Abstract
Decades after the scientific debate about the anthropogenic causes of climate change was settled, climate disinformation still challenges the scientific evidence in public discourse. Here we present a comprehensive theoretical framework of (anti)science belief formation and updating to account for the psychological factors that influence the acceptance or rejection of scientific messages. We experimentally investigated, across 12 countries (N = 6,816), the effectiveness of six inoculation strategies targeting these factors—scientific consensus, trust in scientists, transparent communication, moralization of climate action, accuracy and positive emotions—to fight real-world disinformation about climate science and mitigation actions. While exposure to disinformation had strong detrimental effects on participants’ climate change beliefs (δ = −0.16), affect towards climate mitigation action (δ = −0.33), ability to detect disinformation (δ = −0.14) and pro-environmental behaviour (δ = −0.24), we found almost no evidence for protective effects of the inoculations (all δ < 0.20). We discuss the implications of these findings and propose ways forward to fight climate disinformation.
Subject terms: Human behaviour, Climate-change mitigation
Spampatti et al. examined the efficacy of six psychological inoculation strategies and discovered that these strategies had close to no protective effects against climate disinformation across 12 different countries.
Main
The sixth report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) unequivocally declared that climate change is real and that humans are driving it1,2. Whereas 97–99% of climate scientists agree about the human causation of climate change3–5, one third of the global population doubts or denies its anthropogenic roots6–8. This can be traced back to half a century of disinformation by the climate change countermovement, comprising fossil fuel corporations and their front groups, scientists-for-hire and lobbied politicians, who have contested climate science and are now delaying necessary climate mitigation actions9–17. This multi-million-dollar public relations effort18–22 operates mainly via popular communication avenues3,23,24, such as traditional25–27 and social media28–30, to shape climate discourse and political decision-making17,29. Their claims take up legitimate concerns that people express—such as high costs or uncertain efficacy of climate action—but qualify as disinformation because these concerns are intentionally distorted and amplified into misleading claims31,32 such as bad-faith questioning of the scientific consensus33, overemphasizing the socio-financial burden of climate mitigation policies14,34 and scaremongering citizens into inaction through climate doomism14. Unfortunately, climate disinformation can be more persuasive than scientifically accurate information35–38 (see also ref. 39).
People process scientific messages not as neutral information processors but rather by weighing the messages against their prior convictions40–45, against desired outcomes46–49, against affective associations50,51 and through the lens of their sociocultural and ideological contexts52–55 (see reviews in refs. 56–60). When these psychological factors are misaligned with scientific information about climate change, antiscience beliefs fester39,61 and become resistant to correction57. Two recent reviews offer distinct yet complementary perspectives on how (dis)information and (anti)science beliefs hinge on different communicational bases and psychological drivers. Philipp-Muller and colleagues61 identified the different communicational bases on which (anti)science beliefs can build: the sources of scientific messages, the scientific messages themselves, the recipients of the scientific messages and the recipients’ epistemic style. In parallel, Ecker and colleagues57 grouped the psychological drivers influencing (dis)information belief formation and revision into cognitive and socio-affective drivers, depending on the psychological pathways they act on to facilitate or hinder belief formation and updating. Overall, both analyses affirmed that people’s capacity and motivation to process information and disinformation—(dis)information henceforth—is conditional to the (mis)alignment of scientific information about climate change with specific communicational and/or psychological factors57,61.
Here we adapt these factors to construct a comprehensive framework of (anti)science belief formation and updating (Table 1). In this framework, the processing of scientific (dis)information is mapped onto its core communicational bases61: sources, messages and recipients. These communicational bases are the entry points62 where different psychological factors can influence (anti)science belief formation and updating through cognitive or socio-affective pathways57.
Table 1.
Core communicational bases | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sources of scientific messages | Scientific messages themselves | Recipients of scientific messages | |||
Psychological drivers | Cognitive pathway | Driver | Consideration of scientific sources | Match/mismatch with prior beliefs | (Lack of) analytical thinking and/or deliberation |
Proposed intervention | Scientific consensus inoculation | Transparent communication inoculation | Accuracy inoculation | ||
Socio-affective pathway | Driver | Trust in scientific sources | Match/mismatch with moral convictions | Emotional state during message processing | |
Proposed intervention | Trust inoculation | Moralization inoculation | Positive emotions inoculation |
The table shows the interplay between the communicational bases and psychological drivers of (anti)science belief formation and updating, and the theory-based psychological inoculations designed to address each entry point. Note that the boundaries between the cognitive and socio-affective pathways are permeable, and the effects of most interventions meant to address one pathway will very probably spill out to the other pathway of scientific (dis)information processing. For example, we consider the transparent communication inoculation to act on the cognitive driver ‘match/mismatch with prior beliefs’; however, its effects can spill over towards the socio-affective driver ‘trust in scientific sources’112,113.
Such a framework allows both the systematic mapping of the different entry points of scientific (dis)information, and the targeted, theory-based design of a comprehensive set of psychological intervention strategies using cognitive and socio-affective pathways to protect people from being influenced by disinformation. Among possible interventions, psychological inoculations have been identified as one of the most promising approaches to fighting climate disinformation23,63. They consist of pre-emptive warnings about incoming disinformation coupled with psychological resources64—counterarguments38 and/or rhetorical techniques65–67—to resist disinformation68. We now review how each factor identified in the framework can engender acceptance or rejection of scientific messages, and we propose a set of theory-based psychological inoculations targeting these factors, with the aim of fighting scientific disinformation about climate change.
The first entry point to (anti)science belief formation and updating is the source of scientific messages about climate change. At the cognitive pathway level, it has been shown that the perception that the scientific community agrees about anthropogenic climate change provides diagnostic information that people can use to strengthen their acceptance of climate science. According to the gateway belief model69, accurate information about the scientific consensus makes people more accepting of climate science and of climate actions70. Since the infamous ‘Luntz memo’71 coached Republican politicians to question the scientific consensus about climate change, countermovement actors have been painting the scientific community as divided and biased about the reality of climate change17,33. The result of this strategy has been that people neglect current scientific sources, perceiving the scientific consensus to be magnitudinally lower than the actual consensus (the false consensus effect57,69), climate science to be unsettled and climate action to be therefore unnecessary58. People can thus be psychologically inoculated with arguments explaining the scientific consensus to protect against disinformation at the source basis via the cognitive pathway38. At the socio-affective pathway level, trust in the sources of scientific messages is essential for increasing information processing and climate policy support72–80. People update their beliefs when scientific messages are delivered by trusted sources73,81–83, whereas people who distrust mainstream and scientific information sources are more susceptible to misinformation and to holding wrong beliefs84–86. Moreover, trust in climate stakeholders moderates the association between believing in climate change and supporting mitigation policies such as carbon taxes: people who distrust political climate stakeholders oppose mitigation policies87–89, whereas people who trust them support mitigation policies75,76,87,89 (see also ref. 90). Scientists themselves are the most trusted sources of scientific information78,91,92, and trust in science and scientists predicts support for climate mitigation behaviours more strongly than trust in other climate stakeholders93,94 (see also ref. 72). Emphasizing the trustworthiness of scientists can make this trust more salient80, potentially curbing disinformation uptake95 and thus protecting against disinformation at the source basis via the socio-affective pathway. Two non-peer-reviewed, preregistered studies support this idea, as pre-emptively making trust in key stakeholders of the energy transition salient protected support for renewable energy from multiple negative persuasive attacks96.
The second entry point to (anti)science belief formation and updating is the scientific message itself. People process scientific (dis)information on the basis of the (un)intuitiveness of the messages39,57,97 and the (mis)alignment with their own worldviews, moral values and political ideologies98–102. At the cognitive pathway level, when people detect a conflict between their prior beliefs and incoming scientific messages43, they resist scientific information by generating counterarguments103–107. Unaddressed counterarguments can cement policy opposition108,109 (cf. ref. 110), especially when people’s legitimate concerns—such as the costs of climate actions111—are turned into exaggerated counterarguments to stifle climate policies14,33. To protect against disinformation at the message basis via the cognitive pathway, counterarguing can be addressed by transparently communicating the pros and cons of debated policies112–116. Transparently addressing concerns while highlighting positive outcomes was recently found to increase COVID-19 vaccination intentions and trust in the source of the transparent communication, more than messages ignoring vaccination concerns; the elicited changes were moreover resistant to a subsequent conspiracy message attacking the vaccine112. At the socio-affective pathway level, scientific messages are resisted when they are misaligned with people’s moral values117,118. Multiple studies show that when people’s moral convictions are questioned by scientific messages, “moral convictions have the power to bend people’s factual beliefs, trust in authorities, and evaluations of procedures”119 (p. 87) (see also refs. 120–123), which may result in the rejection of scientific evidence. To protect against disinformation at the message basis via the socio-affective pathway, one can link the importance of climate action to a diversity of worldviews and moral orientations by framing scientific messages in moral terms (for example, refs. 124–127). Linking climate action to morality can moreover increase the likelihood that people will take action128: emblematically, climate activist Greta Thunberg cited moral conviction as her primary driver for the climate strike movement118.
The third entry point to (anti)science belief formation and updating is the message recipient. People who rely on intuitive thinking are more likely to believe and share misinformation129–131, whereas people who rely on reflective, deliberative thinking tend to hold more accurate beliefs132–135 (see review in ref. 43). According to this research, most people are accurate in determining the truthfulness of information when making judgements deliberately134 (see also refs. 109,136). However, they are easily distracted away from deliberation, thus engaging with (dis)information without actively considering their factual basis137. To protect against disinformation at the recipient basis via the cognitive pathway, people can be directed to thinking deliberately by prompting them to evaluate incoming information by its factual accuracy138,139. Untested in the climate domain (but see a similar intervention in ref. 140), accuracy prompts robustly decreased the influence of misinformation on political belief and fake news sharing134 in the lab, in the field135 and across countries141. At the socio-affective level, the processing of scientific (dis)information is influenced by the emotional state of the recipient142. Emotions are a filter that guides people towards relevant and valued information in a noisy environment143–145, and their motivational properties direct and support individuals’ behaviour146. On the one hand, correlational evidence suggests that emotion-laden misinformation spreads more widely in social networks147, and that people tend to believe misinformation more when it contains emotional content148,149. On the other hand, emotions have been found to foster belief updating and climate-related behaviour50,144,150–152. Positive emotions motivate discounting of counterattitudinal information40 and have been suggested as an antidote to overcome a lack of motivation to parse misinformation153. Moreover, multiple recent reviews144,146,154,155 argue that the anticipation and experience of positive emotions elicited by acting pro-environmentally144,154–158 increase pro-environmental behavioural intentions as well as actual behaviour158–161. To protect against disinformation at the recipient basis via the socio-affective pathway, the saliency of experienced positive emotions in the context of climate action can be increased, which should increase resistance to disinformation as well as the likelihood of acting pro-environmentally.
In summary, here we integrated previous analyses into a comprehensive framework of the communicational and psychological factors influencing (anti)science belief formation and updating. On the basis of this integrated, theory-driven perspective, we introduce a set of broad-spectrum psychological inoculations to protect against climate disinformation that act on each of the identified entry points and pathways:
A scientific consensus inoculation explaining that among climate scientists there is virtually no disagreement that humans are causing climate change
A trust inoculation making salient the trustworthiness of IPCC scientists in terms of climate change science and mitigation actions
A transparent communication inoculation addressing the pros and cons of climate mitigation action
A moralization inoculation creating a stronger link between climate mitigation actions and the diversity of moral convictions
An accuracy inoculation reorienting participants towards judging incoming information by its factual accuracy
A positive emotions inoculation eliciting positive emotions towards climate mitigation actions
We investigated the effectiveness of these six broad-spectrum psychological inoculation strategies to protect against climate disinformation in a multi-country, multi-intervention study, against a sequence of 20 real climate disinformation messages spread by members of the climate change countermovement on the social media platform Twitter. We assessed the protective effect of the inoculations on participants’ climate change beliefs162,, appraisal of climate mitigation action and truth discernment capacity—that is, their capacity to correctly distinguish between true and false information163. We moreover investigated whether the protective effects of the psychological inoculations extend to actual pro-environmental behaviour164. The participants were presented with 20 real climate disinformation statements that were selected on the basis of an initial validation study (N = 504, available at https://osf.io/m58zx). The participants saw multiple disinformation statements to assess whether the psychological inoculations are capable of protecting against not only one but multiple occurrences of climate disinformation (which mirrors the preponderance of climate disinformation in certain epistemic communities30). After each disinformation message, the participants rated their current affect towards climate actions (we measured affect towards, rather than political support for, climate mitigation actions because affective reactions predate and motivate policy appraisals and climate-friendly behaviour50,51,144). After having viewed all 20 disinformation statements, the participants also reported their perceptions concerning the reality, causes and consequences of climate change162, performed a version of a validated pro-environmental behaviour task with actual environmental consequences164, and performed a truth discernment task with true and false climate statements. Compared with a passive disinformation control condition where participants were only confronted with the disinformation, we expected the inoculations to significantly protect participants’ affect towards climate action (H1A,B), with a treatment effect bigger than that for the ‘standard approach’ of fact-checking political topics165. We moreover expected the protective effect to extend to people’s climate change beliefs (H2), performance in the pro-environmental behaviour task (H3) and truth discernment capacity (H4). We collected responses from 12 countries across the globe, 7 of which are non-WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic), to be able to make stronger claims about the generalizability of the six psychological inoculations57,60,166,167. We furthermore investigated treatment heterogeneity168 by assessing the effectiveness of the inoculations depending on their thematic match with the climate disinformation statements (for example, testing whether the scientific consensus inoculation protects especially well against disinformation that targets the scientific consensus), and depending on participants’ tendency for intuitive/deliberative thinking. Not only has this tendency previously been shown to directly influence belief in (anti)scientific topics43 and to moderate accuracy prompting135, but it may moreover underlie people’s overall tendency to rely on the socio-affective (for intuitive thinkers) or the cognitive (for deliberative thinkers) pathways to enact belief updating and revision43,57. We hypothesized this tendency to be a moderator depending on its match with the inoculation pathway: cognitive-based inoculation would be more effective for people with a tendency for deliberative thinking, whereas socio-affective-based inoculation would be more effective for people with a tendency for intuitive thinking (Hsecondary 1). The aim of the study was to introduce interventions that can comprehensively address the communicational bases and the main psychological drivers of (anti)science belief formation and updating and thus to provide new interventions in the fight against climate disinformation.
