Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2024 Mar 6;19(3):e0297673. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0297673

Child support grant expansion and cognitive function among women in rural South Africa: Findings from a natural experiment in the HAALSI cohort

Rishika Chakraborty 1,*, Lindsay C Kobayashi 2, Janet Jock 3, Coady Wing 3, Xiwei Chen 4, Meredith Phillips 4, Lisa Berkman 5, Kathleen Kahn 6,7, Chodziwadziwa Whiteson Kabudula 6, Molly Rosenberg 4,6
Editor: Azizur Rahman8
PMCID: PMC10917272  PMID: 38446751

Abstract

Background

Cash transfers are a promising but understudied intervention that may protect cognitive function in adults. Although South Africa has a rapidly ageing population, little is known about the nature of association between cash transfers and cognitive function in this setting.

Objectives

We leveraged age-eligibility expansions to South Africa’s Child Support Grant (CSG) to investigate the association between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function of biological mothers of child beneficiaries in South Africa.

Methods

We analysed 2014/2015 baseline data from 944 women, aged 40–59 years with at least one CSG-eligible child, enrolled in the population-representative HAALSI cohort in Agincourt, South Africa. Duration of CSG eligibility for each mother was calculated based on the birth dates of all their children and the CSG age-eligibility expansion years (2003–2012). Cognitive function was measured using a cognitive battery administered at the HAALSI baseline interview. Linear regression was used to estimate the association between duration of CSG eligibility, dichotomized as low (≤10 years) and high (>10 years) eligibility, and cognitive function z-scores of the mothers.

Results

High vs. low duration of CSG eligibility, was associated with higher cognitive function z-scores in the full sample [β: 0.15 SD units; 95% CI: 0.04, 0.26; p-value = 0.01]. In mothers with one to four lifetime children, but not five or more, high vs. low duration of CSG eligibility, was associated with higher cognitive function z-scores [β: 0.19 SD units; 95% CI: 0.05, 0.34, p-value = 0.02].

Conclusion

Government cash transfers given to support raising children may confer substantial protective effects on the subsequent cognitive function of mothers. Further studies are needed to understand how parity may influence this relationship. Our findings bring evidence to policymakers for designing income supplementation programmes to promote healthy cognitive ageing in low-income settings.

Introduction

In 2019, about 57 million people worldwide were living with dementia and this number is projected to increase to 83 million in 2030, 116 million in 2040 and 153 million by 2050 [1, 2]. Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (ADRD) is one of the leading causes of disabilities, poor health, and decreased quality of life [3]. The burden of ADRD falls disproportionately on people living in low- and middle-income countries, where two-thirds of cases currently occur [4]. Largely due to changes in demographic patterns and population ageing, it is estimated that by 2050 there will be about 7.62 million people living with ADRD in sub-Saharan Africa [5]. Despite this growing public health concern, limited research exists on ADRD risk and prevention in sub-Saharan African countries like South Africa [6, 7].

Prior evidence suggests that socioeconomic conditions throughout the life course shape ADRD risk [812]. A diagnosis of ADRD is typically preceded by a prodromal period of at least five years of accelerated decline in cognitive function [13]. Socioeconomic status indicators across the life course such as education, occupation and income have been linked to cognitive outcomes in older adults [14]. These socioeconomic status indicators may be associated with later life cognitive function by providing individuals with assets or tools to attain resources that can promote healthy cognitive ageing [9]. For instance, higher income allows adults to access nutritious food and healthy housing, which can positively influence their later life cognitive outcomes. In contrast, low income or lack of wealth may lead to chronic stress, which in turn can negatively affect their cognitive health [9].

The cumulative presence of socioeconomic advantages and thus cognitively stimulating exposures across the life course may have a protective effect on cognitive function and ADRD risk by promoting cognitive reserve [15, 16]. Cognitive reserve is a theoretical construct that is thought to improve the brain’s ability to cope with pathological damage and can be accrued across one’s life course [15]. Individuals with higher cognitive reserve may use their pre-existing neural networks more efficiently or may recruit compensatory mechanisms to perform a cognitive task, consequently coping better with brain pathology [17]. Socioeconomic status indicators, such as income, are considered proxy measures for cognitive reserve [18]. However, little is known about the nature of association between income and cognitive function in low-income settings, such as in rural South Africa [6, 19].

South Africa has a rapidly ageing population of people whose lives have been affected by structural racism during Apartheid from 1948 to 1994 [20]. Since the end of Apartheid, South Africa has implemented several social protection programmes, the largest being the Child Support Grant (CSG) [21, 22]. Implemented in 1998, the CSG is an unconditional cash transfer program designed to alleviate child poverty, offset the costs of raising children and improve their health and nutrition by providing financial assistance that supplements household income [23]. While the CSG is intended for the primary caregivers of the child beneficiaries, nearly 90 per cent of recipients are their biological mothers [24, 25]. When the CSG was first rolled out, only children under seven years were eligible. However, between 2003 and 2012, CSG eligibility was progressively expanded over time and, since 2012, children under the age of 18 have been eligible [22]. Prior research has shown that cash transfers like the CSG can raise living standards, improve health outcomes, and empower women within their households and communities [26]. CSG has been associated with several positive outcomes in child beneficiaries including alleviating food insecurity, improving education, school attendance, and anthropometric outcomes [22]. However, few studies have investigated the impacts of CSG transfers on the caregivers of child beneficiaries. One study found that CSG recipient mothers had a higher probability of being in the labor market and being employed [27], while another reported adults in CSG recipient households had improved mental health [28]. Mothers of CSG recipients have also reported having higher financial independence [29], decision making powers [29, 30], lower worry and stress [30], and improved social relationships within households and communities [30].

What remains unclear is whether cash transfers like the CSG can impact cognitive function and ADRD risk among the caregivers of child beneficiaries. Since cash transfers supplement income, they may help accrue cognitive reserve, however, few studies have investigated the causal effects of cash transfers on ADRD risk. Only one study in Mexico [31], one in United States [32], and another in South Africa [33] used natural experiments to demonstrate the causal effects of cash transfers in improving cognitive function in older adults. These studies found that higher social protection income led to substantial improvements in memory and other domains of cognitive function in older adults [31, 32]. Thus, the causal relationship between cash transfers and mid-to-later life cognitive function in adults from low-resource settings such as rural South Africa remain understudied.

We thus aimed to exploit the exogenous variation in CSG eligibility, due to iterative age eligibility expansion policies, as a natural experiment to estimate the causal effect of cash transfers on cognitive function among biological mothers in South Africa. Based on the cognitive reserve theory, we hypothesized that a greater duration of CSG eligibility would improve mid- life cognitive function among mothers, since such exposure increases income, thereby promoting cognitive reserve. This study will contribute to the literature linking social protection policies and cognitive function in an underrepresented and rapidly ageing population in a middle-income country.

Materials and methods

Study setting and design

This study is set in the rural Agincourt area of Bushbuckridge sub-district in Mpumalanga province, South Africa. Since 1992, the study area, currently consisting of 31 villages and approximately 116,000 inhabitants, has been under continuous surveillance through the Agincourt Health and Socio-Demographic Surveillance System (HDSS) run by the Medical Research Council/Wits University Rural Public Health and Health Transitions Research Unit [20, 34]. While the socioeconomic situation of the Agincourt population has improved post-Apartheid, the region still faces difficulties in accessing basic services, such as electricity and piped water [20]. This population is Human Immunodeficiency Virus hyperendemic (~20 per cent prevalence in 2010) [35], and has high unemployment rates [20].

Cash transfers from social protection programmes, such as the CSG, are an important source of household income in the study region [20]. Primary caregivers of age-eligible children whose income falls below a means-tested level (about 10 times the grant amount for single caregivers) are eligible to apply for the CSG [22, 25]. When the CSG was first introduced, uptake rates were low, with only 28 per cent of CSG-eligible households in Agincourt submitting a CSG application in 2001 [25]. However, uptake rates and eligibility have increased over time and the CSG currently reaches over 1.1 million children in Mpumalanga province every month [36]. Since its implementation in 1998, the age eligibility was expanded from children under seven to under age nine in 2003, under age 11 in 2004, under age 14 in 2005, under age 15 in 2009, and through the age of 16 in 2010. Since 2012, children under the age of 18 have been eligible [21, 22]. The grant payment has also increased over time to match inflation, from 100 Rand (~$7 USD) in 1998 to 510 Rand (~$27 USD) in October 2023 per month for each child [37].

