Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Mar 7.
Published in final edited form as: Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2022 Aug 7;8(7):687–694. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.07.008

Economic Choice and Heart Rate Fractal Scaling Indicate That Cognitive Effort Is Reduced by Depression and Boosted by Sad Mood

Andrew Westbrook 1, Xiao Yang 1, Lauren M Bylsma 1, Shimrit Daches 1, Charles J George 1, Andrew J Seidman 1, J Richard Jennings 1, Maria Kovacs 1
PMCID: PMC10919246  NIHMSID: NIHMS1872228  PMID: 35948258

Abstract

BACKGROUND:

People with depression typically exhibit diminished cognitive control. Control is subjectively costly, prompting speculation that control deficits reflect reduced cognitive effort. Evidence that people with depression exert less cognitive effort is mixed, however, and motivation may depend on state affect.

METHODS:

We used a cognitive effort discounting task to measure propensity to expend cognitive effort and fractal structure in the temporal dynamics of interbeat intervals to assess on-task effort exertion for 49 healthy control subjects, 36 people with current depression, and 67 people with remitted depression.

RESULTS:

People with depression discounted more steeply, indicating that they were less willing to exert cognitive effort than people with remitted depression and never-depressed control subjects. Also, steeper discounting predicted worse functioning in daily life. Surprisingly, a sad mood induction selectively boosted motivation among participants with depression, erasing differences between them and control subjects. During task performance, depressed participants with the lowest cognitive motivation showed blunted autonomic reactivity as a function of load.

CONCLUSIONS:

Discounting patterns supported the hypothesis that people with current depression would be less willing to exert cognitive effort, and steeper discounting predicted lower global functioning in daily life. Heart rate fractal scaling proved to be a highly sensitive index of cognitive load, and data implied that people with lower motivation for cognitive effort had a diminished physiological capacity to respond to rising cognitive demands. State affect appeared to influence motivation among people with current depression given that they were more willing to exert cognitive effort following a sad mood induction.


Depression is associated with cognitive control deficits, with adverse implications for regulating moods or pursuing goals in daily life (13). Cognitive control is effortful (48), prompting speculation that deficits reflect a reduced propensity to exert cognitive effort rather than reduced capacity for control (1,912). By one hypothesis, people with depression exert effort less because they are more sensitive to effort costs, are less sensitive to potential benefits, and expect that their attempts at control will be less efficacious (10).

However, evidence that depressed individuals are less willing to exert cognitive effort is mixed. Evidence in favor includes that more severe depression correlates with less self-reported effort (13) and that people with depression stop expending cognitive effort sooner than control subjects do in a progressive ratio task (14). People with current or remitted depression and those with higher anhedonia are also less likely to choose physical effort for rewards (1517) and report greater effort than indicated by their actual handgrip squeeze force (18).

On the other hand, adults diagnosed with major depression choose high cognitive effort to earn money as much as control subjects, despite being less likely to choose high physical effort (19). Psychophysiological data also argue against the hypothesis that people with depression exert less cognitive effort. For example, cardiovascular indices (e.g., cardiac pre-ejection period [PEP] and systolic blood pressure) are elevated among people with depression when cognitive demands are low and are attenuated only when demands are high (2022). Thus, people with depression may not be less willing to exert effort, but simply less able.

If people with depression are less willing to exert effort, an important question is whether lower motivation is traitlike or whether it varies by depression status. Specifically, we asked whether people with remitted depression also differed from control subjects in terms of willingness to expend cognitive effort. This question has clinical implications. In one study, decision making about physical effort predicted future relapse among individuals with remitted depression (15).

Willingness to exert cognitive effort may also depend on affect. Both sadness and amotivation are cardinal features of depression, suggesting intuitively that sadness saps motivation. On the other hand, sad affect can enhance memory and decision making among healthy adults (23,24) and those with depression (25). Also, sad primes can increase cardiac indices of engagement, suggesting that sad stimuli may promote motivation (26). Thus, while we hypothesized a priori that sad affect reduces motivation, it is conceivable that a sad mood induction could increase motivation.

Our study tested the hypotheses that motivation for cognitive effort would be diminished in depression and modulated by sad affect. We measured willingness to exert effort in terms of cognitive effort discounting (COGED) (4): participants’ propensity to choose less money for a low-load working memory task versus more money for a high-load working memory task. COGED choices vary with reward sensitivity (2729), striatal dopamine signaling (28), and clinically relevant deficits in schizophrenia (27,30), yet they have never been used to quantify motivation in depression. Given that COGED choices correlate with effort in daily life among healthy adults (4), we also tested whether choices predicted clinician-rated global functioning across healthy and depressed individuals.

We measured on-task effort exertion by cardiovascular reactivity (3134). Specifically, we used detrended fluctuation analysis (DFA) to test for the (loss of) heart rate fractal scaling with cognitive load. Fractal refers to similarity in complexity across large scales (e.g., at low heart rate frequencies) and small scales (high frequencies). Fractal dynamics arise from sympathetic and parasympathetic system interactions at rest, organizing similar variability across timescales (3537). Fractal structure breaks down when vagal control is withdrawn to reallocate resources in response to external demands (32), producing more correlated or periodic behavior (37,38). Importantly, people with depression show blunted or less context-appropriate vagal withdrawal to diverse (e.g., emotional or stressful) stimuli, perhaps reflecting impaired capacity to respond to external demands (3941) [though see (42)].

