from a large GWAS study to capture the polygenic nature of the alcohol use phenotype. Finally, we used a large sample of adults with a wide age range, allowing us to examine potential age differences in the associations across emerging to middle adulthood.
Several limitations of this study need to be considered in interpreting the findings. First, we only focused on participants of European ancestry in COGA because the discovery samples in the GWAS study were primarily of European descent and mismatch in ancestry between discovery and target samples can result in bias in GPS prediction (Martin et al., 2017). Thus, it is not clear whether our findings are generalizable to non-European populations. Second, social support and alcohol use were both measured at T2 and these measures lack time precedence. Thus, inferences about causal relations need to be made cautiously and it is possible that the association between social support and alcohol use is bidirectional. Third, we examined sensation seeking as an intermediate phenotype that links alc-GPS to alcohol use. However, there are alternative models that could also explain the association between alc-GPS, sensation seeking, and alcohol use, whereby sensation seeking and alcohol use are indicators of a common underlying factor (e.g., externalizing, Krueger et al., 2002) that is influenced by genetic factors (e.g., alc-GPS). Future research is warranted to test these alternative models to better characterize the relations between sensation seeking, alcohol use, and their underlying genetic etiology. Fourth, we examined a conceptual model that considers rGE, with social support serving as an indirect pathway between genetic influences and alcohol use. However, we acknowledge there can be other alternative explanations for the association between genetic risk, social support, and alcohol use, such as epiphenomenon. Finally, COGA is a high-risk sample with participants from extended families enriched for AUD and findings from this study may not be generalizable to other samples with different recruitment strategies (Savage et al., 2018). Future research is needed to replicate our findings in community and population-based samples.
In conclusion, our results show that sensation seeking serves as an important indirect pathway linking genetic influences to alcohol use. Social support plays an important role in linking genetic risk to alcohol use, as well as buffering individuals’ risk predispositions in relation to alcohol use. Our findings emphasize the important role of intermediate phenotypes and gene-environment interplay, including both rGE and GxE processes, in the pathways of risk from genetic predispositions to complex alcohol use outcomes. Our results highlight the potential roles of sensation seeking and social support as targets for prevention and intervention efforts aimed at mitigating risk for alcohol use outcomes in adulthood.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA), Principal Investigators B. Porjesz, V. Hesselbrock, T. Foroud; Scientific Director, A. Agrawal; Translational Director, D. Dick, includes eleven different centers: University of Connecticut (V. Hesselbrock); Indiana University (H.J. Edenberg, T. Foroud, J. Nurnberger Jr., Y. Liu); University of Iowa (S. Kuperman, J. Kramer); SUNY Downstate (B. Porjesz, J. Meyers, C. Kamarajan, A. Pandey); Washington University in St. Louis (L. Bierut, J. Rice, K. Bucholz, A. Agrawal); University of California at San Diego (M. Schuckit); Rutgers University (J. Tischfield, A. Brooks, R. Hart); The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania (L. Almasy); Virginia Commonwealth University (D. Dick, J. Salvatore); Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (A. Goate, M. Kapoor, P. Slesinger); and Howard University (D. Scott). Other COGA collaborators include: L. Bauer (University of Connecticut); L. Wetherill, X. Xuei, D. Lai, S. O’Connor, M. Plawecki, S. Lourens (Indiana University); L. Acion (University of Iowa); G. Chan (University of Iowa; University of Connecticut); D.B. Chorlian, J. Zhang, S. Kinreich, G. Pandey (SUNY Downstate); M. Chao (Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai); A. Anokhin, V. McCutcheon, S. Saccone (Washington University); F. Aliev, P. Barr (Virginia Commonwealth University); H. Chin and A. Parsian are the NIAAA Staff Collaborators.
We continue to be inspired by our memories of Henri Begleiter and Theodore Reich, founding PI and Co-PI of COGA, and also owe a debt of gratitude to other past organizers of COGA, including Ting-Kai Li, P. Michael Conneally, Raymond Crowe, and Wendy Reich, for their critical contributions. This national collaborative study is supported by NIH Grant U10AA008401 from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA).