Using these CSG age eligibility expansions as natural experiments, we analysed baseline data collected in the 2014/15 ‘Health and Ageing in Africa: A Longitudinal Study of an INDEPTH Community in South Africa (HAALSI)’ [34]. HAALSI is a longitudinal cohort study of 5059 adults (2345 men and 2714 women) aged ≥40 years, representative of the Agincourt HDSS as its sampling frame (~86 per cent response rate). Trained, local fieldworkers collected household and individual data using computer assisted personal interviews [34]. All interviews were conducted in the local Xitsonga language with interview materials translated from English and back-translated for reliability.

Ethical approval for HAALSI was obtained from the Wits University Human Ethics Committee (#M141159), the Harvard T. H. Chan School of Public Health Office of Human Research Administration (#13–1608), and the Mpumalanga Provincial Research and Ethics Committee. Each HAALSI participant provided written informed consent prior to being interviewed. Participants unable to read had a witness and used inked fingerprint to sign consent form. This current study was deemed to be a ‘Not Human Subjects Research’ by the Institutional Review Board of Indiana University Bloomington (protocol #2002584956) since the data was de-identified prior to analysis.

Study sample

Study participants were CSG-eligible women, defined as HAALSI women between the ages of 40 to 59 years who had at least one CSG-eligible biological child. We restricted our sample to only biological mothers (excluding other female relatives/fathers/male relatives) since nearly 90 per cent of primary caregivers receiving the CSG are biological mothers of eligible children [25]. After removing women who did not have any children (n = 224), who did not have CSG-eligible children (n = 1363), and with implausible childbirth ages (≤10 or ≥60 years old) (n = 2), there were 1125 CSG eligible women in the HAALSI cohort. We then excluded women who were ≥ 60 years of age (n = 181) since this group may not be representative of other women in their later-life in this rural South African setting and due to their small sample size. The older women would have been at least 38 years old at the time of childbirth in order to have CSG-eligible children, making their reproductive history fall outside the typical childbearing age which was between 20–34 years in South Africa in 1998 [38]. Therefore, the final analytic sample included 944 CSG-eligible mothers (Fig 1).

Fig 1. Flowchart describing the cohort construction of 944 child support grant eligible mothers enrolled in health and ageing in Africa: A longitudinal study of an INDEPTH community in South Africa (HAALSI), 2014/15.

Fig 1

Study measures

Exposure variable: Child Support Grant (CSG) eligibility

The cumulative duration of CSG eligibility at HAALSI baseline, in years, for each CSG-eligible mother was the exposure of interest. This was calculated based on the birth dates (month and year) of all their children and the CSG age-eligibility expansion years. We aggregated the potential years of CSG eligibility for each child beneficiary to calculate the total duration of CSG eligibility for the mothers. Take, for instance, a woman with two children, one born in January 1994 and the other born in January 1995. Based on their birth dates and the expansions implemented between 2003 and 2012, the child born in 1994 will contribute 7.5 years of CSG eligibility, while the child born in 1995 will contribute 12.5 years of CSG eligibility by January 2016. Thus, for the mother, we would calculate a total CSG eligibility of 20 years at HAALSI baseline. An excel workbook showing our CSG eligibility calculation is included in our Supplementary Materials. Among women of the same age and with similar number of children, the variation in CSG eligibility duration plausibly leads to exogenous differences in CSG exposure over time. In contrast, the actual uptake of CSG benefits may be associated with individual risk factors or early outcomes such as education or high early life cognitive functioning that could confound the relationship between CSG and later outcomes [39]. Thus, in this paper, we focus on the “intent to treat” relationship between exogenous variation in years of CSG eligibility (adjusted for age, education, and number of children) and cognitive functioning.

For current analysis, we categorized the duration of CSG eligibility into a binary variable: ≤10 years (“low”) versus >10 years (“high”). This choice of categorisation was informed by a data driven decision making, where we observed a threshold of higher cognitive function scores for mothers who had greater than 10 years of CSG eligibility compared to those who had less. We first mapped the relationship between all CSG eligibility years (as indicator variables) and cognitive function z-scores. To better understand the trend, we then grouped the CSG eligibility years into five-year intervals to visualize the same relationship. We observed that mothers with 1 to 5 years and 5.5 to 10 years of CSG eligibility duration had similar cognitive function scores while those with greater than 10 years had relatively higher cognitive function scores (S3 Fig in S1 File). Given this observation, we decided to use 10 years of CSG eligibility as our threshold for analysis (the CSG variable construction process has been described in further details under Child Support Grant Variable Construction in our Supplementary Materials). We also conducted several sensitivity analyses using CSG as a continuous variable and with different cut-offs for duration of CSG eligibility to assess the robustness of this threshold (Please see Sensitivity Analysis, S1 to S3 Tables in S1 File).

Outcome variable: Cognitive function

A cognitive battery was administered in the HAALSI study interview, which included measures of orientation (ability to correctly state the date, month, year, and South African President; 4 points total) and episodic memory (immediate and delayed recall of ten words that were read out loud by the interviewer; 20 points total) [40]. These measures were selected for use as the inability to orient oneself in time and place and loss of episodic memory are hallmark symptoms of ADRD, and are highly sensitive to ageing-related change [41, 42]. This cognitive battery was adapted from the United States Health and Retirement Study (US HRS) and has been used in other HRS International Partner Studies around the world [43, 44] including South Africa [33, 40, 45]. The cognitive battery also had 2 numeracy measures (forward count and number skip pattern), which we did not include in the present analysis. This is because numeracy is a skill learned through education and given the limited educational opportunities that our study sample faced during Apartheid, those who are not numerate may be particularly disadvantaged for the numeracy tests [40, 41]. For present analysis, we used the continuous z-standardized score of the 24-point composite score (with mean 0 and standard deviation 1) [40].

Sociodemographic variables

The sample of CSG-eligible mothers was described according to their age at the time of the HAALSI baseline interview and CSG implementation (years), number of CSG-eligible children, lifetime number of children (one to four, and five and more), education (none, some primary, and some secondary or more), marital status (never married/ separated/divorced/widowed, and currently married), religion (none, Christianity, and African traditional), country of origin (South Africa, and Mozambique/other), and wealth index quintiles (based on household characteristics and ownership of vehicles, livestock and household items) [34].

Statistical analysis

CSG-eligible mothers were stratified into two groups according to their number of lifetime children, one to four and five and more children, since the number of lifetime children is structurally linked to higher duration of CSG eligibility. This categorization was chosen to distinguish those with grand multiparity of five and more births [46]. For descriptive analysis, we used Chi-square tests and Mann Whitney U tests for categorical and continuous variables, respectively. Ordinary Least Square regression models were used to estimate the association between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function z-score, first in the full sample, and then stratified by lifetime number of children subgroups. All models were adjusted for age of mother (as a linear and a quadratic term), education (as none, some primary, some secondary or more) and lifetime number of children indicator variables. Statistical analyses were conducted using R statistical software (version 3.6.0, Vienna, Austria). Threshold for significance was defined as p value < 0.05.

Results

Characteristics of the sample have been described in Table 1. In the full sample, the median duration of CSG eligibility for the mothers was 26.25 years (IQR: 16 to 41 years, Range: 1 to 98.5 years). These years of eligibility could be simultaneously accrued over multiple children, with the median number of CSG eligible children in the full sample being 2 (IQR: 1 to 3 children, Range: 1 to 9 children). The median age of the mothers at the time of the study interview was 50 years (IQR: 44 to 54 years, Range: 40 to 59 years) while at the time of CSG implementation in 1998, the median age was 34 years (IQR: 28 to 38 years, Range: 24 years to 43 years). Over four-fifths (86 per cent, 808/944) of the full sample had high duration of CSG eligibility (>10 years). About four-fifths (84 per cent, 328/390) of the mothers with one to four lifetime children and 87 per cent (480/554) of the mothers with five and more lifetime children had high duration of CSG eligibility. Approximately one-third of the mothers (~59 per cent, 554/944) had five or more lifetime number of children. Around one-third (~35 per cent, 329/944) of the mothers had received some secondary or higher education and over half (~55 per cent, 515/944) were currently married. About two-thirds of the mothers (69 per cent, 651/944) were originally from South Africa, and majority (95 per cent, 901/944) were Christian (Table 1). Some sociodemographic characteristics significantly differed across the mothers according to their CSG eligibility status (Table 1). Mothers with high duration of CSG eligibility were younger and had more CSG eligible children than mothers with low duration of CSG eligibility. Mothers with high duration of CSG eligibility were marginally more likely to have achieved some secondary or higher education compared to their counterparts with low duration of CSG eligibility. No meaningful differences by duration of CSG eligibility were observed for lifetime number of children, marital status, country of origin, religion, and wealth index.