We chose to examine fractal scaling because of limitations in more widely used cardiovascular measures. Since the seminal work of Obrist (43) and Wright and Kirby (44), multiple cardiovascular dynamics have been mapped to cognitive effort. For example, shorter PEP, higher systolic blood pressure, and lower respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) all are associated with cognitive load (34,4547). Yet, whether such measures scale monotonically with subjective effort remains unclear. For example, one study showed that PEP and systolic blood pressure increase with demands up to a point, but then revert at higher demands (48), suggesting that they index task engagement as a function of ability, rather than pure subjective effort. Furthermore, while these more common cardiovascular measures can distinguish people with depression from control subjects in terms of cognitive demand (20,21,34,49), they have failed to do so in other studies (33,50).

Fractal dynamics are a promising alternative given that they vary monotonically with both physical and cognitive demand (38,51). Moreover, fractal scaling is relatively more stable within individuals (52) and is more normally distributed (53) than many cardiovascular measures and thus may be a more sensitive index of subjective changes in state. Also, in contrast to other methods, DFA is robust to nonstationarity (54).

METHODS AND MATERIALS

Adults (age range, 30.0–58.5 years) with current (n = 36) or remitted (n = 67) depression and healthy control subjects (n = 49) were recruited (demographics in Table S1) from a larger project on risk factors for juvenile-onset mood disorders [for original recruitment details see (55,56)]. Participants were chosen according to DSM-IV criteria, using Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV2Patient Edition (57), by trained clinical interviewers and confirmed by best estimate clinical consensus (58). We assessed participants’ functioning via the Global Assessment of Functioning scale by trained and experienced professional clinical evaluators. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Pittsburgh. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Willingness to exert cognitive effort was measured by the COGED paradigm (4) (Figure 1A, B). In COGED, participants choose between offers of a hard n-back task (n = 2–4) for more money ($2.00 or $5.00) or an easy task (n = 1) for less money. The offer for 1-back for each {task, money} pair starts at half the offer for the hard task (e.g., $2.50 for the 1-back vs. $5.00 for the 3-back). After each choice, the easy task offer amount is titrated up or down, with the adjustment being half as much on each subsequent choice, until participants are indifferent between the offers for each hard task–easy task pair (after 5 choices).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Paradigm overview. Cognitive effort discounting (COGED) paradigm and experimental session overview. (A) Before making COGED decisions, participants experienced 2 rounds of each level of the n-back (n = 1–4). Stimuli consisted of upper case consonants presented one at a time. Participants indicated by button press whether each stimulus was a target or nontarget. Load levels are referred to by lower-case vowels: in the “a” task (the 1-back) a stimulus was a target if repeated after 1 item in sequence, in the “i” task (the 3-back) a stimulus was a target if repeated after 3 items in sequence, etc. (B) After practicing all levels of the nback, participants made a series of choices between paired offers to repeat one of the harder levels of the n-back (n = 2–4) for more money or the easy level (n = 1) for less money. Offers for the easy level were titrated after each choice, asymptotically (5 choices for each load pairing and hard level offer amount) approaching an indifference point, which reflected the degree to which participants subjectively discounted offers based on the perceived difference in effort costs. (C) The session paradigm featured 2 unpaced breathing periods for resting heart rate dynamics; n-back practice; and, immediately following a brief sad or neutral film, COGED choices.

Indifference points define the subjective cost of cognitive effort. For example, if a participant is indifferent between $5.00 for the 3-back and $1.50 for the 1-back, the additional effort of the 3-back subjectively costs $3.50. We defined subjective value (SV) as the indifference point divided by the hard task offer (in this example $1.50/$5.00 = 0.30). Conversely, indifference points quantify willingness to exert effort: Higher SVs indicate greater willingness to choose higher effort for reward. We summarized participants’ motivation by calculating the area under the curve (AUC) defined by mean SV over all hard task load levels and offer amounts (n = 2–4 for $2 and $5).

Participants experienced all levels of the n-back task first (n = 1–4) (Figure 1A). Thus, all participants experienced the n-back under the same conditions. Next, they watched a brief either sad or neutral film, followed immediately by COGED (Figure 1B, C). Participants were asked to execute one of their choices, selected at random among all their choices, by repeating the corresponding n-back task in exchange for the offer they had chosen.

The n-back stimuli (upper-case consonants) were presented for up to 2.5 seconds, during which participants indicated by button press whether stimuli were targets or nontargets. Stimuli were followed by a fixation cross to maintain a fixed trial duration of 3.0 seconds. Participants completed 3 rounds of 40 trials of each n-back level. Feedback about accuracy was given at the end of each round.