References
- Antonucci T, Akiyama H, & Takahashi K (2004). Attachment and close relationships across the life span. Attachment & human development, 6(4), 353–370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr PB, Salvatore JE, Maes HH, Korhonen T, Latvala A, Aliev F, ... & Dick DM (2017). Social relationships moderate genetic influences on heavy drinking in young adulthood. Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs, 78(6), 817–826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baurley JW, Edlund CK, Pardamean CI, Conti DV, & Bergen AW (2016). Smokescreen: A targeted genotyping array for addiction research. BMC Genomics, 17, 145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Begleiter H, Reich T, Hesselbrock V, Porjesz B, Ting-Kai L, Schuckit MA, ... Rice, J. P. (1995). The collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism. Alcohol Research and Health, 19, 228–236. [Google Scholar]
- Bergeman CS, Plomin R, Pedersen NL, McClearn GE & Nesselroade JR (1990). Genetic and environmental influences on social support: the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging. Journal of Gerontology, 45, 101–106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brick L, Nugent N, R., Kahana SY, Bruce D, Tanney MR, Fernandex MI, & Bauermeister JA (2018). Interaction effects of neighborhood disadvantage and individual social support on frequency of alcohol use in youth living with HIV. American journal of community psychology, 61, 276–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI Jr., … Schuckit MA (1994). A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: A report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 149–158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christie-Mizell CA, & Peralta RL (2009). The gender gap in alcohol consumption during late adolescence and young adulthood: Gendered attitudes and adult roles. Journal of health and social behavior, 50(4), 410–426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S, & Wills TA (1985) Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin. 98, 310–357. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cross C, Cyrenne DM, & Brown GR (2013). Sex differences in sensation seeking: A meta-analysis. Scientific Reports, 3, 2486. doi: 10.1038/srep02486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dudbridge F (2013). Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLOS Genetics, 9, e1003348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elam KK, Chassin L, Lemery-Chalfant K, Pandika D, Wang FL, Bountress K, … Agrawal A (2017). Affiliation with substance-using peers: Examining gene–environment correlations among parent monitoring, polygenicrisk, and children’s impulsivity. Developmental Psychobiology, 59, 561–573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elam KK, Chassin L, & Pandika D (2018). Polygenic risk, family cohesion, and adolescent aggression in Mexican American and European American families: Developmental pathways to alcohol use. Development and Psychopathology, 30(5), 1715–1728. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ertel KA, Glymour MM, & Berkman LF (2009). Social networks and health: A life course perspective integrating observational and experimental evidence. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 26(1), 73–92. [Google Scholar]
- Evans-Polce RJ, Schuler MS, Schulenberg JE, & Patrick ME (2018). Gender- and age-varying associations of sensation seeking and substance use across young adulthood. Addictive Behaviors, 84, 271–277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fuller-Iglesias HR, Webster NJ, & Antonucci TC (2015). The complex nature of family support across the life span: Implications for psychological well-being. Developmental psychology, 51, 277–288. doi: 10.1037/a0038665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ge T, Chen CY, Ni Y, Feng YCA, & Smoller JW (2019). Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors. Nature communications, 10(1), 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottesman I, & Gould TD (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 636–645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Saha TD, Chou SP, Jung J, Zhang H, . . .Hasin DS (2015). Epidemiology of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry, 72, 757–766. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Groh DR, Jason LA, Davis MI, Olson BD, & Ferrari JR (2007). Friends, family, and alcohol abuse: An examination of general and alcohol-specific social support. The American Journal on Addictions, 16, 49–55. doi: 10.1080/10550490601080084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grucza RA, Cloninger CR, Bucholz KK, Constantino JN, Schuckit MI, Dick DM, & Beirut LJ (2006). Novelty seeking as a moderator of familial risk for alcohol dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 30(7), 1176–1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harden KP, Quinn PD, &Tucker-Drob EM (2012). Genetically influenced change in sensation seeking drives the rise of delinquent behavior during adolescence. Developmental Science, 15, 150–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hittner J, & Swickert R (2006). Sensation seeking and alcohol use: A meta-analytic review. Addictive Behaviors, 31(8), 1383–1401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humphreys K, Mankowski E, Moos R, & Finney J (1999) Enhanced friendship networks and active coping mediate the effect of self-help groups on substance abuse. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 54–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarnecke AM & South SC (2014). Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol use problems: moderation by romantic partner support, but not family or friend support. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 38(2), 367–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS (1997). Social support: A genetic-epidemiologic analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1398–1404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koopmans JR, Boomsma DI, Heath AC, & van Doornen LJP (1995). A multivariate genetic analysis of sensation seeking. Behavior Genetics, 25(4), 349–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krueger RF, Hicks BM, Patrick CJ, Carlson SR, Iacono WG, & McGue M (2002). Etiologic connections among substance dependence, antisocial behavior, and personality: modeling the externalizing spectrum. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(3), 411–424. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ksinan AJ, Su J, Aliev F, Spit for Science Workgroup, & Dick DM (2019). Unpacking Genetic Risk Pathways for College Student Alcohol Consumption: The Mediating Role of Impulsivity. Alcoholism, Clinical, and Experimental Research, 43(10), 2100–2110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kutschke J, Bengston MB, Seeman TE, & Harris JR (2018). Social support and strain across close relationships: A twin study. Behavior Genetics, 48, 173–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie J, Hummer J, Pedersen E. Reasons for drinking in the college student context: The differential role and risk of the social motivator. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2007;68:393–398. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenzenweger MF (2013). Thinking clearly about the endophenotype-intermediate phenotype-biomarker distinctions in developmental psychopathology research. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 1347–1357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levitt MJ, Weber RA, & Guacci N (1993). Convoys of social support: An intergenerational analysis. Psychology and Aging, 8(3), 323 326. 10.1037/0882-7974.8.3.323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li JJ, Savage JE, Kendler KS, Hickman M, Mahedy L, Macleod J, ... & Dick DM (2017). Polygenic risk, personality dimensions, and adolescent alcohol use problems: A longitudinal study. Journal of studies on alcohol and drugs, 78(3), 442–451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin N, Ensel WM, Simeone RS, & Kuo W (1979). Social support, stressful life events, and illness: A model and an empirical test. Journal of health and Social Behavior, 20(2), 108–119. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu M, Jiang Y, Wedow R, Li Y, Brazel DM, Chen F, . . . Vrieze, S. (2019). Association studies of up to 1.2 million individuals yield new insights into the genetic etiology of tobacco and alcohol use. Nature Genetics, 51(2), 237–244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCormick CM, Kuo SI, & Masten AS (2011). Developmental tasks across the life span. In Fingerman KL, Berg CA, Smith J, & Antonucci TC (Eds.), Handbook of life-span development (p. 117–139). Springer: New York. [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, & Williams J (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 99–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin AR, Gignoux CR, Walters RK, Wojcik GL, Neale BM, Gravel S, . . . Kenny EE (2017). Human demographic history impacts genetic risk prediction across diverse populations. American Journal of Human Genetics, 100, 635–649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulia N, Schmidt L, Bond J, Jacobs L, & Korcha R (2008). Stress, social support, and problem drinking among women in poverty. Addiction, 103, 1283–1293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mustanski BS, Viken RJ, Kaprio J, & Rose RJ (2003). Genetic influences on the association between personality risk factors and alcohol use and abuse. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(2), 282–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peirce RS, Frone MR, Russell M, Cooper ML, & Mudar P (2000). A longitudinal model of social contact, social support, depression, and alcohol use. Health Psychology, 19, 28–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plomin RJ, DeFries JC, & Loehlin JC (1977). Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 309–322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Procidano ME & Heller K (1983). Measures of perceived social support from friends and from family: Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11(1), 1–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salvatore JE, Prom-Wormley E, Prescott CA, & Kendler KS (2015). Overlapping genetic and environmental influences among men’s alcohol consumption and problems, romantic quality, and socials support. Psychological Medicine, 45, 2353–2364. doi: 10.1017/S003329171500029X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sargent JD, Tanski S, Stoolmiller M, & Hanewinkel R (2010). Using sensation seeking to target adolescents for substance use interventions. Addiction, 105(3), 506–514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savage JE, Salvatore JE, Aliev F, Edwards AC, Hickman M, Kendler KS, . . . Dick DM (2018). Polygenic risk score prediction of alcohol dependence symptoms across population-based and clinically ascertained samples. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 42, 520–530 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scarr S, & McCartney K (1983). How do people make their own environment? A theory of gene-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segrin C (2003). Age moderates the relationship between social support and psychosocial problems. Human Communication Research, 29(3), 317–342. [Google Scholar]
- Sher KJ, Walitzer KS, Wood PK, & Brent EE (1991). Characteristics of children of alcoholics: Putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(4), 427–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slutske WS, Heath AC, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Statham DJ, & Martin NG (2002). Personality and the genetic risk for alcohol dependence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 124–133. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.1.124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steptoe A, Wardle J, Pollard TM, Canaan L, & Davies GJ (1996). Stress, social support and health-related behavior: a study of smoking, alcohol consumption and physical exercise. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41(2), 171–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaux A (1985). Variations in social support associated with gender, ethnicity, and age. Journal of Social issues, 41(1), 89–110. [Google Scholar]
- Verhulst B, Neale MC, & Kendler KS (2015). The heritability of alcohol use disorders: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Medicine, 45, 1061–1072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walen HR, & Lachman ME (2000). Social support and strain from partner, family, and friends: Costs and benefits for men and women in adulthood. Journal of social and personal relationships, 17(1), 5–30. [Google Scholar]
- Wang R, Davis O, Wootton RE, Mottershaw A, & Haworth C (2017). Social support and mental health in late adolescence are correlated for genetic, as well as environmental, reasons. Scientific reports, 7(1), 13088. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13449-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. (2018). Global status report on alcohol and health - 2018. Geneva, Switzerland. [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M (1984). Sensation seeking: A comparative approach to a human trait. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(3), 413–434. [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M, Eysenck SB, & Eysenck HJ (1978). Sensation seeking in England and America: Cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(1), 139–149. https://doi-org.proxy.library.vcu.edu/10.1037/0022-006X.46.1.139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.