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of child support grant eligible mothers according to low (≤10 years) and high (>10 years) duration of child support grant eligibility, health and ageing in Africa: A longitudinal study of an INDEPTH community in South Africa, 2014/15.

Characteristics Overall Sample N = 944 Low CSG*duration N = 136 High CSG duration N = 808 P value
Continuous variables [Median (IQR ** )]
Number of CSG eligible children 2 (1, 3) 1 (1,1) 3 (2,4) <0.001
Mother’s age in 2014/15 50 (44, 54) 54 (51, 57) 49 (43, 53) <0.001
Mother’s age in 1998 (CSG implementation) 34 (28, 38) 38 (35, 41) 33 (27, 37) <0.001
Categorical variables [N (%)]
Lifetime Number of Children
1 to 4 children 390 (41.3) 62 (45.6) 328 (40.6) 0.27
5 and more children 554 (58.7) 74 (54.4) 480 (59.4)
Education
None 275 (29.1) 47 (34.6) 228 (28.2) 0.05
Some Primary schooling 340 (36.0) 54 (39.7) 286 (35.4)
Secondary or more schooling 329 (34.9) 35 (25.7) 294 (36.4)
Marital status
Currently married 515 (54.6) 65 (47.8) 450 (55.7) 0.09
Other^ 429 (45.4) 71 (52.2) 358 (44.3)
Country of origin
South Africa 651 (69.0) 94 (69.1) 557 (68.9) 0.98
Mozambique or other 292 (31.0) 42 (30.9) 250 (30.9)
Religion
None 28 (3.0) 2 (1.5) 26 (3.2) 0.17
Christianity 901 (95.4) 134 (98.5) 767 (94.9)
African Traditional 15 (1.6) 0 15 (1.9)
Wealth Index
Quintile 1 (lowest) 180 (19.1) 19 (14.0) 161 (20.0) 0.36
Quintile 2 179 (19.0) 26 (19.1) 153 (18.9)
Quintile 3 172 (18.2) 22 (16.1) 150 (18.6)
Quintile 4 192 (20.3) 33 (24.3) 159 (19.7)
Quintile 5 (highest) 221 (23.4) 36 (26.5) 185 (22.9)

*CSG: Child Support Grant;

**IQR: Inter Quartile Range;

^ never married/separated or divorced/widowed

In the full sample, in unadjusted analysis, high duration of CSG eligibility was associated with higher cognitive function z-scores in the mothers (β = 0.27 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.11, 0.43). After adjustment for mother’s age, education and lifetime number of children, the magnitude of the observed association was attenuated (adjusted β = 0.15 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.04, 0.26; Table 2, Fig 2). In subgroup analyses, high duration of CSG eligibility was associated with higher cognitive function z-scores only among mothers with one to four children (β = 0.37 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.12, 0.62). The magnitude of this association was also attenuated after covariate adjustment (adjusted β = 0.19 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.05, 0.34; Table 2, Fig 2). In mothers with five or more children, unadjusted analysis indicated that high duration of CSG eligibility was marginally associated with higher cognitive function (β = 0.21 SD units, 95 per cent CI: 0.003, 0.41), but no meaningful associations between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function z-scores were observed in adjusted analyses (Table 2).

Table 2. Estimated effects of high child support grant eligibility (>10 years) compared to low child support grant eligibility (≤10 years) on cognitive function in child support grant-eligible mothers, health and ageing in africa: A longitudinal study of an INDEPTH community in South Africa, 2014/15.

Analytic Sample Beta Coefficient (β) 95% Confidence Intervals P value
Full Sample (n = 944)
Unadjusted 0.27 0.11, 0.43 <0.001
Adjusted a 0.15 0.04, 0.26 0.01
Lifetime number of children 1 to 4 (n = 390)
Unadjusted 0.37 0.12, 0.62 <0.01
Adjusted b 0.19 0.05, 0.34 0.02
Lifetime number of children 5 and above (n = 554)
Unadjusted 0.21 0.003, 0.41 0.05
Adjusted b 0.11 -0.07, 0.30 0.21

a Adjusted for mother’s age and age squared, education (none, some primary, some secondary and more) and lifetime number of children (indicator variables)

b Adjusted for mother’s age and age squared, education (none, some primary, some secondary and more) and with lifetime number of children (indicator variables), within each lifetime children subgroup

Fig 2. Adjusteda estimates from ordinary least squares regression models of the cognitive function in child support grant (CSG)-eligible mothers who had high versus low duration of CSG eligibility in the three analytic samples.

Fig 2

aAdjusted for mother’s age, mother’s age squared, mother’s education and lifetime number of children (indicator variables).

In our sensitivity analyses, we analysed CSG in three ways: a) as a continuous variable and dichotomized the years of CSG eligibility using two different thresholds near the 10-year cut-off, b) one at 8 years and c) another at 12 years (Supplementary Materials: Sensitivity Analysis). As a continuous variable, duration of CSG eligibility was not associated with cognitive function z-scores after adjusting for mother’s age, education, and lifetime number of children (S1 Table in S1 File). When dichotomizing duration of CSG eligibility as ≤8 years (“low”) versus >8 years (“high”), the association between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function z-scores for the full sample and subgroups were similar in magnitude and precision (β = 0.27; 95% CI: 0.14, 0.39; p value = 0.007, for mothers with 1–4 children) to the main analysis conducted with the 10-year cut-off (S2 Table in S1 File). When dichotomizing duration of CSG eligibility as ≤12 years (“low”) versus >12 years (“high”), no association was observed between duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function z scores in the full sample and in the lifetime number of children subgroups (β = 0.05; 95% CI: -0.09, 0.19; p value = 0.33, for mothers with 1–4 children) in adjusted analyses.

Discussion

In this study of mothers in a rural, low-income South African population, we found that a higher duration of eligibility for Child Support Grant was associated with higher subsequent cognitive function z-scores among mothers between ages 40–59 years. We also found that in mothers with one to four children in their lifetimes, CSG eligibility duration was associated with higher subsequent cognitive function z-scores. The magnitude of these associations was substantial. In our full sample, for every 5-year increase in age, the cognitive function z score lowered by an average of 0.18 SD units. Therefore, our study results suggest that over 10 years of cumulative eligibility for regular government cash transfers for child support in this group of mothers in their mid-life was equivalent to a nearly 5-year reduction in their cognitive ageing.

Only three other studies that we are aware of have used natural experiments to evaluate the causal effects of cash transfers on cognitive function in older adults [3133]. In Mexico, cash transfers given to adults aged ≥70 years improved immediate and delayed recall scores with magnitudes that were equivalent to reductions in cognitive ageing of 5.5 and 12.4 years, respectively [31]. In the United States, cash transfers from social security income were associated with clinically meaningful improvements in multiple domains of cognitive function in older adults [32]. In South Africa, men who were exposed to expanded pension eligibility had higher cognitive function [33]. While our sample comprised women from a low-income, rural South African setting, our findings are consistent with the results from Mexico, United States and South Africa. We considered cognitive function across an earlier age range than had been examined in prior work by focusing on South African women in their mid-life. Cognitive decline can start during the middle years of adulthood and progressively worsen in later life [47, 48], potentially leading to ADRD [13]. Early detection of cognitive function changes in mid-life may help support interventions to delay or prevent ADRD onset.

A potential mechanism through which cash transfers may exert protective effects on mid- life cognitive function is via promoting cognitive reserve [17]. Accrual of cognitive reserve is thought to occur over the life course through cumulative cognitively stimulating experiences from childhood through adulthood [17, 49, 50]. People with high cognitive reserve are thought to be able to use compensatory behavioral or neural brain mechanisms to stave off the functional impacts of ageing-related brain pathology [17, 49]. In South Africa, CSG benefits account for nearly 40 per cent of income in the poorest recipient households and are accumulated over time in early- to mid-adulthood for mothers while they are raising children [21, 23]. Increased income, especially when sustained over time, can alleviate poverty, improve living conditions, reduce stress, and improve nutrition and health [22, 23], all of which may build cognitive reserve [51]. Prior research has shown that since the mothers were primary CSG recipients, they had a strong role in making financial decisions regarding the use of CSG benefits [23, 52]. It is possible that mother’s economic empowerment within the household may be protective of their cognitive health. Furthermore, the CSG has been found to strengthen social support for the mothers via improved relationships with neighbors in the community and enabling them to participate in stokvels, which is a rotating savings and credit association [30]. High social support and social engagement is protective against cognitive ageing, particularly in adults in their mid-life [53, 54]. This may be due to them engaging in complex relationships and a feeling a sense of belonging and dignity in the community [55]. Another potential pathway to accrue cognitive reserve might have been expanding access to education in adolescent mothers who might still be in school at the time of CSG implementation. However, in our study sample, the youngest mothers were at least 24 years old in 1998, when CSG was first implemented. Therefore, their opportunities for expanding education might have closed by the time their children were CSG eligible. So, higher education levels might not plausibly explain the observed association between CSG and cognitive function in this group of mothers. Future research is needed to better understand the mechanisms through which improving the financial circumstances and independence of low-income middle-aged mothers may improve their cognitive health as they age.