Results
Manipulation check
All psychological inoculations (Table 2) except the scientific consensus inoculation (equivalence test: t(1,651.06) = −2.527; P = 0.006; δ = 0.08; 90% confidence interval (CI), (−0.01, 0.23)) increased participants’ motivation to resist persuasion. Participants who received the trust inoculation (t(1,704.11) = 2.233; P = 0.03; δ = 0.11; 95% CI, (0.02, 0.26)), the transparent communication inoculation (t(1,697.49) = 2.844; P = 0.005; δ = 0.14; 95% CI, (0.05, 0.29)), the moralization inoculation (t(1,730) = 5.333; P < 0.001; δ = 0.26; 95% CI, (0.21, 0.45)), the accuracy inoculation (t(1,687.69) = 3.844; P < 0.001; δ = 0.19; 95% CI, (0.12, 0.35)) or the positive emotions inoculation (t(1,702.58) = 3.258; P < 0.001; δ = 0.16; 95% CI, (0.08, 0.32)) reported more motivation to resist persuasion than participants in the passive control condition.
Table 2.
Cognitive inoculation | Socio-affective inoculation |
---|---|
Scientific consensus inoculation | Trust inoculation |
When confronted with such misleading information about the science of climate change and the actions to mitigate it, remember that the IPCC, the most comprehensive review on the scientific agreement behind climate change and climate action, found that among thousands of climate scientists with the highest degrees of expertise ‘there is virtually no disagreement that humans are causing climate change’. Studies have shown that the consensus about anthropogenic climate change among expert scientists ranges from 97% to 99%. IPCC scientists from all cultural backgrounds and nations stated in the report that ‘It is unequivocal that human influence has warmed the atmosphere, ocean and land’ and they are in agreement that urgent climate action is needed for a better planet and society. | When confronted with such misleading information about the science of climate change and the actions to mitigate it, remember that the IPCC is the most authoritative scientific body in the world assessing the knowledge about climate change and climate action and that the majority of citizens of multiple countries trust scientists. Climate scientists have the highest degrees of expertise and are committed to open and transparent review by other scientists and governments around the world, and value rigorous and balanced scientific information above all else. IPCC scientists come from all cultural backgrounds and nations, to reflect a diverse range of views and expertise in their work and to ensure an objective and complete assessment of the scientific evidence about climate change, to recommend actions and policies for a better planet and society. |
Transparent communication inoculation | Moralization inoculation |
When confronted with such misleading information about the science of climate change and the actions to mitigate it, remember that the IPCC scientists are open about the fact that climate actions will require substantial funding and a significant overhaul of our way of life to keep our planet livable. They also disclosed that there is some uncertainty about if and how these climate actions may reduce our quality of life, but they still concluded with confidence that limiting irreversible climate-induced risks with climate action is less risky than not acting at all. Acting is hard, they admit, but it is through these scientifically supported actions that we can protect our planet, reduce inequality, and generate sustainable growth. | When confronted with such misleading information about the science of climate change and the actions to mitigate it, remember that the IPCC scientists provide valuable and authoritative advice about actions that our communities and nations must take to responsibly keep our planet livable for us and for future generations. As citizens of this earth, we have a moral responsibility to protect our homeland and our community from climate-induced risks and harms, and to stop defiling our pristine natural environment. Through these scientifically supported actions, we can protect our planet, create a more just and fair society with decent living conditions for everyone, and generate sustainable growth beneficial for us, our nations, the world, and generations to come. |
Accuracy inoculation | Positive emotions inoculation |
When confronted with such misleading information about the science of climate change and the actions to mitigate it, remember that it is important to be able to accurately recognize these misinformation to avoid being influenced by them. One good strategy to distinguish between good and bad information is to ask yourself: ‘do I think this information is accurately describing the state of the science of climate change? Is this information not at all accurate, not very accurate, somewhat accurate, or very accurate?’. When you evaluate the information you see on any media about climate change, think about this accuracy question to get in the right frame of mind. | When confronted with such misleading information about the science of climate change and the actions to mitigate it, remember that climate actions are vital actions that will keep our planet livable for the next generation. Actions such as eating delicious and healthy meals with a lower carbon footprint or taking a bike ride instead of getting stuck in traffic are scientifically supported ways to make you happier and more fulfilled in your daily life. When you evaluate the information you see on any media about climate change, imagine the positive changes you can create with climate action, and think about how good you will feel when doing so. |
Cross-condition differences are underlined.
Effects of the climate disinformation statements
Compared with the pure control condition, participants in the passive control condition (who received 20 climate disinformation statements without any psychological inoculation; Table 3) reported significantly less positive affect towards climate mitigation action (t(1,676.83) = −6.774; P < 0.001; δ = −0.33; 95% CI, (−10.48, −5.77)) and significantly lower belief in the reality of climate change (t(1,689.62) = −3.990; P < 0.001; δ = −0.19; 95% CI, (−0.28, −0.10)) and in its anthropogenic causes (t(1,705.95) = −2.496; P = 0.013; δ = −0.12; 95% CI, (−0.21, −0.03)), but not in the negativity of its consequences (t(1,708.79) = −1.942; P = 0.052; δ = −0.09; 95% CI, (−0.18, 0.001); equivalence test: t(1,708.79) = 2.205; P = 0.014; 90% CI, (−0.01, −0.16)). Their overall truth discrimination was significantly worse (t(1,717.26) = −2.877; P = 0.004; δ = −0.14; 95% CI, (−0.52, −0.10)). Exposure to climate disinformation reduced pro-environmental behaviour, as participants in the passive control condition completed significantly less pages of the pro-environmental behaviour task (t(1,713.68) = −5.030; P < 0.001; δ = −0.24; 95% CI, (−0.80, −0.39); Hcontrol 1A–D supported).
Table 3.
Coding | Disinformation tweet |
---|---|
Science_1 | As more wind and solar are added they raise electricity prices and destabilize electric grids. Because they are part-time unreliable weather dependent sources. We want full-time electricity. Not part-time like third world countries. All for silly expensive net zero. CA pays more. |
Science_2 | The current exceptional warming and cooling your seeing is due to the location of the Jet Stream. It’s become very wavy due to the lack of Solar Energy going into the Oceans and nothing to do with Man Made CO2 |
Science_3 | Today’s ‘global warming’ is estimated to be an otherwise unmeasurable 0.4°C (0.72°F) over the 1979-2000 average… despite 50% of all manmade emissions. No 2022 weather event was unprecedented or can be blamed on CO2 emissions. |
Science_4 | This is a portrait of climate fraud, posturing as the saviours of the world. They are a breed of crooks, getting rich by ripping off gullible western nations. The UN led climate hoax has been running since 1988. They want us to believe a pack of lies about earth’s climate. |
Science_5 | Too often, academic reports on climate use highly skewed data that seem to have been carefully selected to support aggressive environmental regulations. One recent and much-cited Lancet report appears deliberately deceptive. |
Science_6 | The climate hoax devised by the UN, supported by rich elitists is endorsed by our treacherous leaders is an attack on freedoms & rights. Climate cultism is a form of global self hatred. It aims to punish western nations by transferring huge reparations to the developing world. |
Science_7 | Top NASA Climate Modeler Admits Predictions Are ‘Mathematically Impossible’ |
Science_8 | Lots of links of studies of the Medieval Warm Period that climate science deniers (alarmists) want to pretend did not exist. Because there is no explanation for natural warming during this time. Studies point out temp was warmer back then, than now. |
Science_9 | According to global warming theory the poles should warm significantly if carbon dioxide is driving temperatures Just the opposite is occurring in the southern hemisphere. |
Science_10 | The evidence for manmade climate change is so thin they cannot debate it. They hide behind the lie of consensus. There is no room for consensus in science. The basis is a provable hypothesis. There is not a single peer reviewed study that proves manmade CO2 is causing warming. |
Action_1 | At Climate Summit, Elites Chow Down on Gourmet Meats While Telling Us to Eat Bugs |
Action_2 | FACT CHECK Results of the Biden administration’s extreme climate agenda cutting emissions by 44% by 2030. Annual Jobs Lost: 1.2 MILLION. Lost Economic Growth: $7.7 TRILLION. Increase in Electric Bills: 23% Increase in Gas Prices: 2$ PER YEAR |
Action_3 | The war on ‘fossil fuels’ is absurd considering the vast fields of coal/oil/gas everywhere on earth. The mantle is brimming over with it. A United Nations bid for control, cash & power has led to an energy crisis that looms as the biggest self-inflicted disaster in human history. |
Action_4 | Death and privation caused by the lack of affordable energy caused by Green Energy policies will not affect the Elites at all. They want us to eat bugs, do a lot less as they carry on with their lives just as they are doing now. Climate scamsters. They should lead by example. |
Action_5 | You are lying. Fossil fuels gave us cheap energy for decades so billions live longer healthier happier lives. Many technologies like carbon capture, filters fuel additives etc reduces emissions. Banning fossil fuels is creating fuel poverty and harming people |
Action_6 | Energy literacy starts with the knowledge that renewable energy is only intermittent electricity generated from unreliable breezes and sunshine, as wind turbines and solar panels cannot manufacture anything for the 8 billion on this planet. |
Action_7 | Imagine sacrificing 500 high-paying coal jobs, ranging up to $60,000/yr, for the climate hoax. Even if you believe in the hoax, global emissions are up 5% from pre-pandemic levels -- 90% because of China. Emissions from a single mine are insignificant. |
Action_8 | Europe’s transition to renewable energy and net zero carbon is not working, except to make life hard on average European citizens. |
Action_9 | Willfully-blind ignorance about the consequences of [the rush to green policies] – deep recessions, broken societies and millions more going hungry – doesn’t make them any less immoral. The road to hell is paved with good intentions. Bingo. |
Action_10 | Solar and wind are far more expensive than established reliable stable secure electricity from pure hydro coal gas nuclear. That’s why your shift to unreliable, unstable, expensive solar and wind; is devastating families; and exporting manufacturing jobs |
To prevent climate change countermovement actors from understanding the net persuasive appeal of each disinformation statement, the identifying numbers of each statement differ from the identifying numbers in the collected data. The correct matching is known only to the authors.
Inoculation effects on affect—preregistered analyses
The protective effects of the inoculations on affect towards climate mitigation action after the 20 climate disinformation statements was, if present, significantly smaller than δ = 0.20 (Supplementary Fig. 1; H1A not supported). There was suggestive evidence that the positive emotions inoculation had a small protective effect compared with the passive control condition, but the effect was significantly smaller than δ = 0.20 (one-tailed t-test: t(1,702.7) = 1.862; P = 0.03; 95% CI, (0.28, ∞); equivalence test: t(1,702.68) = −2.243; P = 0.012; 95% CI, (0.28, 4.54)). Comparisons between the passive control condition and the other psychological inoculations yielded no significant differences for the scientific consensus inoculation (one-tailed t-test: t(1,667) = 0.854; P = 0.43; 95% CI, (−1.05, ∞); equivalence test: t(1,667.05) = −3.239; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−1.46, 3.72)), the trust inoculation (one-tailed t-test: t(1,706.9) = 1.011; P = 0.16; 95% CI, (−0.82, ∞); equivalence test: t(1,705.36) = 3.272; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−1.02, 3.69)), the transparent communication inoculation (one-tailed t-test: t(1,685.78) = −0.2899; P = 0.61; 95% CI, (−2.53, ∞); equivalence test: t(1,685.78) = −3.769; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−1.03, 3.29)), the moralization inoculation (one-tailed t-test: t(1,730) = 1.533; P = 0.06; 95% CI, (−0.15, ∞); equivalence test: t(1,730) = −2.630; P = 0.004; 90% CI, (−0.15, 4.12)) or the accuracy inoculation (one-tailed t-test: t(1,683.2) = −0.150; P = 0.56; 95% CI, (−2.36, ∞); equivalence test: t(1,683.24) = 3.960; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−2.36, 1.97)).