Participants were pseudorandomly assigned to watch a 3-minute, either sad or neutral mood induction film clip. The sad film clip was from the movie The Champ, depicting a boy’s anguish over his father’s death, and has been widely used to induce sadness (59). The neutral film depicted aquatic scenes, inducing minimal physiological arousal (60).

Electrocardiography was continuously recorded using disposable, pre-gelled Ag/AgCl spot electrodes (ConMed Andover Medical) with a modified lead II configuration. Cardiac signals were sampled online at 1000 Hz using a MindWare BioNex system and BioLab software (MindWare Technologies Ltd.). Electrocardiography data were inspected, and artifacts were corrected manually by trained raters. Interbeat (R-R) intervals were then extracted by using MindWare heart rate variability analysis software version 3.1.4, which we further used to compute PEP (a measure of sympathetic activity) and high-frequency heart rate variability (an index of RSA, which reflects parasympathetic activity) during n-back task engagement; see Supplement for results.

To quantify fractal scaling, we computed the short-term scaling exponent, α1, derived via DFA (54), from interbeat (R-R) intervals (35,38,61). First, we took the cumulative sum of the mean-centered interval series, b:

y(t)=t=1Tbtb¯ (1)

Next, we segmented the series into k windows, of sizes ranging from 4 to 16 beats [logarithmically spaced, by convention (62,63)]. Then, the series y(t) was detrended in each segment k by removing the linear least-squares fit, producing yk(t). Next, the fluctuation function FN was computed for each segment of size N.

FN=1Nj=1N(y(j)yk(j))2 (2)

Finally, we estimated the exponent a1 as the slope of a log-log plot of the arithmetic mean of the fluctuation function against its corresponding segment size. DFA values were computed using the algorithm implemented in the PhysioNet Cardiovascular Signal Toolbox version 1.1.0 (62,63).

RESULTS

Participants experienced higher levels of the n-back task as more costly, progressively discounting offers to repeat harder n-back tasks (Figure 2A, B). A multilevel regression SVs on load, controlling for offer amount, n-back performance, group, condition, age, and gender, revealed a reliable effect of n-back load (b = −0.61, SE = 0.029, p < .001). Better n-back performance (sensitivity index d′) also correlated with shallower discounting (b = 0.081, SE = 0.029, p = .005), and there was a trending linear effect of increasing age on steeper discounting (b = −0.092, SE = 0.047, p = .050). While these results replicate prior findings (4), including gender as an exploratory covariate further revealed that men discounted more shallowly than women (b = 0.23, SE = 0.11, p = .030) (Table S2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Subjective values of cognitive effort–discounted offers by group and mood induction. Subjective values were computed separately for each level and hard task offer amount ($2 or $5). Subjective values are shown here, averaged across offer amounts following (A) a neutral film and (B) a sad film. (C) Individual differences in discounting quantified by area under the curve. Points show mean area under the curve ± SEM; p values reflect within-group, or within-condition, effects, controlling for age, gender, and mean n-back performance (d′).

To summarize individual differences, we computed the AUC (Figure 2C inset) capturing participants’ discounting without assuming the shape of their discounting function. A regression of AUC values controlling for age, gender, and mean n-back performance (d′) revealed that control subjects discounted less than participants with depression (b = 0.69, SE = 0.32, p = .036), while participants with remitted depression did not differ from control subjects (b = 0.22, SE = 0.29, p = .45). Steeper discounting supports the hypothesis that people with depression are less willing to exert cognitive effort.

Interestingly, discounting was shallower after a sad versus a neutral priming film (b = 1.0, SE = 0.33, p = .0018) (Figure 2C). This motivation-enhancing effect of a sad film was strongest for participants with current depression. In the regression of AUC values, group differences were captured by a negative group by mood interaction for both remitted (b = 21.0, SE = 0.43, p = .013) and control (b = 21.0, SE = 0.40, p = .021) groups versus participants with depression. Indeed, a simpler AUC regression involving only data for participants with depression revealed a clear effect of a sad versus neutral film increasing the SV of offers (b = 0.96, SE = 0.32, p = .0047). There was no corresponding mood induction effect on the SV of offers among control subjects (b = −0.0017, SE = 0.28, p = .995) or participants with remitted depression (b = −0.020, SE = 0.26, p = .94). Note that self-rated sad mood increased reliably for each group relative to preinduction ratings, and groups were indistinguishably sad following the sad film (Figure S1).

If a sad mood induction boosts motivation in depression, this should counteract group effects on effort discounting. To test this prediction, we regressed AUC on group and film type, controlling for age, gender, and n-back d′. Consistent with the prediction, control subjects discounted less than participants with depression following the neutral film (b = 0.75, SE = 0.33, p = .027), but not following the sad film (b = −0.30, SE = 0.30, p = .32). Moreover, following the sad film, participants with current depression discounted less than those with remitted depression (b = −0.77, SE = 0.30, p = .012). Hence, a brief sad film boosted motivation for people with current depression to levels that exceed motivation among those with remitted depression (Figure 2C).