We did not observe an association between higher duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function in mothers with grand multiparity (with five or more children). Prior studies have found grand multiparity to be strongly associated with cognitive impairment and ADRD risk, which may overwhelm any protective effects of CSG eligibility for these women [56, 57]. Grand multiparity may result in decreased estrogen levels [58], long-term parenting stress [59], and increased risk of cardiovascular diseases [60], all of which can contribute to cognitive decline [59]. Our sample of rural South African women with a median of five lifetime number of children represented a relatively high fertility setting. There is scarce research on the association between parity and cognitive ageing in this setting and there may be unknown sociodemographic, socioeconomic, or psychosocial factors that mediate or moderate this relationship. Prior work using data from Agincourt HDSS found that the receipt of CSG had not motivated women to have more children [61]. Future studies are planned to investigate the relationships between reproductive history and cognitive ageing outcomes in the HAALSI cohort.

The findings from our sensitivity analysis suggest the use of the 10-year threshold may be adequate. As a continuous variable, duration of CSG eligibility was not associated with cognitive function. In South Africa, the inflation-adjusted food poverty line in April 2021 was 624 Rand (~33 USD) per person per month, while the CSG amount in April 2021 was only 480 Rand (~ 25 dollars) per child per month [62]. Therefore, the CSG benefit was of a magnitude smaller than that needed to cover the cost of the child’s minimum required daily energy intake. However, CSG income is predominantly used to buy food for the child and the household [52], and sometimes is the only source of income in the households [63]. Therefore, it is plausible that a difference of one year of CSG eligibility is likely too little in duration and financial amounts to have had any meaningful impacts on the cognitive function of the mothers. We also tested the sensitivity of our threshold cut point at 10 years of CSG eligibility. When using the 8-year cut-off, our results were similar to the main analysis, suggesting that higher duration of CSG eligibility may be protective of the cognitive health of mothers with fewer children but not for those with grand multiparity. However, when we used the 12-year cut-off, we observed no association between CSG eligibility and cognitive function for mothers with 1–4 children, suggesting that the cut-off greater than 10 may be too high to detect any relationships between CSG eligibility and cognitive function. It is also possible that for mothers with 1–4 children, to be eligible for longer durations of CSG means having multiple children within short birth spacing intervals, which may neutralize any effect of CSG on mother’s cognitive function.

Limitations and strengths

Several aspects of the experimental design and data structure warrant careful interpretation of the results. We utilized duration of CSG eligibility and not actual CSG receipt in our analyses. The true relationship between CSG receipt and cognitive function may be different from our findings. However, given that not all mothers with CSG-eligible children applied for the grant [22], our exposed group included eligible mothers who did not receive the full CSG income we attributed to her. Therefore, our study findings plausibly yield more conservative estimates than we would expect if we used the actual amount of CSG received as our exposure. Furthermore, this method of exposure calculation has been previously used in South Africa to assess the relationship between years of Old Age Pension eligibility and health outcomes in older adults [64]. Cognitive function was measured at a single point in time, and we could not assess within-person cognitive change over time or incidence of cognitive impairment or ADRD. As the current cohort ages, studies using extended follow-up in HAALSI have been planned which will allow us to track cognitive change during ageing among the middle-aged women in the current sample. Cognitive function was assessed using measures for orientation and episodic memory, which does not include all possible domains of cognitive function. To address this issue, we ran a sensitivity analysis using cognitive function outcome that included numeracy measures in addition to orientation, and episodic memory measures (S4 Table in S1 File). Our results were similar, with higher CSG eligibility duration associated with higher cognitive function scores in mothers with 1–4 children. Future studies should investigate other domains, such as executive function, language, or processing speed to gain a better understanding of whether and how cash transfers affect different domains of cognitive function. Longitudinal studies with large sample sizes and actual CSG receipt data are necessary to elucidate the relationship between cash transfers and cognitive decline in South African women in their later-life. It is critical to know how long any cognitive health benefits of the CSG received in early- to mid-adulthood may last as beneficiaries age. It is also possible that pre-HAALSI baseline mortality patterns in women may have biased our findings, as aging cohorts are vulnerable to selective survival bias [65]. However, women who died prior to enrollment and women who were enrolled in HAALSI had very similar household asset indices (2.2 vs 2.3, respectively, from unpublished analyses using underlying vital statistics data collected by the Agincourt HDSS [20], covering the source population of the HAALSI cohort). Therefore, we believe that differences in household wealth between those enrolled in HAALSI and those not enrolled may not be large in magnitude to severely bias our results. That said, our findings should be interpreted as conditioned on survival until age 40 years and older in rural South Africa. Finally, our study findings may have limited generalizability outside of a rural South African setting.

This study has several strengths. We used data from a population-representative sample in a setting that is under-represented in the global dementia evidence base [34]. We used a quasi-randomized exposure variable for duration of CSG eligibility based on the CSG age-eligibility expansions and children’s birth timings. This approach has also been used in previous research [28, 66]. This study data, which reports CSG eligibility duration since its inception until January 2016, extends prior work on CSG receipt that have used self-reported data over a shorter follow-up period and/or used self-reported data to identify thresholds of CSG receipt duration [67, 68]. Since the increases in CSG amount over time was to match inflation and not to increase in value, the duration of CSG eligibility was considered an adequate proxy for CSG receipt. This approach allowed us to avoid recall or social desirability bias that can occur when self-reports are used or confounding by individual uptake patterns. This design strengthens causal inference for the association between CSG eligibility and mid- life cognitive function. We used validated and reliable measures of cognitive function, which are harmonized with those used in the US HRS and its International Partner Studies around the world and are meaningful for ADRD risk [43]. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study examining the causal effects of cash transfer eligibility on mid- life cognitive function of mothers in a rural South African setting. Our findings bring evidence to policymakers for designing social protection plans that promote healthy cognitive ageing in mothers from rural, low-income settings.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that eligibility for cash transfers to support young children may protect the cognitive health of maternal recipients. This finding is critical since it indicates cash transfers can make promising interventions to protect cognitive health; and that CSG confers long-term cognitive health benefits to mothers in addition to children and households at the time of transfer. Future research should investigate how parity affects this relationship, how this relationship may vary by the actual financial value of cumulative cash transfer receipt, and whether the protective effects of cash transfers like the CSG extend to later-life cognitive outcomes as beneficiaries age. Better understanding of the relationship between cash transfers and cognitive ageing is crucial, as this evidence would allow for policy recommendations, change in cost-benefit arguments and development of income supplementation programmes to promote healthy cognitive ageing in low-income settings.

Supporting information

S1 File. Child support grant variable construction and sensitivity analyses.

(DOCX)

pone.0297673.s001.docx (152.1KB, docx)
S2 File. Mapping child support grant exposure.

(XLS)

pone.0297673.s002.xls (42KB, xls)

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Audrey Pettifor and Erica Beidelman for their valuable contributions towards this manuscript.

Data Availability

The HAALSI data used in this article are publicly available in the Harvard Dataverse. Data from the “HAALSI Baseline Survey” and the “HAALSI Dementia Wave 1” can be found here https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataverse/harvard?q=HAALSI.