The multilevel model analyses of affect towards mitigation action across the disinformation provision mirrored the previous finding: there was no evidence of a significant interaction between trial and condition (F(6, 112,994) = 1.4749, P = 0.18; Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 1; H1B not supported) or of a main effect of condition (F(6, 6,978) = 1.9400, P = 0.07; Fig. 2). Supplementary control analyses provided no evidence that these non-effects were dependent on matches between specific inoculations and climate disinformation statements (Supplementary Tables 2–4).
Effects of the inoculations on affect towards climate mitigation action—exploratory analyses
As psychological inoculations are usually tested against a single disinformation statement, not multiple ones38,65, we exploratorily tested whether the psychological inoculations protected participants’ affect towards climate mitigation action against the influence of the very first climate disinformation statement only. Except for the participants inoculated with the transparent communication inoculation, all other inoculated participants reported significantly more positive affect than the participants in the passive control condition. Participants in the passive control condition showed a significant decrease in their affect towards climate mitigation from the pre-intervention level after the first climate disinformation statement (paired t-test: t(852) = −3.316; P < 0.001; δ = −0.11; 95% CI, (−3.03, −1.02)). In contrast, participants in all inoculation conditions reported their affect towards climate mitigation action as unmoved from the pre-intervention level after the first climate disinformation statement (scientific consensus inoculation, t-test: t(1,674.5) = 2.526; P = 0.012; δ = 0.12; 95% CI, (0.67, 5.30); paired equivalence test: t(823) = −5.608; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−1.28, 1.09); trust inoculation, t-test: t(1,706.63) = 2.150; P = 0.032; δ = 0.10; 95% CI, (0.22, 4.82); paired equivalence test: t(850) = 5.010; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−1.76, 0.57); transparent communication inoculation, t-test: t(16,897.65) = 1.935; P = 0.053; δ = 0.09; 95% CI, (−0.03, 4.56); paired equivalence test: t(846) = 4.336; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−2.07, 0.11); moralization inoculation, t-test: t(1,725.49) = 3.000; P = 0.003; δ = 0.14; 95% CI, (1.20, 5.75); paired equivalence test: t(878) = −4.815; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−1.90, 0.37); accuracy inoculation, t-test: t(1,687.71) = 2.075; P = 0.042; δ = 0.10; 95% CI, (−0.09, 4.73); paired equivalence test: t(836) = 4.525; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−1.98, 0.48); positive emotions inoculation, t-test: t(1,699.95) = 3.432; P < 0.001; δ = 0.17; 95% CI, (1.70, 6.24); paired equivalence test: t(845) = 4.911; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.43, 1.53)). These exploratory analyses suggest that the inoculations were able to protect participants at least against the negative impact of the first climate disinformation statement.
Inoculation effects on climate change beliefs—preregistered analyses
Belief in climate change was generally high across the 12 countries (meanbelief = 4.04 ± 0.85 on a scale from 1 to 5; Fig. 2a–c). Preregistered multilevel models did not find evidence that inoculated participants believed more in the reality of climate change (main effect of condition: F(6, 5,901.1) = 1.1460, P = 0.33), in its anthropogenic causes (main effect of condition: F(6, 5,901.1) = 0.3824, P = 0.89) or in the negativity of its consequences (main effect of condition: F(6, 5,901.1) = 0.2911, P = 0.94) than participants in the passive control conditions (the full model results are available in Supplementary Table 5). Equivalence tests confirmed that, if present at all, any effect of the inoculations on the overall belief in climate change was smaller than δ = 0.20 (H2 not supported) (scientific consensus inoculation: t(1,670.08) = −3.971; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.08, 0.09); trust inoculation: t(1,705.17) = −4.038; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.08, 0.9); transparent communication inoculation: t(1,694.76) = −3.413; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.10, 0.04); moralization inoculation: t(1,694.76) = −3.413; P = 0.002; 90% C(−0.02, 0.12); accuracy inoculation: t(1,686.82) = −3.727; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.05, 0.09); positive emotions inoculation: t(1,702.63) = −3.817; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.06, 0.08)).
Inoculation effects on behaviour—preregistered analyses
Participants’ engagement overall resulted in 10,969 trees being planted. The multilevel model predicting the performance in the pro-environmental behaviour task164 showed a main effect of condition (χ(6) = 17.074, P = 0.009) but yielded no evidence that inoculated participants accurately completed more task pages than participants in the passive control condition (the contrasts between conditions and the passive control were all not significant; Fig. 2e and Supplementary Table 6). Following the preregistration, upon visual inspection we identified a flooring effect of the Work for Environmental Protection Task (WEPT) response distribution. Model diagnostics confirmed that the distribution was zero-inflated169,170 (P < 0.001). We thus conducted a supplementary multilevel model with a zero-inflated intercept (z = −4.02; P < 0.001; β = −0.12; 95% CI, (−0.17, −0.06)), which curtailed the inflation. However, this analysis also yielded no evidence that the psychological inoculations protected pro-environmental behaviour from the significant decrease due to the climate disinformation statements (main effect of condition: χ(6) = 11.7805, P = 0.07; H3 not supported).
Inoculation effects on truth discrimination—preregistered analyses
We summed all responses correctly identifying true and false statements163 (Table 4) as the dependent variable for the multilevel model. The analysis suggested a significant main effect of condition (F(6, 5,936.4) = 2.4338, P = 0.024; Fig. 2d). Contrasts revealed that the effect was driven by the accuracy inoculation (t(5,936.2) = 3.360; P < 0.001; β = 0.36; 95% CI, (0.15, 0.57)), as participants inoculated to reflect on the accuracy of climate-change-related information were significantly better at discriminating true and false statements about the topic. Equivalence tests showed that, if present at all, the protective effects of the other inoculations were significantly smaller than δ = 0.20 (Supplementary Fig. 8; H4 partial support) (scientific consensus inoculation: t(1,670.06) = −3.782; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.15, 0.22); trust inoculation: t(1,705.24) = −2.195; P = 0.014; 90% CI, (−0.03, −0.39); transparent communication inoculation: t(1,697.78) = −2.387; P = 0.009; 90% CI, (−0.01, −0.37); moralization inoculation: t(1,726.29) = −3.151; P = 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.07, 0.29); positive emotions inoculation: t(1,701.69) = −2.759; P = 0.003; 90% CI, (−0.03, 0.37)).
Table 4.
Coding | Climate-relevant news headline |
---|---|
True_Supporting_1 | Earth’s average temperature continues to rise, setting new record highs each decade. |
True_ Supporting_2 | Human activities, such as burning fossil fuels, are the main cause of climate change. |
True_ Supporting_3 | Climate change is leading to more intense and frequent natural disasters. |
True_ Supporting_4 | The transportation sector is a significant contributor to greenhouse gas emissions. |
True_ Supporting_5 | Rising seas could displace hundreds of millions of people by the end of the century. |
True_Delaying_1 | Projections of Regional Impacts of Climate Change are Subject to Uncertainty. |
True_Delaying_2 | Transportation Sector Transition to Electric Vehicles Can Cost Billions in Infrastructure Upgrades. |
True_Delaying_3 | Brazil Missing Paris Agreement Targets with Deforestation and Agricultural Expansion Driving Up Emissions. |
True_Delaying_4 | China’s continued construction of coal-fired power plants threatens progress on climate goals. |
True_Delaying_5 | Developing Countries Require $40 Billion Annually to Mitigate Climate Change. |
False_Supporting_1 | Climate change will cause the extinction of up to 75% of all species on Earth. |
False_Supporting_2 | Global temperatures may rise by up to 20°C by the end of the century, potentially resulting in widespread drought and famine due to climate change. |
False_Supporting_3 | The Earth may enter a period of ‘runaway warming’ that cannot be stopped, which could lead to the collapse of civilization due to climate change. |
False_Supporting_4 | Germany Leads the Way in Renewable Energy, with Nearly 65% of Electricity Generated from Renewables. |
False_Supporting_5 | Climate Catastrophe: Entire Cities to be Submerged by Rising Seas Within Decades. |
False_Delaying_1 | Extreme Weather: Natural variability, not human activity, is the main driver of extreme weather events. |
False_Delaying_2 | The Climate Challenge Can Be Addressed Through Innovation and Technology Advancements in Fossil Fuels. |
False_Delaying_3 | Carbon Dioxide is Not a Pollutant, but a Benefit to the Environment. |
False_Delaying_4 | Catastrophic Consequences of Global Warming are Inevitable and Unavoidable. |
False_Delaying_5 | Renewable Energy is Costly and Inefficient, and Should Not be Subsidized. |
‘True/False’ refers to true or false statements; ‘Supporting/Delaying’ refers to statements supporting or opposing climate science and action.
Inoculation effects on truth discrimination—exploratory analyses
We conducted additional exploratory analyses because recent studies found that psychological inoculations may increase participants’ bias towards judging any statement as false67,171. First, decomposing the effect of the accuracy inoculation across four types of climate statements (climate support versus delay, true or false) showed that accuracy-inoculated participants were better at discerning false statements delaying climate action (t(1,743.74) = 2.204; P = 0.024; δ = 0.11; 95% CI, (0.02, 0.27)), but not the other types of statements—that is, true statements delaying climate action (t(1,745) = 3.557; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.16, 0.07)), true statements supporting climate action (t(1,744.07) = 2.614; P = 0.005; 90% CI, (−0.19, 0.01)) and false statements supporting climate action (t(1,744.69) = 4.042; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.10, 0.09)).
Second, we applied signal detection theory172 to further scrutinize performance in the truth discrimination task. In brief, signal detection theory posits that stimulus detection is contingent on people’s discriminant ability and their overall response bias towards reporting all (dis)information as true or as false. We conducted these analyses because recent studies found that psychological inoculations may increase participants’ bias towards judging any statement as false67,171. We therefore conducted additional t-tests and equivalence tests to predict participants’ discriminatory ability and response bias. In line with the preregistered results, we found that only the accuracy inoculation increased participants’ discriminatory ability (t(1,687.71) = 3.386; P < 0.001; δ = 0.17; 95% CI, (0.02, 0.06)). In contrast to findings from previously published studies, equivalence tests suggested that the inoculations did not make participants more biased towards considering all task statements as false (scientific consensus inoculation: t(1,675) = 3.502; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.02, 0.01); trust inoculation: t(1,702.07) = 2.534; P = 0.006; 90% CI, (−0.03, 0); transparent communication inoculation: t(1,696.08) = 3.491; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.02, 0.01); moralization inoculation: t(1,716.27) = 2.499; P = 0.006; 90% CI, (−0.03, 0); accuracy inoculation: t(1,681.97) = 3.459; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.02, 0.10); positive emotions inoculation: t(1,703) = 3.978; P < 0.001; 90% CI, (−0.01, 0.02)).
Moderation by CRT-2 score—preregistered analysis
As preregistered, we aggregated the cognitive (scientific consensus, transparent communication and accuracy) and the socio-affective (trust, moralization and positive emotions) inoculations into two factors and tested whether the participants’ tendency for intuitive/deliberative thinking (that is, their scores on the Cognitive Reflection Task, version 2 (CRT-2)173) moderated the inoculations’ protective effect on affect towards climate mitigation action. We did not find evidence of a significant moderation of CRT-2 scores on the overall effectiveness of the inoculation groups (t(5,963) = 0.515; P = 0.61; β = 0.15; 95% CI, (−0.41, 0.70); Supplementary Table 7), but we found suggestive evidence that the preregistered three-way interaction between CRT-2 scores, the psychological drivers and trial was significant (t(96,787) = −2.101; P = 0.036; β = −0.02; 95% CI, (−0.03, −0.001); Supplementary Table 8). Visual inspection of this relationship suggested that participants who rely more on deliberative thinking and are inoculated with a cognitive inoculation are less affected by each of the climate disinformation statements, but participants who rely more on deliberative thinking and are inoculated with a socio-affective inoculation are more affected by each of the climate disinformation statements (Fig. 3).