Relationship Between Effort Discounting and Global Functioning

We tested the prediction that shallower discounting also implies greater propensity to exert effort to pursue goals in daily life by regressing on AUC two correlated (Pearson’s r = 0.96, 95% CI [0.95, 0.97], p < .001) but distinct, measures of social, psychological, and occupational functioning derived from the Global Assessment of Functioning scale: 1) the highest level of functioning from the past year and 2) the lowest level of functioning during the week of poorest functioning in the past month.

We separately regressed Global Assessment of Functioning scores on AUC, controlling for age, gender, mood induction, and n-back performance as well as years of education, current depression symptoms (Beck Depression Inventory-II) (64), anxiety symptoms (Beck Anxiety Inventory) (65), and medication status (whether taking any psychoactive medication or not). Regressions revealed relationships between AUC and the highest level of global functioning over the past year (b = 0.13, SE = 0.055, p = .020) (Figure 3A) and during the week of poorest functioning in the last month (b = 0.14, SE = 0.055, p = .010) (Figure 3B). In both cases, willingness to exert cognitive effort correlated with better global functioning (Tables S4, S5).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) correlated with effort discounting. Higher GAF correlated with shallower discounting (higher area under the curve [AUC]) for both (A) the highest functioning over the past year and (B) the lowest level functioning during the week of poorest functioning over the past month. Figures show the relationship between AUC and GAF scores after regressing out age, gender, Beck Depression Inventory-II scores, medication status, average n-back performance, and mood induction. A regression line is plotted with standard errors.

Autonomic Constraints

Heart rate fractal scaling was quantified to index on-task effort exertion via DFA (54). DFA, in brief, quantifies the increase in heart rate variability from small to large scales (α1) (Figure 4A; see Methods and Materials). Fractal scaling was maximized when variability was matched across scales (α11.0) and was diminished when dynamics were either more random (α1<1.0) or more correlated (α1>1.0).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Heart rate fractal scaling across the paradigm, quantified via detrended fluctuation analyses (DFAs). (A) Two examples of interbeat interval (IBI) series along with the corresponding log-log plot of the fluctuation function (<FN>; see Methods and Materials) for each series versus the window size used to quantify the fluctuation function. The slope of the log-log plot quantified the DFA slope (α1) based on window sizes ranging from 4 to 16 beats. (B) Within-participant α1 values were stable across repeated measurements (unpaced breathing during rest time 1 and 2). (C) When viewing either film across participants, α1 increased reliably. (D) Mean ± SEM of α1 increased parametrically with n-back load. In the plots, α1 values are separated by group and by median split on area under the curve across both sad and neutral film conditions.

Fractal scaling was stable within individuals across time points (across rest periods; Pearson’s r = 0.56, 95% CI [.48, .63]; p < .001) (Figure 4B). By contrast, increases in α1 indicated a similar (p = .84) loss of fractal scaling when participants viewed either sad (mean = 1.2) or neutral (mean = 1.2) films versus rest (mean = 1.0; both p < .001) (green in Figure 4C). This pattern was consistent with vagal withdrawal during film viewing (40). Importantly, there was also a clear parametric effect of n-back load on α1, which rose progressively from the 1-back through the 4-back (Figure 4D). Also, steep discounting predicted a blunting of this pattern among participants with current depression (Figure 4D). In contrast, participants with current depression who discounted shallowly showed the same fractal scaling with load as control subjects and participants with remitted depression. Neither parametric load effects nor discounting effects were apparent with either PEP or RSA (Figures S2, S3).

A blunted physiological response among steep discounters with depression suggests a relationship between autonomic reactivity and cognitive effort. To estimate the relationship between reactivity and discounting, we regressed SV on load-specific α1, load, and group, controlling for offer amount and performance (d′). The multilevel model also controlled for variables that we found to affect discounting: mood induction, age, and gender. Along with reliable effects of load (b = −0.61, SE = 0.068, p < .001), sad mood (b = 0.40, SE = 0.14, p = .0051), gender (men vs. women: b = 0.19, SE = 0.084, p = .025), and n-back performance (b = 0.062, SE = 0.024, p = .0087), there was also a positive effect of DFA α1 values (b = 0.19, SE = 0.077, p = .013) and a trend-level DFA α1 × load interaction (b = 0.12, SE = 0.067, p = .079), both indicating that blunted physiological reactivity to cognitive load correlated with steeper effort discounting. Additionally, a group × DFA α1 interaction indicated that blunted reactivity predicted steeper discounting more strongly among those with current versus remitted depression (b = −0.21, SE = 0.097, p = .034), suggesting that autonomic reactivity constrained cognitive effort, especially in current depression. This interaction was not significant with respect to healthy control subjects (b = −0.15, SE = 0.10, p = .15) (full model in Table S3).

DISCUSSION

Diminished cognitive control may partly explain why people with depression are less likely to pursue goals or more likely to ruminate. Yet, whether these deficits reflect diminished cognitive capacities or reduced motivation for cognitive effort is unclear. If people with depression exert less effort, it is furthermore unclear what constrains their motivation. Here, we showed that people with depression were less willing to exert cognitive effort, controlling for cognitive capacity. We also found evidence that a brief, sad film boosted willingness to exert effort in depression, while limited autonomic reactivity may constrain it.

The reduced willingness to exert cognitive effort is independent of abilities. Indeed, people with depression discounted more steeply than their nondepressed peers, even controlling for n-back performance. By contrast, participants with remitted depression discounted the same as control subjects, indicating that motivational deficits were specific to current depression and did not persist in remission.

Importantly, shallower discounting correlates with better global functioning in occupational, social, and psychological domains. The relationship with global functioning highlights the vital importance of motivation for cognitive effort in diverse domains of daily life. Thus, there is a need to understand underlying motivational mechanisms and how they go awry.

Aberrant striatal dopamine function is one candidate explanation of lower motivation. Robust dopamine signaling among healthy adults predicts greater sensitivity to reward benefits and lesser sensitivity to effort costs, and greater willingness to exert cognitive effort (28). Critically, people with depression have reduced reward sensitivity and striatal reward encoding (18,6670), which may underlie lower motivation [though see (69)]. Serotonin may also play a key role. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors can promote physical effort for reward by selectively reducing effort cost sensitivity without altering reward sensitivity (71). Thus, diminished serotonin signaling may make people with depression more sensitive to effort costs.

We also found an intriguing, albeit counterintuitive effect of a brief, sad film, which boosted motivation to exert effort among participants with depression. One explanation is that sad films produce affective outcomes that people with depression desire, thus enhancing their motivation to pursue other goals. Perhaps people with depression prefer sad over happy media not because it increases their sadness, but because sad media is subjectively calming (72,73). Alternatively, people with depression may prefer sadness itself because it affords a form of validation about selfi-dentity (74,75). Sad media may thus boost motivation because it produces desirable affective states (e.g., calmness or validation). The effect may further depend on the locus and/or the controllability of sad content. For example, the motivational consequences of sadness induced by something bad happening in the life of a person with depression may look very different from the consequences of their sadness induced because they watch a brief sad film. While it is beyond the scope of our article, we are intrigued by the prospect that people with depression may seek out sad stimuli (films, music, etc.) [c.f., (72,73)] precisely because it amplifies a kind of sadness, which may have beneficial effects, including increased motivation. Finally, another account considers that the COGED task itself shares key features with attention refocusing strategies that people use to repair their mood (76). Perhaps attentional refocusing after a sad mood induction enhances subjective control over affect that boosts motivation for cognitive tasks. Such possibilities merit future attention, as they may elucidate a means of increasing motivation for cognitive effort in depression.

Our results extend work showing that autonomic reactivity to cognitive load is constrained in depression. DFA revealed a clear parametric effect of working memory load on heart rate dynamics and blunted reactivity specifically among participants with depression who discounted steeply. Neither PEP nor RSA reliably tracked these dimensions (Figures S2, S3), suggesting that fractal scaling was more sensitive to cognitive effort. We further note that the DFA exponent α1 correlated with individual differences in RSA, but not PEP (Figures S4, S5), suggesting that the blunted DFA profile we observed among participants with depression reflects constraints on vagal withdrawal rather than sympathetic arousal.

A blunted DFA profile is consistent with the interpretation that people with depression have lower motivation and therefore exert less effort during demanding tasks. A reverse causal account is also plausible. Namely, people with depression may have a more limited capacity to adapt, physiologically, to demanding tasks, thus constraining their motivation to exert cognitive effort. Note that people with depression show blunted autonomic reactivity to diverse stimuli, and not just cognitive demands, suggesting that their autonomic reactivity is limited in general. Importantly, the capacity for autonomic adaptability is vital (37,38): Blunted reactivity predicts future cardiovascular disease among people with depression (77). Thus, people with depression may particularly avoid contexts that tax autonomic regulatory systems, e.g., including cognitive control tasks that tap vagal function to optimize performance (78). Perhaps people with depression are more sensitive to the physiological demands of such contexts, amplifying their subjective sense of effort. An account based on peripheral autonomic capacity could explain why day-long physical effort diminishes the willingness to exert cognitive effort among healthy individuals (79). Perhaps physical effort and depression similarly deplete capacity for autonomic reactivity, making people more avoidant of additional cognitive and physical demands. Finally, we note that a tighter autonomic constraint on cognitive effort may also help explain why there was a stronger coupling between discounting and heart rate dynamics in depression than for other groups, despite the fact that discounting was related to global functioning across all groups. While many factors influence willingness to exert cognitive effort (e.g., task performance), cardiac inflexibility (77) may be more important for people with depression in that they are especially sensitive to their cardiovascular state when deciding whether to engage in a demanding task.

Key limitations of our study include the between-subjects design; future studies should address whether, within participants, sad affective media can modulate motivation for those with depression. Relatedly, we did not find any individual difference relationships between effort discounting and sadness after viewing the sad film or the degree to which the sad film increased sadness (see the Supplement), but this may in part reflect the inherently lower power of a between-subjects design and the lack of a corresponding pre-/post-discounting measure. Such effects may be detectable in within-subjects designs. Another limitation is that our design precludes testing whether heart rate fractal scaling during n-back performance varies as a function of mood, as the n-back came before the mood induction. Future work is needed to disentangle the casual mechanisms underlying the correlation between blunted autonomic reactivity, measured here by heart rate fractal scaling, and diminished motivation for cognitive effort.

Our results support the hypotheses that motivation for cognitive effort is reduced in current, but not remitted, depression, is boosted by a sad mood induction, and is higher for individuals with greater physiological capacity to respond to increasing cognitive load. Our results also call for increasing attention to heart rate fractal scaling as an index of cognitive effort that is sensitive to both objective load and motivation to do cognitive work. Finally, our results raise new questions about how sad stimuli can impact motivation in depression.

Supplementary Material

Tables and Figures

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS AND DISCLOSURES

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (Grant Nos. F32 MH115600 and K99 MH125021 [to AW] and Grant No. R01 MH113214 [to MK]).

Footnotes

The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Supplementary material cited in this article is available online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.07.008.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Cohen R, Lohr I, Paul R, Boland R (2001): Impairments of attention and effort among patients with major affective disorders. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 13:385–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hammar Å, Årdal G (2009): Cognitive functioning in major depression—a summary. Front Hum Neurosci 3:26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.LeMoult J, Gotlib IH (2019): Depression: A cognitive perspective. Clin Psychol Rev 69:51–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Westbrook A, Kester D, Braver TS (2013): What is the subjective cost of cognitive effort? Load, trait, and aging effects revealed by economic preference. PLoS One 8:e68210–e68218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Apps MAJ, Grima LL, Manohar S, Husain M (2015): The role of cognitive effort in subjective reward devaluation and risky decision-making. Sci Rep 5:16880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Libedinsky C, Massar SAA, Ling A, Chee W, Huettel SA, Chee MWL (2013): Sleep deprivation alters effort discounting but not delay discounting of monetary rewards. Sleep 36:899–904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kool W, McGuire JT, Rosen ZB, Botvinick MM (2010): Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. J Exp Psychol Gen 139:665–682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sayalı C, Badre D (2019): Neural systems of cognitive demand avoidance. Neuropsychologia 123:41–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hammar Å, Strand M, Årdal G, Schmid M, Lund A, Elliott R (2011): Testing the cognitive effort hypothesis of cognitive impairment in major depression. Nordic J Psychiatry 65:74–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Grahek I, Shenhav A, Musslick S, Krebs RM, Koster EHW (2019): Motivation and cognitive control in depression. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 102:371–381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hartlage S, Alloy LB, Vázquez C, Dykman B (1993): Automatic and effortful processing in depression. Psychol Bull 113:247–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Nishiguchi Y, Takano K, Tanno Y (2016): The need for cognition mediates and moderates the association between depressive symptoms and impaired effortful control. Psychiatry Res 241:8–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Marchetti I, Shumake J, Grahek I, Koster EHW (2018): Temperamental factors in remitted depression: The role of effortful control and attentional mechanisms. J Affect Disord 235:499–505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hershenberg R, Satterthwaite TD, Daldal A, Katchmar N, Moore TM, Kable JW, Wolf DH (2016): Diminished effort on a progressive ratio task in both unipolar and bipolar depression. J Affect Disord 196:97–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Berwian IM, Wenzel JG, Collins AGE, Seifritz E, Stephan KE, Walter H, Huys QJM (2020): Computational mechanisms of effort and reward decisions in patients with depression and their association with relapse after antidepressant discontinuation. JAMA Psychiatry 77:513–522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Treadway MT, Bossaller NA, Shelton RC, Zald DH (2012): Effort-based decision-making in major depressive disorder: A translational model of motivational anhedonia. J Abnorm Psychol 121:553–558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Schwartzman AN, Lambert WE, Zald DH (2009): Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS One 4: e6598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cléry-Melin M-L, Schmidt L, Lafargue G, Baup N, Fossati P, Pessiglione M (2011): Why don’t you try harder? An investigation of effort production in major depression. PLoS One 6:e23178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Tran T, Hagen AEF, Hollenstein T, Bowie CR (2021): Physical- and cognitive-effort-based decision-making in depression: Relationships to symptoms and functioning. Clinical Psychological Science 9:53–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Silvia PJ, Mironovová Z, McHone AN, Sperry SH, Harper KL, Kwapil TR, Eddington KM (2016): Do depressive symptoms “blunt” effort? An analysis of cardiac engagement and withdrawal for an increasingly difficult task. Biol Psychol 118:52–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Silvia PJ, Nusbaum EC, Eddington KM, Beaty RE, Kwapil TR (2014): Effort deficits and depression: The influence of anhedonic depressive symptoms on cardiac autonomic activity during a mental challenge. Motiv Emot 38:779–789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Brinkmann K, Gendolla GHE (2008): Does depression interfere with effort mobilization? Effects of dysphoria and task difficulty on cardiovascular response. J Pers Soc Psychol 94:146–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Forgas JP (2013): Don’t worry, be sad! On the cognitive, motivational, and interpersonal benefits of negative mood. Current Directions in Psychological Science 22:225–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Liew TW, Tan S-M (2016): The effects of positive and negative mood on cognition and motivation in multimedia learning environment. Educational Technology & Society 19:104–115. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Alloy LB, Abramson LY (1979): Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? J Exp Psychol Gen 108:441–485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lasauskaite R, Gendolla GHE, Silvestrini N (2013): Do sadness-primes make me work harder because they make me sad? Cogn Emot 27:158–165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Chang WC, Westbrook A, Strauss GP, Chu AOK, Chong CSY, Siu CMW, et al. (2019): Abnormal cognitive effort allocation and its association with amotivation in first-episode psychosis. Psychol Med 50:2599–2609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Westbrook A, van den Bosch R, Määttä JI, Hofmans L, Papadopetraki D, Cools R, Frank MJ (2020): Dopamine promotes cognitive effort by biasing the benefits versus costs of cognitive work. Science 367:1362–1366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Westbrook A, Lamichhane B, Braver T (2019): The subjective value of cognitive effort is encoded by a domain-general valuation network. J Neurosci 39:3934–3947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Culbreth A, Westbrook A, Barch D (2016): Negative symptoms are associated with an increased subjective cost of cognitive effort. J Abnorm Psychol 125:528–536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Fairclough SH, Venables L, Tattersall A (2005): The influence of task demand and learning on the psychophysiological response. Int J Psychophysiol 56:171–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Thayer JF, Hansen AL, Saus-Rose E, Johnsen BH (2009): Heart rate variability, prefrontal neural function, and cognitive performance: The neurovisceral integration perspective on self-regulation, adaptation, and health. Ann Behav Med 37:141–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Nugent AC, Bain EE, Thayer JF, Sollers JJ, Drevets WC (2011): Heart rate variability during motor and cognitive tasks in females with major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 191:1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Richter M, Gendolla GHE, Wright RA (2016): Three decades of research on motivational intensity theory: What we have learned about effort and what we still don’t know. In: Elliot AJ, editor. Advances in Motivation Science. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, 149–186. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tulppo MP, Kiviniemi AM, Hautala AJ, Kallio M, Seppänen T, Mäkikallio TH, Huikuri HV (2005): Physiological background of the loss of fractal heart rate dynamics. Circulation 112:314–319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Perkiömäki JS, Mäkikallio TH, Huikuri HV (2005): Fractal and complexity measures of heart rate variability. Clin Exp Hypertens 27:149–158. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Goldberger AL (1997): Fractal variability versus pathologic periodicity: Complexity loss and stereotypy in disease. Perspect Biol Med 40:543–561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gronwald T, Rogers B, Hoos O (2020): Fractal correlation properties of heart rate variability: A new biomarker for intensity distribution in endurance exercise and training prescription? Front Physiol 11:550572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bylsma LM, Salomon K, Taylor-Clift A, Morris BH, Rottenberg J (2014): Respiratory sinus arrhythmia reactivity in current and remitted major depressive disorder. Psychosom Med 76:66–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Rottenberg J, Salomon K, Gross JJ, Gotlib IH (2005): Vagal withdrawal to a sad film predicts subsequent recovery from depression. Psychophysiology 42:277–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Shinba T (2014): Altered autonomic activity and reactivity in depression revealed by heart-rate variability measurement during rest and task conditions. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 68:225–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hoffmann A, Ettinger U, Reyes Del Paso GA, Duschek S (2017): Executive function and cardiac autonomic regulation in depressive disorders. Brain Cogn 118:108–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Obrist PA (1981): Cardiovascular Psychophysiology: A Perspective. New York: Plenum. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wright RA, Kirby LD (2001): Effort determination of cardiovascular response: An integrative analysis with applications in social psychology. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 33:255–307. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Jennings JR (1986): Memory, thought, and bodily response. In: Coles MGH, Donchin E, Porges SW, editors. Psychophysiology: Systems, Processes, and Applications. New York: Guilford Press, 290–308. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Mandrick K, Peysakhovich V, Rémy F, Lepron E, Causse M (2016): Neural and psychophysiological correlates of human performance under stress and high mental workload. Biol Psychol 121:62–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Althaus M, Mulder LJM, Mulder G, Roon AM, Minderaa RB (1998): Influence of respiratory activity on the cardiac response pattern to mental effort. Psychophysiology 35:420–430. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Richter M, Friedrich A, Gendolla GH (2008): Task difficulty effects on cardiac activity. Psychophysiology 45:869–875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Silvia PJ, Eddington KM, Harper KL, Burgin CJ, Kwapil TR (2019): Appetitive motivation in depressive anhedonia: Effects of piece-rate cash rewards on cardiac and behavioral outcomes. Motiv Sci 6:259–265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hamilton JL, Alloy LB (2016): Atypical reactivity of heart rate variability to stress and depression across development: Systematic review of the literature and directions for future research. Clin Psychol Rev 50:67–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kardan O, Adam KCS, Mance I, Churchill NW, Vogel EK, Berman MG (2020): Distinguishing cognitive effort and working memory load using scale-invariance and alpha suppression in EEG. Neuroimage 211: 116622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Perkiömäki JS, Zareba W, Ruta J, Dubner S, Madoery C, Deedwania P, et al. (2001): Fractal and complexity measures of heart rate dynamics after acute myocardial infarction. Am J Cardiol 88:777–781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Pikkujämsä SM, Mäkikallio TH, Airaksinen KEJ, Huikuri HV (2001): Determinants and interindividual variation of R-R interval dynamics in healthy middle-aged subjects. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 280:H1400–H1406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Peng C-K, Havlin S, Stanley HE, Goldberger AL (1995): Quantification of scaling exponents and crossover phenomena in nonstationary heartbeat time series. Chaos 5:82–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Liu X, Gentzler AL, George CJ, Kovacs M (2009): Responses to depressed mood and suicide attempt in young adults with a history of childhood-onset mood disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 70:644–652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Miller A, Fox NA, Cohn JF, Forbes EE, Sherrill JT, Kovacs M (2002): Regional patterns of brain activity in adults with a history of childhood-onset depression: Gender differences and clinical variability. Am J Psychiatry 159:934–940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.First MB, Gibbon M (2004): The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Disorders (SCID-II). In: Hilsenroth MJ, Segal DL, editors. Comprehensive Handbook of Psychological Assessment, Vol. 2, Personality Assessment. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 134–143 [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Maziade M, Roy MA, Fournier JP, Cliche D, Mérette C, Caron C, et al. (1992): Reliability of best-estimate diagnosis in genetic linkage studies of major psychoses: Results from the Quebec pedigree studies. Am J Psychiatry 149:1674–1686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gross JJ, Levenson RW (1995): Emotion elicitation using films. Cognition and Emotion 9:87–108. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Yang X, Daches S, George CJ, Kiss E, Kapornai K, Baji I, Kovacs M (2019): Autonomic correlates of lifetime suicidal thoughts and behaviors among adolescents with a history of depression. Psychophysiology 56:e13378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Silva LEV, Silva CAA, Salgado HC, Fazan R (2017): The role of sympathetic and vagal cardiac control on complexity of heart rate dynamics. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 312:H469–H477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Goldberger AL, Amaral LA, Glass L, Hausdorff JM, Ivanov PC, Mark RG, et al. (2000): PhysioBank, PhysioToolkit, and PhysioNet: Components of a new research resource for complex physiologic signals. Circulation 101:E215–E220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Vest AN, Poian GD, Li Q, Liu C, Nemati S, Shah AJ, Clifford GD (2018): An open source benchmarked toolbox for cardiovascular waveform and interval analysis. Physiol Meas 39:105004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK (1996): Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA (1998): An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Psychol 56:893–897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Milders M, Reid I, Steele JD (2008): Abnormal temporal difference reward-learning signals in major depression. Brain 131:2084–2093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gradin VB, Kumar P, Waiter G, Ahearn T, Stickle C, Milders M, et al. (2011): Expected value and prediction error abnormalities in depression and schizophrenia. Brain 134:1751–1764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Robinson OJ, Cools R, Carlisi CO, Sahakian BJ, Drevets WC (2012): Ventral striatum response during reward and punishment reversal learning in unmedicated major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 169:152–159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Halahakoon DC, Kieslich K, O’Driscoll C, Nair A, Lewis G, Roiser JP (2020): Reward-processing behavior in depressed participants relative to healthy volunteers. JAMA Psychiatry 77:1286–1295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Eshel N, Roiser JP (2010): Reward and punishment processing in depression. Biol Psychiatry 68:118–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Meyniel F, Goodwin GM, Deakin JW, Klinge C, MacFadyen C, Milligan H, et al. (2016): A specific role for serotonin in overcoming effort cost. Elife 5:e17282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Yoon S, Verona E, Schlauch R, Schneider S, Rottenberg J (2019): Why do depressed people prefer sad music? Emotion 20:613–624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Yoon S, Rottenberg J (2020): Listening to the blues: An ecological momentary assessment of music choice in depression. Emotion 21:1177–1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Arens EA, Stangier U (2020): Sad as a matter of evidence: The desire for self-verification motivates the pursuit of sadness in clinical depression. Front Psychol 11:238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Millgram Y, Joormann J, Huppert JD, Lampert A, Tamir M (2019): Motivations to experience happiness or sadness in depression: Temporal stability and implications for coping with stress. Clinical Psychological Science 7:143–161. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S (2010): Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 30:217–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Kemp AH, Quintana DS, Felmingham KL, Matthews S, Jelinek HF (2012): Depression, comorbid anxiety disorders, and heart rate variability in physically healthy, unmedicated patients: Implications for cardiovascular risk. PLoS One 7:e30777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Molen MW, Somsen RJM, Jennings JR (2000): Developmental change in auditory selective attention as reflected by phasic heart rate changes. Psychophysiology 37:626–633. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Blain B, Schmit C, Aubry A, Hausswirth C, Meur YL, Pessiglione M (2019): Neuro-computational impact of physical training overload on economic decision-making. Curr Biol 29:3289–3297. e4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Tables and Figures

RESOURCES