Funding Statement

This current work was supported by U.S. National Institute on Aging (grant number 1R01AG069128) to MR and LCK; the HAALSI study was supported by the U.S. National Institute on Aging (grant number P01 AG041710) to LB. HAALSI is nested within the Agincourt Health and Demographic Surveillance System site, which is supported by the University of the Witwatersrand and Medical Research Council, South Africa, and the Wellcome Trust, UK (grant numbers 058893/Z/99/A; 069683/Z/02/Z; 085477/Z/08/Z; 085477/B/08/Z) to KK. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Nichols E, Steinmetz JD, Vollset SE, Fukutaki K, Chalek J, Abd-Allah F, et al. Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. The Lancet Public Health. 2022;7(2):e105–e25. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(21)00249-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Long S, Benoist C, Weidner W. World Alzheimer Report 2023: Reducing dementia risk: never too early, never too late. London, England: Alzheimer’s Disease International., 2023. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Qiu C, Kivipelto M, von Strauss E. Epidemiology of Alzheimer’s disease: occurrence, determinants, and strategies toward intervention. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11(2):111–28. Epub 2009/07/10. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2009.11.2/cqiu . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Guerchet M, Prince M, Prina M. Numbers of people with dementia worldwide: An update to the estimates in the World Alzheimer Report 2015. 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Guerchet MM, Mayston R, Lloyd-Sherlock P, Prince MJ, Aboderin I, Akinyemi R, et al. Dementia in sub-Saharan Africa: challenges and opportunities. London: Alzheimer’s Disease International; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mavrodaris A, Powell J, Thorogood M. Prevalences of dementia and cognitive impairment among older people in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review. Bulletin of the World Health Organization. 2013;91:773–83. doi: 10.2471/BLT.13.118422 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Akinyemi RO, Yaria J, Ojagbemi A, Guerchet M, Okubadejo N, Njamnshi AK, et al. Dementia in Africa: Current evidence, knowledge gaps, and future directions. Alzheimers Dement. 2021. Epub 2021/09/28. doi: 10.1002/alz.12432 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Whalley LJ, Dick FD, McNeill G. A life-course approach to the aetiology of late-onset dementias. The Lancet Neurology. 2006;5(1):87–96. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70286-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Glymour MM, Manly JJ. Lifecourse social conditions and racial and ethnic patterns of cognitive aging. Neuropsychology review. 2008;18(3):223–54. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9064-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dekhtyar S, Wang HX, Scott K, Goodman A, Koupil I, Herlitz A. A Life-Course Study of Cognitive Reserve in Dementia—From Childhood to Old Age. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;23(9):885–96. Epub 2015/03/10. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2015.02.002 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Scazufca M, Menezes PR, Araya R, Di Rienzo VD, Almeida OP, Gunnell D, et al. Risk factors across the life course and dementia in a Brazilian population: results from the Sao Paulo Ageing & Health Study (SPAH). Int J Epidemiol. 2008;37(4):879–90. Epub 2008/06/28. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyn125 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ma J, Yang Y, Wan Y, Shen C, Qiu P. The influence of childhood adversities on mid to late cognitive function: From the perspective of life course. PLoS One. 2021;16(8):e0256297. Epub 2021/08/17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0256297 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wilson RS, Leurgans SE, Boyle PA, Bennett DA. Cognitive decline in prodromal Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Archives of neurology. 2011;68(3):351–6. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Turrell G, Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Everson SA, Helkala E-L, Kauhanen J, et al. Socioeconomic position across the lifecourse and cognitive function in late middle age. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2002;57(1):S43–S51. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.1.s43 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. The Lancet Neurology. 2012;11(11):1006–12. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wilson RS, Scherr PA, Hoganson G, Bienias JL, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Early life socioeconomic status and late life risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25(1):8–14. doi: 10.1159/000085307 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the international neuropsychological society. 2002;8(3):448–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jefferson AL, Gibbons LE, Rentz DM, Carvalho JO, Manly J, Bennett DA, et al. A life course model of cognitive activities, socioeconomic status, education, reading ability, and cognition. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2011;59(8):1403–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2011.03499.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.de Jager CA, Joska JA, Hoffman M, Borochowitz KE, Combrinck MI. Dementia in rural South Africa: A pressing need for epidemiological studies. S Afr Med J. 2015;105(3):189–90. Epub 2015/08/22. doi: 10.7196/samj.8904 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kahn K, Collinson MA, Gómez-Olivé FX, Mokoena O, Twine R, Mee P, et al. Profile: Agincourt health and socio-demographic surveillance system. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41(4):988–1001. Epub 2012/08/31. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys115 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McEwen H, Kannemeyer C, Woolard I. Social assistance grants: analysis of the NIDS Wave 1 dataset. 2009.
  • 22.DSD, SASSA, UNICEF. The South African Child Support Grant Impact Assessment: Evidence from a survey of children, adolescents and their households. Pretoria: UNICEF South Africa. 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Delany A, Ismail Z, Graham L, Ramkissoon Y. Review of the child support grant: Uses, implementation and obstacles. Johannesburg: Community Agency for Social Enquiry. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Case A, Hosegood V, Lund F. The reach and impact of Child Support Grants: evidence from KwaZulu-Natal. Development Southern Africa. 2005;22(4):467–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Twine R, Collinson MA, Polzer TJ, Kahn K. Evaluating access to a child-oriented poverty alleviation intervention in rural South Africa. Scand J Public Health Suppl. 2007;69:118–27. Epub 2007/09/06. doi: 10.1080/14034950701430008 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Handa S, Otchere F, Sirma P, Team TES. More Evidence on the Impact of Government Social Protection in Sub Saharan Africa: Ghana, Malawi and Zimbabwe. Development Policy Review. 2021;n/a(n/a). doi: 10.1111/dpr.12576 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Eyal K, Woolard I. Throwing the Book at the CSG, South African Labour and Development Research Unit. Working Paper series 53, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ohrnberger J, Fichera E, Sutton M, Anselmi L. The effect of cash transfers on mental health–new evidence from South Africa. BMC public health. 2020;20(1):1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Patel L., Poverty Gender and Social Protection: Child Support Grants in Soweto, South Africa. Journal of Policy Practice. 2012;11(1–2):106–20. doi: 10.1080/15588742.2012.625344 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Granlund S, Hochfeld T. ‘That Child Support Grant Gives Me Powers’–Exploring Social and Relational Aspects of Cash Transfers in South Africa in Times of Livelihood Change. The Journal of Development Studies. 2020;56(6):1230–44. doi: 10.1080/00220388.2019.1650170 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Aguila E, Kapteyn A, Smith JP. Effects of income supplementation on health of the poor elderly: The case of Mexico. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(1):70–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414453112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ayyagari P, Frisvold D. The impact of social security income on cognitive function at older ages. American Journal of Health Economics. 2016;2(4):463–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Jock J, Kobayashi L, Chakraborty R, Chen X, Wing C, Berkman L, et al. Effects of Pension Eligibility Expansion on Men’s Cognitive Function: Findings from Rural South Africa. J Aging Soc Policy. 2023:1–20. Epub 20230328. doi: 10.1080/08959420.2023.2195785 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gómez-Olivé FX, Montana L, Wagner RG, Kabudula CW, Rohr JK, Kahn K, et al. Cohort Profile: Health and Ageing in Africa: A Longitudinal Study of an INDEPTH Community in South Africa (HAALSI). Int J Epidemiol. 2018;47(3):689–90j. Epub 2018/01/13. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyx247 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Gómez-Olivé FX, Angotti N, Houle B, Klipstein-Grobusch K, Kabudula C, Menken J, et al. Prevalence of HIV among those 15 and older in rural South Africa. AIDS Care. 2013;25(9):1122–8. Epub 2013/01/15. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2012.750710 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hall K. Child Support Grants South Africa2021 [cited 2021 December 5th]. http://childrencount.uct.ac.za/indicator.php?domain=2&indicator=10.
  • 37.South African Social Security Agency. Child Support Grant South Africa2023 [cited 2023]. https://www.gov.za/services/child-care-social-benefits/child-support-gran.
  • 38.Department of Health/South Africa, Macro International. South Africa Demographic and Health Survey 1998. Pretoria, South Africa: Department of Health/South Africa, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Zembe-Mkabile W, Doherty T, Sanders D, Jackson D, Chopra M, Swanevelder S, et al. Why do families still not receive the child support grant in South Africa? A longitudinal analysis of a cohort of families across South Africa. BMC international health and human rights. 2012;12:24-. doi: 10.1186/1472-698X-12-24 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kobayashi LC, Mateen FJ, Montana L, Wagner RG, Kahn K, Tollman SM, et al. Cognitive function and impairment in older, rural South African adults: evidence from “health and aging in Africa: a longitudinal study of an INDEPTH community in rural South Africa”. Neuroepidemiology. 2019;52(1–2):32–40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bäckman L, Small BJ, Fratiglioni L. Stability of the preclinical episodic memory deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2001;124(1):96–102. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.1.96 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiology of Aging. 2009;30(4):507–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kobayashi LC, Gross AL, Gibbons LE, Tommet D, Sanders RE, Choi S-E, et al. You say tomato, I say radish: can brief cognitive assessments in the US Health Retirement Study be harmonized with its International Partner Studies? The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wu Q, Tchetgen Tchetgen EJ, Osypuk TL, White K, Mujahid M, Maria Glymour M. Combining direct and proxy assessments to reduce attrition bias in a longitudinal study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2013;27(3):207–12. Epub 2012/09/21. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31826cfe90 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kobayashi LC, Glymour MM, Kahn K, Payne CF, Wagner RG, Montana L, et al. Childhood deprivation and later-life cognitive function in a population-based study of older rural South Africans. Soc Sci Med. 2017;190:20–8. Epub 20170814. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.08.009 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mgaya AH, Massawe SN, Kidanto HL, Mgaya HN. Grand multiparity: is it still a risk in pregnancy? BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth. 2013;13(1):241. doi: 10.1186/1471-2393-13-241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hughes ML, Agrigoroaei S, Jeon M, Bruzzese M, Lachman ME. Change in cognitive performance from midlife into old age: Findings from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2018;24(8):805–20. doi: 10.1017/S1355617718000425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ferreira D, Correia R, Nieto A, Machado A, Molina Y, Barroso J. Cognitive decline before the age of 50 can be detected with sensitive cognitive measures. Psicothema. 2015;27(3):216–22. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2014.192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Whalley LJ, Deary IJ, Appleton CL, Starr JM. Cognitive reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing research reviews. 2004;3(4):369–82. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2004.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Berkman LF. The association between educational attainment and mental status examinations: of etiologic significance for senile dementias or not? J Chronic Dis. 1986;39(3):171–5. Epub 1986/01/01. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(86)90020-2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Richards M, Deary IJ. A life course approach to cognitive reserve: a model for cognitive aging and development? Annals of Neurology: Official Journal of the American Neurological Association and the Child Neurology Society. 2005;58(4):617–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.DSD, SASSA, UNICEF. Child Support Grant Evaluation 2010: Qualitative Research Report. Pretoria: UNICEF South Africa. 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kats D, Patel MD, Palta P, Meyer ML, Gross AL, Whitsel EA, et al. Social support and cognition in a community-based cohort: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Age Ageing. 2016;45(4):475–80. Epub 20160421. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afw060 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Harling G, Kobayashi LC, Farrell MT, Wagner RG, Tollman S, Berkman L. Social contact, social support, and cognitive health in a population-based study of middle-aged and older men and women in rural South Africa. Soc Sci Med. 2020;260:113167. Epub 20200706. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113167 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Berkman LF. Which influences cognitive function: living alone or being alone? The Lancet. 2000;355(9212):1291–2. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02107-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Bae JB, Lipnicki DM, Han JW, Sachdev PS, Kim TH, Kwak KP, et al. Does parity matter in women’s risk of dementia? A COSMIC collaboration cohort study. BMC Med. 2020;18(1):210. Epub 2020/08/06. doi: 10.1186/s12916-020-01671-1 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Bae JB, Lipnicki DM, Han JW, Sachdev PS, Kim TH, Kwak KP, et al. Parity and the risk of incident dementia: a COSMIC study. Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2020;29:e176. Epub 2020/10/21. doi: 10.1017/S2045796020000876 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Li FD, He F, Chen TR, Xiao YY, Lin ST, Shen W, et al. Reproductive history and risk of cognitive impairment in elderly women: a cross-sectional study in eastern China. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;49(1):139–47. Epub 2015/10/08. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150444 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Saenz JL, Díaz-Venegas C, Crimmins EM. Fertility history and cognitive function in late life: The case of Mexico. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2021;76(4):e140–e52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Parikh NI, Cnattingius S, Dickman PW, Mittleman MA, Ludvigsson JF, Ingelsson E. Parity and risk of later-life maternal cardiovascular disease. Am Heart J. 2010;159(2):215–21.e6. Epub 2010/02/16. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2009.11.017 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Rosenberg M, Pettifor A, Nguyen N, Westreich D, Bor J, Bärnighausen T, et al. Relationship between Receipt of a Social Protection Grant for a Child and Second Pregnancy Rates among South African Women: A Cohort Study. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0137352. Epub 2015/09/24. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137352 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Statistics South Africa. National Poverty Lines 2021. Pretoria, South Africa: 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Luthuli S, Haskins L, Mapumulo S, Horwood C. Does the unconditional cash transfer program in South Africa provide support for women after child birth? Barriers to accessing the child support grant among women in informal work in Durban, South Africa. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):112. Epub 20220116. doi: 10.1186/s12889-022-12503-7 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Riumallo Herl C, Kabudula C, Kahn K, Tollman S, Canning D. Pension exposure and health: Evidence from a longitudinal study in South Africa. The Journal of the Economics of Ageing. 2022;23:100411. doi: 10.1016/j.jeoa.2022.100411 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Weuve J, Proust-Lima C, Power MC, Gross AL, Hofer SM, Thiébaut R, et al. Guidelines for reporting methodological challenges and evaluating potential bias in dementia research. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(9):1098–109. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.06.1885 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Garman EC, Eyal K, Avendano M, Evans-Lacko S, Lund C. Cash transfers and the mental health of young people: Evidence from South Africa’s child support grant. Soc Sci Med. 2022;292:114631. Epub 20211204. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114631 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Cluver L, Boyes M, Orkin M, Pantelic M, Molwena T, Sherr L. Child-focused state cash transfers and adolescent risk of HIV infection in South Africa: a propensity-score-matched case-control study. The Lancet Global Health. 2013;1(6):e362–e70. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(13)70115-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Zembe-Mkabile W, Ramokolo V, Sanders D, Jackson D, Doherty T. The dynamic relationship between cash transfers and child health: can the child support grant in South Africa make a difference to child nutrition? Public Health Nutr. 2016;19(2):356–62. Epub 20150608. doi: 10.1017/S1368980015001147 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Azizur Rahman