A preregistered supplementary multilevel model analysing the moderation of CRT-2 scores on each condition suggested that the moderation was primarily driven by the scientific consensus (F-ratio = 4.087, P = 0.043), transparent communication (F-ratio = 6.265, P = 0.012) and accuracy inoculations (F-ratio = 5.316, P = 0.021), and that the more participants relied on intuitive thinking, the more the trust inoculation protected them (F-ratio = 4.102, P = 0.043; Supplementary Table 9 and Supplementary Figs. 9–15; H5 moderate support).
Moderation by political ideology—exploratory analyses
As political conservativism is a main predictor of climate skepticism13 and resistance to misinformation interventions173 but its effect on psychological inoculations is unclear38, we explored whether political ideology moderated the protective effects of any psychological inoculation against the 20 climate disinformation statements using a multilevel model (Supplementary Table 10). We did not find evidence that political ideology influenced overall affect (t(6,977) = 0.452; P = 0.65; β = 0.13; 95% CI, (−0.43, 0.69)) or the trial-by-trial decrease in affect towards climate mitigation action after each climate disinformation statement (t(11,300) = −0.871; P = 0.38; β = −0.007; 95% CI, (−0.02, 0.01)). However, we found suggestive evidence that political ideology moderated the trial-by-trial effectiveness of the psychological inoculations (F(112,994) = 2.1846, P = 0.041). In particular, the most liberal participants inoculated via the scientific consensus inoculation (F-ratio = 12.807, P < 0.001) and the positive emotions inoculation (F-ratio = 5.623, P = 0.02) were the most protected from the climate disinformation statements (Supplementary Figs. 15–21). The moderation was not significantly different between the two inoculations and the passive control condition (scientific consensus: t(11,300) = −1.948; P = 0.051; β = −0.21; 95% CI, (−0.04, 0.0001); positive emotions: t(11,300) = −1.122; P = 0.26; β = −0.12; 95% CI, (−0.03, 0.01)) and was limited to affect towards climate mitigation action (Supplementary Tables 10 and 11).
Discussion
In our study, conducted in 12 diverse countries (Fig. 4), we found strong evidence that climate disinformation powerfully influences how people feel about, think of and evaluate climate change and climate actions. Presentation of climate disinformation not only influenced participants’ beliefs in anthropogenic climate change and evaluations of climate action but also reduced their ability to discriminate between true and false climate statements. Moreover, processing climate disinformation directly affected pro-environmental behaviour, lowering participants’ performance in a validated behavioural task with positive real-world climate consequences164. Although exploratory analyses suggested that the psychological inoculations may have been able to counteract the negative impact of the first climate disinformation statement, we found no confirmatory evidence that the six psychological inoculations had a protective effect against the 20 climate disinformation statements, except that the accuracy inoculation significantly protected truth discernment ability. One potential explanation for the lack of significant effects in most preregistered analyses is that the true inoculation effects against the 20 climate disinformation statements may have been lower than our a priori effect size of interest. This effect size was based on the meta-analytic effect of fact-checking one disinformation statement in general165; it should be noted, however, that a more recent meta-analysis found no significant effect and high heterogeneity of fact-checking misinformation about scientific evidence, especially for polarized scientific topics such as climate change174.
Even though our results do not yield evidence for promising main effects of the inoculations, they explained significant (albeit suggestive) trial-by-trial treatment heterogeneity by a preregistered moderator—that is, the tendency for intuitive or deliberative thinking. At the theoretical level, these moderation effects confirm the utility of using a comprehensive model of (anti)science belief formation and updating. A tendency for intuitive thinking increased the protection granted by the trust inoculation—that is, an inoculation acting on the socio-affective pathway—while a tendency for deliberative thinking increased the protection given by all three psychological inoculation strategies acting on the cognitive pathway (scientific consensus, transparent communication and accuracy inoculations). These results echo calls to systematically investigate the treatment heterogeneity of behavioural interventions across multiple moderators of interest168,174,175. They furthermore suggest that the theoretical framework presented here can be used not only to identify coherent sets of psychological inoculation strategies but also to identify moderators explaining treatment heterogeneity due to a tendency for specific thinking styles. Taken together, our results suggest that the protective effects of psychological inoculations may be conditional to tailoring them to the pathways of (anti)scientific belief formation and updating that are prevalent in the target population.
At the applied level, our findings indicate that psychological inoculations have no unintended consequences—for example, backfire effects67,171—but may have no or only very limited capacity to protect against multiple disinformation statements related to climate change67 (cf. ref. 176). Even the strategy of reinforcing the inoculated protection with follow-up ‘booster shots’68,103,177 may be untenable, as our evidence suggests that they would already be needed after encountering a single climate disinformation statement. Weighting the influence and discursive presence30,178,179 of climate disinformation with the partial effectiveness of psychological inoculations, our findings join the recent discussion suggesting that behavioural science interventions do not seem to be efficacious enough to tackle systemic problems such as climate disinformation by themselves59,60,180–182. Systemic interventions, such as content moderation, virality circuit breakers180, deplatforming178,183 or changing online engagement metrics towards the accuracy of information184, may be better at curbing climate disinformation. However, systemic actions are enforceable only by the same platforms that might be incentivized to let climate disinformation spread184–190. For this reason, both behavioural and systemic intervention approaches need to be further developed and applied, especially given the direct evidence provided here that climate disinformation drastically decreases climate-relevant judgements and behaviours.
These conclusions are to be weighed against the study limitations. First, our sample was smaller than the one required to detect an effect closer to a more conservative estimate of the effect of psychological inoculations67. Second, while we aimed for more generalizability across populations than previous studies166, we pragmatically recruited participants from English-speaking countries and presented them one-size-fits-all psychological inoculations. Fully generalizable interventions would also require non-English-speaking participants, with inoculations adapted to the context of delivery. Third, all psychological inoculations were delivered as texts to be passively read, instead of engaging, multimodal videos or activities that could have motivated more processing67,191.
While the psychological inoculations evaluated in this Registered Report were of limited efficacy, the framework proposed here can be helpful to systematize future psychological inoculation research to fight climate disinformation. It may generate new psychological inoculations, which can result in more systematic research192 on psychological inoculations, their moderators and climate-relevant outcomes. For example, one of the reviewers suggested developing a ‘pluralistic ignorance’ inoculation, whereby participants are made aware that most of their peers support climate mitigation action, and that awareness should increase support of climate action193,194. Many-labs and mega-studies approaches, promising recent frameworks for systematically creating and testing multiple interventions195–197 with large sample sizes that could help the detection of small intervention effects, can be applied to test sets of theory-guided psychological inoculation strategies against a validated set of climate disinformation statements, measuring climate-relevant outcomes and mapping the heterogeneity generated by model-identified individual differences. Such a combination could produce the next generation of psychological inoculations, which may yield better protection from climate disinformation.
Methods
Ethics information
The study was approved by the University Commission for Ethical Research in Geneva (CUREG2.0) of the University of Geneva, Switzerland (ID: CUREG-2022-05-43-Corr-Brosch). The participants explicitly consented to the study at the beginning of the survey; they were compensated for their time.
Design
The study followed a mixed design. The participants were randomly assigned to one of eight different between-participants conditions: pure control (no inoculation and no disinformation), passive disinformation control (disinformation without inoculation), scientific consensus inoculation, trust inoculation, transparent communication inoculation, moralization inoculation, accuracy inoculation and positive emotions inoculation. We chose a passive disinformation control condition over an active or positive control to better mimic real-life information environments, where climate disinformation is most frequently encountered passively and in multiple occurrences. The participants and experimenters were blind to the name and aim of the condition that the participants were randomized into (double blind). The experiment contained 20 within-participant repeated measures of affect towards climate mitigation actions, assessed after each of the 20 climate disinformation statements.
Procedure
The participants accessed the survey through an anonymous link made available by the panel provider and provided their explicit consent to the study. After consenting, the participants reported their demographics (gender, age, education and political orientation (single-item, ten-point scale: 1 = ‘extreme liberalism/left’ to 10 = ‘extreme conservativism/right’)), completed a baseline measure of affect towards climate action and completed the CRT-2 (ref. 198), in random order. A two-strikes-out attention check (‘Please select ‘3’ to make sure you are paying attention’) was presented; failing it triggered a warning with a ten-second time penalty. Inattentive participants received the attention check for a second time, and participants found inattentive again afterwards were screened out of the survey (N = 16). Attentive participants were then randomly allocated to one of the eight conditions and received the assigned intervention or, for participants in the passive disinformation control condition, were directly forwarded to the following section of the experiment. Participants in the pure control condition received neither the interventions nor the climate disinformation statements. All interventions were presented sequentially in four screens, with a 5–20 s time lock (depending on the content length of each screen) that did not allow the participants to manually proceed to the next screen until the time had elapsed. A manipulation check measuring the participants’ motivation to resist persuasion170 followed. Afterwards, the participants received 20 real climate disinformation statements in the form of anonymous tweets, in randomized order with a 2 s time lock, and reported their affect towards actions to mitigate climate change after each disinformation statement. Following the disinformation provision, the participants completed the climate change perceptions scale162, a modified version of the WEPT164,199 and the truth discernment task, all described in full detail below. Finally, we probed the participants’ understanding of the experimental aims with an open-ended question to account for potential demand effects. The survey ended with an extended debriefing that contained a reminder of the scientific consensus behind climate change with a link to the latest IPCC report. The survey duration was about 21 minutes.
The six inoculations
The inoculations were adapted to the same presentation format, as textual stimuli divided into two paragraphs. All inoculations contained an opening paragraph referring to the IPCC assessment of anthropogenic climate change, ‘In their latest assessment, the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) has declared that anthropogenic climate change is happening, and urgent action is needed to prevent irreversible negative effects on the planet and society’, followed by a pre-emptive warning of incoming threatening information38: ‘However, some politically-motivated groups use misleading tactics to try to convince the public that there is a lot of disagreement among scientists and that climate action is useless or harmful to society’. The second paragraph contained the inoculation itself (Table 2), introduced by the sentence: ‘When confronted with such misleading information about the science of climate change and the actions to mitigate it,…’. To minimize any differences between experimental conditions other than the theory-derived experimental variations, we created a reference text and maintained the thematic information to be as similar as possible across inoculations operating at the same communication basis, but varying the salience of aspects that make the different psychological drivers salient. Both the scientific consensus inoculation and the trust inoculation operated by changing the perception of the source of scientific messages about climate change, either by stressing the consensus about anthropogenic climate change within the scientific community38 or by making the trustworthiness of IPCC scientists salient96. Both the transparent information inoculation and the moralization inoculation operated by emphasizing specific aspects of the presented climate mitigation actions. For the transparent communication inoculation, a transparent discussion of the societal costs of climate action including pros and cons of climate action preceded the disinformation; for the moralization inoculation, the importance of climate action was framed using moralizing words taken from the Moral Foundation Dictionary200 to increase its link with people’s moral convictions201,202. Finally, both the accuracy inoculation and the positive emotions inoculation operated by changing the internal state of the recipient. In the accuracy inoculation, we reframed the original accuracy prompt135 into a passive psychological inoculation191 where the participants were prompted to consider accuracy when evaluating the information, whereas in the positive emotions inoculation, the participants were prompted to consider positive emotions related to acting in a climate-friendly way. The complete text for all interventions is provided in Table 2, with cross-condition differences underlined.
Disinformation provision
The disinformation provision section of the experiment consists of 20 different actual disinformation statements collected from Twitter and pre-validated in a pilot study (Table 3). We followed a two-step procedure for the selection of the climate disinformation statements. First, we created a list of the available Twitter handles of members of the climate change countermovement by reviewing academic and journalistic resources that identified actors that have been spreading disinformation about climate change science and policies11,13–16 (https://www.desmog.com/climate-disinformation-database/). We augmented this list with all the Twitter-active members of the ‘World Climate Declaration’ (https://clintel.org/world-climate-declaration/), a document with 500 signatories—at the time of data collection—that misinforms the public about anthropogenic climate change. Through academic access to the Twitter API, we collected all the tweets by these users, first from account creation until April 2022, and a second time from 31 October to 20 November 2022, the week leading to and the two weeks of the UNFCCC Conference of the Parties 27. Second, we randomly selected and manually coded 20,000 of their tweets according to climate relatedness (1 = ‘not at all related to climate change’ to 4 = ‘absolutely related to climate change’), disinformation (1 = ‘not at all disinformation about climate change’ to 4 = ‘absolutely disinformation about climate change’) and delay (1 = ‘not at all a delay argument about climate change policies’ to 4 = ‘absolutely a delay argument about climate change policies’) status, following the coding schema and instructions by Coan et al.33 and Lamb et al.14. From a final pool of N = 1,033 tweets identified as climate related and disinformation/delay, we identified N = 79 tweets that were understandable without requiring background information and not including country-specific aspects. These 79 tweets were pretested with a representative sample of N = 504 British participants on the data collection platform Prolific (https://www.prolific.co/), in terms of their impact on affect towards climate action and 12 further variables—for example, perceived political slant—that may affect processing of disinformation about political topics203 (the full list of disinformation, with a description of the pretesting design and all validation materials and data, can be found at https://osf.io/m58zx). Among these 79 statements, we selected N = 20 disinformation statements (Table 3) that deviated the least from the mean ratings across all 15 validation measures, evenly divided between 10 disinformation statements about climate science and 10 disinformation statements to delay climate action (according to coding criteria developed in previous research14,33). During the experiment, the participants were presented with all 20 selected climate disinformation statements in randomized order. Each statement was presented as an anonymous tweet, with the default user image, no identifying information and no engagement metrics. After each disinformation statement, the participants rated their affect towards climate actions on a visual analogue scale.
