Abstract
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders diagnosed in childhood. Two common features of ADHD are impaired behavioural inhibition and sustained attention. The Go/No-Go experimental paradigm with concurrent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning has previously revealed important neurobiological correlates of ADHD such as the supplementary motor area and the prefrontal cortex. The coordinate-based meta-analysis combined with quantitative techniques, such as activation likelihood estimate (ALE) generation, provides an unbiased and objective method of summarising these data to understand the brain network architecture and connectivity in ADHD children. Go/No-Go task-based fMRI studies involving children and adolescent subjects were selected. Coordinates indicating foci of activation were collected to generate ALEs using threshold values (voxel-level: p < 0.001; cluster-level: p < 0.05). ALEs were matched to one of seven canonical brain networks based on the cortical parcellation scheme derived from the Human Connectome Project. Fourteen studies involving 457 children met the eligibility criteria. No significant convergence of Go/No-Go related brain activation was found for ADHD groups. Three significant ALE clusters were detected for brain activation relating to controls or ADHD < controls. Significant clusters were related to specific areas of the default mode network (DMN). Network-based analysis revealed less extensive DMN, dorsal attention network, and limbic network activation in ADHD children compared to controls. The presence of significant ALE clusters may be due to reduced homogeneity in the selected sample demographic and experimental paradigm. Further investigations regarding hemispheric asymmetry in ADHD subjects would be beneficial.
Keywords: ADHD, Go/No-Go, connectivity, networks, ALE, meta-analysis, fMRI
1. Significance statement
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders diagnosed in childhood, with key features of impaired behavioural inhibition and sustained attention. However, there has been no neuroanatomical specificity regarding the localisation of motor inhibition; inhibitory deficits have been attributed to generalised brain areas and even entire lobes. This article quantitatively consolidates effects across various Go/No-Go experiments into holistic, network-based nomenclature to be easily understood by clinicians in the field, with an aim to translate these outcomes in the field of paediatrics, psychiatry, and neurology. We used a coordinate-based meta-analysis that specifically focuses on motor inhibitory coordinates during a Go-/No-Go task in the ADHD paediatric population, ensuring an objective, statistically based approach in an effort to reduce heterogenicity seen in previous task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies. Through this approach, we aimed to enhance clinical understanding of the neural correlates of ADHD.
2. Introduction
ADHD is a prevalent neuropsychiatric condition that is frequently diagnosed in childhood. It is characterized by a persistent pattern of age-inappropriate behaviour such as inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity, leaving profound impacts on development. Since its first recognition in the DSM classification in the 1980s, significant work in the fields of behavioural science and neuropsychiatry advanced and refined the understanding of the ADHD phenotype. In particular, the use of fMRI during cognitively demanding tasks such as the Go/No-Go paradigm has shed light on the neural correlates of certain functions, which may be impaired in patients with ADHD relative to controls. Indeed, various fMRI investigations have attributed deficits in motor inhibition and selective attention during the Go/No-Go task to decreased activation in the anterior and posterior cingulate, large portions of the frontal cortex, the supplementary motor area (SMA), the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and the basal ganglia to list a few anatomical areas [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8]
However, these earlier task-based fMRI investigations can often yield inconsistent results, perhaps due to minor differences in the performance of the task and the sample demographic. Furthermore, recent meta-analyses that seek to summarise the myriad of task-based fMRI data in the literature report a significant degree of heterogeneity, attributing the core neurological deficits in ADHD to an expansive list of regions spanning various cortical and subcortical areas [9,10,11,12,13]. Some have suggested that this inconsistency is due to the inherent differences between neural mechanisms underlying children and adults with ADHD who are often grouped in meta-analyses [13,14,15]. Others have suggested that there is an increasing need to deviate away from abnormalities in discrete anatomical brain regions and instead shift the focus onto altered or aberrant brain connectivity between- and within-functional networks or neural circuitry [14,16,17]. This latter point is further supported by recent advances in brain mapping and connectomics that divide the brain into microscopic parcellations or nodes of interest, thus providing greater nuances and specificity to the neural correlates of ADHD beyond the level of macroscopic lobes or systems [18,19].
Considering the above, there is a need for an objective, statistically based approach to quantitatively synthesize relevant findings from the ADHD neuroimaging literature. While there have been a few meta-analyses for both task-based and resting-state fMRI for patients with ADHD, none has focused specifically on the motor inhibition correlates of children performing the Go/No-Go task. Here, we present a coordinate-based meta-analytical approach that generates activation likelihood estimates (ALEs) to ascertain probabilistic regions of interest that suggest a convergence of multiple studies finding hyper- or hypoactivation in the brain. This voxel-wise approach offers increased spatial resolution, where the foci of each ALE represent the area of the highest likelihood of activation as the centre of a three-dimensional Gaussian probability distribution [20,21]. We also labelled each focus according to Glasser’s atlas [18], a cortical parcellation scheme derived from the Human Connectome Project to provide a clinically relevant nomenclature and a network-based understanding of the neural architecture that may be involved in motor inhibition deficits in children with ADHD. Furthermore, we hypothesize that the reduced heterogeneity in this meta-analysis created by focusing specifically on children and the Go/No-Go task will yield significant results that would be relevant for the understanding of children with ADHD in the critical period of development.