27 Sep 2023

PONE-D-23-01857Child Support Grant expansion and cognitive function among women in rural South Africa: findings from a natural experiment in HAALSI cohortPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Chakraborty,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The authors should carefully read all comments and thoroughly revised the manuscript for further consideration.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 10 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Azizur Rahman, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Additional Editor Comments:

The guidelines and style of PLOS ONE must be followed.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This study aimed to exploit the exogenous variation in CSG eligibility, due to iterative age eligibility expansion policies, as a natural experiment to estimate the causal effect of cash transfers on cognitive function among biological mothers in South Africa. Good job! However, I have the following observations:

1. So far, I know PloS ONE has accepted structured abstracts. So, the authors need to revise the abstract.

2. In the Introduction section, it is suggested to update the statistic “In 2018, nearly 50 million people...”.

3. Add the strengths and limitations of the study as a separate section.

Reviewer #2: In this paper the authors use longitudinal data from the HAALSI cohort in Agincourt, South Africa to investigate the effect of expanded Child Support Grants on cognitive function. Ordinary least squares regression is used to model the relationship between cognitive function and duration of CSG eligibility, adjusted for a number of predictors including mother’s age, education and lifetime number of children.

In terms of the statistical analysis, the authors have tried several categorisations of the duration of CSG eligibility in order to find one that fits best. This suggests that the relationship is not strong to begin with and therefore the results in the paper are of waning interest. Also the fact that the best fitting model has to disregard the quantitative nature of duration so thoroughly means that the support for the hypothesis becomes weaker in practical terms.

I am concerned that the data are not strong enough to support the weighty hypothesis, which is that paying mothers more when their children are young can improve the cognitive function of the mothers later in life.

On the one hand, the proxy measure for cognitive function, a couple of simple short-term memory tests, seems likely to be weakly related to a fuller measure of cognitive function.

On the other hand, the difficulties in even measuring the amount of expanded CSG received by each mother seem likely to have a weak relationship with the actual amount of economic benefit accrued by each mother.

Also, it seems like an attribute such as wealth will be confounded with access to health services and therefore longevity, which could mean fewer women of lower socio-economic status are surviving long enough to exhibit symptoms of cognitive decline.

For these reasons, I am recommending rejection of the paper in its current form as its hypothesis is too stretched to be supported by the data available.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Md. Moyazzem Hossain

Reviewer #2: Yes: Alice Richardson

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Mar 6;19(3):e0297673. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0297673.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


5 Nov 2023

I have listed the changes made including the page number and line numbers [from the track changed document]

Reviewer 1

1: This study aimed to exploit the exogenous variation in CSG eligibility, due to iterative age eligibility expansion policies, as a natural experiment to estimate the causal effect of cash transfers on cognitive function among biological mothers in South Africa. Good job! However, I have the following observations.

Response: We thank the reviewer for their comments and suggestions. We have addressed them as described below.

2: So far, I know PloS ONE has accepted structured abstracts. So, the authors need to revise the abstract.

Response: We thank the reviewer for this observation. We have now structured the abstract under Background, Objectives, Methods, Results, and Conclusion sections (pages 2-3).

3.In the Introduction section, it is suggested to update the statistic “In 2018, nearly 50 million people…”.

Response: We have updated the statistic to a more recent one available for 2019 with projections for 2030, 2040, and 2050 (page 3, line 66-67): “In 2019, about 57 million people worldwide were living with dementia and this number is projected to increase to 83 million in 2030, 116 million in 2040 and 153 million by 2050 (Long et al., 2023; Nichols et al., 2022).”