Affect towards climate mitigation actions
We measured the participants’ affect towards actions to mitigate climate change with a visual analogue scale adapted from previous research96,204 (‘In general, what kind of feelings do you have when you think about actions to mitigate climate change?’; 0 = ‘very negative’, 50 = ‘neutral’, 100 = ‘very positive’; scale anchored at 50).
Climate change beliefs
We assessed the participants’ beliefs about climate change with the climate change perception scale162, a validated scale that encompasses different dimensions of the appraisal of climate science and the consequences of climate change. While the published scale is composed of five different subscales and related factors, the authors note that the climate change perception scale allows for the selection of subscales of interest162. We therefore focused on the three subscales measuring participants’ belief in the reality of climate change, the causes of climate change and the consequences of climate change. Climate change beliefs were collected with nine items (reality subscale: ‘I believe that climate change is real’; ‘Climate change is NOT occurring’ (reverse scored); ‘I do NOT believe that climate change is real’ (reverse scored); Cronbach’s α = 0.81; causes subscale: ‘Human activities are a major cause of climate change’; ‘Climate change is mostly caused by human activity’; ‘The main causes of climate change are human activities’; Cronbach’s α = 0.91; consequences subscale: ‘Overall, climate change will bring more negative than positive consequences to the world’; ‘Climate change will bring about serious negative consequences’; ‘The consequences of climate change will be very serious’; 1 = ‘strongly disagree’, 7 = ‘strongly agree’; Cronbach’s α = 0.82; items for each subscale were mean-scored).
The pro-environmental behaviour task
We used a shortened version199 of the WEPT164,199, a validated, multi-trial web-based procedure to measure actual pro-environmental behaviour. In this task, participants can voluntarily choose to exert effort by screening numerical stimuli for the occurrence of target numbers beginning with an even digit and ending with an odd digit (for example, ‘23’). In this version of the WEPT, the participants were able to complete up to eight different numerical screenings of 60 numbers per page. The participants’ willingness to engage in the screening task was prompted before each new page with a yes/no question: participants who answered positively were directed to screening the numbers; participants who answered negatively were directed to the following section of the study. In the instructions, we explicitly explained to the participants that each screening page they accurately completed would result in an actual tree being planted by an environmental organization, the Eden Reforestation Project (https://www.edenprojects.org/), with whom we partnered to plant trees. In other words, the participants were able to create actual environmental benefits (measured in terms of trees planted by the environmental organization) at an actual behavioural cost (personal time)164,199. They were able to track their tree-planting progress, from zero to up to eight trees, with an image presented between the pages of the numerical screenings. We measured their pro-environmental behaviour in terms of the number of pages that each participant completed while correctly screening more than 90% of the target numbers164,199.
Truth discernment task
Inspired by recent work presenting a measure of domain-general news veracity discernment163, we developed a climate-specific truth discernment task in which the participants had to categorize 20 statements mentioning climate-related topics as either false or real statements (‘Please categorize the following statements as either “False Statement” or “Real Statement”’; binary choice: ‘Real’ or ‘False’; item and response order randomized). These 20 statements were equally divided between true and false headlines and between supporting and opposing climate science and action. All statements were generated by interacting with an AI tool (ChatGPT version 4, by OpenAI). Over 300 true and false statements mentioning climate change or climate mitigation actions were initially created. The statements were then fact-checked and condensed into a longlist out of which 10 true and 10 false statements were selected to be included in the truth discernment task. The final statements are presented in Table 4 (the full list of generated statements is available in the Open Science Framework repository).
Tendency for intuitive versus deliberative thinking (CRT-2)
We assessed the participants’ tendency for intuitive versus deliberative/reflective thinking using version 2 (ref. 198) of the CRT205. This task comprises four open-ended, verbal problems that have an intuitive but incorrect answer and require reflection to correctly answer: ‘If you’re running a race and you pass the person in second place, what place are you in?’ (intuitive answer: first; correct answer: second); ‘A farmer had 15 sheep and all but 8 died. How many are left?’ (intuitive answer: seven; correct answer: eight); ‘Emily’s father has three daughters. The first two are named April and May. What is the third daughter’s name?’ (intuitive answer: June; correct answer: Emily); and ‘How many cubic feet of dirt are there in a hole that is 3’ deep x 3’ wide x 3’ long?’ (intuitive answer: 27; correct answer: none). We used the CRT-2 instead of the traditional version because it shares less variance with numerical skills198. Numeracy skills vary across countries206 and could therefore confound the original measure of tendency for intuitive thinking. We computed the CRT-2 score as the number of correct answers given, ranging from 0 to 4, where lower scores represent an increasing tendency for intuitive thinking, and higher scores represent an increasing tendency for deliberative thinking.
Manipulation check
Following the psychological inoculations literature207, we measured motivation to resist persuasion as a theoretically and experimentally validated manipulation check208 with the four-item motivational threat measure proposed by Banas and Richards209 (‘Indicate your level of agreement with the following statements’: 1 = ‘strongly disagree’, 7 = ‘strongly agree’; ‘I want to defend my current attitudes from attack’; ‘I feel motivated to think about why I hold the beliefs I do about climate change’; ‘I feel motivated to resist persuasive messages about climate change’; ‘I want to counterargue conspiracy theories about climate change’; items were mean scored as Cronbach’s α = 0.76). We expected motivation to resist persuasion to increase for participants receiving the different inoculations, compared with participants in the passive disinformation control condition.
Demand effects check
We probed the participants’ understanding of the aim of the experiment by asking them, ‘Could you please describe what you think the aim of the experiment was?’ Two coders then rated the participants’ belief in the experimental objective with a multiple-choice question (‘To what degree do you think the participant believed we were testing interventions to fight climate disinformation?’; 0 = ‘They seemed very convinced we were not testing interventions to fight climate disinformation’; 1 = ‘They seemed somewhat convinced we were not testing interventions to fight climate disinformation’; 2 = ‘They seemed unsure if we were testing interventions to fight climate disinformation’; 3 = ‘They seemed somewhat convinced we were testing interventions to fight climate disinformation’; 4 = ‘They seemed very convinced we were testing interventions to fight climate disinformation’; the ratings were averaged, and differences in scoring were discussed and resolved).
Sampling plan
We collected the sample with quotas for gender and age from the panel provider Market Science Institute. The sample comprised participants from 12 countries, N = 568 participants per country, for a total of N = 6,816 (mean age, 39.15 ± 14.17; N = 3,555 female) participants. We identified the required sample size a priori, with G*Power (version 3.0; ref. 210), to have 95% power to detect a difference between any intervention condition and the passive disinformation control condition of δ = 0.20 in a one-tailed t-test with α = 0.005, for all main hypotheses separately. We selected the smallest effect size of interest (SESOI) from the lower bound of the confidence interval of the meta-analytically identified effect size211 of fact-checking interventions on political topics165, as we reasoned that a new disinformation intervention would be of interest if and only if it has an effect that is larger than already available interventions such as fact-checks. Incidentally, a recent paper showed that the effects of more established psychological inoculations on sharing intentions of manipulative content is δ = 0.20 (ref. 191), increasing our confidence in the practical interest of this SESOI.
Countries
We recruited participants based in the United States, Canada, the United Kingdom, Ireland, Australia, New Zealand, Singapore, the Philippines, India, Pakistan, Nigeria and South Africa (Fig. 4) to generalize our findings on the effectiveness of the six psychological inoculations across the globe and in non-WEIRD contexts. We settled on 12 countries as the minimum number of countries to provide a reasonably accurate statistical estimation for country-level variation in our dependent variables as a random effect in multilevel models212 rather than conducting cross-country comparisons. The 12 countries were furthermore chosen pragmatically for English being the main or one of the official languages, to maintain the climate disinformation statements in their original language and therefore maintain the highest ecological validity.
Data inclusion
Participants were removed from the survey and replaced with new respondents when they (1) did not consent to the study (N = 118), (2) did not finish the study (N = 1,037) or (3) failed the two-strikes-out attention check (N = 16). All incomplete responses and complete responses where participants no longer consented to the study at the end of the survey were removed; all other responses were included in the data analyses.
Analysis plan
The data were analysed with the most recent version of R available at the time of data collection completion (version 4.1.3), with packages lme4 (ref. 213), lmerTest214, TOSTER215, DHARMa170 and emmeans216. Unless specified, we tested the hypotheses with multilevel models.
Manipulation check
We analysed the differences between the passive disinformation control condition and the six inoculation conditions in terms of the motivation to resist persuasion177 with a set of six independent-sample, one-tailed t-tests. We expected all inoculated participants to report significantly more motivation to resist messages countering climate science and climate mitigation action than participants in the passive disinformation control condition.
Primary hypotheses
Gender, age and political ideology were added as covariates in all models. All random effects of multilevel models were weighed separately with Akaike information criterion (AIC) model comparison, and the random effect structures with an AIC value within 2 of the best model’s AIC were used in each analysis.
We analysed changes in affect towards climate mitigation actions during the disinformation provision as the dependent variable with a multilevel model. We specified three random effects: an intercept for participant; an intercept for country, to account for the variance associated with each country212,190; and a random intercept for the internal numbering of the climate disinformation statements, to proactively account for any variance associated with each particular climate disinformation statement190, as some differences across validation measures remained after the selection of the set of disinformation stimuli. Unless otherwise specified, we specified the following as fixed effects: condition (factor, seven levels, dummy coded with 0 = passive disinformation control condition as the reference contrast), trial (continuous variable, from 1 to 20) and the two-way interactions of trial with condition.
We analysed performance in the modified version of the pro-environmental behaviour task as the dependent variable with a multilevel model. Unless otherwise specified, we specified condition (factor, seven levels, dummy coded with 0 = passive disinformation control condition as the reference contrast) as the fixed effect.
To test whether inoculated participants had more positive affect towards climate mitigation action than participants in the control condition after receiving 20 climate disinformation statements, we first compared affect toward climate action at the end of the intervention (that is, after the 20th disinformation statement) of the participants in the passive disinformation control condition with that of the participants in each inoculation condition separately, with a one-tailed independent-sample t-test with α corrected to 0.005. As none of the contrasts were significant, we first visually inspected the affect curve of the 20 measurements of affect across the processing of the 20 climate disinformation statements and did not visually identify ceiling or flooring effects for the intervention conditions. We then tested whether the difference between the passive disinformation control condition and the inoculation of interest is smaller than our SESOI (δ = 0.20) with equivalence testing (RQ1; see the Design Table of the Stage 1 Protocol).
To test whether inoculated participants had more positive affect towards climate mitigation action than participants in the passive disinformation control condition after each one of the 20 climate disinformation statements, we analysed changes in affect towards climate mitigation actions during the disinformation provision with a multilevel model (RQ1; see the Design Table of the Stage 1 Protocol). We specified three random effects: an intercept for participant; an intercept for the internal numbering of the climate disinformation statements, to account for the variance associated with each disinformation statement190; and an intercept for country (factor, alphabetically coded), to account for the variance associated with each country212. We specified the following as fixed effects: condition (factor, seven levels, dummy coded with 0 = passive disinformation control condition as the reference contrast), trial (continuous variable, from 1 to 20) and the two-way interactions of trial with condition to test whether inoculated participants had more positive affect towards climate mitigation action than participants in the passive disinformation control condition after each climate disinformation statement.
To test whether inoculated participants reported believing more in the reality, causes and consequences of climate change than participants in the passive disinformation control condition after receiving 20 climate disinformation statements, we analysed the climate change perception subscales with three multilevel models (RQ3; see the Design Table of the Stage 1 Protocol). We specified one random effect: an intercept for country (factor, alphabetically coded), to account for the variance associated with each country190. We specified condition (factor, seven levels, dummy coded with 0 = passive disinformation control condition as the reference contrast) as the only fixed effect besides the covariates. We first visually inspected the raincloud distribution of the responses of each climate change perception subscale162, to visually identify ceiling or flooring effects. Upon visual confirmation of a normal distribution, we tested whether the difference between the passive disinformation control condition and the inoculation of interest was smaller than our SESOI (δ = 0.20) with equivalence testing.
To test whether inoculated participants completed more pages in the WEPT than participants in the passive disinformation control condition after receiving 20 climate disinformation statements, we analysed the performance in the modified version of the WEPT with a multilevel model, with the number of completed pages as the dependent variable (RQ2; see the Design Table). We did not specify the expected distribution of the WEPT responses in the Stage 1 report. For transparency, we assumed the data would be Poisson distributed, as the WEPT dependent variable was a count; for completeness, we present the more common linear multilevel model in Supplementary Table 6 (the results do not differ). We specified one random effect: an intercept for country (factor, alphabetically coded), to account for the variance associated with each country212. We specified condition (factor, seven levels, dummy coded with 0 = passive disinformation control condition as the reference contrast) as the only fixed effect besides the covariates. We plotted the WEPT performance data and identified a flooring effect.
To test whether inoculated participants have higher news veracity discernment163 than participants in the passive disinformation control condition after receiving 20 climate disinformation statements, we analysed the performance in the truth discernment task with a multilevel model (RQ4; see the Design Table). We calculated news veracity discernment as the sum of correct identifications of true and false climate-related statements163. We specified one random effect: an intercept for country (factor, alphabetically coded), to account for the variance associated with each country212. We specified condition (factor, seven levels, dummy coded with 0 = passive disinformation control condition as the reference contrast) as the only fixed effect besides the covariates. We tested whether the difference between the passive disinformation control condition and the inoculation of interest was smaller than our SESOI (δ = 0.20) with equivalence testing. We furthermore calculated the real news detection and the false news detection scores163, to investigate whether the inoculations influenced only one of the two underlying factors of the general news veracity discernment score.
Secondary hypothesis
For the secondary hypothesis analysis, we limited our sample to those participants who received one of the six inoculations (N = 5,112). We analysed affect towards climate action mitigation during the disinformation provision with the multilevel model used for Hypothesis H1A. We added the CRT-2 score (continuous, range from 0 to 4) as a fixed predictor; we substituted the ‘condition’ variable with a ‘drivers’ factor (two levels: socio-affective and cognitive), each containing the corresponding psychological inoculations (socio-affective: trust inoculation, moralization inoculation and positive emotions inoculation; cognitive: scientific consensus inoculation, transparent communication inoculation and accuracy inoculation); and we added the two-way interactions of driver with CRT-2 score, the two-way interactions of trial with CRT-2 score and the three-way interaction of driver, trial and CRT-2 score.
Control analyses
For the Hcontrol A–D analyses, we limited our sample to those participants who participated in the pure control and passive disinformation control conditions (N = 1,704). We tested whether consecutively presenting the 20 real climate disinformation statements decreased participants’ (A) affect towards climate mitigation action, (B) beliefs in climate change, (C) performance in the modified version of the WEPT164 and (D) truth discernment. We compared affect toward climate action, belief in climate change, WEPT performance and truth discernment at the end of the intervention (that is, after the 20th disinformation statement) of the participants in the passive disinformation control condition with those of the participants in the pure control condition, separately, with a one-tailed independent-sample t-test with α corrected to 0.005. For hypothesis Hcontrol 1A-bis, we conducted an additional one-tailed, paired-sample t-test within the passive disinformation control condition, with affect towards climate mitigation action as the dependent variable.
To account for potential demand effects, we introduced the ‘demand effects check’ measure as a control variable for H1A,B. If our participants were influenced by demand effects, we would have expected the variable to moderate the effectiveness of the psychological inoculations, such that participants who received a psychological inoculation and understood the experimental aim would have reported more positive affect towards climate action overall. We added the ‘demand effects check’ score (continuous, range from 0 to 4) as a fixed predictor as a main effect and a two-way interaction with condition. We conducted a second multilevel model within the passive disinformation control condition, to assess whether demand effects might influence the disinformation provision. We specified three random effects: an intercept for participant; an intercept for the internal numbering of the climate disinformation statements, to account for the variance associated with each disinformation statement190; and an intercept for country (factor, alphabetically coded), to account for the variance associated with each country212. We specified the following as fixed effects: ‘demand effects check’ (continuous, range from 0 to 4), trial (continuous variable, from 1 to 20) and the two-way interactions of trial with demand effects check. Only N = 78 (1.14%) participants reported having guessed the aim of the study. There was no evidence that potential demand effects moderated the effectiveness of the psychological inoculations or the disinformation provision (Supplementary Tables 13–15).
Finally, although the six psychological inoculations presented here were conceptualized as broad-spectrum inoculations191, it was possible that the content of specific climate disinformation statements matched the thematical content of specific psychological inoculations more closely than others, and that this thematic match would have increased the protective effect of the psychological inoculation. To address this possibility, we manually coded whether specific climate disinformation statements are thematic matches with one of the different psychological inoculations (Supplementary Table 2). To compare the effectiveness of the psychological inoculation between matching and unmatching climate disinformation statements, we analysed changes in affect towards climate mitigation actions during the disinformation provision with four additional multilevel models, one for each psychological inoculation where we could identify at least one thematic match. We specified four random effects: a slope per trial, an intercept per participant, an intercept per climate disinformation statement and an intercept per country. We specified the following as fixed effects: condition (factor, two levels, specific psychological inoculation and passive disinformation control), trial (continuous variable, from 1 to 20) and the interaction between ‘thematic match’ (factor, two levels, matching and not matching) and condition. If a thematic match between climate disinformation statements and specific psychological inoculations did indeed increase the protective effects of the inoculation, we expected the interaction to be significant, and the simple slopes to highlight a significant difference between thematically matching and thematically non-matching climate disinformation statements in the inoculation condition, so that the difference in affect would have been smaller for climate disinformation statements that are thematic matches of the psychological inoculation.
Protocol registration
The Stage 1 protocol for this Registered Report was accepted in principle on 20 April 2023. The protocol, as accepted by the journal, can be found at https://figshare.com/s/f431f656b53ec90396c0.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank the Canton of Geneva and the Service Industriels de Genève for funding this project (grant no. UN10765 awarded to T.B., U.J.J.H. and E.T.). Moreover, U.J.J.H. thanks the Swiss National Science Foundation for providing individual career funding (SNSF Eccellenza PCEFPI_203283). We also thank C. Mumenthaler for help with the Twitter data collection and K. Doell for help on the WEPT version used in this study. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Author contributions
T.S.: conceptualization, methodology, software, formal analysis, investigation, resources, data curation, writing (original draft), visualization and project administration. U.J.J.H.: conceptualization, methodology, resources, writing (review and editing), supervision and funding acquisition. E.T.: conceptualization, writing (review and editing), supervision and funding acquisition. T.B.: conceptualization, methodology, resources, writing (review and editing), supervision, funding acquisition and project administration.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Human Behaviour thanks Mikey Biddlestone, David Mellor and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Data availability
The anonymized data, Qualtrics files and stimuli are available on the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/m58zx.
Code availability
The R code necessary to reproduce our results is available on the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/m58zx.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41562-023-01736-0.
References
- 1.IPCC Climate Change 1995: Synthesis Report (eds Bolin, B. et al.) (Cambridge Univ. Press, 1995).
- 2.IPCC Climate Change 2021: The Physical Science Basis (eds Masson-Delmotte, V. et al.) (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2022).
- 3.Cook, J. in Research Handbook on Communicating Climate Change (eds Holmes, D. C. & Richardson, L. M.) 62–78 (Edward Elgar, 2020).
- 4.Lynas M, et al. Greater than 99% consensus on human caused climate change in the peer-reviewed scientific literature. Environ. Res. Lett. 2021;16:114005. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Myers KF, et al. Consensus revisited: quantifying scientific agreement on climate change and climate expertise among Earth scientists 10 years later. Environ. Res. Lett. 2021;16:104030. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kácha O, et al. Four Europes: climate change beliefs and attitudes predict behavior and policy preferences using a latent class analysis on 23 countries. J. Environ. Psychol. 2022;81:101815. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Leiserowitz, A. et al. International Public Opinion on Climate Change, 2022 (Yale Program on Climate Change Communication and Data for Good at Meta, 2022).
- 8.Funk, C. et al. Science and Scientists Held in High Esteem Across Global Publics (Pew Research Center, 2020).
- 9.Almiron, N. et al. Dominant counter-frames in influential climate contrarian European think tanks. Clim. Change162, 2003–2020 (2020).
- 10.Dunlap, R. E. & Brulle, R. J. in Research Handbook on Communicating Climate Change (eds Holmes, D. C. & Richardson, L. M.) 49–61 (Edward Elgar, 2020).
- 11.Farrell J. Network structure and influence of the climate change counter-movement. Nat. Clim. Change. 2016;6:370–374. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Franta B. Early oil industry knowledge of CO2 and global warming. Nat. Clim. Change. 2018;8:1024–1025. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hornsey MJ, et al. Relationships among conspiratorial beliefs, conservatism and climate scepticism across nations. Nat. Clim. Change. 2018;8:614–620. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lamb WF, et al. Discourses of climate delay. Glob. Sustain. 2020;3:e17. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Levantesi, S. I Bugiardi del Clima: Potere, Politica, Psicologia di Chi Nega la Crisi del Secolo (Laterza, 2021).
- 16.Mann, M. E. The New Climate War: The Fight to Take Back Our Planet (Scribe, 2021).
- 17.Oreskes, N. & Conway, E. M. Merchants of Doubt: How a Handful of Scientists Obscured the Truth on Issues from Tobacco Smoke to Global Warming (Bloomsbury, 2010).
- 18.Aronczyk, M. & Espinoza, M. I. A Strategic Nature: Public Relations and the Politics of American Environmentalism (Oxford Univ. Press, 2021).
- 19.Brulle, R. J. The climate lobby: a sectoral analysis of lobbying spending on climate change in the USA, 2000 to 2016. Clim. Change149, 289–303 (2018).
- 20.Brulle, R. J. et al. Obstructing action: foundation funding and US climate change counter-movement organizations. Clim.Change166, 17 (2021).
- 21.Brulle, R. J. & Werthman, C. The role of public relations firms in climate change politics. Clim. Change169, 8 (2021).
- 22.Michaels, D. The Triumph of Doubt: Dark Money and the Science of Deception (Oxford Univ. Press, 2020).
- 23.Lewandowsky S. Climate change disinformation and how to combat it. Annu. Rev. Public Health. 2021;42:1–21. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-090419-102409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Southwell BG, et al. Defining and measuring scientific misinformation. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 2022;700:98–111. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Franta B. Early oil industry disinformation on global warming. Environ. Polit. 2021;30:663–668. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Petersen AM, et al. Discrepancy in scientific authority and media visibility of climate change scientists and contrarians. Nat. Commun. 2019;10:3502. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09959-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wetts R. In climate news, statements from large businesses and opponents of climate action receive heightened visibility. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2020;117:19054–19060. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1921526117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bloomfield EF, Tillery D. The circulation of climate change denial online: rhetorical and networking strategies on Facebook. Environ. Commun. 2019;13:23–34. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Treen, K. M. D’I. et al. Online misinformation about climate change. WIREs Clim. Change11, 1757–7780 (2020).
- 30.Falkenberg, M. et al. Growing polarization around climate change on social media. Nat. Clim. Change12, 1114–1121 (2022).
- 31.Jankowicz, N. How to Lose the Information War: Russia, Fake News, and the Future of Conflict (Bloomsbury, 2020).
- 32.Lewandowsky S, et al. Misinformation, disinformation, and violent conflict: from Iraq and the ‘War on Terror’ to future threats to peace. Am. Psychol. 2013;68:487–501. doi: 10.1037/a0034515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Coan TG, et al. Computer-assisted classification of contrarian claims about climate change. Sci. Rep. 2021;11:22320. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-01714-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Supran G, Oreskes N. Rhetoric and frame analysis of ExxonMobil’s climate change communications. One Earth. 2021;4:696–719. [Google Scholar]
- 35.McCright AM, et al. Examining the effectiveness of climate change frames in the face of a climate change denial counter‐frame. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2016;8:76–97. doi: 10.1111/tops.12171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Imundo, M. N. & Rapp, D. N. When fairness is flawed: effects of false balance reporting and weight-of-evidence statements on beliefs and perceptions of climate change. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn.11, 258–271 (2022).
- 37.Kurvers RHJM, et al. Strategic disinformation outperforms honesty in competition for social influence. iScience. 2021;24:103505. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2021.103505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.van der Linden S, et al. Inoculating the public against misinformation about climate change. Glob. Chall. 2017;1:1600008. doi: 10.1002/gch2.201600008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Miton H, Mercier H. Cognitive obstacles to pro-vaccination beliefs. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015;19:633–636. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bromberg-Martin ES, Sharot T. The value of beliefs. Neuron. 2020;106:561–565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kahn-Harris, K. Denial: The Unspeakable Truth (Notting Hill, 2018).
- 42.Loewenstein G, Molnar A. The renaissance of belief-based utility in economics. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018;2:166–167. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Pennycook, G. in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Vol. 67 (ed. Gawronski, B.) Ch. 3 (Academic Press, 2023).
- 44.Tetlock PE. Thinking the unthinkable: sacred values and taboo cognitions. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003;7:320–324. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00135-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Williams D. Socially adaptive belief. Mind Lang. 2021;36:333–354. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Bastardi A, et al. Wishful thinking: belief, desire, and the motivated evaluation of scientific evidence. Psychol. Sci. 2011;22:731–732. doi: 10.1177/0956797611406447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Sharot, T. The Optimism Bias: A Tour of the Irrationally Positive Brain (Knopf Doubleday, 2011).
- 48.Sharot, T. et al. Why and when beliefs change. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.18, 142–151 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 49.Tappin BM, et al. The heart trumps the head: desirability bias in political belief revision. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2017;146:1143–1149. doi: 10.1037/xge0000298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Lodge, M. & Taber, C. S. The Rationalizing Voter (Cambridge Univ. Press, 2013).
- 51.Slovic P, Peters E. Risk perception and affect. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2006;15:322–325. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Daggett C. Petro-masculinity: fossil fuels and authoritarian desire. Millennium. 2018;47:25–44. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jylhä KM, Akrami N. Social dominance orientation and climate change denial: the role of dominance and system justification. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2015;86:108–111. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Tesler M. Elite domination of public doubts about climate change (not evolution) Polit. Commun. 2018;35:306–326. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tranter B, Booth K. Scepticism in a changing climate: a cross-national study. Glob. Environ. Change. 2015;33:154–164. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Weber EU. Breaking cognitive barriers to a sustainable future. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2017;1:0013. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ecker UKH, et al. The psychological drivers of misinformation belief and its resistance to correction. Nat. Rev. Psychol. 2022;1:13–29. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Hornsey MJ, Fielding KS. Understanding (and reducing) inaction on climate change. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2020;14:3–35. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Van Bavel JJ, et al. Political psychology in the digital (mis)information age: a model of news belief and sharing. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2021;15:84–113. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Hornsey, M. J. & Lewandowsky, S. A toolkit for understanding and addressing climate scepticism. Nat. Hum. Behav.6, 1454–1464 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 61.Philipp-Muller A, et al. Why are people antiscience, and what can we do about it? Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2022;119:e2120755119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2120755119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bode L, Vraga E. The Swiss cheese model for mitigating online misinformation. Bull. At. Sci. 2021;77:129–133. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Lewandowsky S, van der Linden S. Countering misinformation and fake news through inoculation and prebunking. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2021;32:348–384. [Google Scholar]
- 64.McGuire WJ. Resistance to persuasion conferred by active and passive prior refutation of the same and alternative counterarguments. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1961;63:326–332. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Cook J, et al. Neutralizing misinformation through inoculation: exposing misleading argumentation techniques reduces their influence. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0175799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Iyengar, A. et al. Inoculation against conspiracy theories: a consumer side approach to India’s fake news problem. Appl. Cogn. Psychol.37, 290–303 (2022).
- 67.Roozenbeek J, et al. Psychological inoculation improves resilience against misinformation on social media. Sci. Adv. 2022;8:eabo6254. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abo6254. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Compton J, et al. Inoculation theory in the post-truth era: extant findings and new frontiers for contested science, misinformation, and conspiracy theories. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass. 2021;15:e12602. [Google Scholar]
- 69.van der Linden S, et al. The gateway belief model: a large-scale replication. J. Environ. Psychol. 2019;62:49–58. [Google Scholar]
- 70.van Stekelenburg, A. et al. Scientific-consensus communication about contested science: a preregistered meta-analysis. Psychol. Sci.33, 1989–2008 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 71.Luntz, F. The Environment: A Cleaner, Safer, Healthier America (The Luntz Research Companies, 2002).
- 72.Algan Y, et al. Trust in scientists in times of pandemic: panel evidence from 12 countries. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2108576118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2108576118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Cummings L. The ‘trust’ heuristic: arguments from authority in public health. Health Commun. 2014;29:1043–1056. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2013.831685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Fairbrother M. Trust and public support for environmental protection in diverse national contexts. Sociol. Sci. 2016;3:359–382. [Google Scholar]
- 75.Fairbrother M. Environmental attitudes and the politics of distrust. Sociol. Compass. 2017;11:e12482. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Fairbrother M, et al. Political trust and the relationship between climate change beliefs and support for fossil fuel taxes: evidence from a survey of 23 European countries. Glob. Environ. Change. 2019;59:102003. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Hahn U, et al. How good is your evidence and how would you know? Top. Cogn. Sci. 2018;10:660–678. doi: 10.1111/tops.12374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Hoogeveen S, et al. The Einstein effect provides global evidence for scientific source credibility effects and the influence of religiosity. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2022;6:523–535. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01273-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Siegrist M. Trust and risk perception: a critical review of the literature. Risk Anal. 2021;41:480–490. doi: 10.1111/risa.13325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Sintov, N. D. & Hurst, K. F. Experimental support for a trust heuristic. J. Risk Res.26, 37–63 (2022).
- 81.Benegal, S. D. & Scruggs, L. A. Correcting misinformation about climate change: the impact of partisanship in an experimental setting. Clim. Change148, 61–80 (2018).
- 82.Song H, et al. Source effects on psychological reactance to regulatory policies: the role of trust and similarity. Sci. Commun. 2018;40:591–620. [Google Scholar]
- 83.von Hohenberg, B. C. & Guess, A. M. When do sources persuade? The effect of source credibility on opinion change. J. Exp. Polit. Sci.10, 328–342 (2022).
- 84.Hameleers M, et al. Whom to trust? Media exposure patterns of citizens with perceptions of misinformation and disinformation related to the news media. Eur. J. Commun. 2022;37:237–268. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Safford TG, et al. Questioning scientific practice: linking beliefs about scientists, science agencies, and climate change. Environ. Sociol. 2020;6:194–206. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Vinck P, et al. Institutional trust and misinformation in the response to the 2018–19 Ebola outbreak in North Kivu, DR Congo: a population-based survey. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2019;19:529–536. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(19)30063-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Klenert D, et al. Making carbon pricing work for citizens. Nat. Clim. Change. 2018;8:669–677. [Google Scholar]
- 88.Ward JK, et al. The French health pass holds lessons for mandatory COVID-19 vaccination. Nat. Med. 2022;28:232–235. doi: 10.1038/s41591-021-01661-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Stadelmann-Steffen I, Eder C. Public opinion in policy contexts: a comparative analysis of domestic energy policies and individual policy preferences in Europe. Int. Polit. Sci. Rev. 2021;42:78–94. [Google Scholar]
- 90.Liu L, et al. Effects of competence- and integrity-based trust on public acceptability of renewable energy projects in China and the Netherlands. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020;67:101390. [Google Scholar]
- 91.Sleeth-Keppler D, et al. It’s a matter of trust: American judgments of the credibility of informal communicators on solutions to climate change. Environ. Commun. 2017;11:17–40. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Clements, L. et al. Wellcome Global Monitor—How COVID-19 Affected People’s Lives and Their Views about Science (Wellcome Trust, 2021)
- 93.Cologna V, Siegrist M. The role of trust for climate change mitigation and adaptation behaviour: a meta-analysis. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020;69:101428. [Google Scholar]
- 94.Hornsey MJ, et al. Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nat. Clim. Change. 2016;6:622–626. [Google Scholar]
- 95.Acerbi, A. et al. Research note: Fighting misinformation or fighting for information? Harv. Kennedy Sch. Misinformation Rev.10.37016/mr-2020-87 (2022).
- 96.Spampatti, T. et al. A trust inoculation to protect public support of governmentally mandated actions to mitigate climate change. Preprint at PsyArXiv10.31234/osf.io/zau32 (2022).
- 97.Acerbi, A. Cognitive attraction and online misinformation. Palgrave Commun.5, 15 (2019).
- 98.Campbell TH, Kay AC. Solution aversion: on the relation between ideology and motivated disbelief. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2014;107:809–824. doi: 10.1037/a0037963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Doell KC, et al. Understanding the effects of partisan identity on climate change. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021;42:54–59. [Google Scholar]
- 100.Weintrobe, S. Psychological Roots of the Climate Crisis: Neoliberal Exceptionalism and the Culture of Uncare (Bloomsbury, 2021).
- 101.Wheeler NE, et al. Ideology and predictive processing: coordination, bias, and polarization in socially constrained error minimization. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2020;34:192–198. [Google Scholar]
- 102.White, L. J. The historical roots of our ecologic crisis. Sci.155, 1203–1207 (1967). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 103.Compton, J. in The SAGE Handbook of Persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice 2nd edn (eds Dillard, J. P., & Shen, L.) 220–236 (Sage, 2013).
- 104.Trouche E, et al. Objective evaluation of demonstrative arguments. Argumentation. 2019;33:23–43. [Google Scholar]
- 105.Valli, C. & Nai, A. Let me think about it: cognitive elaboration and strategies of resistance to political persuasion. Media Psychol.26, 89–112 (2022).
- 106.Zuwerink Jacks J, Cameron KA. Strategies for resisting persuasion. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003;25:145–161. [Google Scholar]
- 107.Nyhan B, Reifler J. The roles of information deficits and identity threat in the prevalence of misperceptions. J. Elect. Public Opin. Parties. 2019;29:222–244. [Google Scholar]
- 108.Altay S, et al. Scaling up interactive argumentation by providing counterarguments with a chatbot. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2022;6:579–592. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01271-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Mercier H. The argumentative theory: predictions and empirical evidence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:689–700. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Schmid P, Betsch C. Effective strategies for rebutting science denialism in public discussions. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2019;3:931–939. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0632-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Carattini S, et al. How to win public support for a global carbon tax. Nature. 2019;565:289–291. doi: 10.1038/d41586-019-00124-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Petersen MB, et al. Transparent communication about negative features of COVID-19 vaccines decreases acceptance but increases trust. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2024597118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2024597118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Kerr, J. R. et al. Transparent communication of evidence does not undermine public trust in evidence. PNAS Nexus1, pgac280 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 114.Blastland M, et al. Nature. 2020;587:362–364. doi: 10.1038/d41586-020-03189-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Carattini, S. et al. Overcoming public resistance to carbon taxes. WIREs Clim. Change9, e531 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 116.Hyland-Wood B, et al. Toward effective government communication strategies in the era of COVID-19. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2021;8:30. [Google Scholar]
- 117.Luttrell A, et al. Making it moral: merely labeling an attitude as moral increases its strength. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2016;65:82–93. [Google Scholar]
- 118.Skitka LJ, et al. The psychology of moral conviction. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2021;72:347–366. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-063020-030612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Bayes R. Moral convictions and threats to science. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 2022;700:86–96. [Google Scholar]
- 120.Amin AB, et al. Association of moral values with vaccine hesitancy. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2017;1:873–880. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0256-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Feinberg M, Willer R. Apocalypse soon? Dire messages reduce belief in global warming by contradicting just-world beliefs. Psychol. Sci. 2011;22:34–38. doi: 10.1177/0956797610391911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Lewandowsky S, Oberauer K. Motivated rejection of science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2016;25:217–222. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Rutjens BT, et al. Not all skepticism is equal: exploring the ideological antecedents of science acceptance and rejection. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2018;44:384–405. doi: 10.1177/0146167217741314. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Hahnel UJJ, et al. Ideology as filter: motivated information processing and decision-making in the energy domain. Sustainability. 2020;12:8429. [Google Scholar]
- 125.Hornsey MJ, Fielding KS. Attitude roots and Jiu Jitsu persuasion: understanding and overcoming the motivated rejection of science. Am. Psychol. 2017;72:459–473. doi: 10.1037/a0040437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Schuldt, J. P. et al. Brief exposure to Pope Francis heightens moral beliefs about climate change. Clim. Change141, 167–177 (2017).
- 127.Wolsko C, et al. Red, white, and blue enough to be green: effects of moral framing on climate change attitudes and conservation behaviors. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2016;65:7–19. [Google Scholar]
- 128.Markowitz EM, Shariff AF. Climate change and moral judgement. Nat. Clim. Change. 2012;2:243–247. [Google Scholar]
- 129.Bago B, et al. Fake news, fast and slow: deliberation reduces belief in false (but not true) statements. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2020;149:1608–1613. doi: 10.1037/xge0000729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Martel C, et al. Reliance on emotion promotes belief in fake news. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2020;5:47. doi: 10.1186/s41235-020-00252-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Yelbuz, B. E. et al. Reflective thinking predicts lower conspiracy beliefs: a meta-analysis. Judgm. Decis. Mak.17, 720–744 (2022).
- 132.Ali, A. & Qazi, I. A. Cognitive reflection is associated with greater truth discernment for COVID-19 headlines, less trust but greater use of formal information sources, and greater willingness to pay for masks among social media users in Pakistan. Harv. Kennedy Sch. Misinformation Rev.10.37016/mr-2020-101 (2022).
- 133.Pennycook, G. et al. Science beliefs, political ideology, and cognitive sophistication. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.152, 80–97 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 134.Pennycook G, Rand DG. The psychology of fake news. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2021;25:388–402. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2021.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Pennycook G, Rand DG. Accuracy prompts are a replicable and generalizable approach for reducing the spread of misinformation. Nat. Commun. 2022;13:2333. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-30073-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Sperber, D. & Mercier, H. The Enigma of Reason (Harvard Univ. Press, 2017).
- 137.Capraro, V. & Celadin, T. ‘I think this news is accurate’: endorsing accuracy decreases the sharing of fake news and increases the sharing of real news. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 01461672221117691 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 138.Pennycook, G. et al. Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention. Psychol. Sci.31, 770–780 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 139.Salovich NA, et al. Evaluative mindsets can protect against the influence of false information. Cognition. 2022;225:105121. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2022.105121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Lutzke L, et al. Priming critical thinking: simple interventions limit the influence of fake news about climate change on Facebook. Glob. Environ. Change. 2019;58:101964. [Google Scholar]
- 141.Arechar, A. A. et al. Understanding and combatting misinformation across 16 countries on six continents. Nat. Hum. Behav.7, 1502–1513 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 142.Forgas, J. P. Happy believers and sad skeptics? Affective influences on gullibility. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci.28, 306–313 (2019).
- 143.Brosch T, et al. Beyond fear: rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychol. Sci. 2008;19:362–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02094.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Brosch T. Affect and emotions as drivers of climate change perception and action: a review. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021;42:15–21. [Google Scholar]
- 145.Conte, B. et al. From values to emotions: cognitive appraisal mediates the impact of core values on emotional experience. Emotion23, 1115–1129 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 146.Brosch T, Steg L. Leveraging emotion for sustainable action. One Earth. 2021;4:1693–1703. [Google Scholar]
- 147.Vosoughi S, et al. The spread of true and false news online. Science. 2018;359:1146–1151. doi: 10.1126/science.aap9559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Bago B, et al. Emotion may predict susceptibility to fake news but emotion regulation does not seem to help. Cogn. Emot. 2022;36:1166–1180. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2022.2090318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Rosenzweig, L. R. et al. Happiness and surprise are associated with worse truth discernment of COVID-19 headlines among social media users in Nigeria. Harv. Kennedy Sch. Misinformation Rev.10.37016/mr-2020-75 (2021).
- 150.Bouman T, et al. When worry about climate change leads to climate action: how values, worry and personal responsibility relate to various climate actions. Glob. Environ. Change. 2020;62:102061. [Google Scholar]
- 151.Roeser S. Risk communication, public engagement, and climate change: a role for emotions. Risk Anal. 2012;32:1033–1040. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01812.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Smith N, Leiserowitz A. The role of emotion in global warming policy support and opposition. Risk Anal. 2014;34:937–948. doi: 10.1111/risa.12140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Yeo SK, McKasy M. Emotion and humor as misinformation antidotes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2002484118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2002484118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Schneider CR, et al. Positive emotions and climate change. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021;42:114–120. [Google Scholar]
- 155.Shiota MN, et al. Positive affect and behavior change. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2021;39:222–228. [Google Scholar]
- 156.Andreoni J. Impure altruism and donations to public goods: a theory of warm-glow giving. Econ. J. 1990;100:464–477. [Google Scholar]
- 157.Isen AM. Success, failure, attention, and reaction to others: the warm glow of success. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1970;15:294–301. [Google Scholar]
- 158.Jia, L. & Van Der Linden, S. Green but not altruistic warm‐glow predicts conservation behavior. Conserv. Sci. Pract.2 (2020).
- 159.Doell, K. C. et al. Interindividual differences in environmentally relevant positive trait affect impacts sustainable behavior in everyday life. Sci. Rep.11, 20423 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 160.Hartmann P, et al. Warm glow vs. altruistic values: how important is intrinsic emotional reward in proenvironmental behavior? J. Environ. Psychol. 2017;52:43–55. [Google Scholar]
- 161.Steg, L. Limiting climate change requires research on climate action. Nat. Clim. Change8, 759–761 (2018).
- 162.van Valkengoed AM, et al. Development and validation of a climate change perceptions scale. J. Environ. Psychol. 2021;76:101652. [Google Scholar]
- 163.Maertens, R. et al. The Misinformation Susceptibility Test (MIST): a psychometrically validated measure of news veracity discernment. Behav. Res. Methods (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 164.Lange, F. & Dewitte, S. The Work for Environmental Protection Task: a consequential web-based procedure for studying pro-environmental behavior. Behav. Res. Methods54, 133–145 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 165.Walter N, Murphy ST. How to unring the bell: a meta-analytic approach to correction of misinformation. Commun. Monogr. 2018;85:423–441. [Google Scholar]
- 166.Nguyễn, S. et al. Studying mis- and disinformation in Asian diasporic communities: the need for critical transnational research beyond Anglocentrism. Harv. Kennedy Sch. Misinformation Rev.10.37016/mr-2020-95 (2022).
- 167.Camargo, C. Q. & Simon, F. M. Mis- and disinformation studies are too big to fail: six suggestions for the field’s future. Harv. Kennedy Sch. Misinformation Rev.10.37016/mr-2020-106 (2022).
- 168.Bryan, C. J. et al. Behavioural science is unlikely to change the world without a heterogeneity revolution. Nat. Hum. Behav.5, 980–989 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 169.Mills, B. R. MetBrewer: Color palettes inspired by works at the Metropolitan Museum of Art. R package version 0.1.0 (2021).
- 170.Hartig, F. DHARMa: Residual diagnostics for hierarchical (multi-level/mixed) regression models. R package version 0.4.5 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=DHARMa (2022).
- 171.Modirrousta-Galian, A., Higham, P. A. & Seabrooke, T. Effects of inductive learning and gamification on news veracity discernment. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl.29, 599–619 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 172.Batailler C, Brannon SM, Teas PE, Gawronski B. A signal detection approach to understanding the identification of fake news. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2022;17:78–98. doi: 10.1177/1745691620986135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Pretus, C. et al. The role of political devotion in sharing partisan misinformation and resistance to fact-checking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.152, 3116–3134 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 174.Chan, M. S. & Albarracín, D. A meta-analysis of correction effects in science-relevant misinformation. Nat. Hum. Behav.7, 1514–1525 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 175.Tappin BM, Wittenberg C, Hewitt LB, Berinsky AJ, Rand DG. Quantifying the potential persuasive returns to political microtargeting. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2023;120:e2216261120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2216261120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.McPhedran R, et al. Psychological inoculation protects against the social media infodemic. Sci. Rep. 2023;13:5780. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-32962-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Ivanov B, Parker KA, Dillingham LL. Testing the limits of inoculation-generated resistance. West. J. Commun. 2018;82:648–665. [Google Scholar]
- 178.Painter J, et al. Climate delay discourses present in global mainstream television coverage of the IPCC’s 2021 report. Commun. Earth Environ. 2023;4:118. [Google Scholar]
- 179.Gayle, D. & Barnett, A. One in three GB News presenters cast doubt on climate science, study reveals. The Guardianhttps://www.theguardian.com/media/2023/may/26/one-in-three-gb-news-presenters-cast-doubt-climate-science-study (26 May 2023).
- 180.Bak-Coleman, J. B. et al. Combining interventions to reduce the spread of viral misinformation. Nat. Hum. Behav. 6, 1372–1380 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 181.Chater, N. & Loewenstein, G. The i-frame and the s-frame: how focusing on individual-level solutions has led behavioral public policy astray. Behav. Brain Sci.46, E147 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 182.Lorenz-Spreen P, Lewandowsky S, Sunstein CR, Hertwig R. How behavioural sciences can promote truth, autonomy and democratic discourse online. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:1102–1109. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0889-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Thomas DR, Wahedi LA. Disrupting hate: the effect of deplatforming hate organizations on their online audience. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2023;120:e2214080120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2214080120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Globig LK, Holtz N, Sharot T. Changing the incentive structure of social media platforms to halt the spread of misinformation. eLife. 2023;12:e85767. doi: 10.7554/eLife.85767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Huszár F, et al. Algorithmic amplification of politics on Twitter. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2022;119:e2025334119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2025334119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186.Kozyreva A, et al. Resolving content moderation dilemmas between free speech and harmful misinformation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2023;120:e2210666120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2210666120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Rathje S, Van Bavel JJ, van der Linden S. Out-group animosity drives engagement on social media. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2021;118:e2024292118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2024292118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Van Bavel, J. J., Robertson, C., del Rosario, K., Rasmussen, J. & Rathje, S. Social media and morality. Annu. Rev. Psychol. (in the press); preprint at https://psyarxiv.com/ywevq [DOI] [PubMed]
- 189.Roozenbeek, J. & van der Linden, S. Fake news game confers psychological resistance against online misinformation. Palgrave Commun.5, 65 (2019).
- 190.Brady, W. J., et al. Algorithm-mediated social learning in online social networks. Trends Cog. Sci.27, 947–960 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 191.IJzerman H, et al. Use caution when applying behavioural science to policy. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020;4:1092–1094. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00990-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 192.Lees, J., Colaizzi, G., Goldberg, M. H. & Constantino, S. M. Misperceptions of support for climate policy represent multiple phenomena predicted by different factors across intergroup boundaries. Preprint at OSF10.31219/osf.io/vfbq4 (2023).
- 193.Druckman JN, et al. Correcting misperceptions of out-partisans decreases American legislators’ support for undemocratic practices. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2023;120:e2301836120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2301836120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 194.Doell, K. C. Megastudies to test the efficacy of behavioural interventions. Nat. Rev. Psychol.2, 263 (2023).
- 195.Duckworth AL, Milkman KL. A guide to megastudies. PNAS Nexus. 2022;1:pgac214. doi: 10.1093/pnasnexus/pgac214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 196.Voelkel, J. G. et al. Megastudy Identifying Effective Interventions to Strengthen Americans’ Democratic Attitudes (Stanford University, 2023); https://www.strengtheningdemocracychallenge.org/paper
- 197.Thomson, K. S. & Oppenheimer, D. M. Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgm. Decis. Mak.11, 99–113 (2016).
- 198.Lange, F. & Truyens, M. Mixed evidence for the effect of virtual nature exposure on effortful pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol.81, 101803 (2022).
- 199.Graham J, et al. Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2009;96:1029–1046. doi: 10.1037/a0015141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 200.Brady WJ, et al. The MAD model of moral contagion: the role of motivation, attention, and design in the spread of moralized content online. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2020;15:978–1010. doi: 10.1177/1745691620917336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 201.Brady WJ, et al. Emotion shapes the diffusion of moralized content in social networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114:7313–7318. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1618923114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 202.Pennycook G, et al. A practical guide to doing behavioral research on fake news and misinformation. Collabra Psychol. 2021;7:25293. [Google Scholar]
- 203.Spampatti T, et al. Short and long-term dominance of negative information in shaping public energy perceptions: the case of shallow geothermal systems. Energy Policy. 2022;167:113070. [Google Scholar]
- 204.Frederick S. Cognitive reflection and decision making. J. Econ. Perspect. 2005;19:25–42. [Google Scholar]
- 205.OECD Skills Outlook 2013: First Results from the Survey of Adult Skills (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2013).
- 206.Compton J. Threat and/in inoculation theory. Int. J. Commun. 2021;15:4294–4306. [Google Scholar]
- 207.Chester DS, Lasko EN. Construct validation of experimental manipulations in social psychology: current practices and recommendations for the future. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2021;16:377–395. doi: 10.1177/1745691620950684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 208.Banas JA, Richards AS. Apprehension or motivation to defend attitudes? Exploring the underlying threat mechanism in inoculation-induced resistance to persuasion. Commun. Monogr. 2017;84:164–178. [Google Scholar]
- 209.Faul F, et al. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods. 2007;39:175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 210.Perugini M, et al. Safeguard power as a protection against imprecise power estimates. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2014;9:319–332. doi: 10.1177/1745691614528519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 211.Bryan ML, Jenkins SP. Multilevel modelling of country effects: a cautionary tale. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 2016;32:3–22. [Google Scholar]
- 212.Bates, D. et al. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw.10.18637/jss.v067.i01 (2015).
- 213.Kuznetsova, A. et al. lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw.10.18637/jss.v082.i13 (2017).
- 214.Lakens D. Equivalence tests: a practical primer for t tests, correlations, and meta-analyses. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2017;8:355–362. doi: 10.1177/1948550617697177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 215.Lenth, R. V. emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package version 1.6.3 (2021).
- 216.Judd CM, et al. Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: a new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2012;103:54–69. doi: 10.1037/a0028347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The anonymized data, Qualtrics files and stimuli are available on the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/m58zx.
The R code necessary to reproduce our results is available on the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/m58zx.