3. Methods
3.1. Experimental design
This review was conducted according to the PRISMA 2020 Guidelines [22]. The review and the protocol were not registered. A preliminary search was made using the BrainMap Sleuth 3.0.4 functional imaging database in May 2023 matching the following criteria: “Imaging Modality = fMRI” and “Subject Diagnosis = ADHD.” Sleuth is software that algorithmically screens the BrainMap database for functional and structural neuroimaging results presented in 3-D stereotactic (x, y, z) coordinates which were appropriate for our purpose of conducting a coordinate-based meta-analysis [23,24]. Furthermore, through Sleuth’s archives, we were able to select particular studies which employed the Go/No-Go paradigm and our desired experimental contrasts of No-Go > Go which would provide fMRI foci of interests that correspond to the neural correlates of response inhibition. To cover the literature gap, an additional search in PubMed was conducted, from January 2000 up to May 2023, using the following search terms: “ADHD” AND “fMRI” AND “Go/No-Go.” Two authors independently screened each record. Studies were reviewed and included if they fulfilled the following search criteria: (1) peer-reviewed publication; (2) Go/No-Go paradigm-based fMRI study; (3) regions of interest presented as stereotactic coordinates in either Talairach or Montreal Neuroimaging Institute (MNI) coordinate space; (4) experimental contrasts must be No-Go > Go; (5) includes at least one healthy human control cohort for comparison. Studies were excluded according to the following criteria: (1) subjects’ age > 18; (2) studies tested the action of a variety of neuropsychiatric therapy, e.g. methylphenidate, atomoxetine, behavioural focus intervention, etc., with no baseline measurements; and (3) studies compared between ADHD subjects and subjects with other neurological or psychiatric diagnoses, e.g. autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or major depressive disorder. Only studies in English were included.
While formal bias assessment was not performed, given the strict inclusion and exclusion criteria used to ensure that the same methodology was used in each study, the risk of bias was low.
3.2. Statistical analysis
Coordinates provided in Talairach or MNI space indicating foci of strong convergence in No-Go tasks compared to Go tasks were collected for both ADHD and control groups. All data collection was performed by one author. These coordinates were categorized into four mutually exclusive groups (ADHD activation; controls activation; ADHD < controls; ADHD > controls) and then used to generate ALEs using GingerALE version 3.0.1 (http://www.brainmap.org/ale/) to analyse the probabilistic differences in brain network activation between ADHD and control subjects [20,21,25]. Following the recommended guidelines for coordinate-based neuroimaging studies [26], we performed a single-study analysis using cluster-level inference in MNI coordinate space (cluster-level: p < 0.05; vowel-level: p < 0.001; threshold permutation = 1,000) [20]. Furthermore, all Talairach coordinates were converted into MNI coordinate space before ALE generation using icbm2tal transform SPM conversion in GingerALE. We utilized the Multi-image Analysis GUI (Mango) 4.0.1 (ric.uthscsa.edu/mango) to overlay the ALE foci over an MNI-normalised brain image. To label each focus according to the cortical parcellation scheme derived from the Human Connectome Project18, a sphere was placed at the MNI coordinate of the centroid of each ALE cluster with a radius defined as 15 mm. The sphere was projected onto the HCP-MMP parcellation schema, which is also in MNI coordinates. The degree to which any local HCP parcellations fell within the ALE cluster was calculated as a percentage (percentage of the parcellation that falls within the ALE cluster). The parcellation that had the highest percentage of volume within the ALE cluster was designated as the equivalent HCP parcellation to that ALE cluster. As a result, we were able to map a region of interest suggesting hyper or hypoactivation during the Go/No-Go experiment to one of 360 subdivisions in the cortical architecture, providing specificity beyond the level of lobes and even sub-lobes. We then categorized each parcellation according to its core affiliate network based on Yeo et al.’s 7-network model of the human cerebral cortex [19]. That is, each parcellation was labelled as one of the following core networks: default mode network (DMN); central executive network (CEN); salience; dorsal attention network (DAN); limbic; sensorimotor and visual networks. “Independent group analyses” were conducted by examining the network affiliations of parcellations generated from studies that reported ADHD or control activation only. Similarly, “between-group analysis” was conducted by examining parcellations from studies that reported a comparison of ADHD and control subjects, i.e. “ADHD < controls” or “ADHD > controls.”
4. Results
Figure 1 presents a visual representation of the study retrieval process. From the total studies which have been algorithmically determined by Sleuth’s BrainMap database to include stereotactic coordinate results of Go/No-Go experiments, 14 eligible studies met our inclusion criteria [1,3,4,5,6,7,8,27,28,29,30,31,32,33]. Some studies were excluded due to a lack of control cohorts, comparison between children with ADHD and ASD [34], or testing of neuropsychiatric therapies such as methylphenidate. One study was excluded as it included the same subject population as another study already included in our study [35]. A summary of the main findings in terms of increased brain activation in ADHD and control subjects is given in Table 1. Moreover, the characteristics of each study and the extracted MNI coordinates can be found in Supplementary Table 1. In total, there were 457 included participants (ADHD = 224; controls = 232).
Table 1.
Study | Key findings | |
---|---|---|
↑Activation for ADHD | ↑Activation for controls | |
Durston et al. [4] | R superior frontal gyrus | L caudate |
R middle frontal gyrus | ||
R inferior parietal lobe | ||
R/L posterior cingulate | ||
R/L precuneus | ||
R superior temporal gyrus | ||
R/L occipital cortex | ||
Schulz et al. [27] | R/L middle frontal gyrus | R precentral gyrus |
R/L inferior frontal gyrus | R inferior temporal gyrus | |
L anterior cingulate | L superior temporal gyrus | |
R/L inferior posterior lobule | R fusiform gyrus | |
R precuneus | L hippocampus | |
R lingual gyrus | ||
R/L cerebellum | ||
Tamm et al. [7] | R/L inferior frontal gyrus | R superior frontal gyrus |
R/L frontal operculum | R middle frontal gyrus | |
R/L superior temporal gyrus | R inferior frontal gyrus | |
R/L middle temporal gyrus | R anterior cingulate | |
R/L inferior temporal gyrus | R SMA | |
L angular temporal gyrus | R angular gyrus | |
L anterior cingulate | R supramarginal gyrus | |
Booth et al. [1] | R posterior cingulate | R superior frontal gyrus |
R superior parietal lobule | R middle frontal gyrus | |
L superior temporal gyrus | R/L inferior frontal gyrus | |
L middle temporal gyrus | R/L precentral gyrus | |
L inferior temporal gyrus | L caudate | |
L insula | ||
L posterior cingulate | ||
L fusiform gyrus | ||
R amygdala | ||
Rubia et al. [8] | R superior temporal lobe | R/L infero-orbital prefrontal cortex |
L medial temporal lobe | R/L mesial prefrontal cortex | |
L anterior cingulate | R/L middle frontal gyrus | |
L posterior cingulate | R precentral cortex | |
R medial temporal lobe | ||
R/L parietal lobe | ||
R caudate | ||
L cerebellum | ||
Vaidya et al. [28] | R superior temporal gyrus | R premotor gyrus |
R insula | R/L caudate | |
Durston et al. [3] | R middle frontal gyrus | R superior frontal gyrus |
R IPL | R/L middle frontal gyrus | |
R/L inferior frontal gyrus | ||
R/L anterior cingulate | ||
L premotor cortex | ||
L IPL | ||
Pliszka et al. [5] | R/L insula | R inferior frontal gyrus |
R ventrolateral prefrontal cortex | R superior temporal gyrus | |
R superior temporal gyrus | R anterior cingulate | |
R/L occipital lobe | R/L ventrolateral prefrontal cortex | |
R inferior parietal lobe | R posterior parietal lobe | |
R/L precentral gyrus | ||
Suskauer et al. [29] | R medial frontal wall | R medial frontal wall |
R/L occipital lobe | ||
R DLPFC | ||
R temporal–parietal junction | ||
R cerebellum | ||
R putamen | ||
L precentral gyrus | ||
R fusiform gyrus | ||
R anterior cingulate | ||
L posterior cingulate | ||
L precuneus | ||
Spinelli et al. [30] | R superior frontal gyrus | R middle frontal gyrus |
R middle frontal gyrus | R superior occipital gyrus | |
L inferior frontal gyrus | R angular gyrus | |
L caudate | R middle temporal gyrus | |
L amygdala | R parahippocampal gyrus | |
L cerebellum | R hippocampus | |
L precuneus | ||
R posterior cingulate | ||
Ma et al. [31] | R inferior temporal gyrus | R middle frontal gyrus |
R midbrain | R inferior frontal gyrus | |
R precentral gyrus | R IPL | |
R postcentral gyrus | R SMA | |
R calcarine | ||
R/L middle occipital cortex | ||
R/L inferior occipital cortex | ||
R hippocampus | ||
Wang et al. [32] | L middle frontal gyrus | L anterior cingulate |
L middle temporal gyrus | R precentral gyrus | |
L middle occipital gyrus | L middle temporal gyrus | |
L putamen | R/L parahippocampus gyrus | |
L posterior cingulate | ||
L precuneus | ||
R/L angular gyrus | ||
R cerebellum | ||
Hart et al. [33] | L cerebellum | R/L ventrolateral prefrontal cortex |
L posterior cingulate | R/L superior temporal lobe | |
R/L middle temporal lobe | ||
L inferior temporal lobe | ||
L IPL | ||
R/L posterior cingulate | ||
R/L precuneus | ||
R/L basal ganglia | ||
Van Rooij et al. [6] | L superior frontal gyrus | |
L inferior frontal gyrus | ||
L supramarginal gyrus | ||
R post-central gyrus | ||
R/L temporal-parietal junction | ||
R/L anterior cingulate | ||
L supramarginal gyrus |
In terms of the ALE analysis, there were no significant clusters found at the thresholding value of 0.05 and a minimum cluster volume of 528 mm3 for both the “ADHD activation” and “ADHD > controls” groups. Two significant clusters, both located in the right cerebrum, were detected for the “control activation” group, receiving contributions from 9 foci [3,5,6,8,28]. Cluster 1 was centred at (x, y, z = 39.9, 21.7, −12.7) in MNI stereotactic space with a cluster volume of 976 mm3 and showed involvement from the inferior frontal gyrus (50%), insula (30%), and the claustrum (17.5%). Cluster 2 was centred at (x, y, z = 42.5, 9.5, 35.6) with a volume of 864 mm3 and showed involvement from the precentral gyrus (73.9%), middle frontal gyrus (21.7%), and the inferior frontal gyrus (4.3%). One significant cluster was detected for the “ADHD < controls” group, receiving contributions from three foci [3,6,33]. This cluster corresponded to the left inferior frontal gyrus, centred at (x, y, z = −37.4, 22.3, −15.5) with a volume of 584 mm [3]. Parcellation matching of the above clusters to Glasser’s atlas [18] correlated with area 47 l (x, y, z = 42, 23, −13) and 8Av (x, y, z = 44, 11, 38) in the right frontal lobe and area 47 m (x, y, z = −37, 29, −14) and 47 s (x, y, z = −37, 21, −14) in the left frontal lobe. These four regions of interest are marked in an MNI-normalized volumetric brain surface in Figure 2. Furthermore, un-thresholded ALEs mapped onto an MNI-normalized brain surface for each of the four groups (ADHD activation; controls activation; ADHD < controls; ADHD > controls) are shown in Figures 3–6, respectively.
Table 2 presents the matched Glasser’s parcellations and the affiliated brain network of each activated brain foci in the “ADHD activation” and “controls activation” groups. A network-level analysis shows no significant activation of the limbic network in children with ADHD and less extensive activation of the CEN compared to controls. Furthermore, children with ADHD predominately activated left-sided parcellations of DAN, while control subjects activated predominantly right-sided parcellations. Furthermore, control subjects demonstrated more extensive activation of the DMN. Parcellation p24 of the CEN network demonstrated unique asymmetry, activated on the left for ADHD and on the right for controls. Similar findings can be applied to area POS1 of the DMN. Additionally, the left LIPv of the DAN was activated for ADHD subjects while the right LIPv of the DAN was activated for both ADHD and control subjects. A similar pattern of lateralization can be applied to area FOP4 of the salience network.
Table 2.
ADHD only | Control only | Both ADHD and controls | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
R | L | R | L | R | L | |
CEN | PFm | p24 | p24 | 33pr | AAIC | AAIC |
31a | 8C | 8C | 7Pm | AVI | ||
PCV | 8BM | a10p | IFJa | IFJp | ||
PGs | a9-46v | 8Av | ||||
IP2 | i6-8 | RSC | ||||
TE1m | ||||||
Limbic | EC | 25 | ||||
Amygdala | Amygdala | |||||
H | H | |||||
DMN | 47m | 47m | 47l | 47l | 31pd | 31pd |
TPOJ2 | POS1 | POS1 | 55b | 9p | ||
STGa | 8BL | v23ab | 8Ad | |||
STV | 9m | STSvp | STSva | |||
PGi | PSL | TE1a | ||||
s32 | d32 | TPOJ1 | ||||
DAN | LIPd | LIPv | PEF | PEF | LIPv | |
TE2p | 6a | |||||
MST | AIP | |||||
Salience | FOP1 | FOP4 | PFop | MI | FOP4 | 9-46d |
9-46d | SCEF | PFcm | ||||
23c | p32pr | |||||
6r | 46 | |||||
a24pr | ||||||
PSL | ||||||
Visual | V1 | PIT | FFC | V3B | VMV1 | VMV1 |
V3 | V8 | V8 | V4 | |||
V3B | LO2 | |||||
IP0 | PGp | |||||
ProS | ||||||
TPOJ3 | ||||||
Sensorimotor | LBelt | MBelt | 2 | |||
3a | 52 |
Duplicate parcellations that appeared in both “ADHD” and “Controls” are listed in the third major column, “ADHD and controls”. Glasser’s parcellations matched with activated brain foci in ‘ADHD activation’ and ‘Controls activation’ groups.
Table 3 presents the matched Glasser’s parcellations and the affiliated brain networks of each activated brain foci in the “ADHD < controls” and “ADHD > controls” groups. More parcellations from the DMN limbic networks were activated to a greater extent in controls relative to children with ADHD. One parcellation from the DMN, area 47l, demonstrated unique hemispheric asymmetry, being activated on the left for ADHD and on the right for controls. Left-sided parcellations of the DAN such as 7 am, 7PL, and PH were more activated in ADHD, while right-sided parcellations such as PEF and 6a were more activated in controls. Both left and right p9-46v parcellation of the CEN network was more activated in ADHD along with other CEN parcellations such as PCV, IFJp, 8C, 44, and PFm.
Table 3.
ADHD > controls | ADHD < controls | ADHD = controls | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
R | L | R | L | R | L | |
CEN | p9-46v | p9-46v | IFSp | 33pr | p24 | |
PCV | p10p | RSC | RSC | p10p | ||
IFJp | a47r | IFJa | ||||
8C | POS2 | |||||
44 | ||||||
PFm | ||||||
Limbic | Hippocampus | Amygdala | 25 | |||
H | ||||||
DMN | STSdp | STV | STSdp | |||
31pv | 45 | 47l | STSvp | |||
8Ad | 47l | 47m | 47m | |||
9a | d23ab | 7m | 47s | |||
TGd | 9p | 9p | ||||
a24 | 55b | POS1 | ||||
A5 | 9m | |||||
PHA3 | d32 | |||||
TPOJ1 | PSL | |||||
TE1a | STGa | |||||
DAN | LIPv | 7Am | PEF | PFt | ||
7PL | 6a | |||||
PH | ||||||
Salience | 9-46d | SCEF | MI | p32pr | ||
PoI2 | FOP4 | |||||
46 | ||||||
Visual | V1 | V3B | VMV1 | V1 | ||
V4 | V4 | VMV3 | VMV3 | |||
V6 | VVC | |||||
PH | FFC | |||||
DVT | ||||||
Sensorimotor | 5m | OP4 | 2 | 4 | ||
RI | 3a | 3a |
Duplicate parcellations that appeared in both groups are listed as “ADHD = Controls”. Glasser’s parcellations matched with activated brain foci in “ADHD > Controls” and “ADHD > Controls” groups.
Venn diagrammatic representation of parcellations activated in the “controls only” and “ADHD < controls” groups are presented in Figure 5 for the DMN, DAN, and the salience network. “Concordant” parcellation, that is parcellations belonging to both groups were areas 55b, d32, PSL, and STSvp on the left and areas 47l on the right for the DMN, areas PEF and 6a on the right for DAN, and areas MI (L) and SCEF (R) for the salience network. Figure 6 provides a summary of the networks activated predominantly by ADHD and control subjects as well as the distinct proportions of each network that were shared among both groups.
5. Discussion
A coordinate-based meta-analytical approach was employed to understand neural correlates of motor inhibition in children with ADHD during the Go/No-Go task, which was useful in summarising anatomical information pertaining to inhibitory effort or lack thereof in an unbiased and objective fashion. Furthermore, ALE generation combined with brain network-based nomenclature offers a greater degree of objective and quantitative specificity, allowing for investigation of regions of interest beyond macro-systems and lobes and into finer details of network affiliations and connectivity. We confirmed existing consensus stating broad involvement of the SMA and fronto-striatal circuits while providing additional insights regarding greater DMN and limbic network connectivity in controls, and unique hemispheric asymmetry of the DAN between controls and ADHD children. Additionally, considering the significant impact of ADHD on childhood development (affecting academic performance, interpersonal and family relations, conduct problems, substance experimentations, and abuse [36]) and the inherent differences between adolescents and adults in terms of prevalence [37], presentation [38], neurobiology [13,15], and impact of ADHD [36], we believe our meta-analysis offers clinical interest for the study of the pediatric ADHD population.
Our findings coincide with theories posited by other recent meta-analyses in neuroimaging literature for ADHD. First, control subjects activated more parcellations of the DMN than their ADHD counterparts. This suggests decreased DMN regulation and stability, despite DMN hyper-connectivity in ADHD [39], which could result in deficits like inappropriately “switching on/off” the DMN to fulfil response inhibition. The DMN – the “resting state” network, is a collection of neural nodes responsible for task-irrelevant mental processes, mind-wandering, and self-generated thoughts [40,41,42]. Some studies have suggested that an absence of inverse correlations between cognitive control networks (i.e. CEN) and the DMN reflects DMN intrusion during active tasks, manifesting as attentional lapses or inability to complete tasks [43,44,45,46,47]. Other hypotheses suggest decreased synchrony within the DMN, although findings across multiple studies have not been unanimous [16]. While many of these insights regarding aberrant DMN connectivity stem from the resting-state fMRI literature [9,4,16], the present fMRI meta-analysis considers the prevalence of significant task-based DMN activation differences.
We found that DMN hemispheric asymmetry, particularly for area 47l, is reproducible and valid. Several studies have suggested that the DMN demonstrates functional asymmetries, with the right-sided network composing the right inferior parietal lobule (IPL) responsible for simultaneously maintaining attention on current task goals and responding to salient information or environmental stimuli. However, the left IPL is more involved in language networks [48,49] – area 47l is known for its association with Broca’s area, contributions to the frontal-subcortical circuit and involvement in language functions and semantic processing [50,51]. Similar conclusions can be made regarding area 47s, another parcellation matched to a significant cluster in our analysis [51]. While these language-oriented functions may be applicable for left-hemispheric areas 47l and 47s, there is a lack of data explaining the function of these areas on the right in a right-dominant brain. Interestingly, “concordant” parcellations activated for controls in the DMN such as areas 55b, d32, PSL, and STSvp are also involved in language processing, and controls demonstrated more extensive connectivity in brain language centres during the task. Several studies have reported language deficiencies in ADHD subjects but none have presented evidence for language deficits during the Go/No-Go task [52,53]. We do not believe that there is a causal relationship between language-based learning difficulties and the lateralization of our findings for the DMN. However, we do highlight a co-occurrence of these factors. We also highlight a significant gap in the literature regarding the function of the aforementioned areas (47l, 47s) on the right side and the implications of DMN asymmetry. Overall, further studies to elucidate the nature and extent of DMN abnormalities are necessary.
Shifting focus onto complex network models of ADHD neural circuitry has revealed inappropriate engagement of attentional systems such as the salience network and the DAN [16,17]. We found unique hemispheric asymmetry in DAN activation, with ADHD and controls activating left- and right-sided parcellations of the DAN, respectively. The DAN, primarily composed of the intraparietal sulcus and frontal eye fields of both hemispheres, is active during spatial attention, feature-based attention, saccade planning, and visual working memory [54,55,56]. A significant “concordant” parcellation activated in controls but not in ADHD was the right PEF (parietal eye field) and area 6a. Area 6a along with 6d form subdivisions of the premotor cortex and while their exact functions are still unknown, the role of the premotor cortex in responding to visual or auditory cues to coordinate or plan voluntary movement is well established [57]. Corbetta and Shulman explained how DAN and ventral attention network interactions can lead to a unique dichotomy of attentional coordination. The former applies cognitive “top-down” selection for stimuli and its appropriate response, while the latter detects salient stimuli to act as a “circuit breaker” [58]. Applying this to our Go/No-Go paradigm, the DAN is responsible for attentional processes driven top-down, that is cognitive information like colours, shapes, or numbers (perceptual set) requiring actions like a button press (motor set), forming an “attentional set.” A second attentional network, heavily lateralized to the right brain including the right temporoparietal junction and right ventral frontal cortex, is then responsible to look out for salient and other potentially important stimuli (STOP signals) and interrupt or inhibit ongoing cognitive activity and the aforementioned attentional process. This may explain why ADHD subjects in this meta-analysis lacked right-sided DAN parcellation activation – that is they lacked the core inhibitor or “circuit-breaker” of goal-directed attentional processes lateralized to the right brain. It is known that patients with right-sided attentional network injury and dysfunction present with the clinical syndrome of neglect. Applying this to meta-analysis, the inability to inhibit motor responses during cognitive tasks requiring sustained attention may be a form of “neglecting” salient cues, arising from poor connectivity in right-sided attentional networks. This poses interesting questions: does the inability to inhibit motor responses in the ADHD phenotype stem from a mechanism distinct from deficits in sustained attention? Or, are the two core deficits intimately intertwined in their mechanistic processes? Future research separating the study groups based on these core deficits may shed further light regarding the directional relationship between these key aspects of ADHD.
Another finding was the comparatively minimal limbic network activation in ADHD children compared to controls. In the present-day literature, the core phenotype of ADHD has extended beyond just deficits in motor inhibition and sustained attention to include a variety of non-cognitive symptoms such as dissociated emotional regulation [59], hyper-aggressiveness [60], emotional lability [61], and depressive symptoms [62]. Some studies have attributed these affective symptoms of ADHD to altered limbic circuitry and amygdala activation [17,40]. For example, Posner et al. reported hypoconnectivity within regions of the limbic cortico–striato–thalamo–cortical loops to the presentation of emotional lability in people with ADHD [59]. However, the association between limbic network connectivity and performance in the Go/No-Go task remains unclear.
There are several notable considerations regarding our study. First, our meta-analytical method has yielded insignificant ALE clusters for the ADHD group at our current thresholding level. This is not unlike other meta-analyses which have also failed to find conclusive findings across several ADHD fMRI datasets [9,16,63]. There are several potential reasons for the abundance of negative findings in the ADHD fMRI literature: (1) slight differences in experimental paradigms and contrasts in task-based fMRI studies; (2) heterogeneity in the sample population in terms of age, sex, medication status, and severity of symptoms; (3) heterogeneity in the ADHD phenotype and genotype itself; and (4) random and systematic errors in the fMRI work-flow, e.g. head movements during task-based fMRI [17]. We posit that the significant convergence towards our ALE clusters for the control groups can be attributed to our inclusion of only the adolescent population, hence reducing heterogenicity.
On this note, heterogeneity and the quality of the included studies are key challenges faced by any meta-analyses. In response to this challenge, our study utilized ALEs. ALE assesses the convergence of activation coordinates across included studies, allowing research studies to identify regions of interest consistently associated with the Go-No/Go paradigm in children with ADHD. By aggregating findings from multiple studies, ALE provides a robust statistical framework that accounts for both the variability and consistency of reported activations, and thus aims to derive objective outcomes amidst a collection of heterogeneous data. We believe that this serves to overcome the limitations of individual studies and thus arrive at a more comprehensive understanding of the neurological correlates of motor inhibition in children with ADHD.
Despite the choice of ALEs being appropriate to address the research question, it is often difficult to balance between selecting enough studies to optimise analytic power and ensuring stringent inclusion criterion to minimise the confounding effects of heterogeneity. We estimate that while our meta-analysis had lesser heterogeneity, we lacked the statistical power to adequately analyse the ADHD population and may have allowed for a higher level of confounding. Furthermore, our study utilised Yeo et al.’s 7-network model of the brain with the intention to provide a foundational understanding of core neural correlates [19]. However, brain mapping is becoming increasingly nuanced in congruence with human connectomic complexity, and hence, future studies should use more detailed brain atlases such as Yeo et al.’s 17-network model [19]. Finally, while important, response inhibition is no longer the defining characteristic of ADHD and is now understood to be one of many different manifestations of complex neural circuitry. We recommend future studies exploring connectivity during other cognitively demanding tasks.
We were also limited by our database relying solely on the PubMed and BrainMap databases. PubMed is a widely recognized and frequently used database, especially in the field of neuroimaging studies. However, we believe that considerations for inclusion of other lesser-known databases in future research endeavours may yield a more comprehensive and holistic result. Finally, while our study focused primarily on the No-Go > Go contrast, future studies could explore the Go and No-Go activations separately in ADHD and control groups.
6. Conclusion
The present study summarises task-based fMRI data regarding the neural correlate of motor/response inhibition of children with ADHD during the Go/No-Go tasks using a coordinate-based meta-analytical method. ALEs were generated and matched to parcellations derived from the Human Connectome Project to understand brain connectivity at a network-based level. Our findings align with other recent meta-analyses conducted in this area. Of particular interest to this study was the less extensive activation of parcellations of DMN and the right-sided DAN in ADHD children.
Supplementary Material
Footnotes
Funding information: No external funding or financial assistance was used for this study.
Conflict of interest: Dr Michael Sughrue is the co-founder and Chief Medical Officer of Omniscient Neurotechnology. Dr Stephane Doyen is the co-founder and Chief Scientific Officer of Omniscient Neurotechnology. Isabella Young, Hugh Taylor, and Peter Nicholas are employees and shareholders of Omniscient Neurotechnology. Onur Tanglay is an employee of Omniscient Neurotechnology. No products related to this company were used or mentioned in this study. The other authors state no conflict of interest.
Data availability statement: The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. The extracted data used for this meta-analysis are available in the Supplementary Table.
References
- [1].Booth JR, Burman DD, Meyer JR, Lei Z, Trommer BL, Davenport ND, et al. Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than for visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(1):94–111. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [2].Cubillo A, Halari R, Smith A, Taylor E, Rubia K. A review of fronto-striatal and fronto-cortical brain abnormalities in children and adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and new evidence for dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and attention. Cortex. 2012;48(2):194–215. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [3].Durston S, Mulder M, Casey BJ, Ziermans T, Van, Engeland H. Activation in ventral prefrontal cortex is sensitive to genetic vulnerability for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(10):1062–70. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [4].Durston S, Tottenham NT, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Eigsti I-M, Yang Y, et al. Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53(10):871–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [5].Pliszka SR, Glahn DC, Semrud-Clikeman M, Franklin C, Perez Iii R, Xiong J, et al. Neuroimaging of inhibitory control areas in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder who were treatment naive or in long-term treatment. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(6):1052–60. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [6].Van Rooij D, Hoekstra PJ, Mennes M, Von Rhein D, Thissen AJAM, Heslenfeld D, et al. Distinguishing adolescents with ADHD from their unaffected siblings and healthy comparison subjects by neural activation patterns during response inhibition. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172(7):674–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [7].Tamm L, Menon V, Ringel J, Reiss AL. Event-related fMRI evidence of frontotemporal involvement in aberrant response inhibition and task switching in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2004;43(11):1430–40. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [8].Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Toone B, Taylor E. Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(6):1067–75. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [9].Samea F, Soluki S, Nejati V, Zarei M, Cortese S, Eickhoff SB, et al. Brain alterations in children/adolescents with ADHD revisited: A neuroimaging meta-analysis of 96 structural and functional studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;100:1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [10].Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Xavier Castellanos F, Milham MP. The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(10):1051–62. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [11].Hart H, Radua J, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Meta-analysis of fMRI studies of timing in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012;36(10):2248–56. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [12].Hart H, Radua J, Nakao T, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(2):185. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [13].Lei D, Du M, Wu M, Chen T, Huang X, Du X, et al. Functional MRI reveals different response inhibition between adults and children with ADHD. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(6):874–81. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [14].Cortese S, Kelly C, Chabernaud C, Proal E, Di Martino A, Milham MP, et al. Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: A meta-analysis of 55 fMRI Studies. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(10):1038–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [15].Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Functional connectivity of substantia nigra and ventral tegmental area: maturation during adolescence and effects of ADHD. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(4):935–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [16].Cortese S, Aoki YY, Itahashi T, Castellanos FX, Eickhoff SB. Systematic review and meta-analysis: resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;60(1):61–75. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [17].Castellanos FX, Aoki Y. Intrinsic functional connectivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A science in development. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2016;1(3):253–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [18].Glasser MF, Coalson TS, Robinson EC, Hacker CD, Harwell J, Yacoub E, et al. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. 2016;536(7615):171–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [19].Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [20].Eickhoff SB, Bzdok D, Laird AR, Kurth F, Fox PT. Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. NeuroImage. 2012;59(3):2349–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [21].Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Grefkes C, Wang LE, Zilles K, Fox PT. Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: A random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(9):2907–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [22].Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:n71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [23].Fox PT, Laird AR, Fox SP, Fox PM, Uecker AM, Crank M, et al. Brainmap taxonomy of experimental design: Description and evaluation. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):185–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [24].Laird AR, Lancaster JL, Fox PT. BrainMap: The social evolution of a human brain mapping database. Neuroinformatics. 2005;3(1):65–78. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [25].Turkeltaub PE, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Fox M, Wiener M, Fox P. Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33(1):1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [26].Müller VI, Cieslik EC, Laird AR, Fox PT, Radua J, Mataix-Cols D, et al. Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;84:151–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [27].Schulz KP, Fan J, Tang CY, Newcorn JH, Buchsbaum MS, Cheung AM, et al. Response inhibition in adolescents diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during childhood: An event-related fMRI study. 2004;161(9):1650–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [28].Vaidya CJ, Bunge SA, Dudukovic NM, Zalecki CA, Elliott GR, Gabrieli JDE. Altered neural substrates of cognitive control in childhood ADHD: Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(9):1605–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [29].Suskauer SJ, Simmonds DJ, Fotedar S, Blankner JG, Pekar JJ, Denckla MB, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for abnormalities in response selection in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Differences in activation associated with response inhibition but not habitual motor response. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20(3):478–93. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [30].Spinelli S, Joel S, Nelson TE, Vasa RA, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. Different neural patterns are associated with trials preceding inhibitory errors in children with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50(7):705–15.e3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [31].Ma J, Lei D, Jin X, Du X, Jiang F, Li F, et al. Compensatory brain activation in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder during a simplified Go/No-Go task. J Neural Transm. 2012;119(5):613–9. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [32].Wang S, Yang Y, Xing W, Chen J, Liu C, Luo X. Altered neural circuits related to sustained attention and executive control in children with ADHD: An event-related fMRI study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2013;124(11):2181–90. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [33].Hart H, Chantiluke K, Cubillo AI, Smith AB, Simmons A, Brammer MJ, et al. Pattern classification of response inhibition in ADHD: Toward the development of neurobiological markers for ADHD. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(7):3083–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [34].Lukito S, Norman L, Carlisi C, Radua J, Hart H, Simonoff E, et al. Comparative meta-analyses of brain structural and functional abnormalities during cognitive control in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Psychol Med. 2020;50(6):894–919. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [35].Suskauer SJ, Simmonds DJ, Caffo BS, Denckla MB, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. fMRI of intrasubject variability in ADHD: Anomalous premotor activity with prefrontal compensation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(10):1141–50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [36].Kieling R, Rohde LA. ADHD in children and adults: Diagnosis and prognosis. Springer: Berlin, Heidelberg; 2010. p. 1–16. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [37].Faraone SV, Biederman J, Mick E. The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychol Med. 2006;36(2):159–65. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [38].Barkley RA, Cox D. A review of driving risks and impairments associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the effects of stimulant medication on driving performance. J Saf Res. 2007;38(1):113–28. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [39].Duffy KA, Rosch KS, Nebel MB, Seymour KE, Lindquist MA, Pekar JJ, et al. Increased integration between default mode and task-relevant networks in children with ADHD is associated with impaired response control. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;50:100980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [40].Posner J, Park C, Wang Z. Connecting the Dots: A Review of Resting Connectivity MRI Studies in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Neuropsychol Rev. 2014;24(1):3–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [41].Buckner RL, Dinicola LM. The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019;20(10):593–608. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [42].Raichle ME, Snyder AZ. A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. Neuroimage. 2007;37(4):1083–90 discussion 97–9. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [43].Sonuga-Barke EJS, Castellanos FX. Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: A neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2007;31(7):977–86. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [44].Castellanos FX, Margulies DS, Kelly C, Uddin LQ, Ghaffari M, Kirsch A, et al. Cingulate-precuneus interactions: a new locus of dysfunction in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63(3):332–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [45].Sun L, Cao Q, Long X, Sui M-Q, Cao X, Zhu C, et al. Abnormal functional connectivity between the anterior cingulate and the default mode network in drug-naïve boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging. 2012;201:120–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [46].Cao X, Cao Q, Long X, Sun L, Sui M, Zhu C, et al. Abnormal resting-state functional connectivity patterns of the putamen in medication-naïve children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Brain Res. 2009;1303:195–206. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [47].Hoekzema E, Carmona S, Ramos-Quiroga JA, Richarte Fernández V, Bosch R, Soliva JC, et al. An independent components and functional connectivity analysis of resting state fMRI data points to neural network dysregulation in adult ADHD. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(4):1261–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [48].Singh-Curry V, Husain M. The functional role of the inferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream dichotomy. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(6):1434–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [49].Kajimura S, Kochiyama T, Abe N, Nomura M. Challenge to unity: Relationship between hemispheric asymmetry of the default mode network and mind wandering. Cereb Cortex. 2019;29(5):2061–71. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [50].Ardila A, Bernal B, Rosselli M. Should Broca’s area include Brodmann area 47? Psicothema. 2017;29(1):73–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [51].Baker CM, Burks JD, Briggs RG, Conner AK, Glenn CA, Morgan JP, et al. A Connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum-chapter 2: The lateral frontal lobe. Oper Neurosurg (Hagerstown). 2018;15(suppl 1):S10–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [52].Carter CS, Krener P, Chaderjian M, Northcutt C, Wolfe V. Asymmetrical visual-spatial attentional performance in ADHD: Evidence for a right hemispheric deficit. Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37(11):789–97. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [53].Camarata SM, Gibson T. Pragmatic language deficits in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 1999;5(3):207–14.
- [54].Ptak R. The frontoparietal attention network of the human brain. Neuroscientist. 2012;18(5):502–15. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [55].Jerde TA, Merriam EP, Riggall AC, Hedges JH, Curtis CE. Prioritized maps of space in human frontoparietal cortex. J Neurosci. 2012;32(48):17382–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [56].Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral attention systems. Neuroscientist. 2014;20(2):150–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [57].Baker CM, Burks JD, Briggs RG, Sheets JR, Conner AK, Glenn CA, et al. A connectomic atlas of the human cerebrum—Chapter 3: The motor, premotor, and sensory cortices. Oper Neurosurg. 2018;15(suppl_1):S75–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [58].Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201–15. [DOI] [PubMed]
- [59].Posner J, Rauh V, Gruber A, Gat I, Wang Z, Peterson BS. Dissociable attentional and affective circuits in medication-naïve children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2013;213(1):24–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [60].Ho N-F, Chong JSX, Koh HL, Koukouna E, Lee T-S, Fung D, et al. Intrinsic affective network is impaired in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0139018-e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [61].Hulvershorn LA, Mennes M, Castellanos FX, Di Martino A, Milham MP, Hummer TA, et al. Abnormal amygdala functional connectivity associated with emotional lability in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(3):351–61.e1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [62].Posner J, Siciliano F, Wang Z, Liu J, Sonuga-Barke E, Greenhill L. A multimodal MRI study of the hippocampus in medication-naive children with ADHD: what connects ADHD and depression? Psychiatry Res. 2014;224(2):112–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- [63].Müller VI, Cieslik EC, Serbanescu I, Laird AR, Fox PT, Eickhoff SB. Altered brain activity in unipolar depression revisited: Meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(1):47–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.