4. Add the strengths and limitations of the study as a separate section.

Response: We have now added a separate section under Discussion titled “Limitations and Strengths” below which we discuss study limitations and strengths (page 20, line 743).

Reviewer 2:

1. In this paper the authors use longitudinal data from the HAALSI cohort in Agincourt, South Africa to investigate the effect of expanded Child Support Grants on cognitive function. Ordinary least squares regression is used to model the relationship between cognitive function and duration of CSG eligibility, adjusted for a number of predictors including mother’s age, education and lifetime number of children. In terms of the statistical analysis, the authors have tried several categorisations of the duration of CSG eligibility in order to find one that fits best. This suggests that the relationship is not strong to begin with and therefore the results in the paper are of waning interest. Also, the fact that the best fitting model has to disregard the quantitative nature of duration so thoroughly means that the support for the hypothesis becomes weaker in practical terms.

Response: We thank the reviewer for this observation. We explored a threshold for CSG eligibility exposure in addition to operationalizing it continuously because if the threshold model more closely aligns with the true relationship then the continuous model would have obscured it and led to incorrect inference. In the continuous model, a 1-year increase in CSG, both in eligibility duration and in actual amount, may be too small to result in any meaningful cognitive benefit in the mothers.

To identify whether a CSG eligibility threshold existed for cognitive function, we first visually graphed cognitive scores vs continuous CSG eligibility years, as shown in the supplementary materials. From this graph, we observed that mothers with 1- 10 years of CSG eligibility had similar level of cognitive function, but mothers with > 10 years of CSG eligibility showed noticeably higher cognitive function scores (S3 Fig). Thus, we selected 10 years as our threshold. However, to further test the robustness of this initial threshold we conducted two sensitivity analyses using alternate cut-offs: one at 8 years and the other at 12 years. We found that at the 8-year threshold, there was a stronger protective effect on cognitive function (β = 0.27; 95% CI: 0.14, 0.39; p value = 0.007) compared to the 10-year cut-off used in main analysis (β = 0.19; 95% CI: 0.05, 0.34; p value = 0.02) for mothers with 1-4 kids. However, at 12-year cut-off, we found no association between CSG eligibility and cognitive function for mothers with 1-4 kids (β = 0.05; 95% CI: -0.09, 0.19; p value = 0.33). Thus, our findings seem to suggest that there exists a threshold for the relationship between cumulative CSG eligibility duration and cognitive function, particularly for mothers with 1-4 kids. However, beyond this threshold, that relationship weakens. It is possible that cut-off beyond 10 years is too high to detect differences. It is also possible that mothers with 1 - 4 children with > 10 years of CSG eligibility had smaller birth spacing between children and the cognitively taxing process of child rearing diluted the effect of CSG eligibility and cognitive function. There appears to be complex interplay between CSG, parity, and cognitive function which we are exploring in future studies.

To contextualize this cash benefit, in South Africa, the inflation-adjusted food poverty line in April 2021 was 624 Rand (~33 USD) per person per month, however the CSG benefit amount in April 2021 was only 480 Rand (~25 USD) per child per month. Therefore, the CSG amount was of a magnitude smaller than that needed to cover the cost of the child’s minimum required daily energy intake. However, we know that CSG was predominantly used to buy food for the child and the household (DSD et al., 2011), and often was the only source of income in the households (Luthuli et al., 2022). Therefore, while the CSG helps households alleviate their acute food insecurity, we hypothesize that it will likely take a longer duration of cumulative eligibility to help build cognitive reserve that can protect cognitive function. Establishing a threshold for assessing impact of CSG duration on child outcomes have been used in prior studies (Zembe-Mkabile et al., 2016). For these reasons, we feel confident that a CSG threshold was reasonable to expect for maternal cognitive outcomes.

We have clarified this point in our Discussion section (page 19, line 714- 741) “As a continuous variable, duration of CSG eligibility was not associated with cognitive function. In South Africa, the inflation-adjusted food poverty line in April 2021 was 624 Rand (~33 USD) per person per month, while the CSG amount in April 2021 was only 480 Rand (~ 25 dollars) per child per month (Statistics South Africa, 2022). Therefore, the CSG benefit was of a magnitude smaller than that needed to cover the cost of the child’s minimum required daily energy intake. However, CSG income is predominantly used to buy food for the child and the household (DSD et al., 2011), and sometimes is the only source of income in the households (Luthuli et al., 2022). Therefore, it is plausible that a difference of one year of CSG eligibility is likely too little in duration and financial amounts to have had any meaningful impacts on the cognitive function of the mothers. We also tested the sensitivity of our threshold cut point at 10 years of CSG eligibility. When using the 8-year cut-off, our results were similar to those in the main analysis, suggesting that higher duration of CSG eligibility may be protective of the cognitive health of mothers with fewer children but not for those with grand multiparity. However, when we used the 12-year cut-off, we observed no association between CSG eligibility and cognitive function for mothers with 1-4 kids, suggesting that the cut-off greater than 10 may be too high to detect any relationships between CSG eligibility and cognitive function. It is also possible that for mothers with 1 -4 children, to be eligible for longer durations of CSG means having multiple children within short birth spacing intervals, which may neutralize any effect of CSG on mother’s cognitive function.”

2. On the one hand, the proxy measure for cognitive function, a couple of simple short-term memory tests, seems likely to be weakly related to a fuller measure of cognitive function.

Response: We agree with the reviewer that mental status (orientation measures) and memory status (immediate and delayed recall measures) used to create the cognitive function outcome variable do not capture the full cognitive function of an individual. To address this limitation, we did an additional sensitivity analysis using a different cognitive function outcome variable which includes a numeracy measure (forward count and number skip pattern) in addition to orientation, immediate, and delayed recall measures. It yielded similar results, with higher CSG eligibility duration conferring a protective effect on cognitive function in mothers with 1- 4 children (β = 0.17; 95% CI: 0.02, 0.30; p value = 0.03).

Despite the limitations in the cognitive function measure used in the study, this cognitive battery has been derived from the United States Health and Retirement Study (HRS) screening test for dementia and has been validated and harmonized with other cognitive measures used in US HRS and its International Partner Studies which includes HAALSI (Kobayashi et al., 2020). Furthermore, episodic memory deficits are hallmark symptoms of ADRD, are sensitive to aging-related changes, and can be detected in preclinical ADRD stages (Bäckman et al., 2001; Salthouse, 2009). This cognitive variable has also been used in previous studies (Jock et al., 2023; Kobayashi et al., 2017; Kobayashi et al., 2019).

However, like some prior studies (Kobayashi et al., 2019), we refrain from using the numeracy measure in the main analysis because it is a skill learned through education. Given the limited educational opportunities that our study sample faced during Apartheid, those who are not numerate may be particularly disadvantaged for the numeracy tests that are not reflective of overall cognitive function or decline.

We have further clarified this limitation in our Limitation and Strengths section (page 21, line 774 - 781): “Cognitive function was assessed using measures for orientation and episodic memory, which does not include all possible domains of cognitive function. To address this issue, we ran a sensitivity analysis using cognitive function outcome that included numeracy measures in addition to orientation, and episodic memory measures (Supplementary Materials, S4 Table). Our results were similar, with higher CSG eligibility duration associated with higher cognitive function scores in mothers with 1- 4 children. Future studies should investigate other domains, such as executive function, language, or processing speed to gain a better understanding of whether and how cash transfers affect different domains of cognitive function.”

We have also clarified the use of numeracy measures in our Methods section (page 11, line 463- 472): “These measures were selected for use as the inability to orient oneself in time and place and loss of episodic memory are hallmark symptoms of ADRD, and are highly sensitive to ageing-related change (Bäckman et al., 2001; Salthouse, 2009). This cognitive battery was adapted from the United States Health and Retirement Study (US HRS) and has been used in other HRS International Partner Studies around the world (Kobayashi et al., 2020; Wu et al., 2013) including South Africa (Jock et al., 2023; Kobayashi et al., 2017; Kobayashi et al., 2019).The cognitive battery also had 2 numeracy measures (forward count and number skip pattern), which we did not include in the present analysis. This is because numeracy is a skill learned through education and given the limited educational opportunities that our study sample faced during Apartheid, those who are not numerate may be particularly disadvantaged for the numeracy tests (Kobayashi et al., 2019).”

3. On the other hand, the difficulties in even measuring the amount of expanded CSG received by each mother seem likely to have a weak relationship with the actual amount of economic benefit accrued by each mother.

Response: It is likely that the relationship between the duration of CSG eligibility and cognitive function in our study may be more conservative compared to the relationship between actual CSG receipt and cognitive function. A recent study showed that about 83% of all CSG -eligible children received the grant. Although there is likely relatively high concordance between eligibility and uptake, the discordance could be explained by barriers in accessing CSG and/or misinformation about the CSG eligibility. Thus, our CSG-eligibility exposure may include mothers who were eligible but did not receive the CSG. Therefore, our study estimates are likely more conservative than if we used the actual amount of CSG received as our exposure. It is likely that we would observe a stronger relationship between actual amount of CSG and cognitive function.

Furthermore, women of similar age and similar number of children may have varying CSG eligibility durations (based on childbirth timing) due to CSG age eligibility expansions. Thus, in this study we leverage the exogenous variation in CSG eligibility over time. In contrast, the actual uptake of CSG benefits may be associated with individual risk factors or early outcomes such as education or high early life cognitive functioning that could confound the relationship between CSG and later outcomes. Therefore, we focus on the “intent to treat” relationship between exogenous variation in years of CSG eligibility and cognitive functioning, which yields policy relevant findings. Moreover, this method of calculating years of cash transfer eligibility was used in a recent study to assess the relationship between Old Age Pension and health outcomes in South Africa (Riumallo Herl et al., 2022).

We have clarified this point in our Limitations and Strengths section (page 20, line 747 - 754): “We utilized duration of CSG eligibility and not actual CSG amount in our analyses. Therefore, the true relationship between CSG amount and cognitive function may be different from our findings. However, given that not all mothers with CSG-eligible children applied for the grant (DSD et al., 2012), our exposed group included eligible mothers who did not receive the full CSG income we attributed to her. Therefore, our study findings plausibly yield more conservative estimates than we would expect if we used the actual amount of CSG received as our exposure. Furthermore, this method of exposure calculation has been previously used in South Africa to assess the relationship between years of Old Age Pension eligibility and health outcomes in older adults (Riumallo Herl et al., 2022).”

4. I am concerned that the data are not strong enough to support the weighty hypothesis, which is that paying mothers more when their children are young can improve the cognitive function of the mothers later in life.

Response: We have revised our paper carefully to increase transparency about our data limitations. We would also like to note a strength of our approach relative to approaches represented in the existing literature. We calculated CSG eligibility since its inception in April 1998 to January 2016 and covered an 18-year exposure period. This extends prior studies that have used self-reported CSG receipt data, which may be biased, at one or two timepoints and established thresholds from self-reported data to create CSG exposure variables (Cluver et al., 2013; Zembe-Mkabile et al., 2016). Further, our cognitive measures have been adapted from the US HRS and are reliable and valid for this study setting and harmonized with those used in HRS and its International Partner Studies (Kobayashi et al., 2020).

We acknowledge the above in our Limitations and Strengths section (page 22, line 806-815): “This study data, which reports CSG eligibility duration since its inception until 2016, extends prior work on CSG receipt that have used self-reported data over a shorter follow-up period and/or used self-reported data to identify thresholds of CSG receipt duration (Cluver et al., 2013; Zembe-Mkabile et al., 2016). Since the increases in CSG amount over time was to match inflation and not to increase in value, the duration of CSG eligibility was considered an adequate proxy for CSG receipt. This approach allowed us to avoid recall or social desirability bias that can occur when self-reports are used or confounding by individual uptake patterns. This design strengthens causal inference for the association between CSG eligibility and mid- life cognitive function. We used validated and reliable measures of cognitive function, which are harmonized with those used in the US HRS and its International Partner Studies around the world and are meaningful for ADRD risk(Kobayashi et al., 2020).”

We have also expanded our limitation section, as described above (page 20, line 746- 794): “We utilized duration of CSG eligibility and not actual CSG receipt in our analyses… Cognitive function was assessed using measures for orientation and episodic memory, which does not include all possible domains of cognitive function. To address this issue, we ran a sensitivity analysis using cognitive function outcome that included numeracy measures in addition to orientation, and episodic memory measures… It is also possible that pre-HAALSI baseline mortality patterns in women may have biased our findings, as aging cohorts are vulnerable to selective survival bias… our findings should be interpreted as conditioned on survival until age 40 years and older in rural South Africa. Finally, our study findings may have limited generalizability outside of a rural South African setting.”

5. Also, it seems like an attribute such as wealth will be confounded with access to health services and therefore longevity, which could mean fewer women of lower socio-economic status are surviving long enough to exhibit symptoms of cognitive decline.

Response: We very much agree with this point - only those who survived until at least 40 years of age were included in HAALSI. Therefore, those who died prior to this, will not be represented in this study. Aging research is particularly vulnerable to selective survival bias (Weuve et al., 2015). It is possible that those who died prior to enrollment in HAALSI had different socioeconomic status than those who were enrolled. Interestingly, authors of this paper have also investigated the differences in sociodemographic variables between those with and without pre-baseline mortality in HAALSI (unpublished manuscript). This was possible with underlying vital statistics data on the source population of the HAALSI cohort. We found that those who died prior to enrollment had only slightly lower household asset index scores compared to those who were enrolled (2.2 vs 2.3, respectively). Therefore, wealth may not strongly confound our findings. However, our study conclusions are likely restricted to the current HAALSI population and measures cognitive function at baseline only.

We have added this limitation in our Limitation and Strengths section (page 21, Line 785 -793): “It is possible that pre-HAALSI baseline mortality patterns in women may have biased our findings, as aging cohorts are vulnerable to selective survival bias (Weuve et al., 2015) . However, women who died prior to enrollment and women who were enrolled in HAALSI had very similar household asset indices (2.2 vs 2.3, respectively, from unpublished analyses using underlying vital statistics data collected by the Agincourt HDSS (Kahn et al., 2012), covering the source population of the HAALSI cohort). Therefore, we believe that differences in household wealth between those enrolled in HAALSI and those not enrolled may not be large in magnitude to severely bias our results. That said, our findings should be interpreted as conditioned on survival until age 40 years and older in rural South Africa.”

6. For these reasons, I am recommending rejection of the paper in its current form as its hypothesis is too stretched to be supported by the data available.

Response: Thank you for your valuable comments. We have incorporated your feedback and suggestions and believe that this has strengthened our manuscript. We hope that we have adequately addressed your concerns.

7. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available? - No

Response: The HAALSI baseline data used in this study is publicly available in the Harvard Dataverse. We have clarified this in our Data Availability Statement (page 23, line 841): The HAALSI data used in this article are publicly available as “HAALSI Baseline Survey” here: https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataverse/haalsi

Editor

1. The guidelines and style of PLOS ONE must be followed.

Response: We have revised the manuscript to ensure that it follows the guidelines and style of PLOS ONE.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

pone.0297673.s003.docx (67.3KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Azizur Rahman

11 Jan 2024

Child Support Grant expansion and cognitive function among women in rural South Africa: findings from a natural experiment in the HAALSI cohort

PONE-D-23-01857R1

Dear Dr. Chakraborty,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter, and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Azizur Rahman, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

1) The authors have done a careful revision and adequately addressed all comments.

2) They must meet the journal's technical requirements (or if anything is pending).

Congratulations and thanks for the authors' sincere efforts.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: In this paper the authors use longitudinal data from the HAALSI cohort in Agincourt, South Africa to investigate the effect of expanded Child Support Grants on cognitive function. Ordinary least squares regression is used to model the relationship between cognitive function and duration of CSG eligibility, adjusted for a number of predictors including mother’s age, education and lifetime number of children. My review raised a number of concerns around the strength of the data and whether it was sufficient to inform the far-reaching hypothesis, that a child support grant can affect the cognitive function of the child’s carers in later life.

The authors have addressed all the concerns that I raised and considerably added to the paper to explain and justify their results. 

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Azizur Rahman

12 Feb 2024

PONE-D-23-01857R1

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Chakraborty,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

If revisions are needed, the production department will contact you directly to resolve them. If no revisions are needed, you will receive an email when the publication date has been set. At this time, we do not offer pre-publication proofs to authors during production of the accepted work. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few weeks to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Professor Azizur Rahman

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Child support grant variable construction and sensitivity analyses.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0297673.s001.docx (152.1KB, docx)
    S2 File. Mapping child support grant exposure.

    (XLS)

    pone.0297673.s002.xls (42KB, xls)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    pone.0297673.s003.docx (67.3KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    The HAALSI data used in this article are publicly available in the Harvard Dataverse. Data from the “HAALSI Baseline Survey” and the “HAALSI Dementia Wave 1” can be found here https://dataverse.harvard.edu/dataverse/harvard?q=HAALSI.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES