Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Feb 23.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Med. 2010 May 12;41(1):33–40. doi: 10.1017/S003329171000084X

Psychiatric “diseases” versus behavioral disorders and degree of genetic influence

O Joseph Bienvenu 1, Dimitry S Davydow 2, Kenneth S Kendler 3
PMCID: PMC10885735  NIHMSID: NIHMS1965635  PMID: 20459884

Abstract

Background:

Psychiatric conditions in which symptoms arise involuntarily (“diseases”) might be assumed to be more heritable than those in which choices are essential (behavioral disorders). We sought to determine whether psychiatric “diseases” (Alzheimer’s Disease, schizophrenia, and mood and anxiety disorders) are more heritable than behavioral disorders (substance use disorders and anorexia nervosa).

Methods:

We reviewed the literature for recent quantitative summaries of heritabilities. When these were unavailable, we calculated weighted mean heritabilities from twin studies meeting modern methological standards.

Results:

Heritability summary estimates were as follows: bipolar disorder (85%), schizophrenia (81%), Alzheimer’s Disease (75%), cocaine use disorder (72%), anorexia nervosa (60%), alcohol dependence (56%), sedative use disorder (51%), cannabis use disorder (48%), panic disorder (43%), stimulant use disorder (40%), major depressive disorder (37%), and generalized anxiety disorder (28%).

Conclusions:

No systematic relationship exists between the disease-like character of a psychiatric disorder and its heritability; many behavioral disorders appear more heritable than conditions commonly construed as diseases. These results suggest an error in “common-sense” assumptions about the etiology of psychiatric disorders. That is, among psychiatric disorders, there is no close relationship between the strength of genetic influences and the etiologic importance of volitional processes.

Introduction

Psychiatric patients present with a variety of kinds of difficulties. One way to coherently address this diversity is to recognize and use distinct methods of psychiatric reasoning for particular kinds of difficulties (Ghaemi, 2003, McHugh, 2005, McHugh and Slavney, 1998). One of the most intuitive distinctions in reasoning is between that for psychiatric conditions that manifest primarily with involuntary symptoms and that for conditions that manifest primarily as dysfunctional behaviors.

“Disease” reasoning in psychiatry generally assumes some primary disruption in brain functioning; thus, as noted by McHugh and Slavney, a disease is something a patient has (McHugh and Slavney, 1998). Some diseases with prominent psychiatric symptoms have a known histopathology (e.g., Alzheimer’s Disease), and some even a known etiology (e.g., Huntington’s Disease), while in other putative psychiatric diseases these characteristics are less clear. For example, psychiatrists routinely employ disease reasoning in conceptualizing problems of patients with schizophrenia or mania; the spontaneous nature of the symptoms and their discontinuity from normal human experience suggest a primary disruption in brain functioning (McHugh and Slavney, 1998). Disease logic is also often used in conceptualizing the emergence of depressive or anxiety syndromes (McHugh and Slavney, 1998, Sheehan, 1983).

In contrast, “behavioral” reasoning assumes that patients are ill because of what they are doing (McHugh and Slavney, 1998); thus, behavioral reasoning is strongly tied to individual choices, their antecedents, and their consequences. Some behavioral disorders, e.g., alcohol and drug use disorders, are usually construed as out-of-control motivated behaviors. Other behavioral disorders, like anorexia nervosa, are usually construed as being shaped by culture and social learning (McHugh and Slavney, 1998). Clinicians typically treat behavioral disorders by attempting to persuade patients to choose to alter their behaviors (with help in disrupting the physiologic drives and other factors that provoke and sustain the behaviors). Thus, behavioral reasoning, in particular, invokes learning principles.

A key distinction between these methods of reasoning is the role of volition. That is, symptoms of psychiatric diseases – memory disturbance, hallucinations, and emotional disturbance – are typically understood as arising involuntarily. In contrast, behavioral disorders centrally involve voluntary control (though undoubtedly persons do not choose behavioral disorders). “Common sense” might suggest that neurobiological and genetic factors should be especially influential in the genesis of psychiatric syndromes that are disease-like in their manifestations, while other forces might have greater impact on the genesis of syndromes that manifest with disturbed behaviors. That is, common sense might suggest that any syndromes that centrally involve human decision making (Do I take this next drink? Do I eat this donut?) should be less genetically influenced than syndromes in which symptoms appear unbidden. Such reasoning is consistent with the public’s notion that behavioral disorders like alcoholism are more “self-inflicted” than schizophrenia, depression, or Alzheimer’s Disease (Schomerus et al., 2006).

In recent years, genetic epidemiological studies of psychiatric syndromes have expanded to a sufficient degree to empirically examine this issue. We have reviewed the extant twin study literature to produce the best available estimates of the heritabilities of well-studied adult psychiatric syndromes, with the goal of answering the following simple question: are genetic influences consistently greater in disease-like psychiatric disorders than in syndromes of which the major manifestation is disordered behavior?

Method

For the purposes of this review, we drew upon recent quantitative summaries of the heritabilities of the disorders of interest, if available. To identify relevant studies of phenotypes not previously summarized, we performed PubMed searches (e.g., “Alzheimer Disease [MeSH Terms] AND twins [MeSH Terms]”), and we searched reference lists of source and review articles. We selected reports that met modern methodological standards for twin studies; i.e., they used systematic ascertainment, data collection, and diagnostic procedures, as well as blinding to co-twin diagnostic status (and/or zygosity). In addition, we limited our focus to categorical phenotypes for which heritability had been estimated in at least three separate samples. If more than one study reported on identical participants, we only included the most informative with regard to heritability. We calculated mean heritability estimates, weighted by the number of pairs in which at least one twin was affected, for the tables below. If the number of pairs with an affected twin was not reported, we calculated mean heritability estimates weighted by the total number of affected twins in each study. As used in this study, heritability is the proportion of individual differences in liability to an illness (in a particular population) that results from genetic differences between individuals.

Results

Diseases

We know of no previous quantitative summary of the heritability of Alzheimer’s Disease. Table 1 lists the five studies that met our inclusion criteria (Bergem et al., 1997, Breitner et al., 1995, Gatz et al., 2005, Gatz et al., 1997, Gatz et al., 2006, Meyer and Breitner, 1998, Raiha et al., 1996). The most comprehensive investigation, with the largest number of affected twins, is the recent study by Gatz et al. based on the Swedish Twin Registry (Gatz et al., 2005, Gatz et al., 2006). There was some overlap, in terms of affected subjects, with a smaller previous study based on that registry (Gatz et al., 1997). The weighted mean heritability estimate was 75%, whether or not we included the earlier study (Gatz et al., 1997).

Table 1.

Twin studies of Alzheimer’s Disease

Study Country Age (years) Basis for Diagnosis Diagnostic Criteria Number
of Pairs a
Heritability
Breitner et al., 1995
Meyer and Breitner, 1998
U.S.A. 62-73 Nurse/physician examination, neuropsychological testing, and laboratory tests NINCDS-ADRDA,b DSM-III-R, or CERAD and NIA criteria for post-mortem diagnosis 36 74% c
Raiha et al., 1996
Pedersen et al., 2001
Finland ≥ 18 Hospital discharge medical record review NINCDS-ADRDA, DSM-III-R 84 63% d
Bergem et al., 1997
Pedersen et al., 2001
Norway ≥ 65 Physician examination, laboratory tests NINCDS-ADRDA, DSM-III-R 32 58% d
Gatz et al., 1997
Pedersen et al., 2001
Sweden ≥ 54 Physician examination, neuropsychological testing, and laboratory tests NINCDS-ADRDA, DSM-III-R 40 78% d
Gatz et al., 2005 and 2006 Sweden ≥ 65 Physician examination, neuropsychological testing, and laboratory tests NINCDS-ADRDA, DSM-IV 392 79%
Summary 75%
a

at least one twin affected

b

National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association

c

Best-fitting multiple-threshold (age at onset) model by Akaike Information Criteria (note: the fit of models with no additive genetic component, only a shared environmental component, or a smaller additive genetic component plus a shared environmental component, fit almost as well). Models assumed that all subjects would develop Alzheimer’s Disease if subjects lived long enough.

d

Estimated using a single-threshold model, not accounting for age (potentially inflates the estimate of a shared environmental component) (Pedersen et al., 2001).

Sullivan et al. recently performed a meta-analysis of published twin studies of schizophrenia, incorporating ascertainment corrections (Sullivan et al., 2003). Though only four of the twelve twin studies met modern methodologic standards, results did not vary significantly between studies that did and did not meet these standards. The point estimate of heritability was 81%.

We know of no previous quantitative summary of the heritability of bipolar disorder employing twin studies that meet modern methological standards. Table 2 lists the three studies that met our inclusion criteria (Kendler et al., 1993, Kendler et al., 1995, Kieseppa et al., 2004, McGuffin et al., 2003). Note that the affected subjects in the Maudsley study by Cardno et al (Cardno et al., 1999) were included in the later study by McGuffin et al. (McGuffin et al., 2003). The weighted mean heritability estimate was 85%.

Table 2.

Twin studies of bipolar disorder

Study Country Age (years) Basis for Diagnosis Diagnostic
Criteria
Number
of Pairs a
Heritability
Kendler et al., 1993 and 1995 Sweden 37-57 Questionnaire DSM-III-R 35 79%
McGuffin et al., 2003 U.K. ≥ 15 Medical record review,+/− research interview, outside informants DSM-IV 67 85%
Kiesseppa et al., 2004 Finland 53 (±13) b Medical record review, diagnostic interview, +/− outside informants DSM-IV 25 93%
Summary 85%
a

at least one twin affected

b

mean ± standard deviation

Sullivan et al. conducted a meta-analysis of published twin studies of major depressive disorder in 2000; five studies met inclusion criteria (Sullivan et al., 2000). The point estimate of heritability was 37%. Kendler et al., in a more recent large Swedish twin study, reported a similar heritability estimate for lifetime major depressive disorder (38%) (Kendler et al., 2006b).

Hettema et al. conducted a meta-analysis of published twin studies of panic disorder; three studies met inclusion criteria. The point estimate of heritability was 43% (Hettema et al., 2001a).

Though Hettema et al. meta-analyzed two published twin studies of generalized anxiety disorder (Hettema et al., 2001a), the authors excluded one eligible study because of its complex ascertainment scheme (Roy et al., 1995), and an additional eligible study was published since the meta-analysis (Mackintosh et al., 2006). Table 3 lists the four studies that met our inclusion criteria (Hettema et al., 2001b, Mackintosh et al., 2006, Roy et al., 1995, Scherrer et al., 2000); one study only included males (Scherrer et al., 2000). The weighted mean heritability estimate was 28%.

Table 3.

Twin studies of generalized anxiety disorder

Study Country Age (years) Basis for Diagnosis Diagnostic
Criteria
Number
Affected a
Heritability
Roy et al., 1995 Sweden 53 (±13) b Questionnaire DSM-III-R, broadened 128 49%
Scherrer et al., 2000 U.S.A. ≥ 37 Diagnostic interview DSM-III-R, broadened 827 37%
Hettema et al., 2001 U.S.A. ≥ 20 Diagnostic interview DSM-III-R, broadened 1,173 22%
Mackintosh et al., 2006 Sweden ≥ 55 Diagnostic interview DSM-III-R, broadened 1,415 27%
Summary 28%
a

among subjects in complete pairs, estimated

b

mean ± standard deviation

Behavioral Disorders

Goldman et al. recently reviewed large unselected twin studies of substance use disorders (Goldman et al., 2005). Weighted mean heritability estimates were as follows: cannabis use disorder (abuse or dependence), 43% (five studies); sedative use disorder, 51% (three studies); stimulant use disorder, 40% (three studies); cocaine use disorder, 72% (three studies); and alcohol dependence, 56% (five studies). We identified one more recent eligible study of cannabis use disorder (Kendler et al., 2006a), and, in this study, the heritability estimate was higher than most previous studies (77%); including it (total of 6 studies), the weighted mean heritability estimate was 48%.

We know of no previous quantitative summary of the heritability of anorexia nervosa. Table 4 lists the three studies that met our inclusion criteria (Bulik et al., 2006, Klump et al., 2001, Wade et al., 2000); these studies only included women. The weighted mean heritability estimate was 60%.

Table 4.

Twin studies of anorexia nervosa

Study Country Age (years) Basis for Diagnosis Diagnostic
Criteria
Number
of Pairs a
Heritability
Wade et al., 2000 USA ≥ 17 Diagnostic interview DSM-III-R, broadened 71 58% b
Klump et al., 2001 USA 16-18 Diagnostic interview, questionnaire DSM-IV, broadened 23 74%
Bulik et al., 2006 Sweden ≥ 40 Diagnostic interview, medical record review, questionnaire DSM-IV 49 56%
Summary 60%
a

at least one twin affected

b

estimated in best-fitting bivariate model with major depressive disorder

Summary

Figure 1 shows heritability summary estimates for psychiatric diseases/disease-like conditions and behavioral disorders. Bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, Alzheimer’s Disease, and cocaine use disorder had the highest heritabilities of the phenotypes addressed in this report – all 70% or higher. The phenotypes with the next highest heritabilities (40 – 60%) were anorexia nervosa, alcohol dependence, sedative use disorder, cannabis use disorder, panic disorder, and stimulant use disorder. The least heritable phenotypes (heritabilities < 40%) were major depressive disorder and generalized anxiety disorder.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Heritability summary estimates for psychiatric diseases/disease-like conditions (black) and behavioral disorders (grey)

Discussion

Our results do not strongly support the prediction that psychiatric “diseases” are more heritable than behavioral disorders. On the one hand, Alzheimer’s Disease and two disease-like conditions, schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, have very high heritabilities. On the other hand, panic, major depressive, and generalized anxiety disorders have substantially lower heritabilities, lower than those of most of the behavioral disorders examined. Thus, there does not appear to be a systematic relationship between the disease-like character of a psychiatric disorder and its heritability. Indeed, idiopathic Parkinson’s Disease, another neuropsychiatric condition with a known histopathology, does not appear particularly heritable (Wirdefeldt et al., 2004).

It might be argued that major depression and anxiety disorders should not be considered “disease-like,” as these conditions are frequently accompanied by long-standing personality vulnerabilities (Bienvenu et al., 2004, Bienvenu et al., 2009, Roth et al., 1972). Major depression is likely a heterogeneous condition, including a more disease-like “endogenous” form and a less disease-like “neurotic” form (Roth, 2001, Shorter, 2009). Though some studies suggest that endogenous features are associated with higher heritability (Leckman et al., 1984a, Leckman et al., 1984b, McGuffin et al., 1996), aspects of depressive illness that appear particularly salient for heritability include the related features recurrence, early onset, and perhaps short duration (Kendler et al., 2007). When adjusted for the latter features, endogenous symptoms do not appear to uniquely predict heritability (Kendler et al., 1994, McGuffin et al., 1996, Weissman et al., 1986). In addition, the presence of comorbid anxiety disorders appears to increase, rather than decrease, the heritability of depressive illness (Kendler et al., 1994, Weissman et al., 1986).

Our results are inconsistent with the notion that psychiatric problems can be divided accurately into those which are “biologically” caused and involuntary versus those which involve choices. That is, our findings support the proposition that biological/genetic factors influence volitional behavior, though it seems unlikely that genes simply evoke particular problem behaviors. The mechanisms through which genetic factors influence behavior are probably numerous. For example, genes can alter the hedonic and/or adverse effects of cannabis (Lyons et al., 1997) and alcohol (Thomasson and Li, 1993), making it more or less pleasant to consume these substances. Genes can also influence levels of sensation-seeking (Agrawal et al., 2004), making some individuals more prone to behaviors like drug use that are rewarding in the short term but harmful in the long term. Other heritable temperamental traits may similarly influence the development of eating disorders (Wade et al., 2008). While personal decisions are no doubt important in behavioral disorders, genes clearly influence their development. Thus, genes apparently do not respect the boundary between “free will” and “determinism.”

One final implication of our findings is noteworthy. When we think about diseases in psychiatry, we often think not only about high heritability but also about low levels of personal responsibility and blame-worthiness. By contrast, when we consider behavioral disorders (especially substance use disorders), we sometimes think not only of reduced genetic influences, but also of higher levels of personal responsibility and blame-worthiness. After all, since behavior is under at least partial voluntary control, it seems natural to assume that a behavioral disorder is “the patient’s fault.” Indirectly, our findings challenge this assumption. Given the apparent lack of a consistent relationship between heritability and disease versus disturbed behavior, should we also re-examine our conception about the relationship between disease and disturbed behavior on the one hand and moral responsibility and blame-worthiness on the other? Our results emphasize the important distinction between stigmatizing destructive behaviors, in a treatment context, and stigmatizing patients in a social (especially medical) context. Unfortunately, persons with behavioral disorders like alcoholism are stigmatized, and not just by the general public (Farrell and Lewis, 1990, Schomerus et al., 2006, To and Vega, 2006). Given that vulnerability to behavioral disorders is under substantial genetic control, our results emphasize the inappropriateness of libertarian attitudes towards these devastating conditions. That is, we humans do not appear equally free in our decisions.

Limitations

Three limitations deserve comment. First, it is important to recognize that the genetic influence on a phenotype (heritability) is not immutable; it is dependent upon the environment. Indeed, it is most accurate to characterize heritability of an illness in terms of a specific population at a particular point in time (Kendler, 2005). Since different environmental factors may be relevant for different psychiatric illnesses in particular populations, it is difficult to address this limitation effectively. Nevertheless, within cohorts, the pattern of results in Figure 1 appears consistent. For example, in studies using Swedish Twin Registry data with the same or overlapping birth cohorts, heritability estimates were as follows: bipolar disorder, 79%; Alzheimer’s Disease, 78-79%; anorexia nervosa, 56%; major depressive disorder, 38%; and generalized anxiety disorder, 27% (Bulik et al., 2006, Gatz et al., 1997, Gatz et al., 2006, Kendler et al., 2006b, Kendler et al., 1995, Mackintosh et al., 2006). Also, in studies using adult Virginia Twin Registry data, heritability estimates were as follows: drug use disorder, 66%; alcohol dependence, 49%; major depressive disorder, 39%; panic disorder, 37%; and generalized anxiety disorder, 22% (Hettema et al., 2001b, Kendler et al., 2001, Kendler and Prescott, 1999, Kendler et al., 2003).

Second, unreliability – a form of measurement error – is confounded with unique environmental effects in twin models, and some diagnoses appear more reliable than others in unselected samples. For example, in studies of adults employing the Virginia Twin Registry, the reliabilities of lifetime diagnoses of panic, major depressive, and generalized anxiety disorders tended to be lower (κ = 0.34-0.56) than those of cocaine, sedative, cannabis, and stimulant use disorders (κ = 0.47-0.68) (Foley et al., 1998, Hettema et al., 2001b, Kendler et al., 2001, Kendler et al., 2000). Since no psychiatric diagnosis is made with 100% reliability, heritability estimates tend to be underestimates, but the heritabilities of the disease-like conditions above would be expected to be slightly more underestimated than those of the substance use disorders. Nevertheless, it is unlikely that correcting heritability estimates for unreliability would substantially reorder the results in Figure 1. In fact, the reliability of cocaine dependence (κ = 0.47) was slightly lower than that of panic disorder (κ = 0.56) in the studies cited above, yet the heritability estimates from these studies were 0.79 and 0.37, respectively, for these diagnoses.

Third, etiologic heterogeneity is likely in psychiatric disorders, so our heritability estimates inevitability reflect an averaging across potentially different etiologies. For example, as noted above, certain clinical aspects of major depressive disorder appear associated with higher heritability, including recurrence and early age-at-onset (Levinson et al., 2003, Sullivan et al., 2000). However, it is not clear that problems of etiologic heterogeneity would impact heritability estimates differentially for disease-like versus behavioral conditions.

Acknowledgements

The US National Institutes of Health grant K23-MH64543 (Dr. Bienvenu) supported this study. The authors thank Drs. Paul McHugh, Phillip Slavney, Gerald Nestadt, and anonymous reviewers for helpful comments regarding this project. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Agrawal A, Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (2004). A twin study of personality and illicit drug use and abuse/dependence. Twin Research 7, 72–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bergem ALM, Engedal K, Kringlen E (1997). The role of heredity in late-onset Alzheimer Disease and vascular dementia. Archives of General Psychiatry 54, 264–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bienvenu OJ, Samuels JF, Costa PT, Reti IM, Eaton WW, Nestadt G (2004). Anxiety and depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality: a higher- and lower-order personality trait investigation in a community sample. Depression and Anxiety 20, 92–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bienvenu OJ, Stein MB, Samuels JF, Onyike CU, Eaton WW, Nestadt G (2009). Personality disorder traits as predictors of subsequent first-onset panic disorder or agoraphobia. Comprehensive Psychiatry 50, 209–214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Breitner JCS, Welsh KA, Gau BA, McDonald WM, Steffens DC, Saunders AM, Magruder KM, Helms MJ, Plassman BL, Folstein MF, Brandt J, Robinette CD, Page WF (1995). Alzheimer’s Disease in the National Academy of Sciences - National Research Council Registry of Aging Twin Veterans III: detection of cases, longitudinal results, and observations on twin concordance. Archives of Neurology 52, 763–771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bulik CM, Sullivan PF, Tozzi F, Furberg H, Lichtenstein P, Pedersen NL (2006). Prevalence, heritability, and prospective risk factors for anorexia nervosa. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 305–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cardno AG, Marshall EJ, Coid B, Macdonald AM, Ribchester TR, Davies NJ, Venturi P, Jones LA, Lewis SW, Sham PC, Gottesman II, Farmer AE, McGuffin P, Reveley AM, Murray RM (1999). Heritability estimates for psychotic disorders: the Maudsley twin psychosis series. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 162–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Farrell M, Lewis G (1990). Discrimination on the grounds of diagnosis. British Journal of Addiction 85, 883–890. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Foley DL, Neale MC, Kendler KS (1998). Reliability of a lifetime history of major depression: implications for heritability and co-morbidity. Psychological Medicine 28, 857–870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Gatz M, Fratiglioni L, Johansson B, Berg S, Mortimer JA, Reynolds CA, Fiske A, Pedersen N (2005). Complete ascertainment of dementia in the Swedish Twin Registry: the HARMONY study. Neurobiology of Aging 26, 439–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gatz M, Pedersen NL, Berg S, Johansson B, Johansson K, Mortimer JA, Posner SF, Viitanen M, Winblad B, Ahlbom A (1997). Heritability for Alzheimer’s Disease: the study of dementia in Swedish twins. Journals of Gerontology, Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 52, M117–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gatz M, Reynolds CA, Fratiglioni L, Johansson B, Mortimer JA, Berg S, Fiske A, Pedersen NL (2006). Role of genes and environments for explaining Alzheimer disease. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 168–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Ghaemi SN (2003). The Concepts of Psychiatry. Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD. [Google Scholar]
  14. Goldman D, Oroszi G, Ducci F (2005). The genetics of addictions: uncovering the genes. Nature Reviews Genetics 6, 521–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS (2001a). A review and meta-analysis of the genetic epidemiology of anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 1568–1578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hettema JM, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (2001b). A population-based twin study of generalized anxiety disorder in men and women. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 189, 413–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kendler KS (2005). Psychiatric genetics: a methodologic critique. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 3–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kendler KS, Aggen SH, Tambs K, Reichborn-Kjennerud T (2006a). Illicit psychoactive substance use, abuse and dependence in a population-based sample of Norwegian twins. Psychological Medicine 36, 955–962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Kendler KS, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pedersen NL (2007). Clinical indices of familial depression in the Swedish Twin Registry. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 115, 214–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kendler KS, Gardner CO, Prescott CA (2001). Panic syndromes in a population-based sample of male and female twins. Psychological Medicine 31, 989–1000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kendler KS, Gatz M, Gardner CO, Pedersen NL (2006b). A Swedish national twin study of lifetime major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 109–114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Neale MC, Prescott CA (2000). Illicit psychoactive substance use, heavy use, abuse, and dependence in a US population-based sample of male twins. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 261–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ (1994). The clinical characteristics of major depression as indices of the familial risk to illness. British Journal of Psychiatry 165, 66–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kendler KS, Pedersen N, Johnson L, Neale MC, Mathe AA (1993). A pilot twin study of affective illness, including hospital- and population-ascertained subsamples. Archives of General Psychiatry 50, 699–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kendler KS, Pedersen NL, Neale MC, Mathe AA (1995). A pilot Swedish twin study of affective illness including hospital- and population-ascertained subsamples: results of model fitting. Behavior Genetics 25, 217–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kendler KS, Prescott CA (1999). A population-based twin study of lifetime major depression in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 39–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC (2003). The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 929–937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kieseppa T, Partonen T, Haukka J, Kaprio J, Lonnqvist J (2004). High concordance of bipolar I disorder in a nationwide sample of twins. American Journal of Psychiatry 161, 1814–1821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Klump KL, Miller KB, Keel PK, McGue M, Iacono WG (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on anorexia nervosa syndromes in a population-based twin sample. Psychological Medicine 31, 737–740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Leckman JF, Caruso KA, Prusoff BA, Weissman MM, Merikangas KR, Pauls DL (1984a). Appetite disturbance and excessive guilt in major depression: use of family study data to define depressive subtypes. Archives of General Psychiatry 41, 839–844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Leckman JF, Weissman MM, Prusoff BA, Caruso KA, Merikangas KR, Pauls DL, Kidd KK (1984b). Subtypes of depression: family study perspective. Archives of General Psychiatry 41, 833–838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Levinson DF, Zubenko GS, Crowe RR, Depaulo JR, Scheftner WS, Weissman MM, Holmans P, Zubenko WN, Boutelle S, Murphy-Eberenz K, MacKinnon D, McInnis MG, Marta DH, Adams P, Sassoon S, Knowles JA, Thomas J, Chellis J (2003). Genetics of recurrent early-onset depression (GenRED): design and preliminary clinical characteristics of a repository sample for genetic linkage studies. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 119B, 118–130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lyons MJ, Toomey R, Meyer JM, Green AI, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, True WR, Tsuang MT (1997). How do genes influence marijuana use? The role of subjective effects. Addiction 92, 409–417. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Mackintosh M-A, Gatz M, Wetherell JL, Pedersen NL (2006). A twin study of lifetime generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) in older adults: genetic and environmental influences shared by neuroticism and GAD. Twin Research and Human Genetics 9, 30–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McGuffin P, Katz R, Watkins S, Rutherford J (1996). A hospital-based twin register of the heritability of DSM-IV unipolar depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 53, 129–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McGuffin P, Rijsdijk F, Andrew M, Sham P, Katz R, Cardno A (2003). The heritability of bipolar affective disorder and the genetic relationship to unipolar depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 497–502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. McHugh PR (2005). Striving for coherence: psychiatry’s efforts over classification. Journal of the American Medical Association 293, 2526–2528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McHugh PR, Slavney PR (1998). The Perspectives of Psychiatry. Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD. [Google Scholar]
  39. Meyer JM, Breitner JCS (1998). Multiple threshold model for the onset of Alzheimer’s Disease in the NAS-NRC Twin Panel. American Journal of Medical Genetics 81, 92–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Pedersen NL, Posner SF, Gatz M (2001). Multiple-threshold models for genetic influences on age of onset for Alzheimer’s Disease: findings in Swedish twins. American Journal of Medical Genetics 105, 724–728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Raiha I, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M, Rajala T, Sourander L (1996). Alzheimer’s Disease in Finnish twins. Lancet 347, 573–578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Roth M (2001). Unitary or binary nature of classification of depressive illness and its implications for the scope of manic depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 64, 1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Roth M, Gurney C, Garside RF, Kerr TA (1972). Studies in the classification of affective disorders: the relationship between anxiety states and depressive illnesses: I. British Journal of Psychiatry 121, 147–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Roy M-A, Neale MC, Pedersen NL, Mathe AA, Kendler KS (1995). A twin study of generalized anxiety disorder and major depression. Psychological Medicine 25, 1037–1049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Scherrer JF, True WR, Xian H, Lyons MJ, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, Lin N, Tsuang MT (2000). Evidence for genetic influences common and specific to symptoms of generalized anxiety and panic. Journal of Affective Disorders 57, 25–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Schomerus G, Matschinger H, Angermeyer MC (2006). Alcoholism: illness beliefs and resource allocation preferences of the public. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 82, 204–210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sheehan DV (1983). The Anxiety Disease. Bantam Books: New York. [Google Scholar]
  48. Shorter E (2009). Before Prozac: The Troubled History of Mood Disorders in Psychiatry. Oxford University Press, Inc.: New York, NY. [Google Scholar]
  49. Sullivan PF, Kendler KS, Neale MC (2003). Schizophrenia as a complex trait: evidence from a meta-analysis of twin studies. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 1187–1192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sullivan PF, Neale MC, Kendler KS (2000). Genetic epidemiology of major depression: review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 1552–1562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Thomasson HR, Li TK (1993). How alcohol and aldehyde dehydrogenase genes modify alcohol drinking, alcohol flushing, and the risk for alcoholism. Alcohol Health and Research World 17, 167–172. [Google Scholar]
  52. To SE, Vega CP (2006). Alcoholism and pathways to recovery: new survey results on views and treatment options. Medscape General Medicine 8, 2. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wade TD, Bulik CM, Neale M, Kendler KS (2000). Anorexia nervosa and major depression: shared genetic and environmental risk factors. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 469–471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wade TD, Tiggemann M, Bulik CM, Fairburn CG, Wray NR, Martin NG (2008). Shared temperament risk factors for anorexia nervosa: a twin study. Psychosomatic Medicine 70, 239–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Weissman MM, Merikangas KR, Wickramaratne P, Kidd KK, Prusoff BA, Leckman JF, Pauls DL (1986). Understanding the clinical heterogeneity of major depression using family data. Archives of General Psychiatry 43, 430–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Wirdefeldt K, Gatz M, Schalling M, Pedersen NL (2004). No evidence for heritability of Parkinson disease in Swedish twins. Neurology 63, 305